Ten Points Every Behaviour Analyst Needs To Remember About Reinforcement.

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Ten Points Every Behavior Analyst Needs to Remember About Reinforcement

A. Charles Catania

Operant behavior is behavior that is sensitive But other sources of resistance can be found in
to its consequences. When operant behavior becomes misunderstandings of reinforcement and how it
more likely because of the consequences it has had, works. The relations among behavior and its
we speak of reinforcement. Some consequences consequences in reinforcement seem simple, but they
produce increases in the likelihood of operant have subtle properties, some of which become
behavior, and others do not. Ubiquitous but not evident only in special contexts. When those
universal, reinforcement is not a matter of properties are not taken into account, reinforcement
associations or stimulus-response connections. can appear to be ineffective or to be accompanied by
Rather, it is a variety of selection, in many ways undesirable side-effects. It is therefore prudent to
analogous to the phylogenic selection of biological review the properties of reinforcement and to
populations over evolutionary time. In ontogenic consider the circumstances in which they may
selection, or the selection of operant behavior, mislead us as we deploy techniques of reinforcement
populations of responses are selected by their and evaluate their effects. In the interests of
consequences. As in phylogenic selection, ontogenic preventing misconceptions and misunderstandings, it
selection is constrained by the range of variations is probably even more important to remind ourselves
available in the current population, and it can either of these properties whenever we present what we
occur naturally or be arranged artificially. know about reinforcement to those outside of the
field of behavior analysis. To those who argue that
With regard to phylogenic selection, artificial reinforcement should not be studied because it can be
selection was taken for granted, as in horticulture, misused, the appropriate rejoinder is that detailed
even while arguments for natural selection continued familiarity with its properties may be the best defense
to face serious challenges long after Darwin's insight. against its misuse.
Similarly, most critics of behavior analysis allow that
procedures for the selection of behavior are useful in In its concern for accountability, behavior
some contexts, such as animal training, even as they analysis typically assesses current behavior before
deny the relevance of the ontogenic selection of modifying it in an intervention. Let us be consistent
behavior in natural environments. Shaping is with that concern by assessing our understanding of
artificial selection and is easily demonstrable, but some properties of reinforcement in a brief quiz
natural ontogenic selection is harder to document before we proceed further. Ten true-false questions
The decades of debate over whether any follow that are correlated with ten points we need to
reinforcement contingencies operate in language remember about reinforcement. This is not meant to
acquisition provide an example. imply that these ten points are mutually exclusive or
that they are the only points we need to remember; in
As suggested by Skinner (1971), some of the fact, some items on the list are implicit in other items
resistance to the concept of reinforcement can be and the list is surely incomplete.
attributed to verbal traditions in contemporary
Western culture, including the languages of freedom The reader may wish to answer these
and control. Perhaps that is one reason why research questions in writing, whether on these pages or on a
on how extrinsic reinforcers might undermine the separate paper, so that the answers will be easily
potency of intrinsic reinforcers has led some of its available for later reference. An answer key will be
proponents to argue that these presumed hidden costs offered in the context of successive discussions of the
should bar the use of reinforcement in schools, several questions, although many readers will not
businesses and other institutions (Kohn, 1993), even need it and perhaps some will even disagree with one
though the claimed effects are only inconsistently or more of the keyed answers. In the latter case,
demonstrable and are small and transient when they readers may wish to give themselves partial credit or
do occur. modify their scores in other ways as they see fit.
There is no penalty for guessing and grades will
presumably remain confidential.
useful diagnostic tool when introducing students to
A True-False Quiz on Reinforcement the topic of reinforcement.

1. Chain pulls produce food and lever pressing The vocabulary of reinforcement includes the
increases. This illustrates reinforcement. terms _reinforcer_ as stimulus and _reinforce_ as a
2. The response classes produced by verb. For example, when a rat's lever presses
reinforcement are defined in terms of their produce food and lever pressing increases, we say
forms or topographies. that the food is a reinforcer and that the lever presses
3. Reinforcers work because they make the are reinforced with pellets. The response that
organism feel good or because the organism increases must be the one that produces the
likes them. consequences. For example, if a rat's lever press
4. A more probable response may be reinforced produces shock and only the rat's jumping increases,
by an opportunity to engage in a less it would be inappropriate to speak of either pressing
probable response. or jumping as reinforced.
5. Extinction depends on an active suppression
of the previously reinforced responding. It is because reinforcement operates on
6. If reinforcers have produced problem responses that we speak of reinforcing responses
behavior, the best solution is simply to take rather than organisms. We say that food reinforced a
the reinforcers away: to reduce a child's bad rat's lever press or that a pigeon's key peck was
behavior, extinguish it. reinforced with water, but not that food reinforced the
7. Following a long string of errors, do not miss rat or that the pigeon was reinforced for pecking or
a chance to reinforce the next correct that a child was reinforced. It is too easy to be
response. ambiguous about contingencies when we fail to
8. Cases in which responses seem to be identify the response that was reinforced. If we have
insensitive to their consequences demonstrate been told only that a child has been reinforced, we do
that some responses cannot be reinforced. not know much about actual contingencies. This
9. Extrinsic reinforcers applied to behavior grammatical restriction forces us to be explicit about
maintained by intrinsic reinforcers undermine which response has been reinforced but it does not
the potency of the intrinsic reinforcers. prevent us from mentioning the organism whose
10. The advantages of reinforcement make behavior had consequences. The moral is that in our
techniques of reinforcement more likely to descriptions of reinforcement procedures we should
spread through a culture than techniques of be explicit about what behavior has been reinforced
punishment. by what consequences.

Before proceeding, the reader may wish to review the Point 2: Topography and Function
items to be sure that no answers have been omitted. Reinforcement creates response classes that
are defined by their functions and not by their forms
Point 1: Specificity of Reinforcers or topographies, so _false_ is also the correct answer
By definition, reinforcement always increases to Question 2. Common contingencies select the
responding relative to what it would have been like members of operant classes, and they do so even if
without reinforcement. Also by definition, that the relations among members are arbitrary. A lever
increase must be specific to the response that press is a lever press whether the rat presses with
produced the consequence. If response A produces right paw, left paw, both paws, chin or rump
food and only response B increases, we do not say (Skinner, 1935; consider also the arbitrariness of the
that response B has been reinforced. Thus, _false_ is sets of slides in the discriminated operant classes in
the answer to Question 1: It is not correct to say that Vaughan, 1988).
reinforcement has been demonstrated when chain
pulls produce food and lever pressing increases. The The distinction between function and
specificity of reinforcement effects to the response topography is particularly crucial as it enters into
that produces the reinforcer distinguishes diagnostic categories. The self-injurious behavior of
reinforcement from other processes that produce two children may be similar in topography, but if the
increases in behavior. Question 1 is therefore a behavior of one child is reinforced socially by
attention and the behavior of the other is reinforced
by avoidance of compliance with simple requests, analysis of self-injurious behavior, restraints that
effective treatment programs designed for the two prevented children with severe developmental
children will have to be radically different (Iwata, disabilities from poking or biting themselves were
Pace, Kalsher, Cowdery, & Cataldo, 1990). The first effective in reinforcing arbitrary responses such as
child must be taught more effective ways of engaging putting marbles in a box (Favell, McGimsey, &
the attention of others and must be brought into Jones, 1978).
situations where attention is more readily available.
Requests must be selected for the second child that Whether particular events will be reinforcers
are appropriate to the child’s competence and the can be a difficult judgment. In the final analysis, the
child’s compliance with those requests must be primary criterion for reinforcement remains whether
reinforced (perhaps in the past such behavior has the consequences of behavior have raised the
instead been punished). likelihood of that behavior. Reinforcers are defined
by their behavioral effects and not by inconsistently
What the behavior does is more important correlated properties such as reports of feelings or
than what the behavior looks like. The moral is that preferences, so the moral is that when we try to
we must define the response classes created by identify which events will be effective as reinforcers
reinforcement in terms of their functions and not in we should assess, not guess.
terms of their forms or topographies.
Point 4: Relativity of Reinforcement
Point 3: Assessing Reinforcers Reinforcement is relative, in the sense that it
Our verbal behavior is often correlated with depends on relations between the reinforced response
our nonverbal behavior, so we should not be and the reinforcer. A less probable response may be
surprised that events that are effective as reinforcers reinforced by an opportunity to engage in a more
are sometimes described in terms of positive feelings probable response. The inverse relation, as in
or strong preferences. Such descriptions are subject Question 4, does not hold. Again, _false_ is the
to the inconsistent practices of verbal communities, correct answer. For example, food is not always a
however, and we must therefore be wary of using reinforcer. When a parent only allows a child to go
them to predict whether particular events will serve as out and play with friends after the child has eaten, the
reinforcers. It is tempting to equate reinforcers with opportunity to play may reinforce the eating.
events that are colloquially called rewards. But
contrary to Question 3, which is false, reinforcers do Difficulties in estimating response
not work because they make the organism “feel probabilities at a given moment sometimes
good” or because the organism “likes” them. Our complicate analyses in terms of the relativity of
everyday language does not capture what is important reinforcement, but the reversibility of the
about reinforcers. For example, in assessments of reinforcement relation has been amply demonstrated
the reinforcers that might be effective in managing experimentally (Premack, 1962). For example,
the behavior of people with profound handicaps, levels of food and water deprivation can be selected
predictions based on staff opinion of what would so that drinking is reinforced by an opportunity to eat
work for each individual were inconsistent with the at one time and eating is reinforced by an opportunity
reinforcers identified by systematically assessing to drink at another. In providing an _a_priori_
each individual's nonverbal preferences among those means for predicting whether an opportunity to
events (Green, Reid, White, Halford, Brittain, & engage in one response will reinforce some other
Gardner, 1988; cf. Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, response, the relativity of reinforcement also avoids
Hagopian, Owens, & Slevin, 1992). the problems of circular definition that were inherent
in some earlier definitions of reinforcement. We
We can sometimes make good guesses about soon repeat ourselves if we begin by pointing to an
what will be effective as a reinforcer because increase in responding when asked how we know that
reinforcers often involve events of obvious biological a stimulus was a reinforcer and then explain that the
significance. But reinforcers are not limited to such response was reinforced when asked why the increase
events. For example, sensory stimuli such as occurred.
flashing lights can be powerful reinforcers of the
behavior of autistic children (Ferrari & Harris, 1981). The significance of reinforcers is based on
Restraint also seems an unlikely reinforcer, but in an the opportunities for behavior that they allow. For
example, when time spent in isolation was used in an reading. Responsible teaching adds extrinsic
attempt to punish the tantrums of a 6-year-old autistic reinforcers only when there are no effective intrinsic
girl, her tantrums increased substantially instead of consequences.
decreasing. This child often engaged in
self-stimulation, such as waving her fingers over her The effects of reinforcers are not permanent.
eyes to create visual flicker, but that behavior was The moral is that if we want to maintain behavior
frequently interrupted by the staff. For her tantrums, after we terminate artificial consequences, we should
time in the isolation room was a reinforcer rather than do so only if natural consequences are in place that
a punisher because the isolation room allowed her to will take over that maintenance.
engage in self-stimulation without interruption
(Solnick, Rincover, & Peterson, 1977). Point 6: Side Effects
Reinforcement and extinction sometimes
The relativity of reinforcement reminds us have side effects, effects that are independent of their
that we should not expect the effectiveness of defining properties. For example, aggressive
reinforcers to be constant across different reinforced responding is sometimes a side effect of extinction
responses, different individuals, or even different because extinction terminates reinforcer deliveries.
time samples of the behavior of a single individual. If food is suddenly taken away from a food-deprived
The moral is that when a reinforcer is effective on rat that has been eating, the rat becomes more active
some behavior in some context, we must not assume and perhaps urinates or defecates. If the food was
that it will be effective on other behavior or even on produced by lever presses, the rat may bite the lever.
the same behavior in other contexts. If other organisms are in the chamber, the rat may
attack them (Azrin, Hutchinson, & Hake, 1966).
Point 5: Reinforcement and Extinction These effects and others, though observed in
The effects of reinforcers are not permanent. extinction, are not produced by the termination of the
Extinction demonstrates that reinforcers have reinforcement contingency, because they also occur
temporary effects. When reinforcement stops, the upon the termination of response-independent food
responding that it had maintained returns to its earlier deliveries. In either case, a rat that had been eating
lower levels. Thus, the decrease in responding stops getting food. In extinction, the termination of a
during extinction is not a special process requiring a reinforcement contingency entails the termination of
separate treatment. Rather, it is simply one of the reinforcer deliveries, and the effects of the latter are
properties of reinforcement. Earlier accounts of necessarily superimposed on the decreases in
extinction treated it as a separate process that actively previously reinforced responding.
suppressed responding, but the phenomena that once
seemed to support such accounts, including If reinforcers have produced problem
spontaneous recovery, disinhibition and rapid behavior, taking them away may produce undesired
reacquisition after extinction, were eventually shown side effects. That is why extinction is not the
to be independently determined by different features method of choice for getting rid of behavior that has
of the transition from reinforcement to extinction been created by reinforcement. For Question 6, as
(Catania, 1998, pp. 71-77). It is false to say, as in for its predecessors, _false_ is the correct answer.
Question 5, that extinction depends on an active Suppose a developmentally disabled boy engages in
suppression of previously reinforced responding. severe self-injurious behavior such as head-banging
or eye-poking and we discover that his behavior is in
If the effects of reinforcement are temporary, large part maintained by staff attention as a
then once we have created new behavior with reinforcer. Because of the harm he might do to
reinforcers we cannot count on its maintenance after himself if we ignore the self-injurious behavior,
our intervention ends. Consider children learning to extinction is not well-advised. Giving him attention
read. Only long after they have learned to name independently of the self-injurious behavior is one
letters of the alphabet and to read whole words are possibility, though that might reduce the behavior
they perhaps ready to read stories, so that reading can only slowly. Another is to use attention to reinforce
become “its own reward.” Until that happens, alternative responses, and especially ones
teachers have no choice but to arrange artificial incompatible with the self-injurious behavior. The
contingencies, using consequences such as praise or self-injurious behavior will decrease as the alternative
other extrinsic reinforcers to shape the components of responses increase.
In general, the solution is not to take the not restricted to a single operant class. For example,
reinforcers away. The better way to reduce a child's a pigeon's pecks on one key may be maintained
misbehavior is to reinforce good behavior. because they are followed by reinforced pecks on a
Reinforcers that create problem behavior are second key (Catania, 1971). In a task that involves
important and, unless they are intrinsically harmful, correct responses and errors over trials, correct
taking them away will probably produce more responses and errors can also constitute two separate
problems than making good use of them by making operant classes. If we reinforce every correct
them contingent on other behavior that is more response and repeat any trial with an error until the
manageable and more productive. Presumably that pigeon gets it right, this correction procedure
is why the experimental literature from applied guarantees that any sequence of errors will be
settings provides relatively few examples of the use followed by a reinforced correct response. Correct
of extinction alone to get rid of problem behavior. responses will probably dominate eventually, because
The more typical practice in an applied setting is the reinforcer most closely follows them. But errors
instead to use reinforcers already known to be potent, may diminish only slowly and may even continue at a
by virtue of the behavior they have created, to shape modest level though they never actually produce the
other behavior. reinforcer, because they are reliably followed after a
delay by a reinforced correct response. Thus, always
Sometimes we inadvertently teach the less reinforcing a single correct response after a sequence
effective alternative, especially when we present just of errors will probably maintain errors. An extended
a few basic facts about learning, as in the technical discussion of how to reduce the
introductory psychology course. Based on a strengthening of errors while maintaining correction
superficial account of reinforcement and extinction, a contingencies would take us too far afield, but all
common misconception about what parents should do methods would involve increasing the separation
to get rid of a child’s problem behavior is that they between errors and subsequent reinforcers. One way
should not reinforce it. Instead, they should ignore is to extend the time to the next trial after every error;
it. Left unanswered are the inevitable subsequent another is to draw from a set of error-prone problems
questions, such as how the parents should handle after each error trial rather than repeating the same
things when other problematic behavior emerges that problem, so that errors for any given problem are
is maintained by the same reinforcer. We should not further removed, on the average, from the eventual
teach parents to ignore the behavior of their children; reinforced correct response.
we should teach them how to use reinforcers more
productively. The side effects of extinction typically Teachers must be alert for situations in which
make it an inappropriate method for getting rid of they may be strengthening incorrect responses along
behavior that has been created by reinforcement. with correct ones that they reinforce. A reinforcer
The moral is that rather than taking the reinforcers that follows a sequence of correct responses will
away we should make good use of them to strengthen probably do a lot more good than a reinforcer that
alternative behavior. follows a single correct response after several errors.
Thus, teachers must judge whether correct responses
Point 7: Delay of Reinforcement are so infrequent that they should be reinforced even
The effects of a reinforcer depend on other though preceded by errors or so frequent that the
responses that preceded it besides the one, usually reinforcer can wait until the student has made several
most recent, that produced it. Thus, when one correct responses in a row.
response is followed by a different reinforced
response, the reinforcer may strengthen both. That Many practical applications of reinforcement
means that the reinforcement of a single correct may include other behavior that precedes the
response after a long string of errors will strengthen behavior that we target for reinforcement. When
the errors along with the correct response. It is such behavior shares in the effect of the reinforcer,
therefore false to say, as in Question 7, that one we may mistakenly conclude that the reinforcer is not
should not miss a chance to reinforce the next correct doing its job very well. But if the reinforced
response in such circumstances. behavior includes response classes that we did not
intend to reinforce, it may simply be doing very well
The effects of delayed reinforcers on a job other than the one we wanted it to do. When
responses preceding the ones that produced them are one response is followed by a different reinforced
response, the reinforcer may strengthen both, so the must consider a treatment program that uses attention
moral is that we should keep behavior that we do not to reinforce more effective and appropriate behavior.
want to reinforce from getting consistently close to But the example should also remind us that we cannot
reinforcers produced by other responses. define response classes by what they look like.

Point 8: Higher-Order Classes As in our discussion of Question 3, the


Sometimes when a response class appears criterion for defining response classes is function, but
insensitive to its consequences, it is part of a larger this case is distinguished from earlier examples by
class other members of which continue to have the the embedding of one response class within another.
consequences it once shared with them. In such The self-injurious behavior here was embedded in the
cases, the contingencies operating on the higher-order larger class of attention-getting behavior. When a
class may override those arranged for the original response class seems insensitive to its consequences,
class. For example, once generalized imitation has as when the self-injurious behavior seemed not to
been established, a child may continue to imitate extinguish, we must entertain the possibility that we
some instance even though that particular imitation is have improperly specified the class and that it is part
never reinforced. That imitation may seem of a larger class the other members of which continue
insensitive to contingencies, but it will be maintained to have consequences it once shared with them. The
by the contingencies that operate on the higher-order hierarchical structure of some classes of behavior
class as long as the higher-order class maintains its may sometimes make it appear that reinforcement is
integrity. We would ordinarily expect subclasses for not working but it may be working on a response
which reinforcement has been discontinued to be class larger than the one in which we have been
differentiated from the higher-order class, but that interested.
might not happen if the integrity of the higher-order
class depends on its membership in other interlocking Contingencies operating on a higher-order
higher-order classes that continue to include the class may override those arranged for one or more of
subclass (e.g., for generalized imitation, games such its subclasses and thereby make reinforcement appear
as _Simon Says_ that occur in other contexts). to be ineffective. The moral is that when
reinforcement seems not to be working we should
In any case, to demonstrate that responses are consider whether the response class in which we are
insensitive to their consequences is not to interested is part of another larger class.
demonstrate that they cannot be reinforced, so
Question 8 is another item for which _false_ is the Point 9: Interactions with Verbal Behavior
correct answer (this is not to say that all responses are Reinforcement may be obscured when human
reinforceable, but rather that unreinforceable verbal and nonverbal behavior interact. For
responses are unreinforceable for different reasons). example, instruction-following is more than the
following of particular instructions; it is a
Let us now consider an institutionalized boy higher-order class of behavior held together by
whose self-injurious behavior is reinforced by common contingencies (Catania, Matthews, &
attention. Suppose we try to extinguish his Shimoff, 1990; Estes, 1971). Following orders in
self-injurious behavior by ignoring it. We might have the military is a product of extensive and powerful
trouble from the start because we cannot tolerate the social contingencies, often based on aversive
damage he may do to himself. We persevere consequences, but in actual combat the long-term
nevertheless and discover that his self-injurious contingencies that maintain instruction-following in
behavior does not decrease. One possibility is that general as a higher-order class may be pitted against
we have not adequately identified the relevant the immediate consequences of following a particular
response class. If the function of this behavior is to order. These points are therefore to some extent
produce attention, it is probably part of a much larger corollaries of the arguments considered in the context
class of behavior that includes shouting obscenities, of Question 8.
acting up, hitting or otherwise abusing the caregivers
in the treatment center, and any number of other Verbal behavior is involved in the distinction
responses that might get attention (cf. Lalli, Mace, between intrinsic and extrinsic reinforcers, so it turns
Wohn, & Livesey, 1995). This tells us how out not to be true, as stated in Question 9, that
important attention is to this child; once again, we extrinsic reinforcers applied to behavior maintained
by intrinsic reinforcers undermine the potency of the bribes will no doubt eventually find that the child
intrinsic reinforcers. An intrinsic reward or complies only when a bribe is offered. The child
reinforcer is one that has a natural relation to the will not learn to initiate appropriate behavior if the
responses that produce it whereas an extrinsic one has initiation rests with the one who offers the bribe.
an arbitrary relation to those responses. For Over the long run, compliance with bribes will
example, music is an intrinsic consequence of playing probably become a higher-order class that will
an instrument but the music teacher's praise is an interfere with the effects of more constructive
extrinsic one. Events presumed to function as contingencies. If reinforcement works here at all, it
reinforcers because their function has been instructed is in strengthening compliance with bribes, which is
have been called extrinsic reinforcers (as when a hardly the best way to make use of reinforcers.
child is told that it is important to earn good grades)
but labeling them so does not guarantee their Nonverbal effects of reinforcers must be
effectiveness. distinguished from the social contingencies that
maintain the verbal governance of behavior. The
In one experiment (Lepper, Greene, & moral is that when situations involve verbal behavior
Nisbett, 1973), one group of children received gold there is a good chance that verbal governance will
stars for artwork. After the gold stars were override more direct effects of reinforcement.
discontinued, children in this group engaged in less
artwork than did those in a second group who never Point 10: Cultural Selection
received gold stars. The gold stars, extrinsic We have considered phylogenic selection, as
reinforcers, were said to have undermined the in the evolution of species, and ontogenic selection,
intrinsic reinforcers, the natural consequences of as in the shaping of operant behavior within an
drawing. The children had been told to earn the gold individual lifetime. A third level of selection is
stars, however, and the experiment did not test their cultural selection, which involves the selection of
effectiveness as reinforcers. There were no data to behavior as it is passed on from one individual to
show that children finger painted more when they got another (Skinner, 1981). Selection at any one of
gold stars (cf. the discussion of Question 3). these levels need not be consistent with selection at
the other two. For example, it may not matter how
Although extrinsic reinforcers are said to valuable one way of doing things is relative to some
undermine the potency of intrinsic reinforcers, the other way if one is easy to pass on from one
claimed effects are only inconsistently demonstrable individual to another while the other can be passed on
and are small and transient when they do occur only with difficulty. The one that is easier to pass on
(Cameron & Pierce, 1994). The evidence also may spread quickly and come to dominate in a
suggests that problems are more likely to arise with culture relative to the other one even if the latter
extrinsic reward that is not contingent on would be more beneficial in the long term.
performance than with contingent reward
(Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996). In any case, if A case in point is the application of
there is an effect its transience and small size is techniques of reinforcement relative to the application
hardly consistent with the argument that extrinsic of techniques of punishment. Unfortunately, the
reinforcement may ruin the lives of children. advantages of reinforcement do not make techniques
of reinforcement more likely to spread through a
In the literature of the “hidden costs of culture than techniques of punishment. By now a
reward”, reinforcers have sometimes also been trend should be apparent: None of the statements in
equated with bribes (Kohn, 1993), but it is unlikely Questions 1 through 10 were true.
that arrangements properly described as bribes
involve the direct effects of reinforcers. Given that a The problem is that delivering a punisher is
bribe specifies behavior and its consequences (“If you more likely to produce immediate consequences than
put away your toys you can watch television”), bribes delivering a reinforcer. Whatever else happens over
instead function as a stimuli that set the occasion for the long term, if a parent shouts at or strikes a child
particular contingencies. The child who is thought to be misbehaving, the parent is likely to see
frequently bribed will learn to discriminate between some immediate change in the child’s behavior, such
conditions in which bribes are in effect and those in as the onset of crying. That change will usually
which they are not, so the parent who often uses include the termination of the behavior that was of
concern to the parent but may have little to do with preferable and that limit the applicability of the latter.
whether the child’s behavior will reappear on later We have not dealt with the role of reinforcement
occasions, especially in the parent’s absence. If rather than informativeness in the maintenance of
stopping the child’s behavior is part of what observing behavior and the ways in which it may lead
reinforces the punishment of that behavior by the us to attend to what we want to hear rather than what
parent, its immediacy is an important factor in its we need to know (Dinsmoor, 1983). We have had
effectiveness. little to say here about the interactions between
behavior that has been reinforced and behavior that
With reinforcement, on the other hand, the comes from phylogenic sources, nor about the
effects of delivering a reinforcer may not show up relations between reinforcement and learning.
until some time has elapsed. In the shaping of a Reinforcement is itself a phylogenically evolved
response, if a current response is reinforced that is process. It does not make learning happen; instead,
closer to the target response than any other the shaper the consequences of responding are what is learned.
had seen before, the likelihood of that response will
increase. Even so, many other responses might go These are just a few of many possible
by before the shaper sees another one like it. Unlike additional topics, because ours remains a rich subject
the punishment case, in which the immediate effects matter with many areas still to be explored. The
typically include the stopping of the target behavior, current list could have been a baker’s dozen instead
any immediate effects of reinforcement, as in the of ten, but then the temptation would have been to
eating of a food reinforcer, involve behavior that is expand it still further. Instead, let us close our list
unrelated to the target response. Thus, the time here and pause for a review. Here is a new set of
periods over which deliveries of reinforcers have true-false items.
consequences by changing the probabilities of
subsequent responses probably play an important role Another True-False Quiz on Reinforcement
in determining how long it takes to teach shaping to 1. If chain pulls produce food and lever pressing
students. If that makes it easier to teach aversive increases, this does not illustrate reinforcement.
techniques than to teach techniques of reinforcement, 2. Response classes created by reinforcement are
perhaps it is also why punitive measures are so defined in terms of their functions.
commonly used to maintain civil order in so many 3. Reinforcers do not work because they make the
cultures. organism “feel good” or because the organism “likes”
them.
Even as reinforcement begins to be more 4. A less probable response may be reinforced by an
widely appreciated in our culture, we must not be opportunity to engage in a more probable response.
complacent about teaching what we know about it. 5. Extinction demonstrates that reinforcers have
Some people are really good at shaping though they temporary effects.
have not had explicit instruction, but mostly the 6. If attention has reinforced problem behavior, using
effective use of reinforcers has to be taught. Despite attention to reinforce an alternative response
the advantages of reinforcement, it is easier to teach incompatible with the problem behavior is better than
the use of aversive techniques than to teach the use of removing the attention.
reinforcers, and we have already reviewed here the 7. Whenever possible, prevent errors from being
various ways in which effects of reinforcement can be followed immediately by reinforced correct
misunderstood or can be obscured by other processes. responses.
The moral is that we must remember that the 8. When a response class seems to be insensitive to
effective use of reinforcers has to be carefully taught. its consequences, it may be part of a larger class other
members of which still have the consequences it once
Other Points shared with them.
Much remains. We have not discussed 9. The claimed effects of extrinsic reinforcers on
reinforcement based on stimulus removal (negative behavior maintained by intrinsic reinforcers are only
reinforcement). We have not considered symmetries inconsistently demonstrable and are small and
with consequences that reduce rather than increase transient when they do occur.
the likelihood of responding (punishers), nor have we 10. Delivering a punisher is more likely to produce
examined some practical differences between immediate consequences than delivering a reinforcer,
reinforcement and punishment that make the former
so it is easier to learn techniques of aversive control class we are interested in is part of another
than techniques of reinforcement larger class.
9. Nonverbal effects of reinforcers must be
In the first quiz, all of the items were false, distinguished from the social contingencies
but in this second one they are all true. Now we can that maintain the verbal governance of
summarize the ten points we have discussed and the behavior, so when situations involve verbal
morals we have drawn from each, again with the behavior there is a good chance that verbal
caveat that this list is not intended to be exhaustive governance will override more direct effects
nor are its several items mutually exclusive. of reinforcement.
10. Despite the advantages of reinforcement, it is
1. By definition, reinforcement is specific to the easier to teach the use of aversives than the
response that produces the reinforcing use of reinforcers, so we must remember that
consequences, so we should be explicit about the effective use of reinforcers has to be
what is reinforced by what. carefully taught.
2. We must define the response classes created
by reinforcement in terms of their functions, Conclusion
not in terms of their forms or topographies. Reinforcement is a variety of selection. The
3. Reinforcers are defined by their behavioral relations among behavior and its consequences in
effects and not by inconsistently correlated reinforcement seem simple, but they have subtle
properties such as reports of feelings or properties, some of which become evident only in
preferences, so when we try to identify which special contexts. When those properties are not
events will be effective as reinforcers, we taken into account, reinforcement can appear to be
should assess rather than guess. ineffective or to be accompanied by undesirable
4. Reinforcement is relative, so when a side-effects. Such inherent properties of
reinforcer is effective on some behavior in reinforcement may have affected its acceptance,
some context, we must not assume that it will because they allow its effects to be masked in various
be effective on other behavior or even on that ways. For example, when one response is followed
behavior in other contexts. by a different reinforced response, the reinforcer may
5. The effects of reinforcers are not permanent, strengthen both; when behavior is a member of a
so if we want to maintain behavior after we higher-order operant class, contingencies operating
terminate artificial consequences we should on the higher-order class may override those arranged
do so only if natural consequences are in for the original class; and, perhaps as a corollary,
place that will take over that maintenance. when human verbal and nonverbal behavior interact
6. The side effects of extinction often make it an reinforcement may be obscured, as when human
inappropriate method for getting rid of nonverbal behavior changes in ways inconsistent with
behavior that has been created by reinforcement contingencies because other
reinforcement, so rather than taking the contingencies have operated on relevant verbal
reinforcers away we should make good use of behavior. Alone or in combination, these and other
them to strengthen alternative behavior. factors may give the appearance that reinforcement
7. When one response is followed by a different does not work. On examination, we might instead
reinforced response, the reinforcer may conclude that it works more ubiquitously and more
strengthen both, so we should keep behavior profoundly than we had originally imagined.
that we do not want to reinforce from getting
consistently close to reinforcers produced by
other responses.
8. When behavior is a member of a higher-order
operant class, contingencies operating on the
higher-order class may override those
arranged for the original class and make
reinforcement appear to be ineffective, so
when reinforcement seems not to be working
we should consider whether the response
References systematic assessment of preferences. Journal
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 31-43.
Azrin, N. H., Hutchinson, R. R., & Hake, D. F. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery,
(1966). Extinction-induced aggression. G. E., & Cataldo, M. F. (1990). Experimental
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of analysis and extinction of self-injurious
Behavior, 9, 191-204. escape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1994). Reinforcement, Analysis, 23, 11-27.
reward, and intrinsic motivation: A Kohn, A. (1993). Punished by rewards. Boston:
meta-analysis. Review of Educational Houghton Mifflin.
Research, 64, 363-423. Lalli, J. S., Mace, F. C., Wohn, T., & Livesey, K.
Catania, A. C. (1971). Reinforcement schedules: The (1995). Identification and modification of a
role of responses preceding the one that response-class hierarchy. Journal of Applied
produces the reinforcer. Journal of the Behavior Analysis, 28, 551-559.
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973).
271-287. Undermining children's intrinsic interest with
Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper extrinsic reward: A test of the
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. “overjustification” hypothesis. Journal of
Catania, A. C., Matthews, B. A., & Shimoff, E. H. Personality and Social Psychology, 28,
(1990). Properties of rule-governed 129-137.
behaviour and their implications. In D. E. Premack, D. (1962). Reversibility of the
Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behaviour reinforcement relation. Science, 136,
analysis in theory and practice (pp. 215-230). 255-257.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Skinner, B. F. (1935). The generic nature of the
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1983). Observing and conditioned concepts of stimulus and response. Journal of
reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain General Psychology, 12, 40-65.
Sciences, 6, 693-728. Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity.
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
effects of reward: Reality or myth? American Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences.
Psychologist, 51, 1153-1166. Science, 213, 501-504.
Estes, W. K. (1971). Reward in human learning: Solnick, J. V., Rincover, A., & Peterson, C. R.
Theoretical issues and strategic choice points (1977). Some determinants of the reinforcing
(pp. 16-36). In R. Glaser (Ed.) The nature of and punishing effects of timeout. Journal of
reinforcement. New York: Academic Press. Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 415-424.
Favell, J. E., McGimsey, J. F., & Jones, M. L. (1978). Vaughan, W., Jr. (1988). Formation of equivalence
The use of physical restraint in the treatment sets in pigeons. Journal of Experimental
of self-injury and as positive reinforcement. Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 36-42.
225-241.
Ferrari, M. & Harris, S. L. (1981). The limits and
motivating potential of sensory stimuli as
reinforcers for autistic children. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 339-343.
Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian,
L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A
comparison of two approaches for identifying
reinforcers for persons with severe and
profound disabilities. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 25, 491-498.
Green, C. W., Reid, D. H., White, L. K., Halford, R.
C., Brittain, D. P., & Gardner, S. M. (1988).
Identifying reinforcers for persons with
profound handicaps: Staff opinion versus

You might also like