Comparison of Neuropsychological Functioning Between Adults With Early-And Late-Onset DSM-5 ADHD

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

730609

research-article2017
JADXXX10.1177/1087054717730609Journal of Attention DisordersLin and Gau

Article
Journal of Attention Disorders

Comparison of Neuropsychological
1­–12
© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
Functioning Between Adults With sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1087054717730609
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054717730609

Early- and Late-Onset DSM-5 ADHD journals.sagepub.com/home/jad

Yu-Ju Lin1,2 and Susan Shur-Fen Gau2

Abstract
Objective: We aimed to compare the visually dependent neuropsychological functioning among adults with Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-5) ADHD who recalled symptom onset by and after age 7 and
non-ADHD controls. Method: We divided the participants, aged 17 to 40 years, into three groups—(a) ADHD, onset
<7 years (early-onset, n = 142); (b) ADHD, onset between 7 and <12 years (late-onset, n = 41); (c) non-ADHD controls
(n = 148)—and compared their neuropsychological functioning, measured by the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing
Automated Battery. Results: Both ADHD groups had deficits in attention and signal detectability, spatial working memory,
and short-term spatial memory, but only the early-onset group showed deficits in alertness, set-shifting, and planning after
controlling for age, sex, and psychiatric comorbidities. There was no statistical difference between the two ADHD groups
in neuropsychological functioning. Conclusion: DSM-5 criteria for diagnosing adult ADHD are not too lax regarding
neuropsychological functioning. (J. of Att. Dis. XXXX; XX(X) XX-XX)

Keywords
ADHD, DSM-5, age-of-onset, neuropsychological functioning, comorbidities

Introduction of individuals with ADHD developed after 7 years old


(Applegate et al., 1997; Todd et al., 2008). Diagnosing adult
ADHD is a well-known neurodevelopmental disorder that ADHD requires a recall of childhood symptoms (Adler &
persists into adulthood (Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Chua, 2002) and inevitably suffers from recall bias (Barkley,
Fletcher, 2006). Literature has clearly documented neuro- Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2002). Individuals with
psychological deficits in children (Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, ADHD and their parents frequently reported later ages of
Faraone, & Pennington, 2005) as well as in adults (Alderson, onset during the follow-up interviews than their initial
Kasper, Hudec, & Patros, 2013; Schoechlin & Engel, 2005) reports (Todd et al., 2008). Only half of adults with ADHD
with ADHD. Although not all individuals with ADHD have recalled their symptom onset before 7 years (Kessler et al.,
neuropsychological deficits (Seidman, 2006), some neuro- 2005). Therefore, even only a small number of individuals
psychological deficits, especially those subserved by pre- develop ADHD after 7 years old, the possible recalled bias
frontal brain regions (Sonuga-Barke, 2005), are viewed as of ADHD onset for adolescents and young adults (Todd
endophenotypes (Castellanos & Tannock, 2002) of ADHD et al., 2008) will also result in underrecognition of ADHD
and might play a valuable role in validating the psychiatric in the adult population (Kieling et al., 2010). Adults with
diagnosis (Hyman, 2007). unrecognized ADHD often experience various negative life
Since long ago, there had been criticisms about the arbi- events and have adverse outcomes (Barkley et al., 2006).
trary decision of the age-of-onset criterion for diagnosing Several studies provided evidence to support the revi-
ADHD (Barkley & Biederman, 1997; Kieling et al., 2010) sion of ADHD age-of-onset criterion by showing
and the developmentally inappropriate symptoms as well as
symptom threshold in adult populations (Faraone et al., 1
Far Eastern Memorial Hospital, New Taipei City, Taiwan
2000; Tannock, 2013). Longitudinal studies gave conflict- 2
National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
ing data about the general age range of emerging ADHD
Corresponding Author:
symptoms: Some showed that only a small proportion of
Susan Shur-Fen Gau, Department of Psychiatry, National Taiwan
individuals developed impairing ADHD symptoms after 7 University Hospital and College of Medicine, No. 7, Chung-Shan South
years old (Polanczyk et al., 2010; Todd, Huang, & Road, Taipei 10002, Taiwan.
Henderson, 2008), while others found that more than 10% Email: gaushufe@ntu.edu.tw
2 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

comparable familial transmission, comorbidity pattern inattention, forgetfulness, restlessness, and impulsiveness.
(Faraone, Biederman, Spencer, et al., 2006), functional Besides, neuropsychological deficits frequently found in
impairment (Rohde et al., 2000; Willoughby, Curran, ADHD, including deficits in executive functions, working
Costello, & Angold, 2000), impaired neuropsychological memory, visuospatial memory, psychomotor processing
functions (Faraone, Biederman, Doyle, et al., 2006), and speed, and verbal fluency, are also found in major depres-
treatment response (Biederman et al., 2006; Reinhardt sive disorder (Baune, Fuhr, Air, & Hering, 2014) and oppo-
et al., 2007) between groups of different ages of onset, nam- sitional defiant disorder (ODD)/conduct disorder (CD; Lin
ing, by and after 7 years old, of ADHD. Therefore, the & Gau, 2017; Sergeant, Geurts, & Oosterlaan, 2002).
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th Previous studies either excluded individuals with psychiat-
ed.; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) crite- ric comorbidity (Schoechlin & Engel, 2005) or only
ria for ADHD elevate the age-of-onset threshold from 7 to adjusted for overall psychiatric comorbidity (Gau & Huang,
12 years and lower the symptom threshold from 6 to 5 at 2014; Seidman, Biederman, Weber, Hatch, & Faraone,
each symptom domain for individuals aged 17 years and 1998) while investigating neuropsychological functioning
older (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). After in ADHD. Given that adults with ADHD frequently have
DSM-5 formally published, Chandra, Biederman, and psychiatric comorbidities (Biederman et al., 1993) which
Faraone (2016) reported comparable impairments in quality may cloud the diagnosis of adult ADHD (Shaffer, 1994),
of life and social adjustment, and similar patterns of psychi- controlling for a specific comorbidity one at a time is sug-
atric comorbidity and familial transmission between late- gested to remove the influences of comorbidities on the
onset ADHD and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of neuropsychological deficits found in ADHD (Hervey,
Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Epstein, & Curry, 2004; Schoechlin & Engel, 2005).
Association, 1994) ADHD. Vande Voort, He, Jameson, and The Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated
Merikangas (2014) reported that using DSM-5 criteria Battery (CANTAB) is a widely used measure to assess vari-
caused a moderate increase of prevalence (from 7.38% to ous nonverbal neuropsychological functioning (Levaux
10.84%) as compared with DSM-IV criteria despite no sig- et al., 2007) with good reliability and validity (Luciana,
nificant difference in severity and comorbidity between the 2003). Combining our previous studies and others’ studies,
earlier and later onset groups. Concerns about overinflating we found that many tasks in CANTAB could differentiate
ADHD diagnosis by DSM-5 (Batstra & Frances, 2012; children (Fried, Hirshfeld-Becker, Petty, Batchelder, &
Prosser & Reid, 2013) still call for further studies to vali- Biederman, 2015; Rhodes, Coghill, & Matthews, 2005) and
date the new criteria. adolescents (Gau, Chiu, Shang, Cheng, & Soong, 2009) with
Following our previous findings of no difference in ADHD from typically developing youths, including high-
functional impairment, quality of life (Lin, Lo, Yang, & executive demand tasks, such as Spatial Working Memory
Gau, 2015), and psychiatric comorbid conditions (Lin, (SWM), Stocking of Cambridge (SOC), Intradimensional
Yang, & Gau, 2016) between the early- and late-onset adult and Extradimensional Shifting (IED), and low-executive
ADHD groups, this work intended to compare neuropsy- demand tasks, such as Rapid Visual Information Processing
chological functioning between adults with early- and late- (RVP), Reaction Time, and Spatial Span. Besides, previous
onset ADHD diagnosed by DSM-5. Although such an issue studies suggested that neuropsychological functions mea-
had been investigated by Faraone, Biederman, Doyle, et al. sured by SWM, SOC, IED, Spatial Span (Gau & Shang,
(2006), their work did not address the influence of psychiat- 2010), and RVP (Gau & Huang, 2014) in CANTAB might
ric comorbidities on the neuropsychological performance, be the endophenotypes of ADHD. Therefore, if adults with
and most of their neuropsychological tasks were verbally late-onset ADHD also have similar neuropsychological defi-
dependent. Visuospatial memory and central executive cits measured by the CANTAB as adults with early-onset
functions are more closely related to ADHD behavioral ADHD and these deficits are independent of any specific
symptoms than verbal memory (Alderson et al., 2013). psychiatric comorbidity, late-onset ADHD might be within
Functional neuroimaging studies also demonstrated that the spectrum of ADHD.
frontoparietal network and visual system subserving visuo- In this study, we selected the neuropsychological tasks
spatial (working) memory (Klingberg, 2006) and attention assessing visual sustained attention (RVP), short-term spa-
(Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols, & Rubia, 2013) have tial memory (Spatial Span), spatial working memory
altered activity in adults with ADHD in addition to the fron- (SWM), set-shifting (IED), and visual planning (SOC),
tostriatal network (Cortese et al., 2012; Vaidya, 2012). which were sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction (Owen,
Therefore, we believe visual tasks are highly correlated to Doyon, Petrides, & Evans, 1996), for analysis. To investi-
ADHD diagnosis with the level of correlation no less than gate the neuropsychological performance of ADHD regard-
the verbal ones. ing age-of-onset, we divided participants into three groups:
There are several overlapping symptoms between (a) ADHD with early-onset (onset by 7 years old); (b)
depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, and ADHD, such as ADHD with late-onset (onset between 7 and 12 years old);
Lin and Gau 3

(c) non-ADHD controls. We expected that adults with group. Participants in the control group received the same
ADHD would have significant neuropsychological deficits clinical and psychiatric evaluations as adults with ADHD to
regardless of the recalled age of onset and the psychiatric ensure that they did not have lifetime or current diagnosis of
comorbidity. ADHD.
Participants were excluded if they had any systemic
medical illness such as cardiovascular disease, learning dis-
Method
ability, autism spectrum disorder, and full-scale IQ < 80.
Participants and Procedure Considering the possibility of cognitive decline, we
excluded participants who were older than 40 years. After
We recruited adults (aged 17 years and older) who met the excluding individuals with incomplete data and those older
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th than 40 years, there were 183 adults with DSM-5 ADHD
ed., text rev.; DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric left in the study. Adults with DSM-5 ADHD were classified
Association, 2000) diagnostic criteria for ADHD and sub- into two groups by recalled age-of-onset: early-onset group
threshold ADHD (five symptoms out of nine inattention or/ (onset <7 years old) and late-onset group (onset between 7
and hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms or age of onset and <12 years old). Finally, there were 142, 41, and 148
after 7 years) during 2010-2014 at Department of Psychiatry, individuals in the early-onset ADHD, late-onset ADHD,
National Taiwan University Hospital (NTUH), Taipei, and control groups, respectively. In the late-onset group, six
Taiwan. There were two sources of the participant recruit- were from the clinical follow-up sample.
ment. First, new cases of adult ADHD were recruited by All participants, after clinical and diagnostic interviews,
advertisement or referred by psychiatrists or family physi- were assessed by the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale−III
cians (new recruitment sample). They first received tele- for the intelligence, followed by the CANTAB for neuro-
phone interview by a trained psychologist using the psychological functioning in a fixed order of the tasks. We
modified adult ADHD supplement of the Chinese versions asked the participants who currently received medication (n
of the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorder and = 77) for ADHD to hold medication at least 48 hr before the
Schizophrenia–Epidemiological Version (K-SADS-E; Gau neuropsychological assessment. All the psychologists
& Chang, 2013). After the telephone interview, those who applying the IQ test and CANTAB were blinded for group
were suspected of having the diagnosis of ADHD and their status. This study was approved by the Research Ethics
parents and/or partners were invited. Second, adults with Committee of NTUH, Taiwan (IRB ID, 2010003087R;
medical documents of childhood ADHD at NTUH who ClinicalTrials.gov number, NCT01247610), before the
possibly met the DSM-5 criteria for adult ADHD at the time study implementation.
of recruitment were invited (clinical follow-up sample).
These adults first came to our clinic when they were 6-10
years old, and they and their parents received clinical evalu-
Interviewer Training
ation and a formal diagnostic interview. All of them were The corresponding author was responsible for training
followed up in the Department of Psychiatry, NTUH with a interviewers for the psychiatric interview including the
medical document of their psychopathology, treatment, dif- Chinese K-SADS-E and SADS interviews (including the
ficulties, and improvements. modified adult ADHD supplement). The details of inter-
After signing the informed consent, all the participants viewer training were described in the supplementary
received formal psychiatric diagnostic interview as well as material.
the Conners’ Adult ADHD Diagnostic Interview (Conners,
Erhardt, & Sparrow, 1999) by the corresponding author.
Agreement of Age-of-Onset at Two-Time Points
Then they received the modified adult version of the ADHD
supplement (Gau & Chang, 2013; Lin et al., 2015; Lin et al., Only 40 individuals in the clinical follow-up sample had
2016) of the K-SADS-E (Gau, 2007) and SADS (Ni, Lin, complete data on age-of-onset and ADHD symptoms at
Gau, Huang, & Yang, 2017) for childhood/current diagnosis both baseline and follow-up. According to the parental
of ADHD and other psychiatric disorders, respectively, by reports, all the 40 individuals had the age of onset of ADHD
the trained interviewers. The interviewers were all blind to before 7 years at baseline. Of these 40 individuals with
the sources of referral. Among adults with ADHD, 93 ADHD, 34 individuals reported that their age-of-onset of
(43.5%) had been diagnosed with ADHD at childhood with ADHD was before 7 years old at adulthood and six between
valid medical records at the same hospital, and 121 (56.5%) 7 and 12 years old. All the adults with ADHD retrospec-
were recruited by advertisement and referral. Through tively reported later age-of-onset of ADHD at follow-up—
advertisement on the Internet, newspaper, and posters at the M (SD), 5.51 (1.29) years old in the early-onset group, and
colleges and public places, we recruited adult controls 9.17 (1.03) in the late-onset group—than that reported by
according to the age and sex distributions of the ADHD their parents to the participants with ADHD at baseline—M
4 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

(SD) 4.19 (1.53) years old in the early-onset group and 4.33 Results
(1.72) in the late-onset group. The result is compatible with
the reports of Todd et al. (2008). For the details of age-of- Basic Data and ADHD Symptoms
onset and ADHD symptom agreement, please refer to the Table 2 summarizes the demographics of the three groups.
supplementary material. Adults with ADHD were significantly older than controls,
and there was no group difference in the distributions of
Measures sex, and parental occupation and education. More partici-
pants in the ADHD groups were married/living with the
Six CANTAB tasks were selected in this study and classi- couple, did not attend college and more parents were
fied into low-executive function (low-EF) tasks and execu- divorced or separated as compared to the control group.
tive function (EF) tasks according to the EF demands (Lin, There were significantly more symptoms of hyperactivity
Chen, & Gau, 2013), as described by Rhodes et al. (2005). in the early- than in the late-onset ADHD group and no dif-
The descriptions of neuropsychological tasks are summa-
ference in inattention and impulsivity symptoms between
rized in Table 1.
two ADHD groups (Table 3). There was no difference in the
proportion of ADHD medication use between early-onset
Statistical Analyses and late-onset ADHD groups (Table 3).
We conducted analyses by SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute Inc.,
Cary, NC). For demographic data, we used chi-square for Comparisons of Neuropsychological Functioning
categorical variables and linear regression for continuous Among Groups (Table 4)
variables. All the results of neuropsychological tasks were
Cognitive alertness (Reaction Time).  The early-onset ADHD
calculated and presented with raw scores. For group com-
group had longer reaction time than the control group in the
parisons of neuropsychological functions, we tested three
simple analysis. No group difference was noted in the mul-
models using multiple analyses. In the first model (Model
tiple analysis.
1), we controlled age, sex, and any psychiatric comorbid-
ity (1 for any psychiatric comorbidity and 0 for none). In Sustained attention/signal detectability (RVP).  Both the early-
the second model (Model 2), we added current ADHD and late-onset ADHD groups were impaired in the probabil-
medication use (1 for any ADHD medication use and 0 for ity of hit (sustained attention), A′, and mean latency (signal
none), participant’s education, marital status, job, and detectability) of RVP in both the simple analysis and the
parental marital status into the first model as the covari- first model of multiple analysis. Only the early-onset ADHD
ates, because these demographic variables were signifi- group showed significant impairment in the probability of
cantly different between groups. The third model was hit, and A′ and both ADHD groups showed impairment in
similar to the first model, but the overall “any psychiatric mean latency in the second model of multiple analysis.
comorbidity” was replaced by the specific psychiatric
comorbidity. In the third model (Model 3), a specific psy- Short-term spatial memory (Spatial Span). For the span
chiatric comorbidity was controlled one at a time, includ- lengths, adults in both ADHD groups had shorter span
ing “depressive disorder,” “anxiety disorder,” “sleep length than controls in the simple analysis and only adults
disorder,” “lifetime oppositional defiant disorder (ODD)” in the late-onset ADHD group had fewer span lengths than
and “lifetime conduct disorder (CD),” respectively. We controls in the first model of the multiple analysis. In all
did not control IQ in our analysis because there was no IQ analyses, adults in both ADHD groups made more errors
difference between groups (Table 3). Besides, some than those in the control group.
authors thought factors leading to decreased IQ perfor-
mance might be the same factors contributing to neuropsy- Spatial working memory (SWM).  Both early- and late-onset
chological dysfunction in ADHD. Therefore, controlling groups showed poor strategy utilization and more between
for IQ may remove part of the variance of neuropsycho- errors in the simple analysis. In the multiple analysis, there
logical dysfunction associated with ADHD (Nigg, 2001). was no group difference in strategy utilization in both two
The Bonferroni correction method was applied to adjust models and in between errors in the second model of mul-
for multiple comparisons in the post hoc analysis. The tiple analysis.
effect size was calculated for group comparisons of neuro-
psychological functioning based on Cohen’s definition Spatial planning (SOC). Only the early-onset group had
and was divided into low (0.2-0.5), medium (0.5-0.8), and fewer problems solved in minimum moves in the simple
high (> 0.8) levels. The significant level was preselected analysis and more mean moves in the five-move problem in
as p < .05. both simple and multiple analyses than the control group.
Lin and Gau 5

Table 1.  Neuropsychological Tasks and the Corresponding Functions.


Neuropsychological tasks Functions measured Descriptions

Low-EF tasks
  Reaction Time
   Simple reaction time Alertness This task assesses alerting stage of arousal (including stages of alerting, phasic responding,
   Five-choice reaction time and signal/noise ratio enhancing) in arousal/activation theory (Pribram & McGuinness,
1975), by recording the reaction time in response to a stimulus with minimal influence
by the movement speed. The participants are asked to press a button on the table and
touch the screen while seeing the stimulus presented in the simple circle (simple-
choice) or one of five circles (five-choice) on the screen. The task lasts for 3 min.
Reaction time (i.e., the mean of time taken to release the button after the presence of
the stimulus) of the simple-choice and five-choice tasks was presented.
  Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP)
   Probability of hit Sustained attention This task, a 7-min visual continuous performance test (CPT) modified, is used to measure
  A′ Signal detectability sustained attention capacity. Digits (ranging from 2 to 9) appear one at a time (100 digits
(signal/noise per minute) in a random order. The participants are asked to press a response pad
discrimination) when they note any of three number sequences: 3-5-7, 2-4-6, 4-6-8. Three indices were
   Mean latency (ms) Alertness and sustained reported: (a) probability of hits (h): total hits divided by the sum of total hits and total
attention misses; (b) A′: sensitivity to the target, regardless of response tendency, which ranges
from 0 to 1, calculated as A′ = 0.5 + [(h – f) + (h – f) 2] / [4 × h × (1 – f)], and higher
score indicating higher sensitivity of signal/noise discrimination of arousal (Sahgal, 1987)
(f: probability of false alarm: total false alarms divided by the sum of total false alarms and
total correct rejections); and (c) mean latency: mean time taken to respond in correct
responses.
  Spatial Span
  Span length Spatial short-term This task is the visuospatial analogue of the digit span test and lasts for 5 min. Similar to
  Total errors memory the Corsi blocks task (Milner, 1971), it requires the ability to remember the order of
   Total usage errors visual stimuli presented. There are 9 white boxes presented in fixed locations on the
screen. The color of the boxes are changed one after the other in a predetermined
sequence. The end of the sequence is indicated by a sound. The participants are asked
to point to the boxes on the screen in the order as previously presented on the
screen. The task begins with a level of 2-box then gradually up to a level of 9-box. If
the participant fails in all the three sequences in a particular level, the test terminates.
Three indices were presented: (a) span length, the longest sequence successfully
recalled; (b) total errors, the number of times an incorrect box selected across the
whole task; (b) total usage errors, the number of times a box not in the sequence
selected across the whole task.
EF tasks
  Spatial Working Memory (SWM)
  Strategy utilization Strategy usage It is constructed based on a self-ordered search test (Petrides & Milner, 1982), an
  Between errors Spatial working adaptation of Olton’s radial arm maze (Olton, 1987). It takes 4 min to complete the
memory task. Participants are asked to search through the covered box presented on the
screen to find the blue token hidden inside. Only one single token is hidden in one
of the boxes in each trial and the box that had been found to have the token inside
would not have a token again in the subsequent trials. The SWM includes three
difficulty levels (4-box, 6-box, and 8-box), each included four tests. Two indices were
presented: (a) strategy utilization: the number of search sequences starting with a
novel box in both 6- and 8-box; (b) Between errors: total times the participant opens
a box without a blue token because of ever having a token inside in previous trial
across three difficulty levels.
  Stocking of Cambridge (SOC)
   Problems solved in This task assesses spatial planning based on the Tower of London and takes 10 min. At
minimum moves the beginning of each trial, three suspended vertical stockings and three colored balls
   Mean move, 5-move Spatial planning are presented on the screen. Participants are asked to move the colored balls, one
problems in a single move at a time, between stockings to accomplish a goal position within a
specified number of moves in the problem-solving condition, and then they are asked
to copy each move by following the identical sequence of moves played back by the
computer, based on their employment of problem solving in the control condition.
The SOC comprises of four problem sets (2, 3, 4, and 5 moves) to reflect increasing
demands on planning. Two major indices were presented to tap the thinking accuracy:
(a) problems solved in minimum moves: the number of occasions which were
successfully completed in the minimum possible number of moves; (b) mean moves
of the 5-move task: the mean of the number of moves taken in excess of minimum
moves (5 moves) but within the maximum allowed.

(continued)
6 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

Table 1. (continued)
Neuropsychological tasks Functions measured Descriptions

  Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shifts (IED)


  Extradimensional shift Set-shifting This task assesses set-shifting, the ability to learn new problem and shift to a new
errors strategy from the feedback. It takes 7 min to complete the task. There are two
  Pre-extradimensional Sustained attention artificial dimensions: white line and color-filled shapes. Simple followed by compound
shift errors (combination of line and shape) stimuli are presented and the participant has to select
  Completed stages Set-shifting and which stimulus is right by the feedback. When the participant reaches the criterion
sustained attention (six consecutive correct responses) at a given stage, the task progresses to the next
stage and the stimuli/rules change. If the participant fails to reach 50 trials at any
stage, the test terminates. There are nine stages. During Stages 1 to 7, the participant
has to selectively maintain attention on the rule based on the color-shape dimension,
i.e., intradimensional shift, and then at Stages 8 and 9, the participant has to shift
attention to the rule based on the white line dimension, i.e., extradimensional shift
(Luciana & Nelson, 1998). Three indices were presented: (a) extradimensional shift
errors: the number of errors in the extradimensional stages; (b) pre-extradimensional
errors: the number of errors made prior to the extradimensional shift (Stages 1 to 7)
of the task, i.e., failing to keep current rule; (c) completed stage: the number of stages
that were completed.

Note. EF = executive function.

Table 2.  Demographic Data.


1. ADHD 2. ADHD Statistics
Onset < 7 Onset 7 to <12 3. Control
  (n = 142) (n = 41) (n = 148) F P Comparison
Age, M (SD) 27.60 (6.23) 28.74 (6.12) 24.74 (5.03) 13.73 <.0001 1,2>3
Gender, n (%) χ2  
 Male 85 (69.86) 26 (63.41) 85 (57.43) 0.52 .77  
Education  
  Postgraduate and above 34 (23.94) 8 (19.51) 48 (32.43) 10.78 .03  
 College 84 (59.15) 28 (68.29) 91 (61.49)  
  Senior high school and below 24 (16.90) 5 (12.20) 9 (6.08)  
Job
 Professional 7 (5.93) 2 (4.88) 6 (4.05) Fisher’s exact test .01  
  Technical personnel 66 (46.48) 18 (43.90) 42 (28.38)  
  Nontechnical personnel 69 (48.59) 21 (51.22) 100 (67.57)  
Marital status (n = 140) (n = 41) (n = 131) χ2  
 Singlea 113 (80.71) 27 (73.17) 119 (90.84) 10.57 .03  
  Married or living with couple 27 (19.29) 8 (26.83) 12 (9.16) (df = 4)  
Parental education (n = 133) (n = 40) (n = 138)  
  College and above 79 (59.40) 19 (47.50) 74 (57.81) χ2 (df = 4) = 9.03 .06  
  Senior high school 36 (27.07) 8 (20.00) 36 (28.13)  
  Junior high school and below 18 (13.53) 13 (32.50) 18 (14.06)  
Parental jobb (n = 132) (n = 40) (n = 130)  
 Professional 16 (12.12) 4 (10.00) 17 (13.08) Fisher’s exact test .16  
  Technical personnel 87 (65.91) 20 (50.00) 87 (66.92)  
  Nontechnical personnel 29 (21.97) 16 (40.00) 26 (20.00)  
Parental marital status (n = 138) (n = 40) (n = 132)  
  Married or living with couple 105 (76.09) 32 (80.00) 117 (88.64) Fisher’s exact test .02  
  Separated or divorced 26 (18.84) 5 (12.50) 8 (6.06)  
 Others 7 (5.07) 3 (7.50) 7 (5.30)  
a
Including divorced or separated (two in early-onset group and one in the control group).
b
If both parents had jobs, we presented the category of expected higher income here.

Set-shifting (IED).  Adults with early- and late-onset ADHD simple analysis, but not in the multiple analyses. Only the
had more extradimensional shift errors than controls in the early-onset ADHD group had more pre-extradimensional
Lin and Gau 7

Table 3.  ADHD Symptoms, Intelligence, and Psychiatric History Among Groups.
1. ADHD 2. ADHD Statistics
Onset <7 Onset 7 to <12 3. Control
  (n = 142) (n = 41) (n = 148) F P Comparison
ADHD symptoms_K-SADS
 Past
  Inattention 8.61 (0.76) 8.54 (0.64) 0.34 (0.78) 1755.15 <.0001 1,2>3
  Hyperactivity 4.10 (1.87) 3.34 (2.01) 0.14 (0.45) 235.25 <.0001 1>2>3
  Impulsivity 2.16 (1.07) 1.08 (1.05) 0.09 (0.32) 217.08 <.0001 1,2>3
 Current
  Inattention 8.18 (1.15) 7.80 (1.05) 0.12 (0.4) 2966.04 <.0001 1,2>3
  Hyperactivity 3.04 (1.83) 2.37 (1.74) 0.03 (0.22) 180.26 <.0001 1>2>3
  Impulsivity 1.73 (1.13) 1.44 (1.05) 0.04 (0.24) 155.10 <.0001 1,2>3
Intelligence, M (SD)
  Full IQ 110.95 (11.61) 109.97 (12.58) 112.01 (10.95) 0.88 .42  
  Performance IQ 110.51 (13.20) 109.54 (12.89) 112.29 (12.08) 1.41 .24  
  Verbal IQ 110.13 (11.76) 109.43 (13.29) 110.64 (10.60) 0.31 .73  
History of ADHD medication use, χ2 (df = 2) value  
n (%)a
 Overall 97 (68.31) 24 (58.54) 0 (0.0) 1.36 (1) .24  
 Methylphenidate 81 (57.04) 19 (46.34) 0 (0.0) 1.46 (1) .23  
 Atomoxetine 20 (14.08) 8 (27.59) 0 (0.0) 0.72(1) .40  
Current ADHD medication use, χ2 (df = 1) value  
n (%)a
 Overall 62 (43.66) 15 (36.59) 0 (0.0) 0.65 .42  
 Methylphenidate 62 (43.66) 15 (36.59) 0 (0.0) 0.65 .42  
 Atomoxetine 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0.0) — —  
Psychiatric comorbidity χ2 (df = 2) value  
  Any psychiatric disorder 107 (72.35) 30 (73.17) 11 (7.43) 150.56 <.0001 1,2>3
  Major depression 9 (6.34) 3 (7.32) 0 (0.0) Fisher exact test .001 1,2>3
  Dysthymia 25 (17.61) 5 (12.20) 1 (0.68) <.0001 1,2>3
  Bipolar disorder 2 (1.41) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) .54 1,2>3
   Any anxiety disorder 51 (35.92) 16 (30.02) 6 (4.05) <.0001 1,2>3
  Sleep disorders 42 (29.58) 9 (21.95) 1 (0.68) <.0001 1,2>3
  ODD (lifetime) 71 (50.71) 22 (53.66) 5 (3.38) χ2 (df = 2) 89.84 <.0001 1,2>3
  CD (lifetime) 50 (35.71) 14 (34.15) 5 (3.38) 50.29 <.0001 1,2>3

Note. Anxiety disorders did not include obsessive-compulsive disorder in this study. K-SADS = Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizo-
phrenia; ODD = oppositional defiant disorder; CD = conduct disorder.
a
Only two ADHD groups were compared. History of medication use = current and ever use of medication.

shift errors in the simple analysis, and fewer completed difference were the same as what was revealed in the simple
stages in the simple analysis and the first model of multiple analyses (Supplementary Table S2).
analysis.
For the neuropsychological functions which were
Influence of ADHD Medication Use
impaired in ADHD, the effect size of two ADHD groups
over the control group ranged from 0.4 to 0.9, referring a Adding the current medication treatment for ADHD and
medium to large effect size (Supplementary Table S1). demographic variables into the multiple analyses models
There was no significant difference of neuropsychological (Model 2), we found that the statistical difference of SWM
functions between the two ADHD groups in the simple and between errors, IED extradimensional shift errors, and RVP
multiple analyses. probability of hit and A′ between adults with late-onset
ADHD and the controls did not reach the significant level
(p > .05; Table 4). Becuase current ADHD medication treat-
Influence of the Specific Psychiatric Comorbidity ment was reversely correlated with the performance in sim-
When we performed the multiple analysis that controlled ple Reaction Time (Pearson correlation, r = .22, p < .001),
for age, sex, and the psychiatric comorbidity one at a time RVP probability of hit, (r = −.14, p = .01), RVP A′ (r = −.14,
(Model 3), neuropsychological tasks with significant group p = .01), total usage errors in Spatial Span (r = .21,
8 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

Table 4.  Comparison of Neuropsychological Functioning Among Adults With Early-Onset, Late-Onset ADHD, and Controls.
Multiple analysis
1. ADHD onset 2. ADHD onset 7 3. Control
<7 (n = 142) to <12 (n = 41) (n = 148) Simple analysis Model 1a Model 2b

Neuropsychological tasks M (SD) F Comparison F Comparison F Comparison

Reaction Time
  Simple reaction time (ms) 337.2 (67.54) 320.47 (43.52) 312.01 (43.47) 7.66*** 1>3 2.14 1.88  
  Five-choice reaction time (ms) 354.36 (57.72) 336.68 (36.91) 329.07 (47.12) 9.09*** 1>3 3.56 2.28  
Rapid Visual Information Processing
  Probability of hit 0.69 (0.19) 0.7 (0.14) 0.79 (0.16) 14.48*** 1,2<3 6.86** 1,2<3 5.36** 1<3
  A′ 0.92 (0.05) 0.92 (0.04) 0.95 (0.04) 14.86*** 1,2<3 6.86** 1,2<3 5.40** 1<3
  Mean latency (ms) 426.86 (90.63) 427.26 (84.67) 386.71 (66.23) 10.37*** 1,2>3 5.48** 1,2>3 6.21** 1,2>3
Spatial Span
  Span length 7.1 (1.38) 7.24 (1.22) 7.91 (1.16) 15.83*** 1,2<3 3.51* 1<3 1.76  
  Total errors 13.22 (6.3) 14.76 (5.91) 10.99 (6.91) 7.24*** 1,2>3 5.37** 1,2>3 6.44** 1,2>3
  Total usage errors 1.53 (1.4) 1.63 (1.32) 0.8 (0.97) 15.69*** 1,2>3 8.23*** 1,2>3 4.80** 1,2>3
Spatial Working Memory
  Strategy utilization 32.06 (5.91) 32.8 (5.16) 29.92 (5.85) 6.76*** 1,2>3 0.81 0.55  
  Between errors 21.49 (16.95) 21.71 (14.58) 11.57 (12.92) 18.11*** 1,2>3 5.10** 1,2>3 2.41  
Stocking of Cambridge
  Problems solved in minimum moves 9.05 (1.76) 9.32 (2.11) 9.82 (1.68) 6.90** 1<3 1.85 0.84  
  Mean move, 5-move problem 6.41 (1.27) 6.04 (1.13) 5.82 (1.08) 9.27*** 1>3 4.74** 1>3 3.87* 1>3
Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift
  Extradimensional shift errors 5.78 (7.41) 6.53 (7.61) 3.56 (3.32) 6.77** 1,2>3 3.40* 1.11  
  Pre-extradimensional shift errors 6.52 (4) 6.55 (3.21) 5.32 (2.42) 5.49** 1>3 2.38 1.19  
  Completed stages 8.77 (0.85) 8.78 (0.62) 8.98 (0.18) 4.88** 1>3 3.83* 1>3 1.36  

a
Adjusting for age, sex, and any psychiatric disorder.
b
Adjusting for age, sex, any psychiatric disorder, participant’s education, job, marital status, current medication use for ADHD, and parental marital status.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

p < .001), SWM between errors (r = .27, p < .001), IED Psychiatric Comorbidity and Neuropsychological
extradimensional shift errors (r = .17, p = .006), and com- Functioning in ADHD
pleted stages (r = −.12, p = 0.03), ADHD medication treat-
ment might share the variance of the correlation between Following our previous findings of no difference in func-
neuropsychological deficits and ADHD. Adults with poorer tional impairment, quality of life (Lin et al., 2015), and psy-
neuropsychological performance were more likely to chiatric comorbidity (Lin et al., 2016) between the early- and
receive medication treatment in this naturalistic study, late-onset adult ADHD groups, this work further revealed
implying a self-selection bias. comparable neuropsychological dysfunctions between
adults with early- and late-onset ADHD diagnosis accord-
ing to the DSM-5 criteria. Although this was not the first
Discussion study to validate the age-of-onset criterion by neuropsycho-
Main Findings logical functioning, this was the first study controlling for
each psychiatric comorbidity one at a time. We found no
With the intention to validate the DSM-5 adult ADHD age- other psychiatric disorder could explain the neuropsycho-
of-onset criterion by neuropsychological functioning, this logical deficits in adults with ADHD. Under a comprehen-
study found that adults with DSM-5 ADHD, regardless of sive psychiatric interview, adult ADHD diagnosis, even
age-of-onset, had significant deficits in sustained attention with recalled onset between 7 and 12 years and possible
(RVP probability of hit), signal detectability (RVP A′), spa- recall bias, would be valid. Late-onset adult ADHD is not a
tial working memory (SWM between errors), and set-shift- pseudodiagnosis that just coincidentally presents the symp-
ing (IED extradimensional shift errors) after considering toms of other psychiatric disorders.
the influences of age, sex, and any psychiatric comorbidity,
but deficits in alertness (Reaction Time) and planning (SOC Neuropsychological Deficits in Early- and Late-
five-move task) were only noted in adults with early-onset
Onset ADHD
ADHD. Current medication was correlated with poorer
neuropsychological performance, implying a self-selection In this study, we tested two models of multiple analyses,
bias in this study. which revealed similar results. When we looked into the
Lin and Gau 9

differences of the results between Models 1 and 2, we found In this study, adults with early-onset ADHD had signifi-
that spatial working memory was an important neuropsy- cantly more previous and current hyperactive symptoms
chological function that made participants decide to use than those with late-onset ADHD, but there was no correla-
medication, because, in Model 2, current ADHD medica- tion of hyperactive symptom count and reaction time (sim-
tion use was associated with worse SWM function (p = .04) ple task, F = 0.01, p = .90, five-choice task, F = 0.58, p =
independent of group, age, sex, and other demographic .45) and SOC (five-move tasks, F = 0.02, p = .90) after
variables. Therefore, the loss of group differences of SWM controlling for the inattentive symptom count. Higher
at Model 2 was largely due to the share of the variance of hyperactive symptom count did not explain the differential
medication use and ADHD. The loss of significant differ- neuropsychological presentations of early- and late-onset
ence between the late-onset and the control groups in RVP ADHD in this study.
might be due to small sample size of the late-onset ADHD ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder with a various com-
group and lack of power to detect the differences when bination of neuropsychological deficits across different
there were many variables in the analysis. The demographic affected persons, and the presentations and emergence of
variables had nonsignificant effects on the results. The fol- symptoms are also correlated to individual adaptation to
lowing discussion is therefore mainly based on the results environmental and developmental constraints (Sonuga-
of the first model of the multiple analyses. Barke, 2005). Our finding that these late-onset ADHD
Adults in both early- and late-onset ADHD groups had adults had relatively preserved planning ability implies that
they may have fewer dysfunctional brain circuits and thus
a longer latency of correct response in RVP, suggesting
developed symptoms later or their preserved abilities could
impaired psychological processes involving sustained
compensate for ADHD symptoms during their early child-
attention (van der Meere, Shalev, Borger, & Gross-Tsur,
hood. Nevertheless, when ADHD persisted into adulthood,
1995). Meanwhile, our finding that only adults with
adults with ADHD who recalled onset between 7 and 12
early-onset ADHD had longer reaction time in both sim-
years showed no less functional impairment and had a com-
ple and five-choice reaction time tasks suggested that
parable decrease of life quality as that recalled onset by 7
alertness, which was important in the attentional process
years (Faraone, Biederman, Spencer, et al., 2006; Lin et al.,
(Nigg, 2005), was impaired only in early-onset ADHD, or 2015). More recently, Moffitt et al. (2015) found in their
that adults with late-onset ADHD had milder impairment longitudinal study that individuals with adult-onset ADHD,
in the attentional network than those with early-onset despite subjective complaints of cognitive deficits, had
ADHD. Inattentive symptoms of late-onset ADHD might fewer tested cognitive deficits than those with childhood-
emerge through neuropsychological mechanisms other onset regardless of current ADHD status. Although the
than alertness, such as impaired signal detectability of symptoms of adult-onset ADHD might be explained by
attention, sustained attention, and/or other executive substance dependence and other psychiatric disorders, their
dysfunctions. findings implied that neuropsychological functions might
We had an intriguing finding that adults with late-onset be correlated to the timing of emergence of ADHD symp-
ADHD had the noninferior performance of SOC as com- toms, that is, late-onset ADHD might be a minor form of
pared with controls despite that they had significant deficits ADHD as compared with early-onset ADHD. The author
in spatial working memory (SWM) and short-term spatial also suggested a possibility that adult-onset ADHD might
memory (Spatial Span). During the process of planning, be an independent disorder etiologically different from
one has to organize response sequences and to monitor exe- childhood-onset ADHD (Moffitt et al., 2015). Further lon-
cution which places a load on working memory (Owen gitudinal and imaging studies are needed to elucidate the
et al., 1996). The dissociated correlation of late-onset role of the Brain Function × Environment interaction in the
ADHD with spatial (working) memory and spatial planning onset of ADHD in a developmental view.
might be explained by a different load of working memory
in CANTAB. SOC in the CANTAB, with the maximum
five-move task, required less load of (working) memory
Limitations
than the Spatial Span, with maximum nine digits, and The findings shall be interpreted with some limitations.
SWM, with maximum eight-box tasks. It was possible that First, the participants were either clinic-based subjects or
adults with late-onset ADHD had deficits in spatial (work- recruited via advertisement from the same university hospi-
ing) memory but not planning in the lab setting, and a load tal. Thus, the generalization of the results is questionable.
of spatial (working) memory in the SOC task did not pro- Second, the intelligence level was relatively high in our
voke impaired performance in the late-onset ADHD. sample, and therefore our results could not be generalized
Further studies with well-controlled tasks for the divided to the population of lower intelligence. Third, this study
neuropsychological mechanisms with different loads are might suffer from recall bias of age-of-onset. Fourth,
needed to verify our hypothesis. although we asked the participants who were currently
10 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

treated with medication to hold it for more than 48 hr before American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statis-
they received CANTAB tests, we could not rule out the pos- tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
sibility of residual or withdrawal effect of medication. Fifth, Author.
the sample size of the late-onset ADHD group was rela- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and sta-
tistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.).
tively small and might lack the statistical power to detect
Washington, DC: Author.
the small group difference, especially between early- and
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and sta-
late-onset ADHD. Further longitudinal study with a larger tistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA:
sample size of the late-onset group would be helpful to elu- American Psychiatric Publishing.
cidate the difference between early- and late-onset ADHD Applegate, B., Lahey, B. B., Hart, E. L., Biederman, J., Hynd,
and eliminate the recall bias. G. W., Barkley, R. A., . . .Shaffer, D. (1997). Validity of the
age-of-onset criterion for ADHD: A report from the DSM-IV
field trials. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Conclusion Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1211-1221.
Our study showed that adults with late-onset ADHD had Barkley, R. A., & Biederman, J. (1997). Toward a broader defini-
comparable neuropsychological deficits, including sus- tion of the age-of-onset criterion for attention-deficit hyperac-
tivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
tained attention, signal detectability, spatial working mem-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1204-1210.
ory, short-term spatial memory, and set-shifting as their
Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2002).
early-onset counterparts and these deficits could not be The persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into
explained by the presence of other psychiatric comorbidi- young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition
ties. Therefore, extending the age-of-onset criterion for of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 279-289.
adults did not overdiagnose ADHD in adults, even based on Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2006).
the recalled information. Adults with late-onset ADHD had Young adult outcome of hyperactive children: Adaptive
no deficits in spatial planning and alertness, and the preser- functioning in major life activities. Journal of the American
vation of these neuropsychological abilities might postpone Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 192-202.
the true or recall of occurrences of ADHD-related difficul- Batstra, L., & Frances, A. (2012). DSM-5 further inflates attention
ties during the development. Further longitudinal studies deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 200, 486-488.
are warranted to elucidate the role of the interactions
Baune, B. T., Fuhr, M., Air, T., & Hering, C. (2014).
between brain functions and the environment in the onset of
Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents and young
ADHD at a developmental perspective. adults with major depressive disorder—A review. Psychiatry
Research, 218, 261-271.
Acknowledgments Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Norman,
We would like to express our thanks to Ms. Yu-Lun Lin for data D., Lapey, K. A., . . .Doyle, A. (1993). Patterns of psychi-
management and psychiatric interviews, and all participants for atric comorbidity, cognition, and psychosocial functioning
their time and efforts. in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The
American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1792-1798.
Biederman, J., Mick, E., Spencer, T., Surman, C., Hammerness,
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
P., Doyle, R., . . .Schweitzer, K. (2006). An open-label trial of
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with OROS methylphenidate in adults with late-onset ADHD. CNS
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this Spectrums, 11, 390-396.
article. Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of atten-
tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endopheno-
Funding types. Nature Review Neuroscience, 3, 617-628.
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support Chandra, S., Biederman, J., & Faraone, S. (2016). Assessing the
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This validity of the age at onset criterion for diagnosing ADHD in
work was supported by National Health Research Institute (NHRI- DSM-5. Journal of Attention Disorders. Advance online publi-
EX100-10008PI, NHRI-EX101-10008PI, NHRI-EX102-10008PI; cation. doi: 10.1177/1087054716629717
NHRI-EX103-10008PI), Taiwan. Conners, C. K., Erhardt, D., & Sparrow, E. (1999). Conners’
Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). New York, NY: Multi-
Health Systems.
References
Cortese, S., Kelly, C., Chabernaud, C., Proal, E., Di Martino, A.,
Adler, L. A., & Chua, H. C. (2002). Management of ADHD in Milham, M. P., . . .Castellanos, F. X. (2012). Toward systems
adults. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 63(Suppl. 12), 29-35. neuroscience of ADHD: A meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies.
Alderson, R. M., Kasper, L. J., Hudec, K. L., & Patros, C. H. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 1038-1055.
(2013). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Doyle, A., Murray, K., Petty, C.,
and working memory in adults: A meta-analytic review. Adamson, J. J., . . .Seidman, L. (2006a). Neuropsychological
Neuropsychology, 27, 287-302. studies of late onset and subthreshold diagnoses of
Lin and Gau 11

adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Levaux, M. N., Potvin, S., Sepehry, A. A., Sablier, J., Mendrek,
Psychiatry, 60, 1081-1087. A., & Stip, E. (2007). Computerized assessment of cogni-
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Mick, E., Murray, tion in schizophrenia: Promises and pitfalls of CANTAB.
K., Petty, C., . . .Monuteaux, M. C. (2006b). Diagnosing European Psychiatry, 22, 104-115.
adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Are late onset Lin, Y. J., Chen, W. J., & Gau, S. S. (2013). Neuropsychological
and subthreshold diagnoses valid? The American Journal of functions among adolescents with persistent, subsyndro-
Psychiatry, 163, 1720-1729. mal and remitted attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Seidman, Psychological Medicine, 27, 1-13.
L. J., Mick, E., . . .Doyle, A. E. (2000). Attention-deficit/ Lin, Y. J., & Gau, S. S. (2017). Differential neuropsychological
hyperactivity disorder in adults: An overview. Biological functioning between adolescents with attention-deficit/hyper-
Psychiatry, 48, 9-20. activity disorder with and without conduct disorder. Journal
Fried, R., Hirshfeld-Becker, D., Petty, C., Batchelder, H., & of the Formosan Medical Association. Advance online publi-
Biederman, J. (2015). How informative is the CANTAB to cation. doi:10.1016/j.jfma.2017.02.009
assess executive functioning in children with ADHD? A con- Lin, Y. J., Lo, K. W., Yang, L. K., & Gau, S. S. (2015). Validation
trolled study. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19, 468-475. of DSM-5 age-of-onset criterion of attention deficit/hyperac-
Gau, S. S. (2007). Parental and family factors for attention-deficit tivity disorder (ADHD) in adults: Comparison of life qual-
hyperactivity disorder in Taiwanese children. Australian & ity, functional impairment, and family function. Research in
New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 41, 688-696. Developmental Disabilities, 47, 48-60.
Gau, S. S., & Chang, J. P. (2013). Maternal parenting styles Lin, Y. J., Yang, L. K., & Gau, S. S. (2016). Psychiatric comor-
and mother-child relationship among adolescents with and bidities of adults with early- and late-onset attention-deficit/
without persistent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. hyperactivity disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 1581-1594. of Psychiatry, 50, 548-556.
Gau, S. S., Chiu, C. D., Shang, C. Y., Cheng, A. T., & Soong, Luciana, M. (2003). Practitioner review: Computerized assess-
W. T. (2009). Executive function in adolescence among chil- ment of neuropsychological function in children: Clinical and
dren with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Taiwan. research applications of the Cambridge Neuropsychological
Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 30, Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB). The Journal of Child
525-534. Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 44, 649-
Gau, S. S., & Huang, W. L. (2014). Rapid visual information 663.
processing as a cognitive endophenotype of attention deficit Luciana, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1998). The functional emergence
hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine, 44, 435-446. of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to
Gau, S. S., & Shang, C. Y. (2010). Executive functions as eight-year-old children. Neuropsychologia, 36, 273-293.
endophenotypes in ADHD: Evidence from the Cambridge Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the localization
Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB). The Journal of of psychological processes in man. British Medical Bulletin,
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 51, 27, 272-277.
838-849. Moffitt, T. E., Houts, R., Asherson, P., Belsky, D. W., Corcoran,
Hart, H., Radua, J., Nakao, T., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. D. L., Hammerle, M., . . .Caspi, A. (2015). Is adult ADHD
(2013). Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence
imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention- from a four-decade longitudinal cohort study. The American
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Exploring task-specific, Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 967-977.
stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry, Ni, H. C., Lin, Y. J., Gau, S. S., Huang, H. C., & Yang, L. K.
70, 185-198. (2017). An open-label, randomized trial of methylphenidate
Hervey, A. S., Epstein, J. N., & Curry, J. F. (2004). and atomoxetine treatment in adults with ADHD. Journal of
Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactiv- Attention Disorders, 21, 27-39.
ity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 18, Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder?
485-503. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 571-598.
Hyman, S. E. (2007). Can neuroscience be integrated into the Nigg, J. T. (2005). Neuropsychologic theory and findings
DSM-V? Nature Review Neuroscience, 8, 725-732. in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The state of
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., the field and salient challenges for the coming decade.
& Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1424-1435. doi:10.1016/j.bio-
distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity psych.2004.11.011
Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, Olton, D. S. (1987). The radial arm maze as a tool in behavioral
593-602. pharmacology. Physiology & Behavior, 40, 793-797.
Kieling, C., Kieling, R. R., Rohde, L. A., Frick, P. J., Moffitt, T., Owen, A. M., Doyon, J., Petrides, M., & Evans, A. C. (1996).
Nigg, J. T., . . .Castellanos, F. X. (2010). The age at onset of Planning and spatial working memory: A positron emis-
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The American Journal sion tomography study in humans. European Journal of
of Psychiatry, 167, 14-16. Neuroscience, 8, 353-364.
Klingberg, T. (2006). Development of a superior frontal- Petrides, M., & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered
intraparietal network for visuo-spatial working memory. tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man.
Neuropsychologia, 44, 2171-2177. Neuropsychologia, 20, 249-262.
12 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

Polanczyk, G., Caspi, A., Houts, R., Kollins, S. H., Rohde, L. A., Todd, R. D., Huang, H., & Henderson, C. A. (2008). Poor utility of
& Moffitt, T. E. (2010). Implications of extending the ADHD the age of onset criterion for DSM-IV attention deficit/hyper-
age-of-onset criterion to age 12: Results from a prospectively activity disorder: Recommendations for DSM-V and ICD-11.
studied birth cohort. Journal of the American Academy of The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 210-216. Disciplines, 49, 942-949.
Pribram, K. H., & McGuinness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation, Vaidya, C. J. (2012). Neurodevelopmental abnormalities in
and effort in the control of attention. Psychological Review, ADHD. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 9,
82, 116-149. 49-66.
Prosser, B., & Reid, R. (2013). The DSM-5 changes and ADHD: van der Meere, J., Shalev, R., Borger, N., & Gross-Tsur, V.
More than a tweak of terms. Australian & New Zealand (1995). Sustained attention, activation and MPH in ADHD:
Journal of Psychiatry, 47, 1196-1197.
A research note. The Journal of Child Psychology and
Reinhardt, M. C., Benetti, L., Victor, M. M., Grevet, E. H.,
Psychiatry, 36, 697-703.
Belmonte-de-Abreu, P., Faraone, S. V., . . .Rohde, L. A.
Vande Voort, J. L., He, J. P., Jameson, N. D., & Merikangas, K. R.
(2007). Is age-at-onset criterion relevant for the response to
(2014). Impact of the DSM-5 attention-deficit/hyperactivity
methylphenidate in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder?
The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 68, 1109-1116. disorder age-of-onset criterion in the US adolescent popula-
Rhodes, S. M., Coghill, D. R., & Matthews, K. (2005). tion. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Neuropsychological functioning in stimulant-naive boys with Psychiatry, 53, 736-744.
hyperkinetic disorder. Psychological Medicine, 35, 1109-1120. Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., &
Rohde, L. A., Biederman, J., Zimmermann, H., Schmitz, M., Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function
Martins, S., & Tramontina, S. (2000). Exploring ADHD age- theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-
of-onset criterion in Brazilian adolescents. European Child & analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336-1346.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, 212-218. Willoughby, M. T., Curran, P. J., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A.
Sahgal, A. (1987). Some limitations of indices derived from sig- (2000). Implications of early versus late onset of attention-
nal detection theory: Evaluation of an alternative index for deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Journal of the
measuring bias in memory tasks. Psychopharmacology, 91, American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39,
517-520. 1512-1519.
Schoechlin, C., & Engel, R. R. (2005). Neuropsychological
performance in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disor- Author Biographies
der: Meta-analysis of empirical data. Archives of Clinical
Yu-Ju Lin, MD, MS in epidemiology, is a board-certificated child
Neuropsycholog, 20, 727-744.
Seidman, L. J. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in people psychiatrist. She has worked as child psychiatrist in Far Eastern
with ADHD across the lifespan. Clinical Psychology Review, Memorial Hospital, a medical center in North Taiwan and is the
26, 466-485. adjunct visiting physician in National Taiwan University Hospital
Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Weber, W., Hatch, M., & Faraone, S. and a lecturer in National Taiwan University. Her research interest
V. (1998). Neuropsychological function in adults with atten- is in epidemiology, symptomatology, and neuropsychological
tion-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 44, functioning of ADHD across life span.
260-268.
Susan Shur-Fen Gau, MD, PhD, is the director of Department of
Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., & Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is
a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperac- Medical Genetics and ex-director of Department of Psychiatry,
tivity disorder? Behavioural Brain Research, 130, 3-28. National Taiwan University Hospital and the professor of the
Shaffer, D. (1994). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Department of Psychiatry, Department of Psychology, School of
adults. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 633-638. Occupational Therapy, Graduate Institute of Brain and Mind
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/ Sciences, Graduate Institute of Epidemiology and Preventive
hyperactivity disorder: From common simple deficits to mul- Medicine, and Graduate Institute of Clinical Medicine, National
tiple developmental pathways. Biological Psychiatry, 57, Taiwan University, Taiwan. She has been conducting the clinical,
1231-1238. follow-up, neuropsychological, treatment, neuroimage, and
Tannock, R. (2013). Rethinking ADHD and LD in DSM-5: genetic studies on ADHD and autism spectrum disorder as well as
Proposed changes in diagnostic criteria. Journal of Learning national survey of mental disorders in children and adolescents in
Disabilities, 46, 5-25. Taiwan.

You might also like