Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

124.

Attitude Measurements 1251

124. Attitude Measurements / Messung von Einstellungen

1. Introduction main. Stevens (1983), for example, con-


2. ‘Societal treatment’ approach ducted a field study of attitudes towards
3. Direct and indirect approaches Classical Arabic, Tunisian Arabic, and
4. Presentation techniques French amongst highly literate Tunisian bi-
5. Attitudinal response gathering linguals in a variety of settings. His data was
6. Current issues and directions gathered from a mix of participant observa-
7. Literature (selected) tion and casual conversations, through
friendships and invitations to social gather-
1. Introduction ings and family festivities, as well as from in-
formal and formal interviews with lawyers
Social evaluations of language are rightly re- and other professionals and their clients. He
garded as a central concern in socioling- then considered these fieldwork findings in
uistics. Such evaluations can reveal the dy- relation to a content analysis of official pol-
namic identificational and relational forces icies and other publicly available sources in
at work within sociolinguistic communities. Tunisia. – Other societal treatment studies
These include prejudice held against or in have included the analysis of governmental
favour of regional or social varieties of lan- and educational language policy documents
guage. And they include allegiances and af- and their view on the use of various lan-
filiative feelings towards one’s own or other guages in schools in Catalonia (Cots/Nuss-
groups’ speech norms and stereotypes of baum 1999), job advertisements in Mon-
speech styles. Accordingly, the emphasis treal, and the occupational demands for
below is on the investigation of attitudes at Anglophone and Francophone bilinguals
the level of social groups, rather than the (Lieberson 1981), the use of pidgins and
level of the individual case in, say, interac- creoles by various character-types in novels
tive sequences of talk. What we know about (Rickford/Traugott 1985), media output,
ideologies of language can be said to derive such as ethnocultural stereotypes projected
mainly from grouped data, and it is arguably through the use of foreign languages in ad-
at this group level that attitude measure- vertisements in Japan (Haarmann 1984;
ment has a key role. The term ‘measure- 1986) and through the use of English in
ment’, however, can seem overly restrictive, magazine advertisements in Switzerland
as if limited to attitude scales. As will be seen (Cheshire/Moser 1994), cartoons, proverbs,
below, the term is used to embrace at least and etiquette books, and what they have to
some degree of qualitative identification and say about women (Kramarae 1982), and his-
interpretation. Following Ryan/Giles/Hew- torical writings about purity in language
stone (1988), language attitude measure- (Aitchison 2001). Milroy’s (1999) work on
ment is organised here under three broad ap- socially dominant ideologies in relation to
proaches – ‘societal treatment’, direct, and language could also fall under this rubric.
indirect – with some merging of the latter Analysing contemporary media sources, she
two. investigates the sociohistorical processes
contributing to the forming of sets of beliefs
2. ‘Societal treatment’ approach about language – beliefs that people tend to
take for granted and which can evoke strong
Arguably, the societal treatment approach ‘gut reactions’ (1999, 176). Issues of stan-
tends to be overlooked in contemporary dis- dardness, correctness, and stigma in relation
cussions of language attitudes research, but to the English language, she argues, are re-
it is undoubtedly an important source of in- lated more to race and ethnicity in the USA ,
sights into the relative status and stereotypi- and more to class in Britain and Ireland. Al-
cal associations of language varieties. It gen- though many ‘societal treatment’ studies are
erally investigates the ‘treatment’ given to largely qualitative in approach, some al-
language varieties and their speakers within so employ formal sampling procedures and
a society. Research in this category typically (at least descriptive) statistics (e.g. Lieber-
involves observational, participant observa- son 1981; Cheshire/Moser 1994). – The rela-
tion, and ethnographic studies, or the con- tively minor mention in the attitudes litera-
tent analysis of sources in the public do- ture of societal treatment studies seems

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
1252 VII. Sociolinguistic Methodology

unlikely to reflect any dearth of such work. the matched guise technique (MGT ), devel-
There is probably considerable attitudinal oped in the late 1950s by Lambert and his
data in many ethnographic studies, for colleagues in Canada (e.g. Lambert/Hodg-
example, which simply do not get incorpor- son/Gardner et al. 1960), and with tech-
ated into ‘mainstream’ reviews. Many atti- niques deriving from it. Typically, respon-
tudes researchers (and especially those in dents hear an audiotaped recording of a
the quantitative social psychological tradi- single speaker reading the same ‘factually
tion) view much of this societal treatment re- neutral’ text a number of times, differing on
search as something of a preliminary for each occasion in one respect only (e.g. ac-
more sociolinguistic and social psychological cent or speech rate). Respondents are told
studies (Ryan/Giles/Hewstone 1988, 1069), or that they will be listening to a number of dif-
perhaps as a source of convergent validity to ferent speakers. After hearing each speaker,
data collected through direct or indirect they pause to fill in attitude rating scales.
methods (Knops/van Hout 1988, 7). Knops/ The technique assumes that speech style sets
van Hout have also argued that some socie- off certain social categorisations that then
tal treatment studies may be appropriate lead to a set of group-related trait infer-
where restrictions of time and space do not ences. Indirect approaches are seen as par-
allow access to informants, or where inform- ticularly useful where it is too intrusive or
ants can only be accessed under highly un- detrimental or not possible to administer di-
natural conditions. The flood of work in dis- rect questions through interviews or ques-
course analysis and text analysis makes the tionnaires (Perloff 1993, 44). Lambert/
very different assumption, of course, that Hodgson/Gardner et al. (1960) saw the
this work can stand independently of these MGT as an effective way to elicit people’s
other approaches. private attitudes. This issue is revisited
below, since it underlies much discussion in
3. Direct and indirect approaches decisions about choice of attitude measure-
ment. – For the most part, the decisive dif-
Direct approach studies are generally more ference between direct and indirect ap-
intrusive than ‘societal treatment’ studies, proaches in language attitudes research
and are characterised by elicitation: asking concerns the stage at which the language sti-
direct questions about language evaluation, muli are presented rather than the instru-
preference etc., usually through question- ments through which respondents record
naires and/or interviews. So it is not so much their attitudes, or the subsequent analysis.
the researcher who infers attitudes from the In most direct approach studies, respon-
observed behaviours, as in societal treament dents are required to conceptualise what is
studies, but the respondents themselves who meant by, say, ‘Australian English’, ‘Cock-
are asked to articulate them. One can of ney’, etc. (terms usually provided by the re-
course argue that answering interview ques- searchers) and this requirement precludes
tions, ticking boxes or circling numbers indirectness. In perceptual dialectological
on scales on questionnaires are all them- and folklinguistic studies, ‘conceptual’ pres-
selves merely behaviours from which the re- entation of this sort is also sometimes used
searcher has to infer attitudes. But they are (e.g. Preston 1999a), but it is perhaps more
arguably one remove from the behaviours likely that respondents are representing
analysed in societal treatment research. Per- their own construals to the researchers,
ceptual dialectological and folk linguistic rather than being given predetermined la-
techniques, considered below, can also be bels. For example, they may draw on a map
viewed as a direct approach. where they perceive main dialect boundaries
Indirect approaches to researching atti- to be, and then provide their own labels (see
tudes involve engaging in more subtle (even later section on response gathering). But
deceptive) techniques than directly asking MGT respondents are presented with repre-
overt questions. Ethical issues concerning sentative audio-recorded samples of the lin-
such deception are normally dealt with guistic variety the researcher wants them to
through subsequent debriefing of inform- evaluate. As far as the display of attitudes is
ants. Although a number of strategies exist concerned, however, the use of semantic dif-
under this heading (Dawes/Smith 1985), the ferential scales, although typical in MGT
indirect approach in language attitudes re- studies, is not exclusive to them. Giles
search is generally seen as synonymous with (1970), for example, using both conceptual

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
124. Attitude Measurements 1253

and vocal stimuli in his study of accented in their study in Egypt of Egyptian, British,
English, employed the same semantic differ- and American varieties of English and Clas-
ential scales and analysis for both sets of sical and colloquial Arabic in Egypt), or
data. For this reason, the different ways of have simply asked different speakers to tell a
presenting language stimuli are now con- story of their own (Garrett/Coupland/Wil-
sidered, and then, secondly, the various liams 1999), or taken excerpts from
ways in which respondents are asked to rec- relatively open interviews (Jørgensen/Quist
ord their attitudes. 2001; Kristiansen 2001; Ladegaard 2001, in
their studies of varieties of Danish). As we
4. Presentation techniques move from matched guise to these more in-
dividualised representative aural presenta-
The debate around presentation modes has tions, the gains in reduced contrivance are
tended to centre on issues of validity. arguably offset by the presence of more of
Through the use of vocal renditions of sev- the very interfering idiosyncratic variables
eral speech varieties by the same person, that the MGT seeks to avoid, thus making it
reading the same ‘factually neutral’ text, hard to favour universally one approach
and keeping constant as far as possible other over the other. Other criticisms have been
features such as speech rate, pitch etc. (e.g. made of the MGT, including: controlling the
where accent has been investigated), the effects of, say, prosodic and paralinguistic
MGT claims to control out all but the ma- features may mean that other characteristics
nipulated variable, so that only this variable that tend to belong to accent varieties (e.g.
remains to explain variable patterns of re- intonation characteristics) are also elimin-
sponse among the respondents. MGT has ated. Again, mimicked accents can contain
thus facilitated the investigation of language considerable inaccuracies (Preston 1996).
at different levels of specificity, from ‘whole’ Some further criticisms also extend to some
languages such as Welsh and English in bi- verbal guise studies: e.g. the repeated mess-
lingual settings (e.g. Bourhis/Giles 1976) to age content of a reading passage may make
specific language features in isolation – ac- speech/language much more salient than it
cent, speech rate, pitch, age-related voice otherwise is outside the experimental en-
qualities, lexical diversity, convergence and vironment (Lee 1971); reading texts aloud is
divergence in language use (see Cargile/ likely to produce a relatively formal speech
Giles/Ryan et al. 1994 for an overview). In style compared to spontaneous speech (see
this way, it has provided a detailed demon- Labov 1972); and the notion of a ‘factually
stration of the role of language code and neutral’ text is undoubtedly questionable
style choice in impression formation, as well (Giles/Coupland/Henwood et al. 1990).
as leading to the cumulative development of Furthermore, we are not always sure how re-
theory (Giles/Coupland 1991). The MGT liably judges have perceived the manipulated
tradition has also established the most regu- variables – e.g. Bradac (1990) asks whether
lar dimensions of judgements across many the manipulated variable ‘non-standard ac-
communities: to use Zahn/Hopper’s (1985) cent’ might be misperceived as ‘bad gram-
terms, superiority, social attractiveness, and mar’? (see Levin/Giles/Garrett 1994 for an
dynamism. Dominant though this approach instance of such an association). Finally, re-
was during the 1970s and 1980s, there have garding vocal presentations generally, (Pre-
nevertheless been many recurring criticisms. ston 1989, 3) argues that in many attitudinal
One is that the speech stimuli are too con- studies of geographical variation we cannot
trived. By comparison, in what is referred to always be sure that the judges identify each
as the ‘verbal guise’ technique, a number of speaker as coming from a separate speech
different speakers read the same text onto area, or as being representative of it, since
tape (e.g. Paltridge/Giles 1984, in their study usually no such data is elicited. – Less
of regional accents of French; Nesdale/Roo- strictly controlled modes of vocal presenta-
ney 1996 in their study of Australian English tion, for example, where different speakers
accents; and Bayard/Weatherall/Gallois et are recorded speaking spontaneously in or-
al. 2001, in their study of British, American, der for the researchers to present a number
Australian, and New Zealand English ac- of accents, arguably allow us to investigate
cents). Other studies have used a number of effects from a wider range of individual
different speakers controlled only by topic speech characteristics of each speaker.
or broad content (e.g. El-Dash/Tucker 1975, Choice of grammar, lexis, and idiom, use of

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
1254 VII. Sociolinguistic Methodology

hesitations, pauses, repetition, laughter, methods, such as survey questionnaires and


topic, self-evaluative meta-talk, etc. might be interviews (Alexander/Becker 1978). They
influencing informants’ evaluations as much also enable a wide range of situation charac-
as accent does. In their study of attitudes to teristics to be manipulated, although the
New Zealand English and Standard Ameri- written format precludes the presentation of
can English, Ray/Zahn (1999) found that, many of the language features typical of MGT
overall, paralinguistic features had a greater studies (e.g. accent). The idealisation involved
impact than accent. Use of several different in constructing vignettes, however, removes
speakers for the presentation of each accent them from the domain of everyday behaviour.
may reduce such effects, but in practice this
can mean lengthening the evaluation task 5. Attitudinal response gathering
and causing fatigue effects. Again we see
how the choice of methods offsets one set of General difficulties in the gathering of re-
problems against inheriting another. – One sponses are well-documented (e.g. Baker 1988;
might argue that a strength of ‘conceptual’ 1992; Oppenheim 1992; Perloff 1993). A few
presentations, therefore, is that they do not of the main ones are included here. Some re-
rely on listeners identifying a voice in the late to the formulation of questions in inter-
way that the researchers intend. Instead, the views and questionnaires, and are primarily
identification is done for the informant relevant to direct approach studies. These in-
(thus making vocal representation anyway clude the use of hypothetical questions, which
unnecessary). But this brings with it the dif- tend to be poor predictors of future behaviour
ficulty of knowing what speech features (if when the phenomenon is actually encountered
any) respondents are associating with the la- (e.g. Breckler 1984), the use of strongly
bels they are given, and where they locate ‘loaded’ questions, which tend to influence
such varieties on their mental maps – e.g. people towards a particular response, and the
where and over how large an area do they use of multiple questions, where a positive
believe ‘Birmingham English’ is spoken? As- answer can refer to more than one component
sociations are simply assumed in most cases of the question, or where there is a double
(although perceptual dialectological ap- negative which would make a negative re-
proaches can address this issue). However, sponse ambiguous. Other difficulties concern
some features of speech and conversation, common tendencies in respondents that re-
which can be manipulated in the MGT, such searchers need to be on their guard for when
as speech rate, pitch, or accommodation, are collecting data. Some may affect both direct
difficult or impossible to present conceptually and indirect approaches. Firstly, social desir-
in any reliable way. – A further form of presen- ability bias. Cook/Sellitz (1964, 39), working
tation, less commonly employed, and perhaps in the US, reported that people are often moti-
bridging direct and indirect approaches ac- vated to give replies that make them appear
cording to how it is implemented, is the vi- ‘well-adjusted, unprejudiced, rational, open-
gnette. Vignettes are (usually written) scen- minded, and democratic’. One reason for
arios, constituting a short description of a guaranteeing anonymity to respondents is to
person or social situation. They contain spe- reduce the risk of such bias. It is generally
cific references to what are thought by the re- deemed of greater significance in interviews
searcher to be the main factors in respondents’ than in questionnaires (Oppenheim 1992,
decision making or judgement making pro- 126), and logically one might expect the
cesses (Alexander/Becker 1978). For example, greatest risk of social desirability bias with
in Williams’ (1996) study of attitudes to in- grouped respondents (e.g. focus group inter-
tergenerational accommodation, respondents views) where individual anonymity is absent.
evaluated participants in a written vignette de- Secondly, respondents may tend to agree with
scribing a dialogue between two women in a an item, regardless of its content, as a way of
coffee shop. The participants were described gaining the researcher’s approval. This aqui-
in one version of the scenario as close in age escence bias (Ostrom/Bond/Krosnik et al.
(20 and 25 years old), and in the other as more 1994) can occur in response to either ques-
distant in age (25 and 70 years old). By such tionnaire or interview items, but is claimed
standardising of detail across respondents, vi- by some to be especially pronounced in oral
gnettes are deemed to produce more valid and face-to-face interviews (Gass/Seiter 1999,
reliable response measures than the concep- 45). Social desirability and acquiesence are
tual presentation employed in most direct features that are likely to impact on some at-

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
124. Attitude Measurements 1255

titudes, and some people, more than others. can in principle be conducted at a distance
It may be that they have greater effect where (e.g. with telephone interviews). Both can
the issues are of some personal or socio- generate nominal and ordinal data, though
political sensitivity. These are features that where ranking involves larger numbers of
can prevent researchers tapping into the ‘pri- items, word of mouth procedures are prob-
vate attitudes’ of respondents (Lambert’s lematic because of memory loads. In prin-
motivation for using the MGT ). Thirdly, the ciple at least, interval data can also be
validity of data may be affected by qualities sought through written or oral response.
of the researchers themselves, for example in Most commonly, language attitudes studies
terms of power and solidarity attributes have collected written response interval data
(Wolfson 1976), perceived partiality (Dun- recorded on Likert or semantic differential
nette/Heneman 1956), ethnic differences scales, the former associated with direct ap-
(Bellin/Matsuyama/Schott 1999), and the proach studies, and the latter seen as a more
language chosen for conducting research in or less integral component of MGT studies.
bilingual or multilingual contexts (Price/ Likert scales are prepared by assembling a
Fluck/Giles 1983). Finally, evaluations may number of statements about the attitude to
be influenced by allowing or encouraging be measured, and giving them to a sample of
discussion prior to the completion of ques- respondents to rate how far they agree or
tionnaires, through the group polarisation disagree with them. The statements are
phenomenon (Myers/Lamm 1976), which ideally gathered from pilot work with re-
posits that groups make people more ex- spondents comparable to those anticipated
treme (though not directionally different) in for the main study. There is a balance of
their judgements. For example, Giles/Harri- positive and negative statements, and re-
son/Creber et al. (1983) found significant at- sponses are scored on a scale of, say, 1 to 7.
titudinal differences when respondents were Taking into account the directionality of
given time to discuss the questionnaire be- each scale, summation of scores on related
fore completion. A similar phenomenon was statements provides a single score for each
found by Alderfer (1968) when the re- respondent. Factor analysis is often used on
searcher preceded questionnaire completion such scale ratings in order to identify
with an interview. – Evaluative responses are multidimensionality. For example, conduct-
collected through a range of techniques. At ing a factor analysis on a number of attitude
the broadest level, following Henerson/Mor- statements formulated around attitude to
ris/Fitz-Gibbon (1987), we may classify Welsh may identify subcomponents, with
these under the headings ‘Word of Mouth’ some statements perhaps factoring into atti-
and ‘Written Response’ procedures. To some tudes towards learning Welsh, others into
extent, the choices for any study may be se- attitudes towards Welsh speakers, or to-
verely restricted by the nature of the respon- wards the Welsh language. In practice, re-
dents and context. For example, respon- searchers often prefer to have at least four
dents may lack literacy in the language used statements tapping into each attitudinal
(e.g. in the case of deciding whether to use subcomponent, with two negative, and two
questionnaires with children, or foreign spea- positive. Usually, pilot ratings are again ga-
kers), or the best understood language for thered and a rotation factor analysis is con-
data collection may have no written script. ducted to check correlations among the four
Beyond constraints such as these, decisions items in each attitudinal subcomponent,
may depend on methodological preferences. and between the items and the total for each
Word of mouth procedures can allow more attitudinal component (e.g. Garrett/Grif-
interaction between researcher and respon- fiths/James et al. 1994). – Semantic differen-
dent, and thereby facilitate better respon- tial scales were developed by Osgood and his
se rates, and clarification of ambiguities. colleagues in the 1950s (Osgood/Suci/Tan-
Written response procedures can allow more nenbaum 1957), who established early that
anonymity and uniformity, the avoidance of the use of such scales with a wide variety of
interviewer effects, and the possibility of attitude objects produced ratings that corre-
collecting data from a greater number of re- late into three main dimensions, often ab-
spondents in a shorter time. In other ways, breviated to EPA : evaluation, potency, and
the procedures have much in common. Both activity. Zahn/Hopper (1985) have empiri-
can include open as well as closed items, cally validated these same dimensions in
structured or unstructured formats. Both language attitudes research as, respectively,

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
1256 VII. Sociolinguistic Methodology

social attractiveness, superiority, and dyna- One question common to both Likert and
mism. The (typically 7-point) scales are semantic differential scales concerns the
bounded by bipolar adjectives. In MGT number of points on the scale. Although Li-
studies, respondents are usually asked to kert (1932) himself proposed a 5 point scale,
rate the speakers on a number of such scales, researchers have tended to prefer 7 point
and measurement overlap is then analysed scales for the increased variance that such
to identify the broader evaluative dimen- longer scales allow, since these are sensitive
sions with which the respondents are operat- to smaller differences in measurement. The
ing. – The well-documented pattern of mid-point which such scales allow often cre-
evaluative dimensions has meant that in ates ambiguity (Oppenheim 1992, 200); they
many instances language attitudes studies might reflect an uncertain response to an
have selected their scales from those used in issue the respondent feels little involvement
previous studies, while others have supple- towards, or they might reflect a much-con-
mented them with scales from their own pre- sidered commitment to a mid-position. On
liminary work with comparable judges (e.g. the other hand, the solution preferred by
Nesdale/Rooney 1996) or based them all on some researchers of having an even number
such preparatory work (e.g. Price/Fluck/ of points, thus forcing respondents to com-
Giles 1983; Paltridge/Giles 1984). The prob- mit themselves to one direction or the other,
lem with restricting scales to those used in is also controversial, since, for example, it
previous studies is that it may induce some denies respondents the possibility of ex-
circularity and hence also give a deceptive pressing a considered commitment to a mid-
semblance of exhaustiveness. Heise (1970, point. The debate to some extent under-
236ff.), discussing the EPA dimensions in mines the claim that scales are useful for
general attitudes research, while encourag- measuring the intensity of attitudes, with the
ing adjective selection from previously pub- ends of the scales showing greater intensity.
lished factor analyses, conceded that addi- This is certainly a claim that has been con-
tional dimensions are found in some studies. tested (e.g. Weksel/Hennes 1965). Generally,
It seems premature in the relatively new field researchers have tended to use 5 or 7 point
of language attitudes research to assume scales and to live with the ambiguity of the
that other dimensions are of no import. mid-point. – Rating scales have sometimes
Coupland/Williams/Garrett 1999, for ex- been replaced or augmented by behavioural
ample, found a separate dimension of Welsh- measures. Giles/Baker/Fielding (1975), for
ness that was a powerful independent dis- instance, asked school students to write
criminator amongst the different varieties of open-endedly what they thought psychology
Welsh English. A further issue in relation to was about, delivering this request in differ-
the choice of lexical items for semantic dif- ent accents to different groups. The number
ferential scales is that they must be in a para- of words written was taken as a measure of
digmatic relationship of opposites in a favourability. Giles/Farrar (1979), compar-
paired system, with each member of the pair ing the attitudinal effects of accent and
at opposite ends of a continuum. Hence, in dress-style, analysed responses on the sub-
any preliminary study gathering sponta- ject of effects of inflation both by calculat-
neous items for later use in creating sem- ing communication length (number of words
antic differential scales, responses that are written) and by assessing the relative for-
not gradable along a scale have to be mality of style used in the written responses.
discarded. Those without unambiguous Bourhis/Giles (1976) – comparing attitudes
antonyms are also often ignored, but are towards the Welsh language, broad and mild
sometimes rescued through the use of unidi- South Welsh-accented English, and Receiv-
rectional scales, as long as they are gradable: ed Pronunciation – and Kristiansen (1997) –
e.g. irritating / not at all irritating. However, comparing attitudes towards four accents of
items discarded in this way may constitute a Danish (standard, Copenhagen, broad and
valuable ‘discursive shorthand’, providing mild Zealand) – counted the number of the-
insights into respondents’ attitudes that atre audience members who responded to
semantic differential scales miss, and indeed public address system announcements de-
may send out unmistakable impressions of livered in the different language varieties.
directionality and intensity too, even if they The number of completed questionnaires
cannot provide interval data for statistical was calculated as a proportion of total ticket
analysis. sales, and the results were taken to indicate

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
124. Attitude Measurements 1257

comparative attitudes to the different lan- geographical picture, and indeed at times
guage varieties. This approach is indirect in produced seemingly inconsistent patterns of
the sense that respondents are not aware of results across studies (see Coupland/Williams/
the purpose of the research, and it is less Garrett 1994 on the Welsh context). Conse-
likely that they will respond on the basis of quently, there has been a trend towards col-
social acceptability. However, it is not poss- lecting comparable data from a number of lo-
ible to infer from such behaviour alone the cations over a whole area to investigate not
broader attitudinal profile of such varieties only the general area-wide attitudes to spe-
in terms of a set of evaluative dimensions, cific varieties within that area, but also how
such as ‘superiority’, ‘social attractiveness’ attitudes to the varieties compare amongst
and ‘dynamism’. – As a recent but influen- the various local communities within the
tial new paradigm, perceptual dialectologi- broader area. This has been a growing focus
cal studies have used a variety of other of attitudes research in the U.S. (e.g. Pre-
measures (see Preston 1999b for a sum- ston’s 1989 studies of the perceptions of U.S.
mary). Typically though, respondents draw regional dialects of southern Indiana and
dialect boundaries on a blank or minimally Michigan residents, and Hartley’s 1999
detailed map where they believe regional study of the perceptions in Oregon), in Den-
speech zones exist. They then rank the areas mark (e.g. Ladegaard 2001, on perceptions
for specific features such as correctness, of regional standard Danishes), and in
pleasantness (e.g. Preston 1989), or rate Wales (e.g. Garrett/Coupland/Williams
each region on semantic differential scales 1999), where teachers and teenagers from all
(e.g. Preston 1999a; Coupland/Williams/ over Wales evaluated each other’s regional
Garrett 1994), or give labels of their own dialects of English). Other such areal studies
formulation to these areas. Williams/Gar- have been conducted in France, Germany,
rett/Coupland (1996), for example, asked re- Japan, and Turkey (see Preston 1999c).
spondents to both write labels they would Alongside this development is a growing
use for each of the areas they delineated, and trend (not totally absent earlier – e.g. Giles
provide written characterisations of each 1970) towards collecting and comparing dif-
dialect according to what they found dis- ferent types of data within a single study.
tinctive about them. The data were found to Ladegaard (2001), for instance, presented
have stereotypical evaluative content, and, audiorecorded speech samples of regional
through content analysis, characterisations standard varieties of Danish to his respon-
were found to spread with differentiating dents, and asked them, amongst other
frequencies across a number of coding cat- things, for an appropriate label for each var-
egories (e.g. linguistic features, positive or iety they heard, and their immediate impres-
negative affective features, Welshness or sion of the speaker. Both data sets produced
non-Welshness) to give each variety its own attitudinal data. Hoare (2001) used a combi-
distinctive evaluative profile. nation of direct and indirect techniques, and
supplemented quantitative data with discur-
6. Current issues and directions sive responses, to investigate language atti-
tudes and identity in Brittany. In their study
Much recent language attitudes research has of attitudes to Welsh English dialects, Coup-
pursued perceptual dialectological and folk- land/Williams/Garrett (1994), Williams/
linguistic approaches (see Preston 1989; Garrett/Coupland (1996), and Garrett/
1999c; Niedzielski/Preston 2000). Hence for Coupland/Williams (1999) collected data
the most part, it has focused on attitudes to from perceptual mapping and labelling
geographically based varieties, though also tasks, scales and open-ended data from con-
incorporating standard varieties even where ceptual and audio presented dialects, and
these are primarily social rather than re- data from small group interviews. Gathering
gional varieties. Such work has helped to ad- several types of data, though carrying the
dress one of the limitations of earlier re- same risk of throwing up a confusion of
search, as expressed in particular by Price/ contradictory results that Price/Fluck/Giles
Fluck/Giles (1983) in relation to Wales. (1983) bemoaned, is tending to build up a
MGT studies in particular tended to be one- richer and more solid view and understand-
off relatively small-scale and very localised ing of attitudes and their attributions than
investigations that did not always allow re- reliance on earlier techniques achieved. Col-
searchers to gain insights into the broader lecting qualitative data alongside the usual

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
1258 VII. Sociolinguistic Methodology

scales data can facilitate deeper insights into evolving service-oriented contexts. The gen-
the cultural processes at work beneath the eral loosening of socio-structural norms
evaluative scores attributed to each variety that is associated with late modernity
along the various attitudinal dimensions. (Giddens 1994) may present new directions
This in turn can afford a clearer view of the for attitude measurement.
relative importances of the attitudinal di-
mensions to respondent groups in various
contexts, the nature of regional rivalries and 7. Literature (selected)
affiliations, and of struggles for socio-politi- Aitchison, Jean (2001) “Misunderstandings about
cal and cultural maintenance and change. language: A historical overview”, in: Journal of
Comparing results across different types of Sociolinguistics 5, 611–619.
data can also bring valuable methodological
Alderfer, Clayton (1968) “Comparison of ques-
benefits for attitude measurement. For tionnaire responses with and without preceding
example, some recent studies have found, as interviews”, in: Journal of Applied Psychology 52,
did Giles (1970), that presenting language 335–340.
varieties ‘conceptually’ or as audio-record-
Alexander, Cheryl S./Becker, Henry J. (1978) “The
ings does not necessarily lead to any signifi-
use of vignettes in survey research”, in: Public
cant difference in findings (e.g. Preston Opinion Quarterly 42, 93–104.
1999c, 369). This reinforces the point made
earlier that, given the above-mentioned is- Baker, Colin (1988) Key Issues in Bilingualism and
sues around the construction of MGT Bilingual Education, Clevedon.
audio-recordings, indirect methods are like- –, (1992) Attitudes and Language, Clevedon.
ly to be most useful in contexts where the at- Bayard, Donn/Weatherall, Ann/Gallois, Cynthia
titudes under study are particularly fraught et al., (2001) “Pax Americana? Accent attitudinal
with sensitivities. Furthermore, where the evaluations in New Zealand, Australia and
use of semantic differential scales has pro- America”, in: Journal of Sociolinguistics 5, 22–49.
duced results different from those gained
Bellin, Wynford/ Matsuyama, Akiko/ Schott, Ga-
from open-ended characterising of dialects, reth (1999) “Teaching a ‘dead’ language, teaching
important methodological questions have through a ‘dead’ language: conflict and consensus
been raised about the whether ‘private’ atti- during language revitalisation”, in: Proceedings of
tudes are more likely to relate to this aspect the 7th International Minority Languages Confer-
of study design rather than (or as much as) ence, Eusko Jauralitza/ Gobierno Basco, eds., Bil-
the use of conceptual versus audio-recorded bao, Spain, 49a-d.
presentations (Garrett/Coupland/Williams Bourhis, Richard/Giles, Howard (1976) “The lan-
1995). – Finally, effects on language and guage of co-operation in Wales: a field study”, in:
communication often attributed to late mo- Language Sciences 42, 13–16.
dernity are suggesting some tantalising new
Bradac, James (1990) “Language attitudes and
focal points for language attitudes research. impression formation”, in: Handbook of Lan-
Traditionally labelled ‘standard’ speech var- guage and Social Psychology, Giles, H./ Robinson,
ieties may now be losing their uniquely posi- P., eds., Chichester, 387–412.
tive evaluations. ‘Levelled’ and less main-
Breckler, Steven (1984) “Empirical validation of
stream varieties may be judged more
affect, behaviour, and cognition as distinct com-
favourably (Kerswill/Williams 2000). More- ponents of attitude”, in: Journal of Personality
over, Kristiansen (2001) shows the emerg- and Social Psychology 47, 1191–1205.
ence of more than one standard Danish,
each favoured in different professional con- Cameron, Deborah (2000) Good to Talk?, Lon-
don.
texts (media versus education and business)
and differentiated largely on measurements Cargile, Aaron/Giles, Howard/Ryan, Ellen B./
of dynamism. The growth of service sector Bradac, James (1994) “Language attitudes as a so-
industries in many regions, customer care cial process”, in: Language and Communication
lines and telesales, where impression man- 14, 211–236.
agement is paramount (Cameron 2000), may Cheshire, Jenny/Moser, Lise-Marie (1994) “Eng-
mean less advantage for varieties mainly lish as a cultural symbol”, in: Journal of Multilin-
strong on prestige measurements. Hitherto gual and Multicultural Development 15, 451–469.
neglected dimensions (most notably, Cook, Stuart/Sellitz, Claire (1964) “A multiple-in-
McCroskey/Teven’s 1999 ‘perceived caring’ dicator approach to attitude measurement”, in:
dimension) warrant measurement in such Psychological Bulletin 62, 36–55.

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
124. Attitude Measurements 1259

Cots, Josep/Nussbaum, Luci (1999) “Schooling, Giles, Howard/Coupland, Nikolas/Henwood,


language and teachers’ language awareness and Karen et al., (1990) “The social meaning of RP :
the discourse of the educational reform in Catalo- an intergenerational perspective”, in: Studies in
nia”, in: Language Awareness 8, 174–189. the Pronunciation of English: A Commemorative
Coupland, Nikolas/Williams, Angie/Garrett, Volume in Honour of A.C. Gimson, Ramsaran, S.,
Peter (1994) “The social meanings of Welsh Eng- ed., London, 191–211.
lish: teachers’ stereotyped judgements”, in: Jour- Giles, Howard/Harrison, C./Creber, Clare et al.,
nal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development (1983) “Developmental and contextual aspects of
15, 471–491. children’s language attitudes”, in: Language and
Communication 3, 1–6.
–, (1999) “‘Welshness’ and ‘Englishness’ as atti-
tudinal dimensions of English language varieties Greenbaum, Sidney, ed., (1985) The English Lan-
in Wales”, in: Handbook of Perceptual Dialectol- guage Today, Oxford.
ogy, vol. 1, Preston, D., ed., Amsterdam, 333–343. Haarmann, Harald (1984) “The role of ethnocul-
Dawes, Robyn/Smith, Tom (1985) “Attitude and tural stereotypes and foreign languages in Japan-
opinion measurement” in: Handbook of Social ese commercials”, in: International Journal of the
Psychology, Lindzey, G./Aronson, E., eds., Sociology of Language 50, 101–121.
509–566, New York. –, (1986) “Verbal strategies in Japanese fashion
Dunnette, Marvin/Heneman, Herbert (1956) “In- magazines: A study in impersonal bilingualism
fluence of scale administrator on employee atti- and ethnosymbolism”, in: International Journal of
tude responses”, in: Journal of Applied Psychology the Sociology of Language 58, 107–121.
40, 73–77. Hartley, Laura (1999) “A view from the West: per-
ceptions of U.S. dialects by Oregon residents”, in:
El Dash, Linda/Tucker, G. Richard (1975) “Sub-
Handbook of Perceptual Dialectology, vol. 1, Pre-
jective reactions to various speech styles in
ston, D., ed., Amsterdam, 315–332.
Egypt”, in: International Journal of the Sociology
of Language 6, 33–54. Heise, David (1970) “The semantic differential in
attitude research”, in: Attitude Measurement,
Garrett, Peter/Griffiths, Yvonne/James, Carl/ Summers, G., ed., New York, 235–253.
Scholfield, Phil (1994) “Use of the mother-tongue
in second language classrooms”, in: Journal of Henerson, Marlene/Morris, Lynn L./Fitzgibbon,
Multilingual and Multicultural Development 15, Carol T. (1987) How to Measure Attitudes, New-
371–383. bury Park, CA .
Hoare, Rachel (2001) “An integrative approach to
Garrett, Peter/Coupland, Nikolas/Williams,
language attitudes and identity in Brittany”, in:
Angie (1995) “‘City harsh’ and ‘the Welsh version
Journal of Sociolinguistics 5, 73–84.
of RP ’: some ways in which teachers view dialects
of Welsh English”, in: Language Awareness 4, Jørgensen, Jens N./Quist, Pia (2001) “Native
99–107. speakers’ judgements of second language Da-
nish”, in: Language Awareness 10, 41–56.
–, (1999) “Evaluating dialect in discourse: Teach-
ers’ and teenagers’ responses to young English Kerswill, Paul/Williams, Ann (2000) “Creating a
speakers in Wales”, in: Language in Society 16, new town koine: Children and language change in
369–380. Milton Keynes”, in: Language in Society 29,
65–115.
Gass, Robert/Seiter, John (1999) Persuasion, So-
Knops, Uus/van Hout, Roeland (1988) “Lan-
cial Influence, and Compliance Gaining, Boston.
guage attitudes in the Dutch language area: An
Giddens, Anthony (1994) “Living in a post-tradi- introduction”, in: Language Attitudes in the Dutch
tional society”, in: Reflexive Modernisation: Language Area, van Hout, R./Knops, U., eds.,
Politics, Tradition, and Aesthetics in the Modern Dordrecht, 1–23.
Social Order, Beck, U./Giddens, A./Lash, S., eds., Kramarae, Cheris (1982) “Gender: How she
Cambridge, 56–109. speaks”, in: Attitudes Towards Language Vari-
Giles, Howard (1970) “Evaluative reactions to ac- ation, Ryan, E.B./Giles, H., eds., London, 84–98.
cents”, in: Educational Review 22, 211–227. Kristiansen, Tore (1997) “Language attitudes in a
Giles, Howard/Coupland, Nikolas (1991) Lan- Danish cinema”, in: Sociolinguistics: A Reader
guage: Contexts and Consequences, Buckingham. and Coursebook, Coupland, N./Jaworski, A., eds.,
Giles, Howard/Farrar, Kathryn (1979) “Some be- Basingstoke, 291–305.
havioural consequences of speech and dress –, (2001) “Two standards: One for the media and
styles”, in: British Journal of Social and Clinical one for the school”, in: Language Awareness 10,
Psychology 18, 209–210. 9–24.
Giles, Howard/Baker, Susan/Fielding, Guy (1975) Labov, William (1972) Sociolinguistic Patterns,
“Communication length as a behavioural index of Philadelphia.
accent prejudice”, in: International Journal of the Ladegaard, Hans J. (2001) “Popular perceptions
Sociology of Language 6, 73–81. of standard language: Attitudes to regional stan-

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM
1260 VII. Sociolinguistic Methodology

dards in Denmark”, in: Language Awareness 10, –, (1996) “Whaddyaknow? The modes of folk lin-
25–40. guistic awareness”, in: Language Awareness 5,
Lambert, Wallace E./Hodgson, R./Gardner, Ro- 40–74.
bert C. et al. (1960) “Evaluative reactions to –, (1999a) “A language attitude approach to the
spoken languages”, in: Journal of Abnormal and perception of regional variety”, in: Preston 1999c,
Social Psychology 60, 44–51. 359–373.
Lee, R. (1971) “Dialect perception: a critical re- –, (1999b) “Introduction”, in: Preston 1999c,
view and re-evaluation”, in: Quarterly Journal of xxiii-xi.
Speech 57, 410–17.
–, ed., (1999c) Handbook of Perceptual Dialectol-
Levin, Harry/Giles, Howard/Garrett, Peter (1994) ogy, vol. 1, Amsterdam.
“The effects of lexical formality and accent on
trait attributions”, in: Language and Communi- Price, Susan/Fluck, Michael/Giles, Howard
cation 14, 265–74. (1983) “The effects of the language of testing on
bilingual pre-adolescents’ attitudes towards
Lieberson, Stanley (1981) Language Diversity and
Welsh and varieties of English”, in: Journal of
Language Contact: Essays by Stanley Lieberson,
Multilingual and Multicultural Development 4,
Stanford.
149–161.
Likert, R. (1932) A Technique for the Measure-
ment of Attitudes, New York. Ray, George/Zahn, Christopher (1999) “Lan-
guage attitudes and speech behaviour”, in: Jour-
McCroskey, James/Teven, Jason (1999) “Good- nal of Language and Social Psychology 18,
will: A re-examination of the construct and its 310–319.
measurement”, in: Communication Monographs
66, 90–103. Rickford, John/Traugott, Elizabeth (1985) “Sym-
bol of powerlessness and degeneracy, or symbol of
Milroy, Lesley (1999) “Standard English and lan- solidarity and truth? Paradoxical attitudes toward
guage ideology in Britain and the United States”, pidgins and creoles”, in: The English Language
in: Standard English: The Widening Debate, Bex, Today, Greenbaum, S., ed., 252–261.
T./Watts, R., eds., London, 173–206.
Ryan, Ellen B./Giles, Howard/Hewstone, Miles
Myers, David/Lamm, Helmut (1976) “The group
(1988) “The measurement of language attitudes”,
polarisation phenomenon”, in: Psychological
in: Sociolinguistics: an International Handbook,
Bulletin 83, 602–67.
Ammon, U./Dittmar, N./Mattheier, K., eds., Ber-
Nesdale, Drew/Rooney, Rosanna (1996) “Evalu- lin, 1068–1082.
ations and stereotyping of accented speakers by
pre-adolescent children”, in: Journal of Language ecksel, William/Hennes, James D. (1965) “Atti-
and Social Psychology 15, 133–54. tude intensity and the semantic differential”, in:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2,
Niedzielski, Nancy/Preston, Dennis (2000) Folk 91–94.
Linguistics, Berlin.
Williams, Angie (1996) “Young people’s evalu-
Oppenheim, Bram (1992) Questionnaire Design, ations of intergenerational versus peer under-ac-
Interviewing, and Attitude Measurement, London. commodation. Sometimes older is better?”, in:
Osgood, Charles/Suci, George/Tannenbaum, Journal of Language and Social Psychology 15,
Percy (1957) The Measurement of Meaning, Ur- 291–311.
bana. Williams, Angie/Garrett, Peter/Coupland, Niko-
Ostrom, Thomas/Bond, Charles/Krosnik, Jon et las (1996) “Perceptual dialectology, folk lin-
al. (1994) “Attitude scales: how we measure the guistics, and regional stereotypes: Teachers’ per-
unmeasurable”, in: Persuasion: Psychological In- ceptions of variation in Welsh English”, in:
sights and Perspectives, Shavitt, S./Brock, T., eds., Multilingua 15, 171–99.
Boston, 15–42.
Wolfson, Nessa (1976) “Speech events and natu-
Paltridge, John/Giles, Howard (1984) “Attitudes ral speech: some implications for sociolinguistic
towards speakers of regional accents of French: methodology”, in: Language in Society 5,
Effects of regionality, age and sex of listeners”, in: 189–209.
Linguistische Berichte 90, 71–85.
Zahn, Christopher/Hopper, Robert (1985)
Perloff, Richard (1993) The Dynamics of Persua- “Measuring language attitudes: The speech evalu-
sion, Hillsdale, NJ. ation instrument”, in: Journal of Language and
Preston, Dennis (1989) Perceptual Dialectology: Social Psychology 4, 113–23.
Non-linguists’ Views of Areal Linguistics, Dor-
drecht. Peter Garrett, Cardiff (Great Britain)

Unauthenticated | 187.146.85.77
Download Date | 11/13/12 3:23 PM

You might also like