Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Journal of

Experimental Psychology
VOL. 47, No. 6 JUNE, 1954

THE INFORMATION CAPACITY OF THE HUMAN MOTOR


SYSTEM IN CONTROLLING THE AMPLITUDE
OF MOVEMENT1
PAUL M. PITTS'
The Ohio State University

Information theory has recently ever, by asking S to make rapid and


been employed to specify more pre- uniform responses that have been
cisely than has hitherto been possible highly overlearned, and by holding all
man's capacity in certain sensory, per- relevant stimulus conditions constant
ceptual, and perceptual-motor func- with the exception of those resulting
tions (5, 10, 13, 15, 17, 18). The from S's own movements, we can
experiments reported in the present create an experimental situation in
paper extend the theory to the human which it is reasonable to assume that
motor system. The applicability of performance is limited primarily by
only the basic concepts, amount of the capacity of the motor system.
information, noise, channel capacity, The motor system in the present case
and rate of information transmission, is denned as including the visual and
will be examined at this time. Gen- proprioceptive feedback loops that
eral familiarity with these concepts permit S to monitor his own activity.
as formulated by recent writers (4,11, The information capacity of the
20, 22) is assumed. motor system is specified by its ability
Strictly speaking, we cannot study to produce consistently one class of
man's motor system at the behavioral movement from among several alter-
level in isolation from its associated native movement classes. The greater
sensory mechanisms. We can only the number of alternative classes, the
analyze the behavior of the entire greater is the information capacity of
receptor-neural-effector system. How- a particular type of response. Since
1
This research was supported in part by the measurable aspects of motor responses,
United States Air Force under Contract No. AF such as their force, direction, and am-
33(038)-10S28 with the Ohio State University plitude, are continuous variables, their
Research Foundation, monitored by the Air information capacity is limited only
Force Personnel and Training Research Center.
Permission is granted for reproduction, publica- by the amount of statistical variabil-
tion, use, and disposal in whole or in part by or ity, or noise, that is characteristic of
for the United States Government. repeated efforts to produce the same
1
Charles Kelly, Robert Silverman, and Char-
lotte Christner assisted in collecting the data response. The information capacity
here reported. of the motor system, therefore, can be
381
382 PAUL M. FITTS

inferred from measures of the varia- of specified permissible variability


bility of successive responses that S (movement tolerance).
attempts to make uniform. The need for a unifying concept of
It is possible to determine experi- motor capacity is indicated by the
mentally the noise associated with apparent difficulty of reconciling many
each category of response amplitude of the facts reported in the literature
and rate, and to infer the average on motor skill. These difficulties have
information capacity per response and in large measure stemmed from failure
the maximum average rate of infor- to recognize that the amplitude, the
mation transmission from the ratio of duration, and the variability of move-
the magnitude of this noise to the ments are interrelated.
magnitude of the possible range of
responses. This line of reasoning Early investigators were interested
agrees with Miller's (20) suggestion chiefly in activities such as running, tap-
that the concept of information capac- ping with a telegraph key, writing long-
hand, and keeping time with a baton in
ity can be interpreted as a sort of which visual monitoring of successive
modern version of the traditional movement is minimal and in which suc-
Weber-fraction and is consistent with cessive movements do not have to be
Theorem 17 in Shannon (22).3 terminated precisely. The duration of
The present experiments are limited this type of movement is essentially inde-
to motor tasks in which S is asked to pendent of its extent. Bryan (2), for
make successive responses having example, reported that tapping rate
a specified amplitude of movement. varied only from 4.6 to 6 cps over an
The information in which we are inter- amplitude range from 1 to 40 mm. Not-
ested is generated in discrete incre- ing that his results confirmed earlier
experiments by Von Kries, he concluded
ments, an increment being added by that when movements are made at maxi-
each successive "response." The rela- mum rate ". . . the rate varies slightly
tions to be studied are those holding with different muscles; increases with
among (a) the average amplitude, (b) practice; but is not affected by variations
the average duration, and (c) the dis- of the amplitude of motion within wide
tribution of successive amplitudes limits, a certain medium amplitude re-
(variability) of a series of rapid move- quiring less time than longer or shorter
ments that S attempts to produce with distances" (2, p. 138). Freeman (9)
uniform amplitude. The thesis to be studied handwriting movement and found
examined is that the channel capacity little variation in the total time required
to write letters as their size was varied.
of the motor system, in a task involv- Stetson and McDill state categorically
ing a particular limb, a particular set that "since the duration of the rapid
of muscles, and a particular type of movement is fixed, there can be but two
motor behavior (within limits), is inde- variables, extent and force" (23, p. 25),
pendent of average amplitude and and Hartson (12) makes fixed duration
8
the basic characteristic of the ballistic
Theorem 17 states that "the capacity of a movement.
channel of band W perturbed by white thermal Recent investigators (1, 3, 14, 21),
noise of power N when the average transmitter studying tasks that require £ to exercise
power is limited to P is given by
precise control of movement amplitude
P+N" but rely on his subjective standard of
(22, p. 67).
acceptable movement precision, have
W is in cycles per second and takes the form of reported results that are contrary to
the reciprocal of some value of time. The power those cited in the preceding paragraph.
of a band of noise is equivalent to the variance of An example of such a task is moving a
its amplitude distribution around its mean value. control quickly so as to realign a spot and
AMPLITUDE OF MOVEMENT 383

a cross hair on an oscilloscope after the tinuous signal requires a determination


spot is suddenly displaced. Typically S of the average power of the noise asso-
is instructed to make an "exact" adjust- ciated with the output signal. An
ment by the use of vision. The findings experimental determination of the
for this type of task indicate that the channel capacity of the human motor
duration of quick corrective movements
is a monotonically increasing function of system by this method would require
amplitude. Studies of eye movements that the average amplitude and dura-
(6, 24) reveal a similar increase of move- tion (or frequency) of movements be
ment duration with an increase in controlled by E and that the ampli-
amplitude. tude variability of S's movements be
Although it has been known for many measured by procedures similar to
years that the accuracy of quick move- those used by Wood worth (25). How-
ments decreases with amplitude, the close ever, the recording and measurement
relation of speed, amplitude, and accuracy of successive movement amplitudes are
often has been lost sight of. One of the laborious. Fortunately for the pres-
earliest workers to emphasize the impor- ent purpose man has remarkably good
tance of their interrelation was Wood-
worth (25). Studying a variety of simple ability to adjust his average rate of
motor tasks performed with and without performance to meet the demands of a
visual monitoring, he found that for particular task, such as the require-
quick, visually controlled movements var- ment that his movements fall within
iable error increases both with amplitude some specified tolerance range. Thus
and with speed. Industrial engineers it is possible to assess the tenability of
(19) also have consistently advocated the thesis of fixed information capacity
that small-amplitude movements be used by means of a test of the following spe-
whenever possible in assembly work, on cific hypothesis: // the amplitude and
the grounds that the time required to tolerance limits of a task are controlled by
complete a unit of work increases as a
function of its amplitude as well as of the E, and S is instructed to work at his
precision demanded by the task. maximum rate, then the average time per
response will be directly proportional to
The concept of a fixed information- the minimum average amount of infor-
transmission capacity of the motor mation per response demanded by the
system not only accounts for such particular conditions of amplitude and
divergent results but also suggests a tolerance.
way of relating quantitatively the am- In performing a given task S may
plitude, duration, and variability of actually use movements that are less
motor responses. The concept leads variable, i.e., which generate more
to the expectation that if repetitive information, than the minimum de-
movements of a fixed average ampli- manded by the task. When only the
tude are speeded up, then on the aver- limits of variability are specified, the
age each movement can provide less minimum information condition is
information, and movement variabil- met if all amplitudes within the toler-
ity will increase by a specified amount. ance range are used equally often, i.e.,
Similarly, it suggests that if average when responses are distributed recti-
movement amplitude should be in- linearly over the tolerance range,
creased then variability and/or aver- whereas the actual dispersion of re-
age duration will also increase. The sponse terminations is an approximate
thesis can be tested experimentally in Gaussian distribution. The rate at
several ways. which S makes successive responses in
An accepted method for estimating a given task, therefore, must be inter-
channel capacity in the case of a con- preted as the rate at which he can
384 PAUL M. FITTS

generate, on the average, at least as touched. Thus the first plate touched on either
much information per response as that side was always the one scored.
Four center-to-center distances between the
demanded by the task. This restric- target plates were studied (2, 4, 8, and 16 in.)
tion on the interpretation that can be giving a total of 16 combinations of amplitude
given to results obtained with the (A) and target size. (For A = 2 in. and W, = 1
present procedure is more than com- in. no undershoot errors were possible.)
Two metal-tipped styluses were used. One
pensated for by the fact that the pro- weighed 1 oz. and was about the size of a pencil.
cedure makes it feasible to study a The other weighed 1 Ib. and was slightly larger.
wide variety of task conditions with The use of one or the other of these two styluses
relatively large samples of Ss and permitted the amount of work to be varied with-
responses. out otherwise changing the task.
The S sat with his right arm midway between
EXPERIMENT I: the plates, which were located at a convenient
RECIPROCAL TAPPING height and angle. Each trial lasted 15 sec. and
was followed by a 55-sec. rest period.
In the first experiment two closely The following instructions were read to each
related reciprocal tapping tasks were S:
studied. The Ss were asked to tap "Strike these two target plates alternately.
two rectangular metal plates alter- Score as many hits as you can. If you hit either
nately with a stylus. Movement tol- of the side plates an error will be recorded. You
will be given a 2-sec. warning before a trial.
erance and amplitude were controlled Place your hand here and start tapping as soon
by fixing the width of the plates and as you hear the buzzer. Emphasize accuracy
the distance between them. The task rather than speed. At the end of each trial I shall
was accomplished primarily by move- tell you if you have made any errors."
ments of the lower arm. Subjects.—Sixteen right-handed college men
served as Ss.
Method Sequence of trials.—Each S was tested at each
of the 16 conditions. A different random
Apparatus and procedure.—A schematic draw-
sequence of conditions was used for each S with
ing of the apparatus is shown in Fig. 1. The tar-
the restriction that for the entire group of 16 Ss
get plates were 6 in. long and of four different
each condition occur once on each trial. The
widths (2, 1, ,5, and .25 in.). The widths of the
experiment was replicated on the same day by
plates (hereafter referred to as Wj) established
retesting each S on each condition in a reversed
the tolerance limits within which movements had sequence.
to be terminated in amplitude. An additional
error plate was mounted on either side of each Each S participated in the experiment for two
target plate. The error plates were wide enough days. The lighter stylus was used on the first
to catch all overshoots and undershoots. Six day, the heavier on the second day. All other
counters, activated by thyratron circuits through procedures on the two days were the same. No
the plates and stylus, provided an automatic attempt was made to equalize practice effects
record of hits, overshoots, and undershoots for between the lighter and heavier styluses, because
each set of three plates. After one of the count- E was interested chiefly in the effects of ampli-
ers in each group of three had recorded, none tude and tolerance changes within each variation
of the counters in that group would score again of the task.
until one of the opposite set of plates had been
Results
The time and error data for the two
variations of the tapping task are in-
cluded in Table 1. The data are ex-
pressed as the average time (*), in
seconds, between taps (i.e., the time
for each right-left or left-right move-
ment) and as the percentage of errors
(i.e., movements that were either too
FIG. 1, Reciprocal tapping apparatus. The long or too short). Percentages are
task was to hit the center plate in each group
alternately without touching either side (error) based on from 613 to 2,669 movements
plate. per condition.
AMPLITUDE OF MOVEMENT 385

TABLE 1
TASK CONDITIONS AND PERFORMANCE DATA FOR 16 VARIATIONS OF A
RECIPROCAL TAPPING TASK
(N = the same 16 Ss at each condition)

Tolerance and Amplitude l-oz. Stylus l-lb. Stylus


Conditions

Errors / Errors
W, A id /
(%> ir Rank (%) 1, Rank

.25 2 4 .392 3.35 10.20 11 .406 3.80 9.85 7


.25 4 5 .484 3.41 10.33 9 .510 3.83 9.80 8
.25 8 6 .580 2.78 10.34 8 .649 4.04 9.24 13
.25 16 7 .731 3.65 9.58 14 .781 4.08 8.96 IS
.50 2 3 .281 1.99 10.68 5 .281 0.88 10.68 I
.50 4 4 .372 2.72 10.75 3.5 .370 2.16 10.81 2
.50 8 5 .469 2.05 10.66 6 .485 2.32 10.31 6
.50 16 6 .595 2.73 10.08 12 .641 2.27 9.36 11
1.00 2 2 .212 0.44 9.43 15 .215 0.13 9.30 12
1.00 4 •3 .260 1.09 11.54 1 .273 0.85 10.99 1
1.00 8 4 .357 2.38 11.20 2 .373 1.17 10.72 3
1.00 16 5 .481 1.30 10.40 7 .526 1.32 9.50 10
2.00 2 1 .180 0.00 5.56 16 .182 0.00 5.49 16
2.00 4 2 .203 0.08 9.85 13 .219 0.09 9.13 14
2.00 8 3 .279 0.87 10.75 3.5 .284 0.65 10.56 5
2.00 16 4 .388 0.65 10.31 10 .413 1.72 9.68 9
Note.—Wi is the width in inches of the target plate. A is the distance in inches between the centers of the
two plates. * is the average time in seconds for a movement from one plate to the other. The performance Index,
IP, is discussed In the text.

The average number of errors made undershoots were about twice as


in this task was small, only 1.2% with frequent. However, in spite of such
the lighter stylus and 1.3% with the consistent trends, the relatively small
heavier one. The low incidence of incidence of errors justifies an analysis
errors indicates that Ss successfully of the data in terms of the total num-
adjusted their rate of performance to ber of movements made under the
meet changed task conditions. various task conditions.
The largest proportion of errors, Practice led to relatively small im-
3.6% with the lighter stylus and 4.1% provement in performance. The mean
with the heavier stylus, was made time per movement for all Ss and all
at the most difficult task condition task conditions decreased by 3% from
(W, - | in., A = 16 in.). At each the first to the second block of trials
amplitude step the percentage of errors with the light stylus, and by 5% with
for more exacting tolerances was con- the l-lb. stylus. The total number
sistently slightly larger than for less of errors made by all Ss was almost
exacting tolerances; this indicated exactly the same for the first as for the
that Ss did not slow down their move- second block of trials.
ments quite as much as necessary to For each category of W,, movement
effect the required accuracy at the
time increased progressively as move-
more exacting conditions. Nor were
the errors symmetrically distributed ment amplitude (A) increased. Like-
over the error plates. The Ss were wise for each amplitude, movement
uniformly more accurate in terminat- time increased progressively as toler-
ing flexor than extensor movements. ance was decreased. The relations
With the lighter stylus over- and among the various conditions for
undershoot errors were equally fre- the lighter and heavier styluses were
quent, but with the heavier stylus^ similar.
386 PAUL M. FITTS

EXPERIMENT II: Disc TRANSFER ous experiment. The entire procedure was then
replicated on a second day using a different
The second task was to transfer sequence of trials for each S.
plastic washers from one pin to
another. Again, the amplitude and Results
tolerance of the positioning move- The transfer of a disc involves a
ments were varied through a series of transport-empty plus a transport-
geometric steps, and the rate of per- loaded movement, and the times given
formance was measured. This task dif- are an average of these two aspects of
fered from the previous one in that no each transfer cycle. There was a
errors were permitted. Some amount small (6%) decrease in time per move-
of finger activity was involved in ment on the second set of 16 trials.
grasping a washer by the edges before The average times per movement for
removing it from the pin. each condition, averaged over all 16
Ss and over both days, are given in
Method Table 2 in the column labeled t.
Apparatus and procedure.—A schematic draw- The variations in movement time with
ing of the apparatus is shown in Fig. 2. Round increase in movement amplitude and
plastic discs, | in. thick and 1.5 in. in diameter,
were drilled with four sizes of holes. Eight discs changes in tolerance correspond to
of uniform size were placed over a J-in. pin. those found in Exp. I.
The task was to transfer the discs, one at a time,
to another pin. The transfer of eight discs EXPERIMENT III: PIN TRANSFER
constituted a trial.
The difference between the diameter of the The third task was to transfer
pins and the diameter of the center hole of the pins from one set of holes to another.
discs was varied in four geometric steps (J, J, i, Grasping and releasing movements of
and ^g in.). Four amplitudes of movement the fingers were required in addition
(4, 8, 16, and 32 in.) were used, making a total
of 16 conditions. The S touched a switch plate TABLE 2
beside the empty pin as he began the task. This
started a 1/100-sec. timer. The S touched the TASK CONDITIONS AND PERFORMANCE DATA
switch plate as soon as he had transferred the FOR 16 VARIATIONS or A
last disc. This stopped the timer. Discs were DISC-TRANSFER TASK
always moved from right to left. (N = the same 16 Ss at each condition)
Subjects.—Sixteen right-handed college men
served as Ss. They were different Ss from those Tolerance and Amplitude Performance
used in Exp. I. Conditions
Sequence of trials.—Each S worked at all 16
task conditions in a restricted randomized W, A li ( I, Rank
sequence corresponding to that used in the previ-
.0625 4 7 .697 10.04 2.5
.0625 8 8 .771 10.38 1
.0625 16 9 .896 10.04!; 2.5
.0625
.125
32
4
10
6
1.096
.649
9.9.2412*: 7
6
.125 8 7 .734 9.54 4
.125 16 8 .844 9.48 S
.125 32 9 1.028 8.75 10
.25 4 5 .607 8.241 12
.25 8 6 .672 8.93 9
.25 16 7 .771 9.08 '" 8
.25 32 8 .975 8.20J 13
.5 4 4 .535 7.47$ 16
.5 8 5 .623 8.02 14
.5 16 6 .724 8.29 11
.5 32 7 .902 7.76 15
FIG. 2. Disc transfer apparatus. The task Note.—Wi is the difference in inches between the
was to transfer eight washers one at a time from diameter of the post and the diameter of the center hole
the right to the left pin. The inset gives the of the disc. A is the center-to-center distance between
the two posts. t is the average time in seconds for a
dimensions for the W, «= J in. condition. movement between two posts.
AMPLITUDE OF MOVEMENT 387

to arm movements. No errors were


permitted. The task was varied in
four geometric steps of movement tol-
erance and in five geometric steps of
amplitude. The time for completing
a series of eight responses was recorded.
The data for this task are somewhat
more extensive, and somewhat more
practice was given on this task than in
the two preceding experiments.
Method
Apparatus and procedure.—The pin-transfer FIG. 3. Pin transfer apparatus. The task
apparatus is shown in Fig. 3. Four sizes of was to transfer eight pins one at a time from one
pins(J, J, ^g, and -fa in. in diameter) and five set of holes to the other. The inset gives the
amplitudes of movement (1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 in.) dimensions of pins and holes for the W, = \ in.
were employed, thus making a total of 20 task condition.
conditions. Each set of pins was used with a
set of holes whose diameter was twice that of the among the data correspond closely to
pins. The difference between the two diameters
determined the tolerance (IF,) required in trans-
those found in the previous experi-
ferring a pin from one hole to another. The tips ments. Movement times increased
of the pins were tapered to one-half their diam- progressively as the tolerance require-
eter as shown in the inset of Fig. 3. ment was made more stringent and as
The S started and stopped a timer as in the amplitude was increased.
preceding task, in order to record the over-all
time for making eight transport-empty plus eight INFORMATION ANALYSIS
transport-loaded movements. A trial consisted
of the transfer of eight pins from one side to the The rate of performance of all the
other followed after a short rest by the transfer tasks studied increased uniformly as
of the same pins in the opposite direction. movement amplitude was decreased
The instructions were similar to those em- and as tolerance limits were extended.
ployed in the preceding experiments.
Subjects.—Twenty college students, ten men These results agree qualitatively with
and ten women, served as Ss. These were dif- the predictions from the hypothesis.
ferent Ss from those serving in Exp. I and II. In order to test the results against a
Sequence of trials.—Each S served under all quantitative prediction that the infor-
task conditions. The sequence of conditions
was randomized, with the restriction that for the mation output of the human motor
group of 20 Ss each condition occur once on each system in any particular type of task
trial and once for each S. The experiment was is relatively constant over a range of
repeated three times with the same Ss on three amplitude and accuracy requirements,
separate days. The sequence of trials for each S a difficulty index is needed that will
was different each day.
specify the minimum information re-
Results quired on the average for controlling
There was some improvement in the or organizing each movement.
rate of task performance from the first The rational basis for this estimate
to the second day, but little further of task difficulty is the maximum rela-
improvement on the third day. The tive uncertainty that can be tolerated
over-all means for the three days were for a "correct" movement in a series
.613, .585, and .578 sec. per movement, having a specified average amplitude.4
respectively. The average movement Thus the minimum organization re-
4
times computed for the combined trials Although only amplitude is considered here,
of the second and third replications of the present rationale can be extended to the
question of the rate of information involved in
the experiment are given in Table 3 the selection of the direction, the force, or the
in the column labeled t. The relations duration of movements.
388 PAUL M. FITTS

TABLE 3 effect of adding one bit ( — logs f) per


TASK CONDITIONS AND PERFORMANCE DATA response to the difficulty index. The
FOR 20 VARIATIONS OF A PIN- use of IA makes the index correspond
TRANSFER TASK rationally to the number of successive
(N = the same 20 Ss at each condition) fractionations required to specify the
tolerance range out of a total range
Tolerance and Amplitude
Conditions Performance extending from the point of initiation
of a movement to a point equidistant
W. A u t IT Rank on the opposite side of the target.
.03125 1 6 .673 8.92 20 The index of difficulty (la) is given
.03125 2 7 .70S 9.93 18 in Tables 1, 2, and 3 for each of the
.03125 4 8 .766 10.44 14 tasks. Since the index takes into
.03125 8 9 .825 10.91 13
.03125 16 10 .959 10.43 15 account only the information neces-
.0625 1 5 .484 10.33 16 sary to specify the amplitude of a
.0625 2 6 .518 11.58 11 movement, it is insensitive to the
,0625 4 7 .580 12.07 6
.0625 8 8 .644 12.42 2 weight of the stylus used in the first
.0625 16 9 .768 11.72 9 experiment and hence to the physical
.125 1 4 .388 10.31 17 work required by the task. In its
.125 2 5 .431 11.60 10
.125 4 6 .496 12.10 5 present form, the index is also insensi-
.125 8 7 .557 12.57 1 tive to the information required for
.125 16 8 .668 11.98 7
.25 1 3 .326 9.20 19 specifying the direction of a movement,
.25 2 4 .357 11.20 12 or for specifying additional manipula-
.25 4 5 .411 12,16 4 tory acts such as the finger movements
.25 8 6 .486 12.34 3
.25 16 7 .592 11.82 8 required to grasp and release objects.
Also shown in Tables 1, 2, and 3 is
Note.—Wi is the difference in inches between the
diameter of the pins and the diameter of the hole into
a binary index of performance (7P),
which they were inserted. A is the center-to-center which expresses the results as a per-
distance between the holes, t is the average time in
seconds for a single movement. formance rate. For a task in which
W, and A are fixed for a series of
quired of a particular movement is movements, Ip is defined as
defined by the specification of one from
among k possible categories of ampli- 1 W
IP = - - Iog2 ^ bits/sec., (2)
tude within which the movement is to
terminate.
If we follow the above reasoning where t is the average time in seconds
and conventional information nota- per movement. This index has some
tion, a binary index of difficulty (/,;) resemblance to one by Goldman (11,
is defined as p. 157, Formula 29) for maximum in-
W formation rate, although Goldman's
Id = —logs ^Jbits/response, (1) criterion for determining the number
of possible categories is based on a
where W, is the tolerance range in mean square criterion.
inches and A is the average amplitude We want to test the hypothesis that
of the particular class of movements. movement time varies with task diffi-
The choice of the denominator for this culty in such a way that Ip is constant
index is arbitrary since the range of over a wide range of movement ampli-
possible amplitudes must be inferred. tudes and tolerances. It is apparent
The use of IA rather than A is indi- from the data in Tables 1, 2, and 3
cated by both logical and practical that even though the index is not pre-
considerations. Its use insures that cisely constant, the hypothesis is sub-
the index will be greater than zero for stantially confirmed.
all practical situations and has the For the eight best (out of the 16)
AMPLITUDE OF MOVEMENT 389

conditions of tapping with the light


weight stylus, the rate of performance
varied between 10.3 and 11.5 bits/
sec., a range of only 1.2 bits. Per- •f-t
formance fell off markedly only for the
least exacting bits/sec., condition stud-
ied (A = 2 in., W, = 2 in.).
Performance rate with the 1-lb. sty-
lus also was relatively stable over a
comparable range of amplitudes and
tolerances. The rate at all but two
conditions was reduced slightly by the
added work, and the region of maxi-
mum performance was shifted toward
smaller amplitudes of movement. The
Pearsonian correlation between the 16
IP values for the two variations in the FIG. 4. Three-dimensional representation of
tapping task was large however performance rate (7P) in bits per second for the
pin-transfer task as a function of amplitude and
(r = .97). tolerance requirements
For the 16 conditions of the disc-
transfer task, on which the difficulty to 8 in. were more consistently asso-
index ranged from 4 to 10 bits/re- ciated with good performance than
sponse, the rate of performance varied was any particular tolerance or any
from 7.5 to 10.4 bits/sec. Over the particular difficulty index.
eight best conditions the range was DISCUSSION
only 1.3 bits/sec.
Performance on the pin-transfer Although the four tasks thus far anal-
yzed are a small sample of perceptual-
task varied from 8.9 to 12.6 bits/sec, motor activities, the results are suffi-
for the 20 conditions studied, but the ciently uniform to indicate that the
range of variability was only 1.0 bit/ hypothesized relation between speed,
sec. over the ten best conditions. amplitude, and tolerance may be a gen-
In all tasks it was found that move- eral one. It is not surprising to find that
ment amplitudes and tolerances could performance, measured in information
be varied within limits without much units, is relatively constant over a central
effect on performance rate, but that range of amplitude and accuracy condi-
performance began to fall off outside tions and falls off outside these limits.
Such a relation holds for most perceptual
these limits. This is readily apparent and motor activities and has recently
from an inspection of Fig. 4, which is been found (16) in a study of the opti-
a three-dimensional representation of mum relation between the amplitude of
the performance data for the pin- lever movement and observed display
transfer task. Movements of 1- and movement.
2-in. amplitude were consistently less The absolute level of capacity of the
efficient than movements of 4 to 8 in. motor system probably varies consider-
In the pin-transfer task in particular, ably for different movements, limbs, and
both the smallest amplitude (1 in.) muscle groups. For example, the arm,
and the smallest tolerance (1/32 in.) which in the present experiments was
asked to generate information only in
resulted in relatively low rates of per- respect to successive movement ampli-
formance. In most cases performance tudes, may have a lower information
rate also fell off at the largest ampli- capacity than the hand. Using all of his
tudes and tolerances. Throughout all fingers singly and in combinations, S can
the tasks, movement amplitudes of 4 produce one bit of information per re-
390 PAUL M. FITTS

sponse per finger. Quite likely he can esses. Fenn has pointed out that in
sustain higher level performance by using reciprocal movements made at maximum
finger rather than arm movements. More rate and without any visual control the
complex movement patterns than those muscles work at a level far below their
studied may also have a higher informa- physical capacity. Since maximum rate
tion capacity since in this case infor- of performance in this case is not limited
mation can be generated along several by the muscles, he has proposed that the
dimensions simultaneously. maximum rate is limited by ". . . the
The estimates of information capacity speed with which excitation and inhibi-
arrived at by the present procedures give tion can be made to alternate in the
lower values than would be obtained by central nervous system without at the
other and perhaps equally tenable as- same time losing precise control of the
sumptions regarding the number of pos- magnitude of the force" (8, p. 174). This
sible categories. However, the estimates concept of central organizing time is
are of similar magnitude, in fact slightly entirely consistent with an information
higher, than the figures reported for the point of view. The finding that rela-
average S in a perceptual-motor task, tively small differences in performance
such as pressing keys in response to flash- result from the change in stylus weight,
ing lights (18), in which responses to and the validity of predictions of per-
a series of discrete stimuli have been formance rate from the index of task
studied. difficulty lend support to the basic thesis
The present index of difficulty can be of this paper, that it is the degree of con-
applied to a wide range of motor tasks. trol required over the organization of a
For example the three stick and rudder response, i.e., the amount of information
control movements necessary to make a required to specify a response, which is
single match on the Complex Coordina- the major factor limiting the rate of
tion Test (Model E) require one bit of motor performance.
information to specify each of the three
directions of movement and 3.9 bits to SUMMARY
specify each of the three amplitudes (a The present paper attempts to relate the tra-
total of 14.7 bits); this assumes that S ditional Weber function (variability of a response
starts his adjustments with the controls as a function of its amplitude) to the parallel
in the neutral position. phenomena of variability as a function of response
Ellson (7) has raised the question of duration, using certain concepts of information
theory.
the linearity of motor responses, using An index of the difficulty of a movement is
the superposition theorem as the criterion proposed on the assumption that the average
of linearity. This theorem demands that amplitude, the average duration, and the ampli-
movement duration remain constant as tude variability of successive movements are
amplitude increases. We have noted related in a manner suggested by information
that many recent data violate this theory. The basic rationale is that the minimum
requirement. However, the present amount of information required to produce a
results indicate that over the range in movement having a particular average amplitude
which performance is optimum, move- plus or minus a specified tolerance (variable
ments of differing amplitude but of equal error) is proportional to the logarithm of the
ratio of the tolerance to the possible amplitude
difficulty in terms of information tend range. The specification of the possible ampli-
to be of approximately equal duration. tude range is arbitrary and has been set at twice
Man differs from a linear electronic or the average amplitude. The average rate of
mechanical system, however, in that he information generated by a series of movements
sometimes varies his standard of relative is the average information per movement divided
precision for a movement at the same by the time per movement.
time as he varies its amplitude; unless rel- Three experiments are reported which were
ative precision remains constant, move- designed to test the following hypothesis: If the
amplitude and tolerance limits of a task are fixed
ment time will vary with amplitude. and the S is instructed to work at his maximum
The basis for the uniformity of motor rate, then the average duration of responses will
performance perhaps is to be found in the be directly proportional to the minimum average
time taken for central organizing proc- amount of information per response (i.e.,|the
AMPLITUDE OF MOVEMENT 391

degree of behavior organization) demanded by 9. FREEMAN, F. N. Experimental analysis of


the task conditions. The conditions studied cov- the writing movement. Psychol. Monogr.,
ered the range from 1 to 10 bits/response. 1914,17, No. 4 (Whole No. 75), 1-46.
The results indicate that rate of performance 10. GARNER, W. R., & HAKE, H. W. The
in a given type of task is approximately constant amount of information in absolute judg-
over a considerable range of movement ampli- ments. Psychol. Rev., 1951, 58, 446-459.
tudes and tolerance limits, but falls off outside 11. GOLDMAN, S. Information theory. New
this optimum range. The level of optimum per- York: Prentice-Hall, 1953.
formance was found to vary slightly among the 12. HARTSON, L. D. Contrasting approaches to
three tasks in the range between about 10 to 12 the analysis of skilled movements. /.
bits/sec. The consistency of these results gen. Psycho!., 1939, 20, 263-293.
supports the basic thesis that the performance 13. HICK, W. E. On the rate of gain of infor-
capacity of the human motor system plus its mation, Quart. J. exp. Psychol., 1952,
associated visual and proprioceptive feedback 4, 11-26.
mechanisms, when measured in information
units, is relatively constant over a considerable 14. HICK, W. E., & BATES, J. A. V. The human
range of task conditions. This thesis offers a operator of control mechanisms. London:
plausible way of accounting for what otherwise Ministry of Supply, 1950.
appear to be conflicting data on the durations of 15. HOWES, D. H., & SOLOMON, R. L. Visual
different types of movements. duration threshold as a function of word-
The author feels that the fixed information- probability. /. exp. Psychol., 1951, 41,
handling capacity of the motor system probably 401-410.
reflects a fixed capacity of central mechanisms 16. JENKINS, W. L., & OLSON, M. W. The use
for monitoring the results of the ongoing motor of levers in making settings on a linear
activity while at the same time maintaining the scale. /. appl. Psychol., 1952, 36,
necessary degree of organization with respect 269-271.
to the magnitude and timing of successive 17. KLEMMER, E. T., & FRICK, F. C. Assimila-
movements. tion of information from dot and matrix
patterns. /. exp. Psychol., 1953, 45,
REFERENCES 15-19.
18. KLEMMER, E. T., & MULLER, P. F., JR.
1. BROWN, J. S., & SLATER-HAMMEL, A. T. The rate of handling information. Key
Discrete movements in the horizontal pressing responses to light patterns.
plane as a function of their length and USAF Hum. Fact. Operat. Res. Lab., 1953,
direction. /. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, Rep. No. 34.
84-95.
2. BRYAN, W. L. On the development of vol- 19. MAYNARD, H. B., STEGEMERTEN, G. J., &
untary motor ability. Amer. J. Psychol., SCHWAB, J. L. Methods—time measure-
1892, S, 12S-204. ment. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1948.
3. CRAIG, D. R. Effect of amplitude range on 20. MILLER, G. A. What is information meas-
duration of responses to step function dis- urement? Amer. Psychologist, 1953, 8,
placements. Dayton, 0.: U. S. Air Force, 3-11.
Air Materiel Command, Wright-Patter- 21. SEARLE, L, V., & TAYLOR, F. V. Studies in
son Air Force Base, 1949. (AF Tech. tracking behavior. I. Rate and time char-
Rep. No. 5913.) acteristics of simple corrective move-
4. CRONBACH, L. J. A consideration of infor- ments. /. exp. Psychol., 1948, 38,
mation theory and utility theory as tools 615-631.
for psychometric problems. Urbana, 111.: 22. SHANNON, C, & WEAVER, W. The mathe-
Bureau of Research and Service, Univ. of matical theory of communication. Urbana:
Illinois, 1953. Univ. of Illinois Press, 1949.
5. CROSSMAN, E. R. F. W. Entropy and 23. STETSON, R. H., & McDiLL, J. A. Mecha-
choice time: the effect of frequency unbal- nism of the different types of movement.
ance on choice-responses. Quart. J. exp. Psychol. Monogr., 1923,32, No. 3 (Whole
Psychol., 1953, S, 41-51. No. 145), 18-40.
6. DODGE, R., & CLINE, T. S. The angle veloc- 24. WESTHEIMER, G. The response of the oculo-
ity of eye movements. Psychol. Rev., motor system to visual stimuli in the
1901, 8, 145-157. horizontal plane. Unpublished doctor's
7. ELLSON, D. G. The application of opera- dissertation, Ohio State Univ., 1953.
tional analysis to human motor behavior. 25. WOODWORTH, R. S. The accuracy of volun-
Psychol. Rev., 1949, 56, 9-17. tary movement. Psychol. Monogr., 1899,
8. FENN, W. O. Mechanics of muscular con- 3, No. 2 (Whole No. 13).
traction in man. /. appl. Physics, 1938,
9,165-177. (Received for early publication March 13, 1954)

You might also like