Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6 (2012) 1135–1141

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders


Journal homepage: http://ees.elsevier.com/RASD/default.asp

Early diagnosis of autism: Current status of the Baby and Infant Screen for
Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT-Parts 1, 2, and 3)
Johnny L. Matson *, Kim Tureck
Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, United States

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: The benefits of early intervention for very young children with autism are now well
Received 8 January 2012 established. Hand and hand with these interventions is the necessity of psychometrically
Accepted 27 February 2012 sound diagnostic tools. Not only should these tools be instrumental in differentiating
developmentally delayed and other at risk infants. These measures should also be
Keywords: structured to address challenging behaviors and psychopathology. The latter problems co-
BISCUIT occur at high rates in infants who evince autism. Being able to identify these behaviors and
Autism symptoms, and to monitor the effectiveness of interventions to remediate these problems,
Commorbity
is a high priority. The Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT), is
Challenging behaviors
designed to address all of the issues listed. At this writing, the BISCUIT has the best
psychometrics of any of the early childhood evaluation scales. Over 80 studies have been
published to date using the BISCUIT. The purpose of this paper was to review the current
status and future research trends using this test battery.
ß 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Autism spectrum disorders (referred to as autism going forward in this paper) are lifelong (Ben Itzchak, Lahat, & Zachor,
2011; Gillberg, 2010; Johansson, Gillberg, & Råstam, 2010; Matson & Kozlowski, 2011; Nydén et al., 2010; Wing, Gould, &
Gillberg, 2011). The condition is generally considered to be neurodevelopmental, but individuals do respond to behavioral
interventions (Rivet & Matson, 2011; Weinkauf, Zeug, Anderson, & Ala’i-Rosales, 2011). Among the characteristics which
distinguish the condition are deficits in communication and socialization (Clarke et al., 2011; Duffy & Healy, 2011; Fernell &
Gillberg, 2010; Gillberg, 2010; Kaland, 2011; Worley, Matson, Sipes, & Kozlowski, 2011). These individuals also evince a
number of aberrant behaviors such as stereotypies and repetitive behaviors, which are core features of the disorder (Briegel,
Schimek, & Kamp-Becker, 2010; Suzuki, 2011).
Other behaviors which are not core features of the disorder are often present. Among these problems are challenging
behaviors and co-occurring psychopathology (Bakken et al., 2010; Davis, Fodstad, et al., 2011; Funabiki, Kawagishi, Uwatoko,
Yoshimura, & Murai, 2011; Horovitz, Matson, Sipes, Shoemaker, et al., 2011; Lugnegård, Hallerbäck, & Gillberg, 2011; Mahan
& Matson, 2011; Matson, Belva, Hattier, & Matson, 2011; Matson, Boisjoli, Hess, & Wilkins, 2011; Poppes, van der Putten, &
Vlaskamp, 2010; Worley & Matson, 2011). Motor deficits, both fine and gross motor, and cerebral palsy, are also commonly
co-occurring conditions (Forti et al., 2011). Because of these core symptoms and co-occurring problems, autism is a condition
that requires both early identification and treatment, which continues into adulthood (Fava et al., 2011; Levy & Perry, 2011;
Lung, Chiang, Lin, & Shu, 2011; Matson, Rieske, & Tureck, 2011; Meyer, Ingersoll, & Hambrick, 2011).

* Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, United States.
E-mail address: johnmatson@aol.com (J.L. Matson).

1750-9467/$ – see front matter ß 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2012.02.009
1136 J.L. Matson, K. Tureck / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6 (2012) 1135–1141

The purpose of the present study was to provide an update on the current status of the Baby and Infant Screen for Children
with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT), which is the most heavily researched of the early identification scales. It is the most successful
and best established such measure to date.

1. Psychometrics

Before a new test can be used to explore characteristics of the disorder in question, basic psychometrics must be
established. Reliability, validity, establishing the factor structure of the measure, cut-offs, and norms are all needed to insure
a viable instrument (Matson & Sipes, 2010). The first study on the BISCUIT established the reliability of all three parts of the
test battery: (1) core symptoms of autism and PDD-NOS, (2) comorbid psychopathology, and (3) challenging behaviors. The
authors evaluated test data on 276 children who were 17 to 37 months of age (M = 26.83 months, SD = 5.27 months) (Matson,
Wilkins, et al., 2009). A subset of items from all three tests had robust reliability. Methods included removing items with very
low endorsements, followed by analyzing individual item correlations and internal reliability (internal consistency).
The next step in establishing the psychometrics of the BISCUIT involved a series of validity studies. Matson, Wilkins, and
Fodstad (2011) reported on the convergent and divergent validity of the BISCUIT Part 1 in a sample of 1007 children 17–37
months of age. BISCUIT scores converged with the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) and the Personal–Social
domain from the Battelle Developmental Inventory (Second Edition, BDI-2). (Deficits in social and communication skills are
core features of the disorder.) Divergent validity was demonstrated by low correlations with the Adaptive and Motor
domains of the BDI-2.
Sensitivity and specificity have also been established for the BISCUIT (Matson, Wilkins, et al., 2009). In this study, 1007
children ages 17–37 months were diagnosed with autism, PDD-NOS, or with no diagnosis by a licensed clinical psychologist
using data from the M-CHAT, DSM-IV-TR, and BDI-2. Independent reliability of 99% agreement was established with a second
Ph.D. level clinical psychologist who independently diagnosed 97 children using the same methods. From this sample, 178
were diagnosed with autism, 152 were diagnosed with PDD-NOS, and 677 received no diagnosis. Chi-square analyses were
run on all items of the BISCUIT. Every item significantly differentiated children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Cut-
off scores for autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS) were empirically
established, and then children in defined categories were compared to clinically derived diagnoses. The M-CHAT produced a
sensitivity of 74.1, specificity of 87.5, and correct classification rate of 83.0, while the BISCUIT values were 93.4 for sensitivity,
86.6 for specificity, and 88.8 for correct classification.
The results above were refined and expanded upon in a second study. Matson, Fodstad, and Dempsey (2009) tested 1957
infants 17–37 months of age. Study 1 produced results similar to those noted above on the BISCUIT, and additionally a subset
of items was found to distinguish between persons with an ASD versus those without this diagnosis. Study 2 compared
children diagnosed with autism to children diagnosed with PDD-NOS. (The no diagnosis group was excluded.) The purpose of
the study was to confirm that the BISCUIT was sufficiently sensitive to distinguish between these two highly prevalent ASDs.
This hypothesis was answered in the affirmative.
The notion of using a few items from the scale to identify children with ASD has also been explored. LoVullo and Matson
(2012) tested 2168 at risk infants, 17–37 months of age. They employed Discriminant Functional Analysis (DFA) and Receiver
Operating Characteristics (ROC) analysis. A 5 item scoring algorithm with comparable diagnostic accuracy to the entire
BISCUIT-Part 1 was established. Matson, Fodstad, et al. (2009) also showed that autism or PDD-NOS could be differentiated
from each other. The authors suggested that a few items could be used as a screener, with the entire scale being administered
to children identified as at risk. The additional items help further establish the diagnosis, and also provide a rich source of
information for treatment planning and for evaluating treatment effectiveness (by administering the BISCUIT battery post
intervention).
The BISCUIT has also been factor analyzed. Matson, Boisjoli, et al. (2011) employed 1287 children 12–37 months of age.
The exploratory factor analysis produced a three factor solution that mirrored the triad of core ASD symptoms;
communication, socialization, and stereotyped repetitive behaviors. All three factors yielded higher scores for those with an
ASD diagnosis versus those children without an ASD diagnosis. For Part 2 of the BISCUIT 270 infants with autism or PDD-NOS
were studied (Matson, Boisjoli, et al., 2011). A five factor solution was found: tantrum/conduct, inattention/impulsivity,
avoidance behavior, anxiety/repetitive behavior, and eating/sleeping problems.
Part 3 of the BISCUIT has also been factor analyzed. Matson, Boisjoli, Rojahn, and Hess (2009) evaluated 270 infants 17–37
months old. A three factor solution emerged and conceptually fit a priori established factors: (1) aggressive/disruptive
behavior, (2) stereotypic behavior, and (3) self-injurious behavior.
Cutoffs and norms have also been reported. Earlier in this section, the cut-offs and norms for Part 1 of the BISCUIT were
noted. Cut-offs and norms have also been reported for Parts 2 and 3. For Part 2, the psychopathology comorbidity scale, 309
children with and ASD diagnosis participated in establishing the cut-offs and norms (Matson, Fodstad, Mahan, & Sevin,
2009). Then 460 children, with and without an ASD were compared on all five factors of the BISCUIT-Part 2; tantrum, conduct
behavior, inattention/impulsive, avoidant behavior, anxiety/repetitive behavior, and eating problems. Children with an ASD
had higher scores on all five factors, compared to children without a diagnosis of ASD.
Cut-offs for total and subtest scores for the BISCUIT-Part 3 was reported for 312 children diagnosed with an ASD (Rojahn
et al., 2009). A second study on norms and cut-offs scores used a larger sample (644 children) with developmental delays
J.L. Matson, K. Tureck / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6 (2012) 1135–1141 1137

(with and without ASD) (Matson, Fodstad, Mahan, & Rojahn, 2010). In this study, the authors also note that aggressive/
destructive acts were the most common problems, and they could be readily identified in these very young children.

2. Diagnosis

The big issue in the area of diagnosis is early detection (Matson, Rieske, et al., 2011). This process can be made more
difficult when children are very young, because skill sets which help differentiate those persons with or without ASD have
not fully emerged. Additionally, intellectual disability (ID) can present with symptoms that mimic ASD, further confusing the
diagnostic picture (Rojahn & Matson, 2010). Finally, communication disorders may appear similar for one or two of the three
triads, particularly at young ages, and these must be taken into account when differentially diagnosing ASD (Matson & Neal,
2010). What we do know, however, is that this disorder and its associated behavioral symptoms can be identified at an early
age, and that these symptoms are stable over time (Matson & Horovitz, 2010; Worley, Matson, Mahan, Kozlowski, & Neal,
2011). The major exception to this finding is that a subset of children develop relatively normally, then regress with the
development of numerous ASD symptoms at roughly 20–24 months of age (Matson, Wilkins, & Fodstad, 2010).
Efforts to shape and sharpen diagnostic efforts for very young children have been enhanced by parent reports of first
concerns. Horovitz, Matson, and Sipes (2011b) assessed 1393 toddlers. They noted clear and more pronounced deficits in
socialization, non-verbal communication, and repetitive behavior in children with ASD than for children with other types of
developmental disabilities. These findings were then replicated with the overall subscale scores of the BISCUIT (Horovitz
et al., 2011b). These deficits occur on a number of parameters. For example, while communication problems appear at an
early age for developmentally delayed children in general, they are apparent at the youngest ages for those infants with ASD
(Horovitz, Matson, & Sipes, 2011c; Kozlowski, Matson, Horovitz, Worley, & Neal, 2011). Finally, more severe core symptoms
of ASD are related to co-occurring conditions such as psychopathology (Matson, Mahan, et al., 2011). The BISCUIT has
excellent sensitivity and specificity. It has proven useful in differentiating an ASD from other developmental problems such
as hearing impairments and more general atypical development (Worley, Matson, & Kozlowski, 2011). Additionally, the
BISCUIT has proven to have higher sensitivity and specificity than other well-established early diagnosis/screening measures
(Kozlowski, Matson, Worley, Sipes, & Horovitz, 2012).

3. Prevalence and core symptoms

The BISCUIT has been used to help establish rates of ASD in infants at risk for developmental delays. Worley, Matson, and
Kozlowski (2011) assessed 2027 children 17–37 months of age. As anticipated, rates of ASD were higher for this at risk group
compared to a general population of older children, which is how most prevalence data has been reported. However, male to
female ratios were lower than in previous studies, 1.16 to 1.
A number of other interesting papers have emerged with this age group. For example, communication levels of 168
toddlers with challenging behaviors have been studied (Matson, Boisjoli, & Mahan, 2009). Lower levels of expressive and
receptive communication were correlated with more aggressive, disruptive, and self-injurious behavior. These deficits in
communication along with social deficits are evident at a very early age (Fodstad, Matson, Hess, & Neal, 2009).
Hattier and Matson (2012) evaluated 591 toddlers 17–37 months old with autism, PDD-NOS, or no ASD. Communication
impairments were evaluated with the BISCUIT and the BDI-2. Deficits were greatest for children with autism, followed by
PDD-NOS. Deficits were greater for overall score than for specific items (Horovitz & Matson, 2010). Communication deficits
were the mildest for children at risk for developmental disabilities, but without a diagnosis of ASD. These communication
deficits can also compound other problems such as anxiety symptoms (Davis et al., 2012). More severe symptoms were
found to be related to greater levels of inattention and impulsivity. Hattier, Matson, May, and Whiting (in press) further
noted that repetitive/restrictive behaviors were more common in children with ASD versus cerebral palsy (CP). Interaction
effects are evident between multiple skill areas as well. Thus, not only do children with autism evince greater problems with
communication and social behaviors than persons with CP, they also experience more fine and gross motor problems (Sipes,
Matson, & Horovitz, 2011). Similarly, the co-occurrence of CP with autism resulted in more communication deficits
compared to autism alone (Hattier, Matson, & Kozlowski, 2012). However, the combinations of these problems appears to be
particularly debilitating since children with CP alone had fewer communication deficits than children with Down Syndrome
or children with a history of seizures based on results of the BISCUIT (Hattier, Matson, Sipes, & Turygin, 2011).

4. Relationship to other problems and disorders

A substantial number of studies have been published which look at challenging behaviors (CB) using Parts 1 and 3 of the
BISCUIT. Stereotypic behaviors, for example, were highest for persons with the most severe autism symptoms (Matson,
Dempsey, & Fodstad, 2009). Developmental quotient has also been demonstrated to be a factor in severity of CB. Medeiros,
Kozlowski, Beighley, Rojahn, and Matson (2012) studied 1509 infants 17–36 months of age. They found that atypically
developing infants with no specific diagnosis evinced fewer CB with higher developmental quotient, while children with
autism showed greater CB with higher developmental quotients.
Hattier, Matson, Belva, and Horovitz (2011) evaluated 2131 toddlers 17–37 months old (633 with an ASD and 1498 with
atypical development but no diagnosed ASD). Children with an ASD evinced greater numbers of CB and were also more likely
1138 J.L. Matson, K. Tureck / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6 (2012) 1135–1141

to display multiple CBs. These trends in CB are evident even when CB are divided into age cohorts of 12–18, 19–25, 26–32,
and 33–39 months of age (Fodstad, Rojahn, & Matson, in press). Similarly, lower levels of adult interaction and peer
interactions have been found to be associated with higher levels of stereotypic behavior, aggression, destruction, and self-
injurious behavior (Matson, Neal, Fodstad, & Hess, 2010).
Tantrums and conduct problems are more prevalent when autistic symptoms are more severe (Sipes, Matson, Horovitz, &
Shoemaker, 2011). However, children with autism, Down syndrome, general developmental delay, prematurity, cerebral
palsy, and seizure disorders did not differ by group (Sipes, Rojahn, Turygin, Matson, & Tureck, 2011). Additionally, CB for
these very young children with ASD did not differ by gender (Kozlowski & Matson, 2012). Gender differences were absent on
Part 3 of the BISCUIT for 17–37 month olds with cerebral palsy, Down syndrome, or seizure disorder.
Comorbid psychopathology is also a common problem among persons with autism (Smith & Matson, 2010). An
interaction effect between comorbid symptoms of psychopathology and CB among small children with ASD has also been
studied. Children who scored high on avoidance and tantrum/conduct problems had more aggressive/destructive behaviors,
self-injurious behaviors, and stereotypies. High levels of inattention/impulsivity were also related to a range of CB, while
high rates of anxiety/repetitive behavior only covaried with stereotypies in the CB category (Matson, Mahan, Fodstad, Hess, &
Neal, 2010). Anxiety and CB have also been found to co-occur in high rates on the BISCUIT for 17–37 month old (Matson,
Mahan, et al., 2011). These trends continue across the lifespan (Davis, Hess, Moree, et al., 2011).
Information on psychopathology among very young children with ASD is scant (Matson, Hess, & Boisjoli, 2010). However,
the BISCUIT is providing some useful insights in this area. For example, all types of comorbid psychopathology measured by
the BISCUIT Part 2 are more pronounced in atypically developing children with an ASD versus those who do not have an ASD
(Matson, Hess, et al., 2010). Additionally, Matson, Hess, et al. (2010) evaluated 324 infants and toddlers who had no ASD
diagnosis or who were diagnosed with autism or PDD-NOS. Tantrum/conduct problems, inattention/impulsivity, and eating/
sleeping problems were most common and serious for children diagnosed with autism, followed by those diagnosed with
PDD-NOS. Children at risk for developmental disabilities, but without an ASD had the fewest symptoms of comorbid
psychopathology. Fodstad, Rojahn, and Matson (2010) replicated these findings. Furthermore, they broke children into
groups by age (12–18, 19–24, 25–31, and 32–39 months of age). Comorbid psychopathology symptoms increased with age.
Many of these symptoms tended to be largely associated with autism. For example, developmental quotient had little effect
on symptoms of inattention and impulsivity, while symptoms of autism did affect those behaviors (Matson, Mahan, Hess, &
Fodstad, 2010). Matson, Worley, Neal, Mahan, and Fodstad (2010) studied 240 toddlers 17–37 months of age who were
diagnosed with ASD. They found that those toddlers with fewer symptoms of ASD and inattention/impulsivity had
significantly fewer deficits in social skills.
A series of studies have also found a strong relationship between anxiety and ASD in this very young population using the
BISCUIT. Anxiety and avoidance symptoms were found to be more prevalent among infants and toddlers with autism or PDD-
NOS compared to typically developing infants without an ASD (Davis, Fodstad, et al., 2011). Second, these symptoms are
present at a very early age and persist across the lifespan (Davis, Hess, Moree, et al., 2011). Communication, one of the core
deficits of autism also covaries with ASD and anxiety. Davis, Moree, et al. (2011) found that anxiety decreased as
communication deficits increased. And, this trend was more pronounced for infants and toddlers with autism compared to
children with PDD-NOS or no ASD diagnosis. Both receptive and expressive skills are affected by anxiety in these children
(Davis et al., 2012). Moreover, infants and toddlers with greater numbers of anxiety symptoms had significantly lower
developmental quotients as measured on the BDI-2 (Davis, Hess, Matthews, et al., 2011).

5. Comorbid physical problems

A number of studies have been published looking at a variety of physical conditions often co-occurring with an ASD. For
example, seizures co-occur at high rates among persons with ASD. Matson, Neal, Hess, Mahan, and Fodstad (2010) noted that
when both of these disorders co-occured, deficits in adaptive, personal-social, communication, motor and cognitive skills
were greater than when either problem occurred alone.
ASD also appears to have a significant effect on developmental milestones with greater severity of autism resulting in
more delays in fine and gross motor skills. This result was observed in a sample of 1044 infants and toddlers aged 17–37
months who were at risk for developmental delays (Matson, Mahan, Kozlowski, & Shoemaker, 2010). These findings are
similar to those reported by Matson, Mahan, Fodstad, et al. (2010), who noted that children with autism evinced more fine
and gross motor deficits than atypically developing infants and toddlers. However, the differences between fine and gross
motor skills did not differ within groups. Other factors found to co-occur with autism are low birth weight, premature birth,
birth defects, and obesity (Matson, Matson, & Beighley, 2011).

6. Demographic variables

Studies have been conducted establishing demographic factors that are associated with autism. The preponderance of
evidence suggests that males are much more likely to evince autism across the lifespan (Rivet & Matson, 2011). Several
studies have been conducted to look at gender difference with the BISCUIT. For example, the age of first concern for autism
was significantly younger in girls than boys (Horovitz, Matson, Turygin, & Beighley, 2012). Second, externalizing and
internalizing CB did not differ by gender for 17–37 months old, but children with ASD, regardless of sex were more likely to
J.L. Matson, K. Tureck / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6 (2012) 1135–1141 1139

evince CB than children without ASD (Kozlowski, Matson, & Rieske, 2012). It was also noted that African Americans evince
more CB than Caucasians (Horovitz, Matson, Rieske, Kozlowski, & Sipes, 2011). Finally, on the triad of impairments
(communication, socialization, and repetitive/restrictive behaviors), only restrictive behaviors differed. Females had
significantly fewer endorsements than males (Sipes, Matson, Worley, & Kozlowski, 2011). Additionally, psychiatric
symptoms did not differ by gender in these very young children (Worley & Matson, 2011). Thus, gender differences on ASD
are evident only during some periods of life (Horovitz, Matson, & Sipes, 2011c). It is important to add that symptom
endorsements differ between mothers and fathers (Matson, Hess, Kozlowski, & Neal, 2011). Finally, children with a biological
family member with ASD were compared to infants and toddlers with no family member diagnosed with ASD using the
BISCUIT (Kozlowski, Matson, & Worley, 2012). A larger percentage of children with an ASD had a biological relative with the
condition compared to the group of atypically developing children. However, symptom expression for ASD did not differ
depending on whether there was or was not a relative with ASD.

7. Conclusions

The BISCUIT at this point is an extensively studied measure of ASD for young children. The psychometrics of this test
battery have been studied in a number of papers and are well established. Additionally, the test has proven useful in assisting
in diagnosis and evaluation of a variety of characteristics of infants and toddlers with ASD. One of the most striking findings
in the aggregate is that many characteristics of the disorder, and the co-morbid conditions that accompany them, are present
very early in life. Without effective intervention these problems are also highly persistent and durable. The assumption that
the child will ‘‘grow’’ out of these problems is not tenable.
Further research with the BISCUIT is underway. Among the most interesting developments is a 15 nation study using the
scaling system for early detection and diagnosis. Statewide and countrywide systems are in place or being put in place for
early screening and diagnosis of ASD. Scales that focus on diagnosis at early ages are likely to take on added importance in the
future.

References

Bakken, T. L., Helverschow, S. B., Eilersten, D. E., Heggelund, T., Myrbakk, E., & Martinsen, H. (2010). Psychiatric disorders in adolescents and adults with autism and
intellectual disability: A representative study in one county in Norway. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 1669–1677.
Ben Itzchak, E., Lahat, E., & Zachor, D. A. (2011). Advanced parental ages and low birth weight in autism spectrum disorders—Rates and effect on functioning.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1776–1781.
Briegel, W., Schimek, M., & Kamp-Becker, I. (2010). Moebius sequence and autism spectrum disorders—Less frequently associated than formerly thought. Research
in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 1462–1466.
Clarke, M. T., Newton, T., Griffiths, K., Price, A., Lysley, K. V., & Petrides, K. V. (2011). Factors associated with the participation of children with complex
communication needs. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 774–780.
Davis, T. E., III, Fodstad, J. C., Jenkins, W. S., Hess, J. A., Moree, B. N., Dempsey, T., et al. (2011). Anxiety and avoidance in infants and toddlers with autism spectrum
disorders: Evidence for differing symptom severity and presentation. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 305–313.
Davis, T. E., III, Hess, J. A., Matthews, R. A., Fodstad, J. C., Dempsey, T., Jenkins, W. S., et al. (2011). The moderating effect of anxiety on development in atypically
developing toddlers. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33, 171–177.
Davis, T. E., III, Hess, J. A., Moree, B. N., Fodstad, J. C., Dempsey, T., Jenkins, W. S., et al. (2011). Anxiety symptoms across the lifespan in people diagnosed with
autistic disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 112–118.
Davis, T. E., III, Moree, B. N., Dempsey, T., Hess, J. A., Jenkins, W. S., Fodstad, J. C., et al. (2012). The effect of communication deficits on anxiety symptoms in infants
and toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Behavior Therapy, 43, 142–152.
Davis, T. E., III, Moree, B. N., Dempsey, T., Reuther, E. T., Fodstad, J. C., Hess, J. A., et al. (2011). The relationship between autism spectrum disorders and anxiety: The
moderating effect of communication. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 324–329.
Duffy, C., & Healy, O. (2011). Spontaneous communication in autism spectrum disorder: A. review of topographies and interventions. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 5, 977–983.
Fava, L., Strauss, K., Valeri, G., D’Elia, L., Arima, S., & Vicari, S. (2011). The effectiveness of a cross-setting complementary staff- and parent-mediated early intensive
behavioral intervention for young children with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1479–1492.
Fernell, E., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Autism spectrum disorder diagnoses in Stockholm preschoolers. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 680–685.
Fodstad, J. C., Matson, J. L., Hess, J., & Neal, D. (2009). Social and communication behaviours in infants and toddlers with autism and pervasive developmental
disorder-not otherwise specified. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12, 152–157.
Fodstad, J. C., Rojahn, J., & Matson, J. L. The emergence of challenging behaviors in at-risk toddlers with and without autism spectrum disorders: A cross-sectional
study. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, in press.
Fodstad, J. C., Rojahn, J., & Matson, J. L. (2010). Emergent comorbidity in at risk children with and without ASD: A cross sectional study. Journal of Developmental and
Physical Disabilities, 22, 381–400.
Forti, S., Valli, A., Perego, P., Nobile, M., Crippa, A., & Molteni, M. (2011). Motor planning and control in autism: A kinematic analysis of preschool children. Research
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 834–842.
Funabiki, Y., Kawagishi, H., Uwatoko, T., Yoshimura, S., & Murai, T. (2011). Development of a multi-dimensional scale for PDD and ADHD. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 32, 995–1003.
Gillberg, C. (2010). The ESSENCE in child psychiatry: Early symptomatic syndromes eliciting neurodevelopmental clinical examinations. Research in Develop-
mental Disabilities, 31, 1543–1551.
Hattier, M. A., & Matson, J. L. (2012). An examination of the relationship between communication and socialization deficits in children with autism and PDD-NOS.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 871–880.
Hattier, M. A., Matson, J. L., Belva, B. C., & Horovitz, M. (2011). The occurrence of challenging behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders and atypical
development. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 221–229.
Hattier, M. A., Matson, J. L., & Kozlowski, A. M. (2012). Communication skills in children with cerebral palsy and autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 24, 85–93.
Hattier, M. A., Matson, J. L., May, A. C., & Whiting, S. E. Repetitive/restrictive behaviors and interests in children with cerebral palsy and autism. Developmental
Neurorehabilitation, in press.
1140 J.L. Matson, K. Tureck / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6 (2012) 1135–1141

Hattier, M. A., Matson, J. L., Sipes, M., & Turygin, N. (2011). Communication deficits in infants and toddlers with developmental disabilities. Research in
Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2108–2113.
Horovitz, M., & Matson, J. L. (2010). Communication deficits in babies and infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified
(PDD-NOS). Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 390–398.
Horovitz, M., Matson, J. L., Rieske, R., Kozlowski, A. M., & Sipes, M. (2011). The relationship between race and challenging behaviors in infants and toddlers with
autistic disorder and pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 208–214.
Horovitz, M., Matson, J. L., & Sipes, M. (2011b). Gender differences in symptoms of comorbidity in toddlers with ASD using the BISCUIT-Part 2. Developmental
Neurorehabilitation, 14, 94–100.
Horovitz, M., Matson, J. L., & Sipes, M. (2011c). The relationship between parents’ first concern and symptoms of autism spectrum disorders. Developmental
Neurorehabilitation, 14, 372–377.
Horovitz, M., Matson, J. L., Sipes, M., Shoemaker, M., Belva, B., & Bamburg, J. W. (2011). Incidence and trends in psychopathology symptoms over time in adults
with severe to profound intellectual disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 685–692.
Horovitz, M., Matson, J. L., Turygin, N., & Beighley, J. S. (2012). The relationship between gender and age of first concern in toddlers with autism spectrum
disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 466–471.
Johansson, M., Gillberg, C., & Råstam, M. (2010). Autism spectrum conditions in individuals with Möbius sequence. CHARGE syndrome and oculo-auriculo-
vertebral spectrum: Diagnostic aspects. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 9–24.
Kaland, N. (2011). Brief report: Should Asperger syndrome be excluded from the forthcoming DSM-V? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 984–989.
Kozlowski, A. M., & Matson, J. L. (2012). An examination of challenging behaviors in autistic disorder versus pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise
specified: Significant differences and gender effects. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 319–325.
Kozlowski, A. M., Matson, J. L., Horovitz, M., Worley, J. A., & Neal, D. (2011). Parents’ first concerns of their child’s development in toddlers with autism spectrum
disorders. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 72–78.
Kozlowski, A. M., Matson, J. L., & Rieske, R. D. (2012). Gender effects on challenging behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 6, 958–964.
Kozlowski, A. M., Matson, J. L., & Worley, J. A. (2012). The impact of familial autism diagnoses on autism symptomology in infants and toddlers. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 6, 151–157.
Kozlowski, A. M., Matson, J. L., Worley, J. A., Sipes, M., & Horovitz, M. (2012). Defining characteristics for young children meeting cutoff on the Modified Checklist
for Autism in Toddlers. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 472–479.
Levy, A., & Perry, A. (2011). Outcomes in adolescents and adults with autism: A review of the literature. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1271–1282.
LoVullo, S. V., & Matson, J. L. (2012). Development of a critical item algorithm for the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 6, 378–384.
Lugnegård, T., Hallerbäck, M. U., & Gillberg, C. (2011). Psychiatric comorbidity in young adults with a clinical diagnosis of Asperger syndrome. Research in
Developmental Disabilities, 32, 1910–1917.
Lung, F.-W., Chiang, T.-L., Lin, S.-J., & Shu, B.-C. (2011). Autism-risk screening in the first 3 years of life in Taiwan Birth Cohort Pilot Study. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1385–1389.
Mahan, S., & Matson, J. L. (2011). Convergent and discriminant validity of the Autism Spectrum Disorder-Problem Behavior for Children (ASD-PBC) against the
Behavioral Assessment System for Children, second edition (BASC-2). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 222–229.
Matson, J. L., Belva, B., Hattier, M. A., & Matson, M. L. (2011). Pica in persons with developmental disabilities: Characteristics, diagnosis, and assessment. Research in
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1459–1464.
Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J. A., Hess, J. A., & Wilkins, J. (2011). Comorbid psychopathology factor structure on the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm
Traits-Part 2 (BISCUIT-Part 2). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 426–432.
Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J., & Mahan, S. (2009). The relation of communication and challenging behaviors in infants and toddlers with autism spectrum disorders.
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 21, 253–261.
Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J., Rojahn, J., & Hess, J. (2009). A factor analysis of challenging behaviors assessed with the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm
Traits (BISCUIT-Part 3). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 714–722.
Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T., & Fodstad, J. C. (2009). Stereotypies and repetitive/restrictive behaviours in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder.
Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12, 122–127.
Matson, J. L., Fodstad, J. C., & Dempsey, T. (2009). The relationship of children’s feeding problems to core symptoms of autism and PDD-NOS. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 3, 759–766.
Matson, J. L., Fodstad, J. C., Mahan, S., & Rojahn, J. (2010). Cutoffs, norms, and patterns of problem behaviours in children with developmental disabilities on the
Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT-Part 3). Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 3–9.
Matson, J. L., Fodstad, J. C., Mahan, S., & Sevin, J. A. (2009). Cutoffs, norms, and patterns of comorbid difficulties in children with an ASD on the Baby and Infant
Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT-Part 2). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 977–988.
Matson, J. L., Hess, J., & Boisjoli, J. A. (2010). Comorbid psychopathology in infants and toddlers with autism and pervasive developmental disorders-not otherwise
specified (PDD-NOS). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 300–304.
Matson, J. L., Hess, J. A., Kozlowski, A. M., & Neal, D. (2011). An examination of differences in symptom endorsements of autism spectrum disorders: A comparison
between mothers and fathers. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 1–7.
Matson, J. L., & Horovitz, M. (2010). Stability of autism spectrum disorders symptoms over time. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22, 331–342.
Matson, J. L., & Kozlowski, A. M. (2011). The increasing prevalence of autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 418–425.
Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Fodstad, J. C., Hess, J. A., & Neal, D. (2010). Motor skill abilities in toddlers with autistic disorder, pervasive developmental disorder-not
otherwise specified, and atypical development. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 444–449.
Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Fodstad, J. C., Worley, J. A., Neal, D., & Sipes, M. (2011). Effects of symptoms of comorbid psychopathology on challenging behaviours
among infants and toddlers with autistic disorder and PDD-NOS as assessed with the Baby and Infant Screen of aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT). Developmental
Neurorehabilitation, 14, 129–139.
Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Hess, J. A., & Fodstad, J. C. (2010). Effect of developmental quotient on symptoms of inattention and impulsivity among toddlers with
autism spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 464–469.
Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Kozlowski, A. M., & Shoemaker, M. (2010). Developmental milestones in toddlers with autistic disorder, pervasive developmental
disorder-not otherwise specified and atypical development. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 239–247.
Matson, M. L., Matson, J. L., & Beighley, J. S. (2011). Comorbidity of physical and motor problems in children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32,
2304–2308.
Matson, J. L., & Neal, D. (2010). Differentiating communication disorders and autism in children. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 626–632.
Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Fodstad, J. C., & Hess, J. A. (2010). The relation of social behaviors and challenging behaviors in infants and toddlers with autism spectrum
disorders. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 164–169.
Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Hess, J. A., Mahan, S., & Fodstad, J. C. (2010). The effect of seizure disorder on symptom presentation in atypically developing children and
children with autism spectrum disorders based on the BDI-2. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 310–314.
Matson, J. L., Rieske, R. D., & Tureck, K. (2011). Additional considerations for the early detection and diagnosis of autism: Review of available instruments. Research
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1319–1326.
Matson, J. L., & Sipes, M. (2010). Methods of early diagnosis and tracking for autism and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS).
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22, 343–358.
Matson, J. L., Wilkins, J., & Fodstad, J. C. (2010h). Children with autism spectrum disorders: A comparison of those who regress versus those who do not.
Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 37–45.
J.L. Matson, K. Tureck / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6 (2012) 1135–1141 1141

Matson, J. L., Wilkins, J., & Fodstad, J. C. (2011g). The validity of the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits: Part 1 (BISCUIT-Part 1). Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1139–1146.
Matson, J. L., Wilkins, J., Sharp, B., Knight, C., Sevin, J. A., & Boisjoli, J. A. (2009). Sensitivity and specificity of the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm
Traits (BISCUIT). Validity and cutoff scores for autism and PDD-NOS in toddlers. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 924–930.
Matson, J. L., Worley, J. A., Neal, D., Mahan, S., & Fodstad, J. C. (2010). The effects of inattention/impulsivity and ASD symptom severity on social skills in toddlers.
Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 13, 408–412.
Medeiros, K., Kozlowski, A. M., Beighley, J. S., Rojahn, J., & Matson, J. L. (2012). The effects of developmental quotient on challenging behaviors in toddlers with
developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 1110–1116.
Meyer, K. A., Ingersoll, B., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2011). Factors influencing adjustment in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1413–1420.
Nydén, A., Niklasson, L., Stahlberg, O., Anckarsater, H., Wentz, E., Rastam, M., et al. (2010). Adults with autism spectrum disorders and ADHD neuropsychological
aspects. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 1659–1668.
Poppes, P., van der Putten, A. J., & Vlaskamp, C. (2010). Frequency and severity of challenging behaviour in people with profound intellectual and multiple
disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 1269–1275.
Rivet, T. T., & Matson, J. L. (2011). Review of gender differences in core symptomology in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,
957–976.
Rojahn, J., & Matson, J. L. (2010). Assessment and diagnosis of autism and spectrum disorders in children. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22,
313–315.
Rojahn, J., Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Fodstad, J. C., Knight, C., Sevin, J. A., et al. (2009). Cutoffs, norms, and patterns of problem behaviors in children with an ASD on
the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT-Part 3). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 989–998.
Sipes, M., Matson, J. L., & Horovitz, M. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders and motor skills: The effect on socialization as measured by the Baby and Infant Screen
for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT). Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 290–296.
Sipes, M., Matson, J. L., Horovitz, M., & Shoemaker, M. (2011). The relationship between autism spectrum disorders and symptoms of conduct problems: The
moderating effect of communication. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 54–59.
Sipes, M., Matson, J. L., Worley, J. A., & Kozlowski, A. M. (2011). Gender differences in symptoms of autism spectrum disorders in toddlers. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1465–1470.
Sipes, M., Rojahn, J., Turygin, N., Matson, J., & Tureck, K. (2011). Comparison of problems behaviors in atypical developing infants and toddlers as assessed with the
Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT). Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 261–266.
Smith, K. R. M., & Matson, J. L. (2010). Psychopathology: Differences among adults with intellectually disabled, comorbid autism spectrum disorders and epilepsy.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 743–749.
Suzuki, M. (2011). Mental development and autistic behavior in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,
1517–1525.
Weinkauf, S. M., Zeug, N. M., Anderson, C. T., & Ala’i-Rosales, S. (2011). Evaluating the effectiveness of a comprehensive staff training package for behavioral
interventions for children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 864–871.
Wing, L., Gould, J., & Gillberg, C. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders in the DSM-V: Better or worse than the DSM-IV? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32,
768–773.
Worley, J. A., & Matson, J. L. (2011). Psychiatric symptoms in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders: An examination of gender differences. Research
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1086–1091.
Worley, J. A., Matson, J. L., & Kozlowski, A. M. (2011). The effects of hearing impairment on symptoms of autism in toddlers. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14,
171–176.
Worley, J. A., Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Kozlowski, A. M., & Neal, D. (2011). Stability of symptoms of autism spectrum disorders in toddlers: An examination using the
Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits-Part 1. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 36–40.
Worley, J. A., Matson, J. L., Sipes, M., & Kozlowski, A. M. (2011). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in toddlers receiving early intervention services. Research
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 920–925.

You might also like