Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Sleep Medicine
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/sleep

Review Article

Recent worldwide sleep patterns and problems during adolescence: A review


and meta-analysis of age, region, and sleep
Michael Gradisar ⇑, Greg Gardner, Hayley Dohnt
School of Psychology, Flinders University, G.P.O. Box 2100, Adelaide, 5001 South Australia, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Adolescent sleep health is becoming increasingly recognized internationally as a significant concern, with
Received 22 July 2010 many countries reporting high incidences of sleep disturbance in our youth. Notwithstanding the value of
Received in revised form 29 October 2010 findings obtained from each large-scale survey of adolescent sleep performed within individual countries,
Accepted 7 November 2010
the field lacks synthesis and analysis of adolescent sleep studies into a single review. This review presents
Available online 22 January 2011
findings from a meta-analysis of 41 surveys of worldwide adolescent sleep patterns and problems pub-
lished in the last decade (1999–2010). Sleep patterns tended to delay with increasing age, restricting
Keywords:
school-night sleep. Notably, Asian adolescents’ bedtimes were later than peers from North America
Adolescent sleep patterns
Daytime sleepiness
and Europe, resulting in less total sleep time on school nights and a tendency for higher rates of daytime
Insomnia sleepiness. Weekend sleep data were generally consistent worldwide, with bedtimes 2+ hours later and
Bedtimes more total sleep time obtained. We note a worldwide delayed sleep–wake behavior pattern exists con-
Sleep measurement sistent with symptoms of Delayed Sleep Phase Disorder, which may be exacerbated by cultural factors.
Delayed Sleep Phase Disorder Recommendations for future surveys of adolescent sleep patterns are discussed and provided in light
of current methodological limitations and gaps in the literature.
Ó 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 2. Literature search and inclusion criteria

Sleep problems during adolescence are common. Recently, Surveys of adolescent sleep problems were searched using the
Crowley et al. [1] reviewed literature over the past 30 years on search term ‘‘adolescent sleep’’ in the following electronic dat-
the sleep parameters of adolescents in the USA in light of how abases and on-line journal home pages: OVID Psyc Articles, Pub-
parameters change through this period. However, no review has Med, Behavioral Sleep Medicine, Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine,
covered adolescent sleep surveys worldwide. Consequently the Journal of Pediatrics, Journal of Sleep Research, Pediatrics, Sleep, and
aim of this paper is to review and contrast the recent literature Sleep Medicine. Additionally, reference lists of reviews of adolescent
on surveys of adolescent sleep patterns (bedtimes, sleep duration) sleep problems were used to find further surveys. Inclusion criteria
and problems across the globe, thus possibly providing insights included: the studies contain information on adolescent sleep
into cultural differences and similarities between countries. This parameters, sampled more than 300 participants aged 11–
review concludes by demonstrating that many studies have as- 18 years, and published from 1999 to 2010. This last criterion is
sessed symptoms of Delayed Sleep Phase Disorder (DSPD) [2,3] justified given that the changes in technology (e.g., mobile phones,
and provides recommendations for future surveys so that preva- internet) in the last decade may contribute to differences in sleep
lence estimates of this sleep disorder during adolescence may be parameters between the 1990s and 2000s. As this review presents
determined worldwide. sleep parameters as a function of age (see figures to follow), studies
providing mean sleep estimates for samples extending beyond the
Abbreviations: DSPD, Delayed Sleep Phase Disorder; SOT, sleep onset time; WUT,
age limits (e.g., 8–15 years) were excluded. Using these criteria, 41
wake-up time; TST, total sleep time; BT, bedtime; SSHS, School Sleep Habits Survey; surveys were found (see Table 1). Each survey was analyzed in the
DS, daytime sleepiness; ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale; PDSS, Pediatric Daytime present review in terms of typical sleep parameters measured on
Sleepiness Scale; SOI, sleep-onset insomnia; DIS, difficulty initiating sleep; DSM, school and weekend nights. Although adolescents may experience
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; ICSD, International Classi-
a range of sleep problems (e.g., Restless Legs Syndrome; Obstruc-
fication of Sleep Disorders.
⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.: +61 8 8201 2324; fax: +61 8 8201 3877. tive Sleep Apnea), it was clear during the review process that
E-mail addresses: michael.gradisar@flinders.edu.au, grad0011@flinders.edu.au two major types of sleep problems were commonly reported in
(M. Gradisar). studies: insomnia (predominantly difficulty initiating sleep) and

1389-9457/$ - see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.008
Table 1
Surveys of adolescent sleep.

Demographics School Weekend Unspecified Measures DS Insomn. Impaired funct.


Studies Country N Age BT TST WUT BT TST WUT BT TST WUT SD Act. SSHS
Acebo and Wolfson (2002) [44] USA 3119 13–19 x x x x
Danner and Phillips (2008) [30] USA 10,656 14–17 x x
Johnson et al. (2006) [43] USA 1014 13–16 x x
Knutson and Lauderdale (2009) [45] USA 2978 15–17 x x x x x
NSF (2006) [8] USA 1602 11–17 x x x x x x x x x x
Patten et al. (2000) [46] USA 7960 12–18 x x
Roberts et al. (2004) [47] USA 5118 13–18 x x x
Roberts et al. (2006) [48] USA 4175 11–17 x x
Spilsbury et al. (2007) [18] USA 411 11–17 x x x x
Wolfson et al. (2003) [10] USA 302 13–19 x x x x x x x x x x

M. Gradisar et al. / Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118


Gibson et al. (2006) [29] Can. 3235 14–18 x xa x
Laberge et al. (2001) [49] Can. 1146 10–13 x x x x x x x x x
Iglowstein et al. (2003) [50] Switz. 493 0–16 x
Loessl et al. (2006) [51] Germ. 818 12–19 x x x x
Meijer et al. (2000) [42] Netherl. 449 9–14 x x x x x
Ohayon et al. (2000) [20] Europe 1125 15–18 x x x x x x
Ohayon and Roberts (2001) [34] Europe 2242 15–18 x x
Palessen et al. (2008) [52] Norway 26,288 11–15 x
Giannotti et al. (2002) [41] Italy 6631 14–18 x x x x x x x x x x
Russo et al. (2007) [12] Italy 1073 8–14 x x x x x x x x x
Thorleifsdottir et al. (2002) [6] Iceland 688 1–29 x x x x x x x x x x x
Spruyt et al. (2005) [53] Belgium 3045 6–13 x x x x x x x x
Van den Bulck (2004) [54] Belgium 2546 12–17 x x x x x x x
Voit-Blanc et al. (2006) [35] France 502 15–19 x xa x
Gau and Soong (2003) [21] Taiwan 1572 9–16 x
Gau (2006) [33] Taiwan 2463 6–16 x x x x x x x x
Yen et al. (2010) [55] Taiwan 8319 12–19 x x
Liu et al. (2000) [39] China 1365 12–18 x x
Liu et al. (2008) [19] China 1056 13–17 x x x x x x x x x
Chung and Cheung (2008) [13] Hong Kong 1629 12–19 x x x x x x xa x x
Mak et al. (2010) [22] Hong Kong 29,397 12–18 x x
Ouyang et al. (2009) [56] China 621 11–17 x x x x x
Gaina et al. (2006) [36] Japan 9718 12–13 x
Kaneita et al. (2006) [38] Japan 102,451 13–18 x x
Kaneita et al. (2009) [57] Japan 516 13 x x x
Ohida et al. (2004) [32] Japan 106,297 12–18 x x x
Tagaya et al. (2004) [58] Japan 3478 15–18 x x x
Chol Shin et al. (2002) [28] Korea 3871 16–17 x x x xa
Yang et al. (2005) [9] Korea 1457 12–18 x x x x x x x x x
Abdel-Kahlek (2004) [24] Kuwait 5044 14–19 x x
Warner et al. (2007) [17] Aust. 308 15–18 x x x x x x x x x

BT, bedtime; TST, total sleep time; WUT, wake-up time; SD, sleep diary; Act., actigraphy; SSHS, School Sleep Habits Survey; DS, daytime sleepiness.
a
Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) used to measure DS; NSF, National Sleep Foundation (USA).

111
112 M. Gradisar et al. / Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118

daytime sleepiness. A third and important theme emerged: the im- 3.1. School-night and weekend SOT (Bed time)
pact of sleep problems on adolescents’ general functioning. As the
assessment of sleep parameters and these three sleep problems Of the 41 surveys analyzed, none provided information on
map closely onto a sleep disorder common during adolescence school-night or weekend SOT (see Table 1). But 13 surveys pro-
(i.e., DSPD), this review thus highlights future researchers’ oppor- vided information on school-night and weekend bed time (BT).
tunities to assess the prevalence of this disorder. Meta-analysis in- By definition, SOT is always later than BT, and SOT has been re-
volved correlations between age (in years) with sleep parameters ported to occur on average 16.8 min later than BT for adolescents
(in time units); where comparisons were to be made between re- aged 15–18 [6]. This average will be used to provide an estimate
gions, analysis of covariance (controlling for age) with Bonferroni of SOT for the adolescent surveys reviewed.
corrections applied. Means for regional sleep parameters are ad- Fig. 1 shows that adolescent school-night BTs ranged from
justed for age. Effect sizes are also reported. 8:46 pm to 12:54 am and were clearly related to age,
r(49) = 0.63, p < .0001. A longitudinal study found that adolescents
optimally need on average 9 h sleep per night regardless of their
3. Typical sleep parameters and their measurement pubertal stage [7]. Other reports state less than 8 h sleep as being
insufficient [8]. As school start times range from 7:30 to 8:30 am in
Unlike the sleep patterns of children and adults, adolescent most countries, adolescents may typically wake for school between
sleep is typically more variable across our 7-day week, with 6:30 and 7:30 am (see Fig. 5). This means that an adolescent who
school-night sleep shorter than sleep on weekends. What appears intends to wake up at 7:30 am and desires the optimal 9 h of sleep
to be associated with this variability in sleep duration is a develop- [7,8] must go to bed before 10:30 pm (i.e., in the ‘‘optimal’’ range
mental delay of not only bedtimes, but specifically sleep times. displayed in Figs. 1 and 2). At worst, to avoid getting insufficient
That is, sleep onset time (SOT) and wake-up time (WUT) become sleep (i.e., <8 h), the adolescent needs to sleep before 11:30 pm
increasingly later during adolescence. When this delayed SOT cou- (i.e., within the ‘‘borderline’’ range, and below the ‘‘problematic’’
ples with the need to attend school the following morning (early cut-off). Fig. 1 illustrates half of samples’ BTs (i.e., 50%) fell within
WUT), this invariantly results in a short school-night total sleep the optimal range required for adolescents to obtain 9 h sleep.
time (TST; e.g., 5–8 h). Consecutive nights of short school-night These samples derive from North American, mainland European,
TST lead to the creation of ‘‘sleep debt’’ [4], with adolescents recov- Chinese, and Australian studies, and tend to be younger adoles-
ering this debt on weekends with longer TSTs (e.g., 9–12 h). cents (i.e., most <15 years). Nearly one-third of samples lie in the
Sleep–wake parameters can be measured in several ways. Ide- borderline range and tend to be slightly older (i.e., 15–16 years).
ally, a 7-day sleep diary or wrist actigraphy monitoring are used Icelandic adolescents of all ages tended to go to bed later than
[2]. However, the impracticality and expense of surveying many other samples, ranging from borderline BTs (younger adolescents)
adolescents with actigraphy means that sleep diaries are the mea- to insufficient BTs (older adolescents) [6]. Korean adolescents’
sure of choice. Self-report questionnaires can also be used. The mean BTs were mainly in the insufficient range [9], thus prevent-
School Sleep Habits Survey (SSHS) [5] was found to be the most ing the majority of them obtaining sufficient school-night TST.
widely adopted sleep survey in this review. Wolfson et al. [5] found Worth noting is that Figs. 1 and 2 represent mean bed times.
no significant difference between school-night TST and WUT when Wolfson and colleagues’ [10] data show school-night BT for adoles-
comparing the SSHS to 8-day sleep diary and actigraphy monitor- cents aged 13–19 years fits a normal distribution. In a normal dis-
ing. School-night bedtime (BT) was slightly earlier when using the tribution approximately 34% of adolescents’ BTs will fall within
SSHS, and weekend TST and WUT were longer and later when 1 SD above the mean, 13.5% fall between 1 and 2 SDs above the
using the SSHS. Consequently, survey studies using the SSHS or mean, and 2% fall between 2 and 3 SDs above the mean [11]. Table 2
similar self-report questionnaires are likely to provide accurate shows the distribution of mean school-night BT across studies. The
school-night TST, WUT, and weekend BT, but caution is needed mean BT for all these studies is later than 10:30 pm for all age
for school-night BT, weekend TST and WUTs. groups. BTs are clearly worse for adolescents outside 1 SD. For

Fig. 1. Average school night bed time. Note: North American samples = black diamond; European samples = white diamond; Asian samples = grey circle; Australasian
sample = white circle; Ouyang et al. [56] reported school night bed times for males and females separately, and therefore this study is not included in the above graph.
M. Gradisar et al. / Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118 113

Fig. 2. Average weekend bed time. Note: North American samples = black diamond; European samples = white diamond; Asian samples = grey circle; Australasian
sample = white circle.

Table 2
Bedtime distribution for adolescent samples where school-night BT limits opportunity for sufficient school-night TST.

Studies Age Mean (SD) 0–1 SD above mean (i.e., 34% of sample) 1–2 SD above mean (i.e., 13.5% of sample)
Russo et al. (2007) [12] 12 10:31 (41) 10:31–11:12 11:12–11:53
Russo et al. (2007) [12] 13 10:33 (39) 10:39–11:12 11:12–11:51
Thorleifsdottir et al. (2002) [6] 13 11:10 – –
Chung and Cheung (2008) [13] 13 11:00 (56) 11:00–11:56 11:56–12:52a
Kaneita et al. (2009) [57] 13 11:15 – –
Yang et al. (2005) [9] 13 11:12 (66) 11:12–12:18 12:18–01:24a
Russo et al. (2007) [12] 14 10:43 (38) 10:43–11:21 11:21–11:59
Chung and Cheung (2008) [13] 14 11:23 (63) 11:23–12:26 12:26–01:29a
NSF (2006) [8] 15 10:32 – –
Thorleifsdottir et al. (2002) [6] 15 11:55 – –
Chung and Cheung (2008) [13] 15 11:26 (67) 11:26–12:33 12:33–01:40a
Yang et al. (2005) [9] 15 12:00 (66) 12:00–01:06a 01:06–02:12a
Wolfson et al. (2003) [10] 15 10:59 (53) 10:59–11:52 11:52–12:45a
NSF (2006) [8] 16 10:51 – –
Ohayon et al. (2000) [20] 16 10:36 (62) 10:36–11:38 11:38–12:40a
Van den Bulck (2004) [54] 16 10:54 (42) 10:54–11:36 11:36–12:08a
Chung and Cheung (2008) [13] 16 11:35 (66) 11:35–12:41a 12:41–01:47a
NSF (2006) [8] 17 11:02 – –
Warner et al. (2008) [17] 17 10:47 (51) 10:47–11:38 11:38–12:29a
Thorleifsdottir et al. (2002) [6] 17 12:05 – –
Yang et al. (2005) [9] 17 12:54 (84) 12:54–02:18a 02:18–03:42a
Chung and Cheung (2008) [13] 18 11:47 (76) 11:47–01:03a 01:03–02:19a
Thorleifsdottir et al. (2002) [6] 18 12:15 – –
a
Notes: Bed times within the insufficient range; SOT is likely to be 17 min later for all averages displayed.

these adolescents, bedtimes range from (at best) 15 min near the at 8:30 pm [19] which is likely later than the school-end time in
11:30 pm insufficient BT cut-off for younger Italian adolescents North America. The situation for BTs becomes worse when consid-
[12], through to older adolescents from Korea going to bed over ering school-night BT measured using surveys is earlier than if
4 h later than the 11:30 pm cut-off [9]. Indeed, there is a tendency measured using actigraphy monitoring [10], and SOT is approxi-
for a large proportion of adolescents from Asian samples to lie mately 17 min later than BT [6]. Consequently, a higher proportion
within the insufficient range [9,13]. Overall, although the mean of adolescents are likely to fall in the ‘‘insufficient’’ range when this
school-night BT for Asian samples (11:23 pm) was not significantly underestimation is taken into consideration.
greater than that for European samples (10:46 pm, p = 0.12), a very Interestingly, the Icelandic study [6] demonstrated consistently
large effect nonetheless exists (i.e., d = 1.97). Statistically, the mean later BTs (and WUTs; see Fig. 5) when compared to other Western
Asian BT was later than that for North American samples samples. One explanation for such a delayed BT may be a lack of
(10:06 pm, p = 0.02), representing a very large effect (i.e., morning sunlight to reset their circadian rhythms [23]. However,
d = 3.90). This difference in BT may suggest the influence of cul- data were collected in the springtime, when there is a substantial
tural factors, as there is little difference in the latitude where sur- amount of sunlight (e.g., approx. 18 h). Alternatively, societal
veys were conducted between North America and Asia, suggesting schedules could be delayed with so much daylight. But school
environmental factors (e.g., daylight duration) play a lesser role. reportedly starts at 8 am [15]; thus these adolescents experience
For instance, one Asian study reported adolescents finished school ample morning light exposure. Thorleifsdottir et al. [6] argue that
114 M. Gradisar et al. / Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118

the discrepancy between the sleep habits in Iceland and other 3.2. School-night and weekend TST
Western countries is a ‘‘well-known phenomenon’’ (p. 536), having
been found in other Icelandic studies [16]. They speculate that the Of the 41 surveys analyzed, 14 reported school-night and/or
delay in BTs and WUTs may be an example of a weak dominance of weekend TST (see Table 1). A negative relationship exists between
the external clock in the regulation of the biological clock, given age and school-night TST, r(33) = 0.66, p < .0001, but not for
that Icelandic local time has adopted Greenwich Mean Time weekend TST, r(34) = 0.28, p = .10. Fig. 3 shows that in 53% of
(GMT) all year round, rather than the 1½ h geographical distance samples, average school-night TST was insufficient (i.e., <8 h)
between Iceland and the UK, who use GMT solely in winter. [8,10,13,17–19]. Despite the fact that some studies reviewed here
Fig. 2 shows results from the 13 surveys that reported average are not the same as those reviewed for BTs, there is nonetheless
weekend BT. These surveys show that weekend BT is related to a mismatch between the percentage reporting insufficient
age, r(40) = 0.68, p < .0001, is consistently later than average school-night sleep, yet not an insufficient school-night BT (e.g.,
school-night BT (mean diff. = 122.3 min, t(39) = 11.17, p < .0001), [10,17,19,20]). This anomaly may be primarily explained due to
and yet is not affected by the region the adolescent resides, ‘‘sleep opportunity’’ (i.e., time in bed) being compared to ‘‘sleep
F(2,12) = 2.62, p = 0.10. An adolescent is more likely to go to bed duration’’ (i.e., TST). That is, even though BTs and WUTs are re-
at their preferred BT on the weekend, possibly making this more ported, this does not include the time spent awake in bed attempt-
illustrative of their circadian sleep phase preference or a behavioral ing sleep (sleep onset latency), during the night (wake after sleep
choice due to the removal of a fixed wake-up time. The higher pro- onset), or dozing on and off at the end of the sleep period. Unlike
portion (i.e., from 18% to 59%) of BTs in the insufficient range on the differences in BTs, however, the mean school-night TST for
weekends suggests that an even higher percentage of adolescents Asian samples (7.64 h) was significantly less than that of European
might have a significantly delayed sleep circadian rhythm than samples (8.44 h, p = .04), but not North American samples (7.46 h,
predicted by the school-night statistics. p = .64). Fig. 4 illustrates that weekend TST is on average 91.6 min

Fig. 3. Average total sleep time on school nights. Note: North American samples = black diamond; European samples = white diamond; Asian samples = grey circle;
Australasian sample = white circle.

Fig. 4. Average total sleep time on weekends. Note: North American samples = black diamond; European samples = white diamond; Asian samples = grey circle; Australasian
sample = white circle.
M. Gradisar et al. / Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118 115

longer than school-night TST, t(12) = 9.80, p < .0001, indicating that Fig. 2), longer weekend TSTs (see Fig. 4) and reduced weekend
short school-night TSTs result in the creation of a sleep debt re- morning commitments.
lieved by longer weekend TSTs. No studies reported insufficient
weekend TST, with many (71%) reporting optimal TST regardless 4. Clinical markers of adolescent sleep problems
of age or country. The few studies reporting sufficient weekend
TST were from the USA, Korea and Australia. Somewhat similar 4.1. Daytime sleepiness
to the results of school-night TST, North American adolescents ob-
tained less weekend TST than European adolescents (8.80 vs. Adolescent daytime sleepiness (DS) has been measured in a
10.03 h, p < .0001), but not Asian adolescents (9.23 h, p = 0.11; variety of ways including the number of daytime or classroom naps
see Fig. 5). [8,21,22], the desire to have more sleep [23], morning sleepiness
[24–26], and oversleeping [8]. The Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS)
3.3. School morning and weekend WUT [27], designed to measure adult DS, has measured DS in adoles-
cents [28–30], and scores of 10 or greater indicate DS in adults
Thirteen of the 41 surveys reported school-night and/or week- and adolescents [27,28,30]. Nevertheless, some argue the ESS is
end WUTs (see Table 1). Fig. 6 shows that school morning WUT considered an insensitive measure of DS in adolescents because
is relatively consistent across samples (range = 5:50–8:10 am, situations used to assess DS in adults are often not applicable to
F(2,10) = 2.48, p = 0.13) and across age, r(34) = 0.12, p = 0.50; and adolescents [29]. One validated measure tailored to adolescents
likewise for weekend WUT (region: F(2,10) = 0.29, p = 0.76; age: is the Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scale (PDSS) which was based
r(34) = 0.19, p = 0.28). However, two notable exceptions are the on the ESS but modified to suit school-aged children and adoles-
Icelandic sample [6] who consistently wake later than all other cents [31].
samples, and at the other extreme a Chinese sample who on school The majority of surveys examined (i.e., 61%) reported at least
mornings participate in exercise at 6:15 am, prior to starting one behavioral parameter relating to DS (see Table 1). But most
school [56]. Weekend WUT is significantly later than average only measured one aspect of DS using a variety of unstandardised
school morning WUT (mean diff. = 2 h 31 min, p < .0001; see self-report or parent-report questions. For example, in surveys of
Fig. 7). This is most likely a result of later weekend BTs (see Asian adolescents, 52.7% reported feeling ‘‘very’’ or ‘‘rather’’ sleepy

Fig. 5. School-night vs. weekend total sleep times as a function of region.

Fig. 6. Average wake-up time on school mornings. Note: North American samples = black diamond; European samples = white diamond; Asian samples = grey circle;
Australasian sample = white circle.
116 M. Gradisar et al. / Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118

Fig. 7. Average wake-up time on weekends. Note: North American samples = black diamond; European samples = white diamond; Asian samples = grey circle; Australasian
sample = white circle.

in the morning [13], while another found 33.3% of boys and 39.2% with further options for assessing DS in adolescents using psycho-
of girls reported ‘‘always’’ or ‘‘often’’ feeling excessively sleepy dur- metrically sound instruments, and from this review it seems likely
ing the daytime [32]. Some other findings were more moderate. For that DS is a reasonably significant and prevalent problem in ado-
example, using parental reports, Gau [33] found that 10.9% of lescents from many countries across the world.
Taiwanese adolescents napped inadvertently, and Ohayon and
Roberts [34] found that 5.9% of 15- to 18-year-old European
adolescents reported a tendency to fall asleep easily and anywhere 4.2. Insomnia
in the daytime. These assessments of only one aspect of DS may
either produce wildly differing rates due to measurement error, Insomnia, along with DS, is another clinical sleep parameter
or the variability may reflect a natural response to variable TSTs that is frequently investigated, with more than half of studies re-
across cultures. viewed (i.e., 54%) assessing at least one aspect (see Table 1). Insom-
Several surveys measured a combination of DS parameters. A nia may include difficulty maintaining sleep, early morning
large national survey of 1602 USA adolescents found that 20% of awakening, and unrefreshing sleep [2,3]. Most studies reviewed
adolescents reported at least one problem with daytime sleepiness in this paper, though, chose to assess difficulties with sleep initia-
every day, or almost every day (e.g., fell asleep in school, while tion or sleep-onset insomnia (SOI). The presence of SOI is most
doing homework, too sleepy in general, too sleepy for sports, over- commonly established through the length of sleep onset latency
slept) [8]. The ESS was used in five of the surveys, and scores of (SOL; time taken to fall asleep) [2,37]. SOL is most accurately mea-
greater than 10 varied from 15.9% [28] and 30% [35], to 37% [30], sured using a 7-day sleep diary [37], but can also be measured to
40.9% [36] and 41.9% [13] in adolescents. These results support some degree of accuracy using the SSHS [5,10]. The threshold for
the high rates (i.e., 20–40%) of behavioral parameters of DS found delineating short SOL from long SOL has been considered by some
in surveys that measured one parameter of DS. The use of stan- authors to be 20 min [36] and others to be 30 min [8,12]. Another
dardized measures was minimal, with only one study comparing method of evaluating SOI is through a measure of difficulty initiat-
standardized DS measures against each other (i.e., the PDSS, the ing sleep (DIS). This commonly involves a single item that requires
sleepiness scale in the SSHS, and the authors’ Cleveland Adolescent adolescents to answer the question ‘‘Do you have difficulty falling
Sleepiness Questionnaire) [18]. Unfortunately, no data were pre- asleep at night?’’ on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from ‘‘always’’
sented on the prevalence of excessive sleepiness in their sample to ‘‘never.’’ An answer of ‘‘often’’ or ‘‘always’’ is considered by most
of 411 adolescents. Nonetheless, their study provides researchers surveys as evidence of DIS [38,39].

Table 3
Difficulty initiating sleep (DIS) and sleep onset latency (SOL) in surveys of adolescent sleep problems.

Studies DIS (%) Studies SOL >30 min (%)


Abdel-Kahlek (2004) [24] 14.6 (boys) 20.3 (girls) Chung and Cheung (2008) [13] 20.1
Johnson et al. (2006) [43] 11.6 NSF (2006) [8] 26.0
Kaneita et al. (2006) [38] 14.8 Russo et al. (2007) [12] 20.0
Laberge et al. (2001) [49] 26.5 (boys) 36.2 (girls)
Liu et al. (2000) [39] 10.8
Mak et al. (2010) [22] 19.0
NSF (2006) [8] 11.0
Ohayon et al. (2000) [20] 12.4
Ohayon & Roberts (2001) [34] 14.1
Ohida et al. (2004) [32] 15.3 (boys) 16.0 (girls)
Pallesen et al. (2008) [52] 16.8
Roberts et al. (2004) [47] 16.7
Roberts et al. (2006) [48] 7.1
M. Gradisar et al. / Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118 117

Of the 41 surveys, 21 reported information about SOI (see Ta- monitoring are able to ask questions differentiating DSPD from
ble 1). Table 3 shows that 7–36% of adolescents report DIS, and other sleep disorders common during adolescence (e.g., Inadequate
20–26% of adolescents report SOL greater than 30 min. What is Sleep Hygiene, Behaviorally Induced Insufficient Sleep Syndrome,
interesting is the most reported percentage for DIS is in the ‘‘teens’’ Primary Insomnia) [2,3,43] then the remaining criteria will be
(mean = 16%) despite a consistently low 20s percentage (mean addressed, and the research and clinic fields will have a better
22%) for an SOL >30 min. The slight discrepancy between these idea of the present prevalence of this apparently common sleep
two sets of statistics might illustrate that while a sleep latency disorder in adolescents.
greater than 30 min is problematic by adult standards [40], this
cut-off may be higher by adolescent standards; but this is yet to 6. Conclusions and recommendations
be investigated.
A systematic review and analysis of adolescent sleep patterns
4.3. Impact of sleep problems on general functioning and problems across the world is currently needed, and this review
of 41 studies published in the past decade demonstrates a number
Of the 41 surveys analyzed, 16 surveys reported at least one of insights into age- and culturally-related influences on adoles-
parameter indicative of distress or impairment in functioning (see cent sleep. First, there is a moderate-to-strong age influence on
Table 1). This is a relatively understudied yet significant component adolescent school-night bedtimes and total sleep time worldwide,
of these surveys, as it highlights the importance of healthy sleep with older adolescents going to bed later and obtaining less sleep.
needed by adolescents and its possible impact on aspects of their Second, this effect is amplified for Asian adolescents who go to bed
lives. For example, a survey of 5044 adolescents from Kuwait found later (than North American adolescents), obtain less sleep (than
that 9.2% of girls and 7.7% of boys reported their sleep affects social European samples), and tended to report higher rates of daytime
relations, while a higher percentage said their sleep affects their sleepiness than adolescents from other regions. Third, cultural
work performance (14.9% of girls, 14.2% of boys) [24]. The NSF Sleep influences are virtually ameliorated on the weekends with adoles-
in America Poll found that of those adolescents who had driven in the cents across the world demonstrating delayed bedtimes and wake-
past year, 27% reported having had an accident or near accident due up times of greater than 2 h. Combining this common delayed
to drowsiness, and 5% had fallen asleep while driving [8]. sleep–wake behavior with the often reported sleep-onset insomnia
Several studies reported correlations between lack of sleep or and impacts on adolescents’ general functioning, the findings from
sleepiness and impaired functioning. Giannotti et al. [41] found this review suggest the prevalence and impact of Delayed Sleep
that adolescents with later BT and WUTs had more attention prob- Phase Disorder during adolescence may be currently under-rated.
lems, poor school achievement, more injuries, and more emotional Very few studies measured or reported the broad spectrum of
problems. Another survey found that adolescents with an ESS score sleep–wake behaviors and problems described in this review. Thus,
>10 had more school absenteeism [35]. Meijer et al. [42] found that we recommend that studies planning to survey adolescent sleep
sleep quality had a direct positive relationship with four aspects of patterns attempt to assess multiple sleep parameters simulta-
school functioning: receptivity with regard to the teacher’s influ- neously. For large-scale surveys, the SSHS could be used, and some
ence, self-image as a pupil, achievement motivation and control translations may already exist (e.g., Italian [41], German [51],
over their own aggression. These 16 studies illustrate that im- Mandarin [19]). Ideally, sleep diaries could be used in conjunction
paired functioning due to sleep problems should be incorporated with surveys. During our review, we found it was unfortunately
into surveys of adolescent sleep. common for large-scale studies to assess only bedtimes and thus
calculate ‘‘time in bed.’’ With the growing wealth of knowledge
5. Delayed Sleep Phase Disorder about the importance of ‘‘sleep,’’ it is clear from the present review
that future studies should ask additional questions about total
From the review of these 41 studies, it is clear that adolescent sleep time, difficulties initiating sleep, daytime sleepiness, and
sleep is typified by late BTs and WUTs, resulting in restricted and the impact of sleep on general functioning.
insufficient TST on school nights, with weekend TST being extended Despite the large number of studies that are now investigating
and ‘‘normal.’’ Consequences of insufficient sleep include daytime adolescent sleep with large samples, we know very little about re-
sleepiness, plus a host of broader problems in various areas of func- cent sleep patterns of adolescents in many regions of the world.
tioning. Insomnia, and particularly SOI, appears common. It is worth For instance, two continents were not featured in this review.
noting here that these aforementioned sleep problems highly Despite the excellent work being performed in South America, not
resemble symptoms of Delayed Sleep Phase Disorder (DSPD) [2]. one study was large enough and reported sleep characteristics con-
DSPD is a circadian rhythm disorder where the individual’s sleep sistent with this review’s inclusion criteria. Further, to our knowl-
pattern is timed significantly later so that it conflicts with their edge, the field knows too little about the sleep of teens in Africa.
weekly obligations (e.g., school, weekend morning extra-curricular Although Australasia was featured in this review, only one large-
activities). The prevalence of DSPD is said to be 7–16% and more scale study was found that reported sleep, thus preventing this
common in young adults and adolescents [2,3]. We would posit that region from being included in statistical comparisons with other
due to cultural influences the prevalence of adolescent DSPD may regions. Perhaps in the next decade, with more surveys of adolescent
be higher in specific regions (i.e., Asia, Iceland). sleep patterns and problems conducted worldwide, the field will
Many studies reviewed in this paper represented an opportu- have more data to better investigate ‘‘similarities and differences
nity to observe the percentage of adolescents who possessed a col- in sleep and sleep-related practices within and across countries
lection of DSPD symptoms to warrant the diagnosis, at least when (that) may facilitate an understanding of factors that underlie
using diagnostic criteria from DSM-IV [3]. That is, survey questions sleep–wake regulation during adolescence’’ (LeBourgeois et al., p.
could target a chronic pattern of delayed sleep onset and wake-up 264) [59].
times with associated daytime sleepiness or insomnia (Criterion A)
as well as an associated impact on functioning (Criterion B). But Conflict of Interest
DSPD diagnostic criteria from the International Classification of
Sleep Disorders second edition (ICSD-II) require sleep monitoring The ICMJE Uniform Disclosure Form for Potential Conflicts of
via sleep diaries or actigraphy (Criterion C) [2], which few studies Interest associated with this article can be viewed by clicking on
have used (e.g., [10]). Finally, if surveys coupled with sleep the following link: doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.008.
118 M. Gradisar et al. / Sleep Medicine 12 (2011) 110–118

References [33] Gau SS. Prevalence of sleep problems and their association with inattention/
hyperactivity among children aged 6–15 in Taiwan. J Sleep Res 2006;15:
403–14.
[1] Crowley SJ, Acebo C, Carskadon MA. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and delayed
[34] Ohayon MM, Roberts RE. Comparability of sleep disorders diagnoses using
sleep phase in adolescence. Sleep Med 2007;8:602–12.
DSM-IV and ICSD classifications with adolescents. Sleep 2001;24:920–5.
[2] American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep
[35] Viot-Blanc V, Muzet A, Krieger J, Davenne D, Levy P, Tran F, et al. Sleep and
Disorders Diagnostic and Coding Manual. 2nd ed. Westchester, IL: American
sleepiness in French adolescent population. J Sleep Res 2006;15:A42.
Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2005.
[36] Gania A, Sekine M, Shimako H, Xiaoli C, Hitomi K, Takashi Y, et al.
[3] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Epidemiological aspects of self-reported sleep onset latency in Japanese
Disorders Text Revision. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
junior high school children. J Sleep Res 2006;15:266–75.
Association; 2001.
[37] Chesson A, Hartse K, Anderson WM, Davila D, Johnson S, Littner M, et al.
[4] Van Dongen HP, Rogers NL, Dinges DF. Sleep debt: theoretical and empirical
Practice parameters for the evaluation of chronic insomnia. Sleep 2000;23:
issues. Sleep Biol Rhythms 2003;1:5–13.
1–5.
[5] Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in
[38] Kaneita Y, Ohida T, Osaki Y, Tanihata T, Minowa M, Suzuki K, et al. Insomnia
adolescents. Child Dev 1998;69:875–87.
among Japanese adolescents: a nationwide representative survey. Sleep
[6] Thorleifsdottir B, Bjornsson JK, Benediktsdottir B, Gislason T, Kristbjarnarson H.
2006;29:1543–50.
Sleep and sleep habits from childhood to young adulthood over a 10-year
[39] Liu X, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Kurita H. Prevalence and correlates of self-
period. J Psychosom Res 2002;53:529–37.
reported sleep problems among Chinese adolescents. Sleep 2000;23:1–8.
[7] Carskadon MA, Orav EJ, Dement WC. Evolution of sleep and daytime sleepiness
[40] Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, Lichstein KL, Morin CM. Recommen-
in adolescents. In: Guilleminault C, Lugaresi E, editors. Sleep/wake disorders:
dations for a standard research assessment of insomnia. Sleep 2006;29:
natural history, epidemiology, and long-term evolution. New York: Raven
1155–73.
Press; 1983. p. 201–16.
[41] Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Sebastiani T, Ottaviano S. Circadian preference, sleep and
[8] National Sleep Foundation. 2006 Sleep in America poll. Washington, DC: WBA
daytime behaviour in adolescence. J Sleep Res 2002;11:191–9.
Market Research; 2006.
[42] Meijer AM, Habekothe HT, Van Den Wittenboer GLH. Time in bed, quality of
[9] Yang C, Kim JK, Patel SR, Lee J. Age related changes in sleep/wake patterns
sleep and school functioning of children. J Sleep Res 2009;9:145–53.
among Korean teenagers. Pediatrics 2005;115:250–6.
[43] Johnson EO, Roth T, Schultz L, Breslau N. Epidemiology of DSM-IV insomnia in
[10] Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA, Acebo C, Seifer R, Fallone G, Labyak SE, et al.
adolescence: lifetime prevalence, chronicity, and an emergent gender
Evidence for the validity of a sleep habits survey for adolescents. Sleep
difference. Pediatrics 2006;117:247–56.
2003;26:213–6.
[44] Acebo C, Wolfson MC. Influence of irregular sleep patterns on waking behaviour.
[11] Pelham BW, Blanton H. Conducting research in psychology: measuring the
In: Carskadon MA, editor. Adolescent sleep patterns. UK: Cambridge University
weight of smoke. Pacific Grove, CA: International Thompson Publishing; 2003.
Press; 2002. p. 221–35.
[12] Russo PM, Bruni O, Lucidi F, Ferri R, Violani C. Sleep habits and circadian
[45] Knutson KL, Lauderdale DS. Sociodemographic and behavioral predictors of
preference in Italian children and adolescents. J Sleep Res 2007;16:163–9.
bed time and wake time among US adolescents aged 15 to 19 years. J Pediatr
[13] Chung K, Cheung M. Sleep–wake patterns and sleep disturbance among Hong
2009;154:426–30.
Kong Chinese adolescents. Sleep 2008;31:185–94.
[46] Patten CA, Choi WS, Gillin JC, Pierce JP. Depressive symptoms and cigarette
[15] American Field Service. Focus on Iceland School Program, <http://
smoking predict development and persistence of sleep problems in US
www.afs.org/afs_or/focus_on/high_school/39>; 2008 [retrieved 28.04.09].
adolescents. Pediatrics 2000;106:19.
[16] Kristbjarnarson H, Magnusson H, Sverrisson GI, Arnarson EO, Helgason T. A
[47] Roberts RE, Lee ES, Hernandez M, Solari AC. Symptoms of insomnia among
survey on sleep habits in Iceland. Iceland Med J 1985;71:193–8.
adolescents in the Lower Rio Grande Valley of Texas. Pediatrics
[17] Warner S, Murray G, Meyer D. Holiday and school-term sleep patterns of
2004;27:751–60.
Australian adolescents. J Adolesc 2008;31:595–608.
[48] Roberts RE, Roberts CR, Chan W. Ethnic differences in symptoms of insomnia
[18] Spilsbury JC, Drotar D, Rosen CL, Redline S. The Cleveland Adolescent
among adolescents. Sleep 2006;29:359–65.
Sleepiness Questionnaire: a new measure to assess excessive daytime
[49] Laberge L, Petit D, Simard C, Vitaro F, Tremblay RE, Montplaisir J. Development
sleepiness in adolescents. J Clin Sleep Med 2007;3:603–12.
of sleep patterns in early adolescence. J Sleep Res 2001;10:59–67.
[19] Liu X, Zhao Z, Jia C, Buysse DJ. Sleep patterns and problems among Chinese
[50] Iglowstein I, Jenni OG, Molinari L, Largo RH. Sleep duration from infancy to
adolescents. Pediatrics 2008;121:1165–73.
adolescence: reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics
[20] Ohayon MM, Roberts RE, Zulley J, Smirne S, Priest RG. Prevalence and patterns
2003;111:302–7.
of problematic sleep among older adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
[51] Loessl B, Valerius G, Kopasz M, Hornyak M, Riemann D, Voderholzer U. Are
Psychiatry 2000;39:1549–56.
adolescents chronically sleep deprived? An investigation of sleep habits of
[21] Gau S, Soong W. The transition of sleep–wake patterns in early adolescence.
adolescents in the southwest of Germany. J Sleep Res 2006;15:A207.
Sleep 2003;26:449–54.
[52] Palessen S, Hetland J, Sivertsen B, Samdal O, Torsheim T, Nordhus H. Time
[22] Mak K-K, Ho S-Y, Thomas GN, Lo W-S, Cheuk DK-L, Lai Y-K, et al. Smoking and
trends in sleep onset insomnia among Norwegian children and adolescents:
sleep disorders in Chinese adolescents. Sleep Med 2010;11:268–73.
1983–2005. Scand J Pub Health 2008;36:889–95.
[23] Strauch I, Meier B. Sleep need in adolescents: a longitudinal approach. Sleep
[53] Spruyt K, O’Brien LM, Cluydts R, Verleye GB, Ferri R. Odds, prevalence and
1988;11:378–86.
predictors of sleep problems in school-age normal children. J Sleep Res
[24] Abdel-Khalek AM. Prevalence of reported insomnia and its consequences in a
2005;14:163–76.
survey of 5044 adolescents in Kuwait. Sleep 2004;27:726–31.
[54] Van den Bulck J. Television viewing, computer game playing, and internet use
[25] Henschel A, Lack L. Do many adolescents sleep poorly or just too late? Sleep
and self-reported time to bed and time out of bed in secondary-school
Res 1987;16:354.
children. Sleep 2004;27:101–4.
[26] Tynjala J, Kannas L, Levalahti E. Perceived tiredness among adolescents and its
[55] Yen C-H, King BH, Chang Y-P. Factor structure of the Athens Insomnia Scale
association with sleep habits and use of psychoactive substances. J Sleep Res
and its associations with demographic characteristics and depression in
1997;6:189–98.
adolescents. J Sleep Res 2010;19:12–8.
[27] Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth
[56] Ouyang F, Lu BS, Wang B, Yang J, Li Z, Wang L, et al. Sleep patterns among rural
Sleepiness Scale. Sleep 1991;14:540–5.
Chinese twin adolescents. Sleep Med 2009;10:479–89.
[28] Chol S, Jinkwan K, Sangduck L, Yongkyu A, Soonjae J. Sleep habits, excessive
[57] Kaneita Y, Yokoyama E, Harano S, Tamaki T, Suzuki H, Munezawa T, et al.
daytime sleepiness and school performance in high school students.
Associations between sleep disturbance and mental health status: a
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2003;57:451–3.
longitudinal study of Japanese junior high school students. Sleep Med
[29] Gibson ES, Powles ACP, Thabane L, O’Brien S, Molnar DS, Trajanovic N, et al.
2009;10:780–6.
‘‘Sleepiness’’ is serious in adolescence: two surveys of 3235 Canadian students.
[58] Tagaya H, Uchiyama M, Ohida T, Kamei Y, Shibui K, Ozaki A, et al. Sleep habits
BMC Pub Health 2006;6:116–25.
and factors associated with short sleep duration among Japanese high-school
[30] Danner F, Phillips B. Adolescent sleep, school start times, and teen motor
students: a community study. Sleep Biol Rhythms 2004;2:57–64.
vehicle crashes. J Clin Sleep Med 2008;4:533–5.
[59] LeBourgeois MK, Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Wolfson AR, Harsh J. The relationship
[31] Drake C, Nickel C, Burduvali E, Roth T, Jefferson C, Badia P. The pediatric
between reported sleep quality and sleep hygiene in Italian and American
daytime sleepiness scale (PDSS): sleep habits and school out-comes in middle-
adolescents. Pediatrics 2005;115:257–65.
school children. Sleep 2003;6:455–8.
[32] Ohida T, Osaki Y, Doi Y, Tanihata T, Minowa M, Suzuki K, et al. An
epidemiologic study of self-reported sleep problems among Japanese
adolescents. Sleep 2004;27:978–85.

You might also like