Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Copyright 2000 by the Educational Publishing Foundation

2000, Vol. 4, No. 3,259-271 1089-2699/O0/S5.OO DOI: 10.1037//1089-2699.4.3.259

Social Face in Conflict: Effects of Affronts to Person


and Position in China

Dean Tjosvold Haifa Sun


Lingnan University South China Normal University

Social face concerns have been theorized as greatly affecting management of conflict
and as likely to lead to conflict avoidance, especially among collectivist people.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

However, this study proposed that confirmation of personal face, even with strong
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

negative evaluations of position, can facilitate the effective use of open disagreement
to make decisions. Experimental findings obtained in China indicated that confirmation
of personal face, compared with affronts, developed a cooperative context for conflict
management; specifically, it promoted uncertainty about one's original position, ex-
ploration and understanding of the opposing view, efforts to integrate positions, and
confidence in the relationship. Affronts to position had very modest effects on the
dynamics and outcomes of conflict. Results were interpreted as suggesting that group
members can manage their conflicts constructively when they communicate a direct
confirmation of face.

Social face concerns have been theorized to tions of personal face intensify cooperative
be critical for understanding conflict manage- goals, which in turn create open-mindedness
ment, especially among Chinese and other East toward the opposing view and acceptance of the
Asian people (Cocroft & Ting-Toomey, 1994; other person, whereas affronts to the position
Leung, 1997; Ting-Toomey, 1988). Indeed, di- can increase interest in learning more about the
rect disagreements may be avoided because other's arguments.
they easily communicate disrespect (Ding,
1995; Jehn & Weldon, 1992; Kirkbride, Tang,
& Westwood, 1991; Tse, Francis, & Walls, Conflict and Teamwork
1994). However, direct discussion may not in-
evitably affront social face. Confirming the face Research on groups and conflict have pro-
of the other protagonist may reduce concerns ceeded somewhat independently (Bettenhausen,
and promote direct, effective discussion of dif- 1991; Bettenhausen & Murnighan, 1991; Hack-
ferences. This study suggested that people can man, 1990; Rubin, Pruitt, & Kim, 1994; Van de
accept strong negative evaluations of their po- Vliert & Kabanoff, 1990). However, recent
sition, provided they believe their personal face studies have emphasized the critical role of con-
is confirmed. We hypothesized that confirma- flict in groups (Amason, 1996; Eisenhardt,
1989; Jehn, 1995, 1997; Mason & Mitroff,
1981; Peterson & Nemeth, 1996). Groups must
contend with, among other issues, conflicts over
such task issues as the effective and fair distri-
Dean Tjosvold, Department of Management, Lingnan
University, Hong Kong, China; Haifa Sun, Department of bution of work and the best ways to accomplish
Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, their goals, as well as relational issues such as
China. social loafing and personal hostility (Wageman,
Haifa Sun is now at the School of Management, Zhong- 1995). Groups provide interpersonal contexts in
shan University, Guangzhou, China.
We thank our research assistants from South China Nor-
which conflicts occur and attempts to manage
mal University for their able assistance in conducting this them are made.
study. This work was supported by Research Grants Council Research in the Western countries has em-
of Hong Kong Grant LC890/96H. phasized that group members, by dealing with
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Dean Tjosvold, Department of Management, their conflict constructively, can use their dif-
Lingnan University, Tuen Mun, Hong Kong. Electronic ferences to solve problems and make decisions
mail may be sent to tjosvold@ ln.edu. hk. (Gruenfeld, 1995; Schweiger, Sandberg, &
259
260 TJOSVOLD AND SUN

Rechner, 1989; Tetlock, Armor, & Peterson, accepted (Hodgins, Liebeskind, & Schwartz,
1994). Productive conflict management facili- 1996; Tjosvold, 1983). Goffman (1967), a pio-
tates understanding of opposing views and their neer in social face research, proposed that face
integration into high-quality solutions to prob- is "an image of the self delineated in terms of
lems and strengthens group members' confi- approved social attributes" (p. 2). People use
dence that they can work together effectively culturally approved ways to project a favorable
(Barker, Tjosvold, & Andrews, 1988; Deutsch, image, and in conflict that image should reflect
1973, 1980; Tjosvold, 1998). Dealing with con- strength (Deutsch, 1962). Social face therefore
flict is not only realistic but, when skillfully done, can be defined as the image of personal strength
promotes effective group decision making and protagonists want to project in conflict. Show-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

relationships (De Dreu & Van de Vliert, 1997). ing respect to people confirms their face in that
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Conflict researchers have argued that il is not it communicates an acceptance of this positive
conflict itself that is destructive or constructive image, whereas disrespect affronts their face
but how it is managed (Deutsch, 1973). They (Goffman, 1955, 1959).
have recently distinguished the type of conflict Western research has concentrated on dem-
and found that task conflicts can be constructive onstrating how people respond to believing they
but relationship conflicts tend to interfere with have appeared personally weak, referred to as
decision making (Amason, 1996; Jehn, 1995, an affront, loss, or disconh'rmation of face. Peo-
1997). In the present study, we proposed that ple are expected both to be ready to prevent
people can distinguish between relational issues disconfirmation and to make restitution after
and cognitive ones and, in particular, that they they have suffered an affront to face (Brown,
react differently to beliefs that they have ap- 1968, 1970; Goffman, 1955, 1959). Those who
peared personally weak compared with their have lost face do not act consistently with this
position appearing weak. Affronts to personal image but attempt to assert themselves as
face are expected to make conflict relational and strong. Making concessions in conflict is be-
unproductive, whereas affronts to position em- lieved to confirm weakness, whereas defiance
phasize cognitive disagreement and can pro- and counterattack are the aggressive actions
mote an open discussion of differences. used to reassert face (Deutsch, 1962, 1973).
This study made two contributions to the Threats, negative concessions, claims of su-
conflict management and group literature. First, periority and other aggressive strategies have
it used the theory of cooperative and competi- been found to affront social face (Borah, 1963;
tive conflict developed in North America Deutsch & Krauss, 1962, Tjosvold, 1974,
(Deutsch, 1973) to examine the dynamics of 1977a, 1977b). Experiments have documented
social face among Chinese people as they make that in response negotiators will retaliate, coun-
a decision. Social face has usually been invoked terthreat, use aggression, make concessions
as an explanatory variable in research in both slowly, deceive, and refuse to reach an agree-
Eastern and Western countries rather than di- ment (Brown, 1968; Deutsch & Krauss, 1962;
rectly investigated. The role of social face in Tedeschi, Bonoma, & Brown, 1971; Tjosvold &
conflict situations should be examined vigor- Huston, 1978).
ously. Second, this study experimentally inves- Much of the theorizing about social face has
tigated the hypotheses that affronts to personal focused on its critical role among Chinese and
social face induce competitive goals, closed- other collectivists who are considered collectiv-
mindedness, and rejection of the other, whereas ist with a strong emphasis on maintaining rela-
affronts to position are more acceptable and can tionships (Boisot & Child, 1996; Chan, 1963;
increase curiosity to understand the other. Triandis, 1990; Triandis, McCusker, & Hui,
1990). Social scientists have argued that pro-
Affronts to Personal Face tecting social face, although not restricted to
East Asians, is especially valued in Chinese
Affronts to personal face are expected to society to promote relationships (Bond & Lee,
make conflicts relational and unproductive. So- 1981; Brunner & Wang, 1988; Earley, 1997;
cial face is based on the assumption that people Ho, 1975; Hu, 1944; Hwang, 1985; Kirkbride et
attempt to project a desirable image as an indi- al., 1991; Redding & Ng, 1982). Chinese man-
vidual and want assurance that their image is agers and employees seek harmony and com-
SOCIAL FACE IN CONFLICT 261

municate that they respect their partners as ca- incompatible with the face goals of the other
pable and worthy (Cocroft & Ting-Toomey, person. Protagonists compete over who is stron-
1994; Leung, 1997; Ting-Toomey, 1988). Their ger rather than negotiate in ways in which both
collectivism in general and their understanding reach their goals of appearing strong. On the
of social face lead them to be hesitant about other hand, confirming social face has the im-
engaging in aggressive interaction that may pact of strengthening cooperative goals. Protag-
challenge the face of others. Direct disagree- onists help each other appear strong so that they
ment easily communicates a lack of respect and can engage in an open discussion to develop a
is experienced as an attempt to embarrass and mutually beneficial solution.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

challenge one's competence (Ding, 1995; Kirk- Considerable research supports the proposi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

bride, Tang, & Westwood, 1991; Tse et al., tion that the conclusion of competition has per-
1994; Jehn & Weldon, 1992). Western manag- vasive effects on the dynamics and outcomes of
ers are advised that they should be careful to conflict (Deutsch, 1973; Tjosvold, 1985, 1998).
protect the face of their East Asian colleagues or With competitive goals, protagonists often be-
risk severe disruptions in joint ventures and come locked into mutual hostility and costly
other international arrangements (Tung, 1991; aggression. Negotiators whose face was af-
Yeung & Tung, 1996).
fronted, compared with those whose face was
Although the impact of social face on conflict affirmed, were found to view the conflict as
management has been thought to be powerful competitive and to reject the other person and
and general, little empirical work, especially in position (Tjosvold, 1974; Tjosvold, Johnson, &
East Asia, has directly studied social face (Bond Fabrey, 1980; Tjosvold, Johnson, & Lerner,
& Lee, 1981; Ting-Toomey, 1988). Strong 1981). Affronts to personal face are expected to
needs for social face are invoked to understand induce protagonists to reject the opposing posi-
results indicating that Chinese prefer avoiding tion and discussant, whereas confirmation leads
and accommodating strategies. Research di- to an openness to the opposing person and po-
rectly on social face, especially among Chinese
sition. The present study examined whether
people, is needed to document and clarify the
confirmation of face reinforces cooperative
processes by which social face has its theorized
goals and contributes to productive conflict
effects.
management among Chinese people.
Recent research has suggested that disagree-
Cooperative and Competitive Conflict ment, especially within a cooperative context,
Deutsch (1962, 1973) used the theory of co- induces epistemic curiosity (Berlyne, 1963).
operation and competition to understand re- Protagonists, confronted with an opposing po-
sponses to a loss of face. Denning conflict as sition, begin to doubt the adequacy of their own
incompatible activities, he argued that protago- perspective (Tjosvold, 1985, 1998). Feeling un-
nists can emphasize cooperative or competitive certain that their present views are complete and
goals. With cooperative goals, protagonists be- accurate, they are motivated to search the argu-
lieve that as one moves toward goal attainment, ments and perspective of opposing positions.
the other also moves toward goal achievement. They have been found, for example, to feel
They tend to view a conflict as a mutual prob- more uncertain about the adequacy of their po-
lem that needs common consideration and so- sition, ask more questions of the other, demon-
lution. Protagonists who emphasize their com- strate more understanding, and incorporate ele-
petitive interests believe that as one succeeds, ments of the opposing position into their deci-
the other moves away from goal attainment. sion (Tjosvold, 1982; Tjosvold & Deemer,
They tend to view the conflict as a win-lose 1980; Tjosvold & Johnson, 1977, 1978). Avoid-
struggle; if the other wins, they lose. ing disagreement and discussing topics with
This theoretical perspective describes the persons with a similar position induce a com-
processes of social face on conflict manage- placent acceptance of one's own view as com-
ment. Disconfirming social face has the impact plete and an illusion of understanding the oth-
of strengthening competitive goals. Affronted er's arguments. Direct, open disagreement, not
negotiators believe that they must reassert conflict avoidance, makes cooperative work
themselves. Their goal of appearing strong is more productive and enhancing.
262 TJOSVOLD AND SUN

Affronts to Position Confederates


Theorists on social face suggest that protag- Four male undergraduate students were re-
onists want to be seen as personally strong and cruited to be confederates. They were given 15
effective. Whether others disagree with their hr of training in how to induce participants'
position and evaluate their position negatively involvement and commitment in the experimen-
is secondary to their needs for personal face. tal situation and how to negotiate in a standard
Especially when evaluations of protagonists as manner and carry out the experimental induc-
personally strong are present, evaluations of tions. All confederates were trained to say the
their positions may have little impact on feel- same thing using similar language, syntax, and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ings of losing face or subsequent competitive length, except for the differences required by
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

beliefs. They may react to affronts to their po- the operationalizations of the independent vari-
sition in a task-oriented, cognitive manner by ables. We observed the confederates piloting six
concluding their position may have weaknesses participants each to ensure their competence in
that they should examine and consider. Affronts fulfilling the confederate's role. Each confeder-
to position may then intensify uncertainty and ate participated in all four conditions in a
encourage the search for additional information counter-balanced way.
about the opposing views, resulting in more
openness in disagreement.
Independent Variables
On the basis of the above research and rea-
soning, we hypothesized that (a) confirmation Affront-confirmation of face and affront-
of personal face, compared with affront, induces confirmation of position were crossed to form
perceived cooperative goals, uncertainty, explo- four conditions. The affront-confirmation in-
ration of the opposing position, search for more ductions were implemented through a question-
information, a positive, nonconfrontational ap- naire that was unexpectedly exchanged in the
proach, knowledge of the opposing arguments, middle of the negotiations. The experimenter
confidence in the relationship, effort to integrate gave the participant and the confederate a ques-
positions, and integrated decisions; and (b) af- tionnaire that asked them to rate each other's
fronts to position, compared with confirmation, competence on a 7-point Likert scale, which for
induce uncertainty and exploration of the op- all scales in this study ranged from 1 (a very
posing position. little) to 7 (a great deal). For participants in the
This study supplements previous North face-confirmed condition, the confederate rated
American research by testing the ideas of social the participant as 6 on the two personal face
face and cooperation and competition in Chi- items. They also wrote, "I consider the other
nese people. Although research in the West discussant a strong and capable person." In the
cannot be assumed to apply in the East (Hofst- face-affronted condition, the confederate rated
ede, 1993), theories of conflict management that the participant as 2 on the personal capability
have been validated in the East as well as the items and wrote, "I think the other discussant is
West may be particularly useful in such settings a weak and incapable person." In the position-
as international joint ventures. confirmed condition, the confederate rated the
participant as 6 on the two position items and
wrote, "I find the other's position strong and his
Method reasoning for this position effective." For the
position-affronted condition, the confederate
Participants
rated the participant as 2 on the position evalu-
Eighty male undergraduates majoring in ation items and wrote, "I find the other's posi-
management who were recruited from a univer- tion weak and his reasoning for this position
sity in Guangzhou, China, volunteered to par- ineffective."
ticipate in a study on communication in deci- We adopted these operations because they
sion making. They were randomly assigned to could directly induce confirmation-affront and
four conditions, with 20 participants in each clearly distinguish between personal face and
condition. They received a gift for their positions and therefore have experimental real-
participation. ism. Previous research has tended to use indi-
SOCIAL FACE IN CONFLICT 263

rect ways of affronting face (i.e., threat) and The postdiscussion questionnaire also fo-
then use social face to understand results. It cused on the relationship between the partici-
should be noted that affronts are seldom com- pant and confederate. Relationship confidence
municated in the manner used in this experi- was measured by four items concerning the
ment and therefore the operations are low on participant's willingness to work with the other.
mundane realism. The final set of measures involved integration
of arguments. After the discussion, the partici-
Dependent Variables pants outlined their decision. These decisions
were rated as 1 if they reflected only the partic-
There were seven sets of dependent mea- ipant's assigned position, 2 if they reflected
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sures. To measure the participants' view of the some ideas from the opposing view, and 3 if
negotiations as cooperative, we had them com- they extensively incorporated the other's posi-
plete a postnegotiation questionnaire. Their re- tion. Similarly, participants indicated their rea-
sponses to three 7-point Likert-type scale ques- soning for their decision and this was coded as 1
tions were combined to measure perceived co- if it reflected only the participant's assigned
operation (see Appendix for this and other position, 2 if it reflected some ideas from the
measures included on the postdiscussion ques- opposing view, and 3 if it extensively incorpo-
tionnaire). They also indicated on five items the rated the other's position. Participants also rated
extent to which they had mutual interests with their effort to find a new, integrated solution to
the other discussant. the problem on six items using 7-point scales.
The next set of dependent measures involved As the experimental materials were originally
epistemic curiosity. During the negotiations, the written in English, several bilingual researchers
confederate counted the number of questions translated it into Chinese (Mandarin). They
the participants asked indicating that they reached agreement on the translated version.
wanted more information about the confeder-
ate's position. On the postdiscussion question- Procedure
naire, participants indicated the extent to which
they felt uncertain about their own position. The experiment was conducted in three
They also rated the extent to which they had phases; Participants prepared for a discussion
explored the position of the other discussant on about a work distribution issue with a partner,
six items using 7-point scales. discussed the issue with a person with an op-
Another set of measures dealt with approach posing opinion, and were debriefed. Two par-
to the discussion. Participants indicated on four ticipants and two confederates (posing as par-
items the extent to which they and the other ticipants) were scheduled at each session.
rebuked and confronted each other in a tough To begin Phase 1, Haifa Sun divided partic-
manner. ipants and confederates into two groups, each
The next set focused on the emotional tone of with one participant and one confederate. He
the discussant. The confederates took notes on escorted them into different rooms and outlined
the participants' emotional response to the ex- that the research studied communication be-
perimental inductions. Two students coded the tween persons in decision making and that they
notes independently. Participants who consis- would take the role of supervisors at East Asian
tently showed aggressive and unhappy attitudes Electronics. They were to read the written in-
were coded as negative; those with respectful, structions and discuss them with each other to
happy attitudes were coded as positive; partici- understand the situation and their role.
pants with mild and little sign of satisfaction or The participants read that as supervisors they
dissatisfaction were coded as neutral. The two had to meet with employees about job rotation
coders agreed on 72 out of 80 cases and they (Maier, 1952). The employees had developed a
met and resolved their disagreements. practice of trading their positions every hour.
Other measures involved learning. After the The supervisor, as a representative of manage-
discussion, participants were asked to list the ment, opposed this job rotation. Participants
arguments of the other discussant. They re- read that they would discuss this distribution of
ceived one point for each argument accurately work with a person from the other group in
recalled. Phase 2.
264 TJOSVOLD AND SUN

Participants were also given a briefing sheet person and the extent to which the other showed
outlining six arguments supporting their posi- respect to the participant as a person. (Cronbach
tion. The supervisor arguments emphasized in- alpha = .90). Participants in the face-confirmed
creased efficiency. They read, for example, that condition (M = 5.43, SD = 1.00), compared
a time study revealed that workers differed sub- with the face-affronted condition (M = 3.36,
stantially in their efficiency at different posi- SD = 1.44), indicated that they were seen as
tions and that the competition's new assembly more competent persons by the other discus-
made the need for increased efficiency more sant, F(l, 78) = 62.94, p < .01. Participants in
pressing. the position-confirmed condition (M = 4.75,
In Phase 2, the confederates exchanged SD = 1.48), compared with the position-affront
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

rooms and were introduced as the employee condition (M - 3.54, SD = 1.47), indicated on
representative. Discussants were reminded to two items that the other regarded their position
present their opening positions in 2 min and as strong and their arguments as effective
then to negotiate freely for the remainder of 18 (Cronbach alpha = .90), F(l, 77) = 13.31,
min. Eight min into the negotiations, the exper- p < .01. Therefore, it can be concluded that
imenter entered the room and asked them to the inductions to test the hypotheses were
complete the questionnaires that included the successful.
experimental inductions. Then he unexpectedly
exchanged the questionnaires "to increase com- Results
munication" so that the participant could read
the confederate's ratings. The experimenter After obtaining a significant MANOVA re-
asked them to continue negotiating before he sult, we conducted univariate analyses to test
left the room and later gave them a warning of 2 the hypotheses. The results (see Table 1) sup-
min. Then the experimenter entered the room, port the first hypothesis that affront to face
asked the participant to complete the decision affects the perceived cooperation, dynamics,
report form and the postdiscussion question- and outcomes of conflict. Participants whose
naire, and escorted the confederate out of the face was confirmed compared with those whose
room. face was affronted perceived more cooperative
The participants were then fully debriefed, goals between themselves and the other discus-
thanked, and asked not to discuss their experi- sant and worked more to promote their mutual
ence with others who might participate. All interests than did face-affronted participants.
participants were then given a small gift. They demonstrated more epistemic curiosity in
that they asked more questions of the other
Preparation and Induction Checks discussant. They indicated more uncertainty
about whether their position was completely
At the end of Phase 1, participants were satisfactory and explored the opposing views
asked to complete a short questionnaire to mea- more than did participants whose face was af-
sure their preparation for the upcoming discus- fronted. Face-affronted participants confronted
sion. Overall, participants indicated on 7-point the other in a tough manner. Moreover, face-
Likert-type scales that they had sufficient time confirmed participants demonstrated a generally
and information (M = 5.38, SD = 1.15), felt positive attitude toward the other during the
prepared to defend their position (M = 5.90, discussion, whereas face-affronted participants
SD = 1.04), and were personally involved in the were negative (Table 2).
study (M = 5.86, SD = 1.18). As expected, Participants whose personal face was con-
there were no significant differences among the firmed rather than affronted learned more in that
conditions. These results suggested participants they identified more arguments used by the
were prepared to discuss their opposing position other discussant (Table 1). They also developed
with the other discussant. more confidence that they could work with the
To check on the effectiveness of the induc- other discussant in the future than did face-
tions, we had participants rate four items in the affronted participants.
postdiscussion questionnaire indicating the ex- Personal face-confirmed participants made
tent to which the other person thought the par- more integrated decisions than did face-af-
ticipant was a valuable, important, and capable fronted participants, but this difference did not
SOCIAL FACE IN CONFLICT 265

Table 1
Significant Comparisons on Dependent Measures
Confirm face Affront face
Confirm Affront Confirm Affront Significant
Variable position position position position comparisons F(U 76)
Cooperative goals
M 4.53 4.24 3.87 3.53 Person 5.59*
SD 1.21 1.49 1.33 1.15
Mutual interest
M 5.59 5.12 4.59 3.65 Person 36.49**
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

SD 0.74 1.13 0.90 0.85 Position 11.87**


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Uncertainty
M 4.07 4.62 3.79 3.61 Person 8.02**
SD 1.03 1.02 0.81 1.19
Questions asked
M 2.05 1.50 0.55 1.15 Person 8.06**
SD 1.96 1.43 0.76 1.42
Explore
M 5.45 5.63 5.08 5.04 Person 7.01**
SD 0.88 0.87 0.91 0.83
Confrontation
M 2.15 2.83 3.53 3.36 Person 10.58**
SD 1.00 1.39 1.55 1.26
Arguments identified
M 1.90 2.10 1.50 1.20 Person 5.71*
SD 1.37 1.12 1.19 0.95
Relationship confidence
M 4.75 4.69 3.49 2.86 Person 25.27**
SD 1.56 1.17 1.38 1.35
Integrated decision
M 1.95 2.50 1.75 1.95 Person 2.53f
SD 1.00 1.24 1.07 0.87
Integrative reasoning
M 2.15 2.50 1.75 2.05 Person 2.83f
SD 1.27 1.32 0.85 1.00
Integrative effort
M 3.92 3.97 3.32 3.64 Person 5.59*
SD 1.03 1.25 0.60 1.25
t p < .10 (marginally significant). *p < .05. **p < .03.

reach statistical significance. They also indi- their position or more interest in understanding
cated more integrative reasoning, but this dif- the other's position than did position-confirmed
ference was only marginally significant. Face- participants.
confirmed participants did indicate that they had
made greater efforts to integrate than did face- Discussion
affronted participants.
Results suggest that affronts to position had Our results support the argument that people
much more modest impact on the dynamics and can distinguish between relational and task is-
outcomes of conflict than did affronts to face. sues involved in conflict, and that affronts to
Participants whose position was confirmed personal face make conflict more relational and
compared with affronted indicated that they unproductive. Discussants reacted strongly to
were more committed to pursuing mutual ben- affronts to personal face by becoming closed-
efit. However, contrary to the second hypothe- minded toward the other's position and reject-
sis, position-affronted participants did not indi- ing of the other as a person. Affronts to position
cate more uncertainty about the adequacy of were much less impactive. They did not appear
266 TJOSVOLD AND SUN

Table 2
Emotional Responses of Participants in Each Experimental Condition
Confirm person Affront person
Confirm Affront Confirm Affront
Response position position position position Total cases
Positive 14 7 2 1 24
Neutral 6 13 8 6 33
Negative 0 0 10 13 23
Total cases 20 20 20 20 so
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Note. x*(6, N = 80) = 45.42, p < .01. Data represent number of participants coded with
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

each response.

to induce much rejection of the other nor did tion of personal face and cooperative goals ap-
they encourage uncertainty and openness to- pear to be highly reinforcing, whereas affronts
ward the other's view. induce competition. However, the similarity of
North American research on social face and findings and use of the same theory do not
cooperative and competitive conflict proved to suggest that Chinese and Westerners interact in
be useful for understanding conflict manage- highly similar ways. The actions they take to
ment among Chinese people. As suggested by communicate respect and develop cooperative
previous theorizing about Chinese organiza- goals as well as the extent to which they engage
tions, we found social face concerns to play a in these actions may be quite discrepant (Wall,
significant role in managing conflict and devel- 1990; Westwood & Chan, 1995). The cues that
oping relationships in China. When they felt affront face and the specific ways that affronted
personally accepted as competent, Chinese par- people demonstrate defensive closed-minded-
ticipants were more likely to believe their goals ness may be quite different in China and North
were cooperatively linked and were more open America. The operations of the theory depend
to the other person and position. Under these on the cultural context.
conditions, social face concerns did not lead to However, if cooperative and competitive
conflict avoidance but to the productive use of conflict dynamics are similar in the East and
conflict. When personal face was affronted, so- West, Chinese and Western partners in joint
cial face concerns induced closed-mindedness business ventures and in other international ar-
and rejection that disrupted attempts to integrate rangements may be able to agree that they
positions and undermined relationships. should strive to disagree directly and respect-
Results specified that it is affronts to the fully and develop underlying cooperative goals
person that have such disruptive effects on man- so that they can work productively and syner-
aging conflict. Affronts to position do not ap- gistically. A future research challenge is to un-
pear to have strong effects. Contrary to common derstand how people from the West and from
theorizing about how Chinese people avoid the East can both effectively communicate ac-
conflict, we discovered that the participants did ceptance of personal face and disagree directly
not feel personally slighted because another dis- and cooperatively.
agreed with their position, even when the pro- The affront to position did not induce more
tagonist communicated a strong, clear negative uncertainty and exploration of the opposing
evaluation of their position. As suggested by view. Evidence does not support the reasoning
social face theorizing (Deutsch, 1962; Goffman, that affront to position heightens epistemic cu-
1967), beliefs that one has appeared personally riosity. One possibility is that the disagreement
incapable—not that one's arguments are dis- itself, especially when supplemented with con-
puted—questioned face and provoked rejection. firmation of face, had already developed a sig-
Consistent with its universalistic aspirations nificant level of uncertainty and exploration so
(Deutsch, 1973), the theory of cooperation and that effects of affront to position were masked.
competition proved useful for understanding the Or it may be that affronts to position are gen-
dynamics of affront to face in China. Confirma- erally ineffective for inducing exploration. Pro-
SOCIAL FACE IN CONFLICT 267

tagonists whose position is affronted may sim- suggest that major findings may have both in-
ply reciprocate by becoming defensive and re- ternal and external validity (Tjosvold, 1974;
jecting the other's position. The finding that Tjosvold et al., 1980; Tjosvold et al., 1981;
affronts to position induced less pursuit of mu- Tjosvold, Moy, & Sasaki, 1996). It would be
tual interests supports this reasoning. Indeed, it desirable to provide direct experimental verifi-
may be that affronts to position, when there is cation of the theory in actual organizational
no direct evidence regarding personal face, are settings.
assumed to be affronts to face and provoke
considerable defensiveness. Research is needed Practical Implications
to investigate these speculations.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Overall, our results support the traditional Managing conflict is increasingly seen as im-
theorizing about the importance of social face portant in working as a team and solving orga-
concerns in conflict management, especially in nizational problems (De Dreu & Van de Vliert,
China. However, they do not suggest that these 1997). Results of this study confirm that Chi-
concerns inevitably make constructive conflict nese people can use open discussion effectively,
management unlikely. Social face concerns, especially if they confirm social face. Social
when confirmed, allowed participants to make face concerns do not necessarily impose conflict
use of the conflict to strengthen cooperative avoidance and a reluctance to participate and
goals, search for and understand the opposing voice ideas. Indeed, by directly confirming so-
position, work toward integration, and develop cial face, Chinese managers and employees can
confidence in their relationship. Social face con- use conflict to understand each other's position,
cerns, when appropriately met, can help people strengthen their work relationship, and work
make productive use of conflict to a great toward integrating their views to create new
extent. solutions.
More generally, the results suggest the
Limitations value of using cooperative and competitive
conflict theory to strengthen conflict manage-
The results of this study are of course limited ment capabilities, even in a collectivist cul-
by the sample and operations. The Chinese sam- ture like China. To develop productive coop-
ple may have made identifying effects of social erative relationships, employees can together
face affronts easier, but the sample may be a develop shared goals, integrated roles, com-
particularly stringent test of the value of open mon tasks, and shared reward distributions
discussion and affront to positions for problem that build cooperative goals (Hanlon, Meyer,
solving. The confirmation and affront induc- & Taylor, 1994; Tjosvold & Tjosvold, 1995).
tions were communicated through an unexpect- Then they feel that they are on the same side
edly exchanged questionnaire, whereas direct so that as one succeeds, others succeed; as
verbal and nonverbal behavior are most often one fails, others too are threatened. These
used. Participants had a shorter time perspective conclusions can greatly aid conflict manage-
and fewer tangible outcomes involved than do ment (Deutsch, 1990).
most organizational members. The discussants Our findings support emerging research in
defended positions that could be integrated. Al- the West that suggests relational conflicts are
though this is, a typical assumption in conflict particularly disruptive of making decisions
and decision-making research, some conflicts in and building relationships in conflict (Kramer
organizations cannot be integrated. The sample & Messick, 1995) and specifies that affronts
is not representative. However, students at this to personal face can instigate relational con-
Chinese university came from several provinces flicts. However, findings did not indicate that
of China. Lask conflicts, at least as initiated by strong
Spector and Brannick (1995) have argued negative evaluations of the position, induce
that the most effective way to overcome meth- more exchange and openness. When rela-
odological weaknesses is to test ideas with dif- tional issues were minimized and confirma-
ferent methods. Previous experiments in North tion of personal face confirmed, Chinese pro-
America with different situations and opera- tagonists were found to use open discussion
tions as well as field studies there and in China of differences effectively. They felt they had
268 TJOSVOLD AND SUN

cooperative goals; asked more questions of Chan, W. T. (1963). A source book in Chinese phi-
the other; felt less certain of the adequacy of losophy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
their original position; explored the opposing Press.
view; discussed in a positive, nonconfronta- Cocroft, B. A. K., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1994). Face-
tional manner; understood the opposing argu- work in Japan and in the United States. Interna-
tional Journal of Intercultural Relations, 18, 469-
ments; and worked to integrate the positions. 506.
Efforts to strengthen the relationship through Deal, T. E., & Kennedy, A. A. (1982). Corporate
confirmation of personal face can help pro- culture: The rites and rituals of corporate life.
tagonists discuss problems comprehensively Reading, MA: Addis on-Wesley.
and constructively. De Dreu, C , & Van de Vliert, E. (Eds.). (1997).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Using conflict in organizations. Thousand Oaks,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

CA: Sage.
References Deutsch, M. (1962). Cooperation and trust: Some
theoretical notes. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska
Amason, A. C. (1996). Distinguishing the effects of symposium on motivation (pp. 275-319). Lincoln:
functional and dysfunctional conflict on strategic University of Nebraska Press.
decision making: Resolving a paradox for top Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict. New
management teams. Academy of Management Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Journal, 39, 123-148. Deutsch, M. (1980). Fifty years of conflict. In L.
Barker, J., Tjosvold, D., & Andrews, I. R. (1988). Festinger (Ed.), Retrospections on social psychol-
Conflict approaches of effective and ineffective ogy (pp. 46-77). New York: Oxford University
managers: A field study in a matrix organization. Press.
Journal of Management Studies, 25, 167—178. Deutsch, M. (1990). Sixty years of conflict. The
Berlyne, D. E. (1963). Motivational problems raised International Journal of Conflict Management, 1,
by exploratory and epistemic behavior. In S. Koch 237-263.
(Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 5; pp. Deutsch, M., & Krauss, R. M. (1962.). Studies in
75-93), New York: McGraw-Hill. interpersonal bargaining. Journal of Conflict Res-
Bettenhausen, K. L. (1991). Five years of groups olution, 6, 52—76.
research: What we have learned and what needs to Ding, D. Z. (1995, August). In search of determi-
be addressed. Journal of Management, 17, 345- nants of Chinese conflict management styles in
381. joint ventures: An integrated approach. Paper pre-
Bettenhausen, K. L., & Murnighan, J. K. (1991). The sented at the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the
development of an intragroup norm and the effects Association of Management, Vancouver, Canada.
of interpersonal and structural challenges. Admin- Earley, P. C. (1997). Doing an about-face: Social
istrative Science Quarterly, 36, 20-35. motivation and cross-cultural currents. In P. C.
Boisot, M., & Child, J. (1996). From fiefs to clans Earley & M. Erez (Eds.), New perspectives on
and network capitalism: Explaining China's international industrial/organizational psychology
emerging economic order. Administrative Science (pp. 243-275). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Quarterly, 41, 600-628. Eisenhardt, K. M. (1989). Making fast strategic de-
Bond, M. H., & Lee, P. W. H. (1981). Face saving in cisions in high velocity environments. Academy of
Chinese culture: A discussion and experimental Management Journal, 32, 543-76.
study of Hong Kong students. In A. Y. C. King & Goffman, E. (1955). On face work. Psychiatry, 18,
R. P. L. Lee (Eds.), Social life and development in 213-231.
Hong Kong(pp. 289-303). Hong Kong, China: Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in ev-
The Chinese University Press. eryday life. New York: Doubleday.
Borah, L. A. (1963). The effects of threat in bargain- Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays in
ing: Critical and experimental analysis. Journal of face-to-face behavior. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 4, 107-122. Gruyter.
Brown, B. R. (1968). The effects of the need to Gruenfeld, D. H. (1995). Status, ideology, and inte-
maintain face in interpersonal bargaining. Journal grative complexity on the U.S. supreme court:
of Experimental Social Psychology, 4, 107-122. Rethinking the politics of political decision mak-
Brown, B. R. (1970). Face saving following experi- ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
mentally induced embarrassment. Journal of Ex- 68, 5-20.
perimental Social Psychology, 7, 490-502. Hackman, J. R. (1987). The design of work teams. In
Brunncr, J. A., & Wang, Y. (1988). Chinese negoti- J. Lorsch (Ed.), Handbook of organizational be-
ating and the concept of face. Journal of Interna- havior (pp. 315-342). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren-
tional Consumer Marketing, 1, 27-43. tice Hall.
SOCIAL FACE IN CONFLICT 269

Hackman, J. R. (1990). Groups that work (and those Rubin, J. Z., Pruitt, D. G., & Kim, S. H. (1994).
that don't). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate, and settle-
Hanlon, S. C , Meyer, D. C , & Taylor, R. R. (1994). ment (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Consequences of gainsharing: A field experiment Schweiger, D. M., Sandberg, W. R., & Rechner, P. L.
revisited. Group and Organizational Management, (1989). Experiential effects of dialectical inquiry,
79,87-111. devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to
Ho, D. Y. (1975). On the concept of face. American strategic decision making. Academy of Manage-
Journal of Sociology, 81, 867-884. ment Journal, 32, 745-772.
Hodgins, H. S., Liebeskind, E., & Schwartz, W. Spector, P. E., & Brannick, M. T. (1995). The nature
(1996). Getting out of hot water: Facework in and effects of method variance in organizational
research. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

social predicaments. Journal of Personality and


International Review of Industrial and Organiza-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Social Psychology, 71, 300-314.


tional Psychology (pp. 249-274). Chichester, En-
Hofstede, G. (1993). Cultural constraints in manage-
gland: Wiley.
ment theories. The Academy of Management Ex-
Tedeschi, J. T., Bonoma, T. B., & Brown, R. C.
ecutive, 7, 81-94.
(1971). A paradigm for the study of coercive
Hu, H. C. (1944, January-March). The Chinese power. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 15, 197-
concepts of face. American Anthropologist, 46, 223.
45-64. Tetlock, P. E., Armor, D., & Peterson, R. S. (1994).
Hwang, K. K. (1985). Face and favour: The Chinese The slavery debate in antebellum America: Cog-
power game. American Journal of Sociology, 92, nitive style, value conflicts, and the limits of com-
944-974. promise. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Jehn, K. A. (1995). A multimethod examination of chology, 66, 115-126.
the benefits and detriments of intragroup conflict. Ting-Toomey, S. (1988). A face negotiation theory.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 256—282. In Y. Y. Kim & W. B. Gudykunst (Eds.), Theory
Jehn, K. A. (1997). A qualitative analysis of conflict and intercultural communication (pp. 47-92).
types and dimensions in organizational groups. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 530-557. Tjosvold, D. (1974). Threat as a low-power person's
Jehn. K., & Weldon, E. (1992, August). A compara- strategy in bargaining: Social face and tangible
tive study of managerial attitudes toward conflict outcomes. International Journal of Group Ten-
in the United States and the People's Republic of sions, 4, 494-510.
China: Issues of theory and measurement. Paper Tjosvold, D. (1977a). The effects of the constituent's
presented at the meeting of the Academy of Man- affirmation and the opposing negotiator's self-pre-
agement, Las Vegas, NV. sentation in bargaining between unequal status
Kirkbride, P. S., Tang, S. F. Y., & Westwood, R. I. groups. Organizational Behaviour and Human
(1991). Chinese conflict preferences and negotiat- Performance, 18, 146-157.
ing behaviour: Cultural and psychological influ- Tjosvold, D. (1977b). Low-power person's strategies
ences. Organizational Studies, 12, 365-386. in bargaining: Negotiability of demand, maintain-
Kramer, R. M., & Messick, D. M. (1995). Negotia- ing face, and race. International Journal of Group
tion as a social process. Thousand Oaks, CA: Tensions, 7, 29-42.
Sage. Tjosvold, D. (1982). Effects of the approach to con-
Leung, K. (1997). Negotiation and reward allocations troversy on superiors' incorporation of subordi-
nates* information in decision making. Journal of
across cultures. In P. C. Earley & M. Erez (Eds.),
Applied Psychology, 67, 189-193.
New perspectives on international industrial/orga-
Tjosvold, D. (1983). Social face in conflict: A cri-
nizational psychology, (pp. 640-675) San Fran-
tique. International Journal of Group Tension, 13,
cisco: Jossey-Bass. 49-64.
Maier, N. R. F. (1952). Principles of human behav- Tjosvold, D. (1985). Implications of controversy re-
ior. New York: Wiley. search for management. Journal of Management,
Mason, R. O., & Mitroff, I. I. (1981). Challenging 11, 21-37.
strategic planning assumptions. New York: Wiley. Tjosvold, D. (1998). The cooperative and competi-
Peterson, R. S., & Nemeth, C. J. (1996). Focus versus tive goal approach to conflict: Accomplishments
flexibility: Majority and minority influence can and challenges. Applied Psychology: An Interna-
both improve performance. Personality and Social tional Review, 47, 285-313.
Psychology Bulletin, 21, 14-23. Tjosvold, D., & Deemer, D. K. (1980). Effects of
Redding, S. G., &. Ng, M. (1982). The role of "face" controversy within a cooperative or competitive
in the organizational perceptions of Chinese man- context on organizational decision making. Jour-
agers. Organizational Studies, 3, 201—219. nal of Applied Psychology, 65, 590-595.
270 TJOSVOLD AND SUN

Tjosvold, D., & Huston, T. L. (1978). Social face and (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1989
resistance to compromise in bargaining. Journal of (Vol. 37, pp. 41-133). Lincoln: University of Ne-
Social Psychology, 104, 57-68. braska Press.
Tjosvold, D., & Johnson, D. W. (1977). The effects Triandis, H. C , McCusker, C , & Hui, C. H. (1990).
of controversy on cognitive perspective taking. Multimethod probes of individualism and collec-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 679-685. tivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Tjosvold, D., & Johnson, D. W. (1978). Controversy ogy, 59, 1006-1020.
within a cooperative or competitive context and Tse, D. K., Francis, J., & Walls, J. (1994). Cultural
cognitive perspective taking. Contemporary Edu- differences in conducting intra- and inter-cultural
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cational Psychology, 5, 376—368. negotiations: A Sino-Canadian comparison. Jour-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Tjosvold, D., Johnson, D. W., & Fabrey, L. (1980). nal of International Business Studies, 24,537-555.
Effects of controversy and defensiveness on cog- Tung, R. (1991). Handshakes across the sea: Cross-
nitive perspective-taking. Psychological Reports, cultural negotiating for business success. Organi-
47, 1043-1053. zational Dynamics, 14, 30-40.
Tjosvold, D., Johnson, D. W., & Lerner, J. (1981). Van de Vliert, E., & Kabanoff, B. (1990). Toward
Effects of affirmation and acceptance on incorpo- theory-based measures of conflict management.
ration of opposing information in problem-solving. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 199-209.
Journal of Social Psychology, 114, 103-110. Wageman, R. (1995). Interdependence and group
Tjosvold, D., Moy, J., & Sasaki, S. (1996). Managing effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly,
for customers and employees in Hong Kong: The 40, 145-180.
quality and teamwork challenges. Journal of Mar- Wall J. A., Jr. (1990). Managers in the People's
ket-Focused Management, 1, 339-357. Republic of China. Academy of Management Ex-
Tjosvold, D., & Tjosvold, M. M. (1995). Cooperation ecutive, 4, 19-32.
theory, constructive controversy, and effective- Westwood, R. L, & Chan, A. (1995). The transfer-
ness: Learning from crises. In R. A. Guzzo & E. ability of leadership training in the East Asian
Salas (Eds.), Team effectiveness and decision mak- context. Asia Pacific Business Review. 2, 68-92.
ing in organizations: San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, Yeung, L, & Tung, R. (1996). Achieving business
79-112. success in Confucian societies: The importance of
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross cultural studies of in- guanxi (connections). Organizational Dynamics,
dividualism and collectivism. In J. J. Berman 25, 54-65.
SOCIAL FACE IN CONFLICT 271

Appendix

Postdiscussion Measures

Scale Item Alpha


Cooperation Your benefit and the other's were consistent .64
Your goals and the other's goal went together
You supported each other's interest
Mutual Interest You listened to the other's presentation of his argument .72
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The other was interested in listening to your argument


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Both parties were empathetic toward each other emotionally


The other discussed issue coldly
You liked the way the other discussed with you
Internal Uncertainty The other's way of discussing made you doubt the quality of your argument .69
The other's way of discussing made you question the effectiveness of your
argument
The other's way of discussing reinforced your willingness to understand the
other's argument
The other's way of discussing motivated you to have a deeper discussion
with the other
The other's way of discussing stimulated you to collect more evidence to
persuade the other
The other's way of discussing made you listen more carefully to the other's
arguments
Explore the Other's Position You considered the other's opinion .81
You tried to understand the reasonable components of the other's argument
You understood the issue discussed from the other's position
You tried to understand the other's feelings and emotions
You tried to explore the other's intentions
You tried to explore the other's position
Confrontation To what extent did you rebuke the other .77
To what extent did the other rebuke you
To what extent did you criticize the other impolitely
To what extent did the other criticize you impolitely
Confidence in the Other You would recommend the other to an important position for the way he .87
discussed with you
You found the other to be a creative person
You were willing to work together with the other in the future
Integrate Efforts You tried to find a solution accepted by both parties .72
You thought the other's position and argument had merit
You integrated both parties' opinions
You were willing to satisfy the other's requirement to have job rotation
You agreed with the other's argument for job rotation
You have changed from your original position

You might also like