Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 28

Children, Youth, and Disaster

Children, Youth, and Disaster  


Alice Fothergill
Subject: Vulnerability, Exposed Populations Online Publication Date: Jul 2017
DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780199389407.013.23

Summary and Keywords

Children and youth are greatly affected by disasters, and as climate instability leads to
more weather-related disasters, the risks to the youngest members of societies will con­
tinue to increase. Children are more likely to live in risky places, such as floodplains,
coastal areas, and earthquake zones, and more likely to be poor than other groups of peo­
ple. While children and youth in industrialized countries are experiencing increased risks,
the children and youth in developing countries are the most at risk to disasters.

Children and youth are vulnerable before, during, and after a disaster. In a disaster, many
children and youth experience simultaneous and ongoing disruptions in their families,
schooling, housing, health and access to healthcare, friendships, and other key areas of
their lives. Many are at risk to separation from guardians, long-term displacement, injury,
illness, and even death. In disaster planning, there is often an assumption that parents
will protect their children in a disaster event, and yet children are often separated from
their parents when they are at school, childcare centers, home alone, with friends, and at
work. Children do not have the resources or independence to prepare for disasters, so
they are often reliant on adults to make evacuation decisions, secure shelter, and provide
resources. Children also may hide or have trouble articulating their distress to adults af­
ter a disaster. In the disaster aftermath, it has been found that children and youth—no
matter how personally resilient—cannot fully recover without the necessary resources
and social support.

Social location—such as social class, race, gender, neighborhood, resources, and net­
works—prior to a disaster often determines, at least in part, many of the children’s post-
disaster outcomes. In other words, age intersects with many other factors. Girls, for ex­
ample, are at risk to sexual violence and exploitation in some disaster aftermath situa­
tions. In addition, a child’s experience in a disaster could also be affected by language,
type of housing, immigration status, legal status, and disability issues. Those living in
poverty have more difficulties preparing for disasters, do not have the resources to evacu­
ate, and live in lower quality housing that is less able to withstand a disaster. Thus, it is
crucial to consider the child’s environment before and after the disaster, to realize that
some children experience cumulative vulnerability, or an accumulation of risk factors, and

Page 1 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

that disasters may occur on top of other crises, such as drought, epidemics, political in­
stability, violence, or a family crisis such as divorce or death.

Even as children and youth are vulnerable, they also demonstrate important and often un­
noticed capacities, skills, and strengths, as they assist themselves and others before and
after disaster strikes. Frequently, children are portrayed as helpless, fragile, passive, and
powerless. But children and youth are creative social beings and active agents, and they
have played important roles in preparedness activities and recovery for their families and
communities. Thus, both children’s vulnerabilities and capacities in disasters should be a
research and policy priority.

Keywords: children, youth, disaster, vulnerability, resilience, recovery, preparedness, risk

Introduction
The past decade has seen an increased interest in and a surge of new research conducted
on the topic of children, youth, and disaster. This is a welcome change. Prior to this new
attention, the topic had not been widely researched by social scientists and had not been
a top research or policy priority, in the United States and globally.

In 2005, sociologist William Anderson published an influential essay on children and dis­
aster. In it, he lamented the lack of social science research and attention to children and
disaster. He stated clearly: “A simple question can be raised. Where are children and
youths in social science disaster research?” He noted that there was little mention of chil­
dren in the hazard field summary volumes, known as the “First Assessment” and “Second
Assessment” (Mileti, 1999) and only discussed in Drabek’s (1986) earlier summary of the
disaster field in a mental health context. Anderson continued:

Thus there is a serious need to find a place for children and youths on the disaster
research agenda and to advance knowledge about this segment of the population.
Such knowledge would provide a more complete understanding of the impact of
hazards and disasters on society across the board and result in a firmer basis for
policy and practice. Disaster social scientists should be more committed to deter­
mining the extent to which such social factors as age influence vulnerability and
disaster outcomes . . . The knowledge base on children and disasters is so thin
that studies related to children in this context are needed across the entire mitiga­
tion, preparedness, and response and recovery spectrum.

(pp. 161–162)

As Anderson mentioned, the work that had been done had been on this topic had mostly
been in a mental health context. Indeed, most of the research that was conducted in the
20th century on children and disaster was in the field of psychology and focused on the
psychosocial impacts on children in disasters. Many of these important studies focused on
post-traumatic stress disaster (PTSD), were done in the short term of disaster aftermaths,

Page 2 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

and were conducted at one point in time. This mental health research played a key role in
the establishment of the field.

After Anderson’s call for attention to this knowledge gap, the field slowly but steadily be­
gan to focus on children and youth in disasters beyond the psychological lens. In the
same year, scholars in New Zealand posited that youth and schools play a large role in
community resilience and that collaborative partnerships need to be developed (Ronan &
Johnston, 2005). In 2006, Wisner issued a report on children, education, and disaster risk
reduction from a global perspective, looking at countries around the globe and what they
were doing in the areas of hazards education and risk reduction. Shortly after, there was
a special issue on children and disaster published in the journal Children, Youth and Envi­
ronment in 2008, and there were notable summary articles on age and vulnerability in the
Social Vulnerability to Disasters textbooks (Peek, 2010, 2013) which covered children as
well as the elderly.

Indeed, in the time since Anderson’s call for more research, especially in the last decade,
the social science field has responded with a surge of new research, varied in methods
and contexts. There has been important research and reports on how children factor into
efforts for Disaster Risk Reduction (DRR) and Climate Change Adaptation (CCA), as well
as on projects on youth resilience. There have also been a range of methodologies used,
both qualitative and quantitative, including focus groups, interviews, and community par­
ticipatory research; and a commitment to put children at the center of the research. Addi­
tionally there has been critical policy work and we have seen important new initiatives,
such as the Global Alliance for Disaster Risk Reduction and Resilience in the Education
Sector’s comprehensive school safety framework. The entire field of children and disaster
has evolved dramatically in a short time frame, pushing new ideas in research and policy
to the forefront at the same time children around the globe are facing new challenges
rooted in climate instability.

Children, Youth, and Disaster in the Disaster


Life Cycle
This article will present some of the main findings and important issues on how children
and youth experience disasters. This is not an exhaustive review of all literature on the
topic; instead, this discussion aims to present a bird’s eye view of what is known in this
area and some of the central questions, ideas, and gaps in this area, highlighting some of
the research that has been conducted. An eight-stage typology will be used to organize
the findings.1 This typology has been used previously to organize research reviews on is­
sues of vulnerability, inequality, and disasters—specifically gender (Fothergill, 1996), race
and ethnicity (Fothergill, Maestas, & Darlington, 1999), and income inequality and pover­
ty (Fothergill & Peek, 2004). Other disaster scholars also choose to present findings using
similar “disaster life cycle” frameworks (Phillips, Thomas, Fothergill, & Blinn-Pike, 2010;
Thomas, Phillips, Lovekamp, & Fothergill, 2013).

Page 3 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

The findings are delineated into the following categories: Risk Perception/Understanding
of Hazards, Preparedness, Warning Communication and Response, Physical Impacts, Psy­
chological Impacts, Emergency Response, Recovery, and Reconstruction/Risk Reduction.
While it is clear that people’s experiences, and the research conducted on them, do not
always fall neatly into “disaster life cycle” categories, this typology has been found in the
past to be useful to organize and make sense of a wide range of studies that use a variety
of methods and have a wide range of scope.

Figure 1. Nepal, February 2016. Children helping


with recovery from the 2015 earthquake.

Photo courtesy of Alice Fothergill.

Children and youth’s capacities and strengths will also be discussed in each section.
Children’s capacities in the disaster life cycle have been largely overlooked until recently.
At the same time that children and youth are vulnerable, they also demonstrate important
and often unnoticed capacities and strengths, as they assist themselves and others before
and after a disaster strikes. Children can play an active role in all stages. Nepalese chil­
dren, for example, helped during the recovery and reconstruction periods in their vil­
lages, such as in the village of Bungamati outside of Kathmandu (see Figure 1). As the re­
search on children and youth in disaster has progressed, there has been increasing atten­
tion paid to how children are not just helpless victims, but also active agents who could
be a valuable part of risk reduction and recovery. This attention and recognition is part of
a wider understanding in the sociology of childhood and childhood studies that see chil­
dren as active agents. They are able to make important contributions and create peer cul­
tures of their own, as posited by the childhood sociologist William Corsaro in his work on
children (2011). Thus, when data are available, there will be a discussion of the capaci­
ties of children and youth in each section.

Risk Perception/Understanding of Hazards

It is important to understand how children perceive the risk of a hazard in their environ­
ment and how it is that children learn and understand the risks facing their families and
communities. Who or what is shaping those perceptions? What are the factors that play a

Page 4 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

role in that perception? How does age interact with gender and other factors to form
their risk perception? It is also critical to understand how children and youth’s perception
and understanding of hazards in their communities vary greatly depending on cultural,
environmental, political, and economic factors.

Adults sometimes base their judgments of risks based on their own past experiences;
would this be a less influential factor for children since they have fewer past experiences
from which to draw? How would age affect the reception and personalization process?
Would children be more susceptible to the “heuristic of availability” and think of things as
risky that are easy to imagine because of images in social media? Or do children feel that
adults will protect them from harm, so they view hazards as less threatening? Are chil­
dren and youth more or less likely to trust the risk information source than adults? We do
not yet know the answers to these questions.

Perhaps children perceive threats and risks of hazards in the same way as their parents.
This seems logical, considering what we know about socialization and the way in which
children learn attitudes, beliefs, and values from their parents. And yet, children and
youth spend a good deal of time away from their parents—in school, childcare centers,
with friends—and create their own peer cultures and learn from other socialization
agents, such as teachers and the media. Furthermore, children have less control over
their lives and less power in general than adults and that might influence what seems
“risky” or not; we have seen in past research that low-income individuals have height­
ened levels of risk perception possibly for this reason.

Some research challenges the notion that children and adults share identical perceptions
of risk. Based on their research on El Salvador and the Philippines, Mitchell, Tanner, and
Haynes’s (2009) findings suggest that children have substantially different views on risk
than their parents. They found that children showed more concern about high magnitude,
low frequency events, while their parents perceived low magnitude, high frequency as a
greater worry. In thinking of capacities, the researchers state that if children and youth
are able to address the low magnitude, high frequency events in their communities, it
gives them greater confidence in their abilities and allows them to see themselves as
agents of change. In addition, they argue that in El Salvador they found that children
have a “clear and uncluttered view about risks” (p. 263), and take an all-risk approach; in
their lists of hazards, the children included environmental hazards as well as rape, alco­
holism, and not receiving enough love and care. In Indonesia, children created a more ex­
tensive list of disaster risk than adults, demonstrating an understanding of a broader
range of risks, and they were better able to recall disaster events that took place in their
lifetime than their parents were (Haynes, Lassa, & Towers, 2010).

Babugura (2008) found that children in Botswana learned about the risk of drought from
school, their parents, and their friends. She found that their perceptions were mostly
based in scientific knowledge and were generally well-informed. Children in many com­
munities, it appears, learn a lot about hazards in the school setting, and in some cases
pass that information on o their families. In research on Zimbabwe by Mudavanhu and

Page 5 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

colleagues (2015), children were found to be effective risk communicators; adults report­
ed that they got their risk information from their school-age children. In addition to pass­
ing information on to their parents, they distributed disaster pamphlets to their communi­
ties and helped with the distribution of chlorine tablets during a cholera outbreak. Wisner
(2006) argues that children should learn about risks in their communities and schools by
doing hands-on curriculum in their schools. This, he states, should include children and
youth inspecting their own schools, mapping out the area, and interviewing elders about
past extreme natural events. Many others have also pointed out the effectiveness of haz­
ard mapping exercises with children.

Past research has found that there are gender differences in risk perception for adults, so
it may be that those gender differences also are present for children and youth. Women,
for example, are more likely to perceive a disaster event or threat as risky or threatening
than men, and they are particularly likely to perceive a risk as more threatening if it af­
fects their children. Some of this thinking suggests that men and adolescent boys might
have a tendency to be more risk taking, and women and girls may be more risk averse;
but this may be context and culturally specific and should not be accepted out of hand. In
a study in Indonesia, gender differences in risk perception were found when children
were asked for their risk priorities. Girls ranked landslides as the top risk priority be­
cause they would cause the most damage, while boys ranked flooding as the top priority
because they could deal with that hazard more easily (Haynes et al., 2010).

Another issue for consideration is whether children and youth should be shielded from in­
formation about risks. It is understandable that adults do not want to scare children with
information about disasters that may or may not occur; yet, research finds that the infor­
mation is not frightening or paralyzing, depending on how it is presented. It could even
be empowering to learn about risks and then how they can be mitigated. Children, for ex­
ample, are taught risks of cars and bicycles, and can even be the family enforcers of seat
belts and helmets, and about diseases transmitted from mosquitos, and learn to mitigate
that risk without producing paralyzing fear. The amount of information about particular
hazards that is shared with children, depending on the context, might depend on the
child’s age and culture. Taken as a whole, it is clear that we have made some progress re­
cently on this topic, but it is crucial that we learn more about this in the future.

Preparedness Behavior

Preparedness behavior is a broad category and includes activities such as identifying


evacuation routes, performing safety drills, mitigating homes and schools, gathering and
storing emergency supplies, being trained for disaster response, and working on disaster
risk reduction projects. Some argue that children are not included in many preparedness
activities, they are not in emergency planning groups, and do not make decisions about
how to prepare (see, for example, Mort, Walker, Williams, Bingley, & Howells, 2016). Oth­
ers point out that preparedness programs to reduce the vulnerability of children are tak­
ing place in many countries around the globe, and how children and youth can and should
have a significant role in preparedness (see for example Mitchell et al., 2009). In the Unit­

Page 6 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

ed States, for example, there are programs at the state and federal level, and the Federal
Emergency Management Agency (FEMA) has designed preparedness materials for teach­
ers and children and has resources accessible on their website.

A cornerstone of preparedness is making sure children have safe, sound school struc­
tures. Shaw and Kobayashi (2001) argue for the importance of the earthquake-safer con­
struction of schools, such as retrofitting, but they also argue for this to happen in a par­
ticipatory way making sure local communities and governments are involved. The School
Earthquake Safety Initiative, a program by the United Nations, promotes self-help, coop­
eration, and education as the core of disaster mitigation. In Nepal, Indonesia, India, and
Uzbekistan, they found that disaster education in schools can create a culture of preven­
tion and mitigation throughout the community. Safer schools save lives, and also can
serve as temporary shelters after an earthquake or other disaster. Shaw and Kobayashi
(2001) like many other scholars around the world, are concerned with schools that are
built rapidly without proper seismic design. They found that in Kathmandu Valley in
Nepal that the majority of the schools needed retrofitting and none of the public schools
had any emergency response plans.

There has been some attention paid to school-age children and preparedness. Re­
searchers Johnston, Ronan, and Standring (2014), evaluated the effectiveness of disaster
preparedness education in New Zealand’s schools and examined the implementation of
the national teaching resource “What’s the Plan, Stan?” designed for ages 7–12. Ronan,
Alisic, Towers, Johnson, and Johnston (2015) conducted comprehensive, critical review of
disaster preparedness programs for children, and they found that many existing pro­
grams had positive effects on various risk reduction and resilience preparedness indica­
tors, but they feel more research is needed. One recommendation for New Zealand was
the establishment of a National School Earthquake Exercise Day. Tipler, Tarrant, Tuffin,
and Johnston (2016) found that legislation for preparedness in New Zealand schools was
ambiguous and that schools needed more clear disaster risk reduction guidance and
emergency preparedness benchmarks. In some villages in India, children are trained in
first aid, search and rescue, and warnings, using role-playing, posters, mapping activities,
mock drills, and making lists of all children in each village. In fact, the children’s favorite
class was learning to bandage the arms, faces, and legs of their friends. Also, teams of
children are chosen to be trained to broadcast warnings and guide people into cyclone
structures (Nikku, Karkara, & Ahmed, 2006).

There is very little research on preschool age children, such as those in childcare set­
tings, and preparedness. Large numbers of children attend childcare centers and in-home
childcare programs and it is unclear if childcare settings are prepared for disasters. In
Colorado, a study by Prelog and colleagues (2014) found that while 93% of childcare
providers in that state have a disaster preparedness plan, 83% of them have no budget
for disaster preparedness activities. In Florida, research found that many childcare cen­
ters at risk to hurricanes were not well prepared; many did not have a written hurricane
response plan (Wilson & Kershaw, 2008). Around the United States, because of recent
changes to the federal Administration for Children and Families (ACF) regulations, child­

Page 7 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

care centers are in the process of improving their all-hazard planning, including requiring
plans and training of staff. Future research should review these policy changes to mea­
sure whether they have increased preparedness.

Overall, reports from all over the globe have shown that children and youth can have a
positive role in the preparedness activities of their households, schools, and communities.
They may translate preparedness materials to their family members who do not speak the
dominant language, they can identify escape routes, and they can help stock supplies. In
the Gulf Coast, children and youth helped with numerous preparedness activities before
Hurricane Katrina (Fothergill & Peek, 2015). In El Salvador, youth participate in youth
emergency preparedness brigades and receive disaster prevention training, which may
include emergency drills and sanitation campaigns (Fordham, 2009). Children and youth
are an enormous resource in this stage of a disaster and the more they can be trained and
brought into preparedness activities, the safer they and their communities will be in a dis­
aster. Unfortunately, adults do not always understand this, as seen in Indonesia when men
were asked what girls could do in their village to reduce disasters; some responded that
girls just scream and are frightened (Haynes et al., 2010). In England and Wales, where
there is no systematic flood education in schools, children asked to learn more about
floods—how to make flood plans, for example—and identified it as a priority for their re­
silience (Mort et al., 2016).

Warning Communication and Response

The warning communication and response stage usually includes the dissemination and
receiving of disaster warnings, such as tornado and flood sirens or emergency broad­
casts, and actions taken as a result, such as evacuation. Who is likely to receive this im­
portant risk communication? And who is going to respond with protective actions? Do
warnings reach children? Overall, it can be said that there is very little known about chil­
dren, youth, and warning communication and response, and more attention should be giv­
en to this topic in the future.

Most children and youth are dependent on adults in their lives to communicate the risk
and to instruct them or assist them with the appropriate response, especially if they are
young. These adults could be parents, guardians, teachers, or childcare providers. There
is an assumption that parents will take care of children when there are warnings or pro­
tective actions needed, and this is often the case. However, it is important to keep in mind
that many children are not with their parents when disasters occur. Many are at school,
jobs, with friends, at childcare centers or babysitters’ homes, or in self-care.

In general, the presence of children at home often leads to evacuation when disaster
looms. Research has found that families with children in the home are more likely to
evacuate in times of disaster. In Hurricane Katrina, children’s presence prompted adults
to evacuate more often and earlier than they would have without them. However, this was
true for families who had the resources to evacuate—a working vehicle, money for gas
and lodging, for example. There was evidence that families with children who wanted to

Page 8 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

evacuate but did not have the means to do so, took other protective measures (Fothergill
& Peek, 2015). An important issue to consider is households that have both children and
pets. Family pets in some countries are considered members of the family, and children
especially are opposed to being separated from them in times of crisis. Thus, families are
reluctant to leave pets behind in a disaster, and this could hinder evacuation in some cas­
es. Indeed, one of the most significant reasons people do not evacuate in a disaster is that
they do not want to leave their pets, and this is especially true in households with chil­
dren.

Children often learn how to effectively respond to a disaster from the adults in their
schools. In 9/11, for example, teachers played a critical role in evacuation. There is some
evidence that the teaching of protective actions must be taught carefully and in the full
context of the situation. In Nepal in the 2015 earthquake, children ran into schools even
though school was not in session so that they could hide under their desks, which they
had been taught was the safest place for them in an earthquake. They did not understand
that they were safer outside and were carefully following what they had been taught.
Many of these children perished. This same behavior was also seen during earthquakes
and aftershocks in New Zealand. In addition to schools for older children (ages 5–18), at­
tention is needed on the youngest children, from birth to age five. Many of these very
young children are in the care of childcare centers or home providers, many of which do
not have all-hazard evacuation plans and do not practice drills.

One area for future consideration concerns additional differences among children and
youth. Are there gender and class differences with children and warnings and response?
Most likely yes, but more research is needed. Past research has found gender differences:
Women are more likely to hear and believe warnings than men are and interpret them as
valid and respond more to warnings than men, often because of their social networks. As
many young children are often with their mothers, this might have a direct bearing on
children. Thinking about ability status, children and youth with disabilities may be at par­
ticular risk because they may not be able to take protective actions, such as crawling un­
der their desks, or running to higher ground (Peek & Stough, 2010).

Turning to capacities, children can be helpful during the warning and evacuation phase.
Children are often educated about risks at school and bring that information home. Chil­
dren can be a calming presence as a disaster looms. Children can help prepare younger
siblings, pack up belongings, and do specific chores for their parents as the family pre­
pares to evacuate. In the days and hours before Hurricane Katrina, children and youth
took on various responsibilities to help their families evacuate (Fothergill & Peek, 2015).
Often children can be the translators of warnings for family members who do not speak
the dominant language. In New Zealand, children know to watch for natural warning
signs for tsunamis, learning the slogan “Long, Strong, Be Gone” meaning that if the
earthquake is lasts more than a minute and makes them fall down, then they need to get
to higher ground immediately.

Page 9 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Physical Impacts

Research on injury, disease, and fatality rates finds that children and youth suffer tremen­
dous physical impacts around the globe in disasters.

Children and youth suffer physically particularly when they are in structures, such as
schools or homes that collapse or are badly damaged or destroyed. The data show high
fatalities for children and youth in schools, many of which have been poorly constructed.
One of the earliest examples on record of this was the aptly named “Children’s Blizzard”
in 1888 in the Midwestern United States. The fierce and unexpected storm descended on
children as they walked home from school or as they huddled in poorly constructed
schools; scores of children died or lost their limbs (Laskin, 2004). In the almost 130 years
since then, there have been numerous accounts of children dying in their poorly con­
structed schools. The statistics that have emerged from many disasters are staggering.
The earthquake in China in 2008, for example, killed 10,000 children, and many of them
were in their schools that collapsed, most likely due to poor construction (Wong, 2008).
The school tragedies around the globe have led many to question the soundness of the
construction of the buildings and whether governments are to blame, as in some cases
schools have collapsed but other buildings around the schools have remained standing. In
China, there was widespread outcry over the collapsed schools and many demanded that
the government take responsibility for the deaths of the children.

Related to the poor construction, schools are often also located in risky locations, such as
floodplains, hillsides, and tsunami zones. In the U.S. west coast, for example, many
schools are located in dangerous tsunami zones. In some locations in Washington state,
residents voted against moving their schools even though they are in the tsunami zone,
and decided to keep the schools where they are despite the fact that there is no safe evac­
uation route for most of the schools in case of a tsunami. However, in Westport, Washing­
ton, the Ocosta Elementary School was designed to have a tsunami shelter on top of the
school gym, large enough for the students and many town residents.

After some disasters, communities realize that timing saved lives, as earthquakes oc­
curred during non-school hours, and their schools did not fare well. In 1933 in California,
a 6.3 magnitude earthquake struck on a Friday afternoon, just a few hours after schools
were let out for the day. More than 230 schools had major damage or were destroyed or
were deemed too unsafe to occupy (Olson, 1998; State of California, 2007). The poorly de­
signed buildings had not been built to withstand earthquakes. This type of wake-up call
has occurred in other places. More recently, in 2015, the 7.8 Ghorka Earthquake hit
Nepal on a Saturday morning, when children and their families were largely outside,
working and playing; they would have perished inside their schools had it been a school
day. Nepalese felt that it was a miracle that the earthquake occurred on a weekend day.
Wisner (2006) examined this phenomenon of the “luck” of disasters occurring when chil­
dren are not in schools—either it was a holiday or weekend, during the night, or when
children were out to recess. In his work, Wisner provides a chart of all the disasters that
have fallen in this “lucky” category where fatalities could have been much higher.

Page 10 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Unfortunately, many children and youth and their families are not so fortunate. Some
studies have shed light on how and why children and youth suffer tremendously from the
physical impacts of disasters. Mudavanhu and colleagues (2015) examined children in
Zimbabwe, a country faced with multiple disasters: floods, droughts, fires, epidemics, and
the HIV/AIDS pandemic. Often they are interconnected. In 2007, for example, floods in
one region led to malaria and diarrheal disease that affected over 1000 families. Their re­
search highlights the physical impacts of disaster on children. Here they describe the
children in their study:

About 75% of children who participated in the study have seen their houses and
school infrastructure collapse, have lost their livelihoods, and have suffered either
from malaria, cholera, dysentery, or diarrhoea due to flooding. Children described
their experiences during floods as a time when houses collapse, rivers overflow,
bridges are swept away, roads become slippery and unusable, animals drown,
there is an outbreak of disease, and food becomes scarce at the same time conta­
minated water is widespread. About 80% of children had tried to cross flooded
rivers, missed school, and experienced separation from parents during flooding.

This excerpt exemplifies the numerous risks that children face and how hazards intersect
and multiply, such as the emergence of disease after disasters. Along these lines, children
are also at particular risk to exposure to toxins that can damage their growing bodies.
Thus we need to consider children’s exposure to sewage, asbestos, contamination, oil,
chemicals as well as black mold and mildew, which may appear in flooded buildings. In
Japan after the 2011 earthquake-tsunami-nuclear accident, children were at risk to radia­
tion, yet many fathers put employment and economic stability ahead of their children’s
health (Morioka, 2014). After Hurricane Katrina, children and youth were at risk to expo­
sure to a variety of environmental contaminants, such as at their playgrounds and parks
(Figure 2).

Figure 2. New Orleans, 2005. Children’s playground


after Hurricane Katrina.

Photo courtesy of Alice Fothergill.

Page 11 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Other issues of social vulnerability are also factors. Social class, gender, race, and ability
status affect children’s vulnerability to physical impacts. Past research has found that the
morbidity and mortality rates for female children are often higher than for boys in devel­
oping countries, even though girls have physiological advantages over boys. This is often
the result of discrimination toward females. Children in developing countries, especially
the poorest children, appear to be the most vulnerable to death and injury. Children with
disabilities are also seen as being more at risk to the physical impacts of disasters as they
are unable to take many of the necessary protective actions.

Physical impacts also include illness, starvation, and undernutrition. For example, in a
study in rural Eastern India, researchers found that children in flooded communities that
were repeatedly flooded had a higher incidence of wasting, that is, becoming weak and
emaciated and greatly undernourished (Rodriguez-Llanes, Manuel, Ranjan-Dash,
Mukhopadhyay, & Guha-Sapir, 2016). The cases of severe wasting were over three times
higher for the children aged six months to just under five years of age in the repeatedly
flooded communities than those in non-flooded communities. Another physical impact is
child abuse and neglect, which is of grave concern and needs more attention (Curtis,
Miller, & Berry, 2000). One study found increased traumatic brain injury among children
two years old or younger after Hurricane Floyd (Keenan, Marshall, Nocera, & Runyan,
2004).

The physical impacts that have been documented in disasters are numerous and compli­
cated. One important thing to consider is that children are often present when other chil­
dren are injured, and thus some of the strategies in some parts of the world to train chil­
dren, even young children, in first aid and emergency response might allow children to
save others. It is important to note here that children and youth can have a role in mini­
mizing or preventing grave physical injuries or death. They take actions that save others
in disasters. In New Zealand in 2017, for example, two teen brothers rescued a woman, a
toddler, and a dog when the levees in their town of Edgecumbe broke suddenly during
heavy rains. Wisner (2006) points out the children who learned about disasters in schools
were able to save lives when they struck.

Psychological Impacts

Research has shown that disasters have psychological impacts on children and youth, of­
ten greater than adults. In many ways we know more about this aspect of children and
disaster than the other dimensions in this eight-category model. Data support the idea
that emotional impacts on children vary by age, disaster exposure, and level of support
from parents and other adults. Furthermore, when adults are suffering from emotional
distress, depression, PTSD, they often cannot meet the needs of their children, and the
children themselves may exhibit the symptoms. It is important to point out that because
of the widespread stigmatization of mental health problems, many survivors are hesitant
to admit these impacts or receive treatment for them.

Page 12 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

One of the most significant and often cited contributions to this discussion was the exten­
sive literature review of 250 studies done by Norris, Friedman, and Watson (2002) and
Norris, Friedman, Watson, Byrne et al. (2002). They included published quantitative stud­
ies done from 1981–2001 on acute, collectively experienced, sudden onset disasters. The
majority of the studies were single post-disaster assessments. They found that school-age
children had been more negatively affected by disasters than adults, stating “samples
composed of youth were more likely to fall into the severe range of impairment than sam­
ples of adults” (Norris, Friedman, & Watson, 2002, p. 218). They do note, however, that
the results are not clear for preschool aged children. When examining youth and ethnici­
ty, youth from the majority ethnic groups were more likely to fare better, but the authors
admit there is little information on this topic. The research team also found that disaster
location mattered for the youth outcomes: severe or very severe impairment was ob­
served more often in developing countries than developed countries, and disaster type
made a difference as there were more negative outcomes in situations of mass violence
than natural or technological disasters. For both children and adults, in general the sam­
ples improved as time passed.

In addition, Norris and colleagues found that family factors played a significant role.
Studies concluded that if a child had a distressed parent, he or she was more likely to be
distressed and the “less irritable and more supportive parents had healthier children, and
parents with less psychopathology offered more support” (Norris, Friedman, & Watson,
2002, p. 237). Indeed, they state that parental distress is sometimes the strongest predic­
tor of their children’s distress. Others since then agree with this conclusion (Ronan et al.,
2008).

Other studies have also documented the psychological challenges faced by children and
youth in disasters. Abramson, Park, Stehling-Ariza, and Redlener (2010) found that chil­
dren exposed to Hurricane Katrina were nearly five times as likely as a pre-Katrina co­
hort to have serious emotional disturbance. Some studies on children and youth in disas­
ters have found that they are often worried, afraid, and stressed after a disaster (see for
example, Babugura, 2008; La Greca, Silverman, Vernberg, & Roberts, 2002; Mudavanhu
et al., 2015). They are afraid of being separated from their families, upset about missing
school, stressed by new conflicts in their families, and sometimes overwhelmed by the
amount of work expected and needed from them (Mudavanhu et al., 2015). In The Great
East Japan Earthquake of 2011, researchers found that children were especially vulnera­
ble in terms of mental health, and it was particularly difficult to reach them and help
them because of the stigma surrounding mental illness in that area (Kozu & Homma,
2014). Research in India found that a community that was hit with heavy rains, glacier
melting, flash flooding, landslides, and mudslides experienced deaths, food shortages, ru­
ined temples, and contaminated water supplies. This situation caused post-disaster stress
disorder (PTSD) in adolescents (Watts, 2014).

Children’s reactions can be negatively affected by the degree of exposure to the disaster
event—how life threatening, how much they saw and experienced, the intensity of it—and
how upset and distraught their parents are in the aftermath. Furthermore, other family

Page 13 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

factors may also decrease a child’s emotional health such as family conflict, parents’ sub­
stance abuse, ineffective discipline, and low levels of warmth (Ronan & Johnston, 2005).
The age of the child might also play a role in psychological reactions and how they are ex­
pressed. In a study of school-age children in Hurricane Hugo, the younger children were
more likely to report PTSD symptoms than older children (Shannon, Lonigan, Finch, &
Taylor, 1994).

In addition, there is some indication that the death of a family pet, or even a missing fami­
ly pet in a disaster, can be deeply distressing to children. In Hurricane Katrina, hundreds
of families were not allowed to bring their animals when they were rescued by boat or he­
licopter, or when evacuated from the Superdome in New Orleans because they would not
let animals on public transportation, causing great anguish. It is reported that 15,000 ani­
mals were rescued from New Orleans (Irvine, 2009). In the United States, 70% of house­
holds have dogs or cats, so this may be an important issue to consider when thinking of
children’s emotional health in a disaster.

Some research finds gender differences in how psychological impacts are experienced,
expressed, or conveyed. Shannon and colleagues (1994) found that after Hurricane Hugo
in the United States boys reported more behavior issues, while girls, especially African
American girls, reported more emotional distress. Norris, Friedman, and Watson (2002)
found that females—including children, adolescents, and adults—were more adversely af­
fected; indeed, after some disasters women and girls were at least twice as likely to de­
velop PTSD as men and boys. Babugura (2008) found that both boys and girls were upset
to see dead livestock during droughts. Some of the boys in her study were responsible for
herding livestock and watching the cows die was disturbing and upsetting to them. One
13-year-old admitted that he cried for a long time when the cow he was caring for died in
the drought.

We may not have all the information we need about children and psychological impacts,
as parents might underreport their children’s levels of post-disaster distress. In some cas­
es, parents are so consumed with finding housing and food that they are not aware or
have to overlook their children’s psychological needs (Mudavanhu et al., 2015). Some­
times children do not tell their parents about their emotional distress or anxiety because
they do not want to burden their parents, as described by Fothergill and Peek (2015):

Children sometimes tell adults what they think they want to hear, rather than
what they actually feel or believe. They may also withhold information to “protect”
their parents. As a case in point, Mekana, one of the teens in our study, said she
would “hide” her true emotions and hold her “feelings in” when she was around
her mom. In the months following Katrina, her mother was angry and struggled
with depression. Mekana eventually reached a point where she would simply tell
her mother, “I do not feel like talking about this,” so that she could avoid crying or
“being all sad” in front of her.

(p. 5)

Page 14 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Many children are not proactive in talking about their emotional distress with adults in
their families, but adults also may not ask children and youth about how they are feeling,
and there is an assumption that the children will adapt to the difficult situations after dis­
asters (Babugura, 2008). This reasoning is related to the widely held myth that children
are resilient, like little rubber balls that can just “bounce back” after a disaster without
support (Fothergill & Peek, 2015). Thus, this would lead to the parents underreporting
the children’s levels of emotional distress.

Research has found that children and youth do well with support, consistency, and
warmth in a disaster situation from their parents and other significant adults in their lives
(Ronan & Johnston, 2005). After the Canterbury earthquakes of 2010–2012 in New
Zealand, Mooney and colleagues (2017) found that children coped effectively by using
strategies such as emotional regulation, problem solving, and positive reframing. They al­
so found that the children indicated an increase in empathy and a desire to help others.
New to the field is the idea that children and youth themselves are sometimes able to pro­
vide critical emotional support and warmth to one another. They are able to empathize
with their peers, provide comfort to their younger siblings, and be a listening ear to other
children and youth. They also provide emotional support to adults, as noted by parents
whose children gave them extra hugs, sang to them, and reassured them (Fothergill &
Peek, 2015).

Emergency Response

The emergency response period is the immediate aftermath, the first minutes, hours, and
days after a disaster, perhaps even up to a week, depending on the type and severity of
the disaster event. There are several important considerations about children in this
stage. First, what is the experience of children in the emergency response in general and
specifically in emergency shelters? How are their needs met and how are they kept safe?
Second, children need to stay with parents and guardians, but what happens if they are
separated? Additionally, we need to consider children and youth’s safety in the disaster
scene, their access to medical services, separation from friends and extended family
members, and the specific challenges faced by children with disabilities.

When a disaster strikes, children often take protective actions, such as finding and de­
pending on adults, seeking information, and helping others. Norwegian teens and chil­
dren who were caught in the 2004 Southeast Asian Tsunami in Thailand used various cop­
ing strategies in the immediate aftermath. These included positive thoughts, such as
telling themselves there would not be another earthquake or that they were safe in the
jungle, and self-soothing behaviors, such as holding onto adults or giving first aid to oth­
ers (Jensen, Ellestad, & Dyb, 2013). In some cases, children are separated from their fam­
ilies or guardians in the emergency response phase. This occurred in the aftermath of
Hurricane Katrina and many children were not reunited with parents for weeks or
months. In addition, many children and youth were with one parent but not their second
parent or extended families in the response period (Fothergill & Peek, 2015).

Page 15 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Many children move to mass shelters during the response phase. Children placed in evac­
uee shelters might face various hazards as many shelters are not designed as child-safe
spaces. Some shelters contain multiple hazards such as dangerous chemicals, open elec­
trical outlets, missing smoke detectors, and open tubs of water that are a drowning haz­
ard for young children. In addition to safety, children staying in shelters need stability,
routine, and continuation of education. Children can enroll in local schools and school
buses can pick them up at the mass temporary shelters, as was done after Hurricane Kat­
rina (Fothergill & Peek, 2015).

Research finds that there may be gender differences in this stage. Older girls are often
expected to help take care of younger siblings and take on other caregiving responsibili­
ties (Fothergill & Peek, 2015). Babugura (2008) finds that boys and girls are both taken
out of school in droughts, but because girls have a heavier workload in the family, includ­
ing collecting water for the household and caring for siblings, they are more likely to miss
school than boys. Both boys and girls have fears of being separated from their families
during drought disasters. In addition, research has found that women and girls in lower
income countries have faced discrimination and abuse in the immediate aftermath of dis­
aster. They receive less food; medical services; face greater safety risks; and have fewer
rights, protections, and resources. They may be victims of family violence, and rape and
assault by strangers. There is growing concern about the trafficking of girls and child
marriage in the chaos of the post-disaster emergency period.

In addition, research indicates that children and youth’s experiences and outcomes in the
emergency response might be affected by their race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, im­
migration status, and ability status. Past research has shown that the poor and people of
color in the United States encounter difficulties in the response phase, due to where they
live, language issues, issues of cultural sensitivity, isolation of neighborhoods or rural ar­
eas, fears of government and other institutions, and bias that leads some to receive less
emergency assistance than others. In light of that, some children face greater difficulties
and vulnerabilities in the response period. Children with disabilities may encounter inac­
cessible shelters, improperly trained staff, and discrimination (Peek & Stough, 2010). In
some countries, children may be at risk to rushed and corrupt inter-country adoption. Af­
ter the Haiti 2010 earthquake, safeguards and procedures were not followed and “expe­
dited” adoptions did not allow for the children’s adoptability to be clearly established, did
not give adoptive countries and parents a chance to prepare, and did not provide children
sufficient time to recover in a familiar environment (Dambach & Baglietto, 2010).

There is evidence that children and youth have capacities in the emergency response. De­
pending on their age and the circumstances, children and youth could help with search
and rescue, organization, information sharing and coordination on social media, and oth­
er tasks. In Hurricane Katrina, there were numerous examples of children assisting
adults, other children, and themselves in the emergency response. One child put his un­
cle on a mattress and pulled him through floodwaters to safety, while another boy helped
to rescue his sister from their apartment when the waters rose and they needed to escape
(Fothergill & Peek, 2015). In New Zealand, after the Christchurch earthquakes, a “stu­

Page 16 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

dent army” of assistance-minded youth used social media to organize response teams all
over the city. The youth, many of them university students, engaged in helping activities
such as cleaning up soil liquefaction in the streets. In Nepal, it was found that children
were able to provide more accurate and realistic assessments of damage and how many
people were affected because unlike adults, the children were not politically influenced
(Nikku, Sah, Karkara, & Ahmed, 2006).

Recovery

What is recovery and what does it look like? The definition of the concept is not agreed
upon. Bill Anderson (2005) argued that if we look at children and youth in our research
we might go further in our understanding of the concept of recovery. Often, recovery is
seen as a process that begins to lead a community back to “normal.” Anderson (2005)
raised these important questions:

There is another line of research involving questions related to recovery that


might also be worthwhile pursuing. For example, how do children perceive recov­
ery? Is it when they return to school, their homes have been repaired or replaced,
and their parents (or other significant others) have returned to their jobs or their
places of business?

Figure 3. Three post-disaster trajectories.

Reprinted with permission from Children of Katrina


(University of Texas Press, 2015).

Fothergill and Peek offer this definition of children’s recovery (2015, p. 32):

we conceptualize children’s recovery as when a child has a semblance of stability,


routine, well-being, and predictability in all spheres of life. With that in mind, we
fully acknowledge that there are many children living at the margins of society be­
fore disaster strikes, who live a daily existence lacking any stability, sense of rou­
tine, or predictability.

Page 17 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

After Hurricane Katrina, children and youth followed one of three primary and divergent
post-disaster trajectories (see Figure 3): a Declining Trajectory, a Finding Equilibrium
Trajectory, and a Fluctuating Trajectory (Fothergill & Peek, 2015). For many children, pri­
marily those with preexisting disadvantages, their vulnerability factors tended to build
over time and resulted in cumulative vulnerability. Other children, those who found equi­
librium, were able to find stability because they had the greatest resource depth—finan­
cial, social, cultural, educational, and personal—and were able to use their family’s social
and cultural capital effectively. The third group of children experienced a mixed pattern
of stable and then unstable periods, due to a variety of factors including access to re­
sources, previous vulnerability, and the presence of anchors who keep the children from
falling through the cracks.

Children’s trajectories in recovery were most powerfully influenced by economic, social,


and structural factors that disadvantaged (or advantaged) these children and their fami­
lies both before and after Katrina. These forces affected their ability to find equilibrium,
even though there was evidence that many of the children and youth were strong, re­
silient, hardworking, proactive, and used many creative problem-solving skills to cope
and find solutions for themselves and their families. A child—regardless of individual
traits or how “resilient”—cannot recover from a disaster without the necessary resources
and social structural support.

Displacement is very difficult for children, as they have to move to new homes, enter new
schools, make new friends, and they sometimes encounter new culture, food, and climate
(Fothergill & Peek, 2015). Displacement, especially multiple displacements, often leads
children and youth to miss school, which can have long lasting negative consequences.
Schools and teachers can play a tremendous role for children and youth in the recovery
period. Schools can implement important post-disaster curriculum and provide the day-to-
day routine that children need. Teachers are instrumental in not only using this curricu­
lum, but helping students as they recover. Teachers may be in “host communities” that
are integrating displaced children into their classrooms, which presents unique chal­
lenges, or they may be in the disaster-affected community, teaching an entire classroom
of affected children, who are perhaps dealing with the loss of homes, family members,
neighborhoods. Many teachers themselves are displaced or dealing with loss at the same
time. For children who evacuate from a disaster area and start new schools, the connec­
tions at the new school are critical. After Katrina, students did well if they felt attached to
their new school and if they got positive support from adults at their new school and their
friends (Barrett, Martinez-Cosio, & Barron Ausbrooks, 2008; Fothergill & Peek, 2015).

Community leaders need to prioritize schools in recovery, as they are important to both
children and communities. In Christchurch, New Zealand, despite citizen protests, locally
elected school boards were excluded from the decisions made about schools in the disas­
ter recovery. The Ministry of Education decided to merge and close many schools in the
aftermath, a move that Bronwyn Hayward (2016) called “especially destructive” because
young people need their schools and the social networks and support they provide. She
stated that it was “deeply disturbing” that the officials seized on an overwhelmed and

Page 18 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

grieving community to rush through controversial policies. She states that children and
teens in that area continue to suffer from high rates of mental illness and youth suicide
(p. 3).

Children can play an active role in recovery and listening to children’s voices, and sup­
porting their involvement in recovery activities should be a priority. In Hurricane Katrina,
children and youth helped other children, adults, and themselves, such as writing in jour­
nals and engaging in art (Fothergill & Peek, 2015). Vietnamese youth assisted in the Kat­
rina recovery by translating materials and announcements, such as the location of relief
centers, for their non-English speaking family members, and organizing against a landfill
being planned for their neighborhood (Mitchell, Haynes, Hall, Choong, & Oven, 2008). In
Nepal, children raised money for flood and landslide victims by singing door to door, and
in India, after the 2004 tsunami, children did peer counseling and utilized game and
laughter therapy with other children (Nikku et al., 2006). In New Zealand, teenagers felt
that helping their community after the Canterbury earthquakes gave them a feeling of
purpose and a sense of control (Pine, Tarrant, Lyons, & Leathem, 2015). As seen in Figure
4, children in the United States helped with recovery efforts after Hurricane Irene, such
as these Vermont children sorting donated items.

Figure 4. Vermont, 2011. Children helping in the re­


covery from Tropical Storm Irene.

Photo courtesy of Alice Fothergill.

Reconstruction/Risk Reduction

This section addresses reconstruction, rebuilding, and disaster risk reduction (DRR). This
stage brings us back full circle to risk perception, understanding of hazards, prepared­
ness, and mitigation. Rebuilding goes hand in hand with preparing for the future. As
some have stated there is a “window of opportunity” for change in the disaster aftermath

Page 19 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

and often that window happens as communities reconstruct and decide to “build back”
differently, with new priorities and new understandings of vulnerabilities.

In terms of children and vulnerability, often what we see in this stage is the discussion of
how can we reconstruct in a way that reduces the risk to children in the future. This has
been seen in the past with school reconstruction. For example, the 1933 earthquake in
Southern California destroyed many school structures. The California legislature, with as­
tonishing speed during the “window of opportunity,” passed a new law (The Field Act) re­
quiring schools to be earthquake resistant (Olson, 1998; State of California, 2007). Since
then, post-earthquake studies find that the legislation worked and new schools built un­
der the Field Act have withstood subsequent earthquakes far better and saved lives and
had lower reconstruction costs. Not a single public school building built under the Field
Act has collapsed and no one has died in any earthquakes, due to the seismic design and
strict inspection. Similarly, after the 1888 “Children’s Blizzard” in the Midwestern United
States where many children died in their schools, communities decided to rebuild their
schools to better protect students from storms (Laskin, 2004). In the aftermath of Hurri­
cane Katrina, the U.S. Congress and former President George W. Bush established the Na­
tional Commission on Children and Disasters in an effort to prepare for future disasters
and decrease the losses of children by establishing new policies and procedures, such as
child tracking and child reunification plans. However, most of the goals of the commission
have not been met.

In addition, other school preparedness measures are put into place in disaster recon­
struction, such as teacher education, curriculum changes, establishing safety drills, and
innovative projects and programs. In Australia after the 2009 bushfires in Victoria, the
Department of Education put into place several teacher training programs, designed to
develop teacher skills to help students in future disaster (Trethowan & Nursey, 2015). In
New Orleans after Hurricane Katrina, it was found that an “Edible Schoolyard” project
was an important part of long-term recovery and reconstruction for children and youth,
their families, the teachers, and the community around the school (Fakharzadeh, 2015).
This unique school gardening program helped with the children’s food environmental
knowledge, food security, and overall well-being. The children learned how to grow food,
cook meals, understand the ideas of sustainability, and they often invited residents of the
neighborhood for a community sit-down meal.

Children and youth themselves can play a large and important role in the reconstruction
stage and should be involved in post-disaster planning when possible. In Zimbabwe, one
study found that children wanted to be engaged and help their families reduce disaster
impacts. Many hoped to educate the community about deforestation risks and some even
wished for their families to relocate to safer areas (Mudavanhu et al., 2015). However,
this study found that the adults admitted that they do not seek children’s views and do
not have children participate in DRR related activities. Children said that they were not
respected, listened to, or taken seriously, and adults do not ever involve them in the deci­

Page 20 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

sion-making processes. Some of the adults felt that they wanted to protect them and not
put them under any pressure.

When given the opportunity, children and youth can spearhead important reconstruction
and disaster risk reduction measures. Mitchell and colleagues (2009) found that high
school students in the Philippines led a campaign to have their school moved away from a
landslide prone location. With education and advocacy, the youth were able to win a com­
munity-wide referendum to move the school to a safe place. Children and youth often
wish to talk about what they went through and are happy to contribute to the process of
planning for the next disaster. After Hurricane Katrina, children and youth wanted to
share their stories about their experiences, such as their harrowing escapes, the loss of
their homes (see Figure 5), and the long and difficult displacements specifically so that
they can reduce the risk to and suffering of other children in future disasters (Fothergill
& Peek, 2015).

Figure 5. New Orleans, 2015. A family’s home 10


years after Hurricane Katrina. All that remains are
the front steps.

Photo courtesy of Alice Fothergill.

Conclusion
In 2005, Bill Anderson called for more research on what “disasters do to children” in
terms of their health, education, and employment (especially in poor countries), and re­
covery. He stated that “The disaster research community, then, should cast its net wider
in order to include a younger catch” (p. 163). This was sage advice. In many ways, since
he expressed that view over a decade ago, the disaster research community has been
casting its net wider and has indeed been including a younger catch.

Scholars from around the globe have been focusing more intently on the experiences of
children and youth in disasters and the field has grown and become more diverse in
methods, research questions, and perspectives. For example, there is new recognition of
the importance of children in DRR plans and projects and more understanding of the
challenges of displacement. There has been more attention to the crucial role of schools
Page 21 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

and childcare centers for teaching children about hazards and making schools the center
point of creating a culture of safety and prevention. There has been documentation of the
importance of providing support to immediate family members in order to assist children,
and recognition that many children also need their friends, extended families, and advo­
cates in the wider community. There has been the emergence of research methods de­
signed to place children more at the center of the research, aiming to hear their voices
and ideas more clearly. The field is also placing children’s experiences in a broader cul­
tural and socioeconomic context.

The field of children and disaster is also documenting how poverty increases vulnerability
before, during, and after a disaster. Worldwide, and in the United States, children make
up the majority of those living in poverty. It is estimated that one billion children in the
world do not have at least one of the basic necessities, such as food, clean water, shelter,
or health care (CARE, 2016). As stated by the international group, Save the Children:

Child poverty is a challenge which should bind us globally. In almost every country
in the world children are more likely to be living in poverty than adults, and com­
pounding this, their particular life stage makes them more vulnerable to its devas­
tating effects with potential lifelong consequences for their physical, cognitive and
social development. While children themselves suffer the impacts of their poverty
most severely and immediately, the harmful consequences for societies, economies
and future generations can be felt nationally, regionally and even globally.

(2014, p. 1).

People living in poverty around the globe are more vulnerable to disasters for a multitude
of reasons, including the following: they are less likely to be able to prepare for disasters,
live in more hazardous places, often do not receive or understand warnings, have a hard­
er time evacuating due to a lack of resources, live in unstable or unsafe housing, have
fewer political rights and voice, and have fewer savings for recovery. Children’s vulnera­
bility is usually tied to the vulnerability of their families, often specifically their mothers.
Women have been found to be particularly vulnerable to disasters due to a lack of eco­
nomic and political resources, discrimination and oppression, and the demands of care­
giving responsibilities. As we move forward in our analysis of children, youth, and disas­
ter, we need to keep in mind the ways in which age intersects with multiple factors.

Social location—such as social class, race, neighborhood, resources, and networks—prior


to a disaster often determines, at least in part, many of the children’s post-disaster out­
comes. A child’s experience in a disaster could also be affected by language, type of hous­
ing, immigration status, gender, legal status, poverty, and disability issues. Girls, for ex­
ample, are at risk for sexual violence and exploitation in some disaster aftermath situa­
tions. Thus, it is crucial to consider the child’s environment before and after the disaster;
to realize that some children experience cumulative vulnerability, or an accumulation of
risk factors; and that disasters may occur on top of other crises, such as drought, epi­

Page 22 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

demics, or a family crisis such as divorce or death. It is also important to consider the ex­
periences of children and youth using a human rights perspective.

Finally, one of the significant developments in the work on children, youth, and disaster
has been the attention and recognition of their skills, talents, contributions, unique per­
spectives, problem-solving aptitudes, teaching abilities, creativity, and many other capaci­
ties. Anderson (2005) stated that children are not just passive victims or dependent ob­
servers, and that the actions that they take beg for documentation, which the field has be­
gun. It has become clear that at the same time that children and youth are vulnerable,
they also demonstrate important and often unnoticed capabilities and strengths, as they
assist themselves and others before and after a disaster strikes.

As Anderson (2005) stated, recognizing children’s vulnerabilities and capabilities should


be a research and policy priority. As disaster events increase globally, we need to consid­
er how to reduce the suffering of children and youth, and assist them in disaster after­
maths, with the understanding that they are able to attain and maintain stability in their
lives with social support and the mobilization of resources. The past decade has seen a
steady stream of research, with a wide variety of research methods, often placing chil­
dren and youth at the center of the research and paying closer attention to their experi­
ences. This is a positive and welcome development but much more research is still need­
ed. It is critical—especially as the risks to disasters continue to increase—that we fill our
knowledge gaps and understand the vulnerabilities and capabilities of children and youth
in disasters.

References
Abramson, D. M., Park, Y. S., Stehling-Ariza, T., & Redlener, I. (2010). Children as bell­
wethers of recovery: Dysfunctional systems and the effects of parents, households, and
neighborhoods on serious emotional disturbance in children after Hurricane Katrina. Dis­
aster Medicine and Public Health Preparedness, 4(S1), S17–S27.

Anderson, W. (2005). Bringing the children into focus on the social science research agen­
da. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 23, 159–175.

Babugura, A. A. (2008). Vulnerability of children and youth in drought disasters: A case


study of Botswana. Children, Youth and Environments, 18(1), 126–157.

Barrett, E. J., Martinez-Cosio, M., & Barron Ausbrooks, C. Y. (2008). The school as a
source of support for Katrina-evacuated youth. Children, Youth and Environments, 18(1),
202–236.

CARE. (2016). Child poverty. Retrieved from http://www.care.org/work/poverty/child-


poverty.

Corsaro, W. (2010). Sociology of childhood (3d ed.). Los Angeles: Sage-Pine Forge Press.

Page 23 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Curtis, T., Miller, B. C., & Berry, E. H. (2000). Changes in reports and incidence of child
abuse following natural disasters. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24(9), 1151–1162.

Dambach, M., & Baglietto, C. (2010). Haiti: “Expediting” intercountry adoptions in the af­
termath of a natural disaster: Preventing future harm. Geneva, Switzerland: International
Social Service (ISS).

Drabek, T. (1986). Human systems responses to disaster: An inventory of sociological find­


ings. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Fakharzadeh, S. (2015). Food for thought: The intersection of gardens, education, and
community at Edible School Yard New Orleans. Children, Youth and Environments, 25(3),
175–187.

Fordham, M. (2009). “We can make things better for each other”: Women and girls organ­
ise to reduce disasters in Central America. In E. Enarson & P. G. D. Chakrabarti (Eds.),
Women, gender, and disaster: Global issues and initiatives (pp. 175–188). London: SAGE.

Fothergill, A. (1996). Gender, risk, and disaster. International Journal of Mass Emergen­
cies and Disasters, 14(1), 33–56.

Fothergill, A., Maestas, E., & Darlington, J.-A. (1999). Race, ethnicity, and disasters in the
United States: A review of the literature. Disasters, 23(2), 156–173.

Fothergill, A., & Peek, L. (2004). Poverty and disasters in the United States: A review of
recent sociological findings. Natural Hazards, 32, 89–110.

Fothergill, A., & Peek, L. (2015). Children of Katrina. Austin: University of Texas Press.

Freeman, C., Nairn, K., & Gollop, M. (2015). Disaster impact and recovery: What children
and young people can tell us. Kōtuitui: New Zealand Journal of Social Sciences, 10(2),
103–115.

Gavenus, K. A., Tobin-Gurley, J., & Peek, L. (2013). Children of the spills – phase I: Alaska
and the Exxon Valdez oil spill. Children, Youth and Environments, 23(1), 155–166.

Haynes, K., Lassa, J., & Towers, B. (2010). Child centred disaster risk reduction and cli­
mate change adaptation: Roles of gender and culture in Indonesia. Working paper #2.
Children in a Changing Climate Research Programme. Sydney, Australia.

Hayward, B. (2016). Sustaining democracy in disaster: The seeds of recovery. Foundation


for Democracy and Sustainable Development newsletter. Retrieved from http://
www.fdsd.org/spring-newsletter-april-2016/.

Irvine, L. (2009). Filling the ark: Animal welfare in disasters. Philadelphia: Temple Univer­
sity Press.

Page 24 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Jensen, T. K., Ellestad, A., & Dyb, G. 2013. Children and adolescents’ self-reported coping
strategies during the Southeast Asian Tsunami. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 52,
92–106.

Johnston, D. M., Ronan, K. R., & Standring, S. (2014). Children’s understanding of natural
hazards in Christchurch: Reflecting on a 2003 study. Australian Journal of Emergency
Management, 29, 66.

Johnson, V., Ronan, K., Johnston, D., & Peace, R. (2014). Implementing disaster prepared­
ness education in New Zealand primary schools. Disaster Prevention Management, 23(4),
370–380.

Johnson, V. A., Ronan, K. R., Johnston, D. M., & Peace, R. (2014). Evaluations of disaster
education programs for children: A methodological review. International Journal of Disas­
ter Risk Reduction, 9, 107–123.

Keenan, H. T., Marshall, S. W., Nocera, M. A., & Runyan, D. K. (2004). Increased inci­
dence of inflicted traumatic brain injury in children after a natural disaster. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine, 26(3), 189–193.

Kozu, S., & Homma, H. (2014). Lessons learned from the great East Japan earthquake:
The need for disaster preparedness in the area of disaster mental health for children.
Journal of Emergency Management, 12(6), 431–439.

La Greca, A. M., Silverman, W. K., Vernberg, E. M., & Roberts, M. C. (2002). Introduction.
In A. M. La Greca, W. K. Silverman, E. M. Vernberg, & M. C. Roberts (Eds.), Helping chil­
dren cope with disasters and terrorism (pp. 3–8). Washington, DC: American Psychologi­
cal Association.

Laskin, D. (2004). The children’s blizzard. New York: HarperCollins.

Lauten, A. W., & Lietz, K. (2008). A look at the standards gap: Comparing child protection
responses in the aftermath of Hurricane Katrina and the Indian Ocean tsunami. Children,
Youth and Environments, 18(1), 158–201.

McFarlane, A. C. (1987). Family functioning and overprotection following a natural disas­


ter: The longitudinal effects of post-traumatic morbidity. Australian and New Zealand
Journal of Psychiatry, 21, 210–218.

Mileti, D. (1999). Disasters by design: A reassessment of natural hazards in the United


States. Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press.

Mitchell, T., Haynes, K., Hall, N., Choong, W., & Oven, K. (2008). The roles of children and
youth in communicating risk. Children, Youth and Environments, 18(1), 254–279.

Mitchell, T., Tanner, T., & Haynes, K. (2009). Children as agents of change for disaster
risk reduction: Lessons from El Salvador and the Philippines. Working paper #1. Children
in a changing climate (CCC) Research. Brighton, U.K.: IDS/Plan.

Page 25 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Mooney, M., Tarrant, R., Paton, D., Johal, S., & Johnston, J. (2017). Getting through:
Children’s effective coping and adaptation in the context of the Canterbury, New Zealand,
earthquakes of 2010–2012. Australasian Journal of Disaster and Trauma Studies, 21(1),
19–30.

Morioka, R. (2014). Gender difference in the health risk perception of radiation from
Fukushima in Japan: The role of hegemonic masculinity. Social Science & Medicine, 107,
105–112.

Mort, M., Walker, M., Lloyd Williams, A., Bingley, A., & Howells, V. (2016). Final project
report for children, young people and flooding: Recovery and resilience. Lancaster, U.K.:
Lancaster University.

Mudavanhu, C., Manyena, S. B., Collins, A. E., Bongo, P., Mavhura, E., & Manatsa, D.
(2015). Taking children’s voices in disaster risk reduction a step forward. International
Journal of Disaster Risk Science, 6(3), 267–281.

Nikku, B. R., Sah, N., Karkara, R., & Ahmed, S. (2006). Child rights perspective in re­
sponse to natural disasters in South Asia: A retrospective study. Kathmandu, Nepal: Save
the Children Sweden. Retrieved from http://resourcecentre.savethechildren.se/au­
thors/nikku-bala-raju.

Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., & Watson, P. J. (2002). 60,000 disaster victims speak: Part
II. Summary and implications of the disaster mental health research. Psychiatry, 65(3),
240–260.

Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., Watson, P., Byrne, C. M., Diaz, E., & Kaniasty, K (2002).
60,000 disaster victims speak: Part I. An empirical review of the empirical literature,
1981–2001. Psychiatry, 65(3), 207–239.

Olson, R. (1998). Fits and starts: 20th century seismic safety policy in the U.S. In The
EERI golden anniversary volume: 1948–1998 (pp. 23–27). Oakland, CA: Earthquake Engi­
neering Research Institute.

Peek, L. (2008). Children and disasters: Understanding vulnerability, developing capaci­


ties, and promoting resilience – an introduction. Children, Youth and Environments, 18(1),
1–29.

Peek, L. (2010). Age. In B. D. Phillips, D. S. K. Thomas, A. Fothergill, & L. Blinn-Pike


(Eds.), Social vulnerability to disasters (1st ed., pp. 155–185). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press,
Taylor & Francis.

Peek, L. (2013). Age. In D. S. K. Thomas, B. D. Phillips, W. E. Lovekamp, & A. Fothergill


(Eds.), Social vulnerability to disasters (2d ed., pp. 167–198). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press,
Taylor & Francis.

Peek, L., & Stough, L. M. (2010). Children with disabilities in the context of disaster: A so­
cial vulnerability perspective. Child Development, 81(4), 1260–1270.
Page 26 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Phillips, B. D., Thomas, D. S. K., Fothergill, A., & Blinn-Pike, L. (Eds.). (2010). Social vul­
nerability to disasters. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, Taylor & Francis.

Pine, N., Tarrant, R., Lyons, A. C., & Leathem, J. M. (2015). Rolling with the shakes: An
insight into teenagers’ perceptions of recovery after the Canterbury earthquakes.
Kōtuitui: New Zealand Journal of Social Sciences Online, 10(2), 116–125.

Prelog, A., Weesjes, E., Peek, L., Templeton-Lynch, M., Prelog, R., Gill, S., & Akers, A.
(2014). Childcare and natural hazards: State of Colorado. Fort Collins: Center for Disaster
and Risk Analysis, Colorado State University.

Rodriguez-Llanes, J. M., Ranjan-Dash, S., Mukhopadhyay, A., & Guha-Sapir, D. (2016).


Flood-exposure is associated with higher prevalence of child undernutrition in rural East­
ern India. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 13(2), 210.

Ronan, K. R., Alisic, E., Towers, B., Johnson, V. A., & Johnston, D. M. (2015). Disaster pre­
paredness for children and families: A critical review. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17(7),
1–9.

Ronan, K. R., Crellin, K., Johnston, D. M., Finnis, K., Paton, D., & Becker, J. (2008). Pro­
moting child and family resilience to disasters: Effects, interventions, and prevention ef­
fectiveness. Children, Youth and Environments, 18(1), 332–353.

Ronan, K. R., & Johnston, D. (2005). Promoting community resilience in disasters: The
role for schools, youth, and families. New York: Springer.

Save the Children. (2014). Towards the end of child poverty. Retrieved from https://
www.savethechildren.net/resources.

Shannon, M. P., Lonigan, C. J., Finch, A. J., & Taylor, C. M. (1994). Children exposed to dis­
aster: I. Epidemiology of post-traumatic symptoms and symptom profiles. Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(1), 80–93.

Shaw, R., & Kobayashi, M. (2001). Role of schools in creating earthquake-safer environ­
ment. United Nations Centre for Regional Development (UNCRD), Disaster Management
and Educational Facilities, Greece.

State of California. (2007). The Field Act and public school construction: A 2007 perspec­
tive. Alfred E. Alquist Seismic Safety Commission. Retrieved from http://
www.seismic.ca.gov/pub/CSSC_2007-03_Field_Act_Report.pdf.

Thomas, D. S. K., Phillips, B. D., Lovekamp, W. E., & Fothergill, A. (Eds.). (2013). Social
vulnerability to disasters (2d ed.). New York: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis Group.

Tipler, K., Tarrant, R., Tuffin, K., & Johnston, D. (2016). Legislative requirements and
emergency management practitioner expectations of preparedness in New Zealand
schools. Australian Journal of Emergency Management, 32(1), 32–39.

Page 27 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020


Children, Youth, and Disaster

Trethowan, V., & Nursey, J. (2015). Helping children and adolescents recover from disas­
ter: A review of teacher-based support programs in Victorian schools. Australian Journal
of Emergency Management, 30(4), 17.

Watts, B. (2014). Glacier hazards linked to prolonged PTSD in kids. Glacier Hub. Re­
trieved from http://glacierhub.org/2014/12/18/glacier-hazards-linked-to-pro­
longed-ptsd-in-kids/.

Wilson, S. J., & Kershaw, M. K. (2008). Caring for young children after a hurricane: Child­
care workers reflect on support and training needs. Children, Youth and Environments,
18(1), 237–253.

Wisner, B. (2006). Let our children teach us! A review of the role of education and knowl­
edge in disaster risk reduction. UNISDR. Bangalore, India: Books for Change. Retrieved
from http://www.unisdr.org/2005/task-force/working%20groups/knowledge-educa­
tion/docs/Let-our-Children-Teach-Us.pdf.

Wong, E. (2008). China admits building flaws in quake. The New York Times. A6. Avail­
able at http://www.nytimes.com/2008/09/05/world/asia/05china.html?
rref=collection%2Fbyline%2Fedward-
wong&action=click&contentCollection=undefined&region=stream&module=stream_unit&v

Notes:

(1.) This typology was developed out of conversations with Dennis Mileti in 1994.

Alice Fothergill

Department of Sociology, University of Vermont

Page 28 of 28

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, NATURAL HAZARD SCIENCE (oxfordre.com/naturalhazard­
science). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2020. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited
(for details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 15 March 2020

You might also like