Fajkowska2015 PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Research in Personality


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp

The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical


assumptions q
Małgorzata Fajkowska ⇑
Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Poland

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In this paper I outline my model of personality, which I refer to as the Complex-System Approach to Per-
Available online xxxx sonality (C-SAP). The C-SAP’s meta-theoretical background embodies the general principles of systems
thinking and is designed to reconcile one of personality psychology cardinal concerns—personality coher-
Keywords: ence and incoherence. However, within the approach presented here one can find possible answers to the
Personality as a complex system questions about definition of personality; its organization and development; a fundamental structure of
System of Regulation and Integration personality architecture (here the System of Regulation and Integration Stimulation); explanation of per-
Stimulation
sonality traits (in light of their dominant function—reactive, regulative, or self-regulative—in controlling
Personality coherence/incoherence
Personality traits
stimulation); and their usefulness in an analysis of personality coherence/incoherence, understood at this
Subtypes of anxiety point as functional consistency/inconsistency over stimulation processing between temperament traits
Adaptive significance of personality and other personality traits related to stimulation processing. Moreover, it is demonstrated how coher-
coherence/incoherence ent/incoherent types of personalities are built when types of anxiety are considered as these other per-
sonality traits related to stimulation processing. Also, the methods for analyzing the functional (adaptive)
significance of coherent/incoherent personality structures are shown. And finally, I present the selected
results from my studies aimed at testing the functional role of personality coherence/incoherence in rela-
tion to the quality and dynamics of attentional performance.
Ó 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction This conversation inspired me so much that my model of per-


sonality—the Complex-System Approach to Personality (C-SAP)—
When my daughter Weronika was about eight years old, she comprises meta-theoretical aspects. However, the question arises
unexpectedly asked me to clarify the term ‘‘personality’’ since what exactly we gain by introducing a meta-theoretical perspec-
she wanted to understand why I spent so much time on this topic. tive into a specific model of personality. To answer this question,
I picked up that gauntlet and after my explanation she concluded, I am paraphrasing Cervone and Shoda (1999, p. 18): if we use a
‘‘Well, psychologists—at least you, Mom—have no direct evidence book as an analogy for a personality, then the meta-theory pro-
that personality exists, and I see that what you need is a good vides the vocabulary and the grammatical rules. In other words,
guidebook in discovering personality.’’ meta-theory offers a specialized language and tools that one can
Perhaps my description of personality was not a total failure, use to address specific scientific problems of personality psychol-
since she was absolutely right in her deduction and her answer ogy. Thus, to meet this challenge, I employed General System The-
indirectly pointed to a very important aspect of personality theory. ory (e.g., Bateson, 1979; Odum, 2007; von Bertalanffy, 1968) to
It suggested that personality theory could not be limited only to identify the universal principles, mechanisms, and processes that
rigorous standards of empirical evidence, and that a place should remain constant and explain basic, parallel personality characteris-
be left in this field for meta-theory (viz. ‘‘a good guidebook’’). tics: structure and dynamics, consistency and variability, coher-
ence and incoherence, and its two fundamental functions
(regulation and integration).
Historically, the elements of systems thinking were utilized
q
This paper is adapted from the book Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A with great success in both theoretical and applied branches of psy-
Perspective on Anxiety and Depression by Małgorzata Fajkowska. Copyright Ó 2013 chology—for example, cognitive psychology, existential psychol-
Eliot Werner Publications, Clinton Corners, NY.
⇑ Address: Laboratory of Personality Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Polish ogy, occupational psychology, family psychology, or engineering
Academy of Sciences, Jaracza 1, 00-378 Warsaw, Poland. psychology (e.g., Beishon & Peters, 1976; Brennan, 2002;
E-mail address: mfajkowska@psych.pan.pl Singleton, 1989; Tschacher & Dauwalder, 2003). Therefore why

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
0092-6566/Ó 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
2 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

not apply this approach to personality psychology (see Caprara & 2. Personality as a complex system: a meta-theoretical
Cervone, 2000; Thomas, Segal, & Hersen, 2006) and assume that perspective
introducing a meta-theoretical perspective drawn from a systems
approach into a specific model of personality might advance some If personality can be perceived as a complex system of interact-
aforementioned core concerns of this field of psychology, with a ing elements, the relevant issue in this respect is to go into a more
priority on personality coherence and incoherence. detailed description of the organization, structure, and dynamics of
Theoretical speculations and findings from my studies, as well the system originally provided by General System Theory (von
as particular insights from earlier theories of personality, allowed Bertalanffy, 1950, 1968, 1975) and subsequently developed by
me to move from a meta-theoretical, general systems model of scholars from different disciplines, including personality psychol-
personality to the construction of a specific model of personality. ogy (e.g., Allen & Hoekstra, 1984; King, 1997; Mayer, 1998, 2006,
Above all, I would like to emphasize the understanding of person- 2007; Simon, 1973).
ality and personality coherence by reference to dynamic processes Inspired by the aforementioned theories, I formulated the C-SAP
and analysis of personality with application of correlational–exper- meta-theoretical background in the form of four propositions: (1)
imental procedures. One can identify these topics in the study of personality is a complex, hierarchical system; (2) personality has
complexity in nonlinear, dynamic systems, with special reference a three-level organization; (3) personality develops and changes;
to the social-cognitive theories of personality (e.g., Bandura, and (4) personality reflects either coherent or incoherent structure.
1986, 2006; Cervone, 2004, 2008; Shoda, 1999; Vallacher, However, in this paper I focus mainly on propositions (2) and (4).
Michaels, Wiese, Strawinska, & Nowak, 2013). Besides, I con-
structed my model with the help of theories promoting a structural
and multilayer organization of personality (e.g., the models of 2.1. Personality is a complex, hierarchical system
Eysenck, Murray, or McAdams; for a review see Boyle, Matthews,
& Saklofske, 2008a, Part II; Boyle, Matthews, & Saklofske, 2008b; In short, with reference to the first proposition, I define person-
Campbell, 2008). These theories show how personality organiza- ality as a complex hierarchical system of a set of variables interact-
tion in the structure guides and constrains the cognitive, affective, ing and transacting1 among them and with the environment, which
motivational, and situational processes, and how it co-determines tend to achieve the relatively stable patterns of organization that
the potential behaviors and their interconnections that become may not be entirely predictable. A living system may be analyzed
activated by the relevant features of situations. in terms of its components, entities, or subsystems, or it may be
In other words, to explain personality as a hierarchical system, I viewed as part of a larger system (suprasystem). Thus a hierarchical
combine the dynamic processes in analysis of personality and system is a system of subsystems within a larger system, where the
structural organization of personality, borrowed from (respec- rank is canonical (that is, conforming to rules or established by spe-
tively) the social-cognitive theories of personality and structural cific canons) rather than optional (cf. Lemke, 2000; Simon, 1973; von
theories of personality. Thus my approach seeks to promote the Bertalanffy, 1975). A hierarchical organization has two known
development of integrative theorizing and subsumes particular nested (inclusive) forms: a compositional/scalar hierarchy and a
insights from the earlier perspectives on personality mentioned specification/subsumptive hierarchy.2 The first one, based on scale
above, while permitting both within-person and between-person extension, describes components of hierarchy, organization of the
comparisons. whole system, its stability and controlling mechanisms. A good
Although I acknowledge the influences of the social-cognitive example of compositional hierarchy is the organization of some
and structural theories of personality, I believe that the principles organisms, which can be reduced to organ systems, which are com-
existing within these two approaches connected with the trait sta- posed of organs, which are composed of tissues, which are composed
tus are misconceived. In consequence, the crucial requirement for of cells, which are composed of molecules, which are composed of
proceeding within the unitary framework proposed here should atoms (cf. Salthe, 1985, 1991, 1993). In other words, the premises
be a revised and refined construct of trait, personality structure, of compositional hierarchy allow us to identify how personality is
and personality dynamics to take account of the data and theoret- an ordering of the parts that make up a system (i.e., ‘‘composed’’
ical developments on personality and personality coherence. of these parts) and propose the general controlling mechanisms per-
Accordingly, in this paper I briefly demonstrate how the C-SAP mitting for explaining the relative stability of the whole personality
draws on ‘‘overarching’’ systems theory to deliver a framework for system. (All of these are more specifically and extensively discussed
describing personality. Furthermore, the basic concepts portrayed below with reference to proposition [2].)
have been developed from a meta-theoretical analysis, which com- The second form of hierarchical organization is a specification
poses an explanatory framework that provides an understanding of or a subsumptive hierarchy, based on descriptive complexity
personality, coherence, and incoherence and a fundamental struc- intension. For instance, a taxonomic hierarchy represents a sub-
ture of personality architecture—the System of Regulation and sumptive organization. Applying this hierarchy to personality
Integration Stimulation (SRIS). Also, the selected results from my enables us to understand how parts of personality are ‘‘subsumed’’
studies aimed at testing some aspects of the theory presented here (i.e., classified from the general objects to the specific ones; cf.
are shown. For now, the main focus of this research is the func- Salthe, 1985, 1988, 1991). In other words, this form provides the
tional role of personality coherence and incoherence (understood model of personality system (or subsystems) development. (I refer
here as a functional consistency/inconsistency over stimulation to a subsumptive hierarchy later in this paper, when I am describ-
processing between temperament types and other personality ing the main element of a personality system.)
types related to stimulation processing) and its relation to the Nevertheless, one must remember that hierarchy is a concep-
quality and dynamics of cognitive performance. Thus, to build tual construction, an analytical tool, and its use does not imply that
the coherent/incoherent structures of personality, I introduce to personality itself is actually hierarchically organized. It does seem
this theory affective types (here anxiety) as elements of intraindi-
1
vidual coherence/incoherence. Also, I attempt to integrate affective Here interaction is considered as an action that occurs when two or more
variables of the system have direct influence upon one another. Transaction is an
types into a more comprehensive scheme—a personality system—
action that occurs when two or more variables of the system have indirect (e.g.,
that better captures their complex nature and their relation with through other variable or variables) influence upon one another.
cognitive performance. However, for the moment affective types 2
The concept of a nested (inclusive) hierarchy is frequently exemplified as Russian
are not the core subjects of my research. (Matryoshka) dolls—one contained in another.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx 3

to be in many ways, but to suppose that this is the sole principle useful in understanding individuals’ adaptive mechanisms, in
needed to understand personality would be naive. understanding and predicting individuals’ behaviors, but above
all in describing and explaining personality coherence and incoher-
2.2. Personality has a three-level organization ence. Thus, to satisfy this requirement, I propose to view personal-
ity as organized into three levels that broadly capture these
Relating to the second proposition, I argue that a compositional constructs and demonstrate relations among them. Level L 1
(scalar) hierarchy can be used to support the utility of describing denotes biological, psychological, and situational mechanisms
personality according to its components and control mechanisms. and processes (covering personality components such as energetic
As I indicated before, in this form of hierarchy subsystems of a sys- capacities and abilities); Level L represents structures (addressed
tem are organized into levels. A subsystem is simultaneously a to personality components such as traits, types, patterns); and
whole and a part of another whole, and this other whole exists Level L+1 indicates behaviors and actions (related to personality
at the next higher level of organization (Simon, 1973). Hence the components such as styles, strategies, tendencies). Fig. 1 illustrates
temporal and spatial scales are inseparable from the consideration the three levels of personality, indicated by red arrows.
of a nested hierarchical system (cf. Allen & Starr, 1982; Lavelle I suggest ‘‘three’’ levels not only because (as discussed previ-
et al., 2008). ously) this guarantees a system’s stability and broadly grasps cen-
Guided by higher-level boundary conditions, the elements at tral personality constructs, but also because the identification of
one level emerge as a consequence of the interactions and relation- these three levels seems to be relevant to account for their ‘‘avail-
ships among elements at the next lower level. These emergent ability’’ in empirical analysis of intraindividual and interindividual
properties are the fundamental properties of hierarchically orga- differences. For example, one possibility is to combine measures of
nized systems—functions that are not seen at the lower level. These multiple response systems (e.g., cortical at Level L 1; self-report at
level hierarchies are characterized by same-level causation and Level L; behavioral at Level L+1) as indicators of a particular differ-
bidirectional, cross-level causation: upward causation, which ential construct. However, obviously we cannot interpret them as
may be seen as bottom-up; and downward causation, which may the functionally equivalent indicators of this construct.
be seen as top-down (cf. King, 1997; von Bertalanffy, 1950, An analysis of mutual relationships among personality con-
1975). Some useful properties of the higher level may emerge nat- structs captured by these levels can speak to criteria (running
urally from lower-level behaviors, but not all of them; higher-level alone, in parallel, or divergent) by which upper levels reside above
properties are also subject to selection pressures on heritable var- lower levels (cf. Allen & Hoekstra, 1984; King, 1997; Lavelle et al.,
iation and the elaboration of complex functional adaptations. Thus, 2008). Here, actually, there are three reasons (canons) why the
when postulating multiple levels of organization, I am not positing upper levels of the personality system reside above the lower
that the behaviors of all higher layers emerge automatically from levels:
the lowest layer.
In order not to lose complexity and stability, a system must be  They provide constraints. For example, a Level L+1 positive
explicitly represented by at least three levels. Thus, when using a refocusing—an adaptive strategy of cognitive emotion regula-
compositional hierarchy, the system is usually described by a tion (cf. Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spinhoven, 2002) that developed
three-level triadic structure: a focal Level L, the next lower Level in a neurotic individual across his or her life course owing to
L 1, and the next higher Level L+1. Dynamics of the focal Level L social learning processes— potentially may limit the dynamics
may be explained by being dependent on the interactions at the or expression of a Level L neuroticism, representing here a
next lower level (cf. O’Neill et al., 1986). The highest variability structure of personality (see yellow, left-side arrows in Fig. 1).
might be observed at the lower levels, because the higher levels  They are populated by entities with greater integrity and
of the system consist of the most integrated entities, while the higher bond strength. For, example, Level L 1 physiological
lower ones do not. It is typical for higher levels to have slow inter- mechanisms of attentional control (see Mangun, 2012, for a
nal dynamics and weak interactions among their entities, whereas review) seem to be less integrated entities than a Level L anx-
higher- or medium-frequency dynamics and strong or moderate iety trait, whose dynamics reflects integrated broad range of
interactions are observed among particular elements on the lower different cognitive and somatic mechanisms (cf. Heller &
levels of systems. On the other hand, the levels are relatively dis- Nitschke, 1998).
tinct and nearly decomposable. This three-level organization war-  They contain and are formed by lower levels. For example,
rants stability because with it in place (a third level always Level L+1 nonadaptive strategies of cognitive emotion regula-
anchoring relations between the other two) the focal level cannot tion (cf. Garnefski et al., 2002) have their roots in a Level L
be reduced either upward or downward by assimilation into a depressiveness or trait anxiety (cf. Marszał-Wiśniewska &
neighboring level; the system is implicitly synchronic (moments Fajkowska, 2010).
of different scale are nested within each other). It is worth noting
that a hierarchy might be used to explain how different phenom- In addition, there are limits coming from lower levels L 1 and L
ena manage to be as stable as they are (cf. Feibleman, 1954; (see yellow, right-side arrows in Fig. 1); for example, Level L 1
Odum & Odum, 2000; Weiss, 1971). biological mechanisms of low capacities of stimulation processing3
This general description of a compositional hierarchy might be allow for a limited number of temperament traits that may occur at
applied to a particular phenomenon like personality. Describing the next higher Level L. In other words, for instance, we can expect
personality within a compositional hierarchy means that it assim- high (rather than low) neuroticism at Level L, which then potentially
ilates the crucial features of this type of hierarchy. In doing so, the
assumption that personality is a multilayer entity occurs naturally. 3
Capacities of stimulation processing are determined by biological mechanisms of
Undoubtedly, the idea of arranging personality into levels and ana- temperament, associated with sensitivity and reactivity to stimulation, which in turn
lyzing the connections between these levels is not a new one (cf. may predict individual differences in capacity for tolerating internally or environ-
Eysenck, 1981, 1998; Murray & Collaborators, 1938), although it mentally determined stimulus overloads. Individuals with low stimulation-process-
seems that the existing theories fail to provide a comprehensive ing capacities have stimulation-augmenting biological mechanisms related to high
reactivity and sensitivity to stimulation, and low tolerance to intensive stimulation;
model of personality implementing a wide range of central explan- while individuals with high stimulation-processing capacities have stimulation-
atory personality constructs (e.g., capacities, traits, styles) and clar- reducing biological mechanisms related to low reactivity and sensitivity to stimu-
ifying relations among them. These constructs appear to be very lation, and high tolerance to intensive stimulation (cf. Strelau, 2008).

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
4 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

Fig. 1. A hypothetical hierarchy organization of the personality system. Adapted from Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A Perspective on Anxiety and Depression by
Fajkowska, 2013, p. 19. Copyright 2013 by Eliot Werner Publications, Clinton Corners, NY. Reprinted with permission.

may compose—at the same level—only two structures of tempera- restores the personality system variable to the desired value (e.g.,
ment, melancholic or choleric ones (cf. Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). antidepressant medication influencing neural and cognitive
The subsequent attributes of a hierarchically organized person- responses to negative stimulation).
ality system are organizing mechanisms and emergent properties. The regulative mechanisms can be identified at any level of a
Thus unidirectional, bottom-up, upward causation relates to inte- three-level personality system and they may be activated ade-
grative processes and provides stability (see pink, lower arrow in quately to the demands of situations. For example, physiological
Fig. 1). Integration refers to the processes of accumulating ele- regulatory mechanisms (Level L 1) may be activated when the
ments of a system into one whole system (emergent property). organism’s internal environment deviates from a pre-set stability
Thus the integration—from which originates intrahorizontal or in response to changes in external conditions (cf. Mayer, 2011);
intervertical upper-level emergent properties—is based on three temperament regulatory mechanisms (Level L) can be triggered
kinds of repeated and regular processes: two processes (interac- when the discrepancy between the desired state of stimulation/
tions and intercorrelations) signify that dynamics at the same arousal and actual dose of stimulation/arousal produces a range
levels directly exchange energy/information, while the third of dysfunctional behaviors (cf. Eliasz & Klonowicz, 2001); or behav-
process—transaction—informs us that dynamics at different levels ioral regulatory mechanisms (Level L+1) may be generated when
indirectly exchange energy/information. (It is worth noting that disparities between the expression of a type of behavior and its
within this model interactions and intercorrelations are viewed baseline instigation (a force underlying behavior; product of inter-
as relations between personality components, which can be oper- action between forces of environment and organism) occur (cf.
ationalized and exposed to empirical studies, while transactional Timberlake, 1984).
relations can be only subjected to theoretical analysis.) Moreover, I claim that there is a transactional—rather than a
The other type of causation—which is downward, bidirectional, direct—correspondence among levels—for example, between levels
and based on top-down feedbacks—is responsible for the strong of overt actions and traits, traditionally understood as indicators of
interconnection between the levels and relates to regulative mech- them (see green unequal symbols in Fig. 1). And with regard to the
anisms (see pink, upper arrow in Fig. 1). Regulation is mandated by logic of scalar hierarchy theory, the highest stability is expected at
mechanisms for maintaining the existing state of things, by pre- the top of the personality system L+1 (behaviors and actions; see
serving factors on which the system depends, within the appropri- red ellipse in Fig. 1), compared with its lower levels L (structures)
ate ranges of variability (cf. Tomaszewski, 1967). Thus personality and L 1 (mechanisms and processes). This suggests that behav-
regulative processes (downward causation) allow for a particular ioral tendencies (Level L+1) represent more integrated entities
amount of variability without losing stability allied to homeostasis than traits (Level L), which means they are more stable than traits.
or adaptation to genetic and environmental changes. On a more Behaviors are the most integrated entities within the personal-
elaborated level, personality regulation processes are addressed ity system because they result from the dynamic interplay among
to the personality system variable that is a focus of regulation different entities located at the same level (e.g., interaction
(e.g., depressed mood); the optimal value of the personality system between neuroticism and extraversion, Level L) or different levels
variable (e.g., nondepressed mood); a mechanism that signals (e.g., transactional relations between temperament type, Level L
when the personality system variable deviates from the optimal and style of coping with stress, Level L+1). It suggests that
value (e.g., cognitive biases toward negative information and away behaviors are determined/formed by the lower-level entities. On
from positive information); and a modification mechanism that the other hand, external environmental constraints influence

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx 5

dynamics of behaviors and their situational characteristics. Thus explore integration (cf. Lavelle et al., 2008). Later in this paper I will
different (e.g., affective, cognitive) mechanisms, personality struc- describe a basic element of personality architecture—the System of
tures, and environmental elements reciprocally linked constitute Regulation and Integration Stimulation—according to a subsump-
enduring behaviors. This approach to behavior implies that many tive hierarchy.
elements—not only traits—contribute to the consistency/inconsis-
tency of behaviors. Therefore it is not surprising that traits fail to
2.4. Personality reflects either coherent or incoherent structure
provide an adequate explanatory foundation for consistency/
inconsistency of behaviors across situations and over time.
Considering the fourth proposition, I maintain that personality
Yet, in general, intraindividual stability is far more characteris-
develops into an intraindividual, integrative system whose struc-
tic of one’s behaviors and actions (Level L+1) than one’s structure
ture might be coherent or incoherent. From a meta-theroretical
of personality (Level L) or the biological, psychological, or situa-
perspective, three defining features of personality coherence are
tional mechanisms and processes that underlie this structure
proposed: (a) personality coherence/incoherence emerges as a
(Level L 1). Naturally, this issue is addressed in the particular lev-
high-ordered property (Level L), is relatively stable, with an orga-
els of personality rather than the whole three-level system of per-
nized structure formed by a specific set of internal mechanisms
sonality, which inherently tends toward stability.
(Level L 1), and expresses itself in overt responses and behaviors
And finally, I propose to analyze limited, intraindividual stabil-
(Level L+1); (b) personality represents functional coherent or inco-
ity vertically, across levels in a particular three-level personality
herent integration between different personality qualities; and (c)
system (see blue, vertical arrow in Fig. 1). In other words, under-
highly integrated personality coherence or incoherence reflects
standing intraindividual stability is only possible by studying the
functional correspondence or conflict (respectively) between com-
whole personality system. This holistic approach seems to be ade-
posing complex personality qualities. The question remains of how
quate since all personality levels and components are intercon-
the coherent or incoherent personality structure relate to func-
nected, and intraindividual stability emerges from these
tional and dysfunctional complex systems. Regarding the two
interconnections. Moreover, it has theoretical consequences for
points labeled (b) and (c) above, personality coherence indicates
recognizing personality coherence and incoherence as an emergent
clearly individuated functions of its subsystems, while personality
property (see below, proposition 2.4). By contrast, individual dif-
incoherence reflects not clearly discriminated functions of its sub-
ferences can be studied horizontally (involving the same level of
systems. In consequence, it can be predicted that coherence should
hierarchy), between particular levels of personality across different
be more adaptive than incoherence. However, this was not the case
individuals (see blue, horizontal arrow in Fig. 1). Here the fragmen-
in my studies (cf. the section of this paper reporting the selected
tation of the whole personality system into its specified compo-
summarized findings from my studies on personality coherence
nents situated at the specific level appears as more valid than
and incoherence and attentional processing).
the holistic approach. This is because the subject of analysis is dif-
Moving from the meta-theoretical framework of personality to
ferent—that is, an explanation how individuals differ (for example)
the related theory of personality, the question arises about the cru-
in their styles of coping, personality traits, or attentional selection.
cial subsystem of the personality system that allows individuals
Following this description of the three-level organization, it
their general adaptation. My answer to this question is that a hier-
might be expected that personality tends to maintain homeostasis
archically organized control system—the System of Regulation and
and adaptation—as well as a general continuity of structure and
Integration Stimulation—is the fundamental element of personality
function—to permit changes within permissible limits. In other
architecture that operates within the three levels of personality
words, these three levels are not reducible to each other (e.g.,
structure and functions as both an integrative and regulative
behavior tendencies to personality structures or psychological
system.
mechanisms) and the cumulated results of dynamics at the lower
level are unchangeable at the higher level.
3. The System of Regulation and Integration Stimulation: a basic
2.3. Personality develops and changes structure in personality architecture

Regarding the third proposition, personality changes over time On a more elaborated level, the System of Regulation and Inte-
and across situations are explained by recruiting a subsumptive gration Stimulation is seen as a hierarchically organized control
(specification) hierarchy of a system, informing us how its parts system over organism activity, activation, and arousal. Here activa-
are ‘‘subsumed’’ or classified. By following the model of a specifica- tion is seen as a tonic increment in neural activity connected with
tion hierarchy, we can understand the direction in which the par- motor readiness, motivational system, and increased left-hemi-
ticular level of personality or the component of this level have sphere activity, whereas arousal denotes a phasic increment in
developed through time. In a subsumptive hierarchy, informa- neural activity associated with perceptual receptivity, emotional
tional relations between levels are open ended, transitive, direct, system, and increased right-hemisphere activity (cf. Pribram &
and linked to the energy exchange—in contrast to a scalar hierar- McGuiness, 1975; Sanders, 1983, 1998). In other words, activation
chy organization, where informational relations are generally indi- refers to relatively slow changes in base-level arousal (e.g., the
rect and transactional. There are functionally just two levels at daily cycle of sleep and wakefulness represent changes in activa-
work anywhere in the hierarchy (e.g., the lower-level specific bio- tion). Stimulants (such as caffeine) or depressants (such as alcohol)
logical mechanisms may give rise to a set of higher level classes, also produce notable changes in activation that may last several
like a few specific temperament traits) and new levels may diverge hours (cf. Thayer, 1996). But arousal is stimulus related and more
from anywhere in the hierarchy, potentially giving rise to collec- short lived; for example, when a door is unexpectedly slammed
tions of coordinate classes (cf. King, 1997). The two-level organiza- shut by the wind, we experience a systemic increase in arousal
tion is unstable, allowing new levels to emerge at the top of the due to the release of epinephrine and norepinephrine. Depending
hierarchy, because without the anchoring provided by a third level, on the stimulus, changes in phasic arousal may last from circa a
it can logically be reduced to a single level. A subsumptive hierar- dozen seconds to several minutes (cf. Sanders, 1983, 1998).
chy constitutes levels as nested subclasses. Each level represents I realize that this position on the relation between arousal and
different integration. Higher levels transitively integrate dynamics activation differs from the Eysenckian view (1967, 1981) and
and phenomena at lower levels and two levels are sufficient to attracts criticism. However, I incorporate this description into my

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
6 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

theoretical approach because it provides a fuller understanding of ‘‘coordinators’’ but also as the ‘‘providers’’ of the energetic
the selection of subclasses (specially attention and temperament) resources required by different types of individual activities
composing the SRIS system, and a more valid explanation for reflected in reactions, mechanisms, or behaviors. A relevant
how these subclasses participate in realizing the SRIS’s main assumption of the model is that active or controlled processes or
functions. activities are more energy- and resource-dependent than passive
Thus I hypothesize that from the perspective of a subsumptive or automatic ones. Additionally, the differentiation in the stimula-
organization, the SRIS consists of the subclasses with (a) the physi- tive value of situation or environment (e.g., high versus low
ological mechanisms of temperament traits and attentional pro- demands) affects the level of energy expenditure. Internal or exter-
cesses and (b) other internal stimulation-related elements (cf. nal stimulation alert an organism and activate the appropriate sub-
Fig. 2). Why is it that temperament and attention are so important system(s)—for example, affective, motivational, or motor.
here? This is because none of the other personality traits or cognitive Providing that the response of the activated system involves the
processes—only temperament traits and attentional processes—are energetic supply of resources, direct (via enhancing) or indirect
so closely connected with arousal and activation concepts (cf. (via facilitation) modulation of this response by variations of a
Pribram & McGuiness, 1975; Sanders, 1998; Strelau, 2008). range of temperament traits or attentional processes related to
Among different types of information, the SRIS receives feedback arousal and effort may be expected (Fajkowska & Eysenck, 2008).
concerning the physiological state of organisms and adequacy or On the other hand, the control over purposeful behaviors mainly
effectiveness of individual activity and performance. I believe that deals with other subsystems of the SRIS, which are personality
the SRIS controls reactionary and purposeful behaviors. Reactionary components (e.g., traits) with integrative potentials.
behaviors are connected with more automatic, linearlized control As mentioned above, the SRIS comprises two more subclasses
over one’s need for stimulation (general tendency to seeking or (see Fig. 2), namely other internal stimulation-related elements—
avoiding stimulation), receptivity, and actions, whereas purposeful that is, personality characteristics (e.g., anxiety or depression),
behaviors are linked to nonautomatic control over mental input. cognitive and affective mechanisms (e.g., cognitive sensitivity to
Thus I assume that predominantly temperament traits and atten- negative stimulation), and self-regulative processes (e.g., prolonged
tional processes as subsystems of the SRIS control reactionary worrisome thoughts, rumination)—and also external stimulation-
behaviors and are aimed at modulating the activity and arousal of related elements like environment with its subsystems (e.g.,
organism. These regulators operate as devices, which have the func- sudden unexpected event in a workplace or prolonged marital
tion of managing or maintaining a designated characteristic or distress). However, since these elements of the SRIS are not the
range of values in a system. However, the major function of a hier- main focus of my theory for now, I would like to leave their
archically organized functional system—the System of Regulation description in the present form.
and Integration Stimulation—is to attain and maintain (a) the opti- It is worth noting that the SRIS architecture demonstrates that
mal level of activation and arousal to achieve the optimal level of the processes and structures cannot be dissociated in explaining
functioning of the entire system and (b) intraindividual coherence the functioning of personality as it is postulated by some other
and behavioral integrity. As is shown in Fig. 2, it may be reached approaches (e.g., the social-cognitive theories of personality;
by its two basic elements: the attentional system, which regulates Cervone, 1999). For example, a theoretical analysis of the three-level
the receptivity to signals (reception) and readiness to respond personality composition or subclasses of the SRIS leads us to con-
(action) and relates to emotional and motivational systems; and clude that processes and structures (traits) do not represent antag-
temperament traits (whose physiological mechanisms determine onistic concepts, but rather exist in harmony and are deeply
one’s need for stimulation/arousal) located on the processes (L 1) interrelated constructs. Processes correspond to the dynamic
and structures (traits; L) levels, respectively (cf. Fig. 1). changes of personality (its subsystems—e.g., the SRIS) and contrib-
Temperament traits and attentional processes as functionally ute to the structures’ formation, and at the same time are subjected
essential subclasses of the SRIS are based on the arousal and acti- to the structures (see levels L 1 and L; Fig. 1): the stronger the struc-
vation concepts, which implies that they primarily refer to the ture, the less flexible the process, and vice versa (cf. Smith, 1999).
energetic and effort mechanisms (cf. Sanders, 1998). In other As I said before, these internal elements of the SRIS, based on
words, they may be seen not only as the critical energetic the arousal and activation processes (considered as regulating
and integrating processes), are suggested to have a substantially
important role in forming personality coherence/incoherence.
However, I claim that personality coherence/incoherence is built
System of Regulation and Integration Stimulation upon functional consistency/inconsistency over stimulation pro-
cessing4 between temperament traits and other personality traits
related to stimulation processing. This raises the questions of how
traits are understood within this approach and how traits attend
to realize the basic functions of the SRIS.

External Internal 3.1. Personality traits within the SRIS


environmental e.g., other personality traits, Temperament Attention
subsystems cognitive mechanisms,
self-regulatory processes
I consider the position represented by a majority of personality
researchers (e.g., Allport, 1937; Buss & Plomin, 1984; Costa &
McCrae, 2003; Diamond, 1957; Eysenck, 1998; Strelau, 2008), that
traits indicate the generalized behavioral tendencies or disposi-
tions, as flawed in several respects. Traits and behaviors are located
Other stimulation- Physiological
related elements mechanisms on different levels of the personality system (see Fig. 1), which

4
Fig. 2. Subclasses of the System of Regulation and Integration Stimulation. Adapted I define stimulation processing as a transformation of arousal and activation,
from Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A Perspective on Anxiety and Depression which arises as an effect of flowing (e.g., sensor, emotional, cognitive) stimulus/
by Fajkowska (2013, p. 43). Copyright 2013 by Eliot Werner Publications, Clinton stimuli. This transformation is based on the physiological mechanisms of tempera-
Corners, NY. Reprinted with permission. ment and attention.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx 7

implies that transactional relations connect them and that there is endurance. For example, ER is connected with the efficiency of pro-
no direct correspondence between them. Thus the behavioral con- cessing emotional material; high ER reflects deficits in stimulation
sistency over time and across situations is not mostly ‘‘guaranteed’’ regulation and the increasing reactivity to emotional stimuli,
by traits, but is a product of transactional relations of many which is coupled with attentional biases toward negative stimuli
elements—for example, different components of personality and worsened performance (e.g., Fajkowska & Krejtz, 2007;
(including traits), situational/environmental conditions, and Fajkowska, Krejtz, & Krejtz, 2009).
self-regulative processes. In addition, personality traits are less I assume that traits with reactive and regulative dominants rep-
integrated personality components (Level L) than behaviors (Level resent traits associated with high or moderate dominance of bio-
L+1; see Fig. 1). Although I reject the position that traits imply logical determinacy and are connected to motivational, affective,
the generalized behavioral dispositions, I disagree with social- and cognitive systems (see Derryberry & Rothbart, 1988, 1997;
cognitive theorists that trait constructs do not provide an adequate Rothbart, 1989a, 1989b; Rothbart & Ahadi, 1994). Traits with
explanatory foundation for a science of personality (Cervone & self-regulative dominant, rather, are seen as being influenced by
Shoda, 1999). The conceptualization of trait that I express here environmental determinacy and are attached to more complex
shows that processes and structures (traits) can be reconcilable personality mechanisms. This group of traits indicates individual
and that both are important for describing and explaining person- ability to intentionally control stimulation; they signify effortful
ality coherence and incoherence. control and strategic reception and stimulation processing. They
Accordingly, I understand trait as a hierarchically organized sys- relate to the more complex personality, cognitive, executive, or
tem. In my opinion, a trait (Level L) represents a complex inner affective mechanisms (cf. Lonigan, Vasey, Phillips, & Hazen,
mechanism (Level L 1) that indirectly determines the coherence 2004). For example, trait-like attentional control is one’s ability
of behaviors (Level L+1)—that is, the relative inconsistency of to focus perceptual attention, switch attention between tasks,
behaviors across situations and relative consistency of behaviors and flexibly control thought (see Derryberry, 2002). Supported by
over time (this statement is very close to the trait conceptualiza- the anterior system, effortful attentional control is part of execu-
tion proposed by Eliasz (2004) in his Transactional Model of Tem- tive attention; is viewed as involved in the awareness of one’s
perament; see also Eliasz, 1981; Eliasz & Klonowicz, 2001). A trait planned behaviors and subjective feelings of voluntary control of
denotes underlying, recurrent mechanisms that form a stable pat- thoughts and feelings; and is believed to come into play when
tern and account for the stability of individual characteristics, and resolving conflicts (e.g., discrepant, ambiguous information), cor-
might be described as processes with a slow rate of change that can recting errors, and planning new actions (see Eisenberg, Smith,
be substituted for structure. This understanding of trait includes Sadovsky, & Spinrad, 2004; Fajkowska & Derryberry, 2010;
the ongoing habituation (automatization) responsible for binding Rothbart, 1989a, 1989b; Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994).
underlying mechanisms and processes in increasingly fixed paths; However, it is worth mentioning that the functional complexity
however, such relatively fixed patterns of process have their his- of traits affects their controlling functions, which leads to the fact
tory and may be receptive to change (Smith, 1999). that different controlling functions might coexist in one trait. For
Traits (structures) have different degrees of permanence—for example, in neuroticism, a functionally and structurally complex
example, they are less stable in childhood than in adult life; they trait (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985), one might identify reactive and
may gradually change under specific, persistent internal (e.g., regulative aspects—although the reactive aspect seems to be the
physical illness) or external (e.g., pollution) factors. I believe that dominant one.
their changes depend on their saturation with biological or envi- It should be added that the trait theory presented above, includ-
ronmental components (viz. traits associated with dominance of ing three dominant controlling functions in stimulation processing
biological determinacy are more stable than traits associated with (reactive, regulative, and self-regulative), could be also adapted to
dominance of environmental determinacy; cf. Eliasz, 1981, 2004); personality types. Nonetheless, types are structurally higher-order
their functional complexity (viz. how many functional components systems than traits and embody a group of traits (or types are
might be distinguished in them); and the specific dominating func- ‘‘constellations of traits’’; cf. Eysenck, 1998). Still, the predictive
tion that they play (viz. how they typically control stimulation). validity of the person-centered approaches (categorical, based on
Hence the aforementioned functional consistency among tem- typologies) versus the variable-centered approaches (dimensional,
perament traits and other traits of personality, building personality based on traits) is widely debated (e.g., Asendorpf & Denissen,
coherence/incoherence, is analyzed here in light of the complexity 2006; Chapman & Goldberg, 2011; Sava & Popa, 2011). However,
of traits and the dominant function (reactive, regulative, or self- one good reason for using the type approach is that types are more
regulative; cf. Derryberry & Reed, 2002; Derryberry & Rothbart, integrated entities than traits and fluctuate significantly less across
1988, 1997) that they play in controlling stimulation. time (e.g., temperament types).
Traits with reactive dominant (e.g., anxiety [Spielberger, 1983] Obviously, this conceptualization of traits (also types) remains
or neuroticism [Eysenck, 1998]) relate to the reception of flowing with the question about the trait measurement. My suggestion is
stimulation; they denote the readiness to activity (reaction, behav- to use self-report questionnaires to provide the ‘‘average profile’’
ior), sensitivity, or vigilance (e.g., sensory) to stimuli and relate to of traits reflecting the biological, ‘‘context-free’’ component of each
energy expenditure (in a particular time range). For instance, anx- trait, which seems to be less susceptible to situational influences
ious individuals are hypervigilant to threatening material (e.g., and indirectly contributes to the explanation of one’s stability of
Fajkowska & Krejtz, 2007) or social evaluation (e.g., Eysenck, behavior (see also Eliasz, 1981). In addition, if personality trait is
2006). Traits with regulative dominant denote individual differ- explained by reference to a hierarchical, complex system, it should
ences in energy expenditure, in a particular range of time, to direct be analyzed on multiple levels. Correspondingly, it seems to be
and monitor the flowing stimulation adequately to the organism’s informative to utilize the experimental procedures for the sake of
capacities for processing stimulation. One example might be emo- a construction and development of self-report instruments. Pre-
tional reactivity (ER), a temperament trait formulated within the cisely, the experimental procedures would be useful to determine
Regulative Theory of Temperament (RTT; Strelau, 2008). Despite the underlying mechanisms of traits (Level L 1) and functional
the fact that the term ‘‘reactivity’’ is used in the name of this trait, aspects of traits (Level L+1), and the results obtained should be
its dominant function seems to be regulative. This trait informs us incorporated into self-report instruments. Practically, it suggests
about the tendency to react intensively to emotion-generating that self-report instruments cannot reflect not only the proposi-
stimuli, expressed in high emotional sensitivity and low emotional tional (hypothetical, theoretical) meaning of the differential trait,

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
8 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

but also the substantial (implicational, actual) meaning of the trait traits or types have different adaptive functions, but that all are
derived from real experience. Rather, it is observed no much effort based on the same biological mechanisms responsible for
put by researchers to integrate these two meanings of the differen- regulating the level of arousal/stimulation (e.g., Strelau, 2008).
tial traits what reveals itself in many of existing self-reports instru- And individual differences in the reactivity of these mechanisms
ments (see Robinson, Shaver, & Wrightsman, 1991, for a review). affect the tendency toward chronically elevated or suppressed
However, one can find adequate examples of measures of person- level of arousal (arousability; cf. Gray, 1964) and high or low need
ality where these two meanings of the trait are successfully uni- for stimulation. Based on the concepts of aforementioned augmen-
fied—for example, temperament instruments such as the Eysenck tation/reduction of stimulation (cf. Footnote 3), it might be stated
Personality Questionnaire–Revised (EPQ-R; cf. Eysenck & Eysenck, that individuals who have temperament traits typically associated
1994), Pavlovian Temperament Survey (cf. Strelau, Angleitner, & with a high level of arousability have stimulation-augmenting
Newberry, 1999), or Attentional Control Scale (cf. Derryberry & mechanisms. Stimulation (sensory, motor, cognitive, or affective)
Reed, 2002; Fajkowska & Derryberry, 2010). of a given intensity leads to a higher level of arousal than
It is important to add that the dimensional conception of types that implied by intensity of stimulation. Individuals who have
(cf. Eysenck, 1970; Wundt, 1886) validates employing the dimen- temperament traits typically associated with a low level of arous-
sional scales of traits for assessing types (cf. temperament types ability present stimulation-reducing mechanisms; stimulation of
[e.g., Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985] or stress coping styles [e.g., the same intensity leads to a lower level of arousal than that
Fajkowska & Eysenck, 2008]), and that all these assumptions are implied by intensity of stimulation (Strelau, 2008).
illustrated below as an example of how anxiety subtypes are formed. Thus the particular configuration of temperament traits is
This brief presentation of the concept of trait (and type) allows involved in the process of maintaining the optimal level of stimu-
me to propose a precise definition of intraindividual coherence/ lation and effective stimulation regulation. However, the regula-
incoherence. tion of stimulation might be also ineffective (Eliasz, 1992;
Zawadzki & Strelau, 1997). In other words, depending on the con-
3.2. The SRIS and personality coherence/incoherence figuration of traits, stimulation regulation may have different lev-
els of effectiveness. Effective stimulation regulation reflects the
Generally, I consider personality coherence as the consistency fitness between the amount of the flowing stimulation and individ-
in traits’ controlling functions over stimulation (functional simplic- ual stimulation-processing capacities (SPC), which affects the for-
ity) and personality incoherence as the inconsistency in traits’ con- mulation of harmonious types of temperaments. Hence both
trolling functions over stimulation (functional complexity). At this types of individuals—with high stimulation-processing capacities
point it is useful to remind readers that Level L temperament traits and with low stimulation- processing capacities—might be distin-
(e.g., emotional reactivity, strength of excitation, extraversion) or guished within harmonious structures. However, different ways
temperament types (e.g., melancholic, sanguine) are assigned a of maintaining effective stimulation regulation and the optimal
particularly critical coordinating function between one’s need for level of stimulation may be identified in high-SPC (Sanguine Type)
stimulation (general tendency to seeking or avoiding stimula- and low-SPC (Melancholic Type) individuals—that is, seeking or
tion)—which is relatively stable and achieved in the learning pro- avoiding stimulation, respectively (cf. Zawadzki & Strelau, 1997).
cesses—and other stimulation-related elements of the SRIS (e.g., Ineffective stimulation regulation is connected with the quan-
other personality or environmental features). Thus, on a more elab- tity of stimulation, being beyond the optimal level of stimulation.
orated level, I claim that intraindividual personality coherence/ Relations among temperament traits change adequately to the pri-
incoherence is built upon functional consistency/inconsistency mary functions of these traits, which results in disharmonious
over stimulation processing between temperament types and types of temperament. For example, a high (nonoptimal) level of
other personality traits/types related to stimulation processing. stimulation in individuals with low SPC (Choleric Type) may be
This means that temperament types are linked to other personality associated with a tendency toward reducing arousal, but without
attributes, which results in the emergence of a new quality—intra- a tendency toward avoiding stimulation; a low (nonoptimal) level
individual coherence or incoherence. of stimulation in individuals with high SPC (Phlegmatic Type) may
In the literature on temperament, we can find some attempts to be associated with a tendency toward reducing arousal, coupled
provide definitions of intraindividual personality coherence or with a tendency toward avoiding stimulation (cf. Zawadzki &
incoherence. Yet the definition most suitable to my theoretical Strelau, 1997).
approach postulates that intraindividual coherence between tem- There are several arousal-related theories of temperament (e.g.,
perament capacities and other personality traits means intraindi- Eysenck, 1994; Pavlov, 1928; Strelau, 2008). They propose the
vidual consistency between temperament traits associated with structures of temperament, which resemble the four tempera-
one’s need for stimulation, and other individual differences related ments according to the Hippocrates–Galen typology, and within
to a self-providing dose of stimulation adequate to one’s need for these theories one can identify the harmonious Sanguine Type
stimulation determined by physiological mechanisms of tempera- and harmonious Melancholic Type, and disharmonious Phlegmatic
ment (cf. Eliasz, 1985, 1995, 2001). Analogously, intraindividual Type and disharmonious Choleric Type5 (cf. Fajkowska, 2013).
incoherence between temperament capacities and other personal-
ity traits means intraindividual inconsistency between tempera- 5
From the Pavlovian perspective, harmonious types of temperament—sanguine
ment traits associated with one’s need for stimulation, and other and melancholic—are built on the consistency between levels of strength of excitation
individual differences related to a self-providing dose of stimula- (SE) and of mobility (MO), while disharmonious types of temperament—phlegmatic
tion not adequate to one’s need for stimulation determined by and choleric—emerge from the inconsistency between levels of SE and MO (cf. Strelau
et al., 1999). Within the Eysenckian model, a negative relation between extraversion
physiological mechanisms of temperament. However, I propose
and neuroticism indicates the harmonious types of temperament (sannguine and
to reformulate this understanding of personality coherence and melancholic) and a positive relation between extraversion and neuroticism implies
incoherence by introducing into this definition the aspect of rela- the disharmonious types of temperament (phlegmatic and choleric; cf., Eysenck,
tions in dominant controlling functions over stimulation among 1998). The RTT posits that the fitness between activity (AC) and individual
temperament types and other personality traits/types. stimulation-processing capacities [SPC; relation between level of endurance (EN)
and level of emotional reactivity (ER)] reflects harmonious types of temperaments,
Accordingly, I claim that different structures of temperament while lack of fitness between AC and individual SPC indicates disharmonious types of
represent different controlling functions over stimulation. Why is temperament (sanguine and melancholic and choleric and phlegmatic, respectively;
that? I agree with some researchers who assume that temperament cf. Zawadzki & Strelau, 1997).

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx 9

Based on the appropriate empirical evidence associated with specific set of internal mechanisms and it expresses itself in overt
the arousal-related theories of temperament—that is, Pavlovian, responses and behaviors. Functional correspondence or conflict
Eysenckian, and Strelauvian (e.g., Eysenck, 1967, 1970, 1994; between different personality qualities (e.g., temperament types
Fajkowska, Zagórska, Strelau, & Jaśkowski, 2012; Marszał- and other personality types) reflects personality coherence and
Wiśniewska & Fajkowska-Stanik, 2005; Pavlov, 1928; Strelau, incoherence, respectively. These two definitions point to three
2008; Yermolayeva-Tomina, 1964; Zawadzki, 1992; Zawadzki & important differences. First, from the perspective of social-cogni-
Strelau, 1997)—I assume that the harmonious melancholic and dis- tive theories personality coherence is procesual, while from per-
harmonious choleric types are more reactive than regulative types spective of my approach personality coherence is structural
of temperament, while the harmonious sanguine and disharmoni- (however, it is based on the specific underlying mechanisms and
ous phlegmatic types seem to be more regulative than reactive processes). Second, in the view of social-cognitive theories person-
types of temperament. Each of these three arousal-related theories ality coherence refers to both consistency and variability in overt
of temperament indicates different areas of an organism’s activity behaviors across time and situations, whereas in my model it sig-
engaged in maintaining the optimal level of arousal (stimulation). nifies within-person functional consistency or inconsistency
Thus sustaining the optimal level of arousal depends on (a) Pav- among personality structural elements (which has an indirect
lov’s nervous system control; (b) Eysenck’s content (emotional impact on the quality and dynamics of actions). And third, social-
and social) characteristics of activity; and (c) Strelau’s formal cognitive theories address the issue of the individual’s subjective
(energetic and temporal) characteristic of activity. sense of personal coherence, a question not yet covered in my
Therefore the dominant controlling function over stimulation in approach.
the harmonious or disharmonious temperament type is a founda-
tion for assessing personality coherence and incoherence. Theoret- 4. How to build the coherent and incoherent structures of
ically, there are four possibilities. For the purpose of explaining personality
these four possibilities, I propose to introduce an additional stimu-
lation-related personality trait. I refer to need for achievement, Subsequently, I would like to demonstrate how I am building
understood here as an environmentally determined need for a coherent/incoherent structures of personality when types of anxi-
self-providing high dose stimulation (cf. Eliasz, 1981) and, in con- ety are considered as the other personality components related to
sequence considered as a trait with self-regulative dominant over stimulation processing (also marking the elements of the SRIS).
controlling stimulation. However, before this demonstration I propose to describe particu-
lar types of anxiety. In accordance with the logic of compositional
 Coherent Type of personality based on the harmonious type of hierarchy, and inspired by earlier neuropsychological models of
temperament. For example, high stimulation-processing capac- emotion (e.g., Heller, 1993a; Heller & Nitschke, 1998; Heller,
ities, coupled with maintaining the optimal stimulation level by Nitschke, Etienne, & Miller, 1997), I elaborated three subtypes of
seeking stimulation (it resembles the regulative Sanguine Type anxiety. Then I reviewed the relevant studies and found the typical
in three arousal-related theories of temperament; cf. Footnote patterns of attentional stimulation processing (since the system of
5), may correspond with a (self-regulative) high need for attention is a crucial element of the SRIS) in all types of anxiety,
achievement (cf. Eliasz, 1981). which allowed me to suggest the dominant controlling function
 Incoherent Type of personality based on the harmonious type of over stimulation in a particular type of mood.
temperament. For example, low stimulation-processing capaci-
ties, coupled with maintaining the optimal stimulation level by 4.1. Subtypes of anxiety within the SRIS
avoiding stimulation (it resembles the reactive Melancholic
Type in three arousal-related theories of temperament; cf. Foot- Anxiety as a dimensional emotional phenomenon (cf. Heller,
note 5), may conflict with one’s (self-regulative) high need for 1990; Robinson & Compton, 2006) contributes to stimulation pro-
achievement (cf. Eliasz, 1981). cessing in that it relates to arousal, activation, and activity in differ-
 Coherent Type of personality based on the disharmonious type ent neurobiological and physiological systems. I posit that if the
of temperament. For example, one’s (self-regulative) high need biological mechanisms of arousal and activation bind tempera-
for achievement (cf. Eliasz, 1981) may be consistent with his or ment traits, anxiety, and attentional processes, a strong interrela-
her low (nonoptimal) level of stimulation and high stimulation- tion among them would be expected in response to (emotional)
processing capacities (it resembles the regulative Phlegmatic stimulation.
Type in three arousal-related theories of temperament; cf. Foot- I assume that the repetitive interactions among cognitive mech-
note 5). anisms (e.g., connected with attentional and working memory sys-
 Incoherent Type of personality based on the disharmonious tems) and the repetitive interactions among somatic mechanisms
type of temperament. For example, one’s (self-regulative) high (related to affective and motivational systems) lead to more inte-
need for achievement (cf. Eliasz, 1981) may be inconsistent grated cognitive and somatic components, from which in turn
with his or her high (nonoptimal) level of stimulation and low emerge two essential components composing anxiety types:
stimulation-processing capacities (it resembles the reactive somatic-related arousal and cognitive-related apprehension. In
Choleric Type in three arousal-related theories of temperament; other words, these two interlevel emergent properties originate
cf. Footnote 5). from bidirectional interactions between the cognitive and somatic
components of Level L 1 (see Fig. 3). This explains the fact that by
At this juncture it might be useful to distinguish my concept of interacting with each other, different levels of arousal and appre-
personality coherence from that within social-cognitive theories. hension build different types of anxiety at the higher-level L (see
In previous ones personality coherence represents within-person Fig. 3). As Fig. 3 demonstrates, there are three subtypes of anxiety
integrated organization based on interrelated personality pro- that to some extent capitalize on prior neuropsychological models
cesses and mechanisms; it can be identified in overt psychological of emotion (cf. Heller, 1993a, 1993b).
responses and embraces continuity in phenomenological experi- Thus, as we know from the literature, two types of anxiety—
ence (Cervone & Shoda, 1999). As specified earlier in this paper, I anxious apprehension and anxious arousal—associated with differ-
consider personality coherence as emergent, relatively stable ent patterns of brain and behavioral activity are distinguished
high-ordered property and its organized structure is formed by a (Heller & Nitschke, 1998). Anxious apprehension is primarily

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
10 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

Mechanisms and Behavioral tendencies in


Structures, types
processes contributing to different types of anxiety
anxiety formation of anxiety

Integrated
cognitive Apprehension < Arousal
mechanisms

Ways of
Patterns of
processing reactions
Emergent Apprehension stimulation:
properties Arousal Emergent and
direct
Apprehension > Arousal properties modulation behaviors in
Integrated moderation stimulation
somatic mediation
processing
mechanisms
related to
affective and Apprehension ≈ Arousal
motivational
systems

Level L-1 Level L1 Level L+1

Fig. 3. The organization of anxiety subtypes according to the three-level compositional hierarchy. Adapted from Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A Perspective on
Anxiety and Depression by Fajkowska (2013, p. 107). Copyright 2013 by Eliot Werner Publications, Clinton Corners, NY. Reprinted with permission.

characterized by worry and verbal rumination, typically about Fox, Russo, & Dutton, 2002; Heim-Dreger, Kohlmann,
future events (Barlow, 1991; Heller et al., 1997), while anxious Eschenbeck, & Burkhardt, 2006; Heller et al., 1997; Hock &
arousal is described by Watson et al. (1995; see also Watson, Krohne, 2004; Lovallo & Gerin, 2003; Mathersul, Williams,
2000) as being distinguished by symptoms of physiological hyper- Hopkinson, & Kemp, 2008; Mathews & MacLeod, 2002;
arousal and somatic tension. Although Heller et al. (1997) state Nitschke, Heller, Palmieri, & Miller, 1999; Sapolsky, 1992;
that the two kinds of anxiety are not mutually exclusive, their Wilson & MacLeod, 2003), it may be concluded that the typical
approach seems to be categorical. They claim that extreme degrees patterns of stimulation processing in the Arousal Type of anxiety
of anxious apprehension (e.g., being worried or fearful about the are associated with (a) attentional vigilance (usually in clinical
future) may prompt experiences of anxious arousal (e.g., somatic anxiety) to threat and attentional avoidance (usually in the non-
symptoms and exaggerated physiological responses to stressful patient group) of threat; (b) elevated autonomic reactivity in the
events). Rather, I maintain that the relation between apprehension presence of threat; and (c) right-hemisphere involvement in pro-
and arousal elements is continuous. cessing threatening stimuli (see Fig. 4). Accordingly, as shown in
Hence, in accord with the theoretical line of reasoning pre- Fig. 4, the typical patterns of stimulation processing in the Appre-
sented above, we can distinguish three types of anxiety presented hension Type of anxiety are associated with (a) reduced atten-
in Fig. 3: when the proportion between degree of apprehension tional control and related impaired effectiveness of stimulation
and degree of arousal (a) is in favor of arousal (the Arousal Type: processing and avoidance of threatening stimuli (in clinical and
apprehension < arousal; Type A < a); (b) is in favor of apprehen- nonclinical groups and trait anxiety); (b) reduction in autonomic
sion (the Apprehension Type: apprehension > arousal; Type reactivity; (c) impairment/inhibition of emotional processing,
A > a); or (c) when there is a balance between apprehension both on an attentional and physiological level; and (d) left-hemi-
and arousal (the Balanced Type: apprehension  arousal; Type sphere involvement in processing stimulation (cf. Baxter et al.,
A  a). Panic attacks, high-stress states, and state anxiety as 1987; Borkovec, Ray, & Stober, 1998; Eysenck, 2006; Fletcher &
defined by self-report, behavioral, or physiological response sys- Henson, 2001; Hoehn-Saric, MacLeod, & Zimmerli, 1989;
tems would be covered by the Arousal Type (cf. Heller & Hofmann et al., 2005; Laguna, Ham, Hope, & Bell, 2004;
Nitschke, 1998). It seems more probable that the Apprehension Nitschke & Heller, 2002; Stober, 1998; Swedo et al., 1989;
Type would be characteristic of obsessive–compulsiveness, Thayer, Friedman, & Borkovec, 1996; Tucker, Antes, Stenslie, &
generalized anxiety states, and trait anxiety as identified by Barnhardt, 1978; Wagner, 1999; Wu et al., 1991).
self-reports of anxious apprehension and worry on various ques- It seems probable that the role of anxiety in direct modulation
tionnaires (cf. Heller & Nitschke, 1998). Theoretically, the of stimulation processing is associated with specific patterns of
Balanced Type might be identified among all the categories of stimulation processing formed by dominant controlling functions.
anxiety mentioned above (e.g., OCD, GAD); however, repressors are Identifying these patterns allows us to establish (according to the
good candidates for this type (cf. Fajkowska, Eysenck, Zagórska, definitions of the dominant controlling functions) adequate domi-
& Jaśkowski, 2011). Yet, for the purposes of this paper, I focus nant controlling function of each subtype: reactive rather than reg-
on the Arousal Type and Apprehension Type in my analysis. ulative in arousal anxiety and regulative rather than reactive in
The dominance of a particular component (arousal or appre- apprehension anxiety (see Fig. 4). Thus two types—reactive Arousal
hension) in a particular type of anxiety specifically determines Type of anxiety and regulative Apprehension Type of anxiety—are
the manner of stimulation processing (direct or indirect), as well included in the following theoretical analysis of personality coher-
as patterns of response to stimulation across different response ence and incoherence.
systems. More precisely, with reference to a review of the litera- Accordingly, as an example I demonstrate the coherent and
ture (e.g., Applehans & Luecken, 2006; Calvo & Eysenck, 2000; incoherent types of personalities as built upon two subtypes of
Compton et al., 2003; Engels et al., 2007; Fisher et al., 2010; anxieties and temperament types in the Eysenckian approach.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx 11

1. Attentional vigilance/avoidance
2. Increased RH activation

Arousal Reactive dominant


Type

Apprehension
Regulative dominant
Type

1. Reduced attentional control


2. Increased LH activation

Arousal Type of anxiety (Apprehension < arousal) – Reactive Type


Apprehension Type of anxiety (Apprehension > arousal) – Regulative Type

Fig. 4. Reactive and regulative controlling functions over stimulation and patterns of attentional stimulation processing in anxiety. RH = right hemisphere; LH = left
hemisphere. Adapted from Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A Perspective on Anxiety and Depression by Fajkowska (2013), pp. 128, 130. Copyright 2013 by Eliot Werner
Publications, Clinton Corners, NY. Reprinted with permission.

4.2. Personality coherence/incoherence built upon four temperament arousal (see Melancholic Type, Table 1). Thus personality structure
types within the Eysenckian approach and anxiety subtypes based on the reactive harmonious melancholic temperament, indi-
cating emotional instability and introversion and (5) reactive high-
Construction of theoretical relations among the four tempera- arousal anxiety or (6) low-apprehension anxiety, reflects a coherent
ment types and anxiety subtypes, supported with empirical data,6 personality structure, but associated with (7) regulative high-appre-
allows for the construction of specific structures of coherent/inco- hension anxiety or (8) lack of arousal anxiety denotes incoherent
herent personalities. To be more precise, it looks for functional cor- types of personality.
respondence—or lack of it—among four temperament types By contrast, a positive relation between extraversion and neu-
(regulative sanguine and phlegmatic, and reactive melancholic and roticism reflects the disharmonious type of temperament and inef-
choleric) and mood subtypes (regulative apprehension anxiety and fective stimulation processing (cf. Strelau, 1998; Zawadzki &
reactive arousal anxiety) within the Eysenckian approach (see Strelau, 1997). For example, the arrangement of low extraversion
Table 1). (avoiding stimulation or arousal in social exchanges) and low neu-
According to Eysenckian theory, the negative relation between roticism (low emotional excitability, low anxiety, and low tension)
extraversion and neuroticism indicates the harmonious type of does not allow for an optimal level of arousal. In other words, low
temperament (cf. Strelau, 1998). For example, there is an agree- extraversion and low neuroticism promote a nonoptimal level of
ment between high extraversion (seeking stimulation or arousal arousal in the Phlegmatic Type (see Table 1). The regulative dishar-
in social exchanges) and low neuroticism (low emotional excitabil- monious phlegmatic temperament, indicating emotional stability
ity, low anxiety, and low tension). In other words, low neuroticism and introversion occurring with lack of (9) arousal and (10) appre-
promotes the approach tendencies toward stimulation expressed hension anxiety, or (11) regulative high-apprehension anxiety,
by high extraversion, which encourages maintaining optimal levels characterizes a coherent structure of personality, but when com-
of stimulation (see Sanguine Type, Table 1). bined with (12) reactive high-arousal anxiety results in an Incoher-
The regulative harmonious sanguine temperament, indicating ent Type of personality.
emotional stability and extraversion boosted by low anxiety,7 sig- Moreover, high extraversion—seeking stimulation or arousal
nifying lack of (1) arousal and (2) apprehension anxiety or (3) regu- potentially in social interactions—and high neuroticism (high emo-
lative high-apprehension anxiety, results in a Coherent Type of tional excitability, high anxiety, and high tension) maintain a non-
personality, but combines with (4) reactive high-arousal anxiety optimal level of stimulation in the Choleric Type (see Table 1). Thus
resulting in an incoherent personality structure. In addition, there the reactive disharmonious choleric temperament, indicating emo-
is also consensus between low extraversion—avoiding stimulation tional instability and extraversion linked to (13) reactive high-
or arousal potentially associated with avoiding social interactions— arousal anxiety or (14) low-apprehension anxiety, indicates a
and high neuroticism (high emotional excitability, high anxiety, coherent personality type, but associated with (15) regulative
and high tension). In sum, high neuroticism promotes the with- high-apprehension anxiety or (16) lack of arousal anxiety charac-
drawal tendencies toward stimulation expressed by low extraver- terizes incoherent personality structures.
sion, which in turn allows for maintaining an optimum level of These theoretical assumptions were necessary to take a step
toward empirical validation of some of theoretical predictions pre-
6
There are empirical data from more detailed investigations, with correlational
sented here. Thus, regarding this goal a very important question
analysis among temperament traits within Eysenckian theory and anxiety, and posed in this context is ‘‘What is the functional (adaptive) signifi-
distributional analysis that demonstrate the distribution of particular anxiety type in cance of the proposed coherent/incoherent structures of personal-
particular temperament type defined within Eysenckian model (Fajkowska, 2013, for ity?’’ The particular functional significance of a given personality
a review).
7
coherence/incoherence structure may reveal itself, for instance,
It emerges from the literature that the sanguine and phlegmatic types, opposite to
the melancholic and choleric types, do not incline etiologically to anxiety (cf.
during cognitive performance—specifically, when attentional stim-
Fajkowska, 2013, for a review). ulation processing is activated. Attentional system is a crucial

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
12 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

Table 1
Coherent and incoherent types of personalities built on four temperament types within the Eysenckian approach and anxiety subtype.a

Coherent versus incoherent types of personalities based on harmonious structures of temperament


(1) CPTSanguine low-arousal anxiety (5) CPTMelancholic high-arousal anxiety
Low-arousal anxiety + high E and low N (regulative type) High-arousal anxiety (reactive type) + low E and high N (reactive type)
(2) CPTSanguine low-apprehension anxiety (6) CPTMelancholic low-apprehension anxiety
Low-apprehension anxiety + high E and low N (regulative type) Low-apprehension anxiety + low E and high N (reactive type)
(3) CPTSanguine high-apprehension anxiety (7) INPTMelancholic high-apprehension anxiety
High-apprehension anxiety (regulative type) + high E and low N (regulative type) High-apprehension anxiety (regulative type) + low E and high N (reactive type)
(4) INPTSanguine high-arousal anxiety (8) INPTMelancholic low-arousal anxiety
High-arousal anxiety (reactive type) + high E and low N (regulative type) Low-arousal anxiety + low E and high N (reactive type)
Coherent versus incoherent types of personalities based on disharmonious structures of temperament
(9) CPTPhlegmatic low-arousal anxiety (13) CPTCholeric high-arousal anxiety
Low-arousal anxiety + low E and low N (regulative type) High-arousal anxiety (reactive type) + high E and high N (reactive type)
(10) CPTPhlegmatic low-apprehension anxiety 14) CPTCholeric low-apprehension anxiety
Low-apprehension anxiety + low E and low N (regulative type) Low-apprehension anxiety + high E and high N (reactive type)
(11) CPTPhlegmatic high-apprehension anxiety (15) INPTCholeric high-apprehension anxiety
High-apprehension anxiety (regulative type) + low E and low N (regulative type) High-apprehension anxiety (regulative type) + high E and high N (reactive type)
(12) INPTPhlegmatic high-arousal anxiety (16) INPTCholeric low-arousal anxiety
High-arousal anxiety (reactive type) + low E and low N (regulative type) Low-arousal anxiety + high E and high N (reactive type)

Note. CPT = Coherent Personality Type; INPT = Incoherent Personality Type; E = extraversion; N = neuroticism.
a
Adapted from Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A Perspective on Anxiety and Depression by Fajkowska (2013, p. 145). Copyright 2013 by Eliot Werner Publications,
Clinton Corners, NY. Reprinted with permission.

element of the SRIS associated with stimulation reception and questionnaires—evaluating extraversion and neuroticism (EPQ-R;
readiness to react. Thus it might be hypothesized that reactive Brzozowski & Drwal, 1995; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1994) and state
and regulative functions of personality types analyzed through (arousal) and trait (apprehension) anxiety (STAI; Spielberger,
‘‘correlational’’ and ‘‘interactive’’ ways account for the variance in 1983; Wrześniewski & Sosnowski, 1996); and second, they were
quality and dynamics of attentional performance in a particular administered the Emotional Go/NoGo Task. Contributors were
structure of personality coherence/incoherence. instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible when
a Go stimulus (e.g., threatening face, Th/Go) appeared and to
withhold their reactions when a NoGo stimulus (e.g., friendly face,
5. Methods for analyzing the functional (adaptive) significance
F/NoGo) was displayed (see Fajkowska et al., 2011).
of coherent/incoherent personality structures
Several behavioral indices of attentional biases to affective faces
were taken into account: attentional vigilance/reduced attentional
I suggest two methods for analyzing the functional significance
vigilance, assigned by (respectively) a high or low speed of process-
of the particular coherent/incoherent type of personality. To eluci-
ing emotional material (RT: reaction time); impaired/enhanced
date these methods, I refer to some sample results from my studies
attentional control, reflected in (respectively) a high or low level
aimed at investigating the adaptive meaning of coherent/incoher-
of false alarms (FA); attentional avoidance, marked by a high level
ent personality structures for quality and dynamics of attentional
of omissions (OM); and finally (in)effectiveness of attentional pro-
stimulation processing. Hence the findings in this section should
cessing, expressed in (in)accuracy (respectively), indicative of the
not be treated as conclusive in the context studied, because they
quality of attentional selectiveness.
are used here to gain a better understanding of the ideas presented
Fig. 6 demonstrates that in the incoherent high-arousal anxious
in this paper. The results demonstrated in the next section lead to
Sanguine Type (INPTSanguine high-arousal anxiety) impaired attentional
the final conclusions from my studies.
control over sadness is observed (high level of FAs to S/NoGo),
while the coherent low-arousal anxious Sanguine Type
5.1. Correlational method (CPTSanguine low-arousal anxiety) presents enhanced attentional control over
sadness (low level of FAs to S/NoGo). In other words, it suggests
First, I propose to explore the correlational relations among that higher arousal-anxiety in sanguine (personality incoherence)
controlling functions in focal traits (particular temperament type goes with higher decrease of attentional control over sadness.
and particular anxiety type), which informs us about the relation
between personality coherence/incoherence and quality of stimu-
5.2. Interactive method
lation processing. Put another way, this correlational method tells
us about improving/worsening the quality of stimulation process-
Second, I propose to analyze the interactive relations among
ing on the basis of comparing (1) the quality of processing identi-
dominant controlling functions of particular type of temperament
fied in coherent/incoherent personality structures with elevated
and dominant controlling functions of particular type of anxiety in
levels of anxiety with (2) the quality of processing identified in par-
coherent/incoherent types of personality. This provides an
ticular coherent/incoherent personality structures with low anxi-
indicator of the dynamics of processes engaged in processing
ety (cf. Fig. 5). This goal may be achieved by means of such
statistical techniques as a multi-multivariable analysis of
regression. the quality of processing the quality of processing
For a better understanding, let us consider the following exam- Comparing identified in to identified in
CPT/INPT with elevated anxiety CPT/INPT with low anxiety
ple. Fig. 6 portrays the selected sample result from my studies on
how coherent/incoherent personality structures relate to quality
of (attentional) stimulation processing, which I conducted to vali-
Fig. 5. Correlational relations among controlling functions in focal types (e.g., four
date the theory of personality presented here. types of temperament in Eysenckian approach and types of anxiety) and quality of
Generally, across two separate sessions participants (N = 168, stimulation processing. CPT = Coherent Personality Type; INPT = Incoherent Per-
83 females, M = 25.15 years, SD = 7.11) first completed two sonality Type.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx 13

Sanguine Type 4.06


(defined in Eysenckian approach)

RTRR
FA_Th/NoGo

4.06 4.06 t=1.72


t= 1.43

t=0.97
Arousal FA_F/NoGo t =1.58
anxiety
t=2.51* 4.06
t=1.44

FA_S/NoGo

*p < .05
INPT Sanguine high-arousal anxiety - impaired attentional control over sadness (high level of FAs to
S/NoGo)
CPT Sanguine low-arousal anxiety- enhanced attentional control over sadness (low level of FAs to
S/NoGo)

Fig. 6. Results of the exploratory multi-multivariable analysis of regression (structural equations estimated by LISREL 8.51) for false alarms to NoGo trials in Sanguine Type
(N = 35). FA = false alarms. Nonsignificant results are written in black. Adapted from Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A Perspective on Anxiety and Depression by
Fajkowska (2013, p. 185). Copyright 2013 by Eliot Werner Publications, Clinton Corners, NY. Reprinted with permission.

stimulation. It is operationalized as contrasting the attentional pro- To clarify this theory of functional interactions, I would once
cesses involved in stimulation processing with patterns of stimula- again like to present the sample results from my study on how
tion processing, which are identified in types of temperament and functional interactions in coherent/incoherent personality struc-
anxiety studied separately from those received for types of temper- tures relate to the dynamics of attentional processes engaged in
ament and anxiety studied in interaction. stimulation processing. One hundred ninety-nine participants
I propose three different categories of functional interactions: (156 females, M = 24.80 years, SD = 6.50) completed the same pro-
additive, synergistic, or antagonistic (see Fig. 7). Matching the cat- cedure as described above. Table 2 illustrates patterns of emotional
egory of interaction with the interaction between temperament stimulation processing in the Choleric Type of temperament and
types and types of anxiety is possible with reference to the role anxiety types, studied separately.8
of these other personality types and temperament in stimulation It appears that the Choleric Type is associated with vigilance to
processing when they are examined independently (not in interac- happiness and sadness, avoidance in processing emotional faces,
tion). This may be estimated with the analysis of variance. effective sadness processing, and ineffective threat and happiness
Thus additive functional interactions in this context means that processing, while the Arousal Type of anxiety influences impaired
the same manner of stimulation regulation in both elements of inhibitory control over threat and the Apprehension Type of anxi-
coherent/incoherent structure (e.g., reactive temperament ety affects impaired attentional control over happiness and sad-
type  reactive anxiety type) produces a pattern of processing typ- ness and goes with attentional ineffectiveness in happiness
ical for both of them. As Fig. 7A demonstrates, this pattern is avoid- processing (see Table 2). The question arises how interactions
ance of stimulation processing. Synergistic functional interactions between specific anxiety type and Choleric Type modify these pat-
denote that both elements of coherent/incoherent personality terns of attentional processing.
structure process information in either the same (e.g., reactive Contrasting findings from Table 2 with data from Table 3, one
temperament type  reactive anxiety type) or different (e.g., reac- can notice that on the basis of antagonistic functional interaction
tive temperament type  regulative anxiety type) ways, which in coherent personality structure (CPT), composed of the (reactive)
results in maintaining a pattern of stimulation processing that is Choleric Type and (reactive) Arousal Type, we obtained ‘‘a new
typical for one of them. According to Fig. 7B, it appears that avoid- product’’: enhanced inhibitory control over negativity. But when
ance of stimulation processing specific for type of anxiety—rather we examine the incoherent personality type (INPT) organized
than vigilance in stimulation processing specific for type of temper- around the (reactive) Choleric Type and (regulative) Apprehension
ament—is sustained. Of course one should expect that the intensity Type, we find enhanced control over threat and effective happiness
of this pattern might be changed and observed in such conditions processing (see Table 3). Analogously, this suggests antagonistic
as the extension of behavioral indices or scope of processing tar- functional interactions for the incoherent choleric personality. It
gets. Finally, antagonistic functional interactions signify that both seems that dynamics of attentional processing based on the antag-
elements of coherent/incoherent personality structure produce onistic interactions Choleric Type  Arousal Type and Choleric
the same (e.g., reactive temperament type  reactive anxiety type) Type  Apprehension Type reveal themselves in enhanced top-
or different (e.g., reactive temperament type  regulative anxiety down attentional processing of emotional stimulation, and the
type) ways of processing stimulation, which results in an entirely reduced range of attentional processes and the range of processed
new pattern of stimulation processing that is not identified in even emotional stimuli.
one of these elements. Consistent with Fig. 7C, ineffective stimula-
tion processing is this new pattern not identified in either focal 8
Two-factor multivariate repeated MANOVAs using facial affect as within-subject
type. factor were performed on accuracy, RT, OM, and FA.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
14 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

[A] Reactive E.g.,


coherent/incoherent personality structures for quality and dynam-
Type of Temperament X Reactive Type of Anxiety ics of attentional stimulation processing. I formulated two scien-
tific questions: (a) How does the level of functional complexity
reflected in coherent/incoherent personality structures relate to
Avoidance of stimulation quality of stimulation processing? And (b) How do functional
processing
interactions in coherent/incoherent personality structures relate
to the dynamics of attentional processes engaged in stimulation
Avoidance of stimulation Avoidance of stimulation processing?
processing processing The studies regarded attentional biases in coherent/incoherent
personality structures built upon temperament types in three the-
Reactive E.g., ories of temperament—Pavlovian, Eysenckian, and Strelauvian—
Type of Temperament Reactive Type of Anxiety and anxiety or depression types. It should be explained that an
additional goal of these studies was to compare these three theo-
ries of temperament. However, I am omitting the results connected
Additive functional interactions: with this issue as peripheral to this paper. Moreover, I am omitting
produce a pattern of processing typical for both focal types. the summary of results on attentional biases in personality coher-
ence and incoherence, built upon temperament types and depres-
[B] Reactive
X E.g., sion types, because I did not introduce depression to the line of
Type of Temperament Reactive Type of Anxiety reasoning presented in this paper.
Reactive X E.g.,
Type of Temperament Regulative Type of Anxiety 6.1. Method

Avoidance of stimulation Temperament was assessed with the Pavlovian Temperament


processing
Survey (Strelau & Zawadzki, 1998), the Eysenck Personality Ques-
tionnaire–Revised (Brzozowski & Drwal, 1995; Eysenck &
Vigilance in stimulation Avoidance of stimulation
processing
Eysenck, 1994), and the Formal Characteristic of Behavior–Tem-
processing
perament Inventory (Zawadzki & Strelau, 1997), respectively. Trait
E.g., (apprehension) anxiety and state (arousal) anxiety were evaluated
Reactive
Type of Temperament
Reactive/Regulative by the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, 1983;
Type of anxiety
Wrześniewski & Sosnowski, 1996).
Two attentional tasks were utilized. Thus the Emotional Faces
Synergistic functional interactions: Attention Test was employed to collect data for the intercorrela-
result in maintaining a pattern of stimulation processing that is typical for one of the focal type. tional analysis within the Pavlovian approach.9 The task was to
detect (by crossing out) as quickly as possible a target face (threat-
ening, friendly, or sad) in a matrix of faces within 2 min. Perfor-
[C] Reactive
X
E.g.,
mance on this task imposed substantial demands on the voluntary
Type of Temperament Reactive Type of Anxiety
attentional control system, and it is believed to activate the temper-
Reactive E.g.,
X ament properties as sensitive to challenging conditions. The data
Type of Temperament Regulative Type of Anxiety
gathered with modified Emotional Go/NoGo task were utilized in
intercorrelational10 and interactional11 analysis within all theoreti-
Ineffective stimulation
processing
cal approaches.
To analyze data collected across all experimental procedures, I
took into account several behavioral indices of attentional biases
Vigilance in stimulation Avoidance of stimulation
processing processing to affective faces, which I have already described above.

Reactive
E.g., 6.2. Results
Reactive/Regulative
Type of Temperament Type of Anxiety
To answer these questions, I utilized intercorrelational analyses
Antagonistic functional interactions: (the exploratory multi-multivariable analysis of regression; struc-
result in an entirely new pattern of stimulation processing that is not identified in even one of the tural equations estimated by LISREL 8.51) and interactional analy-
focal elements. ses (two-factor multivariate repeated MANOVAs using facial affect
as a within-subject factor, performed on accuracy, reaction times,
Fig. 7. Three interactive relations among dominant controlling functions of focal
types (e.g., four types of temperament in the Eysenckian approach and types of number of omissions, and false alarms).
anxiety): [A] additive, [B] synergistic, and [C] antagonistic. With reference to the first question, the accumulation of data
clearly reveals that personality coherence (functional overlapping)
or incoherence (functional distinctness) does not elucidate the
6. Findings from research on attentional processing of quality of stimulation processing in the context studied. Generally,
stimulation in individuals with coherent/incoherent the lower-integrated personality structures (e.g., disharmonious or
personality structures built upon temperament types and types harmonious temperaments) relate to the quality of stimulation
of anxiety
9
N = 254, 142 females, M = 22.14 years, SD = 2.93.
10
As I stated above, the main focus of the empirical validation of N = 168, 83 females, M = 25.15 years, SD = 7.11; N = 164, 104 females,
the theory at this stage of its development is on the adaptive signif- M = 25.15 years, SD = 7.09 for Eysenckian and Strelauvian approaches, respectively.
11
N = 58, 42 females, M = 26.43 years, SD = 7.77; N = 199, 156 females,
icance of coherent/incoherent personality structures. Specifically, I M = 24.80 years, SD = 6.50; N = 84, 54 females, M = 24.36 years, SD = 4.78 for Pavlov-
designed studies aimed at investigating the adaptive meaning of ian, Eysenckian, and Strelauvian approaches, respectively.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx 15

Table 2
Choleric Type of temperament (the Eysenckian approach), Arousal Type of anxiety, and Apprehension Type of anxiety in attentional processing of facial affect.a

Attentional patterns of stimulation processing Personality Types studied separately


Choleric Type of temperament Arousal Type of anxiety Apprehension Type of anxiety
Vigilance (low RT) F&S
Avoidance (high OM) Th&F&S
Impaired attentional control (high FA) Th F&S
Attentional effectiveness (high Accuracy) S
Attentional ineffectiveness (low Accuracy) Th&F F

Note. RT = reaction time; OM = omissions; FA = false alarms; Th = threatening faces; F = friendly faces; S = sad faces.
a
Adapted from Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A Perspective on Anxiety and Depression by Fajkowska (2013, p. 195). Copyright 2013 by Eliot Werner Publications,
Clinton Corners, NY. Reprinted with permission.

Table 3
Attentional biases in coherent/incoherent personality structures built upon Choleric Type of temperament (in the Eysenckian approach) and anxiety types.a

Processes-level of stimulation processing Traits-level of stimulation processing


Attentional patterns of stimulation processing Content (emotional and social) characteristics of activity (Eysenckian approach)
CPTCholeric high-arousal anxiety INPTCholeric high-apprehension anxiety

Enhanced attentional control (low FA) Th&S Th


Antagonistic Antagonistic
Attentional effectiveness (high Accuracy) F
Antagonistic

Note. CPT = Coherent Personality Type; INPT = Incoherent Personality Type; FA = false alarms; Th = threatening faces; F = friendly faces; S = sad faces.
a
Adapted from Personality Coherence and Incoherence: A Perspective on Anxiety and Depression by Małgorzata Fajkowska, 2013, p. 190. Copyright 2013 by Eliot Werner
Publications, Clinton Corners, NY. Reprinted with permission.

processing. One possible explanation is that higher-level inte- structures when both types of anxieties were considered. It is
grated types (e.g., personality coherence/incoherence) are useful worth noting that the synergistic type of interaction emerged from
for predicting the quality of performance of more complex behav- the dominance of typical patterns of stimulation processing for
iors, but at the same time are inadequate for predicting patterns of two types of anxiety over the temperament mode of stimulation
simple reactions to attentional tasks. Also, it seems probable that processing.
these structures might be activated in more complex situations To conclude, (a) with reference to the quality and dynamics of
than relatively simple experimental ones. The conclusion is that attentional stimulation processing no regular or specific patterns
the complex, multilevel traits or personality types should be stud- of results for (respectively) personality coherence and personality
ied through more complex behaviors and more ecologically valid incoherence were shown within or across the temperament mod-
situations. In addition, it is believed that the tasks administered els studied here; (b) as regards the quality of attentional stimula-
were not adequate to cover all controlling functions in focal tion processing, it was demonstrated that it is connected with
types—which is considered the main drawback of these studies. personality structures of lower integration level than personality
However, some interesting findings were identified across all coherence/incoherence (this conclusion is valid for all three mod-
the theories of temperament studied here. First, a quite regular els of temperament); in addition, it was evidenced that personality
pattern for quality of processing and personality incoherence was incoherence does not always mean ineffective stimulation process-
found. For example, incoherent high-arousal phlegmatics and inco- ing; and (c) in relation to the dynamics of attentional stimulation
herent high-apprehension cholerics produce poor and improved processing, it appeared that these dynamics originate from two
processing of affective stimulation, respectively. This suggests that types of interactions—synergistic (responsible for maintaining
incoherence does not always mean poor quality of performance, attentional processes engaged in stimulation processing specific
what is particularly visible in case of cholerics. Second, incoherent for types of mood, not for types of temperament) or antagonistic
cholerics with elevated apprehension anxiety present increased (evoking totally new attentional processes engaged in stimulation
attentional processing. One possible explanation is that reactive processing).
choleric type ben efits from the ‘‘regulative nature’’ of this affective
trait, which acting together produce effective stimulation process-
ing. Third, the generalized deteriorating impact of arousal anxiety 7. Looking to the future
on attentional processing was found, and it was not related to
the type of personality – coherent or incoherent – which this anx- It should be emphasized that the findings reported here should
iety belongs to. be viewed as a modest attempt to empirically validate some of the
Considering the second question, it appeared that regular pat- theoretical assumptions associated with personality coherence/
terns of results suggesting relatively stable correspondence incoherence presented here. I am aware that they need to be rep-
between the type of interaction and personality coherence/inco- licated and that further analysis should be long term and
herence were not provided. In other words, it was found that syn- systematic.
ergistic and antagonistic interactions do not specifically explain Thus I would like to characterize three problems as top priori-
how per sonality coherence or incoherence modify dynamics of ties in the near future research and theorizing—that is, (a) contin-
attentional processes, but rather how elevated arousal or appre- uation of empirical validation of the theory presented; (b)
hension anxiety — as elements of lower-integrated personality neurophysiological perspective within the C-SAP; and (c) applica-
structures—influence these dynamics. Actually, it was found that tive utility of the model.
synergistic and antagonistic interactions related to the dynamics With reference to the first issue, I know that a large part of the
of the top-down attentional processes across most personality model embraces theoretical assumptions and the necessity of

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
16 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

long-term empirical validations and exclusion of the limitations. Acknowledgment


But above all, I would like to formulate methodological principles
allowing for more valid measurement addressed to the evaluation This research was partly supported by Grant 2012/07/E/HS6/
of the dominant functions (reactive, regulative, or self-regulative) 04071 from the National Science Centre, Poland.
that traits play in controlling stimulation. Having these methodo-
logical principles, it seems obligatory to empirically validate the References
postulated controlling functions over stimulation in affective traits
and temperament types. It is also important to study the (in)com- Allen, T. F. H., & Hoekstra, T. W. (1984). Nested and non-nested hierarchies: A
patibility between intraindividual coherence/incoherence and significant distinction for ecological systems. In A. W. Smith (Ed.), Proceedings of
the society for general systems research: I. Systems methodologies and isomorphies
coherence/incoherence in overt behaviors.
(pp. 175–180). Lewiston, NY: Intersystems Publications.
Considering the second problem, the study of affective traits Allen, T. F. H., & Starr, T. B. (1982). Hierarchy: Perspectives for ecological complexity.
within coherent/incoherent personality structures will also Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.
likely benefit from the incorporation of evolving developments
Applehans, B. M., & Luecken, L. J. (2006). Attentional processes, anxiety, and the
in the biological bases of personality and individual differ- regulation of cortisol reactivity. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 19, 81–92.
ences, such as those made available through advances in Asendorpf, J. A., & Denissen, J. J. (2006). Predictive validity of personality types
neurotechniques. versus personality dimensions from early childhood to adulthood: Implications
for the distinction between core and surface traits. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52,
With reference to the third problem, one of the crucial ques- 486–513.
tions facing therapists is understanding why people change and Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
what can be done to enhance this process. One approach is to treat Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspectives on
a specific symptom as complex and related to more complex struc- Psychological Science, 1, 164–180.
tures; this is particularly true in the case of affective traits (e.g., Barlow, D. H. (1991). Disorders of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 58–71.
anxiety). Thus the predictive utility of the higher-level, more inte- Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and nature: A necessary unity. New York: Ballantine.
Baxter, L. R., Phelps, M. E., Mazziotta, J. C., Guze, B. H., Schwartz, J. M., & Selin, C. E.
grated personality constructs in (a) minimizing the negative out- (1987). Local cerebral glucose metabolic rates in obsessive-compulsive
comes of overly intensive nonclinical affective disorders or (b) disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 211–218.
curing clinical affective disorders should be expected. All of this Beishon, R. J., & Peters, G. (1976). Systems behavior (2nd ed.). London: Open
University Press.
points to the need for further studies and theory development in Borkovec, T. D., Ray, W. J., & Stober, J. (1998). Worry: A cognitive phenomenon
these areas. intimately linked to affective, physiological, and interpersonal behavioral
processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 561–576.
Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, D. H. (Eds.). (2008a). The SAGE handbook of
personality theory and assessment: Personality theories and models (Vol. 1).
London: Sage.
Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, D. H. (2008b). Personality theories and
models: An overview. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The
SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment. Personality theories and
models (Vol. 1, pp. 1–29). London: Sage.
8. Limitations of the Complex-System Approach to Personality Brennan, J. F. (2002). History and systems of psychology (6th ed.). Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Brzozowski, P., & Drwal, R. Ł. (1995). Kwestionariusz Osobowości Eysencka. Polska
I hope that the limitations of the C-SAP outlined below will adaptacja EPQ-R. Podre˛cznik. [Eysenck personality questionnaire: Polish adaptation
challenge me to continue developing this model further. In short, of the EPQ-R. manual]. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.
in the theory of personality proposed here, the reader may find Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
answers to the following questions that represent basic issues in Calvo, M. G., & Eysenck, M. W. (2000). Early vigilance and late avoidance of threat
personality psychology. processing: Repressive coping versus low/high anxiety. Cognition and Emotion,
14, 763–787.
Campbell, J. B. (2008). Modern personality theories: What have we gained? What
 What is personality? have we lost? In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The SAGE
 How is personality organized? handbook of personality theory and assessment. Personality theories and models
 How does personality develop and change? (Vol. 1, pp. 191–212). London: Sage.
Caprara, G. V., & Cervone, D. (2000). Personality: Determinants, dynamics, and
 What makes personality stable?
potentials. New York: Cambridge University Press.
 What are personality coherence and incoherence and how Cervone, D. (2004). The architecture of personality. Psychological Review, 111,
are they studied? 183–204.
 What is a trait and why is it useful in an analysis of person- Cervone, D. (2008). Explanatory models of personality: Social-cognitive theories
and the knowledge-and-appraisal model of personality architecture. In G. J.
ality coherence/incoherence? Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality
theory and assessment. Personality theories and models (Vol. 1, pp. 191–212).
However, personality psychology has more enduring questions London: Sage.
Cervone, D. (1999). Bottom-up explanation in personality psychology: The case of
that are not answered in this theory—for example, the nature of cross-situational coherence. In D. Cervone & Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of
the self and free will, and the extent to which conscious or uncon- personality: Social-cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and organization
scious motives explain individual behaviors. Moreover, the C-SAP (pp. 303–341). New York: Guilford Press.
Cervone, D., & Shoda, Y. (1999). Social-cognitive theories and the coherence of
does not focus on clinical issues associated with personality, the personality. In D. Cervone & Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality: Social-
‘‘organismic’’ side of personality, or developmental stages of cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and organization (pp. 3–33). New York:
personality—which one can identify in other integrative theories Guilford Press.
Chapman, B. P., & Goldberg, L. R. (2011). Replicability and 40-year predictive power
of personality. In my book on the C-SAP model (Fajkowska, of childhood ARC types. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101,
2013), I selected three of them —that is, social cognitive theories 593–606.
(e.g., Cervone & Shoda, 1999), personal narratives and life story Compton, R. J., Banich, M. T., Mohanty, A., Milham, M. P., Miller, G. A., Scalf, P. E.,
et al. (2003). Paying attention to emotion: An fMRI investigation of cognitive
approach (e.g., McAdams, 2006), and a system framework for per-
and emotional Stroop tasks. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3,
sonality (e.g., Mayer, 2007)—which differ among themselves but 81–96.
inspired me to shape my own vision of an integrative theory of Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (2003). Personality in adulthood. New York: Guilford Press.
personality. However, since there is not enough space to present Derryberry, D. (2002). Attention and voluntary self-control. Self and Identity, 1,
105–111.
these comparisons, I kindly refer the reader to the book for this Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their
information. regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225–236.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx 17

Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Affect, arousal, and attention as Fletcher, P. C., & Henson, R. N. (2001). Frontal lobes and human memory: Insights
components of temperament. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, from functional imaging. Brain, 124, 849–881.
958–966. Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional biases for threat: Evidence for
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16,
organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633–652. 355–379.
Diamond, S. (1957). Personality and temperament. New York: Harper & Brothers. Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2002). CERQ: Manual for the use of the
Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Sadovsky, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Effortful control: cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Leiderdorp, The Netherlands: DATEC.
Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. Gray, J. A. (1964). Strengh of the nervous system and levels of arousal: A
In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, reinterpretation. In J. A. Gray (Ed.), Pavlov’s typology: Recent theoretical and
theory, and applications (pp. 259–282). New York: Guilford Press. experimental developments from the laboratory of B.M. Teplov (pp. 289–364).
Eliasz, A. (1981). Temperament a system regulacji stymulacji [Temperament and Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.
system of regulation of stimulation]. Warsaw: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Heim-Dreger, U., Kohlmann, C.-W., Eschenbeck, H., & Burkhardt, U. (2006).
Naukowe. Attentional biases for theratening faces in children: Vigilant and avoidant
Eliasz, A. (1995). Podmiotowe i środowiskowe czynniki utrudniaja˛ce efektywna˛ processes. Emotion, 6, 320–325.
regulacje˛ stymulacji [Personal and environmental factors disturbing the Heller, W. (1993a). Neuropsychological mechanisms of individual differences in
effective regulation of stimulation]. Czasopismo Psychologiczne, 1, 129–141. emotion, personality, and arousal. Neuropsychology, 7, 476–489.
Eliasz, A. (2001). Temperament, Type A, and motives: A time sampling study. In H. Heller, W. (1993b). Gender differences in depression: Perspectives from
Brandstätter & A. Eliasz (Eds.), Persons, situations, and emotions: An ecological neuropsychology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 29, 129–143.
approach (pp. 55–73). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Heller, W., & Nitschke, J. B. (1998). The puzzle of regional brain activity in
Eliasz, A. (2004). Transakcyjny Model Temperamentu. Analiza właściwości depression and anxiety: The importance of subtypes and comorbidity. Cognition
temperamentu z perspektywy nomotetycznego oraz idiograficznego badania and Emotion, 12, 421–447.
osobowości [Transactional Model of Temperament: The analysis of Heller, W., Nitschke, J. B., Etienne, M. A., & Miller, G. A. (1997). Patterns of regional
temperament from the nomothetic and idiographic perspective of brain activity differentiate types of anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106,
personality]. In Z. Chlewiński & A. Se˛kowski (Eds.), Psychologia w perspektywie 376–385.
XXI wieku (pp. 49–95). Lublin, Poland: Towarzystwo Naukowe KUL. Heller, W. (1990). The neuropsychology of emotion: Developmental patterns and
Eliasz, A. (1992). Rola interakcji temperamentu i środowiska w rozwoju człowieka implications for psychopathology. In N. L. Stein, B. Leventhal, & T. Trabasso
[Role of temperament and environment interaction in human development]. In (Eds.), Psychological and biological approaches to emotion (pp. 167–211).
A. Eliasz & M. Marszał-Wiśniewska (Eds.), Temperament a rozwój młodziez_ y Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 11–27). Warsaw: Instytut Psychologii, Polska Akademia Nauk. Hock, M., & Krohne, H. W. (2004). Coping with threat and memory for ambiguous
Eliasz, A., & Klonowicz, T. (2001). Top-down and bottom-up approaches to information: Testing the repressive discontinuity hypothesis. Emotion, 4, 65–86.
personality and their application to temperament. In A. Eliasz & A. Angleitner Hoehn-Saric, R., MacLeod, D., & Zimmerli, W. D. (1989). Somatic manifestations in
(Eds.), Advances in research on temperament (pp. 14–42). Lengerich, Germany: women with generalized anxiety disorder: Psychophysiological responses to
Pabst Science Publishers. psychological stress. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 1113–1119.
Eliasz, A. (1985). Transactional model of temperament. In J. Strelau (Ed.), Hofmann, S. G., Moscovitch, D. A., Litz, B. T., Kim, H.-J., Davis, L. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A.
Temperamental bases of behavior: Warsaw studies on individual differences (2005). The worried mind: Autonomic and prefrontal activation during
(pp. 41–78). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. worrying. Emotion, 5, 464–475.
Engels, A. S., Heller, W., Mohanty, A., Herrington, J. D., Banich, M. T., Webb, A. G., King, A. W. (1997). Hierarchy theory: A guide to system structure for wildlife
et al. (2007). Specificity of regional brain activity in anxiety types during biologists. In J. A. Bissonette (Ed.), Wildlife and landscape ecology: Effects of
emotion processing. Psychophysiology, 44, 352–363. pattern and scale (pp. 185–212). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Laguna, L. B., Ham, L. S., Hope, D. A., & Bell, C. (2004). Chronic worry as avoidance of
Thomas. arousal. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 269–281.
Eysenck, H. J. (1970). The structure of human personality (3rd ed.). London: Methuen. Lavelle, C., Berry, H., Beslon, G., Ginelli, F., Giavitto, J.-L., Kapoula, Z., et al. (2008).
Eysenck, H. J. (1994). Cancer, personality, and stress: Prediction and prevention. From molecules to organisms: Towards multiscale integrated models of
Advanced Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16, 167–215. biological systems. Theoretical Biology Insights, 1, 13–22.
Eysenck, H. J. (1998). Dimensions of personality. London: Transaction (Original work Lemke, J. L. (2000). Opening up closure: Semiotics across scales. In J. L. R. Chandler &
published in 1947). G. van de Vijver (Eds.), Closure: Emergent organizations and their dynamics
Eysenck, H. J. (1981). General features of the model. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model (pp. 100–111). New York: New York Academy of Sciences (Annals of the New
for personality (pp. 1–37). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 901).
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A Lonigan, C. J., Vasey, M. W., Phillips, B. M., & Hazen, R. A. (2004). Temperament,
natural science approach. New York: Plenum Press. anxiety, and the processing of threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Clinical Child
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1994). Manual of the Eysenck personality and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 8–20.
questionnaire. San Diego, CA: EdITS Publishers. Lovallo, W. R., & Gerin, W. (2003). Psychophysiological reactivity: Mechanisms and
Eysenck, M. W. (2006). Teorie le˛ku i wykonanie zadań _poznawczych [Theories of pathways to cardiovascular disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 36–45.
anxiety and cognitive performance]. In M. Fajkowska, M. Marszał-Wiśniewska, Mangun, G. R. (2012). The neuroscience of attention: Attentional control and selection.
& G. Se˛dek (Eds.), Podpatrywanie myśli i uczuć. Zaburzenia i optymalizacja New York: Oxford University Press.
procesów emocjona lnych i poznawczych. Nowe kierunki badań (pp. 45–62). Marszał-Wiśniewska, M., & Fajkowska, M. (2010). Właściwości psychometryczne
Gdańsk, Poland: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne. Kwestionariusza Poznawczej Regulacji Emocji (Cognitive Emotion Regulation
Fajkowska, M. (2013). Personality coherence and incoherence: A perspective on anxiety Questionnaire; CERQ). Wyniki badań na polskiej próbie [Psychometic properties
and depressed mood. Clinton Corners, NY: Eliot Werner Publications. of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ): Results of the
Fajkowska, M., & Derryberry, D. (2010). Psychometric properties of Attentional studies on the Polish sample]. Studia Psychologiczne, 49(1), 19–39.
Control Scale: The preliminary study on a Polish sample. Polish Psychological Marszał-Wiśniewska, M., & Fajkowska-Stanik, M. (2005). Temperament, tendencje
Bulletin, 41, 1–7. depresyjne i detekcja sygnałów emocjonalnych [Temperament, depressed
Fajkowska, M., & Eysenck, M. W. (2008). Personality and cognitive performance. mood, and detection of emotional signals]. Czasopismo Psychologiczne, 11,
Polish Psychological Bulletin, 39(4), 178–191. 119–130.
Fajkowska, M., Eysenck, M. W., Zagórska, A., & Jaśkowski, P. (2011). ERP responses to Mathersul, D., Williams, L. M., Hopkinson, P. J., & Kemp, A. H. (2008). Investigating
facial affect in low-anxious, high-anxious, repressors and defensive high- models of affect: Relationships among EEG alpha, asymmetry, depression, and
anxious individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 961–976. anxiety. Emotion, 8, 560–572.
Fajkowska, M., & Krejtz, I. (2007). Właściwości indywidualne i ‘‘efekt twarzy Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on
tłumie’’ [Individual differences and the ‘‘face-in-the-crowd effect’’]. Przegla˛d anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 331–354.
Psychologiczny, 50, 401–433. Mayer, J. D. (1998). A systems framework for the field of personality psychology.
Fajkowska, M., Krejtz, I., & Krejtz, K. (2009). Le˛k, temperament i przetwarzanie Psychological Inquiry, 9, 118–144.
bodźców emocjonalnych na podstawie ruchów gałek ocznych i procesów Mayer, J. D. (2006). A new vision of personality and of personality theory. American
pamie˛ci [Anxiety, temperament, and emotional stimuli processing based on eye Psychologist, 61, 331–332.
movement and memory]. In M. Fajkowska & B. Szymura (Eds.), Le˛k. Geneza– Mayer, J. D. (2007). Personality: A systems approach. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Mechanizmy–Funkcje (pp. 311–342). Warsaw: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Scholar. Mayer, E. A. (2011). Gut feelings: The emerging biology of gut-brain
Fajkowska, A., Zagórska, A., Strelau, J., & Jaśkowski, P. (2012). ERP responses to facial communication. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 453–466.
affect and temperament types in Eysenckian and Strelauvian theories. Journal of McAdams, D. P. (2006). The person: A new introduction to personality psychology (4th
Individual Differences, 33, 212–226. ed.). New York: Wiley.
Feibleman, J. K. (1954). Theory of integrative levels. British Journal for the Philosophy Murray, H. A.Collaborators. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford
of Science, 5, 59–66. University Press.
Fisher, J. E., Sass, S. M., Heller, W., Silton, R. L., Edgar, C., Stewart, J. L., et al. (2010). Nitschke, J. B., & Heller, W. (2002). The neuropsychology of anxiety disorders:
Time course of processing emotional stimuli as a function of perceived Affect, cognition, and neural circuitry. In H. D’haenen, J. A. den Boer, H.
emotional intelligence, anxiety, and depression. Emotion, 10, 486–497. Westenberg, & P. Willner (Eds.), Textbook of biological psychiatry (pp. 975–988).
Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003
18 M. Fajkowska / Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2014) xxx–xxx

Nitschke, J. B., Heller, W., Palmieri, P. A., & Miller, G. A. (1999). Contrasting patterns Strelau, J., Angleitner, A., & Newberry, B. H. (1999). Pavlovian Temperament Survey
of brain activity in anxious apprehension and anxious arousal. Psychophysiology, (PTS): An international handbook. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.
36, 628–637. Strelau, J., & Zawadzki, B. (1998). Kwestionariusz Temperamentu PTS. Podre˛cznik [PTS
Odum, H. T. (2007). Environment, power, and society for the twenty-first century: The Temperament Questionnaire: Manual]. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów
hierarchy of energy. New York: Columbia University Press. Psychologicznych PTP.
Odum, H. T., & Odum, E. C. (2000). Modeling for all scales: An introduction to system Swedo, S. E., Schapiro, M. B., Grady, C. L., Cheslow, D. L., Leonard, H. L., Kumar, A.,
simulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. et al. (1989). Cerebral glucose metabolismin childhood-onset obsessive–
O’Neill, R. V., DeAngelis, D. L., Waide, J. B., & Allen, T. F. H. (1986). A hierarchical compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 518–523.
concept of ecosystems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Thayer, R. E. (1996). The origin of everyday mood. New York: Oxford University Press.
Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflexes: Twenty-five years of objective Thayer, J., Friedman, B. H., & Borkovec, T. D. (1996). Autonomic characteristics of
study of the higher nervous activity (behavior) of animals (W. H. Gantt, Trans.). generalized anxiety disorder and worry. Biological Psychiatry, 39, 255–266.
New York: Liveright Publishing. Thomas, J. C., Segal, D. L., & Hersen, M. (Eds.). (2006). Comprehensive handbook of
Pribram, K. H., & McGuiness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation, and effort in the control personality and psychopathology: Personality and everyday functioning (Vol. 1).
of attention. Psychological Review, 82, 116–149. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Robinson, M. D., & Compton, R. J. (2006). The automaticity of affective reactions: Timberlake, W. (1984). Behavior regulation and learned performance: Some
Stimulus valence, arousal, and lateral spatial attention. Social Cognition, 24, misapprehensions and disagreements. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
469–495. Behavior, 41, 355–375.
Robinson, J. P., Shaver, J. P., & Wrightsman, L. S. (Eds.). (1991). Measures of Tomaszewski, T. (1967). Aktywność człowieka [Human activity]. In T. Maruszewski,
personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. J. Reykowski, & T. Tomaszewski (Eds.), Psychologia jako nauka o człowieku
Rothbart, M. K., & Ahadi, S. A. (1994). Temperament and the development of (pp. 219–278). Warsaw: Ksia˛zka _ i Wiedza.
personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 55–66. Tschacher, W., & Dauwalder, J. P. (Eds.). (2003). The dynamical systems approach to
Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994). A psychobiological approach cognition: Concepts and empirical paradigms based on self-organization,
to the development of temperament. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), embodiment, and coordination dynamics. Singapore: World Scientific.
Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biology and behavior Tucker, D. M., Antes, J. R., Stenslie, C. E., & Barnhardt, T. M. (1978). Anxiety and
(pp. 83–116). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. lateral cerebral function. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 380–383.
Rothbart, M. K. (1989a). Temperament in childhood: A framework. In G. Vallacher, R. R., Michaels, J. L., Wiese, S., Strawinska, U., & Nowak, A. (2013). Mental
Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood dynamism and its constraints. Finding patterns in the stream of consciousness.
(pp. 59–73). Chichester, UK: Wiley. In D. Cervone, M. Fajkowska, M. W. Eysenck, & T. Maruszewski (Eds.), Personality
Rothbart, M. K. (1989b). Temperament and development. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. dynamics: Meaning construction, the social world, and the embodied mind
Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 187–247). (pp. 159–181). Clinton Corners, NY: Eliot Werner Publications.
Chichester, UK: Wiley. von Bertalanffy, L. (1950). An outline of general systems therapy. British Journal of
Salthe, S. N. (1985). Evolving hierarchical systems: Their structure and representation. the Philosophy of Science, 1, 134–165.
New York: Columbia University Press. von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). Organismic psychology and systems theory. Worcester, MA:
Salthe, S. N. (1991). Two forms of hierarchy theory in Western discourses. Clark University Press.
International Journal of General Systems, 18, 251–264. von Bertalanffy, L. (1975). In E. Taschdjian (Ed.), Perspectives on general systems
Salthe, S. N. (1993). Development and evolution: Complexity and change in biology. theory: Scientific-philosophical studies. New York: George Braziller.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Wagner, A. D. (1999). Working memory contributions to human learning and
Salthe, S. N. (1988). Notes toward a formal history of the levels concept. In G. remembering. Neuron, 22, 19–22.
Greenberg & E. Tobach (Eds.), Evolution of social behavior and integrative levels. Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Watson, D., Weber, K., Assenmeimer, J. S., Clark, L. A., Strauss, M. E., & McCormick, R.
Sanders, A. F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and human performance. Acta A. (1995). Testing a tripartite model: I. Evaluating the convergent and
Psychologica, 53, 61–97. discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. Journal of
Sanders, A. F. (1998). Elements of human performance: Reaction processes and Abnormal Psychology, 104, 3–14.
attention in human skill. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Weiss, P. A. (1971). The basic concept of hierarchic systems. In P. A. Weiss (Ed.),
Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). Neuroendocrinology of the stress response. In J. B. Becker, S. Hierarchically organized systems in theory and practice. New York: Hafner
M. Breedlove, & D. Crews (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (pp. 287–324). Publishing.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Wilson, E., & MacLeod, C. (2003). Contrasting two accounts of anxiety-linked
Sava, F. A., & Popa, R. I. (2011). Personality types based on the Big Five model: A attentional bias: Selective attention to varying levels of stimulus threat
cluster analysis over the Romanian population. Cognition, Brain, Behavior: An intensity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 212–218.
Interdisciplinary Journal, 3, 359–384. Wrześniewski, K., & Sosnowski, T. (1996). Inwentarz Stanu i Cechy Le˛ku (ISCL). Polska
Shoda, Y. (1999). Behavioral expressions of a personality system: Generation and adaptacja STAI. Podre˛cznik. The State–Trait Anxiety Inventory: Polish adaptation of
perception of behavioral signatures. In D. Cervone & Y. Shoda (Eds.), The the STAI. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.
coherence of personality: Social-cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and Wu, J. C., Buchsbaum, M. S., Hershey, T. G., Hazlett, E., Sicotte, N., & Johnson, J. C.
organization (pp. 155–181). New York: Guilford Press. (1991). PET in generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 29,
Simon, H. A. (1973). The organization of complex systems. In H. H. Pattee (Ed.), 1181–1199.
Hierarchy theory: The challenge of complex systems (pp. 1–27). New York: George Wundt, W. (1886). Elements du psychologie physiologique. 2ieme tome [Elements of
Braziller. physiological psychology] (Vol. 2). Paris: Ancienne Libraire Germer Bailliere et Cie
Singleton, W. T. (1989). The mind at work: Psychological ergonomics. Cambridge, UK: (Translated from German by E. Rouvier).
Cambridge University Press. Yermolayeva-Tomina, L. B. (1964). Concentration of attention and strength of the
Smith, G. J. W. (1999). Trait and process in personality theory: Defined within two nervous system. In J. A. Gray (Ed.), Pavlov’s typology: Recent theoretical and
contemporary research traditions. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, experimental developments from the laboratory of B.M. Teplov (pp. 446–464).
269–276. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for adults: Sampler set manual, Zawadzki, B. (1992). Cechy temperamentu w uje˛ciu Regulacyjnej Teorii Temperamentu
test, scoring key. Redwood City, CA: Mind Garden. i ich pomiar metoda˛ _kwestionariusza [Temperament traits according to the
Stober, J. (1998). Worry, problem elaboration, and suppression of imagery: The role Regulative Theory of Temperament and their measurement by means of a
of concreteness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 751–756. questionnaire]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Warsaw University, Warsaw.
Strelau, J. (1998). Temperament: A psychological perspective. New York: Plenum Zawadzki, B., & Strelau, J. (1997). Formalna Charakterystyka Zachowania–
Press. Kwestionariusz Temperamentu (FCZ-KT). Podre˛cznik. Formal Characteristics of
Strelau, J. (2008). Temperament as a regulator of behavior: After fifty years of research. Behavior-Temperament Inventory (FCB-TI): Manual. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów
Clinton Corners, NY: Eliot Werner Publications. Psychologicznych PTP.

Please cite this article in press as: Fajkowska, M. The Complex-System Approach to Personality: Main theoretical assumptions. Journal of Research in Per-
sonality (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.09.003

You might also like