Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Alcohol and Alcoholism, 2017, 52(5) 610–616

doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agx040
Advance Access Publication Date: 17 June 2017
Article

Article

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/alcalc/article-abstract/52/5/610/3869494 by Adam Ellsworth, Adam Ellsworth on 04 October 2018


Differential Effects of Alcohol on Memory
Performance in Adolescent Men and Women
with a Binge Drinking History
Concepción Vinader-Caerols1,*, Andrew Talk2, Adriana Montañés1,
Aránzazu Duque1, and Santiago Monleón1
1
Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, Blasco Ibáñez, 21, 46010 Valencia, Spain, and 2Department
of Psychology, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia

*Corresponding author: Tel: +34-96-386-46-52; Fax: +34-96-386-46-68; E-mail: concepcion.vinader@uv.es


Received 11 January 2017; Revised 17 May 2017; Accepted 31 May 2017

Abstract
Aims: Binge drinking (BD) is characterized by intermittent consumption of large quantities of alco-
hol in short periods. This pattern of drinking is prevalent among adolescents, and has been asso-
ciated with undermined learning and memory ability. This study investigates the relationships
between a history of BD and the effects of acute exposure to alcohol on learning and memory per-
formance in adolescent men and women.
Methods: A high, acute dose of alcohol or control refreshment was administered to a sample of
172 adolescent undergraduate students, some of which had a history of BD and others of which
had refrained from alcohol consumption. Subsequently, immediate visual memory (IVM) and
working memory (WM) was measured according to the Wechsler Memory Scale in females and
males with different BAC (Experiment 1) and similar BAC (Experiment 2).
Results: In both experiments, IVM was reduced after acute alcohol consumption and there was no
significant main effect of Drinking Pattern. Furthermore, an effect of cognitive alcohol tolerance on
IVM was observed in women but not in men. WM was not affected by alcohol, but a gender differ-
ence was evident in that performance was superior in men than in women.
Conclusions: In adolescents, IVM is more sensitive than WM to impairment by alcohol, and
women are more vulnerable to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol than men, since the cognitive tol-
erance effect of alcohol on IVM develops in BD women but not in BD men. These findings empha-
size the need to investigate the neurotoxic effects of alcohol in adolescent women.
Short summary: In adolescents, immediate visual memory (IVM) is more sensitive than working
memory to impairment by alcohol, and women are more vulnerable to the neurotoxic effects of
alcohol than men, because the cognitive tolerance effect of alcohol on IVM develops in binge
drinking (BD) women but not in BD men.

INTRODUCTION Alcoholism defines BD as a pattern of drinking that brings a per-


Alcohol is one of the most widely consumed psychoactive substances son’s blood alcohol concentration (BAC) to 0.08 g per cent or
in the world, especially among young people and adolescents (Hibell above (NIAAA, 2004). This pattern involves the intake of large
et al., 2007; Chavez et al., 2011). Among Spanish 14- to 18-year- quantities of alcohol in a short time (about 2 h), followed by a peri-
olds, 32.2% develop a pattern of alcohol consumption known as od of abstinence, with the repeated alternation between intoxica-
binge drinking (BD). The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and tion and withdrawal being particularly deleterious for the brain

© The Author 2017. Medical Council on Alcohol and Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. 610
Alcohol and Alcoholism, 2017, Vol. 52, No. 5 611

(Maurage et al., 2012; Petit et al., 2014). Men typically show a METHODS
higher prevalence of alcohol consumption than women, but in
Spain, the incidence of alcohol consumption in 14–18-year-old
Subjects
adolescents is higher among females than males. With respect to Undergraduate students from the University of Valencia, Spain,
BD, this pattern is similar in both sexes in 14–16-year-old adoles- filled in a self-report questionnaire about consumption of drugs, fre-
cents and is more widespread among men than women in the age quency and level of alcohol consumption, hours and quality of sleep,
range of 17–18 years (OED, 2016). and physical and psychological health. One hundred and seventy-
Animal (Guerri and Pascual, 2010; Van Skike et al., 2012; two subjects (79 males and 93 females) were recruited for the study
based on strict inclusion and exclusion criteria. The following inclu-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/alcalc/article-abstract/52/5/610/3869494 by Adam Ellsworth, Adam Ellsworth on 04 October 2018


Alfonso-Loeches et al., 2013) and human (Caldwell et al., 2005; Liu
et al., 2010; Squeglia et al., 2012) studies show that females are sion criteria were used: 18–19 years old, a healthy body mass index
more susceptible than males to the effects of alcohol, which can lead (mean in men: 22.91 ± 0.97, mean in women: 21.54 ± 0.24) and
to a reduction of prefrontal cortical and hippocampal volume (De good health (without major medical problems). The exclusion cri-
Bellis et al., 2000, 2005) and related cognitive deficits, such as teria were as follows: taking medication; a history of mental disor-
undermined reward-related learning, spatial memory, and working ders (diagnosed by a health professional according to DSM criteria);
memory (WM; Squeglia et al., 2012; Ozsoy et al., 2013). Alcohol- an irregular sleep pattern (non-restorative sleep and/or irregular
related cognitive impairments are more pronounced when there is a schedule); having consumed, even sporadically, any drug (apart
pattern of binge drinking and when consumption takes place during from alcohol) or having a history of substance abuse, including caf-
adolescence (Peeters et al., 2014). feine, tobacco and alcohol; and having first-degree relatives with
During adolescence, important physical, social and cognitive alcoholism problems. A telephone interview of approximately 15
skills are acquired and the brain undergoes considerable structural minutes was conducted with each subject in order to confirm the
and functional changes associated with the development of these information provided in the self-report and to fix the date and time
skills (Blakemore, 2008). Any disruptive effects of alcohol on learn- of the test.
ing and memory abilities have a particularly deep impact during this Selected students were invited to participate in the current study
period and can last through adulthood. Moreover, due to its greater if they had reported refraining from alcohol consumption (or very
plasticity, the adolescent brain appears to be particularly susceptible sporadic consumption) or a history of alcohol use classified as a BD
to experience-dependent learning processes associated with con- pattern according to the NIAAA criteria for Spain (see López-
sumption of alcohol that can lead to tolerance and addiction (Nixon Caneda et al., 2014). The mean age at onset of alcohol use was
and McClain, 2010). 15.9 ± 0.23 for sporadic consumers and 14.6 ± 0.11 for binge drin-
There is evidence suggesting that female adolescents are espe- kers. Participants were classified as binge drinkers if they had drunk
cially vulnerable to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol on cognition six or more standard drink units (SDU = 10 g of alcohol) of distilled
(Caldwell et al., 2005; Squeglia et al., 2011; Alfonso-Loeches et al., spirits (alcohol content ≥40 vol.%) in the case of men or five or
2013). Thus, we consider them to be a potential risk population in more SDU in the case of women on a minimum of two or three
which the consequences of alcohol abuse, such as cognitive effects, occasions a month throughout the 12 months prior to the survey.
need to be studied in a more exhaustive manner, as other authors Participants were classified as refrainers if they had never previously
have already suggested (López-Caneda et al., 2014). In the current drunk alcoholic drinks or had drunk very sporadically (less than
study, we explored alcohol tolerance in male and female adoles- one SDU on less than 3 occasions per year; e.g. 250 ml of beer, per
cents, evaluating gender differences and potential relationships occasion) since the onset of their alcohol use.
between a BD pattern and the effects of acute alcohol exposure on Informed consent was obtained from all participants and the
learning and memory. This interaction needs to be studied, because study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines for human
some long-term effects of repeated alcohol exposure in adolescents experimentation of the Ethics Committee of the University of Valencia
(such as alcohol tolerance or damaged cognitive abilities) are only (ethical authorization number: H1327668013118) and with those of
observed, or can be observed more readily, following an acute dose the Helsinki Declaration. Participants were told to abstain from drink-
of alcohol. ing alcohol and performing heavy physical exercise on the evening/
It is known that tolerance can be developed early in adolescents night prior to the experiment, and all subjects were instructed to
and young adults without alcohol use disorder (AUD) (Saha et al., follow their normal sleep patterns and usual meal routine at least
2006; Schuckit et al., 2008). Considering the scarcity of studies that two hours before the experimental session.
evaluate the phenomenon of tolerance in healthy adolescents and Male and female alcohol-refraining and BD subjects were allocated
the potential vulnerability of females to the neurotoxic effects of to one of two groups that received either alcohol or a control drink
alcohol, we hypothesized that: (a) we would observe an interaction during the study. Allocations was random, except for subjects who
between drinking pattern and an acute alcohol dose, in which binge had never previously drunk alcohol, who were assigned to the control
drinkers would perform better in memory tasks than refrainers drink group for ethical reasons, while some of the subjects who gener-
when given alcohol (as they would have developed alcohol toler- ally refrained from alcohol but had reported previously having at least
ance) and binge drinkers would perform worse than refrainers after one drink were assigned to the group that received alcohol. The male
being given the control drink (as their memory would have been subgroups consisted of 11–23 participants each, while the female sub-
damaged) and (b) gender differences would be observed in these groups consisted of 11–27 participants each. In the female subgroups,
effects, being more pronounced in women than men. Two experi- data regarding menstrual cycle were registered in the self-report and
ments were carried out with these objectives in mind; the first one in during the telephone interview, and cycle phase was taken into
men and women separately, with different BACs; and the second account in the test in order to counterbalance this variable.
one in a selection of men and women with similar BAC, which The characteristics of the study population are summarized in
allowed us to study the effect of gender. Table 1.
612 Alcohol and Alcoholism, 2017, Vol. 52, No. 5

Table 1. Characteristics of the study population

Occasional consumers (n = 35) Binge drinkers (n = 96)

Men Women Men + women Men Women Men + women


(n = 17) (n = 18) (n = 35) (n = 45) (n = 51) (n = 96)

Age at first alcohol consumption 15.9 ± 0.29 15.8 ± 0.29 15.9 ± 0.20 14.7 ± 0.16&&& 14.6 ± 0.15&&& 14.6 ± 0.11+++
Mean number of occasions per month 0.2 ± 0.08 0.2 ± 0.04 0.2 ± 0.04 2.7 ± 0.12&&& 2.6 ± 0.12&&& 2.6 ± 0.12+++
Mean number of drinks per occasion 0.7 ± 0.2 0.8 ± 0.15 0.8 ± 0.15 6.8 ± 0.24&&& 6 ± 0.27*&&& 6.4 ± 0.19+++

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/alcalc/article-abstract/52/5/610/3869494 by Adam Ellsworth, Adam Ellsworth on 04 October 2018


Mean number of BD episodes per month NA NA NA 2.5 ± 0.16 2.3 ± 0.17 2.3 ± 0.12
Mean duration of BD pattern (in months) NA NA NA 13 ± 0.91 11.4 ± 0.33 12.1 ± 0.46
until the beginning of experiment

Refrainers + occasional consumers (n = 76) Binge drinkers (n = 96)

Men Women Men + women Men Women Men + women


(n = 34) (n = 42) (n = 76) (n = 45) (n = 51) (n = 96)

Mean number of stimulant drinks: coke, 0.4 ± 0.11 0.5 ± 0.11 0.5 ± 0.08 0.8 ± 0.15 0.7 ± 0.11 0.7 ± 0.09
tea or coffee/day
Smoker: no/yes 34/0 42/0 75/0 45/5 51/8 51/8
BMI 23.2 ± 0.59 21.6 ± 0.37* 22.3 ± 0.34 22.7 ± 0.34 21.5 ± 0.33* 22.1 ± 0.24
STAI trait 30.9 ± 5.14 30.6 ± 3.62 30.8 ± 3.04 24.3 ± 3.29 28 ± 3.53 26.3 ± 2.42
STAI state 22.9 ± 2.41 30.8 ± 4.09 27.2 ± 2.52 27 ± 2.59 30.9 ± 2.8 29.1 ± 1.92

The results are expressed as number or mean ± SEM for refrainers, occasional consumers and binge drinkers.
NA: not applicable; STAI: State and Trait Anxiety Inventory.
*P < 0.05: Statistically significant difference between men and women in the same group according to Student’s t-tests.
&&&
P < 0.001: Statistically significant difference between binge drinkers and occasional consumers in the same sex according to Student’s t-tests.
+++
P < 0.001: Statistically significant difference between binge drinkers and occasional consumers according to Student’s t-tests.

Tests and apparatus in the form of vodka (120 ml) mixed with the refreshment. The sub-
A digital automatic blood pressure monitor (M10-IT, OMRON, jects were instructed to consume their drink within a period of
Spain) was used to measure systolic and diastolic blood pressure 20 min. After finishing the drink, all subjects rinsed their mouths
and heart rate in all the subjects. with water and waited for 20 min.
An alcoholmeter (Alcoquant® 6020, Envitec, Germany) was Following the treatment and wait interval, the concentrations of
employed to measure the concentration of alcohol in the air exhaled alcohol in exhaled air were measured. Blood pressure and heart rate
before and after intake of a drink. were then measured to ensure they were in the normal range (no more
The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (Saunders than 14 mmHg systolic and 9 mmHg diastolic and less than 100
et al., 1993) was employed to measure problematic use of alcohol bpm). Subjects then performed the IVM and WM tests, and alcohol
among the subjects. The AUDIT consists of 10 questions that evalu- concentration was measured once again. All the tests were performed
ate the quantity and frequency of alcohol intake and alcohol-related between 4:00 pm and 8:00 pm and members of the groups that
behaviours and consequences. It uses a range of 0–40, in which a received alcohol remained on the premises until their alcohol concen-
score of 8 or more indicates a problematic use of alcohol. A higher tration dropped to legal limits for driving. The measures of the con-
score is related to greater severity of problematic drinking. centrations of alcohol in exhaled air were transformed to BAC.
Immediate visual memory (IVM) and WM were both assessed In Experiment 1, BAC was 0.00 g/l for men and women before
using the Wechsler Memory Scale 3rd Edition (WMS-III; adapted ver- the alcoholic drink, and 0.33 ± 0.08 g/l for men and 0.5 ± 0.02 g/l
sion for Spanish population) (Wechsler, 2004). The IVM subscales for women after drinking and during the memory tests. This differ-
require the respondent to recognize faces and remember scenes. The ence in BAC between men and women was statistically significant
WM subscales require the respondent to put letter-number sets in order (F(1,67) = 41.5, P < 0.001). A counterbalanced selection of men and
and to reproduce visual-spatial sequences. Subjects’ scores on the WM women with similar BAC (0.38 ± 0.08 g/l) without statistically sig-
and IVM scales were transformed into centiles according to the sub- nificant differences (F(1,42) = 3.54, ns) participated in Experiment 2.
ject’s age.

Statistical analyses
Procedure Data for men and women were analysed separately (Experiment 1)
First, alcohol concentration was measured using the alcoholmeter in or together (Experiment 2). Body mass index (BMI) was included as
the subjects with a history of BD pattern to ensure that they had not a covariate in each analysis to control for the effect of values that
consumed alcohol previously on the day in question. Alcohol use by varied among the subjects. The data were subjected to parametric
the BD subjects was then assessed using the AUDIT test (mean score: analysis after checking that they met the criteria for normality and
7.1 ± 0.45 in men and 6.3 ± 0.35 in women). None of the subjects homogeneity of variances. The data were also checked to confirm
was classified as alcohol-dependent. Each subject then received that the assumptions for analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) were
330 ml of a lime- or orange-flavoured refreshment. The subjects met. An ANCOVA was performed for each measure (IVM and
assigned to the alcohol groups were administered 38.4 g of alcohol WM) for men and women separately (Experiment 1) or together
Alcohol and Alcoholism, 2017, Vol. 52, No. 5 613

(Experiment 2). Each analysis contained the between-subjects factors In women (see Fig. 2), administration of the alcoholic drink led
‘Drinking Pattern’ (refrainers, binge drinkers) and ‘Treatment’ (con- to lower IVM scores than administration of the control drink
trol drink, alcohol) as independent variables and BMI as a covariate. (F(1,88) = 18.206, P < 0.001). There was no significant main effect
When their interaction was statistically significant, pairwise compar- of Drinking Pattern on IVM performance (F(1,88) = 0.005, ns).
isons were carried out. All analyses were performed using the ‘SPSS’ However, the interaction between Treatment and Drinking Pattern
Statistics software package, version 22.0 for Windows (IBM, 2013). was significant (F(1,88) = 7.19, P < 0.01). Pairwise comparisons
indicated that subjects with a history of refraining from drinking
had lower scores when given alcohol than when given the control
RESULTS

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/alcalc/article-abstract/52/5/610/3869494 by Adam Ellsworth, Adam Ellsworth on 04 October 2018


drink (F(1,39) = 30.557, P < 0.001), but there was no significant
A summary of descriptive statistics for IVM and WM in Experiments difference among binge drinkers between those given alcohol versus
1 and 2 is presented in Table 2. the control drink. However, among the women given alcohol, binge
drinkers had higher scores than refrainers (F(1,39) = 4.680, P <
Experiment 1: Effects of a high dose of alcohol on 0.05). This suggests that tolerance develops in binge drinkers so that
the effect of alcohol on their IVM performance after drinking alco-
performance of memory tests in adolescent males (BAC:
hol is less than that seen in refrainers. Finally, binge drinkers given
0.33 ± 0.08 g/l) and females (BAC: 0.5 ± 0.02 g/l) with a
the control drink obtained lower scores than refrainers given
history of BD or having refrained from alcohol use the control drink, but this effect was only marginally significant
Immediate visual memory (F(1,48) = 3.733, P = 0.053), suggesting an association between BD
In men (see Fig. 1), IVM was lower in subjects given alcohol than and reduced performance in this task, even in the absence of alcohol.
in those given the control drink (F(1,74) = 7.119, P < 0.01). There
was no significant main effect of Drinking Pattern on IVM per-
formance (F(1,74) = 0.002, ns). The interaction between Drinking Working memory
Pattern and Treatment was also not statistically significant (F(1,74) = In men, WM was not significantly different between subjects given the
0.646, ns). control drink and those receiving alcohol Treatment (F(1,74) = 1.285,
ns), and Drinking Pattern did not have a statistically significant effect
Table 2. Summary of descriptive statistics for IVM and WM (F(1,74) = 0. 61, ns). The effect of the interaction between Drinking

Experiment 1

Male subgroups IVM WM


BAC 0.33 ± 0.08 g/l

Refraining-Control drink 40.75 ± 6.79 58.24 ± 5.75


Refraining Alcohol 30.62 ± 7.58 48 ± 6.99
BD-Control drink 44.73 ± 5.66 51.37 ± 4.99
BD-Alcohol 24.75 ± 4.72 46.74 ± 5.09

Female subgroups IVM WM


BAC 0.5 ± 0.02 g/l

Refraining-Control drink 55.62 ± 5.49 40.41 ± 4.05


Refraining Alcohol 17.52 ± 3.69*** 34.98 ± 5.07
BD-Control drink 40.51 ± 5.43+ 36.05 ± 3.26
BD-Alcohol 31.65 ± 5.79# 30.37 ± 4.04
Figure 1. Performance of the IVM task (mean ± SEM) in males with a BAC of
Experiment 2 0.33 ± 0.08 g/l. **P < 0.01 versus Control drink.
BAC 0.38 ± 0.08 g/l

Male subgroups IVM WM

Refraining-Control drink 40.75 ± 6.78 58.24 ± 5.75


Refraining Alcohol 34.05 ± 10.95 57.54 ± 8.9
BD-Control drink 44.73 ± 5.65 51.37 ± 4.99
BD-Alcohol 21.32 ± 7.44* 42.2 ± 7.17

Female subgroups IVM WM

Refraining-Control drink 55.62 ± 5.49 40.41 ± 4.05


Refraining Alcohol 18.05 ± 5.5*** 41.12 ± 7.58
BD-Control drink 40.51 ± 5.43 36.05 ± 3.26
BD-Alcohol 33.08 ± 10.94 35.33 ± 7.21

IVM, immediate visual memory; WM, working memory; BD, binge drink-
ing. ***P < 0.001 versus Refraining-Control drink in women of the same
experiment, #P < 0.05 versus Refraining Alcohol in women of the same
experiment, +P = 0.053 versus Refraining-Control drink in women of the Figure 2. Performance of the IVM task (mean ± SEM) in females with a BAC
same experiment, *P < 0.05 versus BD-Control drink in men of the same of 0.5 ± 0.02 g/l. ***P < 0.001 versus Refraining-Control drink; #P < 0.05 ver-
experiment. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. sus Refraining Alcohol.
614 Alcohol and Alcoholism, 2017, Vol. 52, No. 5

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/alcalc/article-abstract/52/5/610/3869494 by Adam Ellsworth, Adam Ellsworth on 04 October 2018


Figure 3. Performance of the IVM task (mean ± SEM) in men and females with a BAC of 0.38 ± 0.08 g/l. ***P < 0.001 versus Refraining-Control drink in women;
*P < 0.05 versus BD-Control drink in men.

Pattern and Treatment on WM was not statistically significant


(F(1,74) = 0.242, ns).
In women, WM was not significantly different in subjects given
the control drink versus those receiving the alcohol Treatment
(F(1,88) = 1.651, ns), and the effect of Drinking Pattern was not
statistically significant (F(1,88) = 1.096, ns). No effect of the inter-
action between Treatment and Drinking Pattern on WM perform-
ance was detected (F(1,88) = 0.001, ns).

Experiment 2: Effects of a high dose of alcohol on


performance of memory tests in adolescent males
versus females (BAC: 0.38 ± 0.08 g/l) with a history
of BD alcohol or having refrained from alcohol use
Immediate visual memory Figure 4. Performance of the WM task (mean ± SEM) in males and females.
+++
Administration of the alcoholic drink led to lower IVM scores than P < 0.005 versus men.

administration of a control drink (F(1,126) = 14.139, P < 0.001).


However, there were no significant main effects of Drinking Pattern (F(1,126) = 0.36, ns), Treatment and Gender (F(1,126) = 0.389, ns)
(F(1,126) = 0.109, ns) or Gender (F(1,126) = 0.226, ns). The interac- and Drinking Pattern and Gender (F(1,126) = 0.578, ns) and
tions between Treatment and Drinking Pattern (F(1,126) = 0.436, Treatment, Drinking Pattern and Gender (F(1,126) = 0.19, ns) were
ns), Treatment and Gender (F(1,126) = 0.620, ns) and Drinking not significant.
Pattern and Gender (F(1,126) = 0.111, ns) were not significant.
However, the three-way interaction between Treatment, Drinking
Pattern and Gender (see Fig. 3) was found to be significant (F(1,126) = DISCUSSION
4.93, P < 0.05). Pairwise comparisons of the interaction indicated that
Alcohol consumption is highly prevalent during adolescence in
women, but not men, with a history of refraining from drinking had
many countries, and an increase in BD has become apparent among
lower scores when given alcohol than when given the control drink
young people, especially women, over recent years (Hibell et al.,
(F(1,32) = 19.073, P < 0.001). Among binge drinking subjects, men
2007; Chavez et al., 2011; OED, 2016). This pattern of consump-
but not women had lower scores when given alcohol than when given
tion (based on repeated alternations between acute intoxication and
the control drink (F(1,32) = 19.073, P < 0.001).
withdrawal periods) is particularly neurotoxic, independently of the
global alcohol intake, with the cognitive consequences perhaps hav-
Working memory ing been underestimated (Maurage et al., 2012).
Subjects receiving the alcohol Treatment were not significantly differ- With this in mind, our first hypothesis was that the long-term
ent from controls with respect to WM performance (F(1,126) = 0.27, effects of repeated alcohol exposure during adolescence (such as
ns), and there was no statistically significant effect of Drinking Pattern alcohol tolerance or damaged cognitive abilities) would be linked to
on WM (F(1,126) = 3.916, ns). The factor Gender was significant relationships between a history of BD and the magnitude of deficit
(F(1,126) = 11.82, P < 0.005), as men performed better than women that occurs following an acute high dose of alcohol. Our results con-
(see Fig. 4). The interactions between Treatment and Drinking Pattern firmed this hypothesis in the case of the IVM abilities of adolescent
Alcohol and Alcoholism, 2017, Vol. 52, No. 5 615

women with an average BAC of 0.5 ± 0.02 g/l. There was an inter- dysfunctions in neurophysiological mechanisms, as well as the
action with IVM task performance, in which women refrainers given recruitment of additional attentional/WM resources to enable said
alcohol performed worse than those given a control drink, while binge drinkers to perform the task adequately (López-Caneda et al.,
alcohol or control drink administration produced no difference in 2013). The effect of gender showed that men performed better than
women binge drinkers. This interaction may be explained by two women. Some evidence obtained in adult subjects suggests that visuo-
separate processes. Firstly, women binge drinkers given the con- spatial functioning of the WM is superior in males than in females
trol drink did worse in the IVM task than refrainers given the con- (Rizk-Jackson et al., 2006). However, it should be mentioned that
trol drink, indicating an association between BD and impaired few adult studies have examined sex differences in WM, and those

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/alcalc/article-abstract/52/5/610/3869494 by Adam Ellsworth, Adam Ellsworth on 04 October 2018


performance in this memory ability, even during the absence of have reported mixed results (Lejbak et al., 2011).
alcohol. Although this effect was only marginally significant, it It has been found that individuals with low sensitivity to the
reinforces the findings of previous studies reporting links between acute effects of alcohol show increased P3 event-related potential
frequent alcohol use in adolescence and brain structural changes reactivity to alcohol cues (Bartholow et al., 2007, 2010). This low
or cognitive deficits (De Bellis et al., 2005; Guerri and Pascual, alcohol sensitivity is a risk factor for the development of AUDs.
2010; Squeglia et al., 2011; Alfonso-Loeches et al., 2013). Despite that men generally report lower sensitivity to alcohol than
Secondly, female binge drinkers given alcohol scored better than women (e.g. Petit et al., 2013), the female adolescent drinkers in
refrainers given alcohol. This effect suggests that tolerance reduces our study showed cognitive tolerance and therefore may be more
the performance deficit in this task after drinking alcohol. This likely than males to develop additional alcohol-related problems.
phenomenon of cognitive tolerance is also reinforced by the fact This is in accordance with the view that women are more suscep-
that the IVM performance of female binge drinkers receiving the tible than men to the cognitive consequences of alcohol use
alcoholic drink did not differ from that of binge drinkers given the (Caldwell et al., 2005; Squeglia et al., 2011). Given the clinical
control drink. implications derived from varying sensitivity to alcohol (Schuckit
Although tolerance to alcohol is a well-known fact, few data are et al., 2008), we will include an alcohol sensitivity questionnaire in
available on the prevalence of tolerance in non-dependent adolescents our future research.
and young adults (Schuckit et al., 2008). Furthermore, there is con- The limitations of this study are the aforementioned lack of an
flicting evidence about the effects of tolerance to alcohol on memory. alcohol sensitivity measure and the inherent limitation of the use of
It has been conjectured that previous failures to detect an effect of tol- self-reported data about alcohol use. Likewise, other variables, as
erance to alcohol on memory could reflect changes in neuropsycho- depression and impulsivity, could have interfered with the interpret-
logical functioning resulting from years of heavy drinking ation of the results. It is also unclear whether the differences
(Poulos et al., 1981). Longitudinal results also show that the observed between BD and refraining subjects were consequences of
effect of tolerance to alcohol is less likely to be developed on the BD pattern or whether they were present before BD commenced,
cognitive skills than on fine motor skills in heavy drinkers in which case they might provide clues to the origins of BD. This is
(Brumback et al., 2017). In fact, the development of tolerance in relevant, as prolonged and heavy alcohol use might further weaken
young non-dependent drinkers could be obscured by additional already existing deficiencies (Peeters et al., 2015). Exploring this
factors. Thus, maturation-related physiological changes that issue is difficult in human studies with adolescent subjects under
occur in the mid-teens-to-early-20s might be mistaken for toler- legal age, but the available literature suggests that BD is associated
ance in that they contribute to a reduced effect of alcohol as the with neural abnormalities during adolescence (Squeglia et al., 2014).
individual grows older and the brain matures, becoming less In humans, structural brain changes resulting from problematic
reactive to alcohol (Chung et al., 2001; Wells et al., 2006). drinking and leading to disrupted memory performance have been
In males with a BAC of 0.33 ± 0.08 g/l, an overall main effect of reported (Bagga et al., 2014). Longitudinal studies that include the
acute alcohol on IVM performance was observed so that those time of onset of adolescent BD would be useful in establishing
receiving alcohol performed worse than those receiving the control the causes and effects of this pattern of alcohol use. Discovering the
drink. There was evidence neither of neurotoxic effects of Drinking causes and effects of individual differences in alcohol consumption
Pattern nor the tolerance phenomenon in men. patterns is instrumental to designing programmes and policy to
In Experiment 2, females and males had a similar BAC of 0.38 ± reduce the impact of drinking in a highly vulnerable population
0.08 g/l and could be statistically compared, evaluating gender differ- such as adolescent and young people.
ences in order to contrast our second hypothesis. In the IVM task, In view of these results, and despite the aforementioned limita-
women developed cognitive tolerance to alcohol, unlike men. This tions, our study suggests that (a) IVM is more sensitive than WM to
effect was less pronounced than in Experiment 1. We consider that impairment by alcohol and (b) adolescent women are more vulner-
the development of tolerance in IVM depends on BAC, as when able to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol than men, because the cog-
BAC increases the effect of tolerance becomes more obvious, with nitive tolerance effect of alcohol on IVM develops in BD women but
significant differences not only between female refrainers who not in BD men. The ‘telescoping effect’ is described as the phenom-
received alcohol versus refreshment, but also between female refrai- enon of women beginning to drink earlier and progressing more
ners versus binge drinkers receiving alcohol, when BAC was higher rapidly from drinking onset to problem drinking compared to men
(Experiment 1). It is possible that men with a BAC higher than that (Piazza et al., 1989; Johnson et al., 2005; Sugarman et al., 2009).
of the subjects in the present study would display a cognitive toler- This effect may help to understand why adolescent women develop
ance effect of alcohol on IVM. cognitive tolerance before men.
In the WM task, males and females with different or similar
BAC were not affected by alcohol consumption or BD pattern.
Other studies have reported that young binge drinkers exhibit
anomalies in neural activity involved in attentional/WM processes,
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
and suggest that this anomalous neural activity reflects underlying The authors wish to thank Mr Brian Normanly for his editorial assistance.
616 Alcohol and Alcoholism, 2017, Vol. 52, No. 5

FUNDING Maurage P, Joassin F, Speth A, et al. (2012) Cerebral effects of binge drink-
ing: respective influences of global alcohol intake and consumption pat-
This work was supported by the ‘Generalitat Valenciana’ [PROMETEO/
tern. Clin Neurophysiol 123:892–901.
2011/048; PROMETEO-II/2015/020] and ‘Ministerio de Economía y
National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (2004) NIAAA council
Competitividad’ [PSI2013−44491-P], Spain.
approves definition of binge drinking. NIAAA Newsletter 3:3.
Nixon K, McClain JA. (2010) Adolescence as a critical window for develop-
ing an alcohol use disorder: current findings in neuroscience. Curr Opin
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
Psychiatry 23:227–32.
None declared. Observatorio Español sobre Drogas (OED). (2016) Encuesta Escolar sobre

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/alcalc/article-abstract/52/5/610/3869494 by Adam Ellsworth, Adam Ellsworth on 04 October 2018


Uso de Drogas en Estudiantes de Enseñanzas Secundarias (ESTUDES)
2014-2015. Madrid: Ministerio de Sanidad, Servicios Sociales e Igualdad.
REFERENCES Ozsoy S, Durak AC, Esel E. (2013) Hippocampal volumes and cognitive
Alfonso-Loeches S, Pascual M, Guerri C. (2013) Gender differences in functions in adult alcoholic patients with adolescent-onset. Alcohol
alcohol-induced neurotoxicity and brain damage. Toxicology 311:27–34. 47:9–14.
Bagga D, Khushu S, Modi S, et al. (2014) Impaired visual information process- Peeters M, Janssen T, Monshouwer K, et al. (2015) Weaknesses in executive
ing in alcohol-dependent subjects: a proton magnetic resonance spectros- functioning predict the initiating of adolescents’ alcohol use. Dev Cogn
copy study of the primary visual cortex. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 75:817–26. Neurosci 16:139–46.
Bartholow BD, Henry EA, Lust SA. (2007) Effects of alcohol sensitivity on P3 Peeters M, Vollebergh WA, Wiers RW, et al. (2014) Psychological changes
event-related potential reactivity to alcohol cues. Psychol Addict Behav and cognitive impairments in adolescent heavy drinkers. Alcohol Alcohol
21:555–63. 49:182–6.
Bartholow BD, Lust SA, Tragesser SL. (2010) Specificity of P3 event-related Petit G, Kornreich C, Verbanck P, et al. (2013) Gender differences in reactiv-
potential reactivity to alcohol cues in individuals low in alcohol sensitiv- ity to alcohol cues in binge drinkers: a preliminary assessment of event-
ity. Psychol Addict Behav 24:220–8. related potentials. Psychiatry Res 209:494–503.
Blakemore SJ. (2008) The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: Petit G, Maurage P, Kornreich C, et al. (2014) Binge drinking in adolescents:
267–77. a review of neurophysiological and neuroimaging research. Alcohol
Brumback T, Cao D, McNamara P, et al. (2017) Alcohol-induced perform- Alcohol 49:198–206.
ance impairment: a 5-year re-examination study in heavy and light drin- Piazza NJ, Vrbka JL, Yeager RD. (1989) Telescoping of alcoholism in women
kers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 234:1749–59. alcoholics. Int J Addict 24:19–28.
Caldwell LC, Schweinsburg AD, Nagel BJ, et al. (2005) Gender and adoles- Poulos CX, Wolff L, Zilm DH, et al. (1981) Acquisition of tolerance to
cent alcohol use disorders on BOLD (blood oxygen level dependent) alcohol-induced memory deficits in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
response to spatial working memory. Alcohol Alcohol 40:194–200. 73:176–9.
Chavez PR, Nelson DE, Naimi TS, et al. (2011) Impact of a new gender- Rizk-Jackson AM, Acevedo SF, Inman D, et al. (2006) Effects of sex on object
specific definition for binge drinking on prevalence estimates for women. recognition and spatial navigation in humans. Behav Brain Res 173:
Am J Prev Med 40:468–71. 181–90.
Chung T, Martin CS, Winters KC, et al. (2001) Assessment of alcohol toler- Saha TD, Chou SP, Grant BF. (2006) Toward an alcohol use disorder con-
ance in adolescents. J Stud Alcohol 62:687–95. tinuum using item response theory: results from the National
De Bellis MD, Clark DB, Beers SR, et al. (2000) Hippocampal volume in Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psychol Med
adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. Am J Psychiatry 157:737–44. 36:931–41.
De Bellis MD, Narasimhan A, Thatcher DL, et al. (2005) Prefrontal cortex, Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, et al. (1993) Development of the
thalamus, and cerebellar volumes in adolescents and young adults with Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative
adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders and comorbid mental disorders. Project on Early Detection of Persons with harmful alcohol consumption-
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 29:1590–600. II. Addiction 88:791–804.
Guerri C, Pascual M. (2010) Mechanisms involved in the neurotoxic, cogni- Schuckit MA, Smith TL, Hesselbrock V, et al. (2008) Clinical implications of
tive, and neurobehavioral effects of alcohol consumption during adoles- tolerance to alcohol in nondependent young drinkers. Am J Drug Alcohol
cence. Alcohol 44:15–26. Abuse 34:133–49.
Hibell B, Guttormsson U, Ahlström S, et al. (2007) Substance Use among Squeglia LM, Jacobus J, Tapert SF. (2014) The effect of alcohol use on
Students in 35 European Countries (The 2007 European School Survery human adolescent brain structures and systems. Handbook Clin Neurol
Project on Alcohol and other Drugs, ESPAD, Report). Stockholm: The 125:501–10.
European Monitoring Center for Drugs and Drug Addiction. Squeglia LM, Schweinsburg AD, Pulido C, et al. (2011) Adolescent binge
IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows [computer program]. (2013) Version 22.0. drinking linked to abnormal spatial working memory brain activation:
Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. differential gender effects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 35:1831–41.
Johnson PB, Richter L, Kleber HD, et al. (2005) Telescoping of drinking- Squeglia LM, Sorg SF, Schweinsburg AD, et al. (2012) Binge drinking differ-
related behaviors: gender, racial/ethnic, and age comparisons. Subst Use entially affects adolescent male and female brain morphometry.
Misuse 40:1139–51. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 220:529–39.
Lejbak L, Crossley M, Vrbancic M. (2011) A male advantage for spatial and Sugarman DE, Demartini KS, Carey KB. (2009) Are women at greater risk?
object but not verbal working memory using the n-back task. Brain Cogn An examination of alcohol-related consequences and gender. Am J Addict
76:191–6. 18:194–7.
Liu IC, Chiu CH, Yang TT. (2010) The effects of gender and a co-occurring Van Skike CE, Novier A, Diaz-Granados JL, et al. (2012) The effect of
depressive disorder on neurocognitive functioning in patients with alcohol chronic intermittent ethanol exposure on spatial memory in adolescent
dependence. Alcohol Alcohol 45:231–6. rats: the dissociation of metabolic and cognitive tolerances. Brain Res
López-Caneda E, Cadaveira F, Crego A, et al. (2013) Effects of a persistent 1453:34–9.
binge drinking pattern of alcohol consumption in young people: a follow- Wechsler D. (2004) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. San Antonio,
up study using event-related potentials. Alcohol Alcohol 48:464–71. EEUU: The Psychological Corporation.
López-Caneda E, Rodríguez Holguín S, Corral M, et al. (2014) Evolution of Wells JE, Horwood LJ, Fergusson DM. (2006) Stability and instability in
the binge drinking pattern in college students: neurophysiological corre- alcohol diagnosis from ages 18 to 21 and ages 21 to 25 years. Drug
lates. Alcohol 48:407–18. Alcohol Depend 81:157–65.
Copyright of Alcohol & Alcoholism. Supplement is the property of Oxford University Press /
USA and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like