Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Top Lang Disorders

Vol. 37, No. 3, pp. 217–228


Copyright ⃝c 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

What Exactly Is Play, and Why


Is It Such a Powerful Vehicle for
Learning?
Peter Gray

“Play” is a word used commonly to refer to children’s preferred activities and to some adult
activities, and it is often said that play promotes learning. But what is play exactly, and what
and how do children learn through play? This essay begins with a description of an evolutionary,
practice theory of play by German philosopher and naturalist, Karl Groos, followed by a system
of categorizing play according to the kinds of skills most obviously practiced: physical/locomotor
play, constructive play, language play, fantasy play, social play, and play with formal rules. Play is
then defined as activity that (1) is self-chosen and self-directed, (2) is motivated by means more
than ends, (3) is guided by mental rules, and (4) includes a strong element of imagination. These
characteristics are elaborated upon to show how each contributes to play’s developmental value.
Two final sections describe the special developmental value of age-mixed play and deleterious
changes in children’s well-being that have accompanied the decline of play in recent decades.
Key words: age-mixed play, learning, play, play deprivation, practice theory of play

P LAY is a concept that fills our minds


with contradictions when we try to think
deeply about it. It is serious, yet not seri-
PLAY AS PRACTICE

The first person to develop a Darwinian,


ous; trivial, yet profound; and imaginative and evolutionary theory of play was the German
spontaneous, yet bound by rules. Play is not philosopher and naturalist, Karl Groos. In his
real, it takes place in a fantasy world; yet it is book, The Play of Animals, published in
about the real world and helps children cope 1898, Groos argued that play came about by
with that world. It is childish, yet it underlies natural selection as a means to ensure that
many of the greatest achievements of adults. animals will practice the skills they need in
The primary goal of this article is to de- order to survive and reproduce. This practice
scribe, briefly, the nature and developmental theory of play is quite well accepted today
functions of play from a biological, evolution- by most researchers who study play in ani-
ary perspective. Related to this, a second goal mals (Bateson, 2014). It explains well a num-
is to describe developmental consequences of ber of basic facts about play, such as why
play deprivation, which is much too common young animals play more than older ones, why
among children in our culture today. those animal species that have most to learn
play the most, and why animals play most
at skills that are crucial to their survival. To
a considerable degree, one can predict how
Author Affiliation: Department of Psychology,
Boston College, Chestnut Hill, Massachusetts.
an animal will play by knowing what skills it
must develop to survive and reproduce. For
The author has indicated that he has no financial and
no nonfinancial relationships to disclose.
example, lion cubs and other young preda-
tors play at stalking or chasing and pounc-
Corresponding Author: Peter Gray, PhD, Depart-
ment of Psychology, Boston College, McGuinn 300,
ing. In contrast, young gazelles and other
140 Commonwealth Ave, Chestnut Hill, MA 02467 animals that are preyed upon by lions and
(grayp@bc.edu). such play at fleeing and dodging (Gomendio,
DOI: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000130 1988).
217

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
218 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2017

In a second book, The Play of Man, Groos high up in trees, run along cliffs, and
(1901) extended his insights about animal so on, as that is how they practice con-
play to humans. He pointed out that human trolling their minds and bodies while ex-
beings, having much more to learn than other periencing fear (Gray, 2011a; Sandseter,

! Constructive play. We are the species


animal species, play more than other animals. 2011). In this way, they develop courage.
He also pointed out that humans, unlike the
young of other animals, must learn not just with opposable thumbs, which survives
the skills that are crucial to their species ev- by building things, such as tools, shel-
erywhere but also those that are unique to ters, and means of conveyance. It is no
the specific culture in which they develop. surprise, therefore, that children every-
Therefore, he argued, natural selection led to where, when free to do so, play at build-
a strong drive, in human children, to observe ing things, though what they build varies
the activities of their elders and incorporate across culture. Extensions of constructive
those activities into their play. play include artistic and musical play, the
Anthropologists have confirmed this aspect construction of psychologically meaning-

! Language play. We are the linguistic


of Groos’s theory many times (Gray, 2012b; ful sights and sounds.
Lancy, 2015). Children in hunter-gatherer cul-
tures play often at hunting and gathering; chil- species, and so we engage in language
dren in farming cultures play often at farming; play to learn to talk. Cooing, babbling, and
and children in many cultures today play often first words are all playful (Bloom & Lahey,
at computers. Although Groos presented his 1978). They are produced for their own
theory as a theory of play, it can be argued that sake, for the joy of producing them, not
it is also a theory of education (Gray, 2013, to get anything. As children grow older,
2016). It is a theory of how children come they play with phrases, puns, rhymes,
into the world biologically designed to learn alliterations, and alternative grammatical
what they must to become effective adults in constructions, which help consolidate
the culture into which they are born. their growing understanding of all as-
From the perspective of Groos’s theory, pects of their native language. Some of
it makes sense to categorize human play in the evidence for this comes from record-
terms of the varieties of skills that children ing young children’s “crib talk,” their
practice in play, with the proviso that the cat- monologues and pretend dialogues when
egory boundaries are not sharp and any given alone, in which they may repeat the same
instance of play might (and usually does) fall phrases, again and again, varying them in
into more than one category. Children every- systematic ways, as if deliberately exper-
where, when free to do so, play in the follow- imenting with nuances of pronunciation

! Physical/locomotor play. This is the kind


ing ways (Lancy, 2015): and meaning (Kuczaj, 1985). When lan-
guage play is carried into adulthood, we

! Fantasy or pretend play. We are the


of play our species shares most clearly call it poetry.
with the young of other mammals. It in-
cludes playful running, leaping, climbing, species that can think of things that are
swinging, chasing, and fighting. All young not actually present. That is the founda-
mammals must develop fit bodies, learn to tion of our inventiveness, our ability to
move in coordinated and effective ways, think of new possibilities, to create hy-
and learn to handle themselves, physically potheses, to reason deductively, or even
and emotionally, in dangerous situations. to think about tomorrow. Children ev-
Young mammals, including our children, erywhere practice these skills in fantasy
are, therefore, motivated to play in ways play, in which they construct pretend
that are physically strenuous and, some- worlds—of princesses, trolls, or heroes—
times, physically risky as well. They climb and then think about and act out what

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Play and Learning 219

might happen in that world. Fantasy play This is true whether they are play fighting,
also feeds into the development of lan- building something together, engaging in
guage, because it exercises the child’s word banter, cocreating a fantasy, or play-
ability to symbolize. A stick can repre- ing a game with formal rules. We are an
sent a horse in fantasy play, just as the intensely social species. We survive by co-
word “horse” can represent a horse in operating and sharing, and social play is
language. Research indicates that devel- how children learn to do that. It is hard to
opments in fantasy play reliably precede imagine any skill more crucial for a satis-
and may help bring on analogous devel- fying human life than that of getting along
opments in language ability (for review, with peers. This cannot be taught; it can
see section on typically developing chil- only be learned through experience, and

! Games with formal rules. All play has


dren in the study by Lewis, 2003). for children that experience comes pri-
marily in play.
implicit rules (as will be explained later),
but in games with formal rules, the DEFINING CHARACTERISTICS OF PLAY
rules are explicit, meaning that they can
be stated rather precisely in words and The aforementioned list identifies varying
passed along verbally from one genera- ways that children play and what they learn
tion of players to the next. Players may in play, but it does not tell us exactly what play
change the rules to meet their desires or is. Two individuals might be engaged in what
needs, but the changes themselves must looks like the same activity, and one might
be stated and agreed upon. In our culture, be playing while the other is not. Play, at least
games with formal rules are generally human play, cannot be defined in terms of the
competitive—like baseball, four square, motor activities involved; it must be defined
chess, and dominoes—and the rules pro- in terms of the motives and attitudes that un-
vide the boundaries for the competition. derlie the activities. Scholars of play tend to
In hunter-gatherer cultures, games with agree that play is best defined in terms of a
formal rules are nearly always coopera- constellation of motives and attitudes, which,
tive and are often dance-like in charac- taken together, make an activity playful.
ter (Gray, 2009; Sutton-Smith & Roberts, In his classic book, Homo Ludens, the
1970). In either case, in playing such Dutch cultural historian Huizinga (1955,
games, children are exercising their abil- p. 13), summed up his extended definition
ity to agree to rules, hold them in mind, of play as follows:
and follow them—skills that are impor- Play is a free activity standing quite consciously

! Social play. This category of play cuts


tant in all human societies. outside “ordinary” life as being “not serious,” but
at the same time absorbing the player intensely
across all the others. All play, regardless of and utterly. It is an activity connected with no ma-
to which other category it belongs, is so- terial interest, and no profit can be gained by it.
cial play when two or more children are It proceeds within its own proper boundaries of
involved together. Children everywhere time and space according to fixed rules and in an
orderly manner.
are naturally drawn to play with other
children, and, except in our own mod- In his influential essay, “The Role of Play
ern society where children are often iso- in Development,” the Russian developmental
lated in homes, social play is far more psychologist, Vygotsky (1978), characterized
common than solo play (Lancy, 2015). children’s play as activity that is “desired” by
Social play is how children learn how to the child, “involves an imaginary situation,”
get along with peers. It is how they learn and “always involves rules.” In a chapter
to compromise, negotiate, recognize one on play in the Handbook of Child Psychol-
another’s needs, and please one another. ogy, Rubin, Fein, and Vandenberg (1983)

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
220 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2017

characterized play as behavior that is (a) are free at any time to quit, and they will quit
intrinsically motivated, (b) focused on means if they are not having fun. Freedom to quit is
rather than ends, (c) distinct from exploratory what makes social play the most democratic of
behavior, (d) nonliteral (involves pretense), all activities. Because players want to keep the
(e) free from externally imposed rules, and game going so as to continue their own fun,
(f) actively (not just passively) engaged in by and because they know that others may quit
the players. at any time, they are motivated to make sure
My analyses of these and other attempts by that others are having fun. That means paying
scholars to define play, coupled with my own attention to what others are saying and even
observations of what everyone refers to as to their nonverbal expressions of happiness
play, have led me to conclude that an activity or unhappiness. In this way, social play pro-
is play, or is playful, to the degree that it con- vides conditions that help children overcome
tains the following four characteristics (Gray, narcissism and learn that they are not, after
2012a).1 That is, play is (1) self-chosen and all, the center of the universe (Gray, 2011a).
self-directed, (2) intrinsically motivated, (3) Social play is where children practice getting
guided by mental rules that leave room for cre- their own needs and desires met while also
ativity, and (4) imaginative. These four char- helping others meet theirs.
acteristics are described, along with their con- If you unobtrusively watch and listen to any
tributions to the developmental and educative group of preschool children playing a make-
value of play, in the subsections that follow. believe game, you will likely observe that they
spend more time talking about how to play
Play Is self-chosen and self-directed than actually playing. What will happen in the
Play is always voluntary. It is what one game? Who will be the princess, or dragon, or
wants to do as opposed to what one is obliged baby sister? Who gets to use which props or
to do. Players not only choose to play but they dress up clothes? You may also be impressed,
also choose what and how to play. They direct as you listen, by the complexity and sophisti-
their own actions in play. If a coach, teacher, cation of the language used in such negotia-
or anyone other than the players themselves tion. The play is worked out verbally before
is directing the action, it is not play, or at least it is acted out, and the negotiations are ver-
not fully play. In play, the child must decide bal. Excellent examples of such negotiations
what to do, follow through on that plan, and can be found in the preschoolers’ play dia-
solve any problems that arise along the way. logues transcribed by Furth (1996). In these
This is how children learn to create their own negotiations, children learn new words and
activities and see them through. constructions from one another in the con-
In social play, the ways of playing must be text of communication that is meaningful to
agreed upon by all of the players. Every idea them. One research study revealed that the
or rule that any player proposes must be ap- language used by preschoolers in their shared
proved, at least tacitly, by all others. There is fantasy play was far more complex than that
a simple reason for this. The most fundamen- used by the same children in a structured
tal freedom in play is freedom to quit. That is teacher-led activity or when they were sitting
part and parcel of play’s voluntary nature. Ev- around a table eating (Fekonja, Marjanovic-
ery player knows that all of the other players Umek, & Kranjc, 2005). Older children, in,
say, a pickup game of baseball, likewise en-
gage in much negotiation, as they work out
balanced teams and ground rules to fit the im-
1 In other writings (e.g., Gray, 2012a), I have listed five
mediate conditions of play and argue about
characteristics of play, but the fifth characteristic—that what is fair or foul.
play is carried out in an active but relatively unstressed When adults take over—such as when chil-
state of mind—follows naturally from all the others. dren’s dramatic play becomes a teacher-led

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Play and Learning 221

activity or a pickup baseball game becomes Competition can turn play into nonplay if
Little League led by an adult coach—the chil- rewards for winning extend beyond the game
dren’s responsibilities for rule-making and ne- itself (Byron & Khazanchi, 2012; Lepper &
gotiation are removed. Now adults create and Henderlong, 2000). “Players” who are mo-
direct the activities and solve the problems, tivated primarily by trophies, praise, or in-
and it is no longer so easy to quit. It is no creased status outside of the game are not fully
longer play and the real lessons of play are playing. Among nonhuman animals, there is a
lost. Little League may be a good place to learn clear distinction between contests (including
how to bunt or to slide into second base, but ritualized battles of bluff as well as actual
it does not provide practice in creating your fights), which are aimed at achieving domi-
own activities, negotiating differences, over- nance, and play, in which strivings for domi-
coming narcissism, and ensuring that other nance must be set aside (e.g., Bekoff, 2001).
players, including those on the other team, Human competitive games can be understood
are having fun (Gray, 2013). as blends of contest and play. The blend can
veer more in one direction or the other, de-
Play is intrinsically motivated—means pending on the degree to which heightened
are more valued than ends out-of-game status or other extrinsic rewards
Play is activity that, from the conscious per- are present for winning.
spective of the player, is done for its own Superficially, the statement that play is ac-
sake more than for some reward outside of tivity done for its own sake may seem to
the activity itself. In other words, it is behav- contradict evolutionary theories about play’s
ior in which means are more valued than ends. functions, which posit that play promotes
When people are not playing, what they value long-term physical, intellectual, social, and
most are the results of their actions. When emotional gains. The contradiction is resolved
people are not playing, they typically opt for by appeal to the players’ conscious motives.
the least effortful way of achieving their goal. To the degree that a person engages in an ac-
In play, however, all this is reversed. In play, tivity deliberately for its long-term benefits as
attention is focused on the means more than opposed to its immediate enjoyment or attrac-
the ends, and players do not necessarily look tion, the activity is not fully play.
for the easiest routes to achieving the ends. People often think of play as frivolous or
Play often has goals, but the goals are ex- trivial, and, in a way, they are right. Play is not
perienced as part and parcel of the activity, directed toward achieving serious real-world
not as the primary reason for the activity. goals such as food, money, praise, or an addi-
Goals in play are subordinate to the means tion to one’s résumé, and it takes place at least
for achieving them. For example, construc- partly in a fantasy world. So, it is indeed trivial.
tive play is always directed toward the goal But here is the most delicious of play’s para-
of creating the object that the players have in doxes: The enormous educational power of
mind, but the primary objective in such play play lies in its triviality.
is the creation of the object, not the having Play is the ideal context for practicing new
of the object once it is created. Children play skills or trying out new ways of doing things
intently at building a beautiful sandcastle, al- precisely because play has no real-world con-
though they know that when the tide rises, sequence. Nobody is judging, no trophy is on
it will be washed to the sea. Similarly, com- the line, no teammates will be let down, and
petitive play is directed toward the goal of so the player is free to fail. With freedom to
scoring points and winning, but if the activity fail comes freedom to experiment. The play
is truly play, then it is the process of scor- world is a simulation world, a safe and fun
ing and winning that matters to the player, place to practice for the real world.
not some subsequent consequence of having Players may exert great effort to move
scored and won. gracefully or create a beautiful product, but

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
222 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2017

the primary reward comes from the doing, The rule-based nature of play is an exten-
not from the product. Attention is focused on sion of the point made earlier about the promi-
the activity itself, which is where it should be nence of means in play. The rules of play are
focused when learning a new skill or trying an essential aspect of the means. The rules
out modifications of an old one. Play is often provide boundaries within which the actions
highly repetitive, which fits with the idea of at- must occur, but they do not precisely dic-
tention to means. Children at play do the same tate each action. The rules always leave room
things again and again, perhaps making small for creativity. Activities that are precisely
changes each time. Repetition and systematic prescribed by rules are better referred to as
variation are part and parcel of practice. rituals than as play.
Many research studies have shown that chil- Different types of play have different types
dren and adults are more creative when they of rules. A basic rule of constructive play, for
are playing than when they are trying to im- example, is that you must work with the cho-
press a judge or win a reward. For example, sen medium in a manner aimed at producing
Amabile (1996) has conducted many exper- or depicting some specific object or design
iments in which participants were asked to that you have in mind, such as a sandcastle.
produce some creative product, such as a In shared fantasy play, the fundamental rule
poem or a collage, under varying conditions. is that players must abide by their shared un-
In some conditions, the participants were in- derstanding of the roles that they are playing;
structed to do this just for fun and were told they must stay in character. Even playful fight-
that their names would not be on the prod- ing and chasing, which may look wild to the
ucts. In other conditions, they were told that observer, is constrained by rules. An always
this was a contest and that those with the most present rule in children’s play fighting, for ex-
highly creative products would win prizes. ample, is that the players mimic some of the
The result, inevitably, was that the products actions of serious fighting but do not really
of those engaged just for fun were found to be hurt the other person. They do not hit with all
more creative than the products of those who their force (at least not if they are the stronger
were trying to win prizes. Other experiments of the two); they do not kick, bite, or scratch.
have shown that research participants are far Because of its rule-based nature, play is always
more able to solve problems that require them an exercise in self-restraint.
to see novel uses for familiar objects when It is interesting to consider the similarity be-
they are in a playful state of mind than in a tween play and language with regard to rules.
serious state of mind (e.g., Isen, Daubman, & All human languages, of course, are based
Nowicki, 1987). on rules—rules of phonology, morphology,
and syntax. Whenever we speak, we are us-
Play is guided by mental rules, but the ing those rules implicitly to produce a struc-
rules leave room for creativity tured statement, yet that statement is a new
Play is freely chosen activity but not free- creation. Like playing, speaking is always a
form or random activity. Play always has struc- creative yet rule-based activity. In linguistic
ture, and that structure derives from rules in play, children play with and practice directly
the players’ minds. In social play, the rules the rules of their native language, but in all
must be shared by all the players. The rule- play, they practice a basic ability that under-
based nature of play is the characteristic that lies language, the ability to create something
Vygotsky (1978) emphasized most strongly in new that nevertheless abides by a set of rules
his essay about the roles of play in devel- (Lewis, 2003).
opment. He argued that play is the primary
means by which children learn to abide by Play is imaginative
socially agreed-upon rules, an ability that is Play always involves some degree of men-
essential to every human society. tal removal of oneself from the immediately

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Play and Learning 223

present real world into an imaginary world. dren ranging from 4 to 8 years of age, or 7 to 14
Imagination is most obvious in fantasy play, years of age. Often, such groups would also
where the players create the characters and include toddlers, who were being cared for
plot, but it is also present to varying degrees by older siblings in the context of play. Only
in all other forms of human play. In rough and with the advent of age-graded schools and age-
tumble physical play, the fight is a pretend graded adult-led activities outside of school
fight, not a real one. In constructive play, the have children been regularly segregated from
players may say that they are building a castle one another by age. It is difficult now to find
from sand, but they know that it is a pretend places where one can observe free age-mixed
castle. In formal games with explicit rules, the play, but my colleagues and I have been able
players must accept an already established fic- to do so at a radically alternative democratic
tional situation that provides the foundation school, where students 4–18 years of age are
for the rules. For example, in the real world, free, all school day, to follow their own in-
you can get home by any of an infinite number terests and associate with whom they please
of different routes, any time you choose, but (Gray, 2011b). The founders of the school
in the fantasy world of baseball, you must get believe that free age mixing is the key to
“home” by running from base to base around the school’s educational success (Greenberg,
a diamond-shaped path, only after a pitch 1992).
occurs. When children who differ widely in age
The imaginative aspect of play is the charac- and ability play together, the older ones,
teristic that Huizinga (1955) emphasized most by necessity, boost the younger ones up
strongly, as he built his argument that play to higher levels of activity. For example,
provides the engine for cultural innovations. children younger than about 9 years generally
This is also the characteristic most strongly cannot play complicated board or card games
emphasized by researchers who focus on the with one another. They lose track of the
role of play in the development of creativity rules, their attention wanders, and the game
and the ability to think in ways that go be- quickly disintegrates. But children younger
yond the concrete here and now. As Vygotsky than that can play and enjoy such games
(1978) pointed out, the imaginative nature when they are playing with older children
of play is, in a sense, the flip side of play’s or adolescents (Gray & Feldman, 2004). The
rule-based nature. To the degree that play older players remind the younger ones what
takes place in an imagined world, the play- to do: “Hold your cards up.” “Pay attention.”
ers’ actions must be governed by rules that “Try to remember what cards have been
are in the minds of the players rather than by played.” “Think ahead.” Paying attention,
laws of nature or impulsive instincts. A great remembering, and thinking ahead are the ele-
human ability, which distinguishes us from ments of intelligence. In keeping the younger
other animals, is our ability to imagine in ways players on task to keep the game going, the
that are not random but are structured by older players in such a game are, in effect,
rules, which allows us to produce potentially boosting the younger players’ intelligence.
useful new products. That is the essence of We have also observed young children learn
creativity. to read or to perform mathematical calcula-
tions by playing games with older children
THE SPECIAL VALUE OF AGE-MIXED that involve reading or calculations (Gray,
PLAY 2011b). Simply to keep the game going, the
older children point out the elements of
Through most of human history, children reading and calculating to the younger ones.
almost always played in age-mixed groups A number of research studies have exam-
(Gray, 2011b; Konner, 1975, 2010). A typical ined the effects of creating multiage class-
group playing together might consist of chil- rooms in traditional schools and preschools.

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
224 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2017

In separate studies, Goldman (1981) and dren climbing trees or solving puzzles, for ex-
Mounts and Roupnarine (1987) found that ample, and then they want to do that, so they
3-year-olds engaged in more advanced levels work at it by emulating the older children’s
of play when they were in classes that in- actions. They hear older children talking and,
cluded 4-year-olds than when they were in in that way, acquire a richer vocabulary, new
classes that included only 3-year-olds. Bailey, linguistic constructions, and new ideas. They
Burchinal, and McWilliam (1993) found that see older children reading and talking about
1- and 2-year-olds in mixed-age childcare what they have read, and that motivates them
groups (which included 3- and 4-year-olds) to learn to read. My own observations suggest
exhibited more rapid development of motor, that children are more prone to learn in these
cognitive, and linguistic abilities than did 1- ways from children who are a little older than
and 2-year-olds in same-age childcare groups. themselves than they are from adults, because
Christie, Stone, and Deutscher (2002) found adults are too far beyond them, too much in a
that kindergarteners in a multiage classroom, different world (Gray, 2011b).
which included first and second graders, Older children also learn crucial lessons
played much more frequently at literary activ- through playing with younger ones. They gain
ities (playful reading) than did kindergarten- a sense of their own maturity as they practice
ers in a kindergarten-only classroom. In a caring, protecting, and leading. Cross-cultural
summer enrichment program that included research has revealed that children who have
children from preschool age through fifth regular contact with younger ones are gener-
grade, Emfinger (2009) noted many instances ally kinder, not just to the younger children
of older children teaching numerical concepts but also to one another, than are children
to younger children in the context of play. who do not have such regular contact with
Angell (1998) reported on instances in which younger ones (Ember, 1973; Whiting, 1983).
the moral reasoning of younger children was Often, in age-mixed play, the older children
enhanced by interaction with older children can be heard explaining rules and concepts
in a Montessori classroom of children 9–12 to the younger ones, and to do so, they must
years of age. make their own implicit understanding ex-
Other research shows that age mixing can plicit (Gray & Feldman, 2004). They must re-
enable true social play in children previously think what they know, so that they can put it
thought to be too young to play socially. Two- into words that the younger ones can under-
and 3-year-olds, when placed only with age stand. Adult teachers regularly discover that
mates, engage in side-by-side parallel play, they learn more by teaching than by being
paying some attention to one another but not taught, and in age-mixed play, children have
merging their play into a socially combined many natural opportunities to teach.
activity. However, as anthropologist Konner Research and observations also suggest that
(1975) has pointed out, such play is an artifact age mixing can help socially withdrawn chil-
of age-segregated nursery schools. Tradition- dren become more socially active and compe-
ally, toddlers have been more or less always tent. In one experiment, socially withdrawn
in the presence of older children who could 4- and 5-year-olds were systematically paired
draw them up into truly social play. In sep- with either same-age peers (within 3 months
arate studies, Howes and Farver (1987) and in age) or younger peers (more than a year
Maynard (2002) found that 4- and 5-year-olds younger) for a series of one-on-one play ses-
can engage 2-year-olds in truly shared fantasy sions. Withdrawn children in both of these
play by structuring the toddlers’ roles and ex- groups showed greater improvement in so-
plaining to them what to do. cial behavior, as measured by their subse-
Even when they are not playing together, quent social activity in their day care center,
younger children learn from older ones by than those in the untreated control group, but
watching and listening. They see older chil- those paired with younger children showed

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Play and Learning 225

the greatest improvement (Furman, Rahe, & ents were asked about their own childhood
Hartup, 1979). In fact, at the end of the se- play life compared with that of their children
ries of sessions, those paired with younger (Clements, 2004; O’Brien & Smith, 2002). This
children were as socially active as the aver- decline has resulted most likely from a va-
age child in the centers. This study was in- riety of social changes, including (a) a rise
spired by an earlier set of studies showing in parental and societal fears about the risks
that socially isolated young monkeys could entailed in children’s free play, away from
acquire social competence through being adults; (b) an increase in children’s time spent
paired with younger monkeys but not through in school and at homework; (c) an increased
being paired with age mates (Novak & Har- tendency for children to be enrolled in adult-
low, 1975; Suomi & Harlow, 1972). In both led activities even out of school rather than
the monkey and human experiments, the im- allowed to play freely; (d) a decline in the de-
provement apparently occurred because the gree to which neighbors know one another,
socially inhibited individuals were less fright- resulting in a decline in neighborhood play;
ened of younger individuals than of age mates and (e) a decline in family size and in total
and were stimulated by the younger ones into number of children in many neighborhoods,
play. Less formally, I have observed at least resulting in fewer potential playmates (Gray,
one instance of a socially withdrawn boy who, 2013).
after enrolling at the age-mixed school where Over the same period of time that play
I was observing, began by playing only with has been declining, there have been large,
much younger and much older children. Over well-documented declines in mental and so-
time, as he lost his social fears and acquired cial well-being among young people, of the
social skills, he began increasingly to play also sort that would be predicted if play indeed
with children of his own age. The idea that serves the functions described earlier in this
free age mixing may help socially inhibited article. The most telling data come from cross-
children overcome their inhibitions warrants temporal meta-analyses of clinical question-
further study. naires that have been used with young people
in unchanged form over the decades. For ex-
CONSEQUENCE OF THE DECLINE IN ample, analyses of scores on the depression
CHILDREN’S PLAY scale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personal-
ity Inventory and scores on Taylor’s Manifest
If play is how children learn to create their Anxiety Scale indicate that the rates of what
own activities, solve their own problems, take today would be labeled as Major Depressive
control of their own lives, get along with Disorder and as Generalized Anxiety Disor-
peers, overcome narcissism, and learn to deal der are, for teenagers and young adults, five
with fear, then the lack of play would be ex- to eight times what they were in the 1950s
pected to have serious consequences. There (Twenge, 2000; Twenge et al., 2010). The
is good evidence that it does. increase over time in these scores has been
Over the past 50–60 years, in the United roughly linear, just as the decline in play has
States, there has been a continuous, well- been roughly linear. In an analysis of clini-
documented, ultimately huge decline in chil- cal assessments of locus of control, Twenge,
dren’s opportunities and freedom to play— Zhang, and Im (2004) also found a continu-
to really play, with other children, in their ous decline in young people’s sense of be-
own ways, without adult interference (Gray, ing in control of their own lives, between the
2011a, 2013). Evidence for this comes from years 1960 and 2000. Clinical psychologists
traditional historical analyses (Chudacoff, have long known that a lack of internal sense
2007), from diary studies of how children’s of control predisposes a person for depres-
time is spent (Hofferth, 2009; Hofferth & Sand- sion and anxiety (Alloy et al., 2006; Weems
berg, 2001), and from surveys in which par- & Silverman, 2006). So, a reasonable causal

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
226 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2017

chain here is that, with reduced freedom to opportunities for age-mixed play. Along with
play, children fail to develop a strong sense the other disorders just noted, there has also
of control over their own lives (as play is been a rise in young people who are diag-
where they do control their own activities), nosed as on the autism spectrum. These peo-
which, in turn, increases their likelihood of ple are typically socially disinterested, not
depression and anxiety. Moreover, research playful, and delayed in language development.
with monkeys and rats has revealed that play Although the tendency toward autism is well
deprivation in animals creates symptoms established to be genetically determined, with
similar to human depression and anxiety symptoms appearing in early infancy, it seems
(LaFreniere, 2011; Pellis, Pellis, & Bell, 2010). quite possible that deprivation of play may
Other cross-temporal research has re- intensify those symptoms or prevent their
vealed in young people increased narcissism amelioration. At one democratic, age-mixed
(Twenge & Foster, 2010), decreased empathy school, I had the opportunity to observe a
(Konrath, O’Brien, & Hsing, 2011), and de- boy on the autism spectrum who had appar-
creased creativity (Kim, 2011) over the past ently made great social strides over the time
several decades. These, again, are changes that he had been enrolled there. He did not
that would be predicted if, as contended pre- at first seek or respond well to social interac-
viously, play is a major vehicle for children’s tion, but he had excellent computer skills. Be-
overcoming narcissism, learning to attend to cause of this skill, others, including younger
others’ needs, and learning to create their children, were drawn to him for computer
own activities. Also consistent with the view play, and this led to increasing levels of social
that play promotes self-control and creative interaction, which sometimes even went be-
thinking are a study showing that children yond the focus on the computer. This obser-
who had more free time to play scored bet- vation is consistent with research showing im-
ter on a test of self-directed executive pro- proved social and linguistic development, in
cessing than did those whose time was more children on the autism spectrum, when they
fully structured by adults (Barker, Semenov, are integrated with typically developing peers
Michaelson, Provan, & Snyder, 2014) and a in a supportive play environment (Wolfberg,
longitudinal study showing that children who Bottema-Beutel, & DeWitt, 2012; Wolfberg,
had more freedom to play and explore ex- 2016).
hibited greater creative potential years later, We are at present a society very much ori-
in adolescence, than those who had less ented toward formal therapy, by adult ther-
such freedom (Harrington, Block, & Block, apists, as routes to trying to help children
1987). with developmental difficulties. Perhaps if we
It seems plausible that some of the dis- could reinstate the levels of age-mixed play
abilities affecting language development to- that, in the past, occurred regularly in homes
day are at least exacerbated by the decline and neighborhoods, some of these problems
in play, perhaps especially by the decline in would resolve themselves.

REFERENCES

Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, Angell, A. V. (1998). Practicing democracy at school:
M. E., Panzarella, C., & Rose, D. T. (2006). Prospec- A qualitative analysis of an elementary class coun-
tive incidence of first onsets and recurrence of depres- cil. Theory and Research in Social Education, 26,
sion in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for 149–172.
depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, Bailey, D. B., Burchinal, M. R., & McWilliam, R. A. (1993).
145–156. Age of peers and early childhood development. Child
Amabile, T. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to the Development, 64, 848–862.
social psychology of creativity. Boulder, CO: West- Barker, J. E., Semenov, A. D., Michaelson, L., Provan, L.
view Press. S., & Snyder, H. R. (2014). Less-structured time in

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Play and Learning 227

children’s daily lives predicts self-directed executive Gray, P. (2012b). The value of a play-filled childhood in
functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–16. development of the hunter-gatherer individual. In D.
Bateson, P. (2014). Play, playfulness, creativity and Narvaez, J. Panksepp, A. Schore, & T. Gleason (Eds.),
innovation. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1, Evolution, early experience and human develop-
99–112. ment: From research to practice and policy. New
Bekoff, M. (2001). Social play behavior: Cooperation, fair- York, NY: Oxford University Press.
ness, trust, and the evolution of morality. Journal of Gray, P. (2013). Free to learn: Why unleashing the in-
Consciousness Studies, 8, 81–90. stinct to play will make our children happier, more
Bloom, L. M., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development self-reliant, and better students for life. New York,
and language disorders. New York, NY: Wiley. NY: Basic Books.
Byron, K., & Khazanchi, S. (2012). Rewards and cre- Gray, P. (2016). Children’s natural ways of learning still
ative performance: A meta-analytic test of theoretically work—even for the three Rs. In D. C. Geary, & D. B.
derived hypotheses. Psychological Bulletin, 138, Berch (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on child de-
809–830. velopment and education (pp. 63–93). New York,
Christie, J. F., Stone, S. J., & Deutscher, R. (2002). Play in NY: Springer.
same-age and multiage grouping arrangements. In J. L. Gray, P., & Feldman, J. (2004). Playing in the zone of
Roopnarine (Ed.), Conceptual, social-cognitive, and proximal development: Qualities of self-directed age
contextual issues in the fields of play. Westport, CT: mixing between adolescents and young children at a
Ablex. democratic school. American Journal of Education,
Chudacoff, H. P. (2007). Children at play: An American 110, 108–145.
history. New York, NY: New York University Press. Greenberg, D. (1992). Sudbury valley’s secret weapon:
Clements, R. (2004). An investigation of the status of out- Allowing people of different ages to mix freely at the
door play. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, school. In D. Greenberg (Ed.), The Sudbury Valley
5, 68–80. School experience (3rd ed.). Framingham, MA: Sud-
Ember, R. (1973). Feminine task assignment and the social bury Valley School Press.
behavior of boys. Ethos, 1, 424–439. Groos, K. (1898). The play of animals. New York, NY:
Emfinger, K. (2009). Numerical conceptions reflected Appleton.
during multiage child-initiated pretend play. Journal Groos, K. (1901). The play of man. New York, NY:
of Instructional Psychology, 36, 326–334. Appleton.
Fekonja, U., Marjanovic-Umek, L., & Kranjc, S. (2005). Harrington, D. H., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1987). Testing
Free play and other daily preschool activities as a con- aspects of Carl Rogers’s theory of creative environ-
text for child’s language development. Studia Psycho- ments: Child-rearing antecedents of creative potential
logica, 47, 103–116. in young adolescents. Journal of Personality and So-
Furman, W., Rahe, D. F., & Hartup, W. W. (1979). Re- cial Psychology, 52, 851–856.
habilitation of socially withdrawn preschool children Hofferth, S. (2009). Changes in American children’s time,
through mixed-age and same-age socialization. Child 1997–2003. International Journal of Time Use Re-
Development, 50, 915–922. search, 6, 26–47.
Furth, H. G. (1996). Desire for society: Children’s knowl- Hofferth, S. L., & Sandberg, J. F. (2001). Changes in Amer-
edge as social imagination. New York, NY: Plenum. ican children’s time, 1981–1997. In T. Owens, & S. L.
Goldman, J. A. (1981). Social participation of preschool Hofferth (Eds.), Children at the millennium: Where
children in same- versus mixed-age groups. Child De- have we come from, where are we going? (pp. 193–
velopment, 32, 644–650. 229). New York: Elsevier Science.
Gomendio, M. (1988). The development of different Howes, C., & Farver, J. (1987). Social pretend play in 2-
types of play in gazelles: Implications for the nature year-olds: Effects of age of partner. Early Childhood
and functions of play. Animal Behaviour, 36, 825– Research Quarterly, 2, 305–314.
836. Huizinga, J. (1955). Homo Ludens: A study of the play-
Gray, P. (2009). Play as the foundation for hunter-gatherer element in culture. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
social existence. American Journal of Play, 1, 476– Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Pos-
522. itive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Jour-
Gray, P. (2011a). The decline of play and the rise of psy- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–
chopathology in childhood and adolescence. Ameri- 1131.
can Journal of Play, 3, 443–463. Kim, K. H. (2011). The creativity crisis: The decrease
Gray, P. (2011b). The special value of age-mixed play. in creative thinking scores on the Torrance Tests of
American Journal of Play, 3, 500–522. Creative Thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23,
Gray, P. (2012a). Definition of play. In Encyclopedia 285–295.
of play science. Retrieved from http://www.scholar Konner, M. (1975). Relations among infants and juve-
pedia.org/article/Encyclopedia_of_Play_Science. Ac- niles in comparative perspective. In M. Lewis, & L. A.
cessed June 24, 2017. Rosenblum (Eds.), The origins of behavior, vol. 4:

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
228 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2017

Friendship and peer relations (pp. 99–129). New Suomi, S. J., & Harlow, H. F. (1972). Social rehabilita-
York, NY: Wiley. tion of isolate-reared monkeys. Developmental Psy-
Konner, M. (2010). The evolution of childhood. Cam- chology, 6, 487–496.
bridge, MA: Belknap. Sutton-Smith, B., & Roberts, J. M. (1970). The cross-
Konrath, S. H., O’Brien, E. H., & Hsing, C. (2011). Changes cultural and psychological study of games. In G.
in dispositional empathy in American college students Lüschen (Ed.), The cross-cultural analysis of sport
over time: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social and games. Champaign, IL: Stipes.
Psychology Review, 15, 180–198. Twenge, J. M. (2000). The age of anxiety? Birth cohort
Kuczaj, S. A. (1985). Language play. Early Child Devel- changes in anxiety and neuroticism, 1952–1993, Jour-
opment and Care, 19, 53–67. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1007–
LaFreniere, P. (2011). Evolutionary functions of social 1021.
play: Life histories, sex differences, and emotion reg- Twenge, J. M., & Foster, J. D. (2010). Birth cohort in-
ulation. American Journal of Play, 3, 446–488. creases in narcissistic personality traits among Amer-
Lancy, D. F. (2015). The anthropology of childhood: ican college students, 1982–2009. Social Psychologi-
Cherubs, chattel, changelings (2nd ed.). Cambridge, cal and Personality Science, 1, 99–106.
UK: Cambridge University Press. Twenge, J. M., Gentile, B., DeWall, C. N., Ma, D., Lace-
Lepper, M. R., & Henderlong, J. (2000). Turning “play” field, K., & Schurtz, D. R. (2010). Birth cohort in-
into “work” and “work” into “play”: 25 years of re- creases in psychopathology among young Americans,
search on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In C. San- 1938–2007: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of the
sone, & J. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrin- MMPI. Clinical Psychology Review, 30 145–154.
sic motivation: The search for optimal motivation Twenge, J. M., Zhang, L., & Im, C. (2004). It’s be-
and performance (pp. 257–307). San Diego, CA: Aca- yond my control: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of
demic Press. increasing externality in locus of control, 1960–
Lewis, V. (2003). Play and language in children with 2002. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8,
autism. Autism, 7, 391–399. 308–319.
Maynard, A. E. (2002). Cultural teaching: The develop- Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). The role of play in develop-
ment of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. ment. In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, &
Child Development, 73, 969–982. E. Souberman (Eds.), Mind in society: The develop-
Mounts, N. S., & Roopnarine, J. L. (1987). Social-cognitive ment of higher psychological processes (pp. 92–104).
play patterns in same-age and mixed-age preschool Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
classrooms. American Educational Research Jour- Weems, C. F., & Silverman, W. K. (2006). An integra-
nal, 24, 463–476. tive model of control: Implications for understand-
Novak, M. A., & Harlow, H. F. (1975). Social recovery of ing emotion regulation and dysregulation in child-
monkeys isolated for the first year of life: 1. Rehabil- hood anxiety. Journal of Affective Disorders, 91,
itation and therapy. Developmental Psychology, 11, 113–124.
453–465. Whiting, B. B. (1983). The genesis of prosocial behavior.
O’Brien, J., & Smith, J. (2002). Childhood transformed? In D. L. Bridgeman (Ed.), The nature of prosocial de-
Risk perceptions and the decline of free play. British velopment: Interdisciplinary theories and strategies.
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65, 123–128. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Pellis, S. M., Pellis, V. C., & Bell, H. C. (2010), The func- Wolfberg, P., Bottema-Beutel, K., & DeWitt, M. (2012).
tions of play in the development of the social brain. Including children with autism in social and imaginary
American Journal of Play, 2, 278–296. play with typical peers. American Journal of Play, 5,
Rubin, K. H., Fein, G. G., & Vandenberg, B. (1983). Play. In 55–80.
P. H. Mussen, & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook Wolfberg, P. (2016). Integrated play groups model: Sup-
of child psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 693–774). New York, porting children with autism in essential play expe-
NY: Wiley. riences with typical peers. In L. A. Reddy, T. M.
Sandseter, E. (2011). Children’s risky play from an evo- Files-Hall, & C. E. Schaefer (Eds.), Empirically based
lutionary perspective. Evolutionary Psychology, 9, play interventions for children (2nd ed.). Washing-
257–284. ton, DC: American Psychological Association.

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like