Buric & Frenzel (2019) Teacher Anger-New Empirical Insights Using A Multi-Method Approach

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Teaching and Teacher Education


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tate

Teacher anger: New empirical insights using a multi-method approach


c a, *, Anne C. Frenzel b
Irena Buri
a
Department of Psychology, University of Zadar, Croatia
b
Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany

h i g h l i g h t s

 Teacher anger was explored using a multi-method approach through a series of four studies.
 Teacher anger is a multi-faceted phenomenon evoked by students, parents, colleagues, and the educational system.
 Facets of teacher anger can be successfully measured via self-report.
 Facets of teacher anger correlate with teachers' emotional labor, well-being, and self-efficacy.

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Anger is a very common emotion for teachers with potential adverse effects for themselves and their
Received 18 December 2018 students. The aim of this research was to thoroughly explore teacher anger, determine and operationalize
Received in revised form its contextual triggers and components, and establish its relationships with teachers’ emotional labor,
8 July 2019
well-being, and self-efficacy. Through a series of four independent studies using diverse methodological
Accepted 1 August 2019
and analytical approaches and involving over 3000 teachers, it was established that teacher anger is a
Available online xxx
multi-faceted construct evoked by diverse contextual triggers (i.e. students, parents, colleagues,
educational system). These facets can be successfully assessed via self-report and meaningfully relate to
Keywords:
Teachers
external variables.
Anger © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Multi-method approach

1. Introduction teaching profession (Chang, 2009; 2013; Taxer & Frenzel, 2015).
Moreover, teacher anger is negatively related to instructional
Research on teachers' emotions indicate that teachers experi- effectiveness (Frenzel, Goetz, Pekrun, & Jacob, 2009) and the
ence a wide variety of emotions while teaching and interacting establishment of positive and supporting relationships with stu-
with students (for a review see Sutton & Wheatley, 2003) of both dents (Klassen, Perry, & Frenzel, 2012). For instance, a diary study
positive (e.g. joy, pride, love) and negative valence (e.g. anger, showed that anger has a potential to change teacher classroom
frustration, hopelessness, anxiety). Among the array of discrete behaviour by negatively affecting teacher concentration on their
negative emotions, anger is considered to be the most prominent teaching activities, planning for the class, and their relationships
one, both in life in general (Scherer, Wranik, Sangsue, Tran, & with students (Sutton, 2007). In addition, experiencing and dis-
Scherer, 2004) as well as among teachers (Buri c, Sliskovi
c & playing anger while teaching is often seen as undesirable and
Macuka, 2018; Chang, 2013; Frenzel, 2014; Hagenauer & Volet, inappropriate (Liljestrom, Roulston, & de Marais, 2007; Sutton,
2014; Keller, Frenzel, Goetz, Pekrun, & Hensley, 2014; Sutton, 2007), which is why teachers report to engage in emotion regula-
2007). Anger is not only prominent and frequent in teachers' tion in order to hold in their anger and keep themselves in check
everyday professional lives, but can also have adverse conse- (Sutton, 2007). However, some of the emotion regulation strategies
quences on teachers' psychological and professional functioning. used by teachers to regulate their anger (e.g., suppression or hiding
Research indicates that teacher anger is negatively related to feelings) can negatively influence their professional well-being
teacher well-being, but positively to burnout and to leaving the (e.g., Hülsheger & Schewe, 2011), their sense of efficacy (Sutton,
2007), and their teaching quality (Buri c, 2019). It is important to
note that all these findings on teacher anger are scattered across the
literature addressing teacher experiences and emotion regulation
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: iburic@unizd.hr (I. Buri
c). most generally. Therefore, the present paper seeks to provide a

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2019.102895
0742-051X/© 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
2 I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895

systematic, in-depth insight into the phenomenology, causes, and whether the students' behaviour in class corresponds with those
possible consequences of teacher anger. More precisely, the aim of goals. More specifically, teachers cognitively appraise the
this research is to thoroughly explore teacher anger, its contextual following: whether they succeeded in meeting their goals (i.e. goal
triggers and components, and its relationships with teachers' consistency); whether student behaviour is seen as beneficial for
emotional labor, well-being, and self-efficacy, by employing a attaining those goals (i.e. goal conduciveness); whether teachers
multi-method approach in a series of four independent studies. We view themselves as capable of attaining goals (i.e. coping poten-
suggest that understanding the contextual triggers and compo- tial); who is responsible for (non)attainment of goals (i.e. goal
nents of teacher anger and their relationships with teachers’ pro- attainment/impediment responsibility); and how important it is for
fessional functioning could prove useful for pre-service teacher teachers to achieve certain goal (i.e. goal importance). The model
education and/or in-service professional development programs postulates that anger is characterized by negative cognitive ap-
aimed at enhancement of teacher effectiveness through supporting praisals of goal consistency and conduciveness, coupled with
them in optimizing the regulation of their anger and thus preser- external goal impediment responsibility (Frenzel, 2014). The (little)
ving their well-being. existing empirical evidence on teacher anger provides support for
those claims, and shows that the teaching context can be a great
1.1. Conceptualization of teacher anger source of anger. For example, it has been shown that teachers
experience anger when students fail and teachers appraise this
Emotions, most generally, can be defined as a multicomponent failure as caused by intentionally low effort invested by the stu-
processes comprising subjective feelings (e.g. pleasure or displea- dents, or when teachers' instructional goals are obstructed by
sure), particular cognitive appraisal patterns, specific motivational students' seemingly deliberate misbehaviour, disrespectful behav-
tendencies, facial and bodily expressions, and physiological iour, or lack of motivation and commitment (Buri c et al, 2017;
changes (Lazarus, 2001; Scherer, 2009). When considering the Chang, 2013; Khajavi, Ghonsooly, Fatemi, & Frenzel, 2018; Sutton,
emotion of anger, it can be understood as unpleasant feeling that is 2007).
typically verbally described using terms such as “angry”, The described reciprocal model on causes and effects of teacher
“annoyed”, or “enraged”. Furthermore, anger is typically accom- emotions is somewhat limited as it only considers goals related to
panied with appraisals of other blame for blocked goals, and tends teaching and interacting with students. Yet, those are probably not
to motivate approach behaviours, that is, attack and aggression the only contexts with the capacity to evoke teacher anger. The
(Berkowitz, 1993). The expression of anger involves frowning eye- teaching job is highly complex and comprises of a multitude of
brows, dilating nostrils, clinching teeth, and a face that flushes red diverse tasks e with corresponding goals e that all have strong
(Harmon-Jones & Harmon-Jones, 2016). Physiologically, anger is potential to trigger a variety of emotions, including anger. The
typically accompanied by increases in cardiac contractility and abundance of teachers’ tasks and obligations was well illustrated by
heart rate, diastolic blood pressure, and finger temperature the OECD Teaching and Learning International Survey (TALIS, 2013)
(Cacioppo, Berntson, Larsen, Poehlman, & Ito, 2000). which showed that teachers do spend the majority of their working
Furthermore, emotions are typically either conceptualized as time in the classroom on teaching and scaffolding learning (ca. 50%
transient and short-lived states or as relatively stable traits (Bieg, of their working hours). However, they also engage in a multitude
2013). Traits can be understood as individuals' tendencies to of activities outside the classroom, such as planning and prepara-
frequently and intensively experience certain emotional states. As tion of lessons (18% of their working hours), correcting student
such, it is reasonable to make inferences from a person's trait work (13% of their working hours), student counselling and guid-
emotions about the emotions they will experience in a corre- ance, and extracurricular activities. Furthermore, teachers, on a
sponding context, and vice versa. Nevertheless, empirically, some regular basis, communicate and cooperate with parents and
discrepancies have been shown between emotions assessed as guardians, participate in team work and interact with colleagues,
states versus traits, while emotions operationalized in a trait-like and carry out a substantial amount of administrative work (up to 5%
manner have been shown to be more predictive of future behav- of their working hours; Burns & Darling-Hammond, 2014; OECD,
iours and choices (e.g. Levine, Lench, & Safer, 2009) than emotions 2014).
operationalized as states, and traits are much more economic in When it comes to school teachers and their anger in particular,
their assessment (single questionnaire which details the context as scattered existing research shows that there are indeed a variety of
opposed to multiple measurement occasions which need be factors outside the classroom that trigger feelings of anger. These
embedded “in situ” in the corresponding situations). Therefore, we factors include uncooperative colleagues, inappropriate parental
chose to focus on teachers' trait anger in the present contribution behaviour (e.g. carelessness and irresponsibility), as well as
by exploring and assessing teachers' reports of their typical (i.e., educational policies (e.g. educational reforms) that make teaching
usual, common) experiences of anger at their workplace. with high quality more difficult to attain (e.g. Liljestrom, Roulston,
While the basic components and appraisal patterns of anger are & Demarrais, 2007; Sutton & Wheatley, 2003). Therefore, more in-
probably universal for all humans, the key question addressed in depth exploration of the relevance of achieving tasks and goals that
this research was to explore the peculiarities of anger as experi- are not directly related to students and classroom activities in
enced by the particular population of teachers. For instance, while explaining teacher anger, poses as an important research question.
it is universally the case that anger occurs when others are blamed By extrapolating the reciprocal model on causes and effects of
for the blockage of one's goals, it remains unclear which circum- teacher emotions (Frenzel, 2014), it can be expected that teachers
stances are typically appraised by teachers in a way that they arouse also set goals for themselves that go beyond the classroom,
anger. According to the reciprocal model on causes and effects of including maintaining positive and constructive relationships with
teacher emotions (Frenzel, 2014; Jacob et al., 2017), teachers seek to colleagues and parents, successful collaboration with members of
achieve four overarching classroom goals, namely students' the school staff, professional development, and keeping up a sense
acquisition of subject-specific competences, students' motivational of being valued as a professional. Obstruction of such goals, that is,
engagement in classroom activities and learning, students' devel- situations and events that are cognitively appraised by teachers as
opment of socio-emotional competences, and the establishment of the ones that make their goals more difficult to attain, may
a well-functioning relationship with students. Teacher emotions, consequently result in the experience of teacher anger.
according to this model, develop as a result of their judgements
I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895 3

2. Correlates of teacher anger: emotional labor, well-being task by making mood-congruent thoughts more available
and self-efficacy (Kavanaugh & Bower, 1985). In turn, self-efficacy beliefs also impact
emotions. For instance, self-efficacy beliefs direct one's attention,
Above and beyond exploring contextual triggers of teacher shape the appraisals of emotional events, and influence the acti-
anger, the present research also sought to explore correlates of this vation of emotion regulation mechanisms (Chemers, Chu, & Garcia,
prominent teacher emotion. Specifically, we deemed teacher 2001). For the emotion of anger in particular, negative relationships
emotional labor, well-being, and self-efficacy to be important with self-efficacy can be expected and have been shown in prior
candidates to consider since they are proposed to be closely research (e.g. Buric & Macuka, 2018; Frenzel et al., 2016).
intertwined with teacher anger (e.g., Buri c, Sliskovic, & Penezi
c,
2019; Chang, 2009, 2013; Sutton, 2007).
2.1. Assessing teacher anger
Emotional labor can be understood as a process in which em-
ployees regulate the internal and expressive components of their
Since emotion is a complex phenomenon which comprises
emotions to align them with the emotional display rules of their
multiple components e subjective, cognitive, motivational,
profession (Hochschild, 1983). Two main forms of emotional labour
expressive, and physiological (Lazarus, 2001; Scherer, 2009), the
can be distinguished, deep acting and surface acting. Deep acting
best research approach to study teacher anger would be one that
refers to the conscious management of internal feelings in order to
addresses all five components. In order to achieve this goal, teacher
consequently experience and express the desirable emotion. For
anger can be measured by different methods like observation,
instance, teachers may engage in positive thoughts about their
neuroimaging techniques, peripheral physiological measures, and
students (such as a student success, or particularly kind behaviour)
self-report (Pekrun & Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2014). The self-report
and consequently start to experience joy and affection. It has been
method dominates in research on emotion in the educational
shown to be a rather effective and adaptive strategy. Surface acting,
context, due to its cost-effectiveness and capability to easily access
in turn, implies modifying the expression of an undesirable
specifically the subjective and cognitive components (Pekrun &
emotion by hiding it, suppressing it, or by faking a more desirable
Bühner, 2014), but also the motivational, physiological, and
one. For example, teachers who are angry with students due to
expressive components of emotion. Finally, the self-report method
their rude and disrespectful behaviour, may try to keep cool and
is particularly suited for the assessment of emotions as traits, that
maintain a poker face, i.e. suppress their observable signs of anger
is, emotions which are typically or frequently experienced in
and fake calmness and indiference instead. Surface acting has been
certain circumstances.
shown to be rather maladaptive (Brotheridge & Lee, 2003;
Recently, considerable research attention has been directed to
Hochschild, 1983).
measuring teacher emotions, including teacher anger. The Teacher
There is ample evidence that teachers frequently apply
Emotions Scales (TES; Frenzel et al., 2016) measure the intensity
emotional labor strategies to comply with the emotional display
and frequency of enjoyment, anger, and anxiety with respect to
rules of their profession, one of which is that anger is an undesir-
classroom teaching (both in general and with respect to a specific
able emotion for teachers (e.g. Sutton, 2007; Sutton, Mudrey-
group of students). The Teacher Emotion Inventory (TEI; Chen,
Camino, & Knight, 2009; Yin & Lee, 2012). Yet, teacher emotional
2016) and Teacher Emotion Questionnaire (TEQ; Buri c et al.,
labor literature typically addresses the regulation and experience of
2018) are self-report instruments which quantify multiple rea-
“emotions” most generally, and evidence is scarce on links between
sons for experiencing a large range of negative and positive emo-
regulation strategies and specific, discrete emotions. However, in a
tions. Both TES and TEQ contain items on the emotion of anger, but
recent study Buri c et al. (2019) found that teacher anger experi-
only in relation to teaching and interacting with students. The
enced about students predicts higher frequency of hiding feelings
anger subscale from the TEI (Chen, 2016) measures reasons for
and faking emotions, that is, two forms of surface acting, over time.
teacher anger in more general manner (its items encompass anger
The present study seeks to further explore the links between
evoked by parents, colleagues, society and policy). Still, it does not
teacher emotional labor strategies and teacher anger with respect
clearly differentiate those contextual facets of teacher anger.
to multiple contexts.
Therefore, developing a valid and reliable measure of different
In turn, teachers’ negative emotions have been recognized as
facets of teacher anger was an important step towards a more
important factors in explaining teacher burnout (Carson, 2006;
precise quantitative investigation of this complex construct.
Chang, 2009; 2013). Since teacher anger, as a negative emotion, is
typically viewed as undesirable and as an emotion that should be
suppressed or masked (e.g. Sutton, 2007; Taxer & Frenzel, 2015), 2.2. The present research
thus creating an emotional dissonance and leading to emotional
exhaustion (Morris & Feldman, 1996), its positive association with Based on the theoretical assumptions and the results of prior
burnout, can be expected. Similarly, as emotions have been research outlined above, it seems that teacher anger can arise from
acknowledged as important factors in determining job-related at- a variety of situations and events. However, not enough is known
titudes and behaviour (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1995; Weiss & yet regarding the exact contextual triggers and components of
Cropanzano, 1996), anger can also be expected to be negatively teacher anger. Moreover, prior research so far has missed to sys-
related to job satisfaction. In other words, if teachers experience tematically quantitatively explore the relationships between such
anger across diverse situations with respect to their work, this fine-grained facets of teacher anger with emotional labor, well-
should not only contribute to higher levels of burnout and impaired being, and teachers’ self-efficacy. This can be attributable to both
mental health, but also negatively impact their job satisfaction and limited prior research on teacher anger most generally, and a lack of
make them consider leaving the profession (Macdonald, 1999). valid and reliable measures of this complex but relatively frequent
Finally, it can be assumed that emotions are reciprocally linked and important teacher emotion. Therefore, by relying on a mixed-
with self-efficacy. Self-efficacy has been conceptualized as a “belief method approach and through a series of four independent
in one's capabilities to organize and execute the course of action studies, the following research questions were addressed:
required to produce given attainments” (Bandura, 1997, p. 3).
Emotions represent a source and a filter of information about one's 1. What are the contextual triggers and components of teacher
performance and determine individual sense of efficacy at a given anger, that is, of which facets is teacher anger constituted?
4 I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895

2. Can different facets of teacher anger be measured through a self- scrutinized by comparing across the widely varying data sources
report instrument with good psychometric properties? (i.e. teachers from different schools, with diverse teaching disci-
3. How are different facets of teacher anger related to their plines and a wide range of years of teaching experiences).
emotional labor, indicators of well-being and self-efficacy?

3.2. Results and discussion


In order to address these research questions, four studies using
independent samples of teachers were conducted. Study 1 used a
The thematic analysis showed that teachers experience anger in
qualitative method to reveal contextual triggers and components of
a variety of situations and events experienced at work or in the
teacher anger. Studies 2 and 3 aimed at development of the self-
relation to their work. These evoking situations and events (i.e.
report measure of different facets of teacher anger as well as at
contextual triggers) could be classified into four major themes.
establishment of its latent structure and psychometric properties
The first category of contextual triggers of teacher anger refers
by combining exploratory and confirmatory approaches. Finally, in
to interactions with students and encompasses situations and
Study 4, theoretically meaningful relations between teacher anger
events such as violation of classroom rules, rudeness and arro-
and external variables of emotional labor, well-being, and self-
gance, disrespect of teacher and peers, aggressive behaviour,
efficacy were tested in order to provide evidence of criterion val-
obstruction of class goals by inappropriate behaviour, etc. Two
idity of the new measure and to emphasize the relevance of the
examples that illustrate this group of triggers are:
teacher anger construct in explaining diverse outcomes of teachers’
professional functioning. I get angry when I see that I don't spend enough time on
teaching, the thing I should do. Instead, sometimes I have a
3. Study 1: A qualitative investigation of components and feeling that I just yell and try to keep order in classroom all day.
contextual triggers of teacher anger (Female Croatian language teacher, 10 years of teaching
experience)
In order to get a more thorough and in-depth insight into
I don't have a problem with the fact that a child is not interested,
different components of teacher anger (i.e. subjective, cognitive,
that a student is not capable enough. I can accept that … There
motivational, expressive, and physiological), and groups of situa-
are kids who do poorly in school for some reason. I think we
tions and events (i.e. contextual triggers) that can evoke teacher
should encourage them, help them not to stay at the bottom. But
anger, a qualitative study based on semi-structured interview
if they are rude and arrogant … I am allergic to it! It really
technique was implemented.
tackles my nerves. (Female English language teacher, 22 years of
teaching experience)
3.1. Participants and procedure

In total, 25 Croatian middle-school teachers (23 female) The second theme refers to relationships and interactions with
employed in 12 different state schools were interviewed. Teachers parents of their students as sources of teacher anger. Triggering
were on average 43.40 years old (SD ¼ 7.50), had 13.80 years of situations include accusations and verbal attacks, interfering with
teaching experience (SD ¼ 7.20), and taught a wide range of sub- teacher's job, parents who are overly oriented towards perfor-
jects (e.g. Croatian language, English language, Geography, Biology, mance of their children while neglecting an acquisition of knowl-
Mathematics, Sport, Religion, ICT, etc.). Participation in the study edge and investment of effort, overestimating their child's real
was voluntarily and teachers were assured that information they capabilities, rudeness in communication, etc. For example, two
shared would be treated with strict confidentiality and used only teachers stated:
for research purposes. Once teachers gave their informed consent,
They [parents] can be so frustrating! I have a feeling that they
in-depth, semi-structured, face-to-face interviews were conducted
are free to enter the school and to spill down the nervousness
and audio-taped. Their statements were transcribed verbatim. All
that they picked up during that day on us [teachers] … and we
interviews were moderated by a hired expert with rich experience
have to stay calm and polite. I really feel trapped then. (Female
in qualitative research on emotional processes. Since the qualitative
ICT teacher, 15 years of teaching experience)
data collection was a part of a larger research project on teacher
emotion and emotion regulation, only data related to teacher anger I feel revolted because of some parents. When a parent comes to
were used and analysed for the purposes of this study. The total school and tells you that you don't do your job properly and that
length of the interviews ranged between 40 and 90 min. his child should achieve better … like, he knows his kid and her
Teachers were asked to list and describe situations and events capabilities, and you don't … And you cannot tell him no, you
that evoked anger in them, to describe how they felt at that are wrong!..Even though you would love to do that. It is really
particular moment, what they were thinking about, how they frustrating. (Female English language teacher, 28 years of
behaved, what bodily sensations they experienced, and what they teaching experience)
wanted/wished they could do. Teachers were prompted to talk
about as many events and situations as they could recall. All tran-
The third category of contextual triggers of teacher anger relates
scripts were subjected to thematic analysis which consisted of the
to relationships and interactions with colleagues. Teachers
following main steps: 1. familiarizing with the data, 2. generation of
mentioned situations such as unfair distribution of tasks among
initial codes, 3. searching for themes, 4. reviewing themes, and 5.
school staff, hostile and uncooperative colleagues, colleagues who
defining and labelling themes (Braun & Clarke, 2006). In order to
have ‘given up’ or do not feel ‘a call to teach’, incompetent col-
ensure the objectivity and reliability of findings, two forms of
leagues, colleagues who do not have enough understanding for
triangulation were employed. First, after familiarizing with the
children, etc. To illustrate this category of emotional triggers, the
data, two researchers with experience in qualitative data analysis
following quotes were excerpted from the transcripts:
independently generated initial codes and searched for emerging
themes/categories. This was followed by peer debriefing in which We [teachers] all work for the same wage, but there are some
researchers analysed and compared the identified categories colleagues in school who don't want to work or don't know how
(Willig, 2008). Second, the emerging themes and categories were to do something … they never get an extra task. And there are
I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895 5

those who are capable and hard-working and they always get also worth noting that the teachers’ descriptions of the phenome-
burdened with extra job. It's just not fair and it makes me angry. nology of their anger were very rich. They contained verbal de-
(Female mathematics teacher, 23 years of teaching experience) scriptions of all theoretically proposed components of emotions
(subjective, cognitive, motivational, expressive, and physiological).
I am angry with my colleagues who refuse to develop profes-
To describe the subjective affective core of anger experiences, the
sionally … they are lazy and not ready for changes … like
teachers used a wide array of adjectives and nouns (angry, frus-
learning new technologies … They want to slow down the
trated, annoyed, irritated, tense, restless, indignant). In depicting
children instead of learn something new and accept changes.
the cognitive processes involved in anger experiences, they spoke
(Male Sport teacher, 16 years of teaching experience)
of thinking about losing control, evaluating the situation as unfair,
and having doubts about usefulness of time, effort, and energy
Finally, the fourth and last category of contextual triggers of invested in their job. To illustrate motivational component of anger
teacher anger refers to the features of the educational system and experience, teachers mentioned urges to yell at someone, to hit
educational policy. Teachers mentioned issues like frequent something, leave the classroom, quit the job. Teachers were also
changes in the curriculum imposed by authorities, degradation of aware of their expressive manifestation of anger experience which
public respect towards teachers, excessive paperwork, lowering was described by terms like blushing and frowning. Finally, the
teachers' professional status in today's society, underestimating the physiological component of anger experience was depicted as
significance of the teacher's job, poor educational policy and reg- sweating, headache, and rise in blood pressure, and heart rate.
ulations, poor material conditions, etc. The following two quotes These findings support the view of emotion as a multicompo-
illustrate this category: nent phenomenon (Harmon-Jones & Harmon-Jones, 2016;
Schuman & Scherer, 2014) and reveal that teachers are indeed
They [the authorities] give really little to us [teachers], but
aware of subjective and cognitive components of anger, but are also
expect from us to work more and more and to deal with enor-
capable of accurate description of other emotional components (i.e.
mous amount of paper work which is useless and doesn't make
physiological, expressive, and motivational) that are typically
sense. This is really frustrating for all of us. (Female Croatian
assessed by methods other than self-report (Pekrun & Bühner,
language teacher, 3 years of teaching experience)
2014).
It is annoying when you have a feeling that no one values your
work. Even your family and friends have an attitude that 4. Study 2: measuring teacher anger
teachers don't work much and have long holidays. (Female
Croatian language teacher, 22 years of teaching experience) Based on the qualitative data obtained in Study 1 and theoretical
assumptions (i.e. multi-component definition of emotion and pre-
vious research on sources of teacher anger), the aim of this study
Triggers of teacher anger related to teaching and students thus
was to create a valid and reliable assessment tool that would effi-
largely confirmed previous research that recognized student mis-
ciently capture teacher anger while taking into consideration its
behaviour, violation of classroom rules, and students' failure that is
components and contextual triggers. Specifically, a list of 25 self-
perceived as intentional or controllable by students, as sources of
report statements covering various emotional components and
teacher anger (Chang, 2009; Prawat, Byers, & Anderson, 1983).
the contextual triggers as identified through the interviews and
Moreover, the results of this qualitative study are in line with
existing studies (Buric et al., 2018; Chen, 2016) were formulated to
scattered existing findings that identified parents (i.e. failure to
create an initial item pool. By relying on expert judgement and
follow institutional norms of appropriate parental behaviour and
criteria of semantic redundancy (Frenzel et al., 2016), 16 items were
careless and irresponsible parents; Lasky, 2000), colleagues (i.e.
retained, that is, four items for each of the four facets of teacher
uncooperative colleagues; Bullough, Knowles, & Crow, 1991), and
anger: (1) students-related (sample item: “The reactions of some
policy related factors like administrative work, educational re-
students frustrate me so much that I would rather just quit the
forms, and school administration (Hargreaves, 2004; Zembylas,
job”),1 (2) parents-related (sample item: “I feel resistance and anger
2003) as relevant sources of teacher anger and frustration. How-
with parents who overestimate their children's real capabilities”),
ever, the current study also provided a more in-depth look into
(3) colleagues-related (sample item: “The anger I feel about un-
significantly wider coverage of contextual triggers of teacher anger
equal workloads among school staff makes me want to quit my
that had not yet been recognized in previous studies. For instance,
job”), and (4) system-related (sample item: “It infuriates me to
our data clearly revealed a parenting trend in which a main goal for
think about how little the government invests in education”). The
one's children is performing well (rather than mastering a subject),
full set of items can be found in the Appendix.
which spills down to teachers who are expected to “produce”
highly performing kids, irrespective of their actual skill and
knowledge levels. Aggravating this, there seems to be a consider- 4.1. Participants and procedure
able number of parents who persistently overestimate their chil-
dren's real capabilities and consequently put unreasonably high Participants of this study were 615 middle-school teachers (464
pressure on teachers. In addition, this research showed that female, 122 male, and 29 did not indicate their gender) employed at
teachers sense that a certain proportion of their colleagues lack 43 Croatian state schools. Teachers were on average 41.15 years old
motivation and are seen as incompetent and passive at their (SD ¼ 10.61) and had 11.17 years of teaching experience
workplace, which makes them frustrated and angry. Finally, (SD ¼ 10.15). In addition to demographic questions, teachers were
teachers in this study raised the concerning issue of degradation of asked to rate the level of agreement with each of the 16 statements
public respect and professional status of teachers in today's society on a five-point scale with possible responses from 1 to 5
as a great source of anger and disappointment, which may have (1 ¼ strongly disagree, 2 ¼ disagree, 3 ¼ neither agree nor disagree,
adverse effects in terms of raising the likelihood of teacher attrition 4 ¼ agree, and 5 ¼ strongly agree). Teachers were instructed to
and shortage.
Apart from these clearly emerging themes within the interviews
1
The items aimed to assess students-related anger are taken from Teacher
pertaining to the various contextual triggers of teacher anger, it is
Emotion Questionnaire (TEQ, Buri
c et al., 2018).
6 I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895

report “how they typically feel” in relation to their students, par- data implying that items assessing teacher anger need to be
ents, colleagues, and educational system. Participation in this study differentiated. And even though the 4-factors CFA model demon-
was voluntarily and anonymous e questionnaires were sent to strated a substantial and sufficient increase in model fit, it still fit
schools via postal service, distributed to teachers with the assis- worse when compared to the 4-factors ESEM model (DCFI ¼ 0.033).
tance of school psychologists, and returned to researchers closed This implies that teacher anger is indeed a multifaceted construct,
each in its own envelope two weeks later. The average response however, the relationships between particular items and imposed
rate across schools was 57%, which is considerably higher than in factors are not that exclusive. Instead, a certain degree of variation
teacher studies reported earlier (e.g. Taxer & Frenzel, 2015, p. 33%; in each item could be predicted by multiple factors, that is, by
Metler, 2003, p. 21% for paper-and-pencil assessment, 11% for web- diverse contextual triggers. Nevertheless, the pattern of factor
based assessment). loadings was as expected by the hypothesized model and sufficient
in size (>0.50). The only exception refers to the first item of anger
4.2. Results and discussion related to colleagues which in ESEM demonstrated poorer
discriminant power (i.e. I wish to yell when I just think about the
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and exploratory structural improper behaviour of some colleagues); see Table 2). Furthermore,
equation modelling (ESEM) were used to test the latent structure of as shown in Table 3, latent correlations between different facets of
the measure of teacher anger. In typical CFA model all items have teacher anger obtained by CFA were mostly moderate in size
zero factor loadings on all factors except on the factor they are ranging from 0.27 to 0.59, whereas those calculated by ESEM were
expected to measure (i.e. cross-loadings are constrained to zero) substantially lower ranging from 0.22 to 0.37. The sizes of these
(McDonald, 1985). Such constraints are often too restrictive in correlations indicate that even though different facets determined
psychological measurement since the existence of cross-loadings by diverse emotional sources belong to the same construct, they
can be typically justified both by substantive theory and by item can be sufficiently differentiated.
content (e.g. method effects). Moreover, fixing cross-loadings to Finally, all scales demonstrated satisfactory levels of internal
zero typically results in inflated correlations between factors, their consistency (Cronbach a ranged from 0.77 to 0.82) implying that
reduced discriminant validity, and biased estimates of relations different facets of teacher anger can be reliably measured. Mea-
with other variables if they are included in structural equation sures of all facets also showed sufficient variation in scores and
model (SEM; Marsh, Morin, Parker, & Kauer, 2014). ESEM was distributions that do not substantially depart from normality.
therefore proposed as a useful technique that may resolve these However, distribution of scores of anger related to educational
issues since the only a priori information required to run an ESEM system was to some degree negatively skewed pointing to the fact
model is the number of factors, and all other parameters (including that most teachers tend to quite strongly endorse the items on
cross-loadings) are freely estimated (Asparouhov & Muthe n, 2009). anger related to the features of educational system and policy. In
Considering the purpose of this study, that is, to examine the latent conclusion, our newly developed measure of teacher anger
structure of the newly developed measure of different facets of demonstrated sufficient degree of convergent and divergent val-
teacher anger, it is reasonable to test and compare the results of idity, reliability, and variation of scores.
both approaches e CFA which is more restrictive but also more
parsimonious and ESEM which is less restrictive but more complex. 5. Study 3: testing the measurement invariance of teacher
Moreover, even though different facets of teacher anger may be anger across educational levels
considered as sufficiently discriminant constructs, they still repre-
sent the same phenomenon (i.e. teacher anger) and moderate The purpose of this study was to test the measurement invari-
correlations between the factors as well as cross-loadings can be ance of the facets of teacher anger across two educational levels e
expected. middle school and high school. In addition to the test of factorial
In total, three models were tested and compared: 1) a one-factor equivalence, the equivalence of latent means was also examined. In
model in which all 16 items were set to load on the same factor, 2) a other words, the goal of the third study was to test whether the
four-factors CFA model in which 16 items were set to load on 4 newly developed measure of teacher anger functions equivalently
respective factors (i.e. students-related anger, parents-related in terms of measurement among middle-school and high-school
anger, colleagues-related anger, system-related anger, and 3) a teachers and whether there are any differences in the intensity of
four-factors ESEM model in which 16 items were set to load on 4 different facets of teacher anger between these two groups of
respective factors and in which, as opposed to more restrictive CFA teachers.
model, cross-loadings are freely estimated. All analyses were con-
ducted using Mplus 6 (Muthe n & Muthen, 1998e2010). Parameters 5.1. Participants and procedure
were estimated by maximum likelihood estimation method (ML)
and missing data were compensated by full information maximum In total, 2095 teachers (1645 female, 446 male, and 4 did not
likelihood method (FIML). In ESEM analysis, an oblique Geomin report their gender) from 133 state schools in Croatia participated
rotation with an epsilon value of 0.5 was utilized (Marsh et al., in the study. Among them, 1099 were middle-school teachers and
2009; Morin, Marsh, & Nagengast, 2013). Model fit was evaluated 996 were high-school teachers. Teachers were on average 41.51
by comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), root-mean- years old (SD ¼ 10.41) and had 14.39 years of teaching experience
square error of approximation (RMSEA) and standardized root- (SD ¼ 10.24). The data collection procedure was the same as in
mean residual (SRMR). CFI and TLI values higher than 0.90 indi- Study 2 (i.e., teachers responded to the same16eitem questionnaire
cate satisfactory model fit, while values above 0.95 reflect an in the same manner as in Study 2) and the average response rate
excellent fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). RMSEA values that fall between across schools was again quite high, namely 50%.The sample of
0.05 and 0.08 indicate adequate fit, and RMSEA and SRMR values participating teachers in the this present study was independent of
smaller than 0.05 point to excellent fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1992). the sample used in Study 2.
For comparisons of tested models, DCFI<0.01 was used e model
with smaller CFI value was chosen as better fitting one (Chen, 5.2. Results and discussion
2007). The results of model testing are shown in Table 1.
It can be seen that the 1-factor model showed the worst fit to the Multi-group ESEM was employed to test the measurement and
I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895 7

Table 1
Model fit indices.

1 factor model 4-factors CFA model 4-factors ESEM model

c2 (df) 1407.13* (104) 268.14* (98) 113.87* (62)


CFI .642 .953 .986
TLI .587 .943 .972
SRMR .105 .051 .017
RMSEA (90% C.I.) .143 (.136, .149) .056 (.049, .064) .037 (.026, .047)

*p < .001.

Table 2
Results of testing the 4efactors CFA model and 4-factors ESEM model.

Source 4-factors CFA model 4-factors ESEM model

Students Parents Colleagues System Students Parents Colleagues System

Item
students 1 .68 .70 -.04 .08 -.01
students 2 .78 .75 .07 .04 .03
students 3 .70 .64 .06 .11 -.02
students 4 .68 .53 .18 .12 .10
parents 1 .71 .09 .57 .14 .08
parents 2 .63 .18 .54 -.02 .07
parents 3 .82 -.02 .79 .08 .08
parents 4 .76 .07 .66 .10 .08
colleagues 1 .61 .03 .20 .39 .24
colleagues 2 .70 .08 .03 .70 -.01
colleagues 3 .72 .07 .09 .64 .04
colleagues 4 .68 .05 .03 .68 .01
system 1 .76 -.03 .15 .01 .70
system 2 .73 -.01 .10 .01 .69
system 3 .66 .08 .03 .07 .62
system 4 .80 .04 .05 .09 .74

Table 3 factor means (Morin et al., 2013). The results of the test of these
Descriptive statistics and latent correlations between different facets of teacher models are shown in Table 4.
anger.
As can be seen in Table 4, imposing restrictions on factor load-
Source of anger Students Parents Colleagues System ings (i.e. weak measurement invariance) did not result in any
Students e .49 .55 .27 substantial loss of the model fit (DCFI ¼ 0.008) when compared to
Parents .27 e .59 .58 baseline, configural model. Next, the difference between weak
Collagues .32 .33 e .44 invariance model and strong invariance model was not substantial
System .11 .37 .22 e
in size either (DCFI ¼ 0.001) implying invariance of item intercepts
Possible Range 1e5 1e5 1e5 1e5
M 2.12 3.10 2.39 4.01 too. Introducing additional restrictions on variances and co-
SD .86 .94 .83 .84 variances did not lead to the loss of model fit either (DCFI ¼ 0.002),
Skewness (S.E.) .78 (.10) -.32 (.10) .32 (.10) 1.05 (.10) indicating that the variability of factors and factor correlations
Kurtosis (S.E.) .40 (.20) -.24 (.20) -.14 (.20) .92 (.20) could be considered equal in both groups of teachers. Finally,
Cronbach a .80 .81 .77 .82
imposing restrictions on factor means did not substantially worsen
Note. All latent correlations were statistically significant at p < .001. Latent corre- the model fit (DCFI ¼ 0.005), which means that middle school and
lations obtained by CFA are shown above the diagonal while latent correlations
high school teachers did not differ in different facets of teacher
obtained by ESEM are shown below the diagonal. Descriptive statistics were
calculated on manifest variables. anger with respect to their latent means. To conclude, in terms of
measurement and underlying latent structure, the newly devel-
oped measure of teacher anger operates similarly across different
latent means invariance of the measure of teacher anger (i.e. educational levels. Moreover, it seems that middle-school and
analysis of mean and covariance structure e MACS; Byrne, 2012) high-school teachers do not differ in their anger levels across the
across two groups of teachers. The analysis was conducted in Mplus various contexts considered.
6 (Muthe n & Muthe n, 1998e2010) and using the same estimation
method and criteria of model evaluation as in Study 2. The
6. Study 4. relationships between facets of teacher anger and
following analysis steps were undertaken: 1) estimating the base-
indicators of teacher emotional labor, well-being, and self-
line 4efactors ESEM model in the two groups of teachers simul-
efficacy beliefs
taneously, while leaving all model parameters free to vary across
the two groups (i.e. configural invariance), 2) testing the mea-
Even though previous research recognized the role of teacher
surement invariance of the model by constraining various model
anger experienced in the classroom in explaining teacher well-
parameters to be identical across the two groups; (a) factor load-
being outcomes as well as their efficiency and instructional
ings (i.e. weak measurement invariance), (b) factor loadings and
behaviour (e.g. Carson, 2006; Chang, 2013; Frenzel, 2014;
item intercepts (i.e. strong measurement invariance), (c) factor
Macdonald, 1999; Sutton, 2007), research on teacher anger trig-
loadings, item intercepts, and factor variances-covariances; (d)
gered by contextual factors other than teaching and students are
factor loadings, item intercepts, factor variances-covariances, and
largely lacking. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine
8 I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895

Table 4
Invariance tests of the 4efactors ESEM model.

Model c2 (df) CFI TLI RMSEA (90% C.I.) SRMR

Configural invariance 506.45* (124) .968 .937 .055 (.050, .060) .022
Weak measurement invariance 638.95* (172) .960 .945 .052 .031
(.047, .056)
Strong measurement invariance 667.35* (184) .959 .947 .051 (.047, 055) .031
Strong measurement invariance þ invariance of factor variances-covariances 699.63* (194) .957 .947 .051 .036
(.047, .055)
Strong measurement invariance þ invariance of factor variances-covariances þ invariance of factor means 765.65* (198) .952 .942 .053 .042
(.049, 057)

Note. *p < .001.

the relationships between different facets of teacher anger (i.e. engagement (4 items, e.g. How much can you do to help your stu-
anger related to students, parents, colleagues, and the educational dents value learning? a ¼ 0.78). Teachers responded on a 9 e point
system) with indicators of emotional labor (i.e. deep acting and scale (1 ¼ nothing, 3 ¼ very little, 5 ¼ some influence, 7 ¼ quite a
surface acting), well-being (emotional exhaustion and job satis- bit, and 9 ¼ a great deal.
faction), and teachers’ self-efficacy (efficacy for instructional stra-
tegies, classroom management, and student engagement). Based
on previous research, it can be expected that all facets of teacher 7. Results and discussion
anger would be positively related to surface acting (Taxer & Frenzel,
2015, Buric et al., 2019) and emotional exhaustion, an indicator of As expected, the four facets of teacher anger included in this
burnout (Carson, 2006; Chang, 2013), and negatively to job satis- research showed theoretically meaningful relations with the other
faction (Macdonald, 1999; Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996) and teacher variables assessed in this study, including gender and work
self-efficacy (Schwarzer & Hallum, 2008). experience.
As shown in Table 5, there was a tendency for females to report
more anger with parents and colleagues, and for increased anger
6.1. Participants and measures across teaching years, however those effects were all quite small in
size. More importantly, all facets of teacher anger were positively
The sample consisted of 505 middle-school teachers employed correlated with surface acting, that is, that is, hiding true emotions
in 32 state schools in Croatia. Of the total sample, 388 declared and faking expected ones. This is in line with previous research on
themselves as female, 101 as males, while 16 of them did not teachers’ anger regulation (Taxer & Frenzel, 2015; Buri c et al., 2019)
indicate their gender. On average, teachers were 41.39 years old as well as with implicit emotional display rules of teaching pro-
(SD ¼ 10.44) and had 14.95 years of teaching experience fession (e.g. Sutton, 2007; Sutton et al., 2009; Yin & Lee, 2012).
(SD ¼ 10.71). The data collection procedure was the same as in Those correlations were significant but small in size and the size of
previous quantitative studies, and the response rate was again as the association was considerably smaller for anger triggered by
high as 49%. Again, this study included a new and independent characteristics of educational system. Deep acting was largely un-
sample of teachers that did not participate in studies 1, 2, and 3. related to teacher anger; only teacher anger evoked by students
Along with items developed to assess facets of teacher anger, the was marginally negatively related to deep acting. This implies that
following additional measures were administered: teachers who invest more effort to really experience and show
1) Emotional Labor Scale (ELS; Brotheridge & Lee, 2003) which emotions they are expected to, tend to experience less anger in
consists of two scales e Surface acting (3 items, e.g. On an average relation to their students, but this does not relate to their anger
day at work, how frequently do you resist expressing your true feel- with respect to parents, colleagues, or the educational system.
ings? a ¼ 0.65) and Deep acting (3 items, e.g. On an average day at Furthermore, in line with expectations, teacher anger proved to
work, how frequently do you actually try to experience the emotions be related to poorer well-being in terms of job satisfaction and
that you must show? a ¼ 0.81). Teachers responded on 5-point scale emotional exhaustion. Teachers who reported to typically experi-
ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). ence more anger related to students, parents, colleagues, and
2) Job-related Emotional Exhaustion Scale (Wharton, 1993) educational system, reported to be less satisfied with their job;
which consists of 6 items measuring emotional exhaustion expe- which is in accordance with previous findings (Macdonald, 1999).
rienced at work (e.g. I dread getting up in the morning and having to In addition, teacher anger, across contexts, was positively associ-
face another day on the job; a ¼ .90) was used as an indicator of ated with emotional exhaustion. These links were considerable in
teacher burnout. Teachers reported their level of agreement with size, emphasizing once again the crucial role of teachers’ negative
each item by using a six-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 emotional experiences, specifically anger experiences, for teacher
(every day). burnout (Carson, 2006; Chang, 2009; 2013).
3) Job Satisfaction Scale (Judge, Parker, Colbert, Heller, & Ilies, Finally, teacher anger was negatively associated with self-
2001) which encompasses 6 items measuring overall job satisfac- efficacy. These associations are in line with previous empirical ev-
tion (e.g. I feel fairly satisfied with my present job; a ¼ 0.85) on a 7 idence on the relationship between self-efficacy and affective ex-
point scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely periences (e.g. Buri
c & Macuka, 2018; Chemers, Hu, & Garcia, 2001).
agree). The present research extends these findings, though, as there is a
4) Teacher Sense of Efficacy Scale (TSES; Tschannen-Moran & clear pattern of teacher anger with respect to students being most
Hoy, 2001) which assesses three domains of teacher self-efficacy: closely related with teachers' self-efficacy, whereas correlations
Efficacy for instructional strategies (e.g. To what extent can you between the other facets of teacher anger and teacher self-efficacy
use a variety of assessment strategies? a ¼ 0.79), Efficacy for class- were small in size, or zero. This implies that teachers’ experience of
room management (e.g. How much can you do to control disruptive anger related to parents and the educational policy and system
behaviour in the classroom? a ¼ 0.90), and Efficacy for student does not undermine their perceived efficacy as teachers.
I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895 9

Table 5
Correlations of teacher anger with emotion regulation, well-being outcomes and self-efficacy.

Facets of tacher anger

Students Parents Colleagues Educational system

Gender .05 .16** .10* .07


Working experience in years .09* .12** .13** .10*
Deep acting -.11* -.01 -.05 -.03
Surface acting .30** .30** .30** .19**
Emotional exhaustion .53** .45** .51** .37**
Job satisfaction -.41** -.32** -.41** -.23**
Efficacy for instructional strategeis -.35** -.19** -.17** -.04
Efficacy for classroom management -.39** -.14** -.11* .06
Efficacy for student management -.28** -.17** -.17** -.06
M 2.40 3.29 2.48 4.00
SD .87 .84 .84 .78
Cronbach a .78 .75 .80 .73

*p < .05; **p < .001

8. Conclusions efficacy). Moreover, the multi-group ESEM analysis showed that


the measure of teacher anger functions equivalently among mid-
With the aim to more thoroughly explore teacher anger, its dle- and high-school teachers, and that these groups of teachers do
sources and components, and its relationships with teachers’ not differ in the level of distinct facets of anger.
emotional labor, their well-being, and self-efficacy, a series of four Third, the new measure presented herein is short and
studies, based on diverse methodological and analytical approaches economical, with four items for each facet. However, this implied
and involving more than 3000 teachers, was conducted. Based on that the measure did not systematically address all five emotional
the results obtained by this line of research, several conclusions can components (affective, cognitive, motivational, expressive, and
be drawn. physiological) within each facet of teacher anger, and it clearly does
First, teacher anger is indeed complex and multifaceted not exhaustively cover all potential contextual triggers for teacher
construct. As any other human emotion, it consists of various anger. Thus, future research may be directed at expanding the
distinguishable emotional components e subjective, cognitive, scales in order to grasp all components in a valid and reliable way.
motivational, expressive, and physiological e which is in accor- Alternatively, future research on teacher anger may involve mea-
dance with contemporary multi-component definitions of emo- sures beyond self-report to include the other components, for
tions (Harmon-Jones & Harmon-Jones, 2016; Schuman & Scherer, example by measuring heart-rates to grasp the physiological
2014). In addition, teacher anger is evoked by contextual triggers component (e.g. Donker, van Gogh, & Mainhard, 2018), or using
that go beyond teaching and interacting with students. While most facial emotion recognition for the expressive component.
existing research on teachers' emotions focused primarily on Finally, teacher anger is related to indicators of teacher well-
emotions experienced in relation to students and classroom ac- being and self-efficacy. All facets of teacher anger were positively
tivities (e.g. Buri
c et al, 2018; Frenzel, 2014; Hagenauer & Volet, related to surface acting, a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy
2014), data from Study 1 reported herein showed that anger in (Hülsheger & Schewe, 2011) and emotional exhaustion, a core
teachers is also evoked by a wide variety of situations and events component of burnout (Maslach, Schaufeli, & Leiter, 2001) as well
related to parents, behaviour of colleagues and encounters in the as negatively to job satisfaction, indicating that teacher anger may
staff room, as well as by features of educational system and policy. play an important role in eroding teacher well-being. In addition,
These results clearly demonstrated that teachers have multiple teacher anger was negatively related to teachers’ sense of efficacy
goals and tasks at their job. Those are not only related to students, for instructional strategies, classroom management, and student
but also to their colleagues, parents, and educational system. As engagement. It thus seems that teachers who engage in maladap-
proposed by the reciprocal model on causes and effects of teacher tive emotion regulation strategies experience more emotional
emotions (Frenzel, 2014; Jacob, Frenzel, & Stephens, 2017), situa- exhaustion and more anger, which in turn undermines their self-
tions and events that teachers perceive as blocking their goals, and efficacy. However, given the correlational nature of our data, such
appraisals concerning the responsibility for this goal blockage be- causal inferences are only speculative and tentative and should be
ing external to them, triggers anger. For example, encounters with confirmed in future research that would need to be based on lon-
parents who refuse to cooperate with a teacher and persist in gitudinal and/or experimental designs in order to establish a true
overestimating their child's real capabilities, may block the direction of causation between teacher anger and well-being or
achievement of teachers' goal related to keeping positive and performance indicators.
constructive relationships with parents and guardians. Similarly, a As another word of caution, when interpreting the findings from
public perception of teachers as working only few hours a day, may this research program, one has to bear in mind that it heavily relies
obstruct teachers' goal of being a member of valued profession in on self-report which clearly has its disadvantages such as inac-
today's society. In sum, the present qualitative study highlighted curacies in recall or giving socially desirable answers (Pekrun &
the importance of incorporating diverse contextual triggers when Bühner, 2014). To avoid corresponding potential common-method
aiming to better understand the emotion of anger among teachers. biases (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003), future
Second, our newly developed measure of different facets of research on teacher anger should involve a greater variety of
teacher anger presented herein proved to have sufficient conver- assessment methods, including behavioural observations, or
gent and divergent validity, exhibited satisfactory internal consis- physiological measures. Lastly, future studies on teachers from
tency and sufficient variations of scores, and resulting scores different countries and cultures are needed in order to replicate our
demonstrated theoretically meaningful relations to external vari- findings since teachers’ emotions may are embedded within spe-
ables (i.e. emotional labour, emotional exhaustion, and self- cific social-cultural contexts (Liljestrom et al., 2007).
10 I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895

In conclusion, we hope that the existence of our newly devel- 1. I wish to yell when I just think about the improper behaviour of
oped measure of different facets of teacher anger inspires future some colleagues.
investigations on the role of anger in predicting various important 2. The anger I feel about unequal workloads among the school staff
outcomes for both teachers, their students, and quality of education makes me want to quit my job.
in general. In addition, the results of this research provide impor- 3. My heart pounds faster when I think about certain annoying
tant starting points for developing interventions targeted to pro- colleagues.
mote efficient and adaptive emotion regulation strategies for 4. Unfair distribution of work tasks gets me so frustrated that I
teachers. More precisely, negative effects of teacher anger can be get all sweaty.
prevented or mitigated by promoting an implementation of effec-
tive emotion regulation strategies targeted at teacher cognitive Educational system-related anger.
appraisals (i.e., deep acting or reappraisal) and/or development of
their professional skills and competencies (Buri c, Penezi
c, & Sori
c, 1. I get annoyed because educational regulations and laws are
2017). Such strategies can efficiently help to down-regulate anger prescribed by people who have never worked in the school.
while preserving teacher well-being and sense of efficacy (Buric 2. It infuriates me to think about how little the government invests
et al., 2017; Sutton, 2007). Moreover, teachers are not the only in education.
ones who can influence their anger e changing the features of 3. I am angry when teaching quality has to suffer from poor ma-
educational policies and school organization can also be effective terial working conditions.
mean to promote teachers' emotional well-being. For instance, 4. Increasing demands, but with no changes in status, salary, and
releasing teachers from excessive administrative workload, working conditions, make me angry.
ensuring fair distribution of work tasks in school, or acknowledging
teachers’ efforts and boosting their societal value by promoting and
actively participating in initiatives such as the OECD “World References
Teacher Day” (https://en.unesco.org/commemorations/
worldteachersday) or the “Global Teacher Prize” by the Varkey Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1995). Emotion in the workplace: A reappraisal.
Human Relations, 48(2), 97e125. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872679504800201.
Foundation (https://www.globalteacherprize.org/) professional Asparouhov, T., & Muthe n, B. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling.
status that would correspond to their level of education and job Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 16(3), 397e438.
complexity, can show useful in preventing and reducing teacher https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510903008204.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.
anger along with its possible adverse consequences for their per-
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York:
formance and well-being. McGraw-Hill.
Bieg, M. (2013). Trait and state academic emotions: Two sides of the same coin?
(Doctoral dissertation). Germany: Universitaet Konstanz. Retrieved from
https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/bitstream/handle/123456789/25394/Bieg_
Funding 253941.pdf;sequence¼1.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative
Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77e101. https://doi.org/10.1191/
This research was supported by Croatian Science Foundation
1478088706qp063oa.
(Grant No. UIP-11-2013-5065). Brotheridge, C. M., & Lee, R. T. (2003). Development and validation of the emotional
labour scale. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76(3),
365e379. https://doi.org/10.1348/096317903769647229.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. So-
Appendix ciological Methods & Research, 21(2), 230e258. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0049124192021002005.
Bullough, R. V., Knowles, J. G., & Crow, N. A. (1991). Emerging as a teacher. London:
Students-related anger2
Routledge.
Buri
c, I. (2019). The role of emotional labour in explaining teachers' enthusiasm and
1. The reactions of some students frustrate me so much that I students' outcomes: A multilevel mediational analysis. Learning and Individual
Differences, 70, 12e20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.01.002.
would rather just quit the job.
Buri
c, I., & Macuka, I. (2018). Self-efficacy, emotions and work engagement among
2. The frustration I feel while working with students undermines teachers: A two wave cross-lagged analysis. Journal of Happiness Studies, 19(7),
my job motivation. 1917e1933. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-017-9903-9.
3. Some students make so angry that my face goes red. Buri
c, I., Penezic, Z., & Sori
c, I. (2017). Regulating emotions in the teacher’s work-
place: Development and initial validation of the Teacher Emotion-Regulation
4. I get anger-caused headache from the behaviour of some Scale. International Journal of Stress Management, 24(3), 217e246. https://
students. doi.org/10.1037/str0000035.
Buri
c, I., Sliskovic, A., & Macuka, I. (2018). A mixed-method approach to the
assessment of teachers' emotions: Development and validation of the Teacher
Parents-related anger. Emotion Questionnaire,. Educational Psychology: An International Journal of
Experimental Educational Psychology, 38(3), 325e349. https://doi.org/10.1080/
1. I feel resistance and anger with parents who overestimate their 01443410.2017.1382682.
Buri
c, I., Sliskovi
c, A., & Penezi c, Z. (2019). A two-wave panel study on teachers'
children's real capabilities. emotions and emotional-labour strategies. Stress and Health, 35(1), 27e38.
2. I feel my pulse speeds up out of anger when a parent tells me https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2836.
how to do my job. Burns, D., & Darling-Hammond, L. (2014). Teaching around the world: What can TALIS
tell us? Stanford, CA: Stanford Center for Opportunity Policy.
3. I am annoyed by parents who do not want to accept their child's
Byrne, B. (2012). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, applica-
real capabilities. tions, and programming. New York: Routledge.
4. I feel revolted towards parents who seem not to “hear” the Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlman, K. M., & Ito, T. A. (2000). The
Psychophysiology of emotion. In R. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), The
teacher.
handbook of emotion (pp. 173e191). New York: The Guilford Press.
Carson, R. L. (2006). Exploring the episodic nature of teachers' emotions as it relates to
Colleagues-related anger. teacher burnout. West Lafayette: Purdue University.
Chang, M.-L. (2009). An appraisal perspective of teacher burnout: Examining the
emotional work of teachers. Educational Psychology Review, 21(3), 193e218.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-009-9106-y.
2
The items aimed to assess students-related anger are taken from Teacher Chang, M.-L. (2013). Toward a theoretical model to understand teacher emotions
Emotion Questionnaire (TEQ, Buri
c et al., 2018). and teacher burnout in the context of student misbehavior: Appraisal,
I. Buric, A.C. Frenzel / Teaching and Teacher Education 86 (2019) 102895 11

regulation and coping. Motivation and Emotion, 37(4), 799e817. https://doi.org/ et al. (2009). Doubly-latent models of school contextual effects: Integrating
10.1007/s11031-012-9335-0. multilevel and structural equation approaches to control measurement and
Chemers, M. M., Hu, L., & Garcia, B. F. (2001). Academic self-efficacy and first year sampling error. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 44(6), 764e802. https://
college student performance and adjustment. Journal of Educational Psychology, doi.org/10.1080/00273170903333665.
93(1), 55e64. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.55. Marsh, H. W., Morin, A. J. S., Parker, P. D., & Kaur, G. (2014). Exploratory structural
Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement equation modeling: An integration of the best features of exploratory and
invariance. Structural Equation Modelling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(3), confirmatory factor analysis. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10(1), 85e110.
464e504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153700.
Chen, J. (2016). Understanding teacher emotions: The development of a teacher Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of
emotion inventory. Teaching and Teacher Education, 55, 68e77. https://doi.org/ Psychology, 52, 397e422. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397.
10.1016/j.tate.2016.01.001. McDonald, R. P. (1985). Factor analysis and related methods. Journal of Educational
Donker, M. H., van Gog, T., & Mainhard, M. T. (2018). A quantitative exploration of Statistics, 12, 3e31. https://doi.org/10.2307/1164692.
two teachers with contrasting emotions: Intra-individual process analyses of Metler, C. A. (2003). Patterns of response and nonresponse from teachers to
physiology and interpersonal behavior. Frontline Learning Research, 6(3), traditional and web surveys. Practical Assessment, Research and Evaluation,
162e184. 8(22).
Frenzel, A. C. (2014). Teacher emotions. In E. A. Linnenbrink-Garcia, & R. Pekrun Morin, A. J. S., Marsh, H. W., & Nagengast, B. (2013). Exploratory structural equation
(Eds.), International Handbook of Emotions in Education (pp. 494e519). New modeling. In G. R. Hancock, & R. O. Mueller (Eds.), Quantitative methods in ed-
York: Routledge. ucation and the behavioral sciences: Issues, research, and teaching. Structural
Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., & Jacob, B. (2009). Antecedents and effects of equation modeling: A second course (pp. 395e436). Charlotte, NC: IAP Infor-
teachers’ emotional experiences: An integrated perspective and empirical test. mation Age Publishing.
In P. A. Schutz, & M. Zembylas (Eds.), Advances in teacher emotion research: The Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1996). The dimensions, antecedents, and conse-
impact on teachers’ lives (pp. 129e152). New York: Springer. quences of emotional labor. Academy of Management Review, 21(4), 986e1010.
Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Durksen, T. L., Becker-Kurz, B., & https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1996.9704071861.
Klassen, R. M. (2016). Measuring Teachers’ Enjoyment, Anger, and Anxiety: The Muthe n, L. K., & Muthe n, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus user's guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles,
Teacher Emotions Scales (TES). Contemporary Educational Psychology, 46, CA: Muthe n & Muthe n.
148e163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych. OECD. (2014). A teachers' guide to TALIS 2013: Teaching and learning international
Hagenauer, G., & Volet, S. E. (2014). Teacherestudent relationship at university: An Survey, TALIS. Paris: OECD Publishing. https://dx.doi.org/10.1787/
important yet under-researched field. Oxford Review of Education, 40(3), 9789264216075-en.
370e388. https://doi.org/10.1080/03054985.2014.921613. Pekrun, R., & Bühner, M. (2014). Self-report measures of academic emotions. In
Hargreaves, A. (2004). Inclusive and exclusive educational change: Emotional re- R. Pekrun, & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in
sponses of teachers and implications for leadership. School Leadership & Man- education (pp. 561e579). New York: Routledge.
agement, 24(2), 8e13. Pekrun, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2014). International handbook of emotions in
Harmon-Jones, E., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2016). Anger. In L. F. Barrett, M. Lewis, & education. New York: Routledge.
J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 774e791). New York: Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common
Guilford Publications. method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879e903. https://
Berkeley CA: University of California Press. doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure Prawat, R. S., Byers, J. L., & Anderson, A. H. (1983). An attributional analysis of
analysis: Coventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation teachers' affective reactions to student success and failure. American Educa-
Modeling, 6(1), 1e55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118. tional Research Journal, 20(1), 137e152.
Hülsheger, U. R., & Schewe, A. F. (2011). On the costs and benefits of emotional Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion: Evidence for the
labor: A meta-analysis of three decades of research. Journal of Occupational component process model. Emotion, 23, 1307e1351. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Health Psychology, 16(3), 361e389. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022876. 02699930902928969.
Jacob*, B., Frenzel*, A. C.*, & Stephens, E. J. (2017). Good teaching feels goodebut Scherer, K. R., Wranik, T., Sangsue, J., Tran, V., & Scherer, U. (2004). Emotions in
what is "good teaching"? Exploring teachers' definitions of teaching success everyday life: Probability of occurrence, risk factors, appraisal and reaction
in mathematics. ZDM Mathematics Education, 49(3), 461e473. https://doi.org/ pattern. Social Science Information, 43(4), 499e570. https://doi.org/10.1177/
10.1007/s11858-017-0848-6 [*both authors contributed equally]. 0539018404047701.
Judge, T. A., Parker, S. K., Colbert, A. E., Heller, D., & Ilies, R. (2001). Job satisfaction: A Schuman, V., & Scherer, K. R. (2014). Concepts and structures of emotions. In
cross-cultural review. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil, & R. Pekrun, & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in
C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work, and organizational psy- education (pp. 13e35). New York: Routledge.
chology (pp. 25e52). London, UK: Sage. Schwarzer, R., & Hallum, S. (2008). Perceived teacher self-efficacy as a predictor of
Kavanaugh, D. J., & Bower, G. H. (1985). Mood and self-efficacy: Impact of joy and job stress and burnout: Mediation analyses. Applied Psychology, 57(1), 152e171.
sadness on perceived capabilities. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9(5), https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00359.x.
507e525. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01173005. Sutton, R. (2007). Teachers' anger, frustration, and self-regulation. In P. A. Schutz, &
Khajavi, G. H., Ghonsooly, B., Fatemi, A. H., & Frenzel, A. C. (2018). Antecedents of R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in education (pp. 259e274). Burlington, MA: Academic
pleasant and unpleasant emotions of EFL teachers using an appraisal- Press.
theoretical framework. Iranian Journal of Language Teaching Research, 6(2), Sutton, R. E., Mudrey-Camino, R., & Knight, C. C. (2009). Teachers' emotion regu-
39e55. lation and classroom management. Theory Into Practice, 48, 130e137. https://
Keller, M. M., Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., & Hensley, L. (2014). Exploring doi.org/10.1080/00405840902776418.
teacher emotions: A literature review and an experience sampling study. In Sutton, R. E., & Wheatley, K. F. (2003). Teachers' emotions and teaching: A review of
P. W. Richardson, S. Karabenick, & H. M. G. Watt (Eds.), Teacher Motivation: the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology Review,
Theory and Practice (pp. 69e82). New York: Routledge. 15, 327e358. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1026131715856.
Klassen, R. M., Perry, N. E., & Frenzel, A. C. (2012). Teachers' relatedness with stu- Taxer, J. L., & Frenzel, A. C. (2015). Facets of teachers' emotional lives: A quantitative
dents: An underemphasized component of teachers' basic psychological needs. investigation of teachers' genuine, faked, and hidden emotions. Teaching and
Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 150e165. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Teacher Education, 42, 78e88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2015.03.003.
a0026253. Tschannen-Moran, M., & Hoy, A. W. (2001). Teacher efficacy: Capturing an elusive
Lasky, S. (2000). The cultural and emotional politics of teachereparent interactions. construct. Teaching and Teacher Education, 17(7), 783e805. https://doi.org/
Teaching and Teacher Education, 16(8), 843e860. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0742- 10.1016/s0742-051x(01)00036-1.
051x(00)00030-5. Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical dis-
Lazarus, R. S. (2001). Relational meaning and discrete emotions. In K. Scherer, cussion of the structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at
A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, work. Research in Organizational Behavior, 18, 1e74.
research (pp. 37e67). New York: Oxford University Press. Wharton, A. S. (1993). The affective consequences of service work: Managing
Levine, L. J., Lench, H. C., & Safer, M. A. (2009). Functions of remembering and emotions on the job. Work and Occupations, 20(2), 205e232. https://doi.org/
misremembering emotion. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(8), 1059e1075. 10.1177/0730888493020002004.
https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1610. Willig, C. (2008). Introducing qualitative research in psychology. Open University
Liljestrom, A., Roulston, K., & Demarrais, K. (2007). “There's no place for feeling like Press.
this in the workplace”: Women teachers' anger in school settings. In Yin, H., & Lee, J. C.-K. (2012). Be passionate, but be rational as well: Emotional rules
P. A. Schutz, & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in education (pp. 275e291). New York: for Chinese teachers' work. Teaching and Teacher Education, 28(1), 56e65.
Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2011.08.005.
Macdonald, D. (1999). Teacher attrition: A review of literature. Teaching and Teacher Zembylas, M. (2003). Emotions and teacher identity: A poststructural perspective.
Education, 15, 835e848. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0742-051X(99)00031-1. Teachers and Teaching: Theory and Practice, 9(3), 213e238. https://doi.org/
Marsh, H. W., Lüdtke, O., Robitzsch, A., Trautwein, U., Asparouhov, T., Muthe n, B., 10.1080/13540600309378.

You might also like