Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts © 2012 American Psychological Association

2012, Vol. 6, No. 3, 231–242 1931-3896/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027307

Creative Thinking in Music:


Its Nature and Assessment Through Musical Exploratory Behaviors

Baptiste Barbot Todd Lubart


Yale University Université Paris Descartes

Creative thinking in music has received only limited attention in the psychology of art and creativity, yet
it appears to be one of the most important issues in the field of music education. As creative thinking in
music exists in the general population and increasing evidence suggests the positive effects of active
engagement with music, it seems that research on this topic offers promising implications beyond musical
education itself. However, there is a lack of suitable measures of creative ability in music for individuals
without prior musical training, and many important facets, such as the original use of sound material, are
often disregarded in existing assessments. To fill these conceptual and empirical gaps—a prerequisite to
any research on musical creativity—we present a new multimethod framework for its evaluation: the
Musical Expression Test (MET). This method combines a systematic observational approach with a focus
on musical exploratory behaviors and a product-based assessment of musical pieces resulting from
musical activities, involving a sound-production set and a computer-based recording system. A study
with the MET on a sample of adolescents with and without musical training is presented and provides
the first empirical evidence of the MET’s reliability, convergence between behavioral and product-based
assessment, and suitability for individuals without particular musical skills. A typology of product-based
“creative styles” and their behavioral correlates is presented. The potential uses and implications of the
MET for the future of musical creativity assessment and research are discussed.

Keywords: creativity, music, divergent thinking, exploratory behavior, problem finding

The lives of eminent creative musicians, such as Bach, Mozart, ford, 1950) and (b) humans show “spontaneous” musical activity,
and Beethoven, have received attention for centuries. However, the including the perception and comprehension of music early in life
empirical study of creative thinking in music is relatively recent. (e.g., Bigand & Poulin-Charronnat, 2006; Kogan, 1994). The
Some literature exists in various fields, including psychology, social representation of musical creativity, long associated with
cognitive science, and artificial intelligence, but most of the re- eminence and giftedness (Adorno, 1976), together with conceptual
search on this topic has been conducted in the field of music debates on its definition and measurement, has delayed the sys-
education. Indeed, music education generally considers creativity tematic study of musical creativity in the general population.
as a fundamentally important dimension that should be promoted However, as new music technologies become increasingly pop-
and integrated in educational programs (e.g., Webster, 1990). ular, many people are now involved in amateur creative-musical
Consequently, research on musical creativity has mainly focused production (e.g., Mellor, 2007; Mellor, 2008; Partti & Karlsen,
on children and adults with musical training (as a target of music 2010; Ward, 2009). An increasing number of studies show the
education research). Nevertheless, musical creativity exists pre- positive effects of active engagement with music on intellectual,
sumably in the general population as (a) it is accepted that cre- social, and personal development (Hallam, 2010). Thus, the study
ativity is a psychological dimension normally distributed (Guil- of musical creativity—as a general psychological dimension—is a
contemporary research topic with important implications beyond
musical education concerns. Yet, a fundamental challenge to ad-
vance the research in musical creativity is its assessment. After
This article was published Online First February 27, 2012. surveying the state of the art on defining and measuring musical
Baptiste Barbot, Yale University, Child Study Center; Todd Lubart,
creativity, we present the Musical Expression Test (MET), a new
Université Paris Descartes, Institut de Psychologie, Laboratoire Adapta-
tion, Travail, Individu, Boulogne-Billancourt, France.
multimethod technique to evaluate creative thinking in music
This project would not have been possible without the help of a number through an innovative behavioral measurement of exploratory mu-
of people. First we thank the eight composers who took part in the sical activity and a classic product-based assessment method.
assessment of the musical pieces. We also thank Dimitri Darcam for his Finally, we present an empirical study using the MET with a
coding work, Alain Laflaquière and Florent Faugère for their guidance in sample of adolescents, to estimate the MET’s psychometric qual-
the development of an early version of the MET, Mei Tan and Vanessa ities, and confirm its suitability for the study of musical creativity
Manca for their editorial assistance. Finally, we are profoundly grateful to in the general population.
the participants involved in the MET and to the staff who facilitated the
implementation of the MET. Defining and Measuring Musical Creativity
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Baptiste
Barbot, Yale University, Child Study Center, 230 South Frontage Road The definition of musical creativity is a research topic in itself.
New Haven, CT 06520. E-mail: baptiste.barbot@yale.edu However, there is consensus on a general definition of creativity,

231
232 BARBOT AND LUBART

which applies to music and other domains of creative work. This which the responses can be classified. For instance, Webster’s
definition proposes that creativity is the ability to produce some- (1994) Measure of Creative Thinking in Music II (MCTM-II)
thing that is both new/original and valuable/appropriate to the task measures flexibility by categorizing the changes within three mu-
or the domain (e.g., Amabile, 1996; Barron, 1988; Sternberg & sical parameters: tempo (fast/slow), dynamics (loud/soft), and
Lubart, 1995). Whereas the originality component can be under- pitch (high/low). Finally, originality refers to the statistical infre-
stood in a straightforward manner, some explanations of “appro- quency of musical responses with regard to the corpus of responses
priateness” in the music domain have been proposed. As “music” observed in a reference sample (e.g., Gorder, 1980; Webster,
refers to organized sounds distinct from noise, most often “appro- 1994).
priateness” refers to the notion of “structure” (e.g., Auh, 2000), as Major issues with the measurement of musical divergent
a creative musical piece is not “random.” Similarly, appropriate- thinking include (a) the multidimensional nature of the con-
ness may refer to the notion of a “plan,” as originality without struct (i.e., there are many parameters leading to the production
intent or a plan does not necessarily make a creative product (e.g., of flexible and original musical ideas including, e.g., pitch,
Hickey & Webster, 2001). However, the notion of appropriateness tempo, dynamics, choice of sound materials, or the rhythmic
in a musical piece does not suggest that the production must be and melodic content) and (b) measures of flexibility and orig-
technically mastered or “musically correct.” Therefore, Webster, inality often involve a transcription of the responses using the
(1990, 1992) deplored that general aptitudes in music (e.g., rhyth- traditional music notation (usually based on audio or video
mic regularity) are often confounded with creative aptitude in recordings). This transcription requires accomplished musicians
music. Webster (1990) also recommended the study of “creative to capture the “imperfections” and parameters (e.g., dynamics,
thinking in music” rather than “musical creativity” to demystify tone) that the musical answer involves and which may represent
the concept while focusing on the creative process and its role in the “true originality” of the musical response. Finally, (c) these
music. Creative thinking in music is defined as a dynamic mental measures are developed by music educators for music students,
process that alternates between divergent and convergent thinking, often limiting their applicability to this specific, academically
moving in stages over time, enabled by internal musical skills and trained population.
external conditions, and resulting in a final musical product that is
new for the creator (Webster, 1990). The final musical product Compositional Creativity Assessment
might be a composition or an improvisation, but the creative
thinking process may also be involved in other musical activities Three main approaches are typically used to measure creative
such as musical performance (e.g., Clarke, 2005) or listening thinking in composing music: think-aloud or retrospective
(Dunn, 1997). semistructured interviews (e.g., Barrett, 1996; Bennett, 1976),
Along with these conceptual distinctions, there are many analyses of musical flow recorded at various stages of the
methodological and technical challenges related to the devel- compositional process (e.g., Folkestad, Hargreaves, & Lind-
opment of valid assessments of musical creativity (Webster, ström,1998; Hickey, 1997; Seddon & O’Neill, 2003), or sys-
1992). A common way to deal with these issues consists of tematic observation of subjects composing music with codifi-
proposing both a definition of creativity and a method for its cation of the musical activity (e.g., Kratus, 1989). The major
assessment (Pachet, 2006). Consequently, there are many in- issue with the study of the compositional process in music
struments, procedures, and methods that have been developed relates to the segmentation, definition, and identification of the
to assess musical creative processes, products, or persons. A processes, “stages,” or actions, which take place during the
systematic review of these tools is not the purpose here (for this creative process. For example, Kratus (1994) focused on com-
see, e.g., Hickey, 2002; Leung, Wan, & Lee, 2009; Nielsen, positional activity through the processes of exploration, devel-
2011; Richardson, 1983; Webster, 1992). Rather, we present an opment, repetition, and silence. Even though the creative pro-
overview of the main measurement approaches—including mu- cess conceptually varies based on the musical activity (e.g.,
sical divergent thinking tests, compositional creativity assess- composition, improvisation, performance), it is not clear
ment, and product-based assessment—to discuss the critical whether this difference can be perceived objectively (e.g., Le-
needs for future measures of musical creativity. hmann & Kopiez, 2010).

Measures of Musical Divergent Thinking Product-based Assessment


Musical divergent thinking tests measure the thinking process of Product-based assessment is a common way to evaluate the
generating many responses to a single musical stimulus or problem result of creative thinking in music. It generally involves the
(e.g., Baltzer, 1988; Gorder, 1980; Richardson, 1983; Webster, achievement of a musical piece (composition or improvisation)
1994), by means of musical fluency, flexibility, and originality based on initial task constraints. Completed in standardized con-
scores. Musical fluency corresponds to the “productivity” of the ditions, the resulting products are often scored using the Consen-
musical ideas in response to a given stimulus or problem (e.g., to sual Assessment Technique (CAT; Amabile, 1982, 1996). This
propose as many melodies as possible starting from three imposed technique involves domain-appropriate judges (e.g., music teach-
notes). It is typically measured through the duration of various ers, composers) who evaluate independently the musical products
improvisations or instrumental explorations (e.g., Webster, 1994) using a Likert scale on several criteria such as originality, musical
or by the number of musical events produced in response to a task structure, expressivity (e.g., Auh & Walker, 1999), and/or other
(e.g., Gorder, 1980). Musical flexibility refers to the variety of the “technical” criteria such as tonal and rhythmic coherence (e.g.,
musical content (e.g., Gorder, 1980) or the number of categories in Hickey, 2001; Kratus, 1994). The consensus between judges is
CREATIVE THINKING IN MUSIC 233

estimated statistically using correlational or Rasch measurement creative thinking in music that are closer to current musical expe-
approach (Leung et al., 2009) to ensure interrater agreement and riences. Such a measure would extend the study of musical cre-
the reliability of the composite scores derived for each musical ativity to the general population for a better understanding of
product. Typically, the agreement estimated in studies of musical musical creativity in relation to intellectual, social, and personal
creativity using the CAT is acceptable for various types of prod- development (Hallam, 2010).
ucts, criteria, and experimental conditions (e.g., Auh, 1997;
Hickey, 2001; Priest, 2006).
The Musical Expression Test
Perspectives for the Measurement of Creative
Thinking in Music Overview
Although many research efforts have concentrated on the de- As reviewed above, the assessment of creative thinking in music
velopment of broad and valid measures of creative thinking in faces many challenges. Through two pilot studies and the present
music (e.g., Webster’s MCTM-II described above), there is a need work, we attempted to address these issues by developing the
to assess musical creativity with alternative methods (Hickey, Musical Expression Test (MET) designed for children, adoles-
2002). In addition to their conceptual and methodological limits,
cents, and adults, with or without prior musical training (minimal
existing measures of musical creativity often involve tasks that
technical skills involved). The MET was developed as a multim-
require a minimum of domain-specific knowledge (i.e., basic mas-
ethod technique integrating the divergent-thinking measurement
tery of a musical instrument and/or of musical notation), which
approach (e.g.,Webster, 1994), creative process analysis in
limits their applicability to samples of individuals with no prior
computer-based composition tasks (e.g.,Folkestad et al., 1998;
music training. In contrast, popular creative thinking assessment
tools in other domains (e.g., Torrance, 1966) do not require such Hickey, 1997), and classic product-based assessment (e.g., Auh,
basic skills, resulting in imbalanced advances in creativity research 1997; Eisenberg & Thompson, 2003, 2011; Priest, 2006). Consis-
across domains. tent with established models of musical creativity (Webster, 1990),
In addition, important dimensions of musical creativity are often and a recent framework for the domain-specific assessment of
disregarded in the assessment literature. Particularly, the ability to potential for creativity (Lubart, Besançon, & Barbot, 2011), the
produce unusual sonorities based on the use of new materials or MET aims specifically to assess two sets of abilities: (a)
the unexpected use of familiar instruments and objects is an Divergent-exploratory thinking processes— expanding the range
important facet of musical creativity that could be even more of solutions in creative problem-solving and (b) convergent-
important than musicality (e.g., Leman, 1999). In fact, this ability integrative thinking processes— combining elements in new ways.
has historically led to major field innovation, as evidenced by These key-processes are measured through four standardized sub-
findings in music archaeology. Many examples of the first vestige tests (musical activities) including free exploration, alternative-
of human musical creativity, such as flutes and whistles, derived uses, composition, and improvisation. These subtests are chained
from objects of the environment (e.g., bones of birds) were dated and presented to subjects as distinct stages of an individual 1-hour-
as early as the early upper Palaeolithic age (e.g., d’Errico et al., long composition workshop.
2003). More recently, the unexpected use of a bottleneck with The general measurement principle of the MET involves two
a guitar fundamentally inspired Blues music. Radio experimen- levels of analysis. The “product-based” level focuses on the mu-
tations accidentally led to the development of the first synthe- sical piece produced in response to the composition and improvi-
sizers and electronic music, whereas current music bands using sation tasks, which involves the Consensual Assessment Tech-
familiar objects or even vegetables to make music are renowned nique (CAT). The “behavioral level” focuses on the exploratory
for their originality (e.g., Stomp, Vegetable Orchestra). Beyond behaviors enacted in response to the tasks and assesses divergent-
the cultural-evolutionary processes by which new instruments, exploratory processes through a systematic observation protocol.
musical genre, and subgenre might emerge (Graham, 2006),
Rather than relying on the musical contents of the test responses
these examples emphasize how the ability to generate new
(involving musical notation), this innovative approach focuses on
sonorities for the purpose of music seems to represent both an
the instrumental gestures—that is, the physical actions involved in
innate feature of human ingenuity and an essential aspect of
the production of sound (Widmer & Goebl, 2004)—which are
musical creativity.
hard-wired to the musical production. This focus on physicality—
On the other hand, as societal and technological progresse have
created a multitude of new ways for people to engage with music inspired by Getzels’ (1964) and Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi’s
(Partti & Karlsen, 2010), many promising research developments (1974) methods for the study of problem-finding behaviors in
support creativity in relation to music technology (e.g., Mellor, visual-art creativity—allows quantitative and qualitative analyses
2007, 2008). These technological advances are viewed as stimu- of the exploratory activity (e.g., number, diversity, and frequency
lators of creative possibilities, allowing the realization of ideas of musical events) enacted in response to the tasks, with a partic-
when motor skills lag behind ideas. Taken together, it appears that ular emphasis on the use of the material conditions, in an ecolog-
these “new” dimensions of creative work in music should be ical perspective (cf. Gibson, 1977). This allows capturing a rarely
captured through suitable assessment methods of creative thinking studied facet of creative thinking in music: The ability to produce
that (a) minimize the involvement of instrumental and musical unusual sonorities based on the new or unusual (i.e., infrequent)
mastery and (b) take into account the creative thinking engaged in use of objects (cf. Gilhooly, Fioratou, Anthony, & Wynn, 2007;
sound production and computer-based composition, two facets of Leman, 1999).
234 BARBOT AND LUBART

Materials iarity with each elements of the sound-production set (so that each
element is seen). After this introduction, the four subtests are
The MET involves a “sound-production set” including 12 ele- administered as follows:
ments,1 which are playful, easy to use, providing diverse physical
properties, potential uses, and sounds. This set represents three 1. Free exploration (3 minutes). Participants are invited to
categories, according to whether the element is mainly Melody- explore freely the sound-production set during 3 min-
oriented (e.g., Glockenspiel, Kalimba), Rhythm-oriented (e.g., utes. This stage focuses on spontaneous explorations,
Tambourine, Cymbal), or “Sound-Effect” oriented, including free of restrictions, and without the presence of the test
“sound objects” (very simple elements designed to produce sound- administrator who could inhibit the very first explor-
effects or atmospheres, such as the Rainstick or Frog-guiro), and atory behaviors.
“heterogeneous objects” whose “natural” use is not to produce
music or sounds (e.g., spoons, a plastic tube). Obviously, even a 2. Mini-Games (10 –15 minutes). The test administrator
melody-oriented element can be used for other functions, such as presents a musical composition recorded with the se-
for sound-effects or rhythmic sequences. Other equipment in- quencer software, involving six elements of the sound-
volved in the MET includes a computer-based audio-numeric production set (recorded in six separate, layered-tracks).
sequencer software and virtual studio (Cubasis VST, Steinberg), While familiarizing the participant with the possibilities
along with an external sound-card and VST controller, a micro- of the sequencer further used to create his or her own
phone, a headset, and a hi-fi system, which together allow multi- composition, this stage leads to two distinct tasks: (a)
track recording and advanced orchestration possibilities. Finally, a identifying the element used for each recorded layer
video camcorder is used for the systematic observation procedure. (each layer is played independently), after which the
The placement of the material is standardized, presented in an participant has to (b) propose as many alternative uses of
identical format for each subject (to control for possible original the identified elements, to produce diverse sounds (2
combinations between elements suggested by their proximity in minutes for each item). This alternating movement be-
placement). tween both tasks is repeated for the six targeted elements
(i.e., there are six items). There is no measurement in the
The MET Procedure first identification task, whereas the second—inspired from
classic “alternative uses” tasks (e.g., Guilford, 1967)—
The MET is divided into four complementary subtests (stages)
aims to measure the ability to generate potential uses for
structured as a workshop introducing composition. This structure
the purpose of sound production. Resulting scores reflect
and the MET subtests’ content (i.e., instructions, material) were
individual differences in this ability, in terms of quantity
selected and calibrated for duration and other parameters, on the
(fluency), diversity (flexibility), and statistical infrequency
basis of our preliminary work. Figure 1 represents the organization
(originality) of the generated answers with regard to those
of the MET procedure.
proposed in the tested population.
As indicated, an initial presentation stage establishes the contact
between the test administrator and the subject (there is no mea- 3. Composition (30 minutes). In this stage, the subject
surement involved at this point). The test administrator presents creates a 30-s musical piece using the sequencer pre-
briefly the MET and asks the subject to indicate his or her famil-
sented through the introductory and the minigames
stages. Specifically, the participants have the possibility
to record, step-by-step, each layer (musical segments) of
their composition and then to overlay the recordings in
an integrated musical piece. Participants are free to use
the sound-production set in any way, and to record,
erase, and edit (delete, copy, and paste sections) musical
sequences. However, the protocol allows a maximum of
eight layers to be included in the final composition. The
test administrator handles the recordings and the soft-
ware to save time and facilitate the process. This subtest
ends when the subject considers the composition com-
plete, within a maximum of 30 minutes.

4. Improvisation (5 minutes). This final stage consists of


recording a 1-min improvisation after a 2-min warm-up/

1
The pilot studies included a wider range of elements to select for the
final sound-production set, (a) elements associated with various degrees of
use and “attractiveness” (estimated through the frequencies of exploratory
behavior toward the elements) and (b) an equivalent number of elements in
each category of main “use” (determined through the analysis of the use
Figure 1. The MET procedure. typically proposed with each element).
CREATIVE THINKING IN MUSIC 235

preparation-time before recording. This subtest is op- MET subtests (free exploration, answers of the “alternative use”
tional and is administered according to the goals (e.g., task, during the compositional activity, and during the improvisa-
for research needs or to monitor progress in tion stage). Finally, behavioral measures are also computed upon
improvisation-based training) and the targeted popula- the exploration units used in the actual musical piece (composition
tion of the test administration. Experience shows that, stage) to refer to the number, the variety, and the originality of the
even placed as the final stage, improvising without par- exploration units recorded and kept in the final musical piece.2
ticular guidelines or constraints can be somewhat diffi-
cult for novices or subjects with low self-confidence.
An Empirical Study With the MET
General Coding and Scoring Principles The goal of this study was to examine empirically the MET and
confirm its appropriateness for administration in the general pop-
Beyond the CAT involved for judgments of the musical prod-
ulation, using a sample of adolescents unfamiliar with composi-
ucts resulting from the composition and improvisation subtests, the
tion. Despite the apparent face validity of the MET as a measure
MET provides behavioral measures of musical explorations ob-
of targeted aspects of creative thinking in music, we sought to
served across the subtests. These measures are derived from a
estimate important properties of the MET’s coding protocol and
systematic observation protocol developed upon the transcription
resulting test scores with a special focus on (a) evidence for
of 30 observations of the pilot version of the MET (using Ad
reliability (including interrater reliability of the exploratory units
libitum sampling) which led to the identification and the definition
coding system and resulting behavioral scores, internal consistency
of inclusion and exclusion parameters for the coding of the ex-
of the divergent-exploratory behavioral scores computed from the
ploratory behaviors. As a result, observation units are defined with
six “Alternative uses” items, and interrater agreement of the
regard to three criteria: (a) Behavioral orientation (element ex-
product-based assessment obtained through the CAT); (b) possible
plored); (b) Behavioral structure (form of the exploratory behav-
gender and musical training effects on the test scores; and (c)
ior); and (c) Function (consequence of the behavior). These criteria
exploration of the convergence between the behavioral measures
are then combined to obtain the observation units (called “explor-
and the product-based assessments (including the examination of
atory units”) referring to distinct musical explorations, which are
behavioral differences associated with typical “creative styles”
tagged and quantifiable.
identified and derived upon the product-based measures). The
Specifically, the orientation criterion is coded using a number
optional improvisation subtest of the MET and the compositional
referring to the element of the sound-production set used (addi-
process data were not analyzed in this study.
tional codes are proposed to transcribe orientations not listed in the
sound-production set such as different parts of the body, table,
keys, and other external sound-sources). The structure criterion is Method
defined according to a catalogue of typical instrumental gestures
identified in the pilot studies and in Mialaret’s catalogue (Mialaret, Participants. Forty-two adolescents (29 girls, 13 boys) in the
1997), which we summarized according to gestures’ morphologi- first year of high school (Mage ⫽ 15.6 years, SD ⫽ 0.6) partici-
cal similarities. Each behavior falls in one of 11 categories (e.g., pated in the MET. Forty-five percent of the participants had prior
hits, rubs, blows, glissandi, and sweeps) coded with one or two musical experience (12 girls, 7 boys), but none of them had prior
letters. Finally, the function is based on a very simplified but experiences of musical composition. Two independent coders (in-
sufficiently exclusive taxonomy which includes three possible cluding the first author and a music teacher) were involved to
consequences of the exploratory behavior: Melody-oriented (unit transcribe the video corpus after the MET systematic observation
including sounds with locatable pitch variation), Rhythm-oriented protocol. Finally, eight experts in music (one woman and seven
(unit with no pitch variations, and the presence of rhythm), and men), consisting of four professional composers including teachers
Sound-Effect oriented (emphasize sound effect, noise, ambiance, of composition, and four amateur musicians with more than 10
and atmosphere sounds). Additional criteria, such as operators and years of musical experience, participated as judges for the CAT.
play-zone locators, are available to reflect complex exploratory Measures and procedure. The MET was implemented in an
behaviors and to set the limits of the diverse exploratory units. afterschool program taking place in the high school where the
Using this transcription system, an exploratory unit such as participants were recruited. The program was advertised as a
“1aHM” would represent an exploratory behavior in which the composition workshop for research purposes (no reference to
subject uses the glockenspiel (orientation criterion coded “1”) by musical creativity as a target of the study was made). Consent
hitting (structure criterion coded “H”) with its mallets (comple- forms authorizing for the audio-recording and video-filming of the
mentary orientation criterion coded “a”), producing a series of session were signed by the participants and their parents. Follow-
sounds with locatable pitch (function criterion coded “M” for ing the MET’s procedure, we administered the MET in individual
“Melody-oriented”). sessions not exceeding one hour. Musical pieces resulting from the
The exploration units are then used as the basis of the compu-
tation of the MET’ behavioral scores, to quantify the exploratory 2
Even though we are not presenting results regarding the compositional
activity enacted in response to the various tasks: fluency (number process here, it is worth noting that the MET scoring protocol includes an
of exploratory units), flexibility (number of different exploratory extensive guideline for the coding of “Compositional units” (to transcribe
units), and originality (relative infrequency of each exploration the activity during the composition stage, such as recording, editing,
unit with respect to the units indexed in the experimental popula- erasing, listening) for further study on the creative process. The full coding
tion for each subtest). These variables are computed for each of the and scoring protocol is available upon request to the corresponding author.
236 BARBOT AND LUBART

MET tasks (composition and improvisation) were exported and Results


given to the participants on an audio CD.
After the test administration period, the MET video recordings Reliability analyses.
were transcribed by the two coders. After a training program based Behavioral coding and scoring reliability. The percentage of
on the pilot studies corpus, each coder handled independently half agreement and interrater correlation coefficients observed for the
of the video recordings. In addition, a double-coding procedure coding of the various MET subtests are presented in Table 1. On
was completed for 25% of the recordings leading to the extraction the qualitative level, the double-coding procedure showed a satis-
of about 1,300 observation units transcribed by both coders. This factory percentage of agreement, with a median of 86% of iden-
overlapping corpus was used as a basis of the interrater reliability tical observation units coded by the two raters. On the quantitative
analyses. The CAT was used to assess several criteria on the 42 level, intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) between the score
musical pieces resulting from the MET composition subtest. The derived independently from each rater, showed a high interrater
eight judges evaluated independently the compositions through a agreement (median ICC ⫽ .92, p ⬍ .001). These results are
consistent across the MET subtests and variables (i.e., fluency,
pilot version of the Consensual Assessment Technique-interface
flexibility, and originality) and suggest the excellent reliability of
(CAT-i; Barbot & Pouyade, 2006 –2011), an Internet-based
the data collected in this study, as well as the general quality and
system to facilitate the application of the CAT and its major
suitability of the MET’s coding and scoring protocols.
methodological guidelines (e.g., Hennessey, 1994), such as the
Internal consistency of the alternative-uses’ scores. Internal
randomization of the order of creative products for each judge,
consistency of the alternative-uses scores (that is, composite scores
while permitting an instantaneous, remote data-collection from
of fluency, flexibility, and originality, computed on the basis of the
geographically distant judges. The compositions were assessed
six items) was very high, with Cronbach’s alphas of .94, .94, and
using a seven-point Likert scale in three independent series for
.96, respectively for fluency (average interitem r ⫽ .73), flexibility
three criteria defined through the interface—these definitions (average interitem r ⫽ .73), and originality (average interitem r ⫽
were based on Auh (1997), and Hickey (1999)’s rubrics: (a) .81). These high reliability coefficients support the calculation of
Creativity (degree of both originality and coherence of the the composite scores and confirm that the six alternative-uses
composition); (b) Technical Quality (degree to which the tonal items involve the same ability.
and rhythmic elements in a composition show technical mastery Product-based assessment reliability. Interjudge agreement
in terms of tonal center and rhythmic regularity); and (c) for the three product-based criteria estimated with Cronbach’s
Expressiveness (degree to which the composition is musically alphas indicated an acceptable consensus for Creativity (.70),
expressive and reflects the participant’s aesthetic sensitivity to excellent consensus for Technical quality (.89), and limited agree-
music). ment for Expressivity (.64). After computing the composite
Data analyses. The interrater reliability analyses conducted product-based scores by averaging the individual judge’s ratings
on the observation corpus involved two levels. On a “qualitative” for each criterion, we examined their intercorrelation. Creativity
level, we compared the exploratory units coded by the raters and was relatively independent from technical quality (r ⫽ .38, p ⬍
we computed the percentage of agreement (exact same unit coded .05) but strongly related to expressivity (r ⫽ .77, p ⬍ .001).
by both raters), disagreement (variation in the coding of a unit Expressivity was also moderately related to technical quality (r ⫽
across raters), and omissions (when one rater coded a unit while .58, p ⬍ .001).
the other did not).3 On a “quantitative” level (MET’s behavioral Convergence between behavioral and product-based mea-
scores resulting from the coding of the observation units), we sures.
calculated intraclass correlation coefficients (Shrout & Fleiss, Correlational approach. Table 2 presents the intercorrela-
1979) between the scores computed separately from the coding of tion4 between the behavioral scores obtained at each MET subtest
the two independent raters. The internal consistency of the fluency, and the experts’ evaluations of the musical piece resulting from the
flexibility, and originality scores derived from the six items of composition subtest. Moderate correlations (ranging between rs ⫽
“alternative use” task (Minigames subtest) was estimated by com- .34 and .55) were observed between the behavioral measures of the
puting Cronbach’s alpha, followed by the computation of compos- free exploration subtest and the Creativity and Expressivity of the
ite scores (reflecting the performance across the 6 items). Simi-
larly, interjudge correlations of the product-based measures and 3
We used an improved version of the Miles and Huberman (1994)
resulting Cronbach’s alpha were computed using the ratings of the formula to treat errors attributable to coding omissions, to calculate the
eight independent judges, before deriving the consensual scores of percentages of agreement (PA) between the two coders. This formula is:
Creativity, Technical quality, and Expressivity. PA ⫽ NA/[NA ⫹ ND⫹(NO1 ⫹ NO2/2)]ⴱ100 (where PA ⫽ percentage of
Gender and musical training effects on the test scores were then agreements; NA ⫽ number of agreements; ND ⫽ number of dissensions;
examined using multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs). NO1 ⫽ number of omissions for observer 1; NO2 ⫽ number of omissions
Finally, we used a correlational approach to examine the conver- for observer 2). We did not correct PA for chance using Cohen’s Kappa
(based on the probability of occurrence of the observation units) because
gence between behavioral and product-based measures, and we
we assume that PA reflects only a minimal, even null influence of chance,
sought to identify “creative styles” (typical profiles observed with as there exists conceptually an infinite number of exploratory units.
the product-based consensual scores) using hierarchical ascendant 4
Whereas behavioral variables were approximately normally distrib-
classification (Ward’s method) followed by MANOVAs to exam- uted, we used Spearman rank-order correlation because of the small num-
ine which behavioral measure would differentiate the identified ber of participants in this study. Nevertheless, analyses using Bravais–
groups. Pearson correlation coefficients yielded a highly similar pattern of results.
CREATIVE THINKING IN MUSIC 237

Table 1 descriptive statistics of the four groups (with results of the univar-
Percentage of Agreement and Inter-observer Correlation iate analyses), and Figure 2 displays the average profiles of these
Coefficients groups.
A MANOVA followed by Tukey’s HSD post hoc tests indicated
Stage the extent to which the four groups were significantly different on
Free
exploration Games Composition Median
the three product-based scores. Based on the most salient aspects
of the four identified groups, we labeled them as follows: The
Qualitative level gifted group (20% of the sample) showed significantly higher
Percentage of agreement 85.0% 86.0% 87.5% 86.0% scores than the others on the three product-based scores; the
Quantitative level†
creative playful group (41%) corresponds to compositions consid-
Fluency .93 .87 .98 .93
Flexibility .94 .95 .98 .96 ered to be fairly creative and expressive but low on Technical
Originality .94 .89 .91 .91 quality; the academic group (24%) reflected compositions with
average levels of Expressivity and Technical quality but weak

Intra-class correlation coefficients significant at p ⬍ .001. creativity scores; finally, the disorganized group (15%) was asso-
ciated with low Creativity, Technical quality, and Expressivity
scores, suggesting a lack of coherence in the musical contents. The
composition (product-based scores). The Technical quality of the MANOVA did not indicate any interaction effects between these
composition was not related to the behavioral scores obtained in creative-styles and the other group-factors taken into account in
this subtest. this study (i.e., gender and prior musical training).
Interestingly, the behavioral measures obtained in the subse- Finally, a MANOVA on the behavioral measures using the
quent subtests appeared to be rather independent from the product- creative-style group as an independent variable revealed a signif-
based measures. Only the number of proposed alternative uses icant effect, F(36, 81) ⫽ 1.68, p ⫽ .03, ␩ ⫽ .43. Post hoc tests
(fluency) in the minigame subtest was weakly correlated with the indicated that the gifted group (associated with the highest
technical quality (rs ⫽ .38, p ⬍ .05) and expressivity (rs ⫽ .37, product-based scores) differed from the disorganized group (re-
p ⬍ .05). Finally, the results indicated a moderate convergence garded as less creative) by a greater quantity (p ⫽ .03) and variety
between the behavioral measures extracted from the musical piece (p ⫽ .02) of instrumental explorations (i.e., exploratory units)
corpus and the product-based measures. Fluency, flexibility, and during the free exploration stage. The creative playful group (mod-
originality of the explorations units incorporated in the musical erate Creativity associated with limited Technical quality) com-
piece were positively correlated with the Creativity of the musical pared with the academic group (average Technical quality and
piece (with correlation coefficients ranging from rs ⫽ .34 to rs ⫽ limited Creativity) showed higher scores of fluency (p ⫽ .02) and
.41), whereas the same variables were negatively correlated with originality (p ⬍ .01) in the behavioral measures extracted from the
the Technical quality of the musical piece (rs ⫽ ⫺.39 to rs ⫽ musical piece. In other words, the participants in the creative
⫺.48), as assessed by the experts. playful group tended to use more exploratory events and to gen-
Training and gender effects. A set of analyses was conducted erate less conventional uses of the sound-production set in their
to investigate possible effects of prior musical training and gender
on the behavioral and product-based measures. Gender was not
associated with individual differences on the three product-based Table 2
scores, F(3, 36) ⫽ 2.6, p ⫽ .18, whereas a slight effect of the prior Spearman Rank–Order Correlation Between the Behavioral and
musical training was obtained, F(3, 36) ⫽ 3.34, p ⫽ .02, ␩ ⫽ .22. the Product-based Measures
Specifically, Tukey’s HSD post hoc tests indicated that partici-
pants with prior musical training scored significantly higher than Product-based measures
participants with no prior training on the Technical quality mea-
sure (p ⬍ .05). No significant differences on Creativity and Technical
Behavioral measures Creativity quality Expressivity
Expressivity measures were observed, and there were no interac-
tions between gender and musical training. Analyses performed on Free exploration
the behavioral measures did not indicate main effects of gender or Fluency .51ⴱⴱⴱ .13 .54ⴱⴱⴱ
prior musical training except for the flexibility scores in the free Flexibility .55ⴱⴱⴱ .11 .52ⴱⴱⴱ
Originality .34ⴱ .10 .38ⴱ
exploration and composition subtests in which participants with Games (alternative uses)
prior musical training tended to score lower than participants with Fluency .22 .38ⴱ .37ⴱ
no prior training (p ⬍ .05). No other effects on the behavioral Flexibility .27 .28 .30
measures were observed. Originality .30 .21 .28
“Creative style” of the musical pieces and their behavioral Composition
Fluency .00 .30 .12
correlates. The Hierarchical Ascendant Classification was used Flexibility ⫺.03 .17 .04
to identify groups of individuals with similar profiles (interpreted Originality .00 .14 .09
as “creative styles”) based on their musical piece’s scores of Musical Piece
Creativity, Technical quality, and Expressivity. This analysis al- Fluency .34ⴱ ⫺.47ⴱⴱ .08
Flexibility .41ⴱⴱ ⫺.48ⴱⴱ .09
lowed the identification of four homogeneous groups of individ- Originality .40ⴱⴱ ⫺.39ⴱ .19
uals, summarizing common profiles in our data with high parsi-
mony (F[9, 87.8] ⫽ 22.9, p ⬍ .001, ␩ ⫽ .70). Table 3 presents the ⴱ
p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001.
238 BARBOT AND LUBART

Table 3
Descriptive Statistic of the Four Creative Styles on the Product-based Measures and Result of the Univariate Analyses

Gifted Creative playful Academic Disorganized

Creative style M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F(3, 39) p ␩2

Creativity 1.24 (.55) .22 (.58) ⫺.73 (.78) ⫺1.13 (.10) 26, 5 ⬍.001 .67
Technical quality 1.22 (.82) ⫺.76 (.48) .57 (.52) ⫺.70 (.30) 31, 3 ⬍.001 .71
Expressivity 1.60 (.49) ⫺.26 (.52) ⫺.30 (.53) ⫺1.13 (.21) 46, 1 ⬍.001 .78

musical piece. Behavioral measures obtained during the minigame to robust and reliable scores. Similarly, we observed high internal
subtest and the composition subtest did not significantly differen- consistency of the alternative-uses scores, confirming that the
tiate the four creative styles. items in this subtest measure the same ability to propose numerous,
various, and original uses of the sound material. Finally, the CAT
Discussion used for the assessment of several qualities of the musical piece,
resulting from the composition subtest, yielded acceptable inter-
This first empirical evaluation of the MET had several objec-
judge agreement between eight experts. As a whole, this evidence
tives. First, we sought evidence of various aspects of the MET’s
highlights the strengths of the MET coding and scoring principles
reliability. Second, we wanted to examine the effects of prior
and the overall quality of the data obtained in the study.
music training on the MET scores and gauge MET’s suitability for
applications in the general population. Finally, our third goal was As the levels of expertise of the judges might interfere with
to explore the relationships between the MET’s behavioral indi- creativity ratings and previous research has shown that composers
cators and product-based assessment through a correlational ap- tend to score musical composition in an idiosyncratic way as
proach and a complementary examination of “creative styles” and compared with other groups of experts (Hickey, 2001), further
their behavioral correlates. Our results are discussed separately in analyses regarding product-based assessment are needed. This
line with these objectives, and some limitations of this study and focus is important as the product-based scores are used as the
directions for future work are presented. principal criterion to evaluate the behavioral indicators and as the
Evidence for reliability. The results showed high interrater basis of the four creative styles in our data. Would similar product-
agreement between two independent coders and the resulting based scores be generated by different groups of experts (e.g.,
scores derived from the observation corpus. This suggests that the music teachers)? To what extent did the levels of expertise of the
coding and scoring protocols used in the MET are suitable and lead judges involved in this study influence their judgment of the

Figure 2. Average creative style profiles.


CREATIVE THINKING IN MUSIC 239

musical compositions? Future work focusing on the judgment of correspond to a “problem-finding” period for the composition
the creative products resulting from the MET could help to answer stage, in which participants would “draft” their musical piece. This
these questions and to better target (a) the appropriate group of hypothesis is consistent with the notion that a creative musical
experts to judge the creativity of the musical compositions, as well piece must have intention or a plan (e.g., Hickey & Webster,
as (b) the optimal criteria to be used for the assessment of the 2001). Alternatively, it is possible that the quantity of the explor-
musical compositions (e.g., melodic originality or rhythmic coher- atory behaviors observed during this free exploration stage reflects
ence). intrinsic motivation toward exploring the material conditions,
Musical training and technical mastery effects. Results which contributes to musical creativity. Similarly, it is possible
indicated that product-based Creativity scores were somewhat that score differences among participants in this subtest may
independent of Technical quality scores, which corroborates the reflect how familiar and comfortable they are with the various
results of several studies (e.g., Auh, 1997; Webster, 1987) under- aspects of the MET settings, which could ultimately intervene in
lining how “general aptitudes” in music should not be confounded the compositional process and the creativity of the resulting mu-
with the ability to think creatively in music (e.g., Webster, 1990, sical piece. Future research to address this possibility is needed,
1992). Accordingly, prior musical training was associated with examining further the extent to which motivation and familiarity
relatively higher scores of Technical quality on the musical piece interacts with the MET and is empirically related to problem-
but no effects on the Creativity scores. This confirms that “musi- finding behaviors in creative problem-solving in music.
cally correct” compositions (i.e., technically mastered) are not Interestingly, the behavioral scores obtained through the com-
necessarily more creative (e.g., Auh & Walker, 1999), as also position subtest and the alternative-uses task appeared relatively
illustrated by the academic and the creative playful styles. independent of the product-based creativity score. These results
On the behavioral level, participants without prior musical train- indicate the need to examine the links between the composition
ing did not show more explorations (fluency) than those with prior subtest scores and the product-based scores in light of “process
training contrary to the Seddon and O’Neill’s (2003) study. How- data” (e.g., is a significant amount of time spent on developing a
ever, they showed more flexible uses of the sound-production set musical idea, or repeating a musical segment?). The results also
than the latter during the compositional process. This tendency to suggest that the alternative-uses scores reflect presumably a facet
focus on limited alternative use of the sound-production elements of musical creativity that is not captured in the product-based
may reflect a preference for “in-depth” musical explorations measures, or which is not involved in compositional creativity. To
and/or the repetition of musical segments among the participants this regard, it is relevant to include this alternative-uses task in the
with musical training, rather than a focus on “in-breadth” musical MET so that it covers a wider sample of creative behaviors in
explorations (associated with a greater diversity of instrumental music. However, future examination of this task is also needed to
explorations). Indeed, relatively lower flexibility (as a behavioral establish its external validity with a relevant criterion such as a
indicator of the diversity of the exploratory activity) might indicate classic “alternative-uses” task in the verbal domain (e.g., Guilford,
a higher focus on a given element of the sound-production set, 1967). This would allow us to gauge whether this facet of musical
which may ultimately result in a better technical “mastery” of the creativity represents a more general divergent-exploratory ability
given element. This is supported by the negative correlations that consists of generating many, varied, and original uses of
between the behavioral measures extracted from the musical com- objects (Gilhooly et al., 2007), also involved in other domains of
position and the Technical quality scores. This result helps clarify creative work.
why the higher Technical quality scores of the group with prior Finally, the convergence between product-based measures and
musical training are also associated with lower flexibility during behavioral scores extracted from the actual musical composition
the composition task. As a whole, these results stress the need for provided some evidence of the importance of musical exploratory
further analyses using process data with a specific focus on “rep- behaviors for creativity. Specifically, our results suggested that
etition” (e.g., Kratus, 1989). This could contribute to understand- “musical completeness” could be an important factor for compo-
ing some of the overlaps between specific behavioral- and product- sitional creativity as a high number of exploratory events in the
based measures in the MET, which may be explained through the musical content was associated with higher creativity scores. Fur-
mediating effect of prior musical training. However, the patterns of thermore, the use of unusual sounds (reflected by rare exploratory
results obtained in this study, along with anecdotal observations units, that is, high originality scores) was related to the creativity
during the administration phase of the MET, confirmed its suit- of the musical piece, confirming the importance of this aspect of
ability for use in the general population. This supplements recent creative thinking in music (cf. Leman, 1999). Conversely, it ap-
efforts to study musical creativity in this population using increas- peared that these behavioral indicators—number and originality of
ingly popular computer-based compositional approaches (e.g., the exploratory events—were negatively related to the Technical
Mellor, 2007). quality of the musical piece. It seems consistent that the integration
Links between MET’s behavioral indicators and product- of a large variety of sound elements in the musical piece will
based assessments. Consistent with results described in the increase the difficulty to achieve coherence between these ele-
problem-finding literature in music (e.g., Brinkman, 1999), the ments and to ensure technical control. As suggested by the gifted
quantity, variety, and originality of the explorations in the free style (reflecting high levels in all product-based scores), it appears
exploration subtest were related to the creativity of the musical that both musical completeness and originality of the sound ele-
piece produced in a later stage of the MET (that is, the composition ments used in the musical piece must be associated with a general
subtest). Indeed, the convergence observed between the free ex- technical mastery to result in a highly creative product.
ploration scores and the product-based measures (up to 30% of Study limitations and future directions. The study pre-
shared variance) suggests that the free exploration subtest could sented here provided the first evidence for the robustness of the
240 BARBOT AND LUBART

MET to measure various aspects of creative thinking in music sical creativity as a facet of human creative potential that can be
through a sound and reliable coding and scoring system. This nurtured and studied in the “general population” represents an
standardized, multimethod framework proved to be applicable to advance for creativity research and its applications. Although such
both musically trained and untrained participants, which suggests a premise is impeded by many challenges related to the musical
great potential for future research on musical creativity. However, domain, researchers must “compose” with these issues and pursue
some limitations of this study have been pointed out, and some efforts to improve our knowledge on this topic, which requires
others must be noted here and addressed in future research on better assessment methods for musical creativity, as a basic con-
musical creativity with the MET. dition.
A first point concerns the limited sample size and the focus on Toward this effort, we presented the MET, a new multimethod
specific goals in this study; we must recognize that the data assessment of creative thinking in music with a focus on instru-
obtained are not sufficient to examine completely the psychomet- mental exploratory behaviors. Although the sample size in this
ric properties of the MET, although our first results are promising. study was restricted, encouraging results regarding reliability and
Future work involving larger samples is needed to establish other convergence between the behavioral measures and classic product-
psychometric properties such as test–retest reliability, external and based assessment were obtained. Built upon increasingly popular
internal validity, as well as for the development of proper norms computer-based compositional approaches (e.g., Mellor, 2007), the
for the application of the MET in research/educational settings. MET proved to be suitable to be administered to (and by) indi-
The focus on external validity is crucial, in particular for the viduals without any prior musical training. Future research efforts
product-based assessment of the musical piece resulting from the are needed to explore in depth the MET’s psychometrics properties
composition subtest. Indeed, by simulating creative work in music using larger samples, and including analyses of the criterion va-
composition with specific task demands and constraints (e.g., lidity— even though the lack of any validated measure of musical
demands placed on the MET’s test administrator, material condi- creativity might limit this investigation. Beyond the MET’s appar-
tions, time limits), we may be restricting the creative ability ent soundness and face validity, such research efforts are essential
measured to this specific task. This is in fact a common problem to determine whether the MET constitutes, at this point, a serious
of any product-based/integrative task of creative thinking. Accord- basis for future research on musical creativity.
ing to confluence models, a person’s profile across several com-
ponents involved in creative work may fit more or less closely the
References
requirements of a given task, and this leads to variations in the
level of creative performance across domains and also across tasks Adorno, T. (1976). Introduction to the sociology of music. New York, NY:
within domains (Lubart & Guignard, 2004). Future research must Seabury Press.
explore the extent to which the specific requirement of the subtests Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual
in the MET capture the key features involved in the broader ability assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43,
to think creatively in music (if such a general ability exists). This 997–1013. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.43.5.997
would help to determine whether some features of the MET tasks Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Auh, M., & Walker, R. (1999). Compositional strategies and musical
are undesirable or should be adjusted.
creativity when composing with staff notations versus graphic notations
In another line, the identification of the four “creative styles” in among Korean students. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music
our data provided the first evidence that different behavioral ap- Education, 141, 2–9.
proaches with the MET’s sound-production set were associated Auh, M. (1997). Prediction of musical creativity in composition among
with different qualities in the musical content (e.g., the creative selected variables for upper elementary students. Bulletin of the Council
playful, vs. the academic styles). This typology appears useful to for Research in Music Education, 133, 1– 8.
describe, albeit broadly, various approaches in musical composi- Auh, M. (2000). Assessing creativity in composing music: Product-
tion, but more replication work is needed to establish typological process-person-environment approaches. Proceedings of the 2000 Na-
stability (can we identify the same “styles” with other samples?) tional Conference of the Australian Association for Research in Educa-
and to set the parameters of a robust classification in these groups. tion. Retrieved from http://www.aare.edu.au/00pap/auh00016.htm
Baltzer, S. (1988). A validation study of a measure of musical creativity.
Finally, we must acknowledge that the sophistication of the coding
Journal of Research in Music Education, 36, 232–249. doi:10.2307/
and scoring system in the MET may reduce its applicability. 3344876
Present work is ongoing, in view of a simplification of the coding Barbot, B., & Pouyade, H. (2006 –2011). Consensual Assessment Tech-
and scoring system, without substantial loss of information. nique - interface. Retrieved from www.cat-i.org
Barrett, M. (1996). Children’s aesthetic decision-making: An analysis of
children’s musical discourse as composers. International Journal of
Conclusion
Music Education, 28, 37– 62. doi:10.1177/025576149602800104
To date, the majority of the research on creative thinking in Barron, F. (1988). Putting creativity to work. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The
music has targeted well-adjusted children with musical training, nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 76 –
limiting the theoretical advances and possible practical implica- 98). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Bennett, S. (1976). The process of musical creation: Interviews with eight
tions of this relevant topic to music education concerns. However,
composers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 24, 3–13. doi:
as the positive effect of the musical activity is pointed out beyond 10.2307/3345061
its role in the music-classroom (e.g., Hallam, 2010), musical Bigand, E., & Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2006). Are we experienced listeners?
creativity appears to be a relevant trigger of change in up-to-date A review of the musical capacities that do not depend on formal musical
therapeutic and educational programs, in phase with the current training. Cognition, 100, 100 –130. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.007
musical experiences of today’s youth. Consequently, treating mu- Brinkman, D. (1999). Problem finding, creativity style and the musical
CREATIVE THINKING IN MUSIC 241

compositions of high school students. The Journal of Creative Behavior, Kogan, N. (1994). On aesthetics and its origins: Some psychobiological
33, 62– 68. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.1999.tb01038.x and evolutionary considerations. Social Research, 61, 139 –165.
Clarke, E. F. (2005). Creativity in performance. Musicae Scientiae, 9, Kratus, J. (1989). A time analysis of the compositional processes used by
157–182. doi:10.1177/102986490500900106 children ages 7 to 11. Journal of Research in Music Education, 37, 5–20.
d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., Tillier, A. M., doi:10.2307/3344949
Soressi, M., . . . Lakarra, J. (2003). Archaeological evidence for the Kratus, J. (1994). Relationships among children’s music audiation and their
emergence of language, symbolism, and music–an alternative multidis- compositional processes and products. Journal of Research in Music
ciplinary perspective. Journal of World Prehistory, 17, 1–70. doi: Education, 42, 115–130. doi:10.2307/3345496
10.1023/A:1023980201043 Lehmann, A. C., & Kopiez, R. (2010). The difficulty of discerning between
Dunn, R. E. (1997). Creative thinking and music listening. Research Studies in composed and improvised music. Musicae Scientiae, 14, 113–129.
Music Education, 8, 42–55. doi:10.1177/1321103X9700800105 Leman, M. (1999). Music. In M. A. Runco, & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.),
Eisenberg, J., & Thompson, W. F. (2003). A matter of taste: Evaluating Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 285–296). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
improvised music. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 237–296. Leung, C. C., Wan, Y. Y., & Lee, A. (2009). Assessment of undergraduate
Eisenberg, J., & Thompson, W. F. (2011). The effects of competition on students’ music compositions. International Journal of Music Educa-
improvisers’ motivation, stress, and creative performance. Creativity tion, 27, 250 –268. doi:10.1177/0255761409337275
Research Journal, 23, 129 –136. doi:10.1080/10400419.2011.571185 Lubart, T. I., Besançon, M., & Barbot, B. (2011). Evaluation du potentiel
Folkestad, G., Hargreaves, D. J., & Lindström, B. (1998). Compositional créatif (EPoC) [evaluation of potential for creativity]. Paris, France:
strategies in computer-based music-making. British Journal of Music Editions Hogrefe France.
Education, 15, 83–97. doi:10.1017/S0265051700003788 Lubart, T. I., & Guignard, J. (2004). The generality-specificity of creativ-
Getzels, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longi- ity: A multivariate approach. In R. J. Sternberg, E. L. Grigorenko & J. L.
tudinal study of problem finding in art. New York, NY: Wiley. Singer (Eds.), Creativity: From potential to realization (pp. 43–56).
Getzels, J. (1964). Creative thinking, problem-solving, and instruction. In Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/
E. D. Hilgard (Ed.), Theories of learning and instruction (pp. 240 –276). 10692-004
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Mellor, L. (2007). Computer-based composition in the primary school: An
Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw, & J. Bransford investigation of children’s “creative” responses using the CD ROM
(Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychol- dance ejay. Musicae Scientiae, 11, 61– 88. doi:10.1177/
ogy (pp. 67– 82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 102986490701100103
Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Diver- Mellor, L. (2008). Creativity, originality, identity: Investigating computer-
gent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel based composition in the secondary school. Music Education Research,
uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 611– 625. 10, 451– 472. doi:10.1080/14613800802547680
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2007.tb00467.x Mialaret, J. P. (1997). Explorations musicales instrumentales chez le jeune
Gorder, W. (1980). Divergent production abilities as constructs of musical enfant (musical instrumental explorations in young child). Paris, France:
creativity. Journal of Research in Music Education, 28, 34 – 42. doi: PUF.
10.2307/3345051 Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis (2nd
Graham, R. G. (2006). New music and the contexts of creativity: Cultural- ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
evolutionary and humanistic perspectives. Journal of New Music Re- Nielsen, L. D. (2011). A study of K-12 music educators’ attitudes toward
search, 35, 347–352. doi:10.1080/09298210701563246 technology-assisted assessment tools. (Doctoral Dissertation, University
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. The American Psychologist, 5, 444 – 454. of Nebraska-Lincoln). Retrieved from http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/
doi:10.1037/h0063487 musicstudent/43/
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: Pachet, F. (2006). Creativity studies and musical interaction. In I. Deliège,
McGraw-Hill. & G. A. Wiggins (Eds.), Musical creativity, multidisciplinary research
Hallam, S. (2010). The power of music: Its impact on the intellectual, in theory and practice (pp. 347–358). London, England: Psychology
social and personal development of children and young people. Inter- Press.
national Journal of Music Education, 28, 269 –289. doi:10.1177/ Partti, H., & Karlsen, S. (2010). Reconceptualising musical learning: New
0255761410370658 media, identity and community in music education. Music Education
Hennessey, B. A. (1994). The consensual assessment technique: An ex- Research, 12, 369 –382. doi:10.1080/14613808.2010.519381
amination of the relationship between ratings of product and process Priest, T. (2006). The reliability of three groups of judges’ assessments of
creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 7, 193–208. doi:10.1080/ creativity under three conditions. Bulletin of the Council for Research in
10400419409534524 Music Education, 167, 47– 60.
Hickey, M., & Webster, P. (2001). Creative thinking in music. Music Richardson, C. P. (1983, Creativity research in music education: A review.
Educators Journal, 88, 19 –23. doi:10.2307/3399772 Bulletin of the Council on Research in Music Education, 74, 1–21.
Hickey, M. (1997). The computer as a tool in creative music making. Seddon, F. A., & O’Neill, S. A. (2003). Creative thinking processes in
Research Studies in Music Education, 8, 56 –70. doi:10.1177/ adolescent computer-based composition: An analysis of strategies ad-
1321103X9700800106 opted and the influence of instrumental music training. Music Education
Hickey, M. (1999). Assessment rubrics for music composition. Music Research, 5, 125–137.
Educators Journal, 85, 26 –32. doi:10.2307/3399530 Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in
Hickey, M. (2001). An application of Amabile’s consensual assessment assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420 – 428. doi:
technique for rating the creativity of children’s musical compositions. 10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
Journal of Research in Music Education, 49, 234 –244. doi:10.2307/ Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating
3345709 creativity in a culture of conformity. New York, NY: The Free Press.
Hickey, M. (2002). Creativity research in music, visual art, theatre, and Torrance, E. P. (1966). The Torrance tests of creative thinking-norms-
dance. In R. Colwell, & C. Richardson (Eds.), The new handbook of technical manual research ed.-verbal tests, forms A and B-figural tests,
research on music teaching and learning (pp. 398 – 414). Auckland, forms A and B. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.
New Zealand: Oxford University Press. Ward, C. J. (2009). Musical exploration using ICT in the middle and
242 BARBOT AND LUBART

secondary school classroom. International Journal of Music Education, Webster, P. (1994). Measure of creative thinking in music-II (MCTM-II).
27, 154 –168. doi:10.1177/0255761409102323 Administrative guidelines. Unpublished Manuscript, Northwestern Uni-
Webster, P. (1987). Conceptual bases for creative thinking in music. In J. versity, Evanston, IL.
Peery, I. Peery, & T. Draper (Eds.), Music and child development (pp. Widmer, G., & Goebl, W. (2004). Computational models of expressive
158 –174). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. doi:10.1007/978-1-4613- music performance: The state of the art. Journal of New Music Research,
8698-8_8 33, 203–216. doi:10.1080/0929821042000317804
Webster, P. (1990). Creativity as creative thinking. Music Educators Jour-
nal, 76, 22–28. doi:10.2307/3401073
Webster, P. (1992). Research on creative thinking in music: The assessment Received October 6, 2011
literature. In R. Colwell (Ed.), Handbook of research on music teaching and Revision received January 10, 2012
learning (pp. 266 –279). New York, NY: Macmillan Publishers. Accepted January 10, 2012 䡲

You might also like