Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/262577570

The importance of cannabis culture in young adult cannabis use

Article  in  Journal of Substance Use · June 2014


DOI: 10.3109/14659891.2013.790493

CITATIONS READS

12 1,434

4 authors:

Soren Holm Sveinung Sandberg


Municipality of Aarhus University of Oslo
4 PUBLICATIONS   20 CITATIONS    82 PUBLICATIONS   1,740 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Torsten Kolind Morten Hesse


Aarhus University Aarhus University
67 PUBLICATIONS   452 CITATIONS    146 PUBLICATIONS   1,802 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Impulsive lifestyle counselling View project

Narrative Criminology View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Sveinung Sandberg on 13 July 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


http://informahealthcare.com/jsu
ISSN: 1465-9891 (print), 1475-9942 (electronic)

J Subst Use, 2014; 19(3): 251–256


! 2014 Informa UK Ltd. DOI: 10.3109/14659891.2013.790493

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The importance of cannabis culture in young adult cannabis use


Søren Holm1, Sveinung Sandberg2, Torsten Kolind3, and Morten Hesse3
1
Municipality of Skive, Skive, Denmark, 2Department of Criminology and Sociology of Law, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway, and 3Centre for Alcohol
and Drug Research, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

Abstract Keywords
J Subst Use Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Oslo universitetssykehus Aker on 05/08/14

Background: Research into drug use initiation has focussed on drug use as risky behaviour. Adolescents, cannabis, culture,
Qualitative research suggests that a culturally derived set of beliefs about the positive effects of experimental use
cannabis play an important role in the production and maintenance of cannabis use.
Methods: An online survey questionnaire was designed to measure both perceived positive and History
negative effects of cannabis. The questionnaire was submitted to a factor analysis, and two
factors were extracted: a positive and a negative effect scale. Using regression analyses, Received 12 December 2012
associations with cannabis use, perception of general use and perception of general Revised 15 March 2013
acceptance of use were assessed. Accepted 25 March 2013
Results: After removing respondents based on age and responses to questions, 1416 valid Published online 5 July 2013
survey questionnaires were collected. In bivariate analyses, both positive effects and negative
effects were associated with use (p50.001), perceived normative use (p50.001) and
acceptance of use (p50.001). In multivariate analyses, only glorification remained consistently
associated with perceived use, acceptance and all categories of use (p50.001).
For personal use only.

Conclusions: Positive beliefs about the effects of cannabis use are important and form part of a
cannabis culture, which are essential for understanding initiation and maintenance of cannabis
use. These findings have important ramifications for cannabis preventive work and also call on
further investigation into the relation between cannabis culture and individual use.

Introduction with greater dependence severity (Connor et al., 2011).


The importance of expectancies for gender, and the onset age
Cannabis is the most commonly used illicit drug in the world.
of cannabis use, onset age of abuse, abuse and dependence
As many as 200 million people use cannabis worldwide
have also been revealed (Guillem et al., 2011). By including
(Degenhardt & Hall, 2012). Cannabis use is associated with a
positive expectancies to drug use, the CEQ makes it possible
significant risk of psychotic disorders (Rossler et al., 2012).
to study how positive and negative individual expectations
Although risk is lower for cannabis than for several other
influence drug use.
substances, a considerable proportion of users eventually
Cannabis can be seen as symbolic consumption, situated in
become dependent on cannabis (Degenhardt & Hall, 2012).
language (Booth, 2004; Matthews, 2003). When cannabis was
Evidence links early onset of cannabis use to later problems,
introduced on a large scale in the West during the 60 s, it was
including both psychosis and dependence (Schneider, 2008).
It is thus not surprising that perceived personal risks are
associated with new age beliefs, interest in Eastern philoso- 14
20
phy and vague political opposition, and became a symbol
negatively associated with cannabis use (Beaudoin & Hong,
amid the political struggles of the 1960s (Booth, 2004; Clarke
2012), and that prevention efforts often rest on the assumption
et al., 2000; Pedersen, 2009; Young, 1971). Although the
that information about drug use should increase risk aware-
symbolic status of cannabis originates in the social move-
ness, or improve desistence skills such as resisting peer
ments of the 60 s and 70 s, it has later developed and merged
pressure (Peretti-Watel & Moatti, 2006).
with other cultural currents such as Rastafarian culture
However, emphasis on risks associated with use may only
(Hamid, 2002) and hip-hop culture (Golub, 2005). It is in
tell half the story. The Cannabis Expectancy Questionnaire
these hybrid cultures that the present day ‘‘cannabis culture’’
(CEQ) was designed to study both negative and the positive
can be identified. Today, cannabis users remain supportive of
use outcome expectancies (Young & Kavanagh, 1997).
anti-authoritarian groups (Pedersen, 2009; Stevenson, 2012),
Recent studies using this questionnaire link positive expec-
and attend rock festivals at high rates (Hesse et al., 2010;
tancies with higher consumption and negative expectancies
Lim et al., 2010).
The cultural meaning of cannabis has received little
attention in the understanding of individuals’ use of cannabis,
Correspondence: Dr Torsten Kolind, Centre for Alcohol and Drug
Research, Aarhus University, Bartholins Allé 10, bygn. 1322, Aarhus, and in cannabis preventive strategies. There have also been
Aarhus C Denmark. E-mail: tk@crf.au.dk few attempts to link qualitative studies of the symbolic
252 S. Holm et al. J Subst Use, 2014; 19(3): 251–256

meaning and narratives of cannabis, with quantitative studies than once in the past month (‘‘monthly’’) and if they
of cannabis expectancies and use. Moreover, positive generally used cannabis more than once per week
expectancies to cannabis use (Connor, et al., 2011; Guillem, (‘‘weekly’’).
et al., 2011) have not been studied in a Scandinavian context The CCQ was developed based on qualitative data from
previously. previous studies (Dahl & Frank, 2011; Pedersen & Sandberg,
In a qualitative study including interviews with 100 2013; Sandberg, 2012, 2013). Items in the positive effects
cannabis users in Norway, we found that apart from scale were directly replicating the dominant cultural meanings
describing cannabis as harmless or common, users elaborate and narratives that appeared in interviews with cannabis
on the benefits of cannabis use, by describing the cannabis users. As opposed to the CEQ (Young & Kavanagh, 1997),
high as peaceful, spiritual, a sharpening of senses, and and in line with the way cannabis user spoke in interviews,
cannabis users as open-minded, creative and anti-authoritar- they were not stated to refer to an individual person, but rather
ian (Sandberg, 2012). Moreover, in both the Danish and as general statements. Harms and benefits of cannabis
Norwegian study, cannabis users emphasize the fact that previously described by cannabis users were in the CCQ
cannabis is a natural product and that it has medical value rephrased into 27 brief items. Responses were then subjected
(Dahl & Frank, 2011; Pedersen & Sandberg, 2013). Together, to factor analysis to derive scales (see below). Each item was
such beliefs and more general narratives of cannabis users can answered on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘‘strongly
be seen as part of current cannabis culture, that is, a relatively disagree’’ to ‘‘strongly agree’’.
J Subst Use Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Oslo universitetssykehus Aker on 05/08/14

homogeneous set of symbols, rituals and narratives


(Sandberg, 2013). This study seeks to identify these qualita-
Statistical analyses
tive findings and the relation between beliefs and cannabis
use in a general population sample of students in a catchment The 27 items in the CCQ were subjected to a series of
area. The beliefs associated with cannabis culture were exploratory factor analyses in a random subset of the
measured on a positive effect scale, and their importance for respondents (n ¼ 747). The first analyses were based on a
use was compared with beliefs associated with the risks of random half of the sample of respondents. Because items
cannabis use on a negative effect scale. More specifically, the were scored on a Likert scale, the factor analyses were done
study was exploratory, aspiring to: (a) develop a measure of using the polychoric correlations. Parallel analysis was used
the cannabis culture, the Cannabis Culture Questionnaire to decide the number of factors. Based on permutation of
For personal use only.

(CCQ); (b) assess the factor structure of the CCQ; (c) identify sample values, 500 random correlation matrices were
what if any CCQ subscales are significantly associated with produced. Factors were retained if they explained more than
cannabis use; and (d) identify what if any CCQ subscales are was explained by the 95th percent of random correlation
associated with acceptance of use among never-users. Our matrices (Timmerman & Lorenzo-Seva, 2011). Factors were
hypothesis was that the beliefs of the cannabis culture are extracted using the unweighted least squares estimator, and
relatively widespread among adolescents and that for canna- results were rotated using Promin rotation. After conducting a
bis use, positive beliefs are more important than beliefs preliminary factor analysis with all items, items were
associated with risks of use. excluded if they had a communality lower than 0.5
(MacCallum et al., 2001; Zimmer & Morgan, 1997), or if
they had loadings 40.3 on more than one factor. The factor
Methods
structure was then replicated using the remaining 772
The setting was 10 educational programs offering education respondents who completed all items, and the coefficient of
for students aged 16–18 years in a single catchment area congruence was used to compare the factor solutions. For
(Skive Municipality) in Denmark, an area with small towns the coefficient of congruence, values above 0.95 indicate that
and rural areas. School programs included high schools, two factors can be considered equal (Lorenzo-Seva & ten
technical schools that offer skills, a high school equivalence Berge, 2006). Factor analysis was carried out using FACTOR
program and a program offering a mixture of educational and (Lorenzo-Seva & Ferrando, 2006).
work activities to young people who are not participating in When the scales were extracted, a series of regression
regular education. All schools (N ¼ 10) in the municipality analyses were conducted to assess correlates of the derived
participated, and all students present in all classes filled in the sub-scales of the CCQ. Multinomial logistic regression was
questionnaire during lessons. Perceived norms were measured conducted to predict the relative risk of use in four categories
by asking two questions: ‘‘How large a proportion of the (never used, lifetime use, used monthly, used weekly). Among
students at my school have used cannabis more than once in never-users, multinomial logistic regression was used to
the past month?’’ and ‘‘How many students at my school find predict the likelihood that a respondent would think that it
it OK to smoke cannabis’’. Response options for both was acceptable to smoke, and the expectation that their
questions ranged from ‘‘10%’’ to ‘‘100%’’. friends would think that it is acceptable if they smoked, with
To assess never-users’ openness to try cannabis, we asked the response variables ‘‘yes’’, ‘‘no’’ and ‘‘maybe’’ for both
two questions: ‘‘I think it is quite OK to smoke cannabis once variables. We reversed the negative effects scale and
in a while’’ and ‘‘My friends would think that it was OK standardized both scales to have a mean of zero and a
if I smoked cannabis’’. For both questions, the response standard deviation of one in order to make coefficients for the
options were ‘‘Yes’’, ‘‘No’’ or ‘‘Don’t know’’. Students were two factor-derived scales comparable in regression analyses.
asked to self-report their cannabis use by indicating if they All analyses were controlled for differences between
had ever smoked cannabis (lifetime), if they had done so more schools, and the gender and age of the respondent.
DOI: 10.3109/14659891.2013.790493 Cannabis culture 253

Additional control variables are described in the text below. identical. The items and the loadings from the replication
Due to multiple significance tests, a was set to 0.001. are shown in Table 1. In the replication subsample, the
internal consistency of the resulting Positive Effect scale was
Results a ¼ 0.84, and the internal consistency of the Negative Effect
Participants scale was a ¼ 0.89. Goodness of Fit Index for the model was
0.99, and the Root Mean Square of Residuals was 0.05,
A total of 1769 students accessed the survey, and 1599
indicating acceptable model fit.
completed all items. Factor analyses were conducted with the
The first factor was comprised of nine items describing
1519 subjects who completed the CCQ and were within the
mainly negative consequences of cannabis use. Examples
age range of 16–22. After removing respondents younger than
include dropping out of normal society, poor brain function-
16 years and older than 21 years (n ¼ 174), and all respond-
ing, the presence of negative consequences and negative
ents who gave the same response to every question (n ¼ 20),
impact of cannabis on the ability to work or finish an
1416 were left for analysis (i.e. 66% of all students in the education. We have chosen to call this factor ‘‘negative effect
area). The mean age of the respondents was 18.1 years
scale’’, because it includes information that points to the
(SD ¼ 1.2), and the sample was comprised of 710 women and
dangers of cannabis use. The second factor was comprised of
706 men.
eight items mainly describing cannabis or cannabis users in a
Of all subjects, 57% were never-users of cannabis, 30%
positive way. Examples include describing cannabis users as
J Subst Use Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Oslo universitetssykehus Aker on 05/08/14

were lifetime users, 7% were past month users and 6% were


relaxed, as a source of creativity and as a positive contribution
weekly users.
to culture. We labelled this factor ‘‘positive effect scale’’, and
Factor analysis we view these kind of statements as indicative of the cannabis
culture which positively shapes the symbolic meaning of the
An initial factor analysis of all items supported a two-factor
drug (‘‘celebrating the drug’’; Sandberg, 2012).
solution. Of the 27 items, 10 either had communalities 50.5
or loadings 40.3 on both factors. When conducting the final
Associations with frequency of use
factor analysis, the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test (KMO)
was ¼ 0.92 (very good), and the parallel analysis confirmed The bivariate Spearman correlations between variables used
that a two-factor solution was optimal for the data. Factor 1 in the analyses are shown in Table 2. Note that for all
can be described as negative effects. The items of the factor analyses, higher scores on the Negative Effects scale represent
For personal use only.

concern health and social consequences of cannabis. Factor 2 disagreement with negative effects. All cannabis variables
can be described as a positive view of cannabis that broadly were significantly inter-related, and male gender was
identifies a range of positive effects for both the individual associated with positive effects, negative effects and fre-
and society. For the purpose of this article, we shall call this quency of use. As would be expected, self-reported frequency
factor Positive Effects. of use was associated with estimated frequency of use at the
When the same variables were analysed with the second school and with the perceived proportion of people who
half of the sample, KMO was 0.90, the recommended number would accept cannabis use at the school.
of factors was 2 and the coefficients of congruence were The purpose of the first regression analyses was to assess
0.982 for Negative Effects and 0.981 for Positive Effects, associations between positive effects, negative effects and
indicating that the factor structures can be considered frequency of use (see Table 3). In the first regression analyses,

Table 1. Items and factor structure (replication sample, n ¼ 772)1.

Mean2 SD F1 F2 H2
1. People who smoke cannabis are more relaxed in the way they are together 2.81 1.09 0.213 0.777 0.430
2. People who do not smoke cannabis stress over all sorts of meaningless things 3.40 1.19 0.192 0.582 0.227
3. People get lazy and lose initiative when they smoke cannabis 2.42 1.02 0.667 0.203 0.306
4. People get more ideas and become more creative when they smoke cannabis 3.13 1.06 0.059 0.572 0.375
5. People can expand their consciousness and gain greater insight in life by smoking cannabis 3.34 1.05 0.082 0.560 0.381
6. People who smoke cannabis regularly have somewhat dropped out of ‘‘normal society’’ 2.73 1.22 0.678 0.086 0.389
7. When people start to smoke cannabis, their brains will function poorly 2.16 1.10 0.666 0.005 0.449
8. Alcohol is more harmful than cannabis 2.78 1.26 0.008 0.577 0.327
9. Cannabis has contributed positively to our culture (e.g. in relation to music or humour) 2.81 1.24 0.049 0.759 0.530
10. The cannabis plant is doing more good than harm, among other things because it can be 2.68 1.06 0.044 0.749 0.518
used as medicine
11. It is important to remember that cannabis is a natural product 2.74 1.22 0.066 0.721 0.462
12. Most people could not finish an education or look after a job if they smoke cannabis every night 2.40 1.31 0.704 0.068 0.437
13. People who smoke cannabis become less ambitious and career minded 2.45 1.14 0.841 0.123 0.585
14. Cannabis can cause dependence 1.64 1.00 0.627 0.075 0.337
15. Smoking cannabis will often lead to ‘‘hard drugs’’ 2.49 1.19 0.683 0.024 0.489
16. If someone smokes cannabis, it will often affect their lives in a negative way 2.37 1.13 0.778 0.106 0.726
17. If someone smokes cannabis, it will often affect their lives in a positive way 3.70 1.08 0.463 0.334 0.532
Eigenvalue 6.520 2.073

F1: Factor 1. F2: Factor 2.


1
Factor scores with salient loadings in the construction sample are in boldface.
2
On a Likert scale from 1 (complete agree) to 5 (completely disagree).
254 S. Holm et al. J Subst Use, 2014; 19(3): 251–256

Table 2. Intercorrelations of variables (n ¼ 1416). Table 4. Prediction of perceived peer acceptance of use among never-
users by multinomial logistic regression analyses (n ¼ 803).
NO Perceived Perceived
GLOR HARM Age Gender accept norm Model 1 Model 2
NO HARM 0.49 b Z b Z
Age 0.04 0.05
Gender 0.27 0.26 0.05 Not OK to smoke REF REF
Perceived accept 0.25 0.14 0.02 0.00 OK to smoke
Perceived norm 0.19 0.05 0.05 0.05 0.62 GLOR *1.47 7.90 *1.15 5.88
Frequency of use1 0.43 0.32 0.08 0.23 0.33 0.27 NO HARM *1.25 7.83 *0.94 5.54
Don’t know
Spearman correlations. GLOR: Glorification. NO HARM: Rejection of GLOR *0.70 4.25 0.33 1.83
harms. Correlations in boldface are significant at p50.001. NEUT *1.09 6.99 *0.98 5.91
1
On a scale from 0 (never) to 4 (more than once per week).
REF: Reference value. GLOR: Glorification. NO HARM: disagreement
with harm-related items.
Table 3. Prediction of frequency of use by multinomial logistic Model 1: Controlling for age, gender, differences between schools.
regression analyses (n ¼ 1416). Model 2: Same as above, plus GLOR and NO HARM entered together.
*p50.001.
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
J Subst Use Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Oslo universitetssykehus Aker on 05/08/14

b Z b Z b Z Table 5. Prediction of perceived peer acceptance of use among never-


users by multinomial logistic regression analyses (n ¼ 803).
Never used REF REF REF
Lifetime
GLOR *0.79 7.56 *0.70 6.44 *0.61 5.20 Model 1 Model 2
NO HARM *0.47 5.11 *0.43 4.50 0.22 2.08 b Z b Z
Monthlyþ
GLOR *2.19 11.20 *2.01 9.99 *1.68 7.77 Would not accept REF REF
NO HARM *1.40 8.87 *1.33 8.15 *0.76 4.28 Would accept
Weeklyþ GLOR *0.96 5.32 *0.74 3.85
GLOR *2.85 12.33 *2.59 10.84 *2.19 8.71 NO HARM *0.79 4.93 0.54 3.14
NO HARM *1.76 10.03 *1.62 8.85 *0.93 4.63 Don’t know
GLOR *0.82 5.53 0.54 3.33
For personal use only.

REF: Reference value. GLOR: Glorification. NO HARM: disagreement NO HARM *0.89 6.46 *0.70 4.71
with harm-related items. Monthlyþ: Use more than once per month.
Weeklyþ: Use more than once per week. REF: Reference value. GLOR: Glorification. NO HARM: disagreement
Model 1: Controlling for age, gender, differences between schools. with harm-related items.
Model 2: Controlling for age, gender, differences between school, Model 1: Controlling for age, gender, differences between schools.
perceived acceptance and perceived norm. Model 2: Same as above, plus GLOR and NO HARM entered together.
Model 3: Same as model 2, plus GLOR and NO HARM entered together. *p50.001.
*p50.001.

Of the never-smokers, 19% indicated that their friends


in which positive effects and negative effects were both
would accept if they smoked, 47% indicated that their friends
entered alone, both were associated with all categories of use would not accept it and 33% were uncertain. The adjusted
(i.e. lifetime, monthly and weekly use). These associations did
associations from multinomial logistic regression are shown
not change substantially when perceived norms and perceived
in Table 5. Both positive effects and negative effects were
accept of cannabis use were controlled (model 2). Positive
associated with both being undecided and answering ‘‘yes’’ to
effects and negative effects both remained significantly
this item. When positive effects and negative effects were
associated with all categories of use. However, only positive
entered together, the only significant association was between
effects was associated with all categories of use, when both
positive effects and answering yes to the item that friends
positive and negative effects were entered into the regression
would accept it if they smoked.
model together, whereas negative effects was not associated
with lifetime use (model 3).
Discussion
Associations between cannabis culture variables and
The items of the CCQ loaded on two latent factors, which we
openness to try hash among never-users
have here labelled negative effects scale and positive effects
A total of 803 respondents had answered that they had never scale. These two factors represent correlated but distinct sets
used cannabis, and could be included for these analyses. Of of beliefs: the first set of beliefs concerns the harmful social
the never-smokers, 22% indicated that it was OK to smoke and physical consequences of cannabis use. The second set of
occasionally, 55% indicated that it was not OK and 23% were beliefs draws on the cannabis culture, which describes the
undecided. The adjusted associations from multinomial drug positively in terms of its effects on both the individual
logistic regression are shown in Table 4. Both the positive and society. Positive effects are associated with cannabis use,
effects scale and the negative effects scale were associated and also associated with acceptance of use among never-
with both being undecided and endorsing this item. When users.
positive effects and negative effects were entered together, all Both scales were associated with all categories of cannabis
remained significant, except that positive effects was no use, from having ever tried use to using cannabis more than
longer associated with being undecided. once per week. This held after controlling for two widely used
DOI: 10.3109/14659891.2013.790493 Cannabis culture 255

indicators of risk of substance use, perceived normative use The study had some limitations. First, it was derived from
and perceived acceptance of cannabis use. However, when one area in one country, and it is uncertain whether and how
both positive effects and negative effects were entered the results will generalize to other countries, especially those
together, positive effects came out as the variable that culturally different from the North European context in which
remained significantly associated with all categories of this study was carried out. In addition, we had limited
cannabis use. Additionally, both scales were associated with information about respondents’ socioeconomic status and
openness to use cannabis among never-users. nothing about their ethnic status. The catchment area for the
Cannabis is an important part of identity (Hammersley study was mainly small town and rural, and the area is
et al., 2001). The importance of positive beliefs for use has predominantly Danish, ethnically and culturally. As opposed
also been found in studies from Australia (Connor et al., to the CEQ, the CCQ also includes fewer questions and is thus
2011), Canada (Osborne & Fogel, 2008), France (Guillem, less sensitive. However, the study also had strengths. The
2011) and the UK (Hammersley & Leon, 2006). There is thus questionnaire was designed based on a four-year qualitative
reason to believe that regardless of culture positive beliefs are study. We covered a wide range of educational institutions,
important for cannabis users. This study adds to this picture, and the respondents were at an age where initiation of
but also adds a new aspect. Previous qualitative studies of cannabis use is likely to occur. In addition, we had an
cannabis in Scandinavia identify a relatively coherent set of acceptable response rate, and only a small number of
beliefs about the positive effects of cannabis. The beliefs questionnaires had to be excluded.
J Subst Use Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Oslo universitetssykehus Aker on 05/08/14

describe cannabis not only as ‘‘harmless fun’’ or ‘‘a good


feeling’’, but also as a valuable contribution to creativity, Conclusion
culture and thinking, and as a ‘‘natural product’’ and a
medicine with great potential. The findings in this study show Young people who use cannabis endorse a relatively coherent
that such beliefs are endorsed by cannabis users and set of beliefs about the positive effects of cannabis. These are
identifiable in young people undergoing education. To indicative of a cannabis culture, which seems to be as
understand the frequency of use, these beliefs are as important important for use as perceived risks and beliefs that cannabis
as whether cannabis use is perceived as harmful, immoral or is commonly used and accepted by schoolmates.
deviant. These findings have important implications for
research. First, it is important to understand how these Declaration of interest
For personal use only.

different sets of beliefs develop in relation to cannabis use. It This study was supported by the Danish Health and
is possible that beliefs about positive effects begin to develop Medicines Authority. Morten Hesse has received funding
prior to experimental use of cannabis and that such beliefs are from Reckitt Benckiser for an unrelated study.
important precursors of cannabis use. It is also feasible that
the endorsement of these beliefs develops concurrently with
References
the development of cannabis use and that they may, in
addition or only, be important for the maintenance of cannabis Beaudoin, C. E., & Hong, T. (2012). Media use and perceived risk as
predictors of marijuana use. American Journal of Health Behavior, 36,
use. Longitudinal research is needed to answer these 134–143.
questions. Booth, M. (2004). Cannabis: A History. London: Bentham Books.
A second question is how the beliefs are developed and Clarke, J., Hall, S., Jefferson, T., & Roberts, B. (2000). Subcultures,
endorsed in friendship groups. Designs that focus on social cultures and class. In S. Hall & T. Jefferson (Eds.), Resistance through
Rituals: Youth Subcultures in Post-War Britain (2nd ed.). Abingdon:
networks may inform our knowledge on the processes Routledge.
whereby such beliefs are transmitted and interpreted in Connor, J. P., Gullo, M. J., Feeney, G. F. X., & Young, R. M. (2011).
social groups. Additionally, research should further address Validation of the Cannabis Expectancy Questionnaire (CEQ) in adult
the relationship between cannabis culture and social identity, cannabis users in treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 115,
167–174.
including ethnic identity, social class, gender and social Dahl, H. V., & Frank, V. A. (2011). Medical marijuana – Exploring the
status. Such research could question the coherency of the concept in relation to small scale cannabis growers in Denmark. In T.
cannabis culture, that is, investigate if different elements of Decorte, G. Potter & M. Bouchard (Eds.), World Wide Weed: Global
the cannabis culture are more important than others depend- Trends in Cannabis Cultivation and Control (pp. 57–74). London:
Ashgate.
ing on the social identity of the user and the context of use. Degenhardt, L., & Hall, W. (2012). Extent of illicit drug use and
Previous cannabis prevention research has revealed the dependence, and their contribution to the global burden of disease.
importance of perceived individual and societal risks Lancet, 379, 55–70.
(Beaudoin & Hong, 2012). The findings in this study suggest Golub, A. (2005). The Cultural/Subcultural Contexts of Marihuana Use
at the Turn of the Twenty-First Century. New York: The Haworth
that prevention efforts that acknowledge positive expectancies Press.
and positive cultural beliefs associated with cannabis use may Guillem, E., Notides, C., Vorspan, F., Debray, M., Nieto, I., Leroux, M.,
be more effective than prevention strategies focusing solely Lépine, J.P. (2011). Cannabis expectancies in substance misusers:
French validation of the marijuana effect expectancy questionnaire.
on the risks of using cannabis. Moreover, because the beliefs
American Journal on Addictions, 20, 543–554.
of cannabis culture are interrelated, it is unlikely that Hamid, A. (2002). The Ganja Complex: Rastafari and Marijuana.
challenging individual statements about cannabis as Oxford: Lexington Books.
‘‘myths’’ will be effective. Instead, we suggest a dialogue Hammersley, R., Jenkins, R., & Reid, M. (2001). Cannabis use and social
identity. Addiction Research & Theory, 9, 133–150.
with young people acknowledging the complexity of the Hammersley, R., & Leon, V. (2006). Patterns of cannabis use and
issues related to perceived cannabis benefits and discussing positive and negative experiences of use amongst university students.
cannabis in terms of the whole picture. Addiction Research & Theory, 14, 189–205.
256 S. Holm et al. J Subst Use, 2014; 19(3): 251–256

Hesse, M., Tutenges, S., & Schliewe, S. (2010). The use of tobacco and The case of drug-taking. Social Science & Medicine, 63,
cannabis at an international music festival. European Addiction 675–679.
Research, 16, 208–212. Rossler, W., Hengartner, M. P., Angst, J., & Ajdacic-Gross, V.
Lim, M. S. C., Hellard, M. E., Hocking, J. S., Spelman, T. D., & Aitken, (2012). Linking substance use with symptoms of subclinical
C. K. (2010). Surveillance of drug use among young people attending psychosis in a community cohort over 30 years. Addiction, 107,
a music festival in Australia, 2005–2008. Drug and Alcohol Review, 1174–1184.
29, 150–156. Sandberg, S. (2012). Is cannabis use normalized, celebrated or
Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Ferrando, P. J. (2006). FACTOR: A computer neutralized? Analysing talk as action. Addiction Research & Theory,
program to fit the exploratory factor analysis model. Behavior 20, 372–381.
Research Methods, 38, 88–91. Sandberg, S. (2013). Cannabis culture. A stable subculture in a
Lorenzo-Seva, U., & ten Berge, J. M. F. (2006). Tucker’s congruence changing world. Criminology and Criminal Justice, 13, 63–80.
coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology: Schneider, M. (2008). Puberty as a highly vulnerable developmental
European Journal of Research Methods for the Behavioral and Social period for the consequences of cannabis exposure. Addiction Biology,
Sciences, 2, 57–64. 13, 253–263.
MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Preacher, K. J., & Hong, S. (2001). Stevenson, C. (2012). Cannabis use: What’s law got to do with it?
Sample size in factor analysis: The role of model error. Multivariate Perceptions and knowledge of cannabis policy from the user
Behavioral Research, 36, 611–637. perspective in Northern Ireland. Drugs: Education, Prevention and
Matthews, P. (2003). Cannabis Culture. London: Bloomsbury. Policy, 19, 129–136.
Osborne, G. B., & Fogel, C. (2008). Understanding the motivations for Timmerman, M. E., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality
recreational marijuana use among adult Canadians. Substance Use and assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis.
Misuse, 43, 539–572. Psychological Methods, 16, 209–220.
J Subst Use Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Oslo universitetssykehus Aker on 05/08/14

Pedersen, W. (2009). Cannabis use: Subcultural opposition or social mar- Young, J. (1971). The Drugtakers: The Social Meaning of Drug Use.
ginality? A population-based longitudinal study. Acta Sociologica, 52, London: MacGibbon and Kee.
135–148. Young, R. M., & Kavanagh, D. J. (1997). The Cannabis Expectancy
Pedersen, W., & Sandberg S. (2013). The medicalisation of revolt: Questionnaire (CEQ). Queensland, Australia: The University of
A sociological analysis of medical cannabis users. Sociology of Health Queensland.
& Illness, 35, 17–32. Zimmer, L., & Morgan, J. P. (1997). Marijuana Myths, Marijuana Facts:
Peretti-Watel, P., & Moatti, J. P. (2006). Understanding risk A Review of the Scientific Evidence. New York: The Lindesmith
behaviours: How the sociology of deviance may contribute? Center.
For personal use only.

View publication stats

You might also like