Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

Drug and Alcohol Dependence 89 (2007) 298–301

Short communication

Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse


control during cocaine abstinence
H.C. Fox ∗ , S.R. Axelrod, P. Paliwal, J. Sleeper, R. Sinha
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, Substance Abuse Center,
Connecticut Mental Health Center, 34 Park Street, New Haven, CT 06519,
United States
Received 14 September 2006; received in revised form 13 November 2006; accepted 11 December 2006

Abstract
Rationale: Prior research has shown that cocaine dependence is associated with dysfunction of brain systems involved in emotions and motivational
states.
Objectives: To examine whether difficulties in emotion regulation are associated with early cocaine abstinence using the recently validated
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS).
Method: Recently abstinent treatment-seeking cocaine patients (n = 60) completed the DERS during their first week of inpatient treatment and at
discharge (3–4 weeks later), and scores were compared with community controls (n = 50).
Results: Compared with controls, cocaine-dependent individuals reported difficulties relating to understanding emotions, managing emotions and
impulse control in the first week of abstinence. With continued abstinence, cocaine-dependent individuals showed continued difficulties only in
impulse control.
Conclusion: Cocaine-dependent individuals report emotion regulation difficulties, particularly during early abstinence. Additionally, protracted
distress-related impulse control problems suggest potential relapse vulnerability.
© 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Cocaine; Emotion regulation; Impulse control; Abstinence; Distress

1. Introduction a stressful situation (Gross et al., 2006). Although such strate-


gies may be considered beneficial for an individual to focus
Human neuroimaging studies have indicated that cocaine on long-term priorities, they are psychologically effortful and
abuse is associated with structural (Liu et al., 1998) and func- under stressful situations a shift in attention may occur towards
tional (Tucker et al., 2004) alterations in the anterior cingulate more immediate, often pleasure-seeking goals (Baumeister et
and regions of the prefrontal cortex (Volkow et al., 1992, 1999), al., 1994). Thus, immediate gratification seeking and procrasti-
with functional impairment in executive function during early nation occur in the context of efforts to emotionally self-regulate
abstinence (Fein et al., 2002). Interactions between these regu- (Tice et al., 2001).
latory control regions and emotion-related subcortical areas are This conflict in regulatory goals may have important impli-
also implicated in a broad network of systems associated with cations for treatment outcome in cocaine patients. First,
the experience and regulation of emotion (Ochsner and Gross, impulse control represents one of the major behavioral ele-
2005). ments of emotion regulation (Gratz and Roemer, 2004)
“Emotion regulation” refers to the strategies used to influ- and has been identified in a large number of studies as
ence, experience, and modulate emotions and may include an important component of addictive processes (Evenden,
dynamics such as suppression and/or cognitive re-appraisal of 1999). Secondly, cocaine abstinence involves a distress state
marked by dysphoria, irritability, fatigue, anxiety and increased
stress (Kampman et al., 2001; Sinha, 2001). As such, a
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 203 9747262; fax: +1 203 9747366. failure to manage affect and impulses may well be exac-
E-mail address: helen.fox@yale.edu (H.C. Fox). erbated in cocaine-dependent individuals with drug-related

0376-8716/$ – see front matter © 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.026
H.C. Fox et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 89 (2007) 298–301 299

neuron-adaptations in specific cortico-limbic neural cir- were used to confirm drug/alcohol abstinence. Controls participated in face-
cuitry. to-face appointments to complete both psychiatric interviews and the DERS at
Using a recently developed and validated measure of baseline only.
emotion regulation, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation
2.3. Measures
Scale (DERS), this study aims to examine emotion regulation
changes over time in newly abstinent treatment-seeking cocaine- 2.3.1. Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (Gratz and Roemer, 2004). The
dependent individuals, compared with community-controlled DERS is a 36-item self-report measure developed to examine difficulties in the
volunteers. ability to regulate emotions. Participants rate how often statements such as “I feel
at ease with my emotions” apply to them, where 1 is “almost never (0–10%)”,
2. Method 2 is “sometimes (11–35%)”, 3 is “about half the time (36–65%)”, 4 is “most of
the time (66–90%),” and 5 is “almost always (91–100%)”. Subscales assess six
dimensions of difficulties (subscales and sample items from each subscale are
2.1. Participants in Table 1).

Sixty treatment-seeking cocaine-dependent individuals (28F/32M) and 50


2.4. Design and statistical analyses
healthy volunteers (24F/26M) were recruited through local advertisements.
Cocaine patients met DSM-IV criteria for current cocaine dependence and
tested positive for cocaine in their urine toxicology screens upon entry into Independent t-tests or χ2 were used to assess group differences in drug use
a locked inpatient facility for 3–4 weeks of treatment and study participation. and demographics. Any group differences in demographic characteristics were
Cocaine patients had no access to drugs/alcohol and limited access to visi- included as co-variates in analyses of co-variance (ANCOVAs) which were used
tors. Exclusion criteria included dependence on substances other than cocaine, to assess group differences on the DERS at baseline and discharge. Paired t-tests
alcohol, or nicotine. Controls with current or past diagnoses of any substance were conducted to compare DERS differences at baseline and discharge within
dependence were also excluded. All participants were excluded if they required the cocaine sample.
psychiatric medications or were not in good health. The study was approved As emotion dysregulation is central to the pathophysiology of borderline
by the Human Investigation Committee of the Yale University School of personality disorder (BPD; Linehan, 1993) and BPD is frequently co-morbid
Medicine. with cocaine abuse (Kranzler et al., 1994), we also conducted the same analyses
excluding the subgroup of abstinent cocaine patients who met DSM-IV criteria
for BPD (n = 19/60).
2.2. Procedures

During the inpatient treatment phase, the baseline DERS assessment was 3. Results
conducted between days 4 and 7 of week 1 of cocaine abstinence and between
weeks 3 and 4, prior to discharge. Breathalyzer and urine toxicology screens 3.1. Participant characteristics

Table 1 Cocaine patients and control participants differed signif-


DERS subscales and sample items for the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation icantly by age (37.2 ± 5.9 and 33.6 ± 10.0; p = .02), racial
Scale status (66.7% and 46.0%; p = .001), and years of education
Nonacceptance of emotional responses (Nonacceptance) (12.3 ± 1.5 and 14.9 ± 2.6; p < .0001), with the cocaine patients
“When I’m upset, I feel ashamed at myself for feeling that way.” being somewhat older, higher proportion minority, and less
“When I’m upset, I become angry with myself for feeling that way.” educated.
Difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior (Goals)
The cocaine group had significantly greater years of cocaine
“When I’m upset, I have difficulty focusing on other things.” use (9.5 ± 6.1 versus 1.0 ± 1.4; p = .03) and days of cocaine use
“When I’m upset, I have difficulty getting work done.” in the past month (18.9 ± 8.7% versus 0 ± 0%; p < .0001), as
well as greater years of alcohol use (11.6 ± 7.5 versus 9.0 ± 8.7;
Impulse control difficulties (Impulse) p < .04) and days of alcohol use in the past month (9.6 ± 9.4 ver-
“When I’m upset I lose control over my behaviors.”
“I experience my emotions as overwhelming and out of control.”
sus 4.8 ± 6.4; p < .02). In addition, a higher number of cocaine
abusers met lifetime criteria for DSM-IV anxiety diagnoses
Lack of emotional awareness (Awareness) including PTSD (28.3% versus 4.0%; p = .005); although groups
“I pay attention to how I feel.” (Reverse-scored) did not differ in mood disorder diagnoses (11.7% versus 8%;
“When I am upset, I take time to figure out what I’m really feeling.” p = .90).
(Reverse-scored)

Limited access to emotion regulation strategies (Strategies) 3.2. Between group differences
“When I’m upset, I believe there is nothing I can do to make myself
feel better.” After adjusting for age, race and years of education, cocaine
“When I’m upset, I believe that wallowing in it is all I can do.” patients showed higher scores at baseline than controls on
Lack of emotional clarity (Clarity)
the Total DERS score (85.8 ± 22.5 compared to 60.9 ± 15.0;
“I have difficulty making sense out of my feelings.” p < .01), but no differences at discharge (77.6 ± 20.7 compared
“I have no idea how I am feeling.” to 60.9 ± 15.0). At baseline, the groups were also significantly
The Cronbach’s coefficient alpha for the cocaine-dependent participants at base-
different on Impulse, Awareness, Strategies and Clarity sub-
line in the current study was .88 for Total DERS, .86 for Nonacceptance, .80 for scales, with only Impulse and Awareness scores remaining
Goals, .78 for Impulse, .79 for Awareness, .85 for Strategy, and .72 for Clarity. significantly different at discharge (see Fig. 1). The exclu-
Adapted from Gratz and Roemer (2004). sion of cocaine patients with BPD resulted in a similar pattern
300 H.C. Fox et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 89 (2007) 298–301

dependent individuals also reported significantly higher scores


on the strategies subscale of the DERS compared with controls
suggesting greater difficulty in developing effective emotional
coping strategies (i.e. they would be more likely to believe that
little could be done to change an emotionally stressful situa-
tion). They were also found to report significantly higher scores
on the Impulse subscale of the DERS compared with controls,
indicating difficulties with regard to inhibiting inappropriate or
impulse behaviors under stressful situations.
Difficulties applying coping strategies and avoiding impul-
sive behavior at times of emotional distress indicate decrements
in emotional flexibility, and may reflect a change in priority from
self-control to affect regulation (Tice et al., 2001). Findings are
also consistent with previous research indicating that impulse
control disorders are more common in substance abusing indi-
viduals (Kisa et al., 2005) and decreased anterior cingulate
Fig. 1. DERS subscales: means and S.E.M. for cocaine patients compared with
activity during stress and a Go–NoGo task in cocaine patients
controls at baseline and discharge. Group differences at baseline—Awareness: compared to controls has been reported (Kaufman et al., 2004;
16.0 ± 5.0 compared to 12.0 ± 4.5; Clarity: 11.7 ± 3.8 compared to 8.3 ± 3.6; Sinha et al., 2005).
Impulse: 13.5 ± 4.8 compared to 8.3 ± 2.4; Strategies: 18.1 ± 6.4 compared to A significant improvement in selective regulatory aspects
11.9 ± 3.1. Group differences at discharge—Awareness: 15.8 ± 4.8 compared to was found in the cocaine patients at discharge. They reported
12.0 ± 4.5; Impulse: 11.1 ± 3.7 compared to 8.3. Improvement between baseline
and discharge in cocaine patients—Clarity: from 11.7 ± 3.8 to 9.8 ± 3.6; Goals:
improved clarity of their emotional experience and ability to
from 14.4 ± 4.6 to 12.6 ± 4.5; Strategies: 18.1 ± 6.4 compared to 16.0 ± 5.4. use strategies to regulate and remain focused on goal comple-
All analyses were co-varied for group differences in age, race and years of tion during distress states. Relative to controls, only problems
education. with impulse control remained elevated after accounting for
BPD. The clinical significance of distress-related impulsivity in
cocaine patients is highlighted within a recent study showing that
with the exception of Clarity not being significantly different impulsivity predicts adverse life events even when statistically
between groups at baseline, and only the Impulse control sub- controlling for substance use (Hayaki et al., 2005). Interestingly,
scale remaining significantly different at discharge1 . No gender our recent findings also indicate that stress-induced craving in
differences were observed in these findings. the laboratory is predictive of shorter time to cocaine relapse
(Sinha et al., 2006). Together, this implies susceptibility to
3.3. Within group differences impulsive behaviors such as drug use, lapses in judgment, poor
decision-making and an inability to control impulses during
From baseline to discharge comparisons, cocaine abusers stress. These findings highlight the need to develop treat-
reported significant improvement on the DERS Total score, the ment strategies focusing specifically on distress regulation and
Goals subscale, the Strategies subscale, and the Clarity sub- impulse control difficulties.
scale. However, there were no differences between baseline and It is important to note that the assessments were conducted
discharge DERS scores in cocaine abusers on the Awareness, over the initial few weeks of abstinence, and it is possible that
Acceptance and Impulse control subscales (see Fig. 1). the pattern of improved emotion regulation would have con-
tinued with sustained abstinence. Also 33% of the sample was
4. Discussion currently dependent on alcohol and 97% were cigarette smokers
suggesting that these findings may reflect the combined effects of
This is the first study to assess emotion regulation difficul- these drugs on emotion regulation differences. Nonetheless, the
ties in cocaine-dependent individuals during early abstinence. DERS instrument was able to discriminate between abstinent
Reported difficulties concerning emotional clarity and aware- cocaine patients and controls and reflect changes in emotion
ness compared with controls at admission suggest that cocaine regulation over the course of cocaine abstinence along with
abusers were less able to acknowledge and/or have a clear good internal consistency within a cocaine-dependent sample.
understanding of their emotions. Clarity and awareness of emo- These data would support the utility of the DERS as a diag-
tion could represent early processing components of emotional nostic tool to assess emotion regulation in treatment-seeking
competence (Salovey et al., 2002) and may be integral to cocaine-dependent individuals.
the maintenance of drug use in vulnerable groups. Cocaine-
Acknowledgements
1 As both control and cocaine patients differed in relation to anxiety disorder,

an extended analysis was also conducted excluding cocaine patients with BPD
This study was supported in part by grants K02-DA17232
and patients with anxiety disorder (n = 27 in total). Findings were no different (Sinha), P50-DA16556 (Sinha), M01-RR00125 (Yale GCRC)
to those shown in the cocaine sample excluding BPD individuals only. from the National Institutes of Health and its Office of Research
H.C. Fox et al. / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 89 (2007) 298–301 301

on Women’s Health (ORWH), Bethesda, MD, USA. We wish to Kranzler, H.R., Satel, S., Apter, A., 1994. Personality disorders and associ-
thank the staff at the Clinical Neuroscience Research Unit and ated features in cocaine-dependent inpatients. Compr. Psychiatry 35, 335–
the General Clinical Research Center at Yale University School 340.
Linehan, M.M., 1993. Dialectical behavior therapy for treatment of border-
of Medicine for their assistance in completing these studies. line personality disorder: implications for the treatment of substance abuse.
NIDA Res. Monogr. 137, 201–216.
References Liu, X., Vaupel, D.B., Grant, S., London, E.D., 1998. Effect of cocaine-related
environmental stimuli on the spontaneous electroencephalogram in polydrug
Baumeister, R.F., Heatherton, T.F., Tice, D.M., 1994. Loosing Control: How and abusers. Neuropsychopharmacology 19, 10–17.
Why People Fail at Self-regulation. Academic Press, San Diego, CA. Ochsner, K.N., Gross, J.J., 2005. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn.
Evenden, J., 1999. Impulsivity: a discussion of clinical and experimental find- Sci. 9, 242–249.
ings. J. Psychopharmacol. 13, 180–192. Salovey, P., Stroud, L.R., Woolery, A., Epel, E.S., 2002. Perceived emotional
Fein, G., Di Sclafani, V., Meyerhoff, D.J., 2002. Prefrontal cortical volume intelligence, stress reactivity, and symptom reports: further explorations
reduction associated with frontal cortex function deficit in 6-week abstinent using the trait Meta-mood scale. Psychol. Health 17, 611–627.
crack-cocaine dependent men. Drug Alcohol Depend. 68, 87–93. Sinha, R., 2001. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse?
Gratz, K.L., Roemer, L., 2004. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regula- Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 158 (4), 343–359.
tion and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation Sinha, R., Garcia, M., Paliwal, P., Kreek, M.J., Rounsaville, B.J., 2006.
of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Stress-induced cocaine craving and HPA responses predict cocaine relapse
Assess. 26, 41–54. outcomes. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 324–331.
Gross, J.J., Richards, J.M., John, O.P., 2006. Emotion regulation in everyday life. Sinha, R., Lacadie, C., Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R.K., Kosten, T.R., Rounsaville,
In: Snyder, D.K., Simpson, J.A., Hughes, J.N. (Eds.), Emotion Regulation B.J., Wexler, B.E., 2005. Neural correlates of stress-induced cocaine craving:
in Families: Pathways to Dysfunction and Health. American Psychological a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychopharmacology 183,
Association, Washington, DC, pp. 13–35. 171–180.
Hayaki, J., Stein, M.D., Lassor, J.A., Herman, D.S., Anderson, B.J., 2005. Adver- Tice, D.M., Bratslavsky, E., Baumeister, R.F., 2001. Emotional distress regula-
sity among drug users: relationship to impulsivity. Drug Alcohol Depend. tion takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! J. Pers.
78, 65–71. Soc. Psychol. 80, 53–67.
Kampman, K.M., Alterman, A.I., Volpicelli, J.R., Maany, I., Muller, E.S., Luce, Tucker, K.A., Browndyke, J.N., Gottschalk, P.C., Cofrancesco, A.T., Kosten,
D.D., Mulholland, E.M., Jawad, A.F., Parikh, G.A., Mulvaney, F.D., Wein- T.R., 2004. Gender-specific vulnerability for rCBF abnormalities among
rieb, R.M., O’Brien, C.P., 2001. Cocaine withdrawal symptoms and initial cocaine abusers. Neuroreport 15, 797–801.
urine toxicology results predict treatment attrition in outpatient cocaine Volkow, N.D., Fowler, J.S., Wang, G.J., 1999. Imaging studies on the role of
dependence treatment. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 15, 52–59. dopamine in cocaine reinforcement and addiction in humans. J. Psychophar-
Kaufman, J.N., Ross, T.J., Stein, E.A., Garavan, H., 2004. Cingulate hypoactiv- macol. 13, 337–345.
ity in cocaine users during a GO–NOGO task as revealed by event-related Volkow, N.D., Hitzemann, R., Wang, G.J., Fowler, J.S., Burr, G., Pascani,
functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 23, 7839–7843. K., Dewey, S.L., Wolf, A.P., 1992. Decreased brain metabolism in neu-
Kisa, C., Yildirim, S.G., Goka, E., 2005. Impulsivity and mental disorders. Turk rologically intact healthy alcoholics. Am. J. Psychiatry 149 (8), 1016–
Psikiyatri Derg. 16, 46–54. 1022.

You might also like