Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Intelligence as a predictor of outcome in short- and


long-term psychotherapy
Paul Knekt a,b,n, Taru Saari a, Olavi Lindfors a
a
National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland
b
Biomedicum Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Intelligence has been suggested as a suitability factor for short-term therapy whereas its possible effect
Received 24 June 2013 on short-term versus long-term therapy still is unknown. The aim of this study was to compare the
Received in revised form prediction of intelligence on the level of psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial functioning in
2 February 2014
psychotherapies of different lengths. A total of 251 outpatients from the Helsinki Psychotherapy Study,
Accepted 5 October 2014
aged 20–46 years, and suffering from mood or anxiety disorders were allocated to two long-term and
Available online 16 October 2014
two short-term therapies. Intelligence was assessed at baseline with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Keywords: Scale (WAIS-R). Psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial functioning were assessed 5–10 times during a
Depression 5-year follow-up using two primary symptom measures (HDRS and HARS) and one primary measure of
Intelligence
psychosocial functioning (GAF). Short-term therapy was more effective than long-term therapy during
Long-term
the first year of follow-up. During the second to fourth follow-up year no differences between short- and
Prediction
Psychotherapy long-term therapies or the intelligence groups were found. At the fifth follow-up year, however, long-
Short-term term psychotherapy showed a statistically significantly larger change in all three primary measures
compared to short-term therapy among those with higher intelligence. No differences between therapy
groups were noted in those with lower intelligence. People with higher intelligence may benefit more
from long-term than from short-term psychotherapy. These findings should be confirmed.
& 2014 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 2010; Watzke et al., 2010), the ability for abstract thinking and
mastery of interaction with the environment (Allen et al., 1986;
It has been suggested that short- and long-term psychotherapies Gabbard, 2004), all suggest greater potential for a more sustained
apparently demand different attributes from the patient (Laaksonen recovery in long-term explorative psychotherapy.
et al., 2013a). In short-term psychotherapy the aims of the therapy As far as these authors are aware, intelligence as a predictor of
are relatively clearly defined and the therapist has an active role in psychotherapy outcome has been studied exclusively in short-
facilitating fast development of the time-limited treatment process. term therapies and after the 1980s covering only cognitive-
In contrast, in open-ended psychoanalysis and long-term psycho- behavioral therapies (Haaga et al., 1991; Luborsky et al., 1996;
dynamic psychotherapy there is less focus on predefined goals and Fournier et al., 2009; Rizvi et al., 2009; D’Alcante et al., 2012 ) and
therapist guidance and more weight is given to improvements in non-directive supportive counseling (Doubleday et al., 2002). The
self-awareness and insight (Gunderson and Gabbard, 1999) and to results of these studies are contradictory. Intelligence did not
implicit learning processes (Wong and Haywood, 2012). These predict the outcome of cognitive-behavioral therapies in out-
require that the patient, besides having the ability to commit to patients suffering from depressive or anxiety disorders (Haaga
the long treatment, is also able to form a fruitful alliance with the et al., 1991; Doubleday et al., 2002). However, higher intelligence
therapist, has tolerance for anxiety, and has the motivation and predicted more reduced anxiety in older adult patients treated
capacity to explore his/her internal world actively and thoroughly with non-directive counseling, thus suggesting its potentially
(Gabbard, 2004). Likewise, patient's good cognitive capacities and greater demands on abstract thinking than what is needed in
ego functions, such as an adequate level of reflective ability (Fonagy, cognitive-behavioral therapy (Doubleday et al., 2002). In one
study, lower intelligence predicted poor response after cognitive
therapy in patients with depression (Fournier et al., 2009). Also, in
n
Corresponding to: National Institute for Health and Welfare, P.O. Box 30 00271
patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder, especially higher
Helsinki, Finland, paul.knekt@thl.fi, Tel.: þ 358 29 524 8774. verbal intelligence predicted a greater reduction of symptoms in
E-mail address: paul.knekt@thl.fi (P. Knekt). cognitive-behavioral therapy (D’Alcante et al., 2012). However,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.10.011
0165-1781/& 2014 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
1020 P. Knekt et al. / Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027

among women with posttraumatic stress disorder, treated with broad area of intrapsychic and interpersonal conflicts. The therapeutic setting and
technique are characterized by facilitating maximum development of transference
cognitive processing therapy, no prediction of intelligence on
by the use of a couch and free association for exploring unconscious conflicts,
outcome in symptoms was found (Rizvi et al., 2009). developmental deficits, and distortions of intrapsychic structures (Greenson, 1985).
Davanloo (1978) developed a comprehensive set of essential The frequency of sessions in PA was four times a week during a period of
suitability selection criteria for short-term psychodynamic psy- approximately 5 years.
chotherapy considering different aspects of ego functions, including Altogether 60 therapists participated in the study. A total of five therapists gave
solution-focused therapy, 11 short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy, 29 long-
intelligence. Greater level of intellectual resources has also been term psychodynamic psychotherapy, and 25 psychoanalysis. The therapists had
suggested beneficial for long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy practiced for at least 2 years after training in the specific form of psychotherapy.
and psychoanalysis (Bacharach and Leaff, 1978; American The mean number of years of experience in the therapy provided was 9 in SFT and
Psychiatric Association, (APA) (1985)). Since it is reasonable to SPP, 18 in LPP, and 15 in PA.
The therapists giving psychodynamic psychotherapy or psychoanalysis had
hypothesize that individuals with higher intelligence may be able
received standard training in psychoanalytically oriented psychotherapy (Knekt
to benefit more in the long run from long-term psychotherapy and et al., 2008). During their training the therapists received a minimum of 3–6 years
since this hypothesis has not been studied previously, the present of analytical (psychoanalysis or psychotherapy) training, and those giving short-
study investigates the prediction of intelligence on changes in term therapy received 1–2 years of additional short-term focal psychodynamic
psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial functioning among patients psychotherapy training. Also therapists giving SFT had been trained to use the
method and received a qualification for the method from a local institute. None of
receiving short- versus long-term psychotherapy during a 5-year
the therapists had received any training in psychodynamic psychotherapy and
follow-up from the start of treatment. Since long-term therapy is vice versa.
less effective during the first year of follow-up and more effective at In psychodynamic psychotherapies, the treatment was provided in accordance
the end of follow-up (Knekt et al., 2013), the comparisons are with clinical practice using “manual like general guidelines”, i.e., the therapists
could modify their interventions according to patients' needs (Knekt et al., 2008).
carried out separately for short and long follow-up.
In contrast, the solution-focused therapy was given according to a manual, and
adherence control was organized. Since the effectiveness of the two short-term
therapies did not differ in this data (Lindfors et al., 2012;Knekt et al., 2013;
2. Methods Laaksonen et al., 2013b) they were combined to one group.

2.1. Patients
2.3. Assessment methods
This study is part of the Helsinki Psychotherapy Study, in which 506 eligible
outpatients (459 for short- and long-term psychotherapy and 47 for psycho- Intelligence was assessed at baseline using an abbreviated version of the test
analysis) were recruited from psychiatric services between June 1994 and June Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) (Wechsler, 1981), covering the
2000 and followed for 5 years (Knekt and Lindfors, 2004). The protocol was full scale intelligence quotient (IQ), the verbal IQ, and the non-verbal performance
approved by the Helsinki University Central Hospital's ethics council. Written IQ. The tests Digit Span, Arithmetic, Comprehension, and Similarities were used for
informed consent was obtained from each patient. The patients and settings, the verbal IQ and the tests Picture Completion, Picture Arrangement, Block Design,
therapies and therapists, and assessment methods and statistical methods have and Digit Symbol for the performance IQ. In this study, all three quotients were
been presented in more detail elsewhere (Knekt and Lindfors, 2004; Knekt et al., used as predictors and were for the purposes of the analyses divided into two
2008), and are summarized briefly here. categories by the median: low and high.
Patients considered eligible were 20–45 years of age and had a long-standing Descriptive and potential confounding factors were assessed at baseline using
( 41 year) disorder causing dysfunction in work ability. They had to meet the questionnaires and interviews. Psychiatric diagnoses (American Psychiatric
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) (American Association, 1994) were assessed using a semi-structured interview (Knekt and
Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for depressive or anxiety disorder. Patients Lindfors, 2004). Psychiatric history (age at the onset of primary psychiatric
were excluded from the study if they were suffering from psychotic disorder or disorder, duration of primary psychiatric disorder, and separation experiences at
severe personality disorder, bipolar I disorder, adjustment disorder, substance childhood), previous psychiatric treatment (i.e., psychotherapy, psychotropic med-
abuse, organic brain disease or other severe organic disease, or mental retardation. ication, or psychiatric hospitalization), and socioeconomic factors (age, sex, marital
Individuals treated with psychotherapy within the previous 2 years, psychiatric status, education, occupation, and current employment status) were assessed using
health employees, and persons known to the research team members were also questionnaires. Personality functioning was assessed by the scales Level of
excluded. Personality Organization (LPO) (Kernberg, 1996; Valkonen et al., 2012), Quality of
Of the eligible patients, 139 refused to participate, and 326 of the remaining Object Relations (QORS) (Azim et al., 1991), Immature Defense Style as per the
367 patients were randomly assigned to solution-focused therapy (SFT), short-term Defense Style Questionnaire (DSQ) (Andrews et al., 1989), Affiliation Toward Self as
psychodynamic psychotherapy (SPP) and long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy per the Structural Analysis of Social Behavior questionnaire (SASB) (Benjamin,
(LPP). In addition, 41 patients were selected for psychoanalysis (PA). The present 1996), Interpersonal Problems (IIP) (Horowitz et al., 2000), and the Suitability for
study was conducted on a randomly selected subpopulation of 251 patients who Psychotherapy Scale (SPS) (Laaksonen et al., 2012) and social functioning by the
had been assessed using the abbreviated Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised scales Social Adjustment (SAS-SR) (Weissman and Bothwell, 1976) and Sense of
(WAIS-R) (Wechsler, 1981). Of these patients, 73 were in the SFT group, 71 in the Coherence (SOC) (Antonovsky, 1993).
SPP group, 66 in the LPP group, and 41 in the PA group. The outcome measures of this study were psychiatric symptoms and psycho-
social functioning assessed during a 5-year follow-up from start of therapy. The
primary measures were based on interviews conducted by experienced clinical
2.2. Therapies and therapists raters at baseline and four times thereafter (at 7, 12, 36, and 60 months from
baseline) and the secondary measures were based on self-report questionnaires
SFT is a brief, resource-oriented, goal-focused therapeutic approach, which filled in at baseline and nine times thereafter (at 3, 7, 9, 12, 18, 24, 36, 48 and 60
helps clients change by constructing solutions (Johnson and Miller, 1994). The months from baseline) (Knekt and Lindfors, 2004).
orientation was based on an approach developed by De Shazer et al. (1986). The The three primary measures were the interviewer-assessed Global Assessment
frequency of sessions in SFT was flexible: usually one session every 2 or 3 weeks, up of Functioning scale (GAF) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), the 17-item
to a maximum of 12 sessions, over a period of no more than 8 months. Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) (Hamilton, 1960), and the 14-item
SPP is a brief, focal, transference-based therapeutic approach which helps Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HARS) (Hamilton, 1959). The secondary measures
patients by exploring and working through specific intrapsychic and interpersonal were the self-report 90-item Symptom Check List, Global Severity Index (SCL-90-
conflicts. The orientation was based on approaches described by Malan (1976) and GSI) (Derogatis et al., 1973), 13-item Symptom Check List, Depression Scale (SCL-
Sifneos (1978). SPP was scheduled for 20 treatment sessions, with one session 90-DEP) (Derogatis et al., 1973), 21-item Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck
per week. et al., 1961), and 10-item Symptom Check List, Anxiety Scale (SCL-90-Anx)
LPP is an open-ended, intensive, transference-based therapeutic approach (Derogatis et al., 1973).
which helps patients by exploring and working through a broad area of intrap- The assessment of compliance was based on information regarding waiting
sychic and interpersonal conflicts. Therapy includes both expressive and supportive time from randomization to the initiation of treatment, the completeness of the
elements, depending on the patient's needs. The orientation followed the clinical treatment (i.e., withdrawal after randomization and discontinuation of treatment),
principles of LPP (Gabbard, 2004). The frequency of sessions in LPP was 2–3 times a and the use of auxiliary treatment (i.e., additional psychotherapy, psychotropic
week during a period of approximately 3 years. medication use, and hospitalization) at baseline and the nine measurement points
PA is an open-ended, highly intensive, transference-based psychodynamic during the 5-year follow-up (Knekt et al., 2011). Auxiliary treatment was assessed
therapeutic approach which helps patients by analyzing and working through a by questionnaires, interviews, and based on nationwide health registers.
P. Knekt et al. / Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027 1021

After assignment to the treatment groups, 17 patients (four SFT patients, two Linear mixed models (Verbeke and Molenberghs, 1997) with random effects
SPP patients, 10 LPP patients, and one PA patient) refused to participate. Of the for patient and time (Harkanen et al., 2005) were used in the statistical analysis.
patients starting the assigned therapy, 32 (seven assigned to SFT, seven to SPP, nine The dependent variables were the outcome measures (HDRS, HARS, GAF, SCL-90-
to LPP, and nine to PA) discontinued the treatment prematurely. Altogether 89 of GSI, BDI, SCL-90-DEP, and SCL-90-ANX). The basic independent variables of the
the 227 patients (30 in SFT, 33 in SPP, 16 in LPP, and 10 in PA) received auxiliary ITT model included the IQ measured at baseline, therapy group (short-term (SFT
treatment at the end of follow-up. or SPP) versus long-term (LPP or PA)), and the time of measurement during
The mean drop-out rate over the measurement occasions during the follow-up follow-up, their first- and second-order interactions, and a correction term
was 16% in the SFT, 14% in the SPP, 11% in the LPP, and 1% in the PA group. The major (including the interaction of the difference between theoretical and realized date
reason for the drop-out from measurement was refusal on the ground that the of measurement, time, and intelligence). The following confounding factors,
study occasion was felt to be mentally stressful or the patient was disappointed measured at baseline, were included in the full model: age, sex, marital status,
with the treatment. education, age at the onset of primary psychiatric disorder, duration of primary
psychiatric disorder, previous depressive episodes, separation experiences at
childhood, psychotropic medication, and the scores of LPO, QORS, DSQ, SASB,
2.4. Statistical methods IIP, SAS, SOC, all satisfying the criteria for confounding (Rothman and Greenland,
1998). The AT model was created by adding to the ITT model the waiting time
A cohort study design with repeated measurements was used. The main analyses from randomization to the initiation of treatment, the completeness of study
were based on the ‘intention-to-treat’ (ITT) design which included all the patients treatment, and auxiliary treatment during follow-up mainly as time-dependent
who had been randomized. Complementary ‘as-treated’ (AT) analyses were also covariates.
performed (Knekt et al., 2008). The primary analyses were based on the assumption The independent variable of main interest was the interaction term between
of ignorable dropouts from the outcome measures (Harkanen et al., 2005). In the IQ, therapy group, and time. The statistical significance of this term was tested
secondary analyses, missing values of one outcome measure were replaced by with the Wald test. The significance of the change in the outcome variable from
multiple imputations using existing information on the other outcome measures the baseline point to the different measurement points for each therapy group
(Rubin, 1987). The imputation was based on Markov chain Monte Carlo methods. The (short-term and long-term) and category (low and high) of intelligence consid-
variables in the imputation model were assumed to follow a multinormal distribu- ered was also tested using the Wald test. The differences in outcome between the
tion. The ITT and AT models were carried out based on both the original data and the therapy by categories of IQ at the different measurement points were model-
imputed data. As there were no notable differences between the ITT and AT models adjusted (Lee, 1981). The delta method was used for the calculation of their
or between the original ITT and the imputed models (data not shown), the results confidence intervals (Migon and Gamerman, 1999). All statistical analyses were
based on the ITT model were presented in the results. performed with SAS software, version 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc., 2008).

Table 1
Baseline characteristics by full scale IQ tertile.

Characteristic All Full scale IQ P-value for


heterogeneity
Low (N ¼ 87) Medium (N¼ 84) High (N ¼ 80)
(range¼ 71–107) (range ¼108–114) (range¼ 115–137)

Demographic variables
Women (%) 71.7 78.2 75.0 61.3 0.04
Age (years), mean (S.D.) 32.0 (6.7) 31.1 (5.3) 31.4 (7.5) 33.5 (7.1) 0.04
Living alone (%) 40.2 41.4 47.6 31.3 0.10
University degree (%) 27.1 14.9 26.2 41.3 o0.001
Low-level employee (%) 28.3 42.5 23.8 17.5 o0.001
Currently employed or studying (%) 83.9 83.5 81.0 87.5 0.52

Diagnoses
Mood disorder (%) 52.6 46.0 51.2 61.3 0.14
Anxiety disorder (%) 19.1 23.0 15.5 18.8 0.46
Co-morbid mood and anxiety disorder (%) 28.3 31.0 33.3 20.0 0.13
Personality disorder (%) 23.1 27.6 22.6 18.8 0.40

Symptoms
Symptom Check List Global Severity Index (SCL-90-GSI), 1.27 (0.5) 1.31 (0.5) 1.32 (0.5) 1.18 (0.5) 0.16
mean (S.D.)
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), mean (S.D.) 17.9 (7.7) 18.1 (8.3) 17.9 (7.1) 17.6 (7.8) 0.91
Hamilton Depression Rating scale (HDRS), mean (S.D.) 15.4 (4.6) 15.0 (4.8) 15.2 (4.3) 16.0 (4.8) 0.31
Hamilton Anxiety Rating scale (HARS), mean (S.D.) 15.1 (5.3) 15.7 (5.3) 14.4 (5.1) 15.2 (5.5) 0.28
Symptom Check List Anxiety scale (SCL-90-Anx), mean (S.D.) 1.24 (0.7) 1.2 (0.6) 1.3 (0.7) 1.2 (0.7) 0.29
Symptom Check List Depression scale (SCL-90-Dep), mean (S.D.) 2.0 (0.7) 2.0 (0.8) 2.1 (0.7) 2.0 (0.7) 0.36
Global Assessment of Functioning scale (GAF), mean (S.D.) 55.5 (7.2) 56.3 (6.9) 55.0 (5.9) 55.0 (8.7) 0.37

Psychiatric history
Previous psychotherapy (%) 23.7 24.7 21.4 25.0 0.84
Psychotropic medication (%) 21.4 24.1 24.4 15.2 0.27
Psychiatric hospitalization (%) 1.2 0 0 4.0 0.04
Separation experiences at childhood (%) 44.6 49.4 45.2 38.8 0.38
Proportion of patients whose age at the onset of primary 62.9 61.9 65.1 61.5 0.88
psychiatric disorder under 22 years (%)
Duration of primary psychiatric disorder more than 5 years (%) 32.9 39.5 27.7 31.3 0.25

Personality and social functioning


Suitability for Psychotherapy Scale (SPS), (proportion of poor 17.9 17.2 17.9 18.8 0.97
score values, %)
Level of Personality Organization scale (LPO), mean (S.D.) 4.16 (0.68) 4.23 (0.70) 4.18 (0.67) 4.07 (0.68) 0.32
Quality of Object Relations Scale (QORS), mean (S.D.) 5.07 (0.63) 5.04 (0.55) 5.00 (0.70) 5.17 (0.62) 0.21
Immature defense style score (DSQ), mean (S.D.) 3.90 (0.74) 3.89 (0.81) 3.84 (0.71) 3.97 (0.70) 0.53
Affiliation toward self score (SASB), mean (S.D.) 4.89 (62.2) 16.6 (60.4) 0.22 (60.2) -2.43 (65.1) 0.11
Interpersonal problems score (IIP), mean (S.D.) 87.3 (31.0) 86.8 (31.9) 90.6 (30.6) 84.4 (30.3) 0.43
Social Adjustment Scale (SAS), mean (S.D.) 2.17 (0.38) 2.15 (0.40) 2.21 (0.37) 2.16 (0.38) 0.57
Sense of Coherence scale (SOC), mean (S.D.) 113 (20.4) 119 (20.6) 109 (19.4) 111 (20.1) 0.003
1022 P. Knekt et al. / Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027

3. Results improvement of functioning in short-term therapy during the first


year of follow-up was seen only in patients with higher perfor-
Descriptive variables of the 251 patients were presented in the mance IQ (Table 5).
whole sample and in tertiles of the full scale IQ in Table 1. The
means of the full scale IQ, the verbal IQ, and the performance IQ
were 110.4, 109.8, and 109.9, respectively (Table 2). The standard
score of different sub-tests in different psychotherapy groups 4. Discussion
ranged from 10.2 to 13.3. No significant differences were found
with regard to intelligence in different psychotherapy groups. As far as these authors know, the present study is the first to
The patients experienced and maintained a statistically signifi- compare the prediction of intelligence on the reduction of psy-
cant symptom reduction for all six measures, HDRS, HARS, GSI, BDI, chiatric symptoms in short-term and long-term psychotherapy.
DEP, and ANX, and a significant improvement in psychosocial We found that, although intelligence did not predict differences
functioning (GAF) at the measurement points during the 5-year between long- and short-term therapies during the first years of
follow-up in comparison with the baseline point in all four follow-up, individuals with higher intelligence were likely to
categories of the combined therapy (short- and long-term) and benefit more from long-term psychotherapy in the long run. For
intelligence (values under the median and above the median) people with lower intelligence, long-term therapy was no more
groups considered (Table 3). The difference in the changes between beneficial than short-term therapy at any follow-up point. This
short- and long-term therapies was not statistically significant finding may be explained by the suggestion that individuals with
between individuals in the higher and lower intelligence categories higher intelligence have more potential to obtain better results
over the total 5-year follow-up (P for interaction¼ 0.11–0.99). At from long-term therapy due to the fact that the explorative
single measurement points, however, differences emerged. orientation of long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy and psy-
During the first year of follow-up, short-term psychotherapy was choanalysis demands good capacity for abstract and metaphoric
more effective than long-term psychotherapy in the reduction of thinking and reflective functioning (Gabbard, 2004; Valbak, 2004).
depressive symptoms with a statistically significantly larger reduc- Accordingly, persons with higher intellectual capacity have more
tion at either the 7- or 12-month measurement point for all three potential for increasing their self-understanding and for gaining
depressive symptom measures, HDRS, BDI and ANX, in both full from the relational corrective experiences by a combination of
scale IQ groups (Table 3). No differences were found with regard to thorough explicit cognitive and implicit emotional learning (Wong
changes in anxiety symptoms. During the next 3 years of the and Haywood, 2012). Our study thus supports the importance of
follow-up no statistically significant differences were found taking the patient's level of intelligence into account when
between long- and short-term therapies in either IQ group. choosing whether to refer the patient to short-term therapy or
At the 5-year follow-up point, however, individuals with IQ to some other treatment.
above the median showed a statistically significant change in the The finding was more evident for the interviewer-rated mea-
three primary measures GAF (Mean difference (M):  6.11, 95% sures on symptoms (HDRS and HARS) and global functioning
confidence interval (CI):  10.8,  1.40), HDRS (M: 2.37, CI: 0.20, (GAF) than for the questionnaire measures BDI and SCL-90. It is
4.54) and HARS (M: 2.95 CI: 0.79, 5.12) in the long-term therapy possible that patients with higher intelligence might be able to
group in comparison with the short-term therapy group. The verbally present their symptoms and functional status in more
corresponding non-significant differences in individuals with IQ detail and complexity, thus increasing the interviewer's ability to
under the median were 3.04, 1.34 and 1.69, respectively. detect more problems in symptomatic and functional status,
Study of verbal IQ (Table 4) separately showed only potentially biasing the evaluation. However, the fact that the
minor differences with the full scale IQ (Table 3). In divergence, self-reported findings were in line with interview assessment
however, statistically significantly more symptom reduction and does not support this hypothesis.

Table 2
Full scale, verbal, and performance IQ and standard scores of part tests by psychotherapy group.

Intelligence test All Therapy group P-value for


heterogeneity
Solution- focused Short-term psycho- Long-term psycho- Psycho- analysis
(N ¼ 73) dynamic (N¼ 71) dynamic (N ¼ 66) (N ¼41)

WAIS-R full scale IQ, mean (S.D.) 110.4 (10.1) 110.0 (11.5) 111.2 (9.4) 108.6 (9.7) 112.5 (8.7) 0.21
WAIS-R verbal IQ, mean (S.D.) 109.8 (10.6) 109.5 (12.4) 110.2 (10.0) 107.9 (9.4) 112.9 (9.3) 0.12
WAIS-R performance IQ, 109.9 (10.8) 109.7 (10.8) 111.0 (10.8) 108.5 (11.8) 110.5 (9.4) 0.59
mean (S.D.)
Digit span, standard score, 10.9 (3.1) 10.9 (2.9) 10.4 (2.1) 11.4 (2.9) 0.33
mean (S.D.)
Arithmetic, standard score, 11.2 (3.0) 11.6 (2.4) 11.2 (2.9) 12.0 (2.3) 0.48
mean (S.D.)
Comprehension, standard score 11.8 (2.1) 12.0 (1.8) 11.6 (1.7) 12.4 (1.6) 0.11
mean, (S.D.)
Similarities, standard score mean 12.1 (2.7) 12.5 (2.3) 12.4 (2.3) 13.3 (2.1) 0.10
(S.D.)
Picture Completion, standard score, 10.5 (2.1) 10.9 (2.6) 10.2 (2.7) 10.7 (1.9) 0.38
mean (S.D.)
Picture Arrangement, standard 10.6 (3.1) 11.4 (2.6) 10.5 (2.8) 11.2 (2.7) 0.23
score, mean (S.D.)
Block Design, standard score, mean 10.9 (2.6) 11.6 (2.8) 11.7 (3.0) 12.0 (2.6) 0.16
(S.D.)
Digit Symbol, standard score, mean 11.8 (1.9) 11.6 (2.4) 11.9 (2.3) 12.0 (3.0) 0.85
(S.D.)
Table 3
Mean valuesa of psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial functions in short-term (S) and long-term therapy (L) groups and mean value differencesb (95% confidence intervals) at 7-, 12-, 24-, 36-, 48- and 60-month follow-up
according to the low and high values of patients' full scale IQ.

Measure Therapy Low full scale IQ (71–110) High full scale IQ (111–137) P-valuec

0 7 12 24 36 48 60 0 7 12 24 36 48 60

GSI S 1.27 0.81 0.83 0.86 0.83 0.73 0.64 1.24 0.93 0.83 0.84 0.80 0.74 0.70 0.99
L 1.39 1.04 1.00 0.90 0.84 0.72 0.62 1.28 0.99 0.95 0.78 0.72 0.57 0.55
S–L  0.18n  0.12  0.01 0.03 0.05 0.07  0.09  0.15 0.03 0.04 0.12 0.10
( 0.35, 0.03) (  0.30, 0.06) (  0.22, 0.20) (  0.19, 0.24) (  0.14, 0.25) (  0.12, 0.26) ( 0.26, 0.07) (  0.31, 0.02) (  0.16, 0.23) (  0.17, 0.24) (  0.06, 0.30) (  0.07, 0.28)
BDI S 17.3 9.32 9.38 9.04 8.80 7.83 8.15 17.0 10.0 9.22 8.79 9.36 7.96 8.61 0.50
L 20.1 13.7 12.7 10.2 9.99 7.06 6.27 17.6 12.0 11.3 8.02 7.41 4.92 5.49
S–L  2.99n  2.09 0.24 0.01 1.86 2.99  2.92n  2.93n  0.22 1.17 2.18 2.02
( 5.89,  0.09) (  5.14, 0.96) (  3.17, 3.64) (  3.68, 3.69) (  1.52, 5.24) (  0.26, 6.24) ( 5.62,  0.22) (  5.76, 0.10) (  3.36, 2.92) (  2.28, 4.61) (  0.93, 5.29) (  0.97, 5.02)
ANX S 1.24 0.89 0.80 0.81 0.81 0.68 0.62 1.27 0.98 0.90 0.87 0.77 0.72 0.66 0.99
L 1.17 0.89 0.90 0.76 0.76 0.66 0.52 1.30 1.01 0.96 0.79 0.67 0.50 0.50
S–L  0.17  0.12 0.03 0.02 0.00 0.08  0.08  0.11 0.04 0.06 0.16 0.12
( 0.39,0.05) (  0.34,0.11) (  0.23,0.29) (  0.24,0.28) (  0.22,0.23) (  0.15,0.32) ( 0.29,0.13) (  0.32,0.11) (  0.20,0.28) (  0.19,0.30) (  0.05,0.37) (  0.10,0.33)

P. Knekt et al. / Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027


HDRS S 14.4 9.66 10.6 9.88 8.33 15.9 10.7 10.6 10.6 10.3 0.49
L 15.4 11.7 12.2 10.0 7.99 15.8 12.5 12.4 9.21 7.51
S–L  1.53  1.00 0.34 0.79  0.97  2.19n 0.93 2.37n
( 3.40, 0.34) (  3.10, 1.09) (  2.17, 2.84) (  1.55, 3.13) ( 2.72, 0.79) (  4.16,  0.22) (  1.50, 3.37) (0.20, 4.54)
HARS S 14.5 9.12 9.31 9.13 7.61 15.2 11.4 10.9 9.89 10.1 0.54
L 15.7 11.4 11.0 9.03 7.51 15.2 11.7 11.9 8.70 7.03
S–L  1.59  1.04 0.72 0.73  0.41  1.07 0.97 2.95n
( 3.36, 0.19) (  3.00, 0.93) (  1.56, 2.99) (  1.61, 3.06) ( 2.08, 1.25) (  2.91, 0.78) (  1.22, 3.16) (0.79, 5.12)
DEP S 1.96 1.26 1.30 1.26 1.29 1.12 0.99 1.97 1.40 1.19 1.24 1.14 1.08 1.06 0.73
L 2.11 1.65 1.62 1.39 1.38 1.13 1.02 2.05 1.60 1.55 1.13 1.17 0.77 0.82
S–L  0.34n  0.26  0.07  0.04 0.04 0.02  0.23  0.41n 0.05  0.12 0.23 0.17
( 0.63,  0.05) (  0.57, 0.04) (  0.43, 0.29) (  0.40, 0.32) (  0.30, 0.37) (  0.30, 0.34) ( 0.50, 0.05) (  0.69,  0.12) (  0.29, 0.38) (  0.46, 0.22) (  0.08, 0.54) (  0.13, 0.47)
GAF S 56.6 67.5 68.0 69.0 74.1 55.0 63.4 63.4 66.6 68.4 0.11
L 55.9 62.8 62.0 66.0 69.9 54.6 64.4 62.0 66.0 69.9
S–L 3.46 4.70n 1.74 3.04 0.15 2.69  1.00  6.11n
( 0.21, 7.11) (0.63, 8.78) (  3.13, 6.60) (  2.02, 8.10) ( 3.29, 3.60) (  1.15, 6.53) (  5.73, 3.73) (  10.8,  1.40)

a
The full ITT model.
b
The full ITT model further including the baseline level of the outcome measure considered.
c
P-value for interaction between the full scale IQ measure and the therapy group throughout the follow-up.
n
P-value for difference between S and L o 0.05.

1023
1024
Table 4
Mean valuesa of psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial functioning short-term (S) and long-term therapy (L) and mean value differencesb (95% confidence intervals) at 7-, 12-, 24-, 36-, 48- and 60-month follow-up according to the
low and high values of patients' verbal IQ.

Measure Therapy Low verbal IQ (71–110) High verbal IQ (111–137) P-valuec

0 7 12 24 36 48 60 0 7 12 24 36 48 60

GSI S 1.27 0.87 0.83 0.90 0.87 0.76 0.67 1.24 0.89 0.82 0.81 0.78 0.73 0.68 0.92
L 1.32 1.02 1.01 0.92 0.80 0.71 0.59 1.29 1.01 0.94 0.77 0.77 0.59 0.58
S–L  0.13  0.16  0.01 0.08 0.07 0.10  0.13  0.12 0.04  0.01 0.12 0.08
( 0.30, 0.04) (  0.34, 0.02) (  0.22, 0.20) (  0.13, 0.29) ( 0.12, 0.26) (  0.08, 0.29) (  0.29, 0.03) (  0.29, 0.04) (  0.16, 0.23) (  0.21, 0.19) ( 0.06, 0.30) (  0.10, 0.25)
BDI S 17.4 9.87 9.79 9.53 8.88 8.28 9.13 17.0 9.66 8.84 8.38 9.39 7.68 7.77 0.30
L 19.3 13.3 12.5 9.88 9.24 6.27 5.22 18.3 12.4 11.5 8.29 8.29 5.78 6.52
S–L  2.90n  2.28 0.23 0.15 2.40 4.35n  2.84n  2.77  0.12 1.01 1.68 0.84
( 5.78,  0.03) (  5.81, 0.07) (  3.14, 3.59) (  3.53, 3.82) ( 0.92, 5.72) (1.20,7.51) (  5.56,  0.12) (  5.61, 0.07) (  3.27, 3.04) (  2.44, 4.45) ( 1.45, 4.80) (  2.19, 3.86)
ANX S 1.22 0.79 0.75 0.85 0.81 0.71 0.60 1.30 0.94 0.93 0.84 0.79 0.71 0.68 0.92
L 1.15 0.90 0.92 0.80 0.72 0.65 0.50 1.31 1.01 0.94 0.76 0.72 0.51 0.53
S–L  0.14  0.20 0.02 0.04 0.03 0.06  0.09  0.04 0.07 0.04 0.16 0.14
( 0.36, 0.09) (  0.43, 0.03) (  0.23, 0.28) (  0.22, 0.30) ( 0.19, 0.25) (  0.16, 0.29) (  0.30, 0.12) (  0.25, 0.18) (  0.18, 0.31) (  0.20, 0.29) ( 0.05, 0.37) (  0.08, 0.36)

P. Knekt et al. / Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027


HDRS S 14.6 10.4 11.0 9.72 8.84 15.8 11.3 10.1 10.7 10.1 0.76
L 15.2 11.9 12.6 9.93 8.28 12.3 12.0 9.27 7.24
S–L  0.88  0.95 0.51 1.34  1.48  2.38n 0.71 2.09
( 2.75, 0.99) (  3.05, 1.16) (  1.98, 3.01) (  0.95, 3.63) (  3.28, 0.33) (  4.38,  0.39) (  1.68, 3.10) (  0.14, 4.33)
HARS S 14.6 10.0 9.92 9.20 8.50 15.2 10.6 10.3 9.85 9.58 0.92
L 15.7 11.9 11.4 8.99 7.72 15.2 11.2 11.5 8.79 6.81
S–L  0.96  0.68 1.09 1.69  0.89  1.43 0.58 2.38n
( 2.74, 0.83) (  2.67, 1.31) (  1.19, 3.38) (  0.62, 3.99) (  2.60, 0.82) (  3.31, 0.44) (  1.60, 2.75) (0.13, 4.63)
DEP S 1.92 1.31 1.27 1.31 1.29 1.10 1.04 2.01 1.36 1.21 1.22 1.14 1.12 1.02 0.67
L 2.07 1.56 1.59 1.36 1.30 1.09 0.91 2.08 1.63 1.56 1.16 1.26 0.82 0.93
S–L  0.24  0.31  0.03 0.02 0.03 0.15  0.03n  0.38n 0.02  0.17 0.25 0.06
( 0.53, 0.05) (  0.61, 0.00) (  0.38, 0.33) (  0.34, 0.37) ( 0.30, 0.36) (  0.17, 0.45) (  0.58,  0.03) (  0.67,  0.10) (  0.31, 0.35) (  0.51, 0.16) ( 0.06, 0.56) (  0.24, 0.36)
GAF S 56.6 66.0 66.3 69.1 72.6 54.8 64.7 65.1 66.5 69.0 0.42
L 55.0 62.3 61.3 65.1 69.7 55.5 65.0 62.6 69.5 75.7
S–L 2.06 3.19 2.22 1.22 1.33 4.04n  1.22  5.30n
( 1.60, 5.72) (  0.93, 7.32) (  2.61, 7.05) (  3.77, 6.20) (  2.19, 4.85) (0.14, 7.95) (  5.86, 3.42) (  10.2,  0.44)

a
The full ITT model.
b
The full ITT model further including the baseline level of the outcome measure considered.
c
P-value for interaction between the verbal IQ measure and the therapy group throughout the follow-up.
n
P-value for difference between S and L o 0.05.
Table 5
Mean valuesa of psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial functioning in short-term (S) and long-term therapy (L) and mean value differencesb (95% confidence intervals) at 7, 12, 24, 36, 48 and 60 month follow-up according to the
lower and higher values of patients' performance IQ.

Measure Therapy Low performance IQ (71–110) High performance IQ (111–137) P-valuec

0 7 12 24 36 48 60 0 7 12 24 36 48 60

GSI S 1.27 1.07 0.87 0.92 0.85 0.74 0.72 1.23 0.86 0.79 0.78 0.78 0.71 0.63 0.32
L 1.29 0.99 0.93 0.79 0.85 0.70 0.59 1.32 1.04 1.02 0.84 0.72 0.59 0.58
S–L  0.09  0.06 0.13  0.01 0.03 0.13  0.17n  0.22n  0.09 0.06 0.12 0.06
(  0.26, 0.08) (  0.23, 0.16) (  0.08, 0.34) ( 0.22, 0.21) (  0.16, 0.29) (  0.07, 0.32) ( 0.34,  0.01) (  0.39,  0.06) (  0.29, 0.10) (  0.14, 0.25) ( 0.05, 0.30) (  0.12, 0.24)
BDI S 16.8 10.4 9.98 9.87 9.81 8.07 9.45 17.5 9.09 8.60 8.03 8.55 7.72 7.61 0.12
L 19.1 11.8 11.7 8.41 10.5 6.61 6.01 18.5 13.7 12.5 9.70 7.15 5.35 5.73
S–L  0.32  0.62 2.38 0.26 2.15 4.03n  5.29n  4.58n  2.25 0.89 1.88 1.26
(  3.16, 2.51) (  3.59, 2.34) (  0.99, 5.75) ( 3.42, 3.95) (  1.23, 5.54) (0.77, 7.29) ( 7.95,  2.62) (  7.39,  1.78) (  5.33, 0.83) (  2.48, 4.25) ( 1.24, 5.00) (  1.75, 4.26)
ANX S 1.29 0.86 0.91 0.95 0.86 0.75 0.73 1.23 0.86 0.79 0.75 0.73 0.65 0.57 0.60
L 1.22 0.94 0.87 0.74 0.79 0.61 0.54 1.25 0.97 0.98 0.80 0.64 0.53 0.47
S–L  0.11  0.00 0.17 0.01 (  0.25, 0.27) 0.08 0.14  0.13  0.22n  0.08 0.06 0.09 0.08
(  0.33, 0.11) (  0.23, 0.22) (  0.09, 0.43) (  0.14, 0.30) (  0.10, 0.37) ( 0.34, 0.08) (  0.43,  0.00) (  0.32, 0.16) (  0.18, 0.30) ( 0.11, 0.29) (  0.14, 0.29)

P. Knekt et al. / Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027


HDRS S 14.5 10.5 10.3 10.4 9.51 15.8 11.2 10.8 10.2 9.42 0.34
L 14.8 9.14 11.3 9.86 7.32 16.5 13.6 13.4 9.48 8.11
S–L  0.88  0.95 0.51 1.34  1.48  2.38n 0.71 2.09
(  2.75, 0.99) (  3.05, 1.16) ( 1.98, 3.01) (  0.95, 3.63) ( 3.28, 0.33) (  4.38,  0.39) (  1.68, 3.10) (  0.14, 4.33)
HARS S 14.5 9.68 9.24 9.17 8.87 15.2 10.9 10.9 9.90 9.21 0.78
L 15.1 10.5 10.9 9.16 7.21 15.8 12.6 12.1 8.77 7.26
S–L  0.34  1.21 0.38 1.97  1.49  0.99 1.19 2.21n
(  2.11, 1.43) (  3.17, 0.76) ( 1.93, 2.68) (  0.37, 4.31) ( 3.15, 0.17) (  2.84, 0.86) (  0.98, 3.36) (0.07, 4.34)
DEP S 1.95 1.37 1.33 1.32 1.31 1.12 1.13 1.98 1.30 1.15 1.19 1.11 1.06 0.94 0.16
L 2.04 1.58 1.49 1.22 1.36 1.06 0.94 2.11 1.61 1.66 1.28 1.19 0.84 0.89
S–L  0.20  0.15 0.12  0.05 0.05 0.20  0.35n  0.54n  0.12  0.12 0.19 0.02
(  0.49, 0.09) (  0.45, 0.14) (  0.24, 0.45) ( 0.41, 0.31) (  0.28, 0.39) (  0.13, 0.52) ( 0.62,  0.07) (  0.82,  0.26) (  0.45, 0.20) (  0.45, 0.21) ( 0.12, 0.50) (  0.28, 0.32)
GAF S 66.6 67.7 67.9 71.0 64.1 63.7 67.4 70.5 0.97
L 64.8 63.6 66.6 73.1 62.4 60.0 68.2 72.6
S–L 1.19 3.40 0.62  2.79 2.21 4.23n  0.37  1.82
(  2.45, 4.82) (  0,63, 7.43) ( 4.25, 5.49) (  7.93, 2.36) ( 1.19, 5.62) (0.43, 8.02) (  4.96, 4.22) (-6.52, 2.87)

a
The full ITT model.
b
The full ITT model further including the baseline level of the outcome measure considered.
c
P-value for interaction between the performance IQ measure and the therapy group throughout the follow-up.
n
P-value for difference between S and L o 0.05.

1025
1026 P. Knekt et al. / Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027

The faster early symptom reduction in short-term therapy In conclusion, individuals suffering from anxiety or depressive
did not depend on the intelligence level, implying that factors disorder with a higher intelligence benefitted more from long-
other than intelligence may be more important to successful term than from short-term psychotherapy. This finding suggests
short-term psychotherapy. This effect did not emerge, however, that the intelligence quotient could be a useful tool in identifying
with regard to anxiety symptoms, which is in accordance with patients who would benefit more from long-term than short-term
the general results on effectiveness of the HPS, which showed psychotherapy. Using this and other suitability criteria would help
that short-term psychotherapy was equally or more beneficial create clinically meaningful recommendations for treatment pro-
than long-term psychotherapy during the first year of follow-up, viders. This finding should, however, be confirmed in large-scale
especially in the reduction of depressive symptoms (Knekt et al., studies before firm conclusions can be made.
2008).
Previous research on the prediction of intelligence on therapy
outcome is limited. Apart from a dozen early studies of relatively
poor methodology, reviewed by Luborsky et al. (1996), which Acknowledgment
indicated intellectual functioning to be a moderate predictor of
treatment success in diverse, mostly short-term therapies, there The study was financially supported by the Academy of Finland
has been very limited interest during the last decades. A few (Grant no. 138876).
recent studies on mostly short-term cognitive therapies have
given controversial results on the prediction of intelligence on
treatment outcome (Haaga et al., 1991; Doubleday et al., 2002; References
Fournier et al., 2009; D’Alcante et al., 2012). No previous studies
are available on psychodynamic therapies or on comparisons Allen, J.G., Coyne, L., David, E., 1986. Relation of intelligence to ego functioning in an
between short- and long-term psychotherapy. The importance of adult psychiatric population. Journal of Personality Assessment 50, 212–221.
American Psychiatric Association (APA), 1985. The psychodynamic psychotherapies.
assessing patients' pre-treatment level of intelligence has been Criteria for their prescription and concurrent review, APA, Manual of Psychiatric
highlighted by a study indicating that the ability to benefit from Peer Review, 3rd ed. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, pp.
mental health services is better among patients with higher IQ 93–96.
American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
(4 110) (Slade et al., 2006), a level similar to what appeared to
Disorders. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
predict a different outcome in long-term versus short-term psy- Andrews, G., Pollock, C., Stewart, G., 1989. The determination of defense style by
chotherapy in this study. questionnaire. Archives of General Psychiatry 46, 455–460.
Antonovsky, A., 1993. The structure and properties of the sense of coherence scale.
The results on the prediction of IQ were largely similar when
Social Science and Medicine 36, 725–733.
the predictor variable was the full scale, the verbal, or the Azim, H.F., Piper, W.E., Segal, P.M., Nixon, G.W., Duncan, S.C., 1991. The quality of
performance quotient. However, for patients with higher pre- objects relations scale. Bulletin of the Meninger Clinic 55, 323–343.
treatment performance IQ, the benefits of short-term therapy Bacharach, H.M., Leaff, L.A., 1978. “Analyzability”: a systematic review of the clinical
and quantitative literature. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association
were more evident during the first year of follow-up than among 26, 681–920.
those with higher full scale IQ and verbal IQ, perhaps indicating Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., Erbaugh, J., 1961. An inventory for
their greater behavioral propensities, beyond verbal capacity, to measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 4, 561–571.
Benjamin, L.S., 1996. A clinician-friendly version of the interpersonal circumplex:
adopt the brief therapy orientation. structural analysis of social behavior (SASB). Journal of Personality Assessment
The present prospective study has several strengths: Firstly, we 66, 248–266.
used a validated intelligence assessment method and several D’Alcante, C.C., Diniz, J.B., Fossaluza, V., Batistuzzo, M.C., Lopes, A.C., Shavitt, R.G.,
et al., 2012. Neuropsychological predictors of response to randomized treat-
validated outcome measures, which were based on both inter- ment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology
views and questionnaires. Secondly, the relatively large sample & Biological Psychiatry 39, 310–317.
size, the long follow-up period, and the frequent assessments of Davanloo, H., 1978. Evaluation and criteria for selection of patients for short-term
dynamic psychotherapy. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 29, 307–308.
the outcome measures during follow-up gave a reliable basis for De Shazer, S., Berg, I.K., Lipchik, E., Nunnally, E., Molnar, A., Gingerich, W., et al.,
the analyses (Knekt et al., 2008). Finally, the small dropout, the 1986. Brief therapy: focused solution development. Family Process 24, 207–221.
wide selection of potential confounding factors considered, and Derogatis, L.R., Lipman, R.S., Covi, L., 1973. SCL-90: an outpatient psychiatric rating
scale – preliminary report. Psychopharmacology Bulletin 9, 13–28.
the inclusion of auxiliary treatment in the models minimized the
Doubleday, E.K., King, P., Papageorgiou, C., 2002. Relationship between fluid
risk of biased results. intelligence and ability to benefit from cognitive-behavioural therapy in older
There are, however, also issues that may complicate the adults: a preliminary investigation. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 41,
interpretation of the results. Firstly, despite the relatively large 423–428.
Fonagy, P., 2010. Psychotherapy research: do we know what works for whom?
sample the two short-term therapies and the long-term therapy British Journal of Psychiatry 197, 83–85.
and psychoanalysis groups were combined to achieve more Fournier, J.C., DeRubeis, R.J., Shelton, R.C., Hollon, S.D., Amsterdam, J.D., Gallop, R.,
strength in the statistical analyses. For this reason, possible 2009. Prediction of response to medication and cognitive therapy in the
treatment of moderate to severe depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
differences within these groups have been neglected in this study Psychology 77, 775–787.
and the emphasis was placed on the length of therapy. Secondly, Gabbard, G.O., 2004. Long-Term Psychodynamic Psychotherapy: A Basic Text.
residual confounding cannot be fully excluded despite the com- American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., Washington, DC.
Greenson, R.R., 1985. The Technique and Practice of psychoanalysis. The Hogarth
prehensive adjustment for potential confounding factors. Thirdly, Press and The Institute of Psycho-Analysis, London.
due to the multiple comparisons performed, the possibility of a Gunderson, J.G., Gabbard, G.O., 1999. Making the case for psychoanalytic therapies
chance finding cannot be excluded. Fourthly, despite the similarity in the current psychiatric environment. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
Association 47, 679–704.
of the results of the ITT and the AT analyses, lack of compliance Haaga, D.A., DeRubeis, R.J., Stewart, B.L., Beck, A.T., 1991. Relationship of intelligence
with the study treatment may potentially cause bias (Knekt et al., with cognitive therapy outcome. Behaviour Research Therapy 29, 277–281.
2011). Fifthly, although no notable differences were found Hamilton, M., 1959. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. British Journal of
Medical Psychology 32, 50–55.
between the results derived from the original and imputed data,
Hamilton, M., 1960. A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosur-
it cannot be excluded that still other than ignorable dropouts may gery & Psychiatry 23, 56–61.
bias the results (Harkanen et al., 2005). Finally, although the WAIS- Harkanen, T., Knekt, P., Virtala, E., Lindfors, O., 2005. A case study in comparing
R is considered a reliable measure giving an adequate estimate of therapies involving informative drop-out, non-ignorable non-compliance and
repeated measurements. Statistics in Medicine 24, 3773–3787.
the patients' intelligence for the purposes of this study, it does not Horowitz, L.M., Alden, L.E., Wiggins, J.S., Pincus, A.L., 2000. Inventory of the
give a complete picture of intelligence. Interpersonal Problems Manual. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio.
P. Knekt et al. / Psychiatry Research 220 (2014) 1019–1027 1027

Johnson, L.D., Miller, S.D., 1994. Modification of depression risk factors: a solution- Luborsky, L., Crits-Christoph, P., Mintz, J., Auerbach, A., 1996. Who will benefit from
focused approach. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training 31, psychotherapy? Predicting therapeutic outcomes. Basic Books, New York.
244–253. Malan, D.H., 1976. The Frontier of Brief Psychotherapy: An Example of the
Kernberg, O.F., 1996. A psychoanalytic theory of personality disorders. In: Clarkin, Convergence of Research and Clinical Practice. Plenum Medical Book, New York.
aJ.F., Lenzenweger, M.F. (Eds.), Major Theories of Personality Disorders. The Migon, H.S., Gamerman, D., 1999. Statistical Inference: An Integrated Approach.
Guilford Press, New York, pp. 106–140. Arnold, London.
Knekt, P., Lindfors, O., 2004. A randomized trial of the effect of four forms of Rizvi, S.L., Vogt, D.S., Resick, P.A., 2009. Cognitive and affective predictors of
psychotherapy on depressive and anxiety disorders. Design, methods, and treatment outcome in cognitive processing therapy and prolonged exposure
results on the effectiveness of short-term psychodynamic psychodynamic for posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 47, 737–743.
psychotherapy and solution-focused therapy during a one-year follow-up. Rothman, K.J., Greenland, S., 1998. Modern Epidemiology, second ed. Lippincott-
Available from 〈http://www.thl.fi/hps〉 (accessed 11.08.11.). Raven, Philadelphia.
Knekt, P., Lindfors, O., Harkanen, T., Valikoski, M., Virtala, E., Laaksonen, M.A., et al., Rubin, D.B., 1987. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. John Wiley,
2008. Randomized trial on the effectiveness of long-and short-term psycho- New York.
dynamic psychotherapy and solution-focused therapy on psychiatric symptoms SAS Institute Inc., 2008. SAS Online Doc. SAS/STAT User's Guide, Version 9.2. SAS
during a 3-year follow-up. Psychological Medicine 38, 689–703. Institute Inc., Cary, NC.
Knekt, P., Lindfors, O., Renlund, C., Sares-Jaske, L., Laaksonen, M.A., Virtala, E., 2011. Sifneos, P.E., 1978. Short-term anxiety provoking psychotherapy. In: Davanloo, H. (Ed.),
Use of auxiliary psychiatric treatment during a 5-year follow-up among
Short-Term Dynamic Psychotherapy. Spectrum, New York, NY, pp. 35–42.
patients receiving short- or long-term psychotherapy. Journal of Affective
Slade, M., Leese, M., Gillard, M., Kuipers, E., Thornicroft, G., 2006. Pre-morbid IQ and
Disorders 135, 221–230.
response to routine outcome assessment. Psychological Medicine 36, 1183–1191.
Knekt, P., Lindfors, O., Sares-Jäske, L., Virtala, E, Härkänen, T., 2013. Randomized trial
Valbak, K., 2004. Suitability for psychoanalytic psychotherapy: a review. Acta
on the effectiveness of long- and short-term psychotherapy on psychiatrics
Psychiatrica Scandinavica 109, 164–178.
symptoms and working ability during a 5-year follow-up. Nordic Journal of
Valkonen, H., Lindfors, O., Laaksonen, M.A., Knekt, P., 2012. Association between the
Psychiatry 67, 59–68.
Rorschach Ego Impairment Index (EII-2) and the Level of Personality Organiza-
Laaksonen, M.A., Knekt, P., Lindfors, O., 2013a. Psychological predictors of the recovery
tion. Psychiatry Research 200, 849–856.
from mood or anxiety disorder in short-term and long-term psychotherapy
Watzke, B., Ruddel, H., Jurgensen, R., Koch, U., Kriston, L., Grothgar, B., et al., 2010.
during a 3-year follow-up. Psychiatry Research 208, 162–173.
Laaksonen, M.A., Knek,t, P., Sares-Jäske, L., Lindfors, O., 2013b. Psychological Effectiveness of systematic treatment selection for psychodynamic and
predictors on the outcome of short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy and cognitive-behavioural therapy: randomised controlled trial in routine mental
solution-focused therapy in the treatment of mood and anxiety disorder. healthcare. British Journal of Psychiatry 197, 96–105.
European Psychiatry 28, 117–124. Wechsler, D., 1981. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised.
Laaksonen, M.A., Lindfors, O., Knekt, P., Aalberg, V., 2012. Suitability for Psychother- Psychological Corporation, New York.
apy Scale (SPS) and its reliability, validity, and prediction. British Journal of Weissman, M.M., Bothwell, S., 1976. Assessment of social adjustment by patient
Clinical Psychology 51, 351–375. self-report. Archives of General Psychiatry 33, 1111–1115.
Lee, J., 1981. Covariance adjustment of rates based on the multiple logistic Verbeke, G., Molenberghs, G., 1997. Linear mixed models in practice: an SAS-
regression model. Journal of Chronic Diseases 34, 415–426. oriented approach. Springer, New York.
Lindfors, O., Knekt, P., Virtala, E., Laaksonen, M.A., The Helsinki Psychotherapy Study Wong, P.S., Haywood, D.M., 2012. Foundations of psychodynamic therapy: implicit
Group, 2012. The effectiveness of solution-focused therapy and short- and long- emotional learning. In: Levy, R.A., Ablon, J.S., Kächele, H. (Eds.), Psychodynamic
term psychodynamic psychotherapy on self-concept during a 3-year follow-up. Psychotherapy Research: Evidence-based Practice and Practice-based Evidence.
The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 200, 946–953. Humana Press, New York, pp. 269–282.

You might also like