Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/7065967

The molecular genetics of cognition: dopamine, COMT and BDNF

Article  in  Genes Brain and Behavior · July 2006


DOI: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00163.x · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

249 254

3 authors, including:

Jonathan B Savitz Raj Ramesar


Laureate Institute for Brain Research University of Cape Town
136 PUBLICATIONS   5,968 CITATIONS    255 PUBLICATIONS   9,398 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Inherited Retinal Degenerative Disorders View project

Pseudoxanthoma elasticum View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jonathan B Savitz on 24 January 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328 Copyright # Blackwell Munksgaard 2006

Review

The molecular genetics of cognition: dopamine, COMT


and BDNF

J. Savitz*,†, M. Solms‡ and R. Ramesar† hat genes play in the development of the intellect. While

Galton’s conceptualization of intelligence as an instinct (see
MRC/UCT Human Genetics Research Unit, Institute of
Plotkin 1997) is no longer tenable, most modern studies
Infectious Disease and Molecular Medicine, ‡ Departments of
Psychology and Neurology, University of Cape Town, South acknowledge the importance of genetic factors, with herit-
Africa ability estimates of standardized intelligence quotient (IQ)
*Corresponding author: J. Savitz, MRC/UCT Human Genetics tests converging on the 50–80% range (Bouchard 1998).
Research Unit, University of Cape Town Medical School, Discrete (as opposed to global) cognitive processes such as
Observatory, 7925, South Africa. E-mail: js@cormack.uct.ac.za processing speed, short-term and working memory have
also been shown to be significantly influenced by genes
The important contribution of genetic factors to the
with heritability values ranging from 30 to 60% (Luciano
development of cognition and intelligence is widely
et al. 2001).
acknowledged, but identification of these genes has
Cognition is a complex trait and is therefore likely to be
proven to be difficult. Given a variety of evidence impli-
underpinned by many genes, each with a relatively small
cating the prefrontal cortex and its dopaminergic cir-
effect. To reduce the complexity involved, most investigators
cuits in cognition, most of the research conducted to
have attempted to fractionate global cognitive function into
date has focused on genes regulating dopaminergic
discrete cognitive processes via neuropsychological investi-
function. Here we review the genetic association studies
gations. Performance in these specific domains can then be
carried out on catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT)
statistically linked to the functional activity of particular pro-
and the dopamine receptor genes, D1, D2 and D4. In
teins, and by extension to the genetic variants accounting for
addition, the evidence implicating another promising
these functional differences. This is known as an association
candidate gene, brain-derived neurotrophic factor
study and accounts for a major component of genetic
(BDNF) in neuropsychological function, is assessed.
research into complex traits like cognition. Genes are usually
Both the COMT val158met polymorphism and the
considered good contenders for involvement in the genesis
BDNF val66met variant appear to influence cognitive
of a particular phenotype, if they are functionally polymorphic
function, but the specific neurocognitive processes
and have some prima facie physiological relevance to the
involved continue to be a matter of debate. Part of the
cognitive trait under consideration (Glatt & Freimer 2002).
difficulty is distinguishing between false positives,
These criteria will inform our evaluation of two of the most
pleiotropy and the influence of a general intelligence
promising genes implicated in neurocognitive function, cate-
factor, g. Also at issue is the complexity of the relevant
chol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and brain-derived neuro-
neuromolecular pathways, which make the inference of
trophic factor (BDNF). Given the pivotal role of dopamine
simple causal relationships difficult. The implications of
(DA) in the functioning of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (vide
molecular genetic cognitive research for psychiatry are
infra), the burgeoning database of genetic association stu-
discussed in light of these data.
dies involving DA receptor genes will also be covered here.
Keywords: BDNF, cognition, COMT, dopamine, genetics,
neuropsychological, prefrontal cortex
Dopamine and the prefrontal cortex
Received 18 April 2005, revised 25 May 2005, accepted for
publication 25 May 2005 Our linguistic and cognitive abilities are the characteristics
that set us apart from other primates, and these distinctive
functional differences presumably have neurological correl-
The genetics of cognition and intelligence have long fasci- ates. Rilling and Insel (1999), in a magnetic resonance ima-
nated both the scientific community and society at large. ging (MRI) investigation, compared neuroanatomical data
Ever since Francis Galton’s famous study of eminent across 11 different species of primates. Humans were
families, the pièce de résistance of the 1865 ‘Hereditary found to have significantly larger neocortices and had signifi-
Talent and Character’, controversy has raged over the role cantly more gyrified (a measure of cortical folding) prefrontal

doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00163.x 311


Savitz et al.

cortices than expected for a non-human primate of the same In addition, spatial working memory tasks have been shown
size (Rilling & Insel 1999). Secondly, a plethora of data from to increase dopaminergic activity in the dorsolateral PFC of
neuropsychological studies of brain-injured patients and primates (Kodoma et al. 1997).
more recently neuroimaging work has indicated that the In human studies, Luciana et al. (1998) improved the work-
PFC is critical for ‘executive functioning’, a miscellaneous ing memory performance of individuals by using the DA
category of cognitive processes that includes working mem- agonist bromocriptine, while set-shifting ability was reduced
ory, response inhibition, planning, concentration, attention, by DA antagonists (Lange et al. 1992). Ageing populations
perceptual organization, judgement, decision making and with decreased PFC DA levels and cognitive inflexibility have
self-monitoring (Benton 1994). also been shown to benefit from the administration of DA
Genes and gene families that are expressed in the PFC are agonists (Ollat 1992). More contemporary neuroimaging stu-
therefore obvious candidates for involvement in cognition. dies have demonstrated a correlation between the density of
Unfortunately, with approximately half of the 32 000 Homo dopaminergic fibres in the caudate nucleus and performance
sapiens genes expressed in the brain and significant portion on a range of executive and memory tests (Remy & Samson
of these genes expressed in the PFC, narrowing down the 2003). Recent data suggest, however, that there is not a
list of candidates becomes a speculative exercise. Previc simple linear relationship between DA and cognition.
(1999) has hypothesized that DA is the key regulator of six Dopamine does not always enhance cognitive perform-
predominantly left-hemisphere cognitive skills – the ‘execu- ance and may in fact retard it (Cools & Robbins 2004).
tive functions’ that are critical to human language and Bromocriptine, a DA D2 receptor agonist, was demonstrated
thought. In Previc’s schema, physiological adaptations to to improve cognition in individuals with a low-baseline mem-
ecological changes in sub-Saharan Africa catalysed the ory capacity but impair performance in high performers
expansion of dopaminergic pathways and with it our ascend- (Kimberg et al. 1997). Similarly, Mattay et al. (2000) showed
ing intellectual abilities. While this hypothesis may be specu- that dextroamphetamine, a drug that potentiates dopaminer-
lative, the evidence linking DA to prefrontal function and gic activity, improved cognitive performance in subjects with
cognition is well established (Cools & Robbins 2004). Given low-baseline levels of DA but that once DA receptor stimula-
the pivotal role of DA in the mediation of PFC function (vide tion reached a certain threshold, working memory function
infra) and the empirical evidence garnered from genetic asso- deteriorated. Thus, an inverted ‘U’ shaped curve charac-
ciation studies, we have in the first part of the paper terizes the relationship between DA levels and cognitive
focussed our attention on three candidates, COMT and the performance, with both suboptimal and supra-optimal DA
DA receptors, D2 and D4. activity impairing prefrontal function (Cools & Robbins 2004).
One of the first clues that DA is involved in cognition came These hypothesized effects may be mediated by the dif-
from studies of patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD). ferential effects of D1 and D2 receptor binding. Theoretically,
Working memory and executive type deficits are often pre- D2 receptor binding signals the presence of important (often
sent in the early stages of PD (Cools & Robbins 2004). reward-based) information and allows the PFC network to
Dubois and Pillon (1997) presented data indicating that respond to this new information by updating its working
about 93% of PD patients present with cognitive – mostly memory system (Weinberger et al. 2001). D1 receptor sti-
executive – deficits. Administration of L-DOPA to PD mulation, on the other hand, plays a gating role by controlling
patients has been reported to improve working memory the threshold of significance above which information must
(Mattay et al. 2002), cognitive flexibility (Cools et al. 2001) pass before it can be admitted to working memory and
and planning as evinced by the Tower of London (TOL) processed by the PFC. D1 receptor activation thus stabilizes
(Cools et al. 2002). In addition, the neurotoxin 1-methyl- the pattern of activity within the PFC neural network and
4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrohydropyridine, which results in a protects the system from distracters until the appropriate
degeneration of the nigrostriatal pathway, produces similar behavioural response is generated (Weinberger et al. 2001).
executive dysfunction to PD (Stern & Langston 1985). When the D1/D2-binding ratio is disturbed, cognitive per-
The second strand of evidence implicating DA in cognition formance is theoretically impaired in one of two directions:
comes from animal studies. Non-human primate studies of (a) A lack of D1-mediated signalling in the PFC (a hypodopa-
working memory make widespread use of the delayed minergic state) theoretically causes neural representations to
response task. The animal is shown the location of a piece be vulnerable to decay and competing inputs, resulting in
of food, the food is then hidden from view by an opaque poor differentiation of target from background (Winterer &
screen, and the primate must then choose the correct loca- Weinberger 2004). This may be manifested in impulsive and
tion of the treat from two or more options (Goldman-Rakic distractible behaviours and poor working memory and plan-
1995). Working memory deficits on this task were shown to ning, abilities which are mediated by the PFC. Attention is
result from experimental depletion of PFC DA in rhesus drawn to contextually weak stimuli, leading perhaps in
monkeys (Brozoski et al. 1979), and more contemporary extreme cases to the delusions, paranoia and neurocognitive
studies have shown that deficits on tests of working mem- dysfunction characteristic of schizophrenia (Winterer &
ory can be reversed with DA agonists (Goldman-Rakic 1995). Weinberger 2004). (b) Hyperdopaminergic states associated

312 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


The molecular genetics of cognition

with excessive D1 stimulation, on the other hand, may allow congruent with empirical data linking genotype with cogni-
representations in the PFC to be maintained but not effec- tive performance? We are aware of 26 studies that have
tively updated, leading in extreme cases to perseverative or examined the association between the COMT val158met
stereotypic behaviour (Weinberger et al. 2001) and reduced polymorphism and cognitive function, and these data are
performance on tasks requiring cognitive flexibility (Cools & summarized in Table 1. The results are impressive even if
Robbins 2004). one acknowledges the possible effects of publication bias.
Given the delicate balancing act that the regulators of PFC DA Twenty out of 26 studies report an association between the
activity need to achieve for optimal cognitive function, these COMT val158met polymorphism and cognitive function, and
enzymes make excellent candidate genes for influencing inter- all of them bar two suggest that the low-activity met allele
individual variation in cognition. Dopamine catabolism in the PFC allows for better performance on cognitive tasks that have a
is regulated by monoamine oxidase which is found on the working memory component.
mitochondrial membranes of catecholamine neurons and The latter point is important, because if all the positive
COMT which is concentrated in the extrasynaptic spaces findings reported to date were spurious, then it would be
(Deutch & Roth 1999). The dopamine transporter (DAT) is expected that the relevant studies would implicate all alleles
found primarily in dopaminergic neurons of the substantia of COMT with equal frequency. In fact, 90% of statistically
nigra and ventral tegmental area (VTA) and allows for the rapid significant results reported in the literature suggest that the
reuptake of DA from the synaptic cleft (Deutch & Roth 1999). low-activity met allele is associated with enhanced working
While all three of these enzymes are important regulators memory. Assuming that no publication bias exists, these
of subcortical and cortical DA transmission, COMT appears data suggest that the COMT val158met polymorphism may
to play the pivotal role in the modulation of fronto-striatal exert a genuine effect on cognition.
networks. An evolutionary recent functional single nucleotide Egan et al. (2001), in the original study, reported that the
polymorphism (SNP) of the COMT gene (val158met) located high-activity val allele was associated with poorer perform-
on chromosome 22q11 that results in the substitution of ance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), a putative
valine (val) with methionine (met) at codon 158 of the protein measure of ‘executive’ function, reduced efficiency of phy-
sequence has been described (GenBank accession no. siological response in the dorsolateral PFC during a working
Z26491). The met allele produces an enzyme that is unstable memory task and was transmitted more often than expected
at body temperature and has only a quarter the activity of the by chance to probands with schizophrenia. Egan et al. (2001)
val-containing polypeptide (Egan et al. 2001). suggested that the COMT genotype accounted for 4% of the
In the PFC, burst firing of VTA neurons causes synaptic DA variance in performance on the WCST, and this was echoed
release in pyramidal cells, but these cortical neurons contain by Joober et al. (2002), Bilder et al. (2002) and Goldberg et al.
very little DAT (Dougherty et al. 1999), allowing high levels of (2003). Despite this seemingly small effect size, however,
DA to diffuse out of the synaptic cleft and bind to positive results have been generated by studies with very
extrasynaptic D1 receptors where the neurotransmitter is modest sample sizes. Fourteen of the 20 studies reporting
inactivated principally by COMT (Bilder et al. 2004). The significant genotype – cognition associations – made use of
high-activity val allele decreases extrasynaptic DA concentra- sample sizes below 100 (Table 1). Whether this is a tribute to
tions and therefore D1 activation, shifting the balance in the sensitivity of the neuropsychological and neuroimaging
favour of intrasynaptic D2 receptor activation (Bilder et al. techniques or whether the COMT val158met polymorphism
2004; Winterer & Weinberger 2004). exerts a more significant effect on cognition is unclear.
Given the hypothetical differential effects of D1 and D2 There is some evidence for the latter possibility. Bilder
receptor stimulation discussed above, the effect of COMT et al. (2002) reported that the COMT genotype accounted
genotype on PFC-mediated cognitive functions may be as for 11% of the variance in processing speed and attention.
follows: The val (high-activity) allele theoretically results in Diamond et al. (2004) found that 26% of the variance on their
lowered stability of PFC neural networks which may mani- dots-mixed task was due to the effects of genotype, while
fest in decreased ability to maintain information in working Nolan et al. (2004) suggested that 28–41% of the statistical
memory (Bilder et al. 2004; Winterer & Weinberger 2004) but variance on their competing programs task was due to
enhanced ability to update the contents of working memory COMT variants.
with new information or switch cognitive sets (Cools & Despite the impressive congruency in genotype-cognition
Robbins 2004). The met allele, which is associated with an correlations, the specific cognitive functions or modules that
elevated PFC DA level and increased D1 receptor binding, is are sensitive to dopaminergic modulation remain a source of
predicted to enhance the stability of PFC networks and thus debate. Most of the initial studies used the WCST as a
the performance of cognitive tasks involving maintenance of measure of prefrontal cognitive function. The original test
information, but lead to decreased cognitive flexibility (Bilder developed by Berg in 1948 was designed to assess cognitive
et al. 2004; Winterer & Weinberger 2004). flexibility, hypothesis testing and problem solving. Frontal
This hypothesis offers an elegant model of PFC modula- cortex dysfunction may interfere with the planning of tasks
tion and cognition, but are these theoretical speculations and the ability to generate and select alternative hypotheses

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328 313


Table 1: Association between the Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) val158met polymorphism and cognitive performance

314
Study Sample (mean age) Ethnicity Cognitive tasks Outcome
Savitz et al.

Eisenberg et al. (1999) 48 ADHD triads (9.4) Ashkenazi and CPT Met/met subjects made less errors of commission
non-Ashkenazi jews (false alarms) on the CPT, indicating
lower levels of impulsivity
Egan et al. (2001) 175 schizophrenics (36), 219 Caucasian (USA) WCST Valine allele is associated with a greater number of
unaffected siblings and 55 C (34) perseverative errors on the WCST and reduced
efficacy of prefrontal function during a
working memory task
Bilder et al. (2002) 58 individuals with chronic Mixed (USA) Executive (digit-span, WCST, letter Met allele associated with better performance in
schizophrenia (40.5) fluency, block design, visual reproductions); the processing speed and attention domain
declarative memory (paragraph recall,
word-list learning); processing speed and
attention (TMT A þ B, DSST); simple
motor (tapping)
Fossella et al. (2002) 220 healthy adults (18–50) Not stated (USA) Attention network test (ANT) There was a non-significant trend towards higher
executive attention scores in the met/met
homozygotes
Joober et al. (2002) 94 schizophrenics Not stated (Canadian) WCST Met/met homozygotes performed better on the
(not given) þ 31 C (not given) WCST compared with heterozygotes
or val/val subjects
Malhotra et al. (2002) 73 healthy subjects (31.3) Mixed (USA) WCST Met/met homozygotes performed better on the
WCST compared with heterozygotes or
val/val subjects
Bates et al. (2003) 79 healthy subjects Not stated (USA) Not stated (long-term visual memory) Met/met homozygotes performed better than
val/val homozygotes on the visual memory task
Gallinat et al. (2003) 49 schizophrenics Caucasian (German) Auditory oddball ERP Met/met subjects had lower frontal P300 amplitudes
(32.8) þ 170 C (43.9) than val/val homozygotes. Frontal P300 amplitude
correlates negatively with prefrontal cognitive
task performance
Goldberg et al. (2003) 74 schizophrenics (37), 108 94% Caucasian (USA) N-back working memory task þ CPT CPT and 0-back performance (attention) not related
siblings (37) and 68 C (35) to genotype. Val/val genotype associated with
worst performance in 1 þ 2-back conditions
(working memory)
Mattay et al. (2003) 25 healthy subjects (33) Not stated (USA) WCST and N-back working memory task Val/val subjects on placebo made more errors on
imaged on amphetamine the WCST than met/met subjects. Val/val subjects
improved on the amphetamine, while met/met
subjects performed worse on the
WCST and N-back task
Tsai et al. 2003) 120 healthy females (19–21) Han Chinese WAIS þ WCST Met/met homozygotes showed reduced P300
response latencies. There was no difference
between the groups on the cognitive tasks
Bearden et al. (2004) 44 children (11.1) with 22q11 Predominantly Animal naming, TMT-B, arithmetic (WISC), Met hemizygotes performed better than val
deletion syndrome Caucasian (USA) digit span hemizygotes on digit span and TMT-B

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


Bertolino et al. (2004) 30 schizophrenics (28.6) Not stated (Italian) N-back working memory task No difference between groups at 4 weeks
treated with olanzapine over 8 weeks treatment. At 8 weeks, met carriers performed
better on 2-back task. Met/met homozygotes
showed least dorsolateral prefrontal
activation on fMRI
de Frias et al. (2004) 286 men (57.8) Not stated (Swedish) Episodic and semantic memory (battery) Met/met homozygotes performed better than
heterozygotes or val/val homozygotes on the
episodic and semantic memory tasks
Diamond et al. (2004) 39 healthy children (9) Predominantly Dots-mixed (working memory and inhibition); Met/met homozygotes performed better than
Caucasian (USA) self-ordered pointing task (working memory val/val subjects on the dots-mixed task only.
only); recall memory; mental rotation There was no genotype effect on the other tasks
(posterior parietal lobe)
Foltynie et al. (2004) 288 patients with Parkinson’s Not stated (British) TOL, CANTAB (pattern and spatial Met allele associated with poorer performance
disease (65.5) recognition), verbal fluency on the TOL only

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


Mills et al. (2004) 114 children with ADHD (9.2) Caucasian (British) Digit span, MFFT, CPT, Go No Go (MARS) No significant differences
Nolan et al. (2004) 26 schizophrenics (41.4) Predominantly Competing programs task (tests imitation Met allele associated with better imitation
Caucasian (USA) and reversal of rule) (cognitive stability) but val allele associated with
better performance in the reversal condition
(flexibility)
Rosa et al. (2004) 89 sibling pairs discordant for Not stated (Spanish) WCST No association between genotype and
schizophrenia (not given) performance in schizophrenics.Val/val genotype
associated with greater number of perseverative
errors in healthy siblings
Stefanis et al. (2004) 543 males from the general Caucasian (Greek) N-back working memory task; CPT; No significant association between genotype and
population (21) antisaccade task task performance
Strauss et al. (2004) 63 young adults with 65% Caucasian (USA) Logical memory tests from the WMS; No significant association between genotype and
childhood-onset mood disorder (18.4) paired associates (delayed recall); task performance
RCF; WAIS; WISC
Taerk et al. (2004) 118 ADHD children (9) Not stated (Caucasian) SOPT, TOL, WCST No significant association between genotype and
task performance
Tsai et al. (2004) 154 schizophrenics (43.5) Chinese MMSE No significant differences reported
Weickert et al. (2004) 20 schizophrenics (approximately 32) Not stated N-back working memory task; WCST; Met/met subjects on medication improved on
given antipsychotics or placebo verbal fluency; WAIS; WMS the N-back task. Val/val subjects showed no
improvement with treatment and were more
impaired than met/met homozygotes
Bellgrove et al. (2005) 61 children and adolescents Caucasian (Irish) WISC; TEA-Ch Val/val homozygotes performed better than
with ADHD (12) heterozygotes or met/met homozygotes on the
‘Walk Don’t Walk’ subtest which measures
sustained attention and response inhibition
Ho et al. (2005) 159 schizophrenics (26.5) Caucasian (USA). WCST, digit span (R), No significant differences reported
and 84 C (27) TMT (A þ B); N-back working
memory task

ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; C, controls; CANTAB, Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery; CPT, continuous performance test; DSST, digit symbol
substitution test; ERP, event related potential; MARS, maudsley attention and response suppression battery; MFFT, matching familiar figures test; MMSE, mini mental state examination;
RCF, rey complex figure; SOPT, self-ordered pointing task; TEA-Ch, tests of everyday attention for children; TMT, trail-making test; TOL, tower of London; WAIS, Wechsler adult intelligence
test; WCST, Wisconsin card sorting test; WISC, Wechsler intelligence scale for children; WMS, Wechsler memory scale.
The molecular genetics of cognition

315
Savitz et al.

after negative feedback (shifting cognitive set), traits which with the left or right hand according to the side of the screen
the WCST putatively measures (Lezak 1995). The inability to that the dot appears on, while for the striped dots, the sub-
shift cognitive set is usually recorded as ‘percentage of per- ject is required to use the opposite hand (Diamond et al.
severative errors’ made by the individual. 2004). In the self-ordered pointing task, a series of line draw-
A consistent association between the val/val genotype and ings or abstract designs are presented. The subject’s task is
greater percentage of perseverative errors on the WCST to touch each stimulus only once, but after each response,
characterizes the literature (Egan et al. 2001; Joober et al. the computer screen refreshes, and the order of the stimuli
2002; Malhotra et al. 2002; Mattay et al. 2003; Rosa et al. changes (Diamond et al. 2004). Although both tasks depend
2004), a trend that appears to run counter to the hypothe- on the integrity of the dorsolateral PFC, only the dots-mixed
sized sequelae of hypodopaminergic states, discussed test was associated with genotype (met homozygotes per-
above. The val allele, reducing D1 receptor binding, should formed better), and this led Diamond et al. (2004) to propose
allow for more cognitive flexibility, the negative aspect of that working memory per se is not sensitive to PFC DA
which may be greater distractibility as evinced by a WCST levels and COMT genotype. It is the ability to inhibit prepo-
measure such as ‘failure to maintain cognitive set’. On the tent responses and shut out distracting stimuli, along with
other hand, individuals with the met/met genotype should working memory that is sensitive to DA regulation (Diamond
theoretically be more cognitively stable, less distractible and et al. 2004).
in certain cases therefore more likely to make perseverative Nolan et al. (2004) examined the effect of the COMT
errors. genotype on a computerized task that requires shifting
The difficulty with the WCST is that it is a complex pro- between two rules of responding, imitation and reversal. In
blem-solving task with multiple cognitive components and the imitation condition, the subject presses once on a key in
thus reasons for failure (Bilder et al. 2004; Goldberg et al. response to a single stimulus and twice in the case of two
2003). Nevertheless, the complexity of the task does not stimuli. The rules are then reversed, and the individual is
explain the consistent association (five studies) between required to press once in response to two stimuli and twice
the presence of the val allele and the increased number of in the case of a single stimulus. Acquisition and maintenance
perseverative errors on the WCST. Perhaps val/val homo- of the imitation rule reportedly requires cognitive stability,
zygotes are less able to inhibit prepotent responses (see whereas the reversal condition requires flexibility and the
below) – in this case, the prepotent response may be the ability to inhibit the previously learned, more intuitive
tendency to continue with the same strategy – than met response (Nolan et al. 2004). In line with the hypothesized
allele carriers, and therefore make more perseverative errors. effects of D1 and D2 receptor binding, Nolan et al. (2004)
The lack of specificity of the WCST has led to the use of found that the met allele was associated with better cogni-
simpler neuropsychological tasks with more focussed tive stability but poorer flexibility.
effects. Goldberg et al. (2003) made use of the N-back- While it may be premature on the basis of a small number
working memory test in which the individual must continu- of studies to engage in theoretical speculation about the
ously recall information that is 0 or 1, or more, back in a evolutionary trade-off between these two alleles, the high
sequence. Catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype was not frequency of the low performance val allele in most popula-
related to performance in the 0-back condition, which is more tions may be explained by hypothesizing that greater cogni-
a measure of attention than working memory (Goldberg et al. tive stability and enhanced working memory makes the PFC
2003). In the 1-back and 2-back conditions, however, system less sensitive to internal or external stimuli, a trade-
individuals with the met allele outperformed their counter- off that presumably incurred costs in a hostile evolutionary
parts, suggesting that the process of information updating environment.
and maintenance in the face of competing stimuli is sensitive A recent report has suggested that the influence of COMT
to prefrontal DA levels (Goldberg et al. 2003). In a sample of on cognition may extend beyond working memory and inhib-
schizophrenics treated with olanzapine, met carriers also ition. de Frias et al. (2004) found an association between the
performed better than val/val individuals on the 2-back task COMT genotype and episodic and semantic memory. The
(Bertolino et al. 2004). Bilder et al. (2004), however, argue effect was specific to recall rather than recognition, with
that the N-back task requires both stability (the maintenance met/met individuals outperforming heterozygotes, but not
of information) and flexibility (updating the currently relevant val/val homozygotes (de Frias et al. 2004). In an earlier
stimulus with new information) and thus suffers from some paper, Bates et al. (2003) found that met/met homozygotes
of the same drawbacks as the WCST. performed better than val/val homozygotes on a delayed
Diamond et al. (2004) tested a group of children on a task visual memory-recall task. Whether these are false-positive
that putatively requires both working memory and inhibition results or are indicative of a role for DA in memory function
(the dots-mixed task) and a task that requires working mem- remains to be seen. Diamond et al. (2004) and Strauss et al.
ory only (self-ordered pointing). The dots-mixed task pre- (2004) failed to implicate COMT in memory performance,
sents subjects with two types of dots, shaded or striped. although these studies may have been under-powered with
For the shaded dots, the person is required to press a button sample sizes of 39 and 63, respectively.

316 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


The molecular genetics of cognition

Ho et al. (2005) have hypothesized that age differences amphetamine, which blocks the action of dopamine, seroto-
between samples may be responsible for the failure to nin and noradrenaline transporters, improves working mem-
detect a significant gene – cognition association in some ory in individuals with suboptimal D1 receptor activation (i.e.
studies. The rationale behind this argument is a putative val/val individuals) but has a detrimental effect on PFC cogni-
decline in frontal lobe DA function of 11–13% per decade, tion in met/met individuals. In contrast, antipsychotics were
which may render older groups more sensitive to a gene- found to improve cognitive function as evinced by the N-back
tically mediated (val allele) increase in DA catabolism (Ho task in met homozygotes, but val/val individuals did not
et al. 2005). Because the mean age of the sample of Ho improve cognitively on treatment (Weickert et al. 2004).
et al. (2005) was approximately 26, we divided the data in Inada et al. (2003) examined the response of 100 schizo-
Table 1 into two groups: those studies that sampled indivi- phrenics to neuroleptics and found that met/met homozy-
duals below 30 and those studies with a mean subject age of gotes were on a significantly higher dose of maintenance
greater than 30. We excluded the data derived from children therapy than the other patients. This effect was partially
with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and stu- replicated by Illi et al. (2003) who detected a significantly
dies where age was not reported. higher rate of conventional neuroleptic treatment resistance
Preliminary support for the hypothesis of Ho et al. was in met/met patients. Thus, the effect of the met allele which
detected. Three out of five studies including individuals with shifts the D1/D2 activation ratio in favour of D1 is presumably
a mean age of less than 30 were unable to detect a signifi- accentuated by D2 receptor antagonists.
cant gene – neurocognitive function relationship, while 10
out of 11 studies with a mean sample age of more than 30
reported positive results. However, the effect of COMT vari-
DRD1 and DRD2
ants on cognitive function in children is not entirely consist-
ent with this hypothesis. Out of six studies that sampled The important influence of COMT variants on the D1/D2
children, three reported the met allele to be associated receptor-activation ratio raises the possibility that functional
with better performance on tests of ‘executive’ function, polymorphisms of these two receptor genes may also con-
one found the met allele to be associated with worse cogni- tribute to population variation in cognitive performance. The
tive function, and two studies were non-significant. D1 receptor is highly expressed in the glutamatergic pyrami-
The difficulty with the interpretation of childhood studies dal cells of the PFC (Lidow et al. 1991). Arnsten et al. (1994)
of cognition is that the PFC is yet to reach full maturity. demonstrated that a D1 receptor antagonist impaired the
Segalowitz & Davies (2004) hypothesized that the PFC devel- working memory of monkeys, while a D1 agonist improved
ops until late adolescence, while Welsh et al. (1991) memory performance. Similarly, D1 receptor knockout mice
extended this neurodevelopmental process until early adult- display spatial learning deficits (El-Ghundi et al. 1999), and
hood. In the absence, therefore, of a lucid understanding of antipsychotic induced downregulation of PFC D1 receptors
the developmental status of these inchoate dopaminergic was shown to produce severe working memory deficits
pathways, extrapolating genetic association data from chil- which could be reversed by D1 receptor stimulation
dren to adults may be problematic. (Castner et al. 2000). Okubo et al. (1997) reported a positive
In summary then, on the basis of the genetics literature, correlation between the density of prefrontal D1 receptors
neurocognitive tasks that require the individual to hold infor- and performance on the WCST in a schizophrenic population,
mation ‘on-line’ (i.e. working memory) and perhaps inhibit and Muller et al. (1998) showed that visuospatial working
responses to prepotent or distracting stimuli appear to be memory improved in individuals administered pergolide, a
most sensitive to differential COMT activity. Some prelimin- D1 and D2 receptor agonist.
ary evidence suggests that the effect of COMT variants may D2 receptors are most commonly found on the GABAergic
extend to other cognitive domains such as memory (Bates interneurons of the PFC and appear to play a role in depres-
et al. 2003; de Frias et al. 2004). The problem with simply sing N-Methyl-D-Aspartate (NMDA) receptor-mediated exci-
reporting scores obtained on neuropsychological tests of tatory neurotransmission (Kotecha et al. 2002). Mehta et al.
memory in isolation, however, is that low scores may be (1999) administered sulpiride, a D2 receptor antagonist, to
indicative of poor executive functioning rather than a bona healthy volunteers and found that the drug impaired spatial
fide memory deficit (that is a deficit mediated by hippocam- working memory, planning and attentional set-shifting. In a
pal dysfunction). In our opinion, therefore, the balance of follow-up study, the same group showed that shifting of
evidence indicates that it may be premature at this stage to cognitive set is particularly sensitive to D2 receptor antagon-
invoke a role for COMT in mediating medial temporal ism (Mehta et al. 2004). Kimberg et al. (1997) demonstrated
function. that a D2 receptor agonist improved cognition in individuals
While debate will doubtless continue over the nature of with a low-baseline memory capacity. In addition, an age-
the neurocircuitry modulated by COMT, important implica- related decrease in D2 receptors has been hypothesized to
tions for the treatment of a variety of psychiatric conditions contribute to cognitive decline in the elderly. As a result, the
loom on the horizon. Mattay et al. (2003) demonstrated that D2 agonist piribedil has been used in the treatment of

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328 317


Savitz et al.

patients with mild cognitive impairment, apparently with Using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, a measure of
some success (Peretti et al. 2004). verbal memory and learning, Bartres-Faz et al. (2002)
On the basis of the pharmacological data described above, reported a result in the same direction as Tsai et al. (2002):
it is entirely plausible that executive functions such as work- reduced performance in A2 homozygotes. However, in a
ing memory are influenced by functional variation in both the sample of 10–14-year-old boys, Noble et al. (1994) and
D1 and D2 receptors. We were unable to find any studies Berman and Noble (1995) reported prolonged P300 latency
that made reference to a relationship between cognitive and reduced visuospatial performance in A1 allele carriers.
performance and polymorphisms of the D1 receptor. A hand- These contradictory findings together with the modest sam-
ful of publications have, however, addressed the role of D2 ple sizes of the relevant studies suggest the possibility of
receptor polymorphisms on cognition (Table 2). All of these false positives. Nevertheless, given the wide distribution of
studies concentrated on one particular SNP within the DRD2 the DRD2 receptor in the caudate, putamen, nucleus accum-
gene, the TaqIA polymorphism. bens, amygdala, hippocampus and cerebral cortex and the
The DRD2 gene is about 270 kb long and contains eight effect of COMT on the D1/D2 receptor-activation ratio,
exons, with the TaqIA locus located in the 30 untranslated further investigation of this gene may prove worthwhile.
region of the gene (Noble 2000). The functional status of the We suggest that because of its putative functional effect,
TaqIA variant is not entirely clear, but some preliminary evi- use of the C957T polymorphism may provide more decisive
dence indicated that A1 allele was associated with reduced results than have been achieved up to now.
density of DRD2 receptors in the striatum (Pohjalainen et al.
1998). More recently, Duan et al. (2003) found that a synon-
ymous SNP (C957T) affects mRNA stability and therefore
DRD4
receptor expression, and this was confirmed in vivo by Wang et al. (2002) proposed that DRD4 impacts prefrontal
Hirvonen et al. (2004). The C957T variant was found to be cognition by virtue of its effects on GABAergic signalling. The
in linkage disequilibrium with the TaqI A variant in a authors demonstrated that activation of DRD4 receptors,
Caucasian but not an African-American sample (Duan et al. located in the vicinity of GABAA receptors on dendrites,
2003). reduces GABAA receptor-mediated currents in the PFC
Five studies examining the effect of the DRD2 TaqIA poly- (Wang et al. 2002). GABAergic activity in the PFC has been
morphism and cognition are listed in Table 2. Tsai et al. 2002) shown to modulate the processing of information and the
and Petrill et al. (1997) made use of standardized IQ tests. planning of future actions (Constantinidis et al. 2002). Thus,
The former reported that A1 homozygotes outperformed both DRD4 and genes coding for components of the
their counterparts on the performance subscale of the GABAergic system are also candidates for influencing cogni-
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)-R, while the latter tion. Egan et al. (2004) showed that a potentially functional
found no significant association between genotype and IQ. variant in the metabotropic glutamate receptor, GRM3,

Table 2: Relationship between dopamine D2 variants and cognitive performance

Study Sample Ethnicity Cognitive tasks Outcome

Noble et al. (1994) 98 10–14-year-old boys Caucasian (USA) CPT ERP-derived P300 latency was prolonged
in Taq A1 allele 1 carriers
Berman and Noble 182 10–14-year-old boys Caucasian (USA) JLO Reduced visuospatial performance
(1995) associated with the Taq A1 allele
Petrill et al. (1997) 51 high IQ, 51 average IQ, Caucasian (USA) WISC-R No significant association between IQ
and 35 low IQ children and genotype
Bartres-Faz et al. 49 memory impaired subjects Not stated (Spanish) MMSE, RAVLT, Subjects homozygous for the Taq A2
(2002) (mean age 65 years) logical memory, allele exhibited reduced left caudate
visual reproduction, volumes and performed worse on the
visual paired associates, RAVLT and MMSE
TMT-A þ B; FAS;
TOH; JLO
Tsai et al. (2002) 112 female volunteers, Han Chinese WAIS-R The A1/A1 group scored significantly
19–21 years old higher on the performance scale than the
A2/A2 group

CPT, continuous performance test; ERP, event related potential; JLO, judgement of line orientation; MMSE, mini mental status examination;
RAVLT, Rey auditory verbal learning test; TMT, trail making test; TOH, tower of Hanoi; WAIS-R, Weschler adult intelligence scale (revised);
WISC-R, Wechsler intelligence scale for children (revised).

318 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


The molecular genetics of cognition

impacts verbal fluency and memory and is associated with mediates intracellular signalling (Asghari et al. 1995). The
abnormal PFC and hippocampal activation as evinced by strategic position of the polymorphism suggests that the
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). To our know- DRD4 receptor variants may exert a functional effect.
ledge, this is the only genetic association study that has Asghari et al. (1995) found differences in cyclic adenosine
correlated GRM3 variants with cognition. More work has monophosphate inhibition between the 4R and 7R alleles, as
been carried out on DRD4, most probably because of its well as between a 2R and 4R combined group, and the 7R
hypothesized role in ADHD. allele. It is unclear whether the various alleles in question are
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder is characterized by differentially sensitive to endogenous DA, although the 4R
deficits in impulse control, attention, working memory, plan- allele has been reported to be more responsive to pharma-
ning and self-regulation (Barkley 1997), traits that are usually cological DA agonists (Fossella et al. 2002).
associated with damage to or dysfunction of PFC circuits. We are aware of four papers that have described an asso-
The treatment of choice, methylphenidate (Ritalin), which ciation between alleles of the DRD4 48 bp VNTR polymorph-
promotes the release of DA and blocks its reuptake, is sug- ism and cognitive functioning (see Table 3). Interestingly,
gestive of an intimate link between this neurotransmitter and three out of the four studies were carried out on individuals
ADHD. Thus, any genes involved in the aetiology of ADHD with ADHD, possibly because the DRD4 gene is a candidate
may be good candidates for modulating cognitive function. gene for this disorder. While Swanson et al. (2000), Fossella
One such candidate is the DA D4 receptor gene. The asso- et al. (2002) and Manor et al. (2002) found that children with
ciation between an exonic DRD4 variable number tandem a 4R allele or other shorter (2–5R) alleles performed more
repeat (VNTR) variant (see below) and ADHD is one of the poorly on tests of attention and executive functioning,
most robust findings in psychiatric genetics (DiMaio et al. Langley et al. (2004) report increased impulsivity and there-
2003). Both case-control and family-based association stud- fore worse performance on a test of behavioural inhibition in
ies have suggested that the longer or 7R allele is a risk factor carriers of the 7R allele.
for ADHD (DiMaio et al. 2003). If the 4R variant does indeed increase sensitivity to DA,
The DRD4 belongs to the same class of receptor as DRD2, then one would expect more efficient signal transduction in
exerting an inhibitory effect on the adenylyl cyclase-mediated 4R carriers and a subsensitive response to DA in 7R carriers.
secondary messenger system and is found primarily in the Theoretically, the 7R allele could therefore be associated
limbic regions and on PFC pyramidal neurons (Wang et al. with under-activity of the mesocorticolimbic DA pathway
2002). The DRD4 gene is located on the short arm of chro- (Swanson et al. 2000), although, to our knowledge, this
mosome 11 and contains a 48 bp VNTR polymorphism in the remains to be conclusively demonstrated. However, this
third exon of the gene which has been widely typed in hypothesis will have to reconcile better neurocognitive per-
genetic association studies (Ding et al. 2002). Between 2 formance in ADHD individuals with the DRD4 7R allele (pre-
and 11 repeated elements have been reported in the litera- sumably with an under-active mesocortical DA pathway) and
ture, although the two predominant alleles in Caucasians the association between improved cognition and the COMT
consist of four (4R) and seven repeats (7R), respectively met allele (which elevates the PFC DA level).
(Ding et al. 2002). The polymorphic repeated segment The interpretation of these results is further complicated
codes for amino acids in the third intracellular loop of the by the hypothesis of Swanson et al. (2000) that there may be
receptor, a region that couples to G proteins and therefore two basic aetiological subtypes of ADHD. One sector of the

Table 3: Associations between the dopamine D4 receptor gene and cognition

Study Sample Ethnicity Cognitive tasks Outcome

Swanson et al. (2000) 32 ADHD 68% Caucasian (USA) Stroop, Individuals with a 7R allele performed at the same level as
subjects cued detection task, the control group. ADHD children without a 7R allele
go-change task performed poorly on the battery
Fossella et al. (2002) 200 adults Not stated (USA) ANT Individuals without a 4R allele performed better on the
executive attention component than carriers of the allele
Manor et al. (2002) 178 ADHD Not stated (Israeli) CPT (TOVA) Children with the short (2–5) exon III repeats performed
triads worse on the CPT
Langley et al. (2004) 133 children Caucasian (British) CPT, MFFT, Children with the 7-R allele were more impulsive on the
with ADHD Go/No Go Task, MFFT and stop task
Stop Task

ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ANT, attention network test; CPT, continuous performance test; MFFT, matching familiar figures
test; TOVA, test of variables of attention

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328 319


Savitz et al.

ADHD population displays a profile suggestive of significant synaptic transmission at both excitatory and inhibitory
cognitive (and neuropsychological task) impairment. The bal- synapses (Baldelli et al. 2002). Brain-derived neurotrophic
ance of the population is hypothesized to carry the 7R poly- factor is also secreted in response to neuronal activity
morphism implicated in novelty-seeking behaviour (Savitz & (Farhadi et al. 2000; Goodman et al. 1996, cited in Egan
Ramesar 2004). This genetic subtype may represent an et al. 2003) where it acts as a retrograde or paracrine mes-
extreme of temperament, manifesting the behavioural but senger allowing neuronal activity to modify synaptic connec-
not the neurocognitive problems associated with ADHD tions (Egan et al. 2003; Poo 2001). This modulation of
(Swanson et al. 2000). If this postulate is correct, then synaptic plasticity and neuronal transmission is particularly
those individuals with a ‘genetic subtype’ of ADHD (i.e. salient in the hippocampus where BDNF-mediated enhance-
carriers of the 7R allele) may outperform their counterparts ment of long-term potentiation (LTP) has been shown to
who have sustained some exogenous form of damage to the facilitate memory formation (Poo 2001).
brain during development. This latter ADHD group will prob- Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is widely expressed in
ably not display an excess of any particular DRD4 VNTR the rat hippocampus, and studies of BDNF knockout mice
allele. Thus, in ADHD samples, although the 7R allele may have demonstrated impairment of hippocampal LTP (Pozzo-
be associated with better cognitive performance, it may play Miller et al. 1999), and treatment with antisense BDNF oli-
no role in cognition and may simply be a proxy for ADHD gonucleotide or anti-BDNF antibody impaired spatial memory
aetiology. formation in rats (Mu et al. 1999). Knockout mice, with a
In addition, a C to T transition 521 bp upstream from the deletion of the TrkB gene which codes for the BDNF recep-
transcription-initiation site has attracted attention because of tor, show LTP and maze-learning deficits (Minichiello et al.
its possible functional effect. According to Okuyama et al. 1999). The causal mechanism behind BDNF-mediated modu-
(1999), the T variant is transcribed with 40% less efficiency lation of LTP is not clear, but one suggestion is that the
than the C allele. More recently, D’Souza et al. (2004) protein enhances synaptic vesicle docking by phosphorylat-
reported that the longer allele of a 120-bp tandem repeat in ing synaptic proteins (Pang & Lu 2004).
the 50 -flanking region of the DRD4 gene, which is known to The BDNF gene is located on chromosome 11p13 and is
contain predicted consensus sequence transcription factor- composed of five or more exons, each with its own promoter
binding sites, has lower levels of transcriptional activity than region allowing for differential mRNA splicing (Jiang et al.
the shorter allele. These variants have also been implicated 2005). A frequent, non-conservative SNP in the gene (dbSNP
in ADHD (Kustanovich et al. 2004) and novelty-seeking number rs6265), producing a val to met amino acid substitu-
behaviour (Rogers et al. 2004) and may be worthwhile geno- tion at codon 66 (val66met) of the pro-BDNF sequence, was
typing in future investigations of the genetics of cognition. shown by Egan et al. (2003) to affect the activity-dependent
Theoretically, the 48 bp VNTR could be in linkage disequili- secretion of BDNF. Depolarization-dependent secretion of
brium with either or both of these polymorphisms (or some BDNF is impaired by the met allele. The met-BDNF may
other unknown variant), although no linkage disequilibrium not be correctly transferred from the Golgi apparatus to its
between the 521 C/T polymorphism and the 48 bp VNTR appropriate secretory granules, despite the fact that mature
was detected by Fossella et al. (2002) in their sample. protein function is unaltered by the polymorphism (Egan
et al. 2003).
On the basis of these data, Egan et al. (2003) and Hariri
et al. (2003) reasoned that the val66met variant may impact
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor human hippocampal function and memory. In a sample of
Neurotrophins are regulatory factors that mediate the differ- schizophrenic and healthy subjects, the met allele was asso-
entiation, proliferation and survival of cholinergic, dopaminer- ciated with poorer episodic memory as evinced by immedi-
gic and serotonergic neurons (Poo 2001). Brain-derived ate and delayed recall of Wechsler Memory Scale stories, a
neurotrophic factor, one of these neurotrophins, is disruption of the normal hippocampal fMRI disengagement
expressed throughout the brain, particularly in the PFC and pattern during a working memory task, and lower hippocam-
hippocampus (Pezawas et al. 2004) where it exerts long- pal n-acetyl aspartate, an intracellular marker of neuronal
term effects on neuronal survival, migration, dendritic and function (Egan et al. 2003). Similarly, Hariri et al. (2003)
axonal growth (Pang & Lu 2004). Brain-derived neurotrophic demonstrated that met-BDNF carriers displayed reduced hip-
factor has been shown to prevent the spontaneous death of pocampal engagement during both the encoding and retrie-
dopaminergic rat neurons (Hyman et al. 1994), exert a pro- val of a spatial task and made significantly more recognition
tective effect in the presence of neurotoxins (Hung & Lee errors on this task than val/val homozygotes. These results
1996) and elevate forebrain 5-HT neuronal fiber density were partially replicated by Dempster et al. (2005). Another
(Mamounas et al. 1995). study of declarative memory in affectively ill individuals,
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor also appears to act on a however, failed to confirm these original findings (Strauss
much shorter temporal scale. In vitro experiments demon- et al. 2004). In MRI investigations, Pezawas et al. (2004)
strate that BDNF application causes rapid changes in and Szeszko et al. (2005) extended the findings of Egan

320 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


The molecular genetics of cognition

et al. (2003) and Hariri et al. (2003) by demonstrating that val/ BDNF synthesis, respectively (Duman et al. 1997). Because
met heterozygotes displayed lower hippocampal volumes the hippocampus is particularly vulnerable to the action of
than their val/val counterparts. glucocorticoids (Nestler et al. 2002), this may explain the
The putative effects of the val66met variant on working memory deficits described above. A potentially useful future
memory and retrieval of encoded information suggest that study would be to examine the effects of the val66met
the influence of BDNF may extend beyond its role in LTP polymorphism on cortisol release and hypothalamic pitui-
(see Table 4). Rybakowski et al. (2003) found that carriers of tary-adrenal axis dysfunction.
the met allele performed significantly worse than val/val A second possibility is that the val66met variant influences
homozygotes on the WCST, a measure of PFC function. the efficacy of both early- and late-phase LTP in the hippo-
This was confirmed by the recent Pezawas et al. (2004) campus (Egan et al. 2003; Hariri et al. 2003), consistent with
report, demonstrating that met-BDNF carriers display age- reported BDNF-driven variation in verbal and visual declara-
and gender-independent dorsolateral PFC gray matter tive memory. Interestingly, three out of four studies hypothe-
volume reductions. Pezawas et al. (2004) propose that this sizing a significant role for BDNF in neurocognition have
variation in cortical morphology is at least partly the result of reported that heterozygotes perform more poorly on cogni-
long-term BDNF-mediated effects on the developmental pro- tive tasks than val/val homozygotes. Whether this is an arte-
cess and is not simply a reflection of transient val66met- fact of the low frequency of met homozygotes in the
moderated neurotransmitter fluctuations during memory population, limiting potential group comparisons in genetic
formation. association studies, or indicative of the sensitivity of LTP to
Tsai et al. (2004) presented data indicating that val/val BDNF level is debatable.
homozygotes performed significantly better than carriers of Thirdly, BDNF has diffuse effects on monoaminergic neu-
the met allele on the performance scale of the WAIS, rotransmitter systems. It promotes the sprouting of mature
although the statistical significance was marginal, and the serotonergic neurons (Mamounas et al. 1995), augments
result stands in contradistinction to that of Egan et al. central serotonergic activity following a midbrain infusion
(2003). On the other hand, Foltynie et al. (2005) reported (Siuciak et al. 1996), moderates the firing of neurons in the
that in patients with PD, the met allele was associated with dorsal raphe nucleus (Celada et al. 1996) and modulates
better performance on the TOL, a measure of planning abil- serotonin transporter function (Mossner et al. 2000). Brain-
ity. The apparently differential effect of the BDNF val66met derived neurotrophic factor has also been shown to influence
polymorphism in PD may possibly be explained by the com- dopaminergic activity (Narita et al. 2003). Administration of
plex interactions between BDNF secretion and degeneration BDNF has been shown to potentiate nigrostriatal dopaminer-
of the nigro-striatal dopaminergic neurons which may result gic function and locomotor activity (Horger et al. 1999; Pierce
in a prefrontal-striatal DA imbalance (Foltynie et al. 2005). et al. 1999, cited in Narita et al. 2003), and chronic BDNF
Thus, out of six studies that have implicated BDNF variants treatment enhances the reward response to cocaine (Horger
in cognition, four have reported an association between the et al. 1999).
met allele and worse performance on sundry neurocognitive In summary, (a) Animal studies suggest that BDNF pro-
tasks. One study is equivocal (Tsai et al. 2004), and one tects neurons from the effects of damage, plays a role in
study (Foltynie et al. 2005) finds evidence for improved per- modifying synaptic connections and modulates hippocampal
formance in met carriers (see Table 4). LTP. (b) Human studies have demonstrated that the met
A potentially confounding variable is the discovery by Jiang allele is associated with lower hippocampal volume or func-
et al. (2005) of a functional polymorphism 281 bp upstream tional activity. (c) Genetic studies with the functional val66-
from the putative transcription-initiation site of exon 1 of met variant have indicated that the met allele is associated
BDNF. The ‘A’ variant which may be protective against anxi- with weaker performance on tests of memory and ‘execu-
ety and psychiatric illness appears to exert an independent, tive’ function. Given these parallel sources of evidence, we
but opposite effect to the met 66 allele which has been suggest that it is likely that val66met BDNF variant exerts an
implicated in psychopathology (Jiang et al. 2005). The two effect on memory performance and perhaps executive func-
genetic variants may, however, be in linkage disequilibrium tion, although more evidence is required for the latter.
with each other, although, given the differential anxiety-
related effects of the 281C>A variant on the val 66 haplo-
type background (Jiang et al. 2005), future research may
benefit from controlling for this SNP.
Caveats
There appear to be at least three different mechanisms A number of theoretical obstacles may impede the resolution
through which BDNF impacts cognition. One possibility is of gene-cognition relationships.
that stress, especially chronic stress, may result in excessive (a) Gene-cognition correlations reported in the literature
release of glucocorticoids from the adrenal gland, directly usually rest on the assumption that the effect of the genetic
and indirectly causing atrophy and death of vulnerable neu- variant in question is specific to a particular cognitive process
rons through the actions of cortisol and the inhibition of and is not reflective of variation in a general intelligence

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328 321


322
Savitz et al.

Table 4: Association between the BDNF val66met polymorphism and cognition

Study Sample Ethnicity Cognitive tasks Outcome

Egan et al. (2003) 203 schizophrenics and 305 90% Caucasian (USA) WMS (episodic memory) Met/met homozygotes performed worse thanheterozygotes or
siblings and 133 C and CVLT val/val homozygotes on WMS. No difference on CVLT
Hariri et al. (2003) 28 healthy subjects Predominantly Caucasian Visual declarative memory (IAPS) Memory-related hippocampal activity greaterduring both
(USA) encoding þ retrieval in val/val
homozygotes. Met carriers made more errors
on recognition memory task
Rybakowski et al. (2003) 54 bipolar patients Not stated (Polish) WCST Val/val subjects performed significantly better than
met allele carriers
Nacmias et al. (2004) 83 Alzheimer patients and 97 C Caucasian (Italian) IMCT, digit span, CTT, NS
babcock story,
paired words
Strauss et al. (2004) 63 adults with childhood (Canadian) Declarative memory NS
-onset mood disorder
Tsai et al. (2004b) 114 healthy females Han Chinese WAIS Val/met heterozygotes but not met/met homozygotes
performed worse than the val/val group on the performance
scale of the WAIS
Dempster et al. (2005) 92 schizophrenics and 114 Not stated (British) WMS The met allele was associated with worse performance on the
unaffected relatives delayed score of the logical memory subscale
Foltynie et al. (2005) 291 individuals with Not stated (British) TOL, verbal fluency, spatial and Patients with a met allele performed better on the TOL than val/
Parkinson’s disease pattern recognition (CANTAB) val homozygotes

C, controls; CANTAB, Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery; CTT, corsi tapping task; CVLT, California verbal learning test; IAPS, international affective picture system; IMCT,
international memory and concentration test; NS, not significant; Schiz, schizophrenics; Sibs, siblings; TOL, tower of London: WAIS, Wechsler adult intelligence test; WCST, Wisconsin card
sorting test; WMS, Wechsler memory scale.

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


The molecular genetics of cognition

factor. It has long been recognized that scores on different 100 or less report positive results (see Table 1). The rela-
IQ tests emphasizing distinctive types of cognitive tasks are tively low rate of positive findings in studies with large sam-
positively correlated with each other, suggesting the exist- ple sizes and statistical power is a cause for concern and
ence of a general intelligence factor eponymously named raises the possibility that some of these results may be
Spearman’s g (Spearman 1904). Persuasive evidence that spurious. Concerning BDNF, the two studies with the largest
IQ scores predict concurrent neuropsychological perfor- sample sizes, 203 schizophrenics, 305 siblings and 133 con-
mance across the entire spectrum of intelligence in neurolo- trols (Egan et al. 2003), and 291 patients with PD (Foltynie
gically normal individuals has also been mustered (Jung et al. et al. 2005) both reported significant results, while the two
2000). As the number of discrete cognition-genotype correla- non-significant findings were obtained with sample sizes of
tions reported in the literature grows, so does the difficulty of less than 100 (see Table 4). This picture is more consistent
distinguishing between (a) false positives, (b) the potentially with a small but genuine effect of the val66met BDNF variant
pleiotropic influence of the polymorphism on multiple but on cognition.
discrete cognitive processes and (c) the possible effect of These crude estimates of publication bias are best
genotype on g – that is a higher-order over-arching cognitive assessed with a formal meta analysis or funnel plot. The
process. This can be seen in the data reviewed above. While difficulty in this field is the diversity of tasks used to measure
the COMT val158met variant has been reported to impact cognition. Different neuropsychological tasks vary in their
general executive function (Egan et al. 2001), working mem- properties and therefore exert a differential influence on
ory (Goldberg et al. 2003), working memory and inhibition effect size and study outcome. Secondly, results are differ-
(Diamond et al. 2004) and episodic and semantic memory (de entially reported across studies: for example, the homozy-
Frias et al. 2004), the BDNF val66met polymorphism has gote and heterozygote groups are sometimes grouped
been postulated to influence hippocampal function and together and sometimes considered separately in the statis-
memory (Egan et al. 2003; Hariri et al. 2003) and different tical analyses. Given the additional frustration that the data
components of executive function (Foltynie et al. 2005; required to calculate effect size and mean standard error are
Rybakowski et al. 2003). not always available, more formal estimates of publication
A related problem is that the use of neuropsychological bias are not presented here.
tests to pinpoint cognitive modules that are sensitive to (d) False-positive results due to lack of correction for multi-
genetic variation is limited by the absence of a simple linear ple testing is a potential problem when many different neu-
relationship between task performance and activation of a ropsychological measures are being used. Theoretically, the
particular neural circuit. This is because there are many rea- cut-off value for significance (a value) should be divided by
sons for failure on a neuropsychological task. Goldberg and the number of comparisons being made in order to avoid
Weinberger (2004) suggest that the use of an alternative Type I errors. Some researchers, however, contend that
phenotype, such as fMRI activity, which may be more clo- conservative corrections such as the Bonferonni method
sely related than psychological testing to the underlying are unnecessary when the research is driven by an a priori
function of the neural system, may ameliorate this difficulty. hypothesis and hampers the detection of weak effects. The
(b) The complexity of neuromolecular pathways makes it best approach is probably some kind of compromise
dangerous to draw simple causal relationships between a between the two extremes. Numerous multiple compar-
genetic variant and a cognitive process. The putative relation- isons will be problematic for the interpretation of results,
ship between, for example, the COMT val158met SNP and but unnecessarily strict methodological requirements will
working memory probably indicates that COMT is one cog in hamper the detection of weak effects.
a complex circuit involved in PFC function. Does the COMT
val158met variant exert a direct causal effect on PFC func-
tion, or is it simply correlated with a true causal factor such
The genetics of cognition: implications for
as the efficacy of DA receptor binding or the developmental
psychiatry
trajectory taken by the embryonic brain? The détente between the once disparate worlds of cognition
(c) If a true but weak relationship between COMT and and emotion research (see Damasio 1994; Panksepp 1998)
BDNF variants and cognitive function exists, then it would has coincided with the growing recognition that cognitive
be expected that a greater percentage of positive findings impairment is a central facet of many psychiatric disorders.
would have been derived from studies employing large num- Deficits of executive function, working memory and declara-
bers of participants. In fact, in the case of COMT, the pattern tive memory are widely reported in the schizophrenia litera-
is diametrical. Four of the largest studies with sample sizes ture (Hoff & Kremen 2003). Similarly, two recent reviews of
of 159 schizophrenics and 84 controls (Ho et al. 2005), 154 the neuropsychology of bipolar disorder concluded that
schizophrenics (Tsai et al. 2004), 543 healthy males (Stefanis executive dysfunction and verbal memory deficits are
et al. 2004) and 220 healthy adults (Fossella et al. 2002) failed pathognomonic of a bipolar diathesis (Glahn et al. 2004;
to show statistically significant gene-cognition associations. Savitz et al. 2005). Cognitive dysfunction has also been
On the other hand, 15 studies making use of sample sizes of reported in a multitude of other psychiatric conditions

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328 323


Savitz et al.

including ADHD (Barkley 1997), unipolar depression References


(Davidson et al. 2002), eating disorders (Lena et al. 2004),
Arnsten, A.F., Cai, J.X., Murphy, B.L. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S.
obsessive compulsive disorder (Shin et al. 2004) and border- (1994) Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive
line personality disorder (Monarch et al. 2004). performance of young adult and aged monkeys.
Given the extensive iterative connections between the Psychopharmacology 116, 143–151.
PFC and the limbic system, these data suggest that an occult Asghari, V., Sanyal, S., Buchwaldt, S., Paterson, A., Jovanovic, V.
biological anomaly in the aforementioned neural networks & Van Tol, H.H.M. (1995) Modulation of intracellular cyclic
AMP levels by different human dopamine receptor variants.
underpins both a disruption of cognition and affect. Another
J Neurochem 65, 1157–1165.
possibility is that a primary affective or cognitive disturbance Baldelli, P., Novara, M., Carabelli, V., Hernandez-Guijo, J.M. &
leads to dysfunction of the other modality. Regardless of Carbone, E. (2002) BDNF up-regulates evoked GABAergic
which possibility turns out to be correct, ‘cognitive genetics’, transmission in developing hippocampus by potentiating pre-
far from being an esoteric backwater of modern psychiatry, synaptic N- and P/Q-type Ca2þ channels signalling. Eur J
is most probably indispensable to a comprehensive account Neurosci 16, 2297–2310.
of both the aetiology and pathophysiology of psychiatric Barkley, R.A. (1997) Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention and
executive functions: constructing a theory of ADHD. Psychol
illness.
Bull 121, 65–94.
Bartres-Faz, D., Junque, C., Serra-Grabulosa, J.M., Lopez-
Alomar, A., Moya, A., Bargallo, N., Mercader, J.M., Moral, P.
& Clemente, I.C. (2002) Dopamine DRD2 Taq I polymorphism
Conclusion associates with caudate nucleus volume and cognitive
Traditional behavioural genetics methodologies have long performance in memory impaired subjects. Neuroreport 13,
suggested a role for genes in cognition, but it is only in the 1121–1125.
Bates, J.A., Goldman, D. & Malhotra, A.K. (2003) COMT and
past few years with the rapid evolution of molecular and
neurocognition: new evidence for a Role in visual memory.
computational technology for genetic analysis, coupled with Schiz Res 60, S123.
the extraordinary flow of data from the human genome pro- Bearden, C.E., Jawad, A.F., Lynch, D.R., Sokol, S., Kanes, S.J.,
ject, that identification of these genes has become a viable McDonald-McGinn, D.M., Saitta, S.C., Moss, E., Wang, P.P.,
proposition. One of the defining features of our species is a Zackai, E., Emanuel, B.S. & Simon, T.J. (2004) Effects of a
significantly enlarged neocortex and gyrified PFC, and this functional COMT polymorphism on prefrontal cognitive func-
tion in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Am J
together with a plethora of evidence implicating in particular,
Psychiatry 161, 1700–1702.
the PFC in cognition, suggests that the genes expressed in Bellgrove, M.A., Domschke, K., Hawi, Z., Kirley, A., Mullins, C.,
this part of the brain are good candidates for involvement in Roberts, I.H. & Gill, M. (2005) The methionine allele of the
cognitive function. COMT polymorphism impairs prefrontal cognition in children
One such of these candidates is COMT, an enzyme which and adolescents with ADHD. Exp Brain Res 163, 352–360.
influences the activity of DA in the PFC by virtue of its Benton, A.L. (1994) Neuropsychological assessment. Annu Rev
catabolic effects on the neurotransmitter. Given its pivotal Psychol 45, 1–23.
Berman, S.M. & Noble, E.P. (1995) Reduced visuospatial performance
role in the regulation of dopaminergic activity and the large
in children with the D2 dopamine receptor A1 allele. Behav
number of studies that have implicated the val158met SNP Genet 25, 45–58.
in cognition, we suggest that functional variation in this gene Bertolino, A., Caforio, G., Blasi, G., De Candia, M., Latorre, V.,
contributes to inter-individual differences in cognitive func- Petruzzella, V., Altamura, M., Nappi, G., Papa, S., Callicott, J.H.,
tion. The DA receptor genes, in particular D1, D2 and D4, are Mattay, V.S., Bellomo, A., Scarabino, T., Weinberger, D.R. &
all theoretically good candidates for a role in cognition, but Nardini, M. (2004) Interaction of COMT Val108/158Met
the number of genetic association studies carried out thus genotype and olanzapine treatment in prefrontal cortical
function in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 161,
far is too small to draw any firm conclusions. Another candi-
1798–1805.
date gene is BDNF which appears to exert a more diffuse Bilder, R.M., Volavka, J., Czobor, P., Malhotra, A.K., Kennedy, J.L.,
effect via its role in LTP and neuronal survival and repair. Ni, X., Golman, R.S., Hptman, M.J., Sheitman, B., Lindenmayer, J.P.,
Physiological, genetic and neuroimaging data all favour the Citrome, L., McEvoy, J.P., Kunz, M., Chakos, M., Cooper, T.B.
hypothesis that BDNF influences neurocognitive function. As & Lieberman, J.A. (2002) Neurocognitive correlates of the
is the case with COMT though, a resolution of the precise COMT Val158Met polymorphism in chronic schizophrenia. Biol
Psychiatry 52, 701–707.
cognitive processes regulated by BDNF remains elusive.
Bilder, R.M., Volavka, J., Lachman, H.M. & Grace, A.A. (2004)
The catechol-o-methyltransferase polymorphism: relations to
the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neuropsychiatric
phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1943–1961.
Acknowledgment Bouchard, T.J. Jr (1998) Genetic and environmental influences
The support of the Medical Research Council of South Africa is on adult intelligence and special mental abilities. Hum Biol 70,
acknowledged. 257–279.

324 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


The molecular genetics of cognition

Brozoski, T., Brown, R.M., Rosvold, H.E. & Goldman, P.S. (1979) D’Souza, U.M., Russ, C., Tahir, E., Mill, J., McGuffin, P.,
Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in the Asherson, P.J. & Craig, I.W. (2004) Functional Effects of a
prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. Science 205, 929–932. Tandem Duplication Polymorphism in the 50 Flanking Region
Castner, S.A., Williams, G.V. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (2000) of the DRD4 Gene. Biol Psychiatry 56, 691–697.
Reversal of antipsychotic-induced working memory deficits Egan, M.F., Goldberg, T.E., Kolachana, B.S., Callicott, J.H.,
by short-term D1 receptor stimulation. Science 287, Mazzanti, C.M., Straub, R.E., Goldman, D. & Weinberger, D.R.
2020–2022. (2001) Effect of COMT Val108/158Met genotype on frontal lobe
Celada, P., Siuciak, J.A., Tran, T.M., Altar, C.A. & Tepper, J.M. function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98,
(1996) Local infusion of brain-derived neurotrophic factor modi- 6917–6922.
fies the firing pattern of dorsal raphe serotonergic neurons. Egan, M.F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J.H., Goldberg, T.E.,
Brain Res 712, 293–298. Kolachana, B.S., Bertolino, A., Zaitsev, E., Gold, B.,
Constantinidis, C., Williams, G.V. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (2002) A Goldman, D., Dean, M., Lu, B. & Weinberger, D.R. (2003) The
role for inhibition in shaping the temporal flow of information in BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secre-
prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 5, 175–180. tion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell
Cools, R. & Robbins, T.W. (2004) Chemistry of the adaptive 112, 257–269.
mind. Philos Transact A Math Phys Eng Sci 362, 2871–2888. Egan, M.F., Straub, R.E., Goldberg, T.E., Yakub, I., Callicott, J.H.,
Cools, R., Barker, R.A., Sahakian, B.J. & Robbins, T.W. (2001) Hariri, A.R., Mattay, V.S., Bertolino, A., Hyde, T.M., Shannon-
Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson’s dis- Weickert, C., Akil, M., Crook, J., Vakkalanka, R.K., Balkissoon, R.,
ease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task Gibbs, R.A., Kleinman, J.E. & Weinberger, D.R. (2004)
demands. Cereb Cortex 11, 1136–1143. Variation in GRM3 affects cognition, prefrontal glutamate,
Cools, R., Stefanova, E., Barker, R.A., Robbins, T.W., and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101,
Owen, A.M. (2002) Dopaminergic modulation of high-level 12604–12609.
cognition in Parkinson’s disease: the role of the prefrontal Eisenberg, J., Mei-Tal, G., Steinberg, A., Tartakovsky, E.,
cortex revealed by PET. Brain 125, 584–594. Zohar, A., Gritsenko, I., Nemanov, L. & Ebstein, R.P. (1999)
Damasio, A.R. (1994) Descartes’ Error. Emotion, Reason and the Haplotype relative risk study of catechol-o-methyltransferase
Human Brain. Putnam, New York. (COMT) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Davidson, R.J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J.B. & Putnam, K. (2002) Am J Med Genet 88, 497–502.
Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annu El-Ghundi, M., Fletcher, P.J., Drago, J., Sibley, D.R., O’Dowd, B.F.,
Rev Psychol 53, 545–574. George, S.R. (1999) Spatial learning deficit in dopamine D (1)
Dempster, E., Toulopoulou, T., McDonald, C., Bramon, E., receptor knockout mice. Eur J Pharmacol 27, 95–106.
Walshe, M., Filbey, F., Wickham, H., Sham, P.C., Foltynie, T., Goldberg, T.E., Lewis, S.G.J., Blackwell, A.D.,
Murray, R.M. & Collier, D.A. (2005) Association between Kolachana, B.S., Weinberger, D.R., Robbins, T.W. &
BDNF val (66) met genotype and episodic memory. Am J Barker, R.A. (2004) Planning ability in parkinson’s disease is
Med Genet 134B, 73–75. influenced by the COMT Val158Met polymorphism. Mov
Deutch, A.Y. & Roth, R.H. (1999) Neurotransmitters. In Zigmond, M.J., Disord 19, 885–891.
Bloom, F.E., Landis, S.C., Roberts, J.L. & Squire, L.R. (eds), Foltynie, T., Lewis, S.G.J., Goldberg, T.E., Blackwell, A.D.,
Fundamental Neuroscience. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. Kolachana, B.S., Weinberger, D.R., Robbins, T.W. &
193–234. Barker, R.A. (in press) The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism
Diamond, A., Briand, L., Fossella, J. & Gehlbach, L. (2004) has a gender specific influence on planning ability in
Genetic and neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cognitive Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol.
functions in children. Am J Psychiatry 161, 125–132. Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Fan, J., Wu, Y., Swanson, J.M., Pfaff, D.W.
DiMaio, S., Grizenko, N. & Joober, R. (2003) Dopamine & Posner, M.I. (2002) Assessing the molecular genetics of
genes and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a review. attention networks. BMC Neurosci 3, 14–25.
J Psychiatry Neurosci 28, 27–38. de Frias, C.M., Annerbrink, K., Westberg, L., Eriksson, E.,
Ding, Y.C., Chi, H.C., Grady, D.L., Morishima, A., Kidd, J.R., Adolfsson, R. & Nilsson, L.G. (2004) COMT gene polymorph-
Kidd,K.K., Flodman, P., Spence, M.A., Schuck, S., Swanson, J.M., ism is associated with declarative memory in adulthood and
Zhang, Y.P. & Moyzis, R.K. (2002) Evidence of positive selec- old age. Behav Genet 34, 533–539.
tion acting at the human dopamine receptor D4 gene locus. Gallinat, J., Bajbouj, M., Sander, T., Sclattmann, P., Xu, K.,
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99, 309–314. Ferro, E.F., Goldman, D. & Winterer, G. (2003) Association of
Dougherty, D.D., Bonab, A.A., Spencer, T.J., Rauch, S.L., the G1947A COMT (Val158Met) gene polymorphism with pre-
Madras, B.K. & Fischman, A.J. (1999) Dopamine transporter fronal P300 during information processing. Biol Psychiatry 54,
density in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. 40–48.
Lancet 354, 2132–2133. Glahn, D.C., Bearden, C.E., Niendam, T.A. & Escamilla, M.A.
Duan, J., Wainwright, M.S., Comeron, J.M., Saitou, N., Sanders, A.R., (2004) The feasibility of neuropsychological endopenotypes in
Gelernter, J. & Gejman, P.V. (2003) Synonymous mutations the search for genes associated with bipolar affective disorder.
in the human dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) affect mRNA Bipolar Disord 6, 171–118.
stability and synthesis of the receptor. Hum Mol Genet 12, Glatt, C.E. & Freimer, N.B. (2002) Association analysis of candi-
205–216. date genes for neuropsychiatric disease: the perpetual cam-
Dubois, B. & Pillon, B. (1997) Cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s paign. Trends Genet 18, 307–313.
disease. J Neurol 244, 2–8. Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1995) Cellular basis of working memory.
Duman, R.S., Heninger, G.R. & Nestler, E.J. (1997) A molecular Neuron 14, 477–485.
and cellular theory of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54, Goldberg, T.E. & Weinberger, D.R. (2004) Genes and the parsing
597–606. of cognitive processes. Trends Cogn Sci 8, 325–335.

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328 325


Savitz et al.

Goldberg, T.E., Egan, M.F., Gscheidle, T., Coppola, R., Weickert, T., Kotecha, S.A., Oak, J.N., Jackson, M.F., Perez, Y., Orser, B.A.,
Kolachana, B.S., Goldman, D. & Weinberger, D.R. (2003) Van Tol, H.H. & MacDonald, J.F. (2002) A D2 class dopamine
Executive subprocesses in working memory. Arch Gen receptor transactivates a receptor tyrosine kinase to inhibit
Psychiatry 60, 889–896. NMDA receptor transmission. Neuron 35, 1111–1122.
Hariri, A.R., Goldberg, T.E., Mattay, V.S., Kolachana, B.S., Kustanovich, V., Ishii, J., Crawford, L., Yang, M., McGough, J.J.,
Callicott, J.H., Egan, M.F. & Weinberger, D.R. (2003) Brain- McCracken, J.T., Smalley, S.L. & Nelson, S.F. (2004)
derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects Transmission disequilibrium testing of dopamine-related
human memory-related hippocampal activity and predicts candidate gene polymorphisms in ADHD: confirmation of
memory performance. J Neurosci 23, 6690–6694. association of ADHD with DRD4 and DRD5. Mol Psychiatry
Hirvonen, M., Laakso, A., Nagren, K., Rinne, J.O., Pohjalainen, T. 9, 711–717.
& Hietala, J. (2004) C957T polymorphism of the dopamine D2 Lange, K.W., Loschmann, P.A., Wachtel, H., Horowski, R.,
receptor (DRD2) gene affects striatal DRD2 availability in vivo. Jahnig, P., Jenner, P. & Marsden, C.D. (1992) Terguride stimu-
Mol Psychiatry 9, 1060–1061. lates locomotor activity at 2 months but not 10 months after 1-
Ho, B.C., Wassink, T.H., O’Leary, D.S., Sheffield, V.C. & Methyl-4-phyl-2, 3, 6-tetrahydropyridine treatment of common
Andreasen, N.C. (in press) Catechol-O-methyl transferase marmosets. Eur J Pharmacol 212, 247–252.
Val158Met gene polymorphism in schizophrenia: working Langley, K., Marshall, L., van den Bree, M., Thomas, H., Owen, M.,
memory, frontal lobe MRI morphology and frontal cerebral O’Donovan, M. & Thapar, A. (2004) Association of the dopa-
blood flow. Mol Psychiatry 10, 229–235. mine D4 receptor gene 7-repeat allele with neuropsychological
Hoff, A.L. & Kremen, W.S. (2003) Neuropsychology in schizo- test performance of children with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 161,
phrenia: an update. Curr Opin Psychiatry 16, 149–155. 133–138.
Horger, B.A., Iyasere, C.A., Berhow, M.T., Messer, C.J., Lena, S.M., Fiocco, A.J. & Leyenaar, J.K. (2004) The role of
Nestler, E.J. & Taylor, J.R. (1999) Enhancement of locomotor cognitive deficits in the development of eating disorders.
activity and conditioned reward to cocaine by brain-derived Neuropsychol Rev 14, 99–113.
neurotrophic factor. J Neurosci 19, 4110–4122. Lezak, M.D. (1995) Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford
Hung, H.C. & Lee, E.H. (1996) The mesolimbic dopaminergic University Press, New York.
pathway is more resistant than the nigrostriatal dopaminergic Lidow, M.S., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., Gallagher, D.W. & Rakic, P.
pathway to MPTP and MPPþ toxicity: role of BDNF expres- (1991) Distribution of dopaminergic receptors in the primate
sion. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 41, 14–26. cerebral cortex: quantitative autoradiographic analysis using
Hyman, C., Juhasz, M., Jackson, C., Wright, P., Ip, N.Y. & [3H]spiperone and [3H]SCH23390. Neuroscience 40, 657–671.
Lindsay, R.M. (1994) Overlapping and distinct actions of the Luciana, M., Collins, P.F. & Depue, R.A. (1998) Opposing roles
neurotrophins BDNF, NT-3, and NT-4/5 on cultured dopaminer- for dopamine and serotonin in the modulation of human spatial
gic and GABAergic neurons of the ventral mesencephalon. J working memory functions. Cereb Cortex 8, 218–226.
Neurosci 14, 335–347. Luciano, M., Wright, M.J., Smith, G.A., Geffen, G.M.,
Illi, A., Mattila, K.M., Kampman, O., Anttila, S., Roivas, M., Geffen, L.B. & Martin, N.G. (2001) Genetic covariance among
Lehtimaki, T. & Leinonen, E. (2003) Catechol-O-methyltrans- measures of information processing speed, working memory,
ferase and monoamine oxidase a genotypes and drug and IQ. Behav Genet 31, 581–592.
response to conventional neuroleptics in schizophrenia. J Clin Malhotra, A.K., Kestler, L.J., Mazzanti, C., Bates, J.A.,
Psychopharmacol 23, 429–434. Goldberg, T. & Goldman, D. (2002) A functional polymorphism
Inada, T., Nakamura, A. & Iijima, Y. (2003) Relationship between in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal
catechol-o-methyltransferase polymorphism and treatment- cognition. Am J Psychiatry 159, 652–654.
resistant schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet 120B, 35–39. Mamounas, L.A., Blue, M.E., Siuciak, J.A. & Altar, C.A. (1995) Brain
Jiang, X., Xu, K., Hoberman, J., Tian, F., Marko, A.J., Waheed, J.F., derived neurotrophic factor promotes the survival and sprouting
Harris, C.R., Marinia, A.M., Enoch, M.A. & Lipsky, R.H. (in press) of serotonergic axons in the rat brain. J Neurosci 15, 7929–7939.
BDNF variation and mood disorders: a novel functional promoter Manor, I., Tyano, S., Eisenberg, J., Bachner-Melman, R.,
ploymorphism and val66met are associated with anxiety but have Kotler, M. & Ebstein, R.P. (2002) The short DRD4 repeats
opposing effects. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 1353–1361. confer risk to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a
Johnson, M.H. (1997) Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. family-based design and impair performance on a continuous
Blackwell, Oxford. performance task (TOVA). Mol Psychiatry 7, 790–794.
Joober, R., Gauthier, J., Lal, S., Bloom, D., Lalonde, P., Mattay, V.S., Callicott, J.H., Bertolino, A., Heaton, I., Frank, J.A.,
Rouleau, G., Benkelfat, C. & Labelle, A. (2002) Catechol-O- Coppola, R., Berman, K.F., Goldberg, T.E. & Weinberger, D.R.
methyltransferase Val-108/158-Met gene variants associated (2000) Effects of dextroamphetamine on cognitive perform-
with performance on the wisconsin card sorting task. Arch ance and cortical activation. Neuroimage 12, 268–275.
General Psychiatry 59, 662–663. Mattay, V.S., Tessitore, A., Callicott, J.H., Bertolino, A.,
Jung, R.E., Yeo, R.A., Chiulli, S.J., Sibbitt,W.L. & JrBrooks, W.M. Goldberg, T.E., Chase, T.N., Hyde, T.M., Weinberger, D.R.
(2000) Myths of neuropsychology: intelligence, neurometabol- (2002) Dopaminergic modulation of cortical function in patients
ism, and cognitive ability. Clin Neuropsychol 14, 535–545. with Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 51, 156–174.
Kimberg, D.Y., D’Esposito, M. & Farah, M.J. (1997) Effects of Mattay, V.S., Goldberg, T.E., Fera, F., Hariri, A.R., Tessitore, A.,
bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory Egan, M.F., Kolachana, B., Callicott, J.H. & Weinberger, D.R.
capacity. Neuroreport 8, 3581–3585. (2003) Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype
Kodoma, T., Hikosaka, K. & Watanabe, M. (1997) Differential and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine.
changes in glutamate concentration in the primate prefrontal Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100, 6186–6191.
cortex during delayed spatial alteration and sensory-guided Mehta, M.A., Sahakian, B.J., McKenna, P.J. & Robbins, T.W.
tasks. Exp Brain Res 145, 133–141. (1999) Systemic sulpiride in young adult volunteers simulates

326 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328


The molecular genetics of cognition

the profile of cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Ollat, H. (1992) Dopaminergic insufficiency reflecting cerebral
Psychopharmacology 146, 162–174. ageing: value of a dopaminergic agonist, piribedil. J Neurol
Mehta, M.A., Manes, F.F., Magnolfi, G., Sahakian, B.J. & 239, S13–S16.
Robbins, T.W. (2004) Impaired set-shifting and dissociable Pang, P.T. & Lu, B. (2004) Regulation of late-phase LTP and long-
effects on tests of spatial working memory following the term memory in normal and aging hippocampus: role of
dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride in human volun- secreted proteins tPA and BDNF. Ageing Res Rev 3, 407–430.
teers. Psychopharmacology 176, 331–342. Panksepp, J. (1998) Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of
Mills, S., Langley, K., Van den Bree, M., Street, E., Turic, D., Human and Animal Emotions. Oxford University Press, New
Owen, M.J., O’Donovan, M.C. & Thapar, A. (2004) No evi- York.
dence of association between Catechol-O-methyltransferase Peretti, C.S., Gierski, F. & Harrois, S. (2004) Cognitive skill learn-
(COMT) Val158Met genotype and performance on neuropsy- ing in healthy older adults after 2 months of double-blind
chological tasks in children with ADHD: a case-control study. treatment with piribedil. Psychopharmacology 17, 175–181.
BMC Psychiatry 4, 15–20. Petrill, S.A., Plomin, R.A., McClearn, G.E., Smith, D.L., Vignetti, S.,
Minichiello, L., Korte, M., Wolfer, D., Kuhn, R., Unsicker, K., Chorney, M.J., Chorney, K., Thompson, L.A., Detterman, D.K.,
Cestari, M.V., Rossi-Arnaud, C., Lipp, H.P., Bonhoeffer, T. & Benbow, C., Lubinski, D., Daniels, J., Owen, M. & McGuffin, P.
Klein, R. (1999) Essential role for TrkB receptors in hippocam- (1997) No association between general cognitive ability and
pus-mediated learning. Neuron 24, 401–414. the A1 allele of the D2 dopamine receptor gene. Behav Genet
Monarch, E.S., Saykin, A.J. & Flashman, L.A. (2004) 27, 29–31.
Neuropsychological impairment in borderline personality disor- Pezawas, L., Verchinski, B.A., Mattay, V.S., Callicott, J.H.,
der. Psychiatr Clin North Am 27, 67–82. Kolachana, B.S., Straub, R.E., Egan, M.F., Meyer-Lindenberg, A.
Mossner, R., Daniel, S., Albert, D., Heils, A., Okladnova, O., & Weinberger, D.R. (2004) The brain-derived neurotrophic fac-
Schmitt, A. & Lesch, K.P. (2000) Serotonin transporter function tor val66met polymorphism and variation in human cortical
is modulated by brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) but morphology. J Neurosci 24, 10099–10102.
not nerve growth factor (NGF). Neurochem Int 36, 197–202. Plotkin, H. (1997) Evolution in Mind: An Introduction to
Mu, J.S., Li, W.P., Yao, Z.B. & Zhou, X.F. (1999) Deprivation of Evolutionary Psychology. Penguin, London.
endogenous brain-derived neurotrophic factor results in impair- Pohjalainen T., Rinne J.O., Nagren K., Lehikoinen P., Anttila K.,
ment of spatial learning and memory in adult rats. Brain Res Syvalahti E.K., Hietala J. (1998) The A1 allele of the human D2
835, 259–265. dopamine receptor gene predicts low D2 receptor availability
Muller, U., von Cramon, D.Y. & Pollmann, S. (1998) D1 versus D2 in healthy volunteers. Mol Psychiatry 3, 256–260.
receptor modulation of visuospatial working memory in Poo, M. (2001) Neurotrophins as synaptic modulators. Nat Rev
humans. J Neurosci 18, 2720–2728. Neurosci 2, 24–32.
Nacmias, B., Piccini, C., Bagnoli, S., Tedde, A., Cellini, E., Bracco, L. Pozzo-Miller, L.D., Gottschalk, W., Zhang, L., Du McDermott, K.J.,
& Sorbi, S. (2004) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor, apolipo- Gopalakrishnan, R., Oho, C., Sheng, Z. & Lu, B. (1999)
protein E genetic variants and cognitive performance in Impairments in high-frequency transmission, synaptic vesicle
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 367, 379–383. docking, and synaptic protein distribution in the hippocampus
Narita, M., Aoki, K., Takagi, M., Yajima, Y. & Suzuki, T. (2003) of BDNF knockout mice. J Neurosci 19, 4972–4983.
Implication of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the release Previc, F.H. (1999) Dopamine and the origins of human intelli-
of dopamine and dopamine-related behaviors induced by gence. Brain Cogn 41, 299–350.
amphetamine. Neuroscience 119, 767–775. Remy, P. & Samson, Y. (2003) The role of dopamine in cognition:
Nestler, E.J., Barrot, M., DiLeone, R.J., Eisch, A.J., Gold, S.J. & evidence from functional imaging studies. Curr Opin Neurol
Monteggia, L.M. (2002) Neurobiology of depression. Neuron 16, S37–S41.
34, 13–25. Rilling, J.K. & Insel, T.R. (1999) The primate neocortex in
Noble, E.P., Berman, S.M., Ozkaragoz, T.Z. & Ritchie, T. (1994) comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging.
Prolonged P300 latency in children with the D2 dopamine J Hum Evol 37, 191–223.
receptor A1 allele. Am J Hum Genet 54, 658–668. Rogers, G., Joyce, P., Mulder, R., Sellman, D., Miller, A.,
Noble, E.P. (2000) The DRD2 gene in psychiatric and neurological Allington, M., Olds, R., Wells, E., Kennedy, M. (2004)
disorders and its phenotypes. Pharmacogenomics 1, 309–333. Association of a duplicated repeat polymorphism in the 50
Nolan, K.A., Bilder, R.M., Lachman, H.M. & Volavka, J. (2004) untranslated region of the DRD4 gene with novelty seeking.
Catechol O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism in Am J Med Genet 126B, 95–98.
schizophrenia: differential effects of val and met alleles on Rosa, A., Peralta, V., Cuesta, M.J., Zarzuela, A., Serrano, F.,
cognitive stability and flexibility. Am J Psychiatry 161, Martinez-Larrea, A. & Fananas, L. (2004) New evidence of
359–361. association between COMT gene and prefrontal neurocogni-
Okubo, Y., Suhara, T., Suzuki, K., Kobayashi, K., Inoue, O., tive function in healthy individuals from sibling pairs discordant
Terasaki, O., Someya, Y., Sassa, T., Sudo, Y., Matsushima, for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 161, 1110–1112.
E., Iyo, M., Tateno, Y., Toru, M. (1997) Decreased prefrontal Rybakowski, J.K., Borkowska, A., Czerski, P.M., Skibinska, M. &
dopamine D1 receptors in schizophrenia revealed by PET. Hauser, J. (2003) Polymorphism of the brain-derived neuro-
Nature 385, 634–636. trophic factor gene and performance on a cognitive prefrontal
Okuyama, Y., Ishiguro, H., Toru, M. & Arinami, T. (1999) A test in bipolar patients. Bipolar Disord 5, 468–472.
genetic polymorphism in the promoter region of DRD4 asso- Savitz, J.B. & Ramesar, R.S. (2004) Genetic variants implicated in
ciated with expression and schizophrenia. Biochem Biophys personality: a review of the more promising candidates. Am J
Res Commun 258, 292–295. Med Genet 131B, 20–32.

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328 327


Savitz et al.

Savitz, J., Solms, M. & Ramesar, R. (2005) Neuropsychological & Joober, R. (2004) Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT)
dysfunction in bipolar affective disorder: a critical opinion. Val108/158 Met polymorphism does not modulate
Bipolar Disord 7, 216–235. executive function in children with ADHD. BMC Med Genet
Segalowitz, S.J. & Davies, P.L. (2004) Charting the maturation of 5, 30–38.
the frontal lobe: an electrophysiological strategy. Brain Cogn Tsai, S.J., Yu Y.W.Y., Lin, C., Chen, T., Chen, S. & Hong, C.
55, 116–133. (2002) Dopamine D2 receptor and N-methyl-D-aspartate
Shin, M.S., Park, S.J., Kim, M.S., Lee, Y.H., Ha, T.H. & Kwon, J.S. receptor 2B subunit genetic variants and intelligence.
(2004) Deficits of organisational strategy and visual memory in Neuropsychobiology 45, 128–130.
obsessive compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology 18, 665–672. Tsai, S.J., Yu, Y.W.Y., Chen, T.J., Chen, J.Y., Liou, Y.J., Chen, M.C.
Siuciak, J.A., Boylan, C., Fritsche, M., Altar, C.A. & Lindsay, R.M. & Hong, C.J. (2003) Association study of a functional catechol-O-
(1996) BDNF increases monoaminergic activity in rat brain methyltransferase gene polymorphism and cognitive function in
following intracerebroventricular or intraparenchymal adminis- healthy females. Neurosci Lett 338, 123–126.
tration. Brain Res 710, 11–20. Tsai, S.J., Hong, C.J., Liao, D.L., Lai, I.C. & Liou, Y.J. (2004a)
Spearman, C. (1904) General intelligence, objectively determined Association study of a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase
and measured. Am J Psychol 15, 201–293. genetic polymorphism with age of onset, cognitive function,
Stefanis, N.C., Van Os, J., Avramopolous, D., Smynis, N., symptomatology and prognosis in chronic schizophrenia.
Evdokimidis, I., Hantoumi, I. & Stefanis, C.N. (2004) Variation Neuropsychobiology 49, 196–200.
in catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met genotype asso- Tsai, S.J., Hong, C.J., Yu, Y.W.Y. & Chen, T.J. (2004b)
ciated with schizotypy but not cognition: a population study Association study of a brain-derived neurotrophic factor
in 543 young men. Biol Psychiatry 56, 510–515. (BDNF) Val66Met polymorphism and personality trait and
Stern, Y. & Langston, J.W. (1985) Intellectual changes in patient intelligence in healthy young females. Neuropsychobiology
with MPTP-induced parkinsonism. Neurology 35, 1506–1509. 49, 13–16.
Strauss, J., Barr, C.L., George, C.J., Ryan, C.M., King, N., Wang, X., Zhong, P. & Yan, Z. (2002) Dopamine D4 receptors
Shaikh, S. & Kennedy, J.L. (2004) BDNF and COMT poly- modulate GABAergic signalling in pyramidal neurons of the
morphisms: relation to memory phenotypes in young adults prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 22, 9185–9193.
with childhood-onset mood disorder. Neuromolecular Med 5, Weickert, T.W., Goldberg, T.E., Mishara, A., Apud, J.A., Kolachana, B.S.,
181–192. Egan, M.F. & Weinberger, D.R. (2004) Catechol-O-methyltransferase
Swanson, J.M., Oosterlaan, J., Murias, M. et al. (2000) ADHD Val108/158Met genotype predicts working memory response to
children with a 7-repeat allele of the dopamine receptor D4 antipsychotic medications. Biol Psychiatry 56, 677–682.
gene have extreme behaviour but normal performance on Weinberger, D.R., Egan, M.F., Bertolino, A., Callicott, J.H.,
critical neuropsychological tests of attention. Proc Natl Acad Mattay, V.S., Lipska, B.K., Berman, K.F. & Goldberg, T.E.
Sci USA 97, 4754–4759. (2001) Prefrontal neurons and the genetics of schizophrenia.
Szeszko, P.R., Lipsky, R., Mentschel, C., Robinson, D., Gunduz- Biol Psychiatry 50, 825–844.
Bruce, H., Sevy, S., Ashtari, M., Napolitano, B., Bilder, R.M., Welsh, M.C., Pennington, B.F. & Groisser, D.B. (1991) A norma-
Kane, J.M., Goldman, D. & Malhotra, A.K. (in press) Brain- tive developmental study of executive function: a window on
derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and prefrontal function in children. Dev Neuropsychol 7, 131–149.
volume of the hippocampal formation. Mol Psychiatry. Winterer, G. & Weinberger, D.R. (2004) Genes, dopamine and
Taerk, E., Grizenko, N., Ben Amor, L., Lageix, P., Mbekou, V., cortical signal-to-noise ratio in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci
Deguzman, R., Torkaman-Zehi, A., Ter Stepanian, M., Baron, C. 27, 683–690.

328 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2006) 5: 311–328

View publication stats

You might also like