Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

3256 • The Journal of Neuroscience, February 20, 2013 • 33(8):3256 –3258

Journal Club

Editor’s Note: These short, critical reviews of recent papers in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral
fellows, are intended to summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. For more
information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

Gambling Rats and Gambling Addiction: Reconciling the


Role of Dopamine in Irrationality
Guillaume Sescousse1* and Hanneke E.M. den Ouden1,2*
1Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands, and 2Center for Neural Science
and Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003
Review of Cocker et al.

Pathological gambling is a behavioral ad- nal biases seen in human pathological Next, the authors studied the effect of
diction that is characterized by excessive gambling. the nonspecific dopamine enhancer am-
(monetary) risk-taking in the face of neg- In their study, Cocker et al. (2012) as- phetamine and the dopamine D2/3 recep-
ative consequences, like bankruptcy or re- sessed risk-taking in a group of 32 rats us- tor antagonist eticlopride on risk-taking
lationship problems. Brain dopamine has ing a novel gambling task. On every trial, behavior. Amphetamine increased overall
been suggested to play an important role the rats chose between a “safe” lever deliv- risk-taking specifically in the wager-
in both risky behaviors and gambling ering a known number of sugar pellets sensitive rats, whereas eticlopride reduced
addiction. Yet, we know relatively little (range 1–3), versus an “uncertain” lever risk-taking in the wager-insensitive rats
about the specific mechanisms that offering a 50/50 chance of doubling this (Cocker et al., 2012, their Fig. 2C–F ). No-
drive interindividual differences in risk amount or receiving nothing. Thus, for tably, a D1 antagonist did not produce any
attitudes, or the factors that determine any safe option of x pellets, the alternative detectable effects. Finally, the authors
whether one becomes a gambling addict. gambling option would on average also re- measured striatal dopamine D2/3 receptor
A recent study in rats (Cocker et al., 2012) sult in 0.5*2x ⫽ x pellets, allowing the au- density in a subset of rats (n ⫽ 9) using
investigated the relationship between do- thors to assess risk attitudes in the absence of [ 11C]raclopride PET and autoradiogra-
pamine and risky decision-making, using differences in the expected values of the two phy. They found a negative correlation
a combination of behavioral assessment, options. A rat that would preferentially pick between wager sensitivity and D2/3 recep-
pharmacology, and brain imaging. The the small certain reward was classified as tor density in the dorsal striatum (Cocker
authors demonstrate a clear association “risk-averse,” whereas a “risk-seeking” rat et al., 2012, their Fig. 4). In the discussion,
between striatal dopaminergic transmis- would prefer to gamble for the large uncer- the authors propose that wager sensitivity
sion and the sensitivity to stake size, which tain reward. Overall, the rats showed risk- has similarities with pathological gam-
they posit is linked to human pathological seeking behavior, choosing the uncertain bling, and suggest that the association
gambling. In this review, we critically ex- lever on ⬃60% of the trials. with lower striatal D2/3 receptor density is
amine the evidence supporting this link. The authors then investigated how in line with results observed in substance
We argue that mapping risk-taking be- risk-taking was modulated between trials addiction.
havior from rats to humans should be that differed in stake size, i.e., in the num- The authors linked wager sensitivity to
done with the utmost caution and that the ber of sugar pellets at play. While risk- dopamine activation of striatal D2/3 recep-
sensitivity to stake size reported by Cocker taking remained constant across the three tors using a multidisciplinary approach
et al. (2012) is different from the irratio- stake levels in approximately two thirds of combining behavior, pharmacological
the rats, a subset of “wager-sensitive” rats manipulations, and PET imaging. The
Received Dec. 20, 2012; revised Jan. 15, 2013; accepted Jan. 15, 2013. changed from risk-seeking to risk-averse neurobiological specificity of these results
G.S. and H.E.M.d.O. received funding from the Netherlands Organisa-
tion for Scientific Research (NWO Rubicon/VENI). We thank Luke Clark, Ivan
behavior as the stake size increased fosters important insights into individual
Toni, and Roshan Cools for their helpful comments. (Cocker et al., 2012, their Fig. 2 A, B). The differences in risky decision-making in
*G.S. and H.E.M.d.O. equally contributed to this work. authors interpret the behavior of wager- rats. However, extrapolating these find-
Correspondence should be addressed to Guillaume Sescousse, Radboud sensitive rats as irrational, because the in- ings to human risk-taking and pathologi-
University Nijmegen, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour,
6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands. E-mail: g.sescousse@donders.ru.nl.
crease in stake size did not change the cal gambling is problematic. First, in
DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5828-12.2013 relative expected value of the certain ver- striking contrast to humans, the rats in
Copyright©2013theauthors 0270-6474/13/333256-03$15.00/0 sus uncertain option. this study favored the uncertain option
Sescousse and den Ouden • Journal Club J. Neurosci., February 20, 2013 • 33(8):3256 –3258 • 3257

in more than half of their choices. In sim- ases such as illusion of control and beliefs in they showed persistent loss chasing across
ilar contexts, humans tend to be risk- luck (Fortune and Goodie, 2012). These bi- stakes after administration of the dopamine
averse, with a clear preference for sure ases correspond to objectively erroneous enhancer methylphenidate (Campbell-
amounts of money over risky gambles of conceptions of chance processes, as op- Meiklejohn et al., 2012). In line with these
equal expected value. This behavior has posed to a stake-dependent risk aversion results, wager-sensitive rats in the Cocker et
been translated into a concave utility pattern. As a result of such irrational cogni- al. (2012) study showed increased levels of
function in modern theories of risky tive biases, pathological gamblers tend to risk-seeking when administered amphet-
decision-making, reflecting the idea that show exacerbated risk-taking, which is the amine, while wager-insensitive rats (who we
doubling the size of a reward does not exact opposite of the behavior seen in suggest might be at risk for gambling addic-
double its subjective utility (Fox and wager-sensitive rats. For example, in proba- tion) became less risk-seeking in response to
Poldrack, 2008). Whether this discrep- bility discounting protocols, which engage the D2/3 receptor antagonist eticlopride.
ancy reflects intrinsic differences between the same type of decision-making under risk This differential response between the two
species or is due to procedural differences, as in the Cocker et al. study, gamblers show groups is further consistent with prior find-
e.g., primary versus secondary rewards or a consistent shift toward risky options ings showing that dopaminergic drug effects
one-shot versus repeated choices, is an (Ligneul et al., 2012). As a corollary to this depend on differences in baseline dopamine
open question (for a discussion, see Hayden observation, we would like to speculate that levels (Cools et al., 2009).
and Platt, 2009). Nonetheless, these diver- the rats potentially at risk for gambling ad- In summary, the study by Cocker et al.
gent findings emphasize the need for cau- diction in the Cocker et al. study are in fact (2012) provides a valuable contribution
tion when translating results from animals those who are insensitive to wager size, or to the literature on risky decision-making,
to humans. even show increased risk-taking with in- demonstrating a clear link between indi-
Second, the concept of irrationality as creasing stakes (Cocker et al., 2012, their Fig. vidual differences in wager sensitivity and
used by Cocker et al., 2012, and its link to 2B). Such a tolerance for risk at high stakes striatal dopamine transmission via D2/3
pathology, is debatable. The authors argue parallels one of the core symptoms of path- receptors. The aim of this commentary
that the behavior of wager-sensitive rats is ological gambling as defined in the DSM- was to discuss the parallel drawn by the
irrational, because their transition from IV, namely the “need to gamble with authors between wager sensitivity and
risk seeking to risk aversion as the stakes increasing amounts of money to achieve the pathological gambling, and to consider al-
increase does not confer any real benefits. desired excitement.” ternative explanations for the observed
Then they link this irrational behavior to This alternative view would place the behavioral and pharmacological results.
pathological gambling in humans, rea- observed dopamine results in a different We argue that mapping wager sensitivity
soning that irrational biases in decision- light. Cocker et al. report a negative rela- in rats to pathological gambling in hu-
making set gamblers apart from healthy tionship between dopamine D2/3 receptor mans is far from straightforward, and we
controls. We think this is a bold leap. A density and wager sensitivity, which they speculate that any “irrationality” in gam-
behavior is defined as irrational in terms use to explain the differential effects of do- blers may even be the reverse from what
of a deviation from a specific normative paminergic manipulations between the the authors suggest. Far from discouraging
perspective that defines the translation of wager-sensitive and wager-insensitive rats. translational approaches, we hope that our
objective value into subjective utility. The This reduction of D2/3 receptor density remarks trigger discussion and foster future
wager-sensitive rats can be viewed as irra- seems difficult to reconcile with our sug- research focused on narrowing the gap be-
tional if subjective utility equates to ex- gestion that wager-insensitive rats are tween animal and human gambling.
pected value, which prescribes constant those at risk for gambling addiction, since
risk preferences across stakes. However, such a reduction has been consistently as- References
Boileau I, Payer D, Chugani B, Lobo D, Behzadi A,
their increasing risk aversion might ratio- sociated with substance addiction in hu- Rusjan PM, Houle S, Wilson AA, Warsh J, Kish
nally follow from an alternative utility mans (Volkow et al., 2010). However, it is SJ, Zack M (2012) The D(2/3) dopamine re-
function, for example, one that trades off important to note that human PET stud- ceptor in pathological gambling: a PET study
expected value and risk. In fact, increasing ies to date have failed to report any differ- with [(11) C]-(⫹)-propyl-hexahydro-naphtho-
risk aversion with increasing stakes is well ence in D2/3 receptor availability between oxazin and [(11) C]raclopride. Addiction. Ad-
documented in humans (Holt and Laury, pathological gamblers and controls vance online publication. Retrieved Nov. 30,
2012. doi:10.1111/add.12066. CrossRef
2002). This behavior, sometimes referred (Boileau et al., 2012; Clark et al., 2012). This
Campbell-Meiklejohn D, Simonsen A, Scheel-
to as the “peanuts effect,” can be consid- suggests that the biochemical mechanisms Krüger J, Wohlert V, Gjerløff T, Frith CD,
ered adaptive, as one does not have much underlying pathological gambling might be Rogers RD, Roepstorff A, Møller A (2012) In
to lose when taking a gamble over a pea- at least partly different from those identified for a penny, in for a pound: methylphenidate
nut, but probably should think twice in substance addiction. reduces the inhibitory effect of high stakes on
when gambling one’s house. From this Alternatively, pathological gambling persistent risky choice. J Neurosci 32:13032–
13038. CrossRef Medline
perspective, the behavior of wager- might be modeled by heightened lev-
Clark L, Stokes PR, Wu K, Michalczuk R, Benecke
sensitive rats corresponds to what is typi- els of dopamine, consistent with the A, Watson BJ, Egerton A, Piccini P, Nutt
cally observed in healthy humans and psychostimulant-mimetic model of this dis- DJ, Bowden-Jones H, Lingford-Hughes AR
perhaps should therefore not be viewed order (Zack and Poulos, 2009). Support for (2012) Striatal dopamine D(2)/D(3) receptor
as pathological. this model comes from a recent study that binding in pathological gambling is correlated
Furthermore, even if we do regard this looked at loss chasing, another prominent with mood-related impulsivity. Neuroimage
wager sensitivity as irrational, it is different characteristic of pathological gambling in 63:40 – 46. CrossRef Medline
Cocker PJ, Dinelle K, Kornelson R, Sossi V,
in nature from the type of irrational behav- which gamblers keep increasing their bets to Winstanley CA (2012) Irrational choice
ior seen in disorders of risky decision- recover past losses. While healthy partici- under uncertainty correlates with lower
making. In pathological gambling for pants showed a typical increase in risk aver- striatal D2/3 receptor binding in rats. J Neu-
instance, irrationality refers to cognitive bi- sion with increasing stakes under placebo, rosci 32:15450 –15457. CrossRef Medline
3258 • J. Neurosci., February 20, 2013 • 33(8):3256 –3258 Sescousse and den Ouden • Journal Club

Cools R, Frank MJ, Gibbs SE, Miyakawa A, Jagust (Glimcher P, Fehr E, Camerer C, Poldrack RA, ences in pathological gambling. Psychol
W, D’Esposito M (2009) Striatal dopamine eds). San Diego: Academic. Med 30:1–10. Medline
predicts outcome-specific reversal learning Hayden BY, Platt ML (2009) Gambling for Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Telang
and its sensitivity to dopaminergic drug ad- Gatorade: risk-sensitive decision making for F, Baler R (2010) Addiction: decreased reward
ministration. J Neurosci 29:1538 –1543. fluid rewards in humans. Anim Cogn 12: sensitivity and increased expectation sensitivity
CrossRef Medline 201–207. CrossRef Medline conspire to overwhelm the brain’s control cir-
Fortune EE, Goodie AS (2012) Cognitive distor- Holt CA, Laury SK (2002) Risk aversion and cuit. Bioessays 32:748 –755. CrossRef Medline
tions as a component and treatment focus of incentive effects. Am Econ Rev 92:1644 – Zack M, Poulos CX (2009) Parallel roles for do-
pathological gambling: a review. Psychol Ad- 1655. CrossRef pamine in pathological gambling and psycho-
dict Behav 26:298 –310. CrossRef Medline
Ligneul R, Sescousse G, Barbalat G, Domenech stimulant addiction. Curr Drug Abuse Rev
Fox CR, Poldrack RA (2008) Prospect theory and
P, Dreher JC (2012) Shifted risk prefer- 2:11–25. CrossRef Medline
the brain. In: Handbook of neuroeconomics

You might also like