Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 2000; 28: 119–25 Copyright C Munksgaard 2000

Printed in Denmark . All rights reserved

ISSN 0301-5661

Manal A. Awad1,
Determinants of patients’ Stanley H. Shapiro2,3,
James P. Lund1,4,5 and
Jocelyne S. Feine1,2,3
treatment preferences in a clinical 1
2
McGill University Faculty of Dentistry,
McGill University Faculty of Medicine, Joint
Departments of Epidemiology and

trial Biostatistics, and Occupational Health,


3
Jewish General Hospital, Lady Davis
Institute for Medical Research, 4McGill
University Faculty of Medicine, Department
of Physiology, 5Université de Montréal, Centre
de recherche en science neurologiques,
Monatreal, Quebec, Canada

Awad MA, Shapiro SH, Lund JP, Feine JS: Determinants of patients’ treatment
preferences in a clinical trial. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 2000; 28: 119–25.
C Munksgaard, 2000

Abstract – Several researchers have suggested that patients’ preferences for a


particular form of treatment should be taken into account in clinical trials. Prefer-
ences may influence the outcome of treatment, especially in trials when patients
cannot be blinded to the type of treatment received and the outcome is based on
patients’ evaluations of therapy. Participants in this study were 136 edentulous
patients who took part in a randomised controlled clinical trial comparing two
types of treatments for edentulism: conventional dentures and implant-support-
ed prostheses. Prior to receiving treatment, subjects were required to complete a
questionnaire regarding their satisfaction with their present prostheses. In addi-
tion, they were asked to indicate which treatment they would prefer if given a
choice. The objective of this study was to determine whether there are important
differences among study participants between patients who have a treatment pref-
erence and those who do not. The effects of satisfaction with pre-treatment pros-
theses, age, gender and level of education on preferences were examined. Level of
satisfaction with the original dentures and level of education were significant
predictors of preference. Compared to subjects who rated their satisfaction with
their current condition as ‘low’, the odds ratios associated with having a prefer-
ence for implant treatment were 0.31 (95% CI: 0.09 to 0.96) for subjects who rated
their prostheses in the ‘medium’ range and 0.11 (95% CI: 0.03 to 0.41) for those
Key words: clinical trials; oral prostheses;
who rated in the ‘high’ range. In addition, subjects with high levels of education study designs; treatment preferences
were significantly less likely to have a preference for either conventional or im-
plant treatments (ORΩ0.18, 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.77 and ORΩ0.20, 95% CI: 0.05 to 0.76, Jocelyne S. Feine, McGill University,
3640 University Street, Montreal,
respectively) compared to those with low education. Neither age nor gender was Quebec H3A 2B2, Canada
a significant predictor of preference. We suggest that study designs which incorpo- Tel: π514 398 7203
rate patients’ preferences must take into account possible differences between Fax: π514 398 8900
preference groups that might confound the relationship between preference and e-mail: feine/med.mcgill.ca
the outcome of interest. Accepted for publication 27 August 1999

The importance of considering patients’ preferenc- decisions about their health care (1), and it seems
es for a particular form of treatment has been a to be generally accepted that patients should be
topic of debate (1–7). It is clear that this is an im- given the opportunity to select their preferred
portant issue in daily clinical practice, and it is now treatment whenever possible from options that
emerging as a major concern in clinical trial design. have already been shown to be appropriate and ef-
Patients are becoming more active participants in ficacious (2). However, when the efficacy of treat-

119
Awad et al.

ment is assessed in clinical trials, preference is not treatment among 273 women suffering from heavy
taken into account. This has led some authors to menstrual bleeding. One hundred and thirty-five
question the generalisability estimates of the effec- subjects were randomly assigned to a partially ran-
tiveness of therapy from trials (3–12). Patient pref- domised patient preference trial (PRPP) in which
erence can also cause other problems in clinical patients who prefer a particular treatment are giv-
trials (5, 6). For example, people with strong prefer- en their choice, while those who have no prefer-
ences for one treatment may decide not to partici- ence are randomly assigned to either treatment
pate in a randomised trial. If they do decide to en- (12). Another 138 subjects were assigned to a con-
rol in a non-blinded trial, the motivation of those ventional randomised controlled trial (CRCT). Sub-
who are not given their treatment of choice may be jects randomised to the PRPP were asked about
low, leading to drop-outs or non-compliance. To- their preferences and were given the treatment of
gether, the effects of these phenomena would re- their choice, while those who expressed no prefer-
duce the general applicability of the results. Allow- ence were randomly allocated to receive either
ance for preference may be particularly important treatment. Five subjects (3%) refused to participate
in non-blinded or poorly blinded randomised clin- in the PRPP arm, while in the CRCT group, 30% of
ical trials (RCTs) that use patient-based outcomes, the subjects refused participation because of strong
because it is probable that patients’ emotional re- preference, suggesting that subjects who agreed to
sponses to treatment assignment would influence participate in the CRCT were women who did not
the estimates of effectiveness. For instance, pa- have strong preferences. This hypothesis is sup-
tients’ ‘‘disappointment’’ at having been assigned ported by the authors, who report that no statis-
a non-preferred treatment may be reflected in their tically significant difference was observed between
ratings of satisfaction. subjects who remained in the CRCT and those who
McPherson et al. (11) developed a theoretical were randomly allocated to treatments in the PRPP
model to evaluate the effect of preference in RCTs. group. However, women who selected the medical
In this model, the treatment effect is not simply the intervention were significantly more likely to find
measured ‘‘biologic’’ difference between treat- the treatment acceptable than women who were
ments; the difference is modified depending on randomly allocated to it. This difference was not
whether subjects receive their preferred treatment observed among the women who chose the surgi-
and by the proportion of subjects who prefer each cal procedure and those who were randomised to
treatment. According to this model, the effect of the this treatment. The authors suggested that women
‘‘biologically’’ superior treatment can be overesti- who selected the surgical intervention had high ex-
mated if a higher proportion of patients prefer that pectations for this treatment but suffered more,
treatment. On the other hand, underestimation of and their disappointment led to these results. In-
effectiveness would occur if more patients ex- formation regarding the role of preference on out-
pressed a preference for the inferior treatment. come would not have been possible if patient pref-
Consequently, Brewin & Bradley (12) suggested erence were not part of this trial design.
that studies should be designed to achieve a ‘‘good Henshaw et al. (16) measured the preferences of
fit’’ between patients’ preferences and the type of women for two methods of abortion (mifepri-
treatment received. stone/gemeprost or vacuum aspiration). The re-
Clearly the RCT is the most appropriate design sults showed that both interventions were highly
when the efficacy of a particular form of therapy is acceptable to women who were assigned to the
being first established, or when the probabilities of procedure that they preferred. On the other hand,
different outcomes associated with each treatment the surgical procedure was more acceptable than
modality are unknown (13). However, once efficacy the medical intervention among subjects who were
has been estimated and the objective is to ascertain randomly allocated to treatment. The authors con-
which form of therapy would yield better results in cluded that women who prefer a specific method
the field, then including preference into the study of terminating a pregnancy should be given their
design creates conditions that more closely resem- choice, while women who are not sure which pro-
ble clinical practice. As Till et al. (14) pointed out, cedure they prefer might be offered the surgical op-
preference trials should be viewed as supplements tion.
to RCTs rather than alternatives. However, conclusions from studies such as these
Cooper et al. (15) recently examined the relation- that consider patients’ preferences in the evalua-
ship between preferences and satisfaction with tion of treatment outcome could be misleading if

120
Patients’ treatment preferences and study designs

there are differences in other potential explanatory fort, ease of chewing, stability, ability to speak and
variables between patients with preference for aesthetics of their oral prostheses explained 89% of
treatments and those without preferences (11). This the variation in ratings of general satisfaction (17).
led to our attempt to determine whether there are Therefore, ratings of general satisfaction were used
factors that distinguish between patients who ex- in this analysis as an indicator of their current oral
press a preference and those who have no prefer- condition.
ence for one of two types of treatment for eden- It was conceivable that patients who were rela-
tulism. tively satisfied with their present condition might
express a preference for the same form of treat-
ment. In contrast, those who were very dissatisfied
Material and methods
might prefer the alternative treatment, while those
Participants in this study were patients who were in the middle of the scale might be neutral. There-
originally recruited to evaluate the efficacy of two fore, it was decided to divide the variable ‘overall
types of treatments for the mandible: a standard satisfaction’ into three levels: low, medium and
oral prosthesis and a prosthesis partially supported high (0–25 mm, 26–75 mm, 76–100 mm, respec-
by two endosseous implants. The efficacy of this tively). It was assumed that the effect of age on
type of implant-supported prosthesis had not been preference would more likely depend on whether
previously evaluated in a randomised controlled the patients are considered relatively young or old
clinical trial. with respect to a chronic condition like edentulism.
One hundred and thirty-six male and female A 40-year-old edentulous subject would be consid-
edentulous subjects seeking replacement of their ered relatively young to be missing all of his or her
standard oral prostheses responded to an invita- teeth. Therefore, the mean age (approximately 50
tion to participate in this clinical trial. For patients years) was taken as a cut-off point above which it is
to be eligible to participate in this study, they had more common to find people who are edentulous.
to fulfil the following criteria: 1) must be wearing Level of education was initially defined by four
their conventional dentures on a regular basis; 2) categories: less than 7 years of education, 8–12
be between the ages of 35 and 65 years; 3) have years of education, 13–15 years (technical or college
been edentulous for at least 10 years. Subjects who degree) and 16 years or more (university). How-
met these criteria attended an information session ever, due to sample size considerations, level of
in which they were told that the study was a ran- education was defined as ‘high’ when subjects
domised controlled clinical trial to compare the ef- completed university or college, and ‘low’ when
ficacy of two types of treatments for the mandible. they did not (12 or fewer years of education).
They were given a description of the procedures Chi-square tests were used to evaluate whether
involved with each treatment modality, and in- the univariate distribution of age, gender, level of
formed that there was no evidence to suggest that education and satisfaction with current prostheses
an implant prosthesis using only two implants is differed according to preference status. Additional
better than a well-made standard prosthesis. They multivariate polytomous regression was carried
were asked to complete a questionnaire on socio- out to evaluate the multivariate association be-
demographic factors (age, sex, and level of educa- tween type of preference and subjects’ characteris-
tion) and to indicate which treatment they would tics. This method of analysis allows for the assess-
prefer to receive if given a choice. In addition, pa- ment of nominal scaled outcome variables with
tients were asked to rate their level of general satis- more than two categories. Analyses were imple-
faction, comfort, ability to chew, stability, ability to mented using the statistical package SAS (18).
speak and aesthetics of their present oral prosthesis
on a 100-mm Visual Analogue Scale (VAS), with
Results
higher values indicating greater satisfaction.
Table 1 shows the distribution of subjects according
Analysis to gender, age, level of education and ratings of
This analysis focused on estimating the effect of level of satisfaction with the present oral prosthe-
gender, level of education, age and general satisfac- ses. The majority of the subjects (79%) expressed a
tion with the original conventional dentures on treatment preference, 19% for the conventional and
subjects’ preference for treatment. Previously, we 60% for the implant treatments. Most subjects who
had shown that subjects’ ratings of level of com- preferred the implant treatment were patients 50

121
Awad et al.

Table 1. Associations between patients’ characteristics and preferences

Preferences N (%)
Neutral Conventional Implant
Variables 28 (21) 26 (19) 82 (60) P-value

Gender
Females 11 (39) 17 (65) 43 (53) 0.15
Males 17 (61) 9 (35) 39 (47)
Age
∞50 years 15 (54) 11 (42) 24 (29) 0.05
Ø50 years 13 (46) 15 (58) 58 (71)
Education level
High* 25 (89) 14 (54) 46 (56) 0.004
Low† 3 (11) 12 (46) 36 (44)
Level of satisfaction‡
Low (0–25 mm) 6 (22) 12 (46) 50 (61) 0.002
Medium (26–75 mm) 11 (39) 7 (27) 23 (28)
High (76–100 mm) 11 (39) 7 (27) 9 (11)

* University or college completed.



University or college degree incomplete.

Rating based on 100-mm Visual Analogue Scale (VAS).

Table 2. Multivariate adjusted odds ratios (ORs)* according to preference and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for patients’
characteristics

Variables OR† CI P-value OR‡ CI P-value

Gender
Males§
Females 2.86 0.89, 2.22 0.08 1.86 0.69, 4.90 0.23
Age
∞50 years§
Ø50 years 1.43 0.44, 4.66 0.54 2.33 0.88, 6.23 0.09
Education level
Low§
High 0.18 0.04, 0.77 0.02 0.20 0.05, 0.76 0.02
Level of satisfaction
Low (0–25 mm)§
Medium (26–75 mm) 0.41 0.10, 1.67 0.22 0.31 0.09, 0.96 0.05
High (76–100 mm) 0.36 0.08, 1.54 0.17 0.11 0.03, 0.41 0.001

* ORs estimated from polytomous logistic regression including all the above variables, using neutrality as reference outcome
category.

Estimated ORs for preference for conventional treatment.

Estimated ORs for preference implant treatment.
§
Reference category.

years and older (71%); while 46% of patients who neutral group rated their overall satisfaction with
were neutral and 58% of subjects who preferred their present prostheses in the ‘medium’ (39%) and
conventional prostheses were in this age category ‘high’ category (39%), while significantly more sub-
(PΩ0.05). The majority of patients who were neu- jects who preferred implant treatment (61%) rated
tral had a college or university degree (89%). their present prostheses in the ‘low’ category (PΩ
Slightly more than half of those who preferred the 0.002).
newer treatment or the conventional treatment had Table 2 displays the findings using polytomous
a college or university degree (56% and 54% re- logistic regression analysis. This model allowed for
spectively, PΩ0.004). The majority of subjects in the the simultaneous estimation of the odds ratios

122
Patients’ treatment preferences and study designs

(ORs) for preference for implant treatment or stan- without preferences, it is equally important to con-
dard treatment, using neutrality regarding treat- sider the form of the preferred therapy (Table 2).
ments as the reference outcome category. Although The belief that preferences for treatment need to
the ORs for the association between preferences be considered when estimating the potential effec-
and gender were not significantly different from 1, tiveness of unblinded therapies prompted Brew-
there was a tendency for females to prefer conven- in & Bradley (12) to propose a preference design
tional treatment (ORΩ2.86, 95% CI: 0.89 to 2.22, PΩ for clinical trials. In this form of preference con-
0.08). Likewise, compared to younger subjects, trolled trial (PCT), those patients who express no
older patients were more likely to prefer the im- strong treatment preference are randomised, while
plant treatment. However, the OR was not statis- those with preference are offered their choice. It
tically significant (ORΩ2.33, 95% CI: 0.88 to 6.23, was hypothesised that comparing the two arms in
PΩ0.09). As shown in Table 2, compared to ‘low’ the randomised group would provide similar re-
ratings of satisfaction, the odds ratios associated sults to those obtained from an RCT design, while
with preference for implant treatment were signi- comparing patients who selected a particular treat-
ficant for ratings of satisfaction in the ‘medium’ ment modality to those randomly allocated to the
range (ORΩ0.41, 95% CI: 0.09 to 0.96), as well as in same treatment would indicate the effect of moti-
the ‘high’ range (ORΩ0.11, 95% CI: 0.03 to 0.41). vational factors on outcome of treatment (5). This
The ORs associated with preference for standard proposed design is based on the assumption that
treatment did not significantly differ from 1. In ad- preference for treatment is independent of other
dition, compared to subjects with ‘low’ education factors. However, our results suggest that there are
(university or college degree incomplete), subjects factors that may confound the association between
with ‘high’ education (university or college degree preference and outcome that cannot be properly
completed) were significantly (P∞0.05) less likely controlled in the Brewin & Bradley design.
to have a preference for either conventional treat- Moreover, in many cases, confounding factors
ment (ORΩ0.18, 95% CI: 0.04 to 0.77) or implant are not known to the investigators prior to the trial
treatment (ORΩ0.20, 95% CI: 0.05 to 0.76). and therefore cannot be controlled in the analysis
of the data.
We found a significant association between level
of education and preference. The suggestion by
Discussion
Hack et al. (23) that well-educated patients might
It has been advocated strongly that therapeutic tend to be more knowledgeable about disease
choices should be based not only on predetermined seems to be a plausible explanation for this finding.
clinical criteria, but also on patient preferences (13, Subjects who are well educated might better
14, 19), especially since more and more studies are understand the reasons for conducting compara-
demonstrating that patients differ in their views of tive research and be more likely to accept our state-
possible outcomes (8, 20–22). ment that there was no strong evidence to suggest
Feine et al. (7) have previously discussed some that the new treatment was superior to the stan-
of the problems that may arise when patients’ pref- dard. Also, well-educated individuals might be less
erences are not taken into consideration. Using ex- susceptible to marketing claims for new treatments
amples from different studies where patients’ eval- and therefore tend to be neutral.
uations of treatment differed from that of the clini- A relationship between patients’ perception of
cians, they pointed out that, when subjects are their level of satisfaction and preference was dem-
given a choice, anxiety and disappointment may be onstrated in the study by Cooper et al. (15), who
reduced. This could be reflected in treatment out- found that women who preferred a surgical proce-
come. The authors (7) also mentioned preliminary dure for their heavy menstrual bleeding had sig-
results from the present study that suggested that nificantly higher baseline bleeding and pain scores
there were differences between patients who have than patients who had no preference or who pre-
a preference for a particular treatment for edentul- ferred the medical intervention. In the present
ism and those who did not. In this paper we have study, higher ratings of satisfaction were associated
presented the full analysis on the determinants of with less likelihood of having a treatment prefer-
treatment preferences among edentulous patients. ence. This suggests that patients who perceive their
These data show that, in addition to the previously previous treatment positively are willing to try ei-
(7) reported differences between those with and ther treatment, while those who are dissatisfied

123
Awad et al.

wish to have the implants. However, satisfaction fect of preference on the measured outcome. If one
with current prostheses did not distinguish be- wishes to exercise tighter control, then stratification
tween patients who prefer conventional treatment could be done for factors such as age and gender.
and those who were neutral. This finding is not In summary, our data suggest that there are dif-
surprising, as patients who opted for the standard ferences between patients who express preferences
treatment were as satisfied with this therapy as for one form of therapy and those who have no
those who had no treatment preference. At the con- strong preference. In our population, level of edu-
clusion of the trial, a sample of 50 subjects partici- cation and level of perceived satisfaction with their
pated in focus groups to gather qualitative infor- original treatment were the factors that distin-
mation that was used to complement and interpret guished the two groups.
quantitative data gathered during the trial. These
data provided some explanation for why subjects,
who were relatively satisfied with their original
Acknowledgements
prostheses and preferred the conventional treat- This study was funded by Canadian Medical Research Coun-
ment, wished to participate in the trial. They indi- cil Grant .UI-11195 and Health and Welfare Canada NHRDP
cated that they had confidence in studies done in Grant .6605–4409–402. We wish to thank Nobel Biocare Can-
ada Inc.A for its generous support.
university settings and that they knew that at some
point they would need a new prosthesis to replace
the one they currently wore. References
The association between age and preference was
1. Laine C, Davidoff F. Patient-centered medicine: a profes-
observed to be significant only in the univariate sional evolution. JAMA 1996;275:152–6.
analysis. After adjustment for other factors, age 2. Hlatky MA. Patient preferences and clinical guidelines.
was not as strongly correlated with preference (PΩ JAMA 1995;273:1219–20.
0.09), possibly because older patients were also less 3. Silverman WA, Altman DG. Patients‘ preferences and
randomized trials. Lancet 1996;347:171–4.
satisfied with their prostheses than younger ones. 4. Richards MA, Ramirez AJ, Degner LF, Fallowfields, Mah-
Likewise, there was a non-significant tendency for er EJ, Neuberger J. Offering choice of treatment to pa-
females to have a preference, especially for the con- tients with cancer. Eur J Cancer 1995;31:112–6.
5. Bradley C. Designing medical and educational interven-
ventional treatment (Table 2). However, with a
tion studies. Diabetes Care 1993;16:509–18.
larger sample size it may be possible that these 6. Kramer MS, Shapiro SH. Scientific challenges in the ap-
variables have significant effects on preference in a plication of randomized trials JAMA 1984;252:2739–45.
multivariate model. 7. Feine JS, Awad MA, Lund JP. The impact of patient pref-
erence on the design and interpetation of clinical trials.
Few researchers have disputed the impact of pa-
Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 1998;26:70–4.
tients’ characteristics on the choice of a particular 8. McNeil BJ, Pauker SG, Sox HC, Tversky A. On the elicita-
treatment modality, which in turn could affect rat- tion of preferences for alternative therapies. N Engl J
ings of treatment outcome (4). The present study Med 1982;306:1259–62.
9. Cassileth BR, Zupkis RV, Sutton-Smith K, March V. Infor-
addressed this issue by reporting on preferences mation and participation preferences among cancer pa-
for treatment and characteristics of edentulous sub- tients. Ann Int Med 1980;92:832–6.
jects participating in a clinical trial. In light of our 10. Degner LF, Sloan JA. Decision making during serious ill-
results, we suggest that the design advocated by ness: what role do patients really want to play? J Clin
Epidemiol 1992;45:941–50.
Brewin & Bradley (12) can lead to unreliable esti- 11. McPherson K, Britton AR, Wennberg JE. Are randomized
mates of the effect of preference on outcome meas- controlled trials controlled? Patient preferences and un-
ures, because differences in uncontrolled variables blinded trials. J R Soc Med 1997;90:652–6.
between neutral subjects assigned to the RCT arm 12. Brewin CR, Bradley C. Patient preferences and random-
ized clinical trials. Br Med J 1989;299:313–5.
and the subjects with preference in the PCT will 13. McPherson K. The best and the enemy of the good: ran-
modify the effects of preference in ways that are domized controlled trials, uncertainty, and assessing the
impossible to estimate. role of patient choice in medical decision making. J
Epidemiol Community Health 1994;48:6–15.
One possible solution is the design proposed by
14. Till JE, Sutherland HJ, Meslin EM. Is there a role for pref-
Wennberg et al. (24), who suggested the random erence assessments in research on quality of life in oncol-
allocation of subjects to either a PCT or a classic ogy? Qual Life Res 1992;1:31–40.
RCT. If the study is sufficiently large, this initial 15. Cooper KG, Grant AM, Garratt AM. The impact of using
partially randomized patient design when evaluating al-
randomisation should control for differences be-
ternative management for heavy menstrual bleeding. Br
tween the neutral and the preference groups and J Obstet Gynaecol 1997;104:1367–73.
allow a better estimate of the magnitude of the ef- 16. Henshaw RC, Naji SA, Russel T, Tempeton AA. Compar-

124
Patients’ treatment preferences and study designs

ison of medical abortion with surgical vaccum aspira- 22. Barry MJ, Mulley AG Jr, Fowler FJ, Wennberg JW. Watch-
tion: women’s preferences and acceptability of treatment. ful waiting vs immediate transurethral resection for
Br Med J 1993;307:714–7. symptomatic prostatism: the importance of patients’
17. Awad MA, Lund JP, Clokie CML, Dufresne E, Feine JS. preferences. JAMA 1988;259:3010–7.
Factors that affect patients’ general satisfaction with 23. Hack TF, Degner LF, Dyck DG. Relationship between
mandibular prostheses. J Dent Res 1997;76 Spec Iss: 2103. preferences for decisional control and illness information
18. Statistical analysis system (SAS). Version 6. 3rd ed. 1996. among women with breast cancer: a quantitative anal-
19. Angell M. Patients’ preferences in randomized clinical ysis. Soc Sci Med 1994;39:279–89.
trials. N Engl J Med 1984;310:1385–7. 24. Wennberg JE, Barry MJ, Fowler FJ, Mulley A. Outcomes
20. Kassirer J. Incorporating patients’ preferences into medi- research, PORTs and the dealth care reform. In: Warren
cal decisions. N Engl J Med 1994;330:1895–6. KS, Mosteller F, editors. Doing more good than harm.
21. Kassirer JP. Adding insult to injury: usurping patients’ The evaluation of the health care intervention. Ann N Y
prerogatives. N Engl J Med 1983;308:898–901. Acad Sci 1993;703:52–62.

125

You might also like