Oxford University Press Journal of Consumer Research

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Innovativeness: The Concept and Its Measurement

Author(s): David F. Midgley and Grahame R. Dowling


Source: Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 4, No. 4 (Mar., 1978), pp. 229-242
Published by: Oxford University Press
Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/2488813
Accessed: 07-03-2020 06:59 UTC

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Oxford University Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access
to Journal of Consumer Research

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Innovativeness: The Concept
and Its Measurement

DAVID F. MIDGLEY
GRAHAME R. DOWLING*

The nature of innovativeness, and its relationship to adoption, are explored


in this article. It is argued that innovativeness should be conceptualized at a
higher level of abstraction, and that explicit recognition should be given to the
complex communication processes intervening between this construct and
observable behavior.

A QUESTIONABLE ASSUMPTION as those listed in Rogers and Shoemaker. These char-


acteristics relate to major functional or discontinuous
There is little doubt that the study of the adoption ofand not necessarily to all the various new
innovations
new products and the innovativeness of individual con- products studied in the marketing literature, many of
sumers is well established in the literature. Since 1970, which are minor product modifications. However, it
there have been in excess of 25 articles, research notes, appears more likely that the fundamental difficulty lies
and communications on this topic in relevant journals.1
in the definition and measurement of the innovativeness
While the study of new product adoption has aided in construct.
discovering the characteristics of consumer innova-
It can be argued that researchers in this area make
tors, it has also initiated a series of controversies. Con- two implicit assumptions. First, that innovativeness is
sider some examples. We have seen dispute over the a personality trait possessed, to a greater or lesser
relation between innovativeness and psychological
degree, by all members of a society, and second, that
variables (Robertson and Myers 1969, 1970; Bruce and
what is being measured (in the reported studies) is, in
Witt 1970; Blake, Perloff, and Heslin 1970; Jacoby
fact, this trait. Our notion of a personality trait here
1971; Coney 1972), over possible innovativeness over- is one of a persisting characteristic or disposition by
lap (Ostlund 1972, 1973; Tatham and Dornoff 1973),
which one individual can be distinguished from another
and over the characteristics of the buyers of a major (English and English 1958; Wolman 1973), where in-
automobile innovation (Feldman and Armstrong 1975a, novativeness is viewed as a continuum from high to
1975b; Peat, Gentry and Brown 1975). Robertson (1971,
low. These assumptions are central both to the theory
p. 100-1), Pizam (1972, p. 203-9) and Engel, Kollat, of innovative behavior as it currently stands, and per-
and Blackwell (1973, p. 600) have tabulated the con- haps more importantly to the research methodologies
fused and contradictory nature of the empirical studies employed in the published studies of new product
relating consumer innovativeness to sociodemographic, adoption.
attitudinal, and personality factors. For such factors
In this respect, it should be noted that the underlying
we have numerous studies suggesting both positive and
goal of most reported studies is to establish criteria for
negative relationships with innovativeness and many
segmenting the consuming population into innovators
indicating no relationship whatsoever. Rogers and
and noninnovators. This is combined with the assump-
Shoemaker (1971, p. 352-375) provide a similarly con-
tion that any criteria thus established will be applicable
fused picture for the field of diffusion studies as a
to future new products and therefore useful from the
whole. As far as consumer innovativeness is con-
point of view of product development, market re-
cerned, part of the problem may stem from an inappro- search, media selection, etc., i.e., an individual's in-
priate selection of variables from such characteristics
novativeness is a persisting characteristic. Many such
studies also implicitly make the second, and in our view
* David F. Midgley is Lecturer, Australian Graduate School of weaker, assumption of direct correspondence between
Management, University of New South Wales, P. O. Box 1, Kensing- observable behavior and the trait innovativeness.
ton, New South Wales, Australia 2033. Grahame R. Dowling is
Lecturer, Department of Commerce, University of Newcastle,
New South Wales, Australia 2308. 1 The Journal of Consumer Research, Journal of Marketing, and
Journal of Marketing Research.

229 ? JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH * Vol. 4 . March 1978

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
230 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE A adoption is concerned has distinct limitations. By elect-


THE EXISTING MODEL OF INNOVATIVENESS ing for this particular, perhaps radical, stance it is
hoped to stimulate further discussion on this vital topic.
PSYCHOLOGICAL CIOLOGICAL In one small way, we are also taking up the recent
TRAITS TRAITS
plea by Rogers (1976) for a more process-oriented ap-
CONSTRUCT proach to research in this field.

FIELD { INNOVATIVENESS
THE MEASUREMENT OF
--- --- --- ---- ---1 ---- - -- - - - INNOVATIVENESS
TRAIT MEASURE, TRAIT MEASURE,
i.e., QUESTIONNAIRE i.e., QUESTIONNAIRE Rogers and Shoemaker (1971 p. 27) define innova-
tiveness as "the degree to which an individual is rela-
PHENOMENA
PLANE
tively earlier in adopting an innovation than other mem-
bers of his system," with an accompanying footnote to
the effect that: "By relatively earlier is meant earlier in
terms of actual time of adoption, rather than whether
INNOVATIVENESS MEASURE, i.e., the individual perceives he adopted the innovation rela-
RELATIVE TIME OF ADOPTION
tively earlier than others in his system." This is essen-
tially an operational definition since it is couched di-
The purpose of this paper is to question the validity rectly in terms of the measurement of innovativeness,
of the second assumption. It will be argued that some viz., the time taken for an individual to adopt.
contemporary methodologies do not provide reliable This temporal conception of innovativeness is cen-
measurements of a personality trait, "innovativeness," tral to much of the published research (Rogers 1976
and indeed that much of the current practice regarding p. 295) and leads directly to the employment of the
the definition and measurement of innovativeness is "relative time of adoption" methodology in many stud-
tautological. The prime focus of our attack is on meth- ies. By and large, in the marketing literature re-
odologies which equate time of adoption with innova- searchers have used two main techniques to measure
tiveness, and in doing so ignore the dynamic social innovativeness, either a variant of the time of adoption
processes which characterize the diffusion of innova- method, or what might be termed the "ownership of
tions. Essentially, the conceptualization of innovative- new products" or "cross-sectional" method ("cross-
ness accepted by many previous authors (depicted in sectional" after Robertson and Myers.1969). The first
Figure A) is that of a simplistic trait-behavior model, technique usually involves defining as innovators those
and as such represents a theoretical stance largely dis- individuals who purchase in the first X weeks, months,
credited in other areas of the behavioral sciences (e.g., etc., after product launch or those individuals who con-
Mischel 1968; Peterson 1968). stitute the firstX percent of a given market to purchase.
What is observable is the act and time of adoption or The second technique basically involves determining
purchase of a new product. Innovativeness itself is a how many of a prespecified list of new products a par-
hypothetical construct postulated to explain and/or pre- ticular individual has purchased at the time of the
dict such observable phenomena, but existing only in survey.
the mind of the investigator and at a higher level of Some examples from the literature will serve to dem-
abstraction. A "hypothetical construct" is an interpre- onstrate the prevalence of these techniques. Haines
tive concept which refers to some unobservable prop- (1966) used purchasers of products which had been on
erty of objective reality, but which can be related to the market "several months," while Donnelly and
human experience via some intervening theoretical sys- Ivancevich (1974) and Peat, Gentry, and Brown (1975)
tem (a view of science mainly derived from the work defined innovators as those who purchased in the first
of Bunge 1967). Thus, the construct, innovativeness, three and four months, respectively. Feldman and
only has meaning within the relevant theoretical sys- Armstrong (1975a) defined "early buyers" as the first
tem, that is, the theory of the diffusion of innovations. 2,500 to purchase an automobile innovation. Bell
The essence of the argument in this paper is that the (1963), Robertson (1968), and Robertson and Kennedy
trait-behavior model is an inadequate representation of (1969) used a 10 percent market penetration definition.
this system, and that in the context of any specific Those using the cross-sectional or ownership method
innovation, complex situational and communication ef- include Summers (1971, 1972), Darden and Reynolds
fects intervene between individuals' innovativeness (1974), Green, Langeard, and Favell (1974), Green and
and their observed time of adoption. Some of the de- Langeard (1975), King and Sproles (1973), and Baum-
fects of the current model of innovative behavior stem garten (1975).
from the operationalist philosophy adopted by many In fact, this paper cites 23 single methodology studies
researchers, a philosophy which the authors do not of innovativeness thus providing a reasonably compre-
share, and which as far as the study of new product hensive, although not exhaustive, survey of the litera-

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
INNOVATIVENESS 231

ture.2 Of these studies, 48 percent utilized the "relative ever, the view that it is a surrogate for time of adoption
time of adoption technique," 39 percent the cross-sec- is a misconception; its value derives because it meas-
tional technique, and the remainder used purchase in- ures a more meaningful construct of innovativeness.
tentions as a measure of innovativeness. Very little As will be discussed subsequently, if we admit a more
space is devoted in these publications to the nature of complex model of innovative behavior than the simple
innovativeness as a construct (Darden and Reynolds trait-behavior scheme, it becomes necessary to take ac-
being one exception); in particular, few explicit defini- count of the effects of situational factors and interper-
tions of innovativeness are evident. However, implicit sonal communication. In turn, this implies that the in-
within all is the notion of innovativeness as relative novators of one new product are not necessarily the
time of adoption. Phrases such as "early purchasers," innovators of the next product introduced into the cate-
''early triers," "adopt new ideas or products earlier," gory, and indeed, they may not even be adopters of the
"how likely she was to buy in the first weeks" are next product innovation. The worth of the cross-sec-
common, and denote these researchers' temporal con- tional technique is that by taking a large battery of new
ception of innovativeness. This conclusion is further products in a particular category, we control for some
reinforced by noting that 70 percent of these studies of the situational and communication effects associated
cite the seminal work of Rogers, either as Rogers (1962) with each individual product on the list. The technique
or as Rogers and Shoemaker (1971). Thus, by implica- also controls for respondent interest in the product
tion there is widespread acceptance of the temporal category as a whole.
concept and definition of innovativeness just given. In essence, the cross-sectional technique measures
Few, if any, alternative conceptualizations have ap- a deeper and more abstract construct of innovative-
peared in the literature. ness, and one which is closer to some basic expression
One important distinction which can be made be- of an individual's personality. Instead of having only
tween these 23 studies concerns the level of generality one expression of this personality trait as observed be-
or abstraction at which innovativeness was measured. havior we have several, thus, gaining confidence in the
The majority of the researchers utilizing relative time obtained measure. However, to further confuse the is-
of adoption (i.e., by respondent recall or time of pur- sue, we also have studies of the type exemplified by
chase data) are studying innovativeness in the context Summers (1971) which seek to detect innovativeness
of a single product innovation. Those employing the across several product categories. This is innovative-
cross-sectional technique are, by the very nature of this ness implicitly conceived and measured at a third, yet
measurement device, studying innovativeness with re- higher, level of abstraction, namely, with respect to all,
spect to a product category. Now, to a certain extent, or at least many, consumer product categories, thus
researchers using the cross-sectional methodology ap- approaching the idea of innovativeness as a generalized
pear to view this merely as a convenient surrogate personality trait. Therefore, while it would appear that
for a direct measure of time of adoption. For instance, most researchers may have basically the same temporal
Summers (1971 p. 314) provides the rationale: concept of innovativeness, the measures they use oper-
ate at different levels of abstraction. We have direct
Since respondents whose average time to adoption is
shorter tend to own more products, innovativeness measures of time of adoption with respect to single
scores for each product category were based on the new products and cross-sectional measures applied
number of products reported owned from lists included both to individual product categories and across several
in the questionnaire. such categories.
Carried to its logical conclusion, this argument would
In his discussion of methodologies, Robertson (1971
imply that the different measurement techniques really
p. 90) states:
relate to different constructs of innovativeness, and
However, it also may be expected that at a given point therefore, that studies using different methodologies
of time, individuals who are typically the first to adopt could not be expected to yield comparable results.
will also have adopted more items.
There is some support for this conclusion in previous
There is little doubt that the cross-sectional tech- empirical research. Kohn and Jacoby (1973, p. 837) re-
nique suffers from fewer problems of respondent recall port a test of three measures of innovativeness applied
than does the direct time of adoption method and may to five product categories in an experimental situation.
therefore be a more practical measure to employ. How- The measures were:
A. purchase or failure to purchase an innovation alterna-
2 Baumgarten 1975; Bell 1963; Blake, Perloff, and Heslin 1970; tive within the context of a shopping simulation
Coney 1972; Darden and Reynolds 1974; Donnelly and Ivancevich experiment;
1974; Feldman and Armstrong 1975a; Green, Langeard, and Favell
1974; Green and Langeard 1975; Haines 1966; Jacoby 1971; King B. the score on a self-designation measure of innova-
and Sproles 1973; Lambert 1972; Ostlund 1972, 1974; Peat, Gentry, tiveness; and
and Brown 1975; Peterson 1973; Robertson 1968; Robertson and
Myers 1969; Robertson and Kennedy 1968; Summers 1971, 1972; C. the score on a questionnaire designed to assess past
and Taylor 1977. innovative purchasing behavior.

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
232 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Measure C is the cross-sectional method in the context novativeness as noted earlier, a view which leads im-
of a product category, while Measure A is the closest mediately to the well-known adoption curve and to the
analog to the time of adoption of a single product that classification of individual adopters by use of standard
is possible in an experimental situation. Low correla- deviations from the average time of adoption. Given a
tions were found between the three measures, leading normal adopter distribution then, one finds 2.5 percent
Kohn and Jacoby to conclude that the methods were of the adopting population being classed as innovators,
not measuring the same hypothetical constructs. 13.5 percent as early adopters, and so on.
The fundamental issue is the construct validity of our Indeed, given this distribution, it is possible to carry
measurement devices (Cronbach and Meehl 1955). The Rogers' and Shoemaker's ideas to their logical conclu-
work of Kohn and Jacoby suggests that we have em- sion and define a standardized innovativeness (relative
pirical evidence, as well as compelling logical argu- time of adoption) score as follows:
ments, for concluding that the relative time of adoption
I = (m - t)ls, (1)
and cross-sectional techniques relate to substantially
different concepts of innovativeness. Some limited evi- where I is one current metric for innovativeness, m is
dence to the contrary is provided by Uhl, Andrus, and the mean time of adoption for the social system, s the
Poulsen (1970, p. 51) who in a study of grocery in- standard deviation of adoption times and t the time at
novation laggards stated that these two methods yielded which the particular individual in question adopts.
"approximately the same results." However, it is diffi- Thus, an individual gaining a score greater than or
cult to extrapolate from laggards to innovators, and equal to 2 is an innovator in the Rogers and Shoemaker
therefore, difficult to give this evidence much weight. scheme. Equation (1) represents a symbolic summary
For major farming innovations, Rogers and Rogers of the construct of innovativeness most widely ac-
(1961) concluded that the cross-sectional method is cepted by contemporary researchers. 'As one of the
roughly consistent with both self-image and the ratings main methods by which this construct is measured, it is a
of external judges, but unfortunately, they did not summary which exposes the theoretical and logical de-
examine time of adoption techniques. Overall, then, ficiencies of this essentially operationalist approach.
there does not appear to be any strong empirical The definition of innovativeness given at the start of this
evidence for the proposition that the two main section was effectively in terms of relative time of adop-
measurement devices relate to the same construct. tion. Yet, the above measure is also in terms of relative
It is more likely that measurements made at the three time of adoption, i.e., innovativeness is what we meas-
levels of abstraction relate to three innovativeness con- ure and what we measure is innovativeness.
structs, each with a different domain or extension, This tautology is a barrier to progress in the field. By
namely, a construct related to specific single products, anchoring the construct directly to its measurement,
a construct related to single product categories, and a researchers employing this relative time of adoption
construct related to several product categories (a gen- methodology have rendered their version of innova-
eralized innovativeness construct). By implicitly as- tiveness innovation specific, leading to severe prob-
suming that the various techniques are measuring the lems of inter-study comparison. The authors accept
same generalized construct, we have made the current that (m - t)ls is a valid, reliable, and useful measure,
concept of innovativeness extensionally vague. And but only of overt behavior with respect to a specific
any concept which is extensionally vague is automati- innovation, and not necessarily of a meaningful con-
cally intensionally vague, i.e., its characteristics or at- struct of innovativeness. To explain reality we need
tributes are imprecisely specified (Bunge 1967, p. 100). constructs postulated at a higher (nonobservable) level
This is exactly the situation at present in regard to the of abstraction which, precisely because they are not
"known" characteristics of innovativeness. tied to specific innovations or specific measurement
At this juncture, we could follow an operationalist devices, can explain both individuals' overt behavior
line of reasoning: define three constructs in terms of the over several innovations and the measurements we ob-
ways they are measured and subsequently study these tain with different methodologies. An obvious corollary
constructs as separate entities. However, this would is the relevant theoretical system by which such hypo-
seem philosophically unsound and likely to lead to fur- thetical constructs may be linked to a specific situation
ther confusion. An operationalist will only ascribe or measurement.
meaning to concepts which can be measured directly, These arguments should not be taken to imply that
and will only define these concepts in the exact terms of some variables cannot be defined in terms of their meas-
their measurement. Thus temperature measured by a urement. Rather, such definition is only legitimate for
mercury thermometer would not be synonymous with variables with a relatively low level of abstraction
temperature measured by thermoelectric effects. (height, income, pupil dilation, purchase, etc.), that is,
The danger in this philosophy is that it introduces concepts which may serve to link our theories to em-
circularity into our thinking and lowers all concepts to pirical data. However, epistemologically, such low-
the plane of empirical experience. This can be seen if level variables are of minor importance. Where we
we re-examine Rogers' and Shoemaker's view of in- have a hypothetical construct, such as innovativeness,

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
INNOVATIVENESS 233

which is central to the theory of the diffusion of innova- By doing so, it will be possible both to suggest the level
tions, it is desirable not only that the construct and of abstraction at which the construct innovativeness
measurement be logically distinct (Rozeboom 1966 p. should be postulated, and to investigate the links be-
206), but also that the "rules of correspondence" link- tween this particular construct and the measure of overt
ing construct to data be specified (Margenau 1935; Car- behavior, that is, (m - t)ls.
nap 1956).
By measuring innovativeness in several situations THE MEANING OF INNOVATIVENESS
researchers using cross-sectional methodologies have,
whether this was recognized or not, made the distinc- The theory of innovative behavior presented by
tion between construct and measurement. Their con- Rogers and Shoemaker (1971) is built around certain
struct of innovativeness is less situation specific and social processes, a major one being interpersonal com-
therefore at a higher level of abstraction, wherein lies munication and its associated influence over the atti-
the greater value of these methods as opposed to rela- tudes and behavior of individuals. About 25 percent of
tive time of adoption techniques. Unfortunately, and the empirical generalizations presented by Rogers and
as with other methods, researchers using cross-sec- Shoemaker are concerned with interpersonal com-
tional measurements have generally failed to articulate munication, and, as Midgley (1977, p. 36) notes it
their notion of innovativeness and to specify their rules would be difficult to account for the observed non-
of correspondence. Presumably, this failure is a result linear cumulative adoption curves without this key
of viewing the cross-sectional method as merely a sur- process. In situations where there is little or no inter-
rogate for the more direct methodologies, i.e., a result personal influence, this schema could not be expected
of the operationalist temporal concept of innovative- to apply, and indeed it would be philosophically un-
ness common to most research in the field. sound to utilize the theory outside its valid domain.3 We
This operationalist notion of innovativeness also im- therefore restrict the following arguments to markets
plies that we may only ascribe meaning to those con- where word-of-mouth communication is a factor in
structs which can be observed and measured or em- adoption decisions.
pirically tested in some way. This confuses meaning Hence, the diffusion of a new product is, to a con-
with testability. It would be more philosophically sound siderable extent, determined by a communication proc-
to view meaning as deriving from the incorporation of ess in which individual experiences with the product
the construct into a theoretical system whose overall are disseminated verbally through a particular social
adequacy is judged by empirical tests. We cannot test system. While it is considered that the mass media
concepts in isolation, only in their relevant context and mostly generates awareness, it is chiefly the favorable
only by generating testable statements from these con- personal recommendation of a socilal contact which is
cepts. Innovativeness or intelligence are not in them- thought to be instrumental in influencing an individual
selves testable, but the statement, "intelligence is posi- to adopt.4 This conclusion fits not only with the evi-
tively correlated with innovativeness," is testable and dence cited later, but also with a plausible economic
derived from the theory of the diffusion of innovations. theory which views advertising as informative rather
Bunge (1967, p. 159) defines a scientifically valid con- than persuasive (Nelson 1975). It is generally con-
cept as one which is both intensionally and exten- sidered that the persuasive impact of interpersonal
sionally precise, and which occurs within some theo- communication increases as the risk perceived in the in-
retical system. Therefore, the precision of measure- novation by an individual increases, and therefore, has
ment derives not only from the measurement devices greatest impact for products such as durables. How-
themselves, but also from the adequacy of the theory ever, this notwithstanding, some studies show signifi-
itself, and in particular, the adequacy of the links be- cant word-of-mouth effects for supposedly low-risk in-
tween hypothetical constructs and those limited as- novations (Arndt 1967a; Sheth 1971; and Day 1974).
pects of experience which can actually be measured. Day also suggests that advertising may have a subsidi-
Thus, the construct validity of a measurement device ary role of stimulating some interpersonal com-
essentially reduces to an issue of the adequacy of the munication.
relevant theory or model.
In our context the debate can be centered on whether 3 Such low interpersonal influence situations are characterized
time of adoption is solely and directly related to in- by a linear cumulative adoption curve (Mendez 1968).
4 Witness the 18 studies cited by Rogers and Shoemaker (1971,
novativeness as a personality trait (trait-behavior
p. 382, Generalization 8-1), "Mass media channels are relatively
model), whether it is merely a reaction to the extrinsic more important at the knowledge function and interpersonal
situation (situational model), or whether it is related channels are relatively more important at the persuasion func-
to a combination of both trait and situation (interaction tion in the innovation-decision process." In the marketing litera-
ture, the relative effectiveness of word-of-mouth communication
or contingent model). But to progress any further with
has been documented by Whyte (1954), Haines (1966), Arndt
these arguments it is first necessary to elucidate the (1967a, 1967b), Engel, Blackwell, and Kegerreis (1969), and
meaning of innovativeness -as an integral and central Sheth (1971). In the social psychological literature, see Katz and
construct of the theory of the diffusion of innovations. Lazarsfeld (1955) and the review by McGuire (1969).

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
234 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

The "innovators" of any one innovation are those Thus, the specific communication network generated
members of society prepared to adopt this new product during the diffusion of any innovation is an important
early in its diffusion, and therefore without the personal determinant of the observed individual adoption times,
or social support gained from discussions with prior a conclusion which holds solely on the basis of our
users.5 It is widely accepted that the messages these assumption of a key role for interpersonal communica-
innovators transmit to others initiate the interper- tion in the adoption decision process. Furthermore, as
sonally influenced adoption process.6 If these messages a consequence of this communication dependence, a
are favorable to the product, further adoption is en- particular individual's time of adoption may vary con-
sured, if unfavorable, the product may well be a failure. siderably between innovations, since it is likely that the
While this outline of the diffusion of innovations is of messages relating to different product innovations
necessity brief, and therefore simplified, it is sufficient reach the individual via different paths on each occa-
to lead us to one important conclusion. If the majority sion. This is particularly so given the complex feed-
of any population only adopts after receipt of favorable back effects in the communication system-since fu-
interpersonally-communicated information, then the ture adoption is dependent on the messages existing at
time of adoption is dependent on when this message (or a point in time, and these messages are in turn de-
messages) was received. While this may be a reflection pendent on previous adoption.
of personal characteristics (self-confidence, gregarious- This view of the diffusion of innovations is some-
ness, inner or other directedness, etc.), it is chiefly what akin to a "contagious" social experience transfer
dependent on the unique path by which the message or learning process leading to models such as those of
reaches any particular individual. This path will be Bass (1969) and Midgley (1976). Hence, it may be ex-
largely situationally, if not randomly, determined. For tremely difficult to establish a clear-cut connection be-
many individuals to adopt requires, among other tween a personality characteristic and time of adoption,
noncommunication factors, that: for intervening between the two is this complex com-
munication process. For example, in the context of
* they discuss the new product in question with another
individual (determined by gregariousness, interest,
two successive innovations an individual with a certain
the discussion situation, etc.); degree of empathy may display entirely different rela-
tive times of adoption, or in the context of one innova-
* this other person is willing to transmit favorable in- tion, a dogmatic individual may receive the necessary
formation on the product (determined by their experi-
influence before someone possessing a lesser degree of
ence or knowledge of it, etc.);
this characteristic. Other possibilities are not difficult to
* the receiver is receptive to such influence at that point envisage.
in time (determined by source credibility, persuasibil- The only individuals who will not display this com-
ity, message content, etc.); and munication dependence will be those who do not utilize
* the individual is in, or will be in, a situation where interpersonal information in their decision making.
purchase can follow the receipt of favorable informa- This provides us with our first insight into the nature
tion. of innovativeness. However, before pursuing this in-
sight it is worth noting, albeit briefly, that communica-
Put another way, if interpersonal information is most
tion is not the only variable intervening between per-
effective at the persuasion stage in an individual's adop- sonal characteristics and time of adoption; there are
tion decision process, then these conditions are prereq- also interest and other situational factors. On the basis
uisites for their favorable persuasion. Other prereq- of his study of generalized innovators (cross-sec-
uisites would include that the individual had been
tionally defined and measured) Summers (1971, p. 316)
made aware of and become interested in the innova-
suggests:
tion (presumably via mass media channels). More rele-
vantly, the time of persuasion is dependent on the time . . . innovativeness may be a function both of situa-
elapsed before message reception. tional variables, such as income and product involve-
ment, and behavioral considerations. It may be that situ-
ational factors are unique to specific products and prod-
5 Witness the eight studies cited by Rogers and Shoemaker uct categories and serve to constrain the individual's
(1971, p. 383, Generalization 8-3), "Mass media channels are rela-
innovativeness to particular areas, while his behavioral
tively more important than interpersonal channels for earlier
(sociological, psychological, etc.) make-up influences
adopters than for later adopters."
6 As evidenced by, among others, Bell (1963), Green and Langeard
his basic tendency to innovate.
(1975). Rogers and Shoemaker (1971, p. 375, Generalization 5-3,
42 studies supporting) provide some evidence that earlier adopters There is thus, a substantial argument for stating that
are also more likely to be opinion leaders. Evidence from the time of adoption has no direct or simple relationship
marketing literature (Robertson and Myers 1969; Summers 1971; to an individual's personal characteristics (and hence
and Baumgarten 1975) indicates a small, but significant overlap
between innovativeness and opinion leadership. As Midgley (1977,
with innovativeness if conceptualized at this high level
p. 72) notes, only a small effect is necessary to account for ob- of abstraction, especially in the context of any specific
served diffusion processes. single innovation). For a new product, the observed

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
INNOVATIVENESS 235

pattern of adoption is a complex function of product and Shoemaker, we propose to call this particular con-
interest, individual situations, personal characteristics, struct "innate innovativeness," while what is observed
and most importantly for the majority of the population, and measured as innovative behavior will be termed
a network of interpersonal influence. For the re- "actualized innovativeness" (Midgley 1977, p. 107).
mainder, i.e., those individuals who do not use inter- This distinction is crucial, for between innate and
personal communication, time of adoption is some actualized innovativeness lies a complex communica-
function of product interest, individual situations, and tion and influence process. While actualized innova-
personal characteristics. tiveness can be validly measured by Equation (1) e.g.,
What then is the nature of innovativeness? Since AI = (m - t)ls, it is incorrect to equate this to innate
the diffusion of innovations is a communication proc- innovativeness (II). In this conceptualization, innate
ess, it seems intuitively appealing to define the con- innovativeness is some inverse function of a threshold
struct in communication terms. Following this line of amount of interpersonal information required by an
reasoning, Midgley (1977, p. 49) has advanced one pos- individual, that is,
sible definition of innovativeness as:
II = f(I/E), (2)
Innovativeness is the degree to which an individual
where E is the quantum of communicated experience
makes innovation decisions independently of the com-
required by the individual.
municated experience of others.
For any one innovation the relationship between in-
"Communicated experience" is information passed nate and actualized innovativeness might be expected
verbally between individual consumers and is generally to be extremely intricate. To oversimplify a little, there
based on actual experience with the new product in is absolutely no guarantee that some observed innova-
everyday usage.7 tors are not in fact innate later adopters (who received
This definition is one of innovativeness as a gen- the requisite information relatively quickly), and that
eralized trait possessed by individuals-indeed a trait some observed later adopters are not innate innovators
which has some parallels with self-confidence, social (for situational or interest reasons). If we were to study
character (Riesman 1950) and self-monitoring (Snyder several innovations in a specific product category
1974). However, instead of equating this particular (while controlling for product involvement/interest
view of innovativeness with any single other trait, it using a standardized and strictly comparable meth-
seems more plausible to view it as a function of a num- odology), we would expect individuals with a high de-
ber of (yet to be specified) dimensions of the human gree of innate innovativeness to display high actualized
personality. innovativeness on more occasions than other, less in-
Note that the definition is no longer couched in terms novative, individuals. In other words, innate innova-
of explicitly observable behavior, but is logically dis- tors will be observed as actual innovators more fre-
tinct from any such possible measures. We cannot ex- quently because their adoption is essentially independ-
plicitly observe a decision nor can we observe the ent of the interpersonal communication network.
weight given to interpersonally communicated experi- It should also be noted that individuals may evaluate
ence in that decision. What is feasible to observe, in the and reject the innovation. Therefore, 100 percent adop-
language of Rozeboom (1966, p. 206), is the "paradigm tion does not automatically follow the launch of a new
instances" of this formulation of innovativeness, that product. This raises problems, for it introduces yet
is, to utilize our theory of innovative behavior to con- another intervening factor. Unfortunately, little guid-
nect the construct with measurable properties via some ance exists in the literature because previous writers
set of intervening lower level hypotheses or rules of have not reported on the market performance of the
correspondence. One of these "instances" (measura- products concerned. (We will return to this point sub-
ble properties) is the act of adoption; another may be sequently, particularly in the context of the problems
a pencil and paper test on the appropriate variables. it poses for cross-sectional methodologies).
Implicit in this definition is the assumption that all The innovativeness construct advanced here ties in
members of society possess a greater or lesser degree neatly with the the research demonstrating a connec-
of innovativeness, and that the position an individual tion between this variable and product category usage
takes on this continuum is dependent on how much in- rates, as documented by Robertson (1971) and more
terpersonal information is required before making an recently Taylor (1977). This appears to hold both for
adoption/rejection decision. Hence, in this view inno- product categories and at the more generalized across-
vativeness is postulated at the highest level of abstrac- category level (Robertson 1971, p. 101). If, for a variety
tion, i.e., with respect to all product innovations. To of exogenous (and yet to be determined) factors, an
avoid confusion with the earlier definition of Rogers individual is a heavy user of several product categories,
he is likely to be more experienced at decision making,
7 An interesting point is whether this information can be "hear-
and therefore less reliant on the interpersonally com-
say," or whether it must pass directly between an adopter and municated experiences of social contacts. However,
a potential adopter. There appears to be no research on this topic. rather than devoting further space to the empirical sup-

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
236 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE B tiveness is the degree to which an individual is receptive


THE NEW MODEL OF INNOVATIVENESS to new ideas and makes innovation decisions independ-
ently of the communicated experience of others."
PSYCHOLOGICAL SOCIOLOGICAL However, given the preceding evidence, there seems
TRAITS TRAITS
no pressing reason to make this added complication
CONSTRUCT
FIELD at this point in time. Undoubtedly, our knowledge will
improve in the future, and with it the sophistication
FIELD { ~ ~~~~INNATE INNOVATIVENESS 7 of our construct. Here it is sufficient to propose that
future definitions should include the idea of an individ-
ual's degree of dependence on communicated experi-
INTEREST IN PRODUCT CATEGORY
ence. For this is the key to distinguishing between
those individuals who display low variance in their
INTERVENING relative times of adoption across several innovations
VARIABLES COMMUNICATED EXPERIENCE
(high innovativeness by our definition), and those who
display high variance (low innovativeness). Nor does
PHENOMENA SITUATIONAL EFFECTS
the above problem impinge on the arguments advanced
PLANE
here other than peripherally. For the moment we may
protect the given definition by adding the (hopefully
testable) assumption that high receptivity to new ideas
REJECTION } OBSERVED is correlated with a low dependence on communicated
EF 3 } ~BEHAV IOR experience, and proceed by discussing the model
shown in Figure B.
OBEVD ACTUALIZED INNOVATIVENESS, Some explanatory comments are in order for this dia-
BEHAV IOR ADPTO
OBSEHAVIOE ie. RELATIVE TIME OF gram. Under the heading "psychological traits," would
be included variables such as empathy, dogmatism,
achievement motivation, self-monitoring, intelligence,
port for this construct, it is perhaps more useful to etc. Under "sociological variables" would be included
question whether this definition completely captures such things as social participation, social integration,
our essential notions of innovativeness. This question cosmopolitism, social character, and so on. These theo-
hinges on whether the degree to which an individual retical constructs may have a relation not only to an
makes innovation decisions independently of the com- individual's innate innovativeness, but also to other
municated experience of others is a sufficient criterion phenomena associated with the diffusion of an innova-
to discriminate innate innovators from the rest of the tion. For instance, an individual's susceptibility to in-
population. There might be some individuals who do terpersonal messages may be governed by psychologi-
not rely on communicated experience but have a lower cal factors such as empathy, while the receipt of these
receptivity to new product ideas. Therefore, this defini- messages will be a function of their integration with
tion may misclassify such individuals as innate a social system.
innovators. As we move from the construct field into the realm
Since it appears to be accepted that the early adopter, of observable (though not necessarily measurable) phe-
early majority, and late majority are, to some degree, nomena, the first intervening variable is "interest in the
reliant on interpersonal information (Rogers and Shoe- product category." This is included to indicate that
maker 1971, p. 259), the only problem would appear most individuals do not have all-encompassing inter-
to be with the tails of the innovativeness distribution. ests or activities, especially with respect to the pur-
In other words, it is possible that innate laggards also chase of goods or services. It is also to be expected
do not utilize interpersonal information sources, per- that interest would be dependent on psychological,
haps because of their social isolation, and therefore, sociological, and demographic factors.8
might be misclassified as "innovators." There is some The next intervening variable is "communicated ex-
evidence that this is not the case. For Iowa farmers, perience," a shorthand way of denoting the network
Beal and Rogers (1960) indicated that the laggards of interpersonal messages relating to the product and
placed greater importance on interpersonal communi- the effects of these messages on individuals. The essen-
cation channels than other individuals. In the marketing tial points are that this network is unique to each innova-
literature, only Uhl, Andrus, and Poulsen (1970) ap- tion, and that the receipt of these messages by any
pear to have addressed this topic, finding for grocery one individual is in some way dependent on sociological
innovations that the laggards were exposed to inter-
personal information, but mainly from relatives rather
8 For the sake of simplicity, demographic variables have been
than outsiders. omitted from the diagram. These are obviously measurable and
We could, of course, posit a solution to this problem have relationships, both with other observable phenomena and
by refining the definition of innovativeness: "innova- with the various higher level constructs.

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
INNOVATIVEN.ESS 237

factors. On the other hand, the effectiveness of inter- FIGURE C

personal information in changing an individual's behav- SIMPLIFIED TREE VERSION OF THE MODEL
ior relates to his innate innovativeness and possibly
to other psychological factors. I NNATE l IAT
LINNOVATOR l NNINNOVATOR
The third intervening variable, "situational effects,"
subsumes a variety of situation-specific and person-
specific factors.9 Among such factors are the situation
in which the individual discusses the new product,
whether he has sufficient financial resources, or per-
haps most important of all, whether he has a latent I NTER ESTED _ NOT _I NTERESiTED
IN INTERESTED IN
need for the particular innovation's perceived benefits. PRODUCT IN PRODUCT
CATEGORY PRODUCT CATEGORY
Last, we have that which can be directly observed-
adoption-and that which is in some ways unobserva-
ble-rejection.
A simplified version of this model is presented in the
alternative form of a tree diagram in Figure C. Here,
FAVORABLE NFAVORABLE FAVORAB
the other traits have been omitted, and the innate in- SIATUATBIONEXPERIENCE
SITUATION COMMUNICAT
EXPERIENCE EXPERIENCE
novativeness continuum arbitrarily dichotomised into RECEIVED RECEIVED
A I EARL IER I I LATER I I
innovator and noninnovator. While this is an oversim-
plification of reality, it serves to illustrate the relative
communication independence of the innate innovators
as opposed to the communication dependence of the
|FAVORABLE UNFAAVORABLE FrAVORABLE |
innate noninnovators. An interesting and crucial re- LSITUATION | |SITUATION | SITUATION |

search question yet to be answered is whether the nega-


tive branches of the tree, i.e., not interested, unfavor-
able communicated experience, unfavorable situation,
lead to outright rejection or to delayed adoption.
If we compare the conceptualization presented in EARLIER ADOPTION J LATER ADOPTION
Figures B and C with the major existing model of in-
novative behavior (Figure A), the deficiencies of the
reached this position, for in many ways it has similar-
latter become more clear. By conceptualizing and
ities with recent developments in oth?er areas of the be-
measuring innovativeness at the same low level of ab-
havioral sciences. Within psychology the stance of the
straction, some previous researchers have largely been
trait theorists (e.g., Cattell 1950; Guilford 1959) has
misled into understating the effects of the complex
been criticized by, among others, Mischel (1968),
causal chain between trait and behavior. Earlier it was
which led to the rise of "situationism," a theoretical
argued that innovativeness has implicitly been viewed
position derived from the Skinnerian model of man.
in the literature as a trait or persisting characteristic.
However, as noted by Bowers (1973), neither traits nor
Here, it has been demonstrated that relative time of
situations have been shown to account for much of the
adoption is not a persisting individual characteristic,
variance in observable human behavior. Bowers argues
and can, in fact, have a high variance over several
for theories which take into account the interaction
innovations.
between person and situation. More recently, Sarason,
Innovativeness and relative time of adoption are not
Smith, and Diener (1975) concluded that neither the
synonymous concepts. The former is a hypothetical
construct, while the latter is a low-level operational
person, the situation, nor the interaction between the
two accounted for a high percentage of the variance.
variable, and between the two lies a system of inter-
They suggest that:
vening variables. If we believe in the idea of a trait,
innovativeness, then it is desirable that we formulate the more theoretically relevant a personality or demo-
the higher-level construct. It should then be feasible graphic variable is to the situation to be manipulated and/
to explore the complex mechanism intervening be- or the behavior to be studied, the more variance will be
tween trait and behavior in a more meaningful fashion accounted for by the Person X Situation interaction.
(p. 203-204).
than is possible with the existing paradigm. Hopefully,
this will lead to the establishment of consistent em- Concurrently, Snyder and Monson (1975) argue that
pirical relationships between traits, intervening varia- some individuals are more likely to display cross-situa-
bles, and behavior. tional consistency than others. It, therefore, appears
It should not be surprising that the authors have unlikely that the simple interaction theory will remain
unchanged for long.
9 The order of presentation of the intervening variables is not While it is to be expected that the study of new
meant to imply a causal hierarchy. product adoption will undergo similar revolutions, it

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
238 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE D (innate innovativeness). In particular, it detects innate


CONSTRUCTS, EFFECTS, AND MEASURES innovators through their theoretically predicted lower
variance, i.e., their higher probability of being actual in-
EFFECTS INTERVENING BETWEEN

novators. Given all our previous remarks, we will con-


LEVEL OF ABSTRACTION CONSTRUCT AND RELATIVE TIME OF ADOPTION

centrate on the measurement of innate innovativeness


AINTEREST IN PRODUCT CATEGORIESA
/ \ ~~~~~~COMMUNICATED EXPERIENCE/ \
HIGH SITUATIONAL FACTORS

-first discussing existing techniques and then the pos-


/ GENERALIZED \ XROSS-~~~~~~~~~~~SECTIONAL
/ TRAIT \ / ~~~~~~~ ~~~~~~~~ACROSS \
,, INNOVATIVENESS \ / S~~~~~~~~~~~EVERAL CATEGORE
sibilities for constructing new measures.
Within the theoretical framework advanced here,
only one of the existing methodologies would be viewed
/ \ ~~~~~~~~COMUNICATED EXPERIENCE /\
INTERMEDIATE PRODUCT CATEGORY SITUATIONAL FACTORS CROSS-SECTIONAL

SPECIFIC WITHIN SINGLE \


/ INNOVATIVENESS \ RODUCT CATEGORIES
as a potentially valid and reliable measure of innate
innovativeness. This is the cross-sectional method ap-
/ \ SIT~~~~~~~~~~~~UATIONA /

/ \ FA~~~~~~~~~~~~~~CTORS / plied across several product categories, an essentially


LOW

/SlrlGLE PRODUCT \ /RELATIVE TIME\ inferential route to identifying innate innovators. How-
/ SPECIFIC INNOVATIVENESS \ /OF ADOPTI ON\

ever, the previous theoretical discussions indicate that


to achieve this potential some modifications will be
required. First and foremost, it will be necessary to
should be stressed that this phenomenon has an added have strictly comparable measurements of actualized
dimension, that of interpersonal communication and innovativeness, that is, it must be possible to place
influence. Having elucidated the nature of innovative- all the sampled individuals into the adopter distribution
ness and argued for a construct at a high level of ab- of each and every new product on the questionnaire
straction, we now turn to ways of obtaining reliable list. This can only be done post hoc after all the various
measures of this construct. diffusion processes are substantially, completed and
only with detailed information on the market penetra-
tion of each product. Essentially, actualized innova-
METHODOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS
tiveness must be measured in standardized form similar
It is clear that if the conceptualization just advanced toAI = (m - t)Is.
mirrors empirical reality, it has some profound meth- It will not be enough to present a list of new products
odological implications. First and foremost, innate in- and ask respondents to indicate which they have al-
novativeness should be closely related to other socio- ready adopted. Such products would all be in different
logical and psychological dimensions, thereby capable stages of their respective life cycles and ownership (or
of being identified and studied as a variable in its own nonownership) of one may mean entirely the opposite
right. Second, actualized innovativeness can only of ownership (or nonownership) of another. Nor in-
meaningfully be analyzed within the context of a com- deed, can we know what ownership at that point in
munications network and in relation to the inherent time implies until the adoption of the product is com-
stance individuals take toward new ideas and products. plete.
A comparison between actualized innovativeness when One practical alternative is to use whatever data are
measured for two different new product introductions available to forecast the adoption curve of each of the
makes very little sense unless we have a benchmark products under consideration, say by a simple growth
for the two sets of individuals concerned, their innate curve model, thus, predicting individual actualized in-
innovativeness, and some pictures of the flow of favor- novativeness in the (m - t)ls form. Second, there is
able and unfavorable information relating to both prod- some evidence that the cross-sectional method does not
ucts. The observed adoption pattern cannot be disas- yield a unidimensional innovativeness scale (Rogers
sociated from the context of the concurrent communi- and Rogers 1961, p. 334). To an extent this problem
cation processes. may be resolved by the modification just mentioned,
Figure D provides a summary of the existing con- but it may also be due to the fact that the researcher
structs and methodologies. The left-hand triangle rep- will always be using a partial rather than complete list
resents the constructs at each of the three levels of of innovations and product categories. Whatever the
abstraction mentioned previously, while the central in- reason this question deserves research attention.
verted triangle portrays those variables which inter- Thus, while this variant of the cross-sectional tech-
vene between each of these constructs and overt be- nique will probably yield a valid and reliable measure
havior. In the right-hand triangle we have the main of innate innovativeness, its application may be difficult
measurement techniques associated with each con- and resource consuming. In a practical sense, there
struct. At the base we have the relative time of adop- may be benefits to seeking a less inferential and more
tion measure associated with a low-level, product- direct measure of this variable. After all, it is not usual
specific construct, while at the apex we have the to measure intelligence or dogmatism from "real-life"
cross-sectional method applied across several product instances of intelligent or dogmatic behavior, but rather
categories. Since the latter measures relative time of it is more normal to measure these traits via artificial
adoption for a large number of innovations, it is asso- paper and pencil tests. However, before discussing the
ciated with the highest-level construct postulated here latter possibility, we should mention the three other

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
INNOVATIVENESS 239

methods mentioned in the literature: self-designation, We therefore turn to a brief discussion of how such a
purchase intentions, and perceived innovation at- direct measure might be achieved.
tributes. To formulate a direct measure of innate innovative-
An example of the self-designation method is given ness would first require the identification of a set of
by Kohn and Jacoby (1973), who asked respondents questionnaire items relating to the essential elements of
whether they were usually among the first to buy new the construct, thought likely to discriminate between
products. It might be considered that this method could innate innovators and innate noninnovators. Since in-
be used in the context of several product categories, novativeness has been defined as "the degree to which
thereby yielding a measure of innate innovativeness, an individual makes innovation decisions independ-
since in some ways it can be likened to the cross- ently of the communicated experience of others," these
sectional approach. However, there is a critical prob- elements are associated with an individual's decision-
lem in extrapolating from a respondent's perception of making ability, independence, use of interpersonal
"usually among the first" to the theoretical require- communication, relations with others, and so on. This
ment of accurately detecting early adoption of several would lead to questionnaire items such as "I have dif-
innovations in each product category. In our opinion ficulty in deciding whether or not to buy a new food
this does not appear to be a promising approach. The product," "I often talk with my friends about new
purchase intentions method used with respect to prod- appliances," "I rely on my friends' advice when mak-
uct categories would encounter similar problems, and ing up my mind about new fashions," and so on.
since we would be asking for predictions of future be- These examples have been couched in terms of dif-
havior rather than recall of past behavior, it would ferent product categories in order to indicate that we
seem even more suspect. would probably ask similar questions with respect to
The perceived innovation attribute method has been several categories, thereby obtaining the breadth nec-
used by Ostlund (1974) in connection with two low-risk essary for a measure of innate innovativeness. In the
innovations. This method relies on obtaining respond- normal manner of constructing such measures, item
ents' perceptions of the new product before launch analysis would be used to reduce an initial battery of
along the dimensions of relative advantage, compati- 50 or more items to the 20 or so which might constitute
bility, complexity, observability, and trialability (Rogers a meaningful and practical scale. In the cross-sectional
and Shoemaker 1971, p. 137), together with perceived technique, we have an invaluable aid to constructing a
risk (Ostlund 1974, p. 24). Unfortunately, the technique direct scale. Specifically, the cross-sectional technique
can only really be applied to specific new products, provides an independent measure of innate innovative-
and therefore, suffers from the defect that, theoretically ness which may (a) assist in the selection of items, and
speaking, it measures a function of innovativeness, in- (b) serve in a test of the convergent validity of the
terest, and situational factors-a requirement which reduced item battery. If innate innovativeness is meas-
would impose severe practical limitations unless a suf- ured for the same respondents by both the cross-sec-
ficiently wide and varied set of potential new products tional method and a large initial battery of direct items,
can be used. If it could be shown that innate innovators the item-cross-sectional correlations can be used to se-
and innate noninnovators have different perceptions lect the most useful items to include in the final scale.
of the world in general, a powerful measurement tech- Alternatively, the cross-sectional score may be used
nique might be evolved. While the work of Ostlund as the dependent variable in a multiple regression
(1974) and Lambert (1972) is suggestive in this regard, where each direct item forms an independent variable.
it is too early to make any definitive statement. It is Again, the most significant items would be chosen,
necessary to devise a study whereby innate innovators but in this procedure we are also provided with an
and innate noninnovators are first differentiated by an optimal set of weights for the selected items-weights
independent methodology, and then their perceptions which can be used to generate scores from future sam-
established. ples. Obviously, these regression weights maximize the
Therefore, not only do the self-designation, purchase prediction of actual previous early adoption over a wide
intentions, and perceived innovation attributes meth- variety of product categories by the direct-scale items.
ods pose difficult methodological problems, but also In which case the direct innate-innovativeness score
none of these methods is consonant with the concept can be expressed as:
of innate innovativeness advanced earlier. If innova- n

tiveness is concerned with communication depend- 11= E w1s1,


ence, then our measures should attempt to tap the es- i=l

sential elements of this construct in a more straight-


where II = the individual's innate-innovativeness
forward manner than any of these (all three of which
score,
would require the introduction of yet more intervening
variables). Of existing techniques the cross-sectional n = the number of selected (significant) items,
method provides one valid measure of innate innova- wi = the regression weight attached to the ith
tiveness. To the authors, the only other promising al- item, and
ternative is the formulation of a specific test or scale. si = the individual's score on the ith item.

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
240 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

This has a superficial resemblance to the multi-attrib- tinuum is assumed to run from a value of six (high inno-
ute attitude model often employed in the marketing vativeness) to zero (low innovativeness), where the
literature (see Wilkie and Pessemier 1973) but is en- population mean score is three. Then we have approxi-
tirely different in derivation and conception. Having mately 2.5 percent of the population with an innate-
obtained a usable direct scale, a fresh sample would innovativeness score between five and six, 13.5 percent
be obtained, and the direct and cross-sectional meas- with a score between four and five, 34 percent between
ures generated again, except that on this occasion the three and four, and so on. Now we further postulate
former would be achieved using the weights derived that for a particular innovation the observed, or ac-
from the first sample. The correlation between the two tualized, innovativeness score also takes some distribu-
measures would then provide some indication of con- tion along a similar scale from six to zero (though
vergent validity, that is, "the degree to which two not necessarily with the same shape or mean score).
attempts to measure the same concept through maxi- The question posed is what would be the correlation
mally different methods are convergent" (Zaltman, between trait (in this case innate innovativeness) and
Pinson, and Angelmar 1973, p. 44). Taking a fresh sam- behavior (in this case actualized innovativeness)? This
ple would also correct for the bias inherent in deriving correlation is dependent on the process by which innate
and testing regression equations on the same set of innovativeness is transposed to actualized innovative-
respondents. ness. If we assume a direct one-to-one transfer, i.e., all
While the authors favor the construction of a direct those with an innate score of six take an actualized
instrument as leading to a more practical measure for score of six, those with an innate score of five an ac-
many research situations, much remains to be done, tualized score of five, and so on, then we obviously
for instance, choosing those questions which are para- observe perfect correlation. However, if we assume
digm instances of the construct and developing a valid that the process is random, that is, those individuals
and reliable psychological scale. Apart from conver- with any specific innate score are equally distributed
gent validity some other tests would be necessary to across the range of actualized innovativeness scores,
establish the overall construct validity of these meas- then the correlation is zero.
ures, namely, tests of discriminant and nomological For the purposes of this example let us postulate that
validity. reality is a mixture of these two processes, and that
Discriminant validity is "the extent to which a con- for the innate innovators (defined as those with an in-
cept differs from other concepts" (Zaltman, Pinson, nate score between six and four) the transfer is direct,
and Angelmar 1973, p. 44). An appropriate test might but for the rest (those with scores between zero and
be the degree to which a pattern of interrelationships four) it is random. A simple calculation reveals that in
between directly measured innovativeness and some this case, the correlation between trait and behavior
set of independent variables was similar to the pattern would be in the order of 0.25.
depicted between cross-sectionally measured innova- Now, the authors would be the first to admit that this
tiveness and these variables, and dissimilar to the pat- example is simplistic and that several factors have been
terns depicted between the latter and other variables ignored, factors which would tend to make the process
such as self-confidence, social character, etc. more complex. However, while we could go on adding
Nomological validity is perhaps the most important more realistic postulates, no doubt producing a range
requirement of all, in that it refers to the adequacy of of trait-behavior correlations, for our purposes here
the theoretical framework, and in particular, to the these sophistications are not necessary, the main con-
subsystem of laws, hypotheses, and assumptions which clusion of the paper being clear. Without a model of the
relate the construct to observable behavior (Cronbach processes intervening between trait and behavior we
and Meehl 1955). Given a measure of innate innovative- are in no position to ascribe any meaning to empirical
ness (direct or cross-sectional) and measures of com- correlations. Such a model is a vital stepping stone
munication and situational effects, we would attempt to to further progress in the field.
predict actualized innovativeness in its standardized
form and in the context of one, or more, specific in- [Received June 1977. Revised November 1977.]
novations. The question of construct validity is a ques-
tion of the adequacy of our model of innovative be-
REFERENCES
havior.
Arndt, J. (1967a), "The Role of Product-Related Conversa-
tions in the Diffusion of a New Product," Journal of
A CONCLUDING ILLUSTRATION Marketing Research, 4, 291-5.
(1967b), "Word of Mouth Advertising: A Review of
A simple hypothetical example will serve to sum- the Literature," Advertising Research Foundation Mono-
marize the main argument of this paper. Suppose a pop- graph, New York.
ulation is normally distributed along the innate-innova- Bass, F. M. (1969), "A New Product Growth Model for
tiveness continuum, or along any other relevant trait for Consumer Durables," Management Science, 15, 215-
that matter. For the purpose of the example, this con- 227.

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
INNOVATIVENESS 241

Baumgarten, S. A. (1975), "The Innovative Communicator Haines, G. H. (1966), "A Study of Why People Purchase
in the Diffusion Process," Journal of Marketing Re- New Products," in Proceedings of the American Mar-
search, 12, 12-18. keting Association, ed. R. N. Haas, Chicago: American
Beal, G. M. and Rogers, E. M. (1960), "The Adoption of Marketing Association.
Two Farm Practices in a Central Iowa Community," Jacoby, J. (1971), "Personality and Innovation Proneness,"
Special Report 26, Iowa Agricultural and Home Eco- Journal of Marketing Research, 8, 244-7.
nomics Experimental Station, Ames, IA. Katz, E. and Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1955) Personal Influence,
Bell, W. E. (1963), "Consumer Innovators: A Unique Market New York: The Free Press.
for Newness," in Proceedings of the American Mar- King, C. W. and Sproles, G. B. (1973), "Predictive Efficacy
keting Association, ed. S. Greyser, Chicago: American of Psychopersonality Characteristics in Fashion Change-
Marketing Association, 85-95. Agent Identification," in Proceedings of the American
Blake, B., Perloff, R., and Heslin, R. (1970), "Dogmatism Psychological Association, Washington, DC: American
and Acceptance of New Products," Journal of Market- Psychological Association.
ing Research, 7, 483-6. Kohn, C. A., and Jacoby, J. (1973), "Operationally De-
Bowers, K. S. (1973), " Situationism in Psychology: An Anal- fining the Consumer Innovator," in Proceedings of the
ysis and a Critique," Psychological Review, 80, 307- American Psychological Association, Washington, DC:
336. American Psychological Association.
Bruce, G. D. and Witt, R. E. (1970), "Personality Correlates Lambert, Z. V. (1972), "Perceptual Patterns, Information
of Innovative Buying Behavior," Journal of Marketing Handling, and Innovativeness," Journal of Marketing
Research, 7, 259. Research, 9, 427-31.
Bunge, M. (1967), Scientific Research I and II, Berlin: Margenau, H. (1935), "Methodology of Modern Physics,"
Springer-Verlag. Philosophy of Science, 2, 48-72.
Carnap, R. (1966), Philosophical Foundations of Physics, McGuire, W. J. (1969), "The Nature of Attitudes and Atti-
New York: Basic Books. tude Change," in Handbook of Social Psychology 2nd
Cattell, R. B. (1950), Personality: A Systematic Theoretical ed., eds. G. Lindzey, and E. Aronson, Boston: Addison-
and Factual Study, New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. Wesley Publishing Co.
Coney, K. A. (1972), "Dogmatism and Innovation: A Repli- Mendez, A. (1968), "Social Structure and the Diffusion of
cation," Journal of Marketing Research, 9, 453-5. Innovation," Human Organization, 27, 241-9.
Cronbach, L. J. and Meehl, P. E. (1955), "Construct Valid- Midgley, D. F. (1976), "A Simple Mathematical Theory of
ity in Psychological Tests," Psychological Bulletin 52, Innovative Behavior," The Journal of Consumer Re-
281-302. search, 3, 31-41.
Darden, W. R. and Reynolds, F. D. (1974), "Backward Pro- (1977), Innovation and New Product Marketing, New
filing of Male Innovators," Journal of Marketing Re- York: Halsted Press, John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
search, 11, 75-88. Mischel, W. (1968), Personality and Asse?ssment, New York:
Day, G. S. (1974), "Attitude Change and the Relative In- John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
fluence of Media and Word-of-Mouth Sources," in Nelson, P. (1975), "The Economic Consequences of Ad-
Models of Buyer Behavior ed. J. N. Sheth, New York: vertising," Journal of Business, 48, 213-241.
Harper & Row. Ostlund, L. E. (1972), "A Study of Innovativeness Over-
Donnelly, J. H. and Ivancevich, J. M. (1974), "A Methodol- lap," Journal of Marketing Research, 9, 341-3.
ogy for Identifying Innovator Characteristics of New (1973), "A Further Caution: It's Innovativeness
Brand Purchases," Journal of Marketing Research, 11, Overlap," Journal of Marketing Research, 10, 225-6.
331-4. (1974), "Perceived Innovation Attributes as Predic-
Engel, J. F., Blackwell, R. D. and Kegerreis, R. J. (1969), tors of Innovativeness," The Journal of Consumer Re-
"How Information is Used to Adopt an Innovation," search, 1, 23-29.
Journal of Marketing, 33, 15-19. Peat, N. C., Gentry, J. W. and Brown, T. L. (1975), "A
Comment on 'Identifying Buyers of a Major Automotive
, Blackwell, R. D. and Kollat, D. T. (1973), Consumer
Innovation,' " Journal of Marketing, 39, 61-2.
Behavior New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Peterson, D. R. (1968), The Clinical Study of Social Behavior,
English, H. B. and English, A. C. (1958), A Comprehensive
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Dictionary of Psychological and Psychoanalytical Terms,
Peterson, R. A. (1973), "A Note on Optimal Adopter
New York: Longmans, Green and Co.
Category Determination," Journal of Marketing Re-
Feldman, L. P. and Armstrong, G. M. (1975a), "Identifying
search, 10, 325-9.
Buyers of a Major Automotive Innovation," Journal of
Pizam, A. (1972), "Psychological Characteristics of Inno-
Marketing, 39, 54-62.
vators," European Journal of Marketing, 6, 203-210.
and Armstrong, G. M. (1975b), "A Reply to Peat, Riesman, D., Glazer, N. and Denny, R. (1950), The Lonely
Gentry, and Brown," Journal of Marketing, 39, 63-4. Crowd, New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Green, R. T., Langeard, E. and Favell, A. C. (1973), "Inno- Robertson, T. S. (1968), "Purchase Sequence Responses:
vation in the Service Sector: Some Empirical Findings," Innovators vs. Non-Innovators," Journal of Advertising
Journal of Marketing Research, 11, 323-6. Research, 8, 47-52.
Green, R. T. and Langeard, E. (1975), "A Cross-National (1971), Innovative Behavior and Communication,
Comparison of Consumer Habits and Innovator Char- New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
acteristics," Journal of Marketing, 39, 34-41. and Myers, J. H. (1969), "Personality Correlates of
Guilford, J. P. (1959), Personality. New York: McGraw-Hill Opinion Leadership and Innovative Buying Behavior,"
Book Co. Journal of Marketing Research, 6, 164-8.

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
242 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

and Myers, J. H. (1970), "Personality Correlates of (1972), "Media Exposure Patterns of Consumer Inno-
Innovative Buying Behavior: A Reply," Journal of Mar- vators," Journal of Marketing, 36, 43-9.
keting Research, 7, 260. Synder, M. (1974), "The Self-Monitoring of Expressive
and Kennedy, J. N. (1968), "Prediction of Con- Behavior," Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
sumer Innovators: Application of Multiple Discriminant ogy, 30, 526-537.
Analysis," Journal of Marketing Research, 5, 64-9. and Monson, T. C. (1975), "Persons, Situations, and
Rogers, E. M. (1962), Diffusion of Innovation, Glencoe, IL: the Control of Social Behavior," Journal of Personality
The Free Press. and Social Psychology, 32, 637-44.
(1976), "New Product Adoption and Diffusion," The Tatham, R. L., and Dornoff, R. J. (1973), "A Cautionary
Journal of Consumer Research, 2, 290-301. Note on Innovative Overlap," Journal of Marketing
Research, 10, 224-5.
and Rogers, E. L. (1961), "A Methodological Analy-
Taylor, J. W. (1977), "A Striking Characteristic of Innova-
sis of Adoption Scales," Rural Sociology, 26, 326-31.
tors," Journal of Marketing Research, 14, 104-107.
and Shoemaker, F. F. (1971), Communication of
Uhl, K., Andrus, R. and Poulsen, L. (1970), "How are
Innovations, New York: The Free Press.
Laggards Different? An Empirical Inquiry," Journal of
Rozeboom, W. W. (1966), Foundations of the Theory of
Marketing Research, 7, 51-4.
Prediction, Homewood Il: Dorsey Press.
Whyte, W. H., (1954), "The Web of Word-of-Mouth,"
Sarason, I. G., Smith, R. E., and Diener, E. (1975), "Per- Fortune, November, 140.
sonality Research: Components of Variance Attrib- Wilkie, W. L. and Pessemier, E. A. (1973), "Issues in
utable to the Person and the Situation," Journal of Marketing's Use of Multi-Attribute Attitude Models,"
Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 199-204. Journal of Marketing Research, 10, 428-41.
Sheth, J. N. (1971), "Word-of-Mouth in Low-Risk Innova- Wolman, B. B. (1973), Dictionary of Behavioral Science,
tions," Journal of Advertising Research, 11, 15-18. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Company.
Summers, J. 0. (1971), "Generalized Change Agents and In- Zaltman, G., Pinson, R. A., and Angelmar, R. (1973),
novativeness," Journal of Marketing Research, 8, Metatheory and Consumer Research, New York: Holt,
313-6. Rinehart & Winston.

This content downloaded from 14.139.157.21 on Sat, 07 Mar 2020 06:59:14 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like