TP2 - Love For Logos Evaluating The Congruency Between Brand Symbols and Typefaces and Their Relation To Emotional Words

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Original Article

Love for logos: Evaluating the


congruency between brand
symbols and typefaces and their
relation to emotional words
Received (in revised form): 23th October 2014

Alejandro Salgado-Montejo
Currently conducting his DPhil studies at the Crossmodal Research Laboratory, University of Oxford. His research focuses on
how visual information can convey emotion and influence decision making and reward estimation. He has worked with several
multinational corporations in topics, such as packaging development and evaluation, food, behavioural finance, design, film,
marketing, innovation, social behaviour and entrepreneurship. He has presented in diverse events and academic symposia
which include TEDxOxford, The Royal Institution, ESOMAR, SXSW, FENS, EBBS and The Pangborn Society Meeting.

Carlos Velasco
DPhil student of Experimental Psychology at the Crossmodal Research Laboratory, Department of Experimental
Psychology, University of Oxford. His research focuses on crossmodal correspondences and visual attention, and its
applications to multisensory experience design. He has participated in both academic and industry congresses including
SPSP, IMRF, Pangborn, SXSW, ABA, Digital Biscuit and ESOMAR. His research has been published in journals, such as Food
Quality and Preference, Journal of Sensory Studies and Flavour, and has been covered by several media news, for example, BBC,
ScienceShot, Daily Mail and NPR.

Juan Sebastián Olier


Electronics engineer and MSc in intelligent systems design. He has focused on topics of AI and the development of models of
cognition and conceptual representations, as well as the simulations of social behaviour and the evolution of emerging
patterns. He has worked and carried out research with psychologists on evaluating and enhancing experiences through
understanding, modelling and predicting human behaviours. His work is driven by interests in intelligence, the emergence of
complex behaviours and the recognition of patterns in human activities for proposing more embedded and natural ways of
interaction, and the creation of collaborative learning between humans and machines.

Jorge Alvarado
Assistant Professor of Analytics at Pontificia Universidad Javeriana. He has a MSc in Analytics from North Carolina State
University and is pursuing a PhD in Engineering. His research interests are in analytics of databases of human behaviour and
Human–Computer Interaction in the field of judgmental forecasting. His work has been published in refereed academic
journals including Computers in Human Behavior and the Journal of Quantitative Methods for Economics and Business
Administration.

Charles Spence
Received his PhD in experimental psychology from the University of Cambridge, UK, in 1995. He is a university professor at
the Department of Experimental Psychology at the University of Oxford and head of the Crossmodal Research Laboratory.
He has published over 500 articles in top-flight scientific journals over the last decade and has been awarded the
Correspondence: 10th Experimental Psychology Society Prize, the British Psychology Society: Cognitive Section Award, the Paul Bertelson
Alejandro Salgado-Montejo, Award, recognizing him as the young European Cognitive Psychologist of the Year and the Friedrich Wilhelm Bessel Research
Department of Experimental
Psychology, University of Oxford, Award from the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation in Germany.
Oxford OX1 3UD, UK
E-mail: alejandro.salgado@
psy.ox.ac.uk

© 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649

www.palgrave-journals.com/bm/
Salgado-Montejo et al

ABSTRACT Simple line segments and shapes convey emotional meaning, with rounder
shapes being linked with positive emotions and generally preferred over more angular
shapes. We assessed the hypothesis that brand components – specifically typeface and
logo symbol – with similar scores on visual analogue scales (anchored by sound/shape
symbolic stimuli) would be associated with a higher frequency of positive emotions. We
also evaluated whether roundness or angularity were correlated with positively
valenced emotions. Nine different brand logos were tested; each was separated into its
logo symbol and typeface. Half of the 80 participants rated the logotype while the other
half rated the logo symbol using a variety of shape symbolism scales. The participants
were also asked to choose the emotions that they associated with each of the brands
from a list of 20 emotions. Brand components that presented (dis-) similar scores in
terms of the shape symbolism scales were coded as (in-) congruent. Those brands with
more congruent scores also presented more positive as compared with negative emo-
tions. These results support the view that more congruent design elements in brand
logos can give rise to higher emotional engagement. They also help to explain the way
in which consumers perceive brands.
Journal of Brand Management (2014) 21, 635–649. doi:10.1057/bm.2014.29

Keywords: brands; shape symbolism; emotion; logo design

INTRODUCTION owners, shape the perception of the


Brand management and brand development consumer.
have become increasingly challenging in In their seminal work, Henderson and
recent years (Shocker et al, 1994). The glo- Cote (1998) pointed out that brands are
bal nature of competitive niches, the com- important assets for companies. For this
plexity of brand–market interactions and reason, it is necessary to ensure that logos
the massive amount of information that is communicate effectively and engage the
available to consumers constitute just a target consumers/clients. This, in turn,
handful of the challenges that have been poses an important challenge for those
inherited from the end of the last century brand managers wanting to choose the right
(see Aaker, 2010 for a brief mention of design. According to Henderson and Cote,
some of the most pressing marketing chal- a good logo needs to be both easily and
lenges). It is becoming increasingly difficult quickly identified, to transmit positive affect
to control what is being talked about or and to communicate the same meaning
even what information consumers are read- across different consumers. In an eight-
ing, listening to or viewing concerning a country study, Madden et al (2000) reported
particular brand (Karaosmanoglu and that particular colour combinations could
Melewar, 2006; Wu and Fang, 2010; more easily communicate the meaning of a
Labrecque et al, 2013). This means that it is logo, thus suggesting that sensory informa-
increasingly important to make the most of tion could indeed influence brand recogni-
any interaction between the consumer and tion and positioning (see also Hynes, 2009).
brands. One needs to understand how Another key attribute in logo design and
design elements that are part of brands and evaluation is typeface. Several studies have
products, which are in the control of brand already investigated how to evaluate logos

636 © 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649
Love for logos

(see Henderson et al, 2004) and to help necessarily reflect the complexity, or the
determine the congruency between type- underlying drivers, of consumer behaviour
face and product (see Doyle and Bottomley, (Shapiro and Krishnan, 2001; Friese et al,
2006). 2006; Ariely and Berns, 2010; Parise and
In order to further our understanding of Spence, 2012). The interest of both industry
the many factors that influence brand per- experts and academics in increasing their
ception and brand management, Bartholmé understanding of consumer behaviour has
and Melewar (2009) have suggested that it is led to renewed interest in the adoption of
necessary to take into account the role that techniques from the field of experimental
sensory information has in defining corpo- psychology (for example, shape symbolism
rate and brand identity. As Schmitt (1999) scales) in order to better understand the
suggests, experiences that take sensory and behaviour of the consumer (for example,
emotional components into account will be see Spence and Gallace, 2011; Ngo et al,
more effective at engaging the consumer. 2012; Yoon et al, 2012).
However, while there is growing evidence The use of shape symbolism scales allows
concerning how sensory information influ- one to measure the associations that con-
ences the consumer’s perception of packa- sumers may have towards different sensory
ging and products (see Hirschman and attributes in different modalities, using
Holbrook, 1982; Becker et al, 2011; nonsense words (for example, Bouba, Kiki,
Spence and Piqueras-Fiszman, 2012), less Takete, Maluma, Lula, Tuki and so on),
attention has been given over by sensory rounded and angular shapes, and sensory
researchers to the brand itself (see Hultén, properties such as bright, dim, high pitch or
2011). This is especially true when it comes low pitch. The idea behind shape symbo-
to research addressing how different design lism is that the brain associates stimuli in
elements, such as typography and shapes, different modalities (often making connec-
influence the perception that consumers tions that might at first seem surprising) and
have about brands. is able to associate the information obtained
For the above-mentioned reasons, there is from one sensory modality (for example,
still a basic need to try and understand vision) in another sensory modality (Spence
how various design elements within brands and Parise, 2012; Deroy and Spence, 2013;
influence the perception of consumers con- Marks and Mulvenna, 2013). The last cou-
cerning companies and products. This could ple of decades have provided extensive
help to enhance the influence of pre- and behavioural and neurophysiological evi-
post-purchase factors (that is, packaging, dence to show that the brain associates
brand logos and advertising) on consumers’ information across the senses (Calvert et al,
decision making. This, in turn, could poten- 2004; Ghazanfar and Schroeder, 2006;
tially help to reduce design evaluation times Spence and Deroy, 2013; Connolly, 2014).
(and hence costs) since part of the process Since design involves different sensory
of brand and product evaluation could be modalities and, as Noble and Kumar (2010)
conducted as part of the design phase instead have pointed out, design is an important
of as a separate, subsequent, process. factor when it comes to establishing and
In the case of logos, which constitute the building competitive advantage. It seems
focus of the present study, it is important to interesting to explore how shape symbolism
assess how distinctive typefaces and sym- could be applied to the fields of marketing
bolic elements convey affective information and branding.
to the consumer. It has often been noted It is only recently that research on
that the explicit reports of consumers do not the topic of shape symbolism has been

© 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649 637
Salgado-Montejo et al

introduced to the field of marketing symbols and shapes) and how this influences
(Spence, 2012; Ngo et al, 2013; Velasco emotion in the context of logo design.
et al, 2013, 2014; Spence et al, 2014). That said, further research is needed to
Nevertheless, the suggestion is that it might help operationalize logo design and provide
potentially provide a powerful tool with better tools for brand managers when it
which to understand what meaning is con- comes to choosing, modifying or evaluating
veyed by shapes and, ultimately, influence the logo of a company or product. We are
the perceptions/decisions of the consumer particularly interested in trying to under-
(Cheskin, 1981). There is already a sub- stand how various design elements can
stantial body of evidence to support the idea influence brand perception via the affective
that angular geometric shapes tend to com- value that is conveyed by shapes and type-
municate threat (for example, Larson et al, faces that are part of brand logos. For this
2011; Watson et al, 2011), while rounded reason, in the present study, we sought to
shapes tend to be linked to more positive evaluate the shape symbolic profiles of the
emotions and are generally preferred (Leder elements that can communicate brand
and Carbon, 2005; Bar and Neta, 2006, identity (that is, typeface, logo symbol and
2007; Silvia and Barona, 2009; Jadva et al, packaging shape) to determine whether this
2010; Dazkir and Read, 2011; Vartanian would have an effect on the explicit report
et al, 2013; Westerman et al, 2013). A study of consumers. We also wanted to assess
by Westerman et al (2012) has further cor- whether roundness and angularity can
roborated these findings in the context of influence the explicit emotional associations
consumer behaviour, showing that con- that consumers have with a brand.
sumers (at least in certain product cate- By utilizing shape symbolism scales with
gories) prefer rounder, over angular, a list of emotion-related words, we investi-
contours and packaging. gated whether it would be possible to gain
There is, however, no evidence that has information concerning certain visual fea-
addressed the question of whether rounder tures of brands and products and their
and angular shapes have a similar effect in influence on the perception of the con-
the context of logo design. Jansson-Boyd sumer. To the best of our knowledge, this is
(2011) found that it is actually possible to the first time that brands have been eval-
operationalize aesthetic concepts by mod- uated using both shape symbolism scales and
ifying colours and shapes. This provides an emotion-related words. This may be parti-
interesting standpoint for logo design since cularly important for those companies
they are usually made up of typefaces, col- wanting to enter new markets, seeking to
ours and shapes; all of which are susceptible increase the value of their brand and/or for
to study using shape symbolism scales. The those companies wanting to export their
fact that particular design elements influ- products to unfamiliar regions. For exam-
ence aesthetic preference also links logo ple, the attributes of brands, packaging or
design to appetitive and aversive responses. advertising may, or may not, convey the
It is possible that the congruency between same information to those consumers who
design elements as well as the angularity or happen to live in different countries (for
roundness of the overall design can influ- example, Piqueras-Fiszman et al, 2012;
ence participants’ emotions towards a logo. Bremner et al, 2013).
So far, however, there has been no research A key issue when studying emotion is
that has used shape symbolism scales to the variety of words or ways that a partici-
determine the congruency between differ- pant can use to describe or express what
ent design elements (for example, typeface, they feel towards, for example, a brand.

638 © 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649
Love for logos

For this reason, presenting the participants participants will choose less positively
with a set of words that has already been valenced words for those logo types and
validated across a range of contexts and symbols that do not present consistent scores
languages (Scherer, 2005) provides a in the shape symbolism scales. We used
straightforward and easy means of studying internationally renowned brands as well as
the perceptions that participants may have local brands across different categories (given
of a brand. While the choices that partici- that this study was conducted in Colombia,
pants make cannot be regarded as emo- these were local Colombian brands).
tions themselves, they do reflect the
participants’ explicit emotional valence
towards the brand. As Deonna and Teroni METHODS
(2014) have recently suggested, emotions
can be construed as evaluations and later Participants
they can become evaluative attitudes that Eighty participants (49 women, Mage = 28.9
influence the way in which a consumer years, SD = 5.14, rangeage = 25–45 years)
can perceive an experience. In a sense, from different universities and two Colom-
then, emotions provide input as to the bian companies took part in this study. The
success, failure, correctness, inappropri- participants were divided into two groups
ateness, pleasure or pain of an experience. and were presented with either the typeface
Emotions can therefore be understood as or logo symbols associated with nine differ-
being part of how we weight information ent brands. The participants signed a standard
and/or are themselves a source of infor- consent form. The experiment lasted for
mation (Ragunathan and Pham, 1999; approximately 20 min, and was reviewed
Ragunathan et al, 2006). Hence, by asking and approved by the Research Committee
the participants to select different emo- of the Escuela Internacional de Ciencias
tion-related words we can better under- Económicas y Administrativas at Universidad
stand their evaluations and attitudes de la Sabana, Chía, Colombia.
towards different experiences (in this case
brands) and what sort of impressions they
have formed throughout the many inter- Apparatus and materials
actions with the brand, advertisements and Customized computer-based software was
other strategies that can influence brand developed using JAVA in order to integrate
perception. In the end, the focus should the scales for word and shape symbolism and
not be about separating the design from the emotions taken from the Geneva Emo-
the experience, rather, it should be about tion Wheel (Scherer, 2005). The participants
understanding the brand in the context of were seated in front of a 15″ LED monitor,
consumer experience. with a screen resolution of 1024×768 pixels,
We seek to determine if similar scores on and a screen refresh rate of 60 Hz.
the shape symbolism scales for typefaces and Adobe Illustrator and Adobe Photoshop
logo symbols or packaging shapes (depend- CS5 were used to separate the typeface from
ing on the brand) can influence the explicit the logo symbol. Nine brands1 from a variety
affective responses of consumers towards a of different categories were chosen for use in
brand. Our expectation was that brands that the study: Carrefour, Coca-Cola, Davi-
are rated as rounder in the shape symbolism vienda, Falabella, Fanta, Homecenter, Man-
scales in both their logo type and symbol antial, Samsung and Trident (Weblinks).
will also be associated with more positive These included both local and foreign brands
emotional words. We also predicted that that were well-known in the country. For

© 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649 639
Salgado-Montejo et al

Data analysis
SPSS 22 (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.) was
used to analyse the data. A multivariate ana-
lysis of variance (MANOVA) was used taking
the six visual analogue scales as dependent
variables and the typeface or logo symbol
as the independent variable. Participants’
choices on the emotion list were added and
coded according to their emotional valance
(see Appendix); the difference between the
number of positive and negative emotions
was calculated and is referred to (within the
text) as the emotional valence difference.
A Kruskal–Wallis test was used to compare
Figure 1: Visual analogue scales used to evaluate shape differences in emotional valence between the
symbolism on each of the typefaces and logo symbols: (i) circle– various presented brands; Mann–Whitney
square, (ii) curve–triangle, (iii) Bouba–Kiki image, (iv) Bouba–Kiki
text, (v) Maluma–Takete and (vi) low pitch–high pitch. U tests were then used for post-hoc analyses.
Pearson’s correlations between the visual
analogue scales scores and the emotional
valence difference were also performed.
Coca-Cola and Fanta we used the outline of
the bottles as symbols.
The participants responded using six RESULTS
visual analogue scales (anchored by sound/ Two different criteria were used to deter-
shape symbolic stimuli); circle–square, mine the congruency between typefaces
curve–triangle, Bouba–Kiki image, Bouba– and logo symbols. First, brand components
Kiki text, Maluma–Takete and low pitch– (typefaces, logos, symbols or packaging
high pitch (see Figure 1). outlines) that did not display statistically
significant differences in any of the visual
analogue scales (that is, similar mean scores
Procedure and closely tied negative values or positive
The participants were divided into two values) were classified as more congruent.
groups and were presented with either the Brand components that displayed statisti-
typeface or the symbol associated with each cally significant differences were deemed
logo for 5000 ms. After that, they had to incongruent. Second, logos that presented
rate each typeface or symbol using the six small effect sizes were categorized as more
visual analogue scales (see Figure 1), and congruent, whereas brand components that
then select, from a list of 20 possible emo- presented significant differences and large
tions, those that corresponded best to each effect sizes were labelled as incongruent (see
of the stimuli (without a limit to the total Figure 2 and Table 1). For the MANOVA,
number of emotions that could be chosen). Pillai’s trace was used because of its robust-
The order of presentation of each of the ness against assumption violations. The sig-
typefaces and logo symbols was rando- nificance level was set at P < 0.05 (Table 2).
mized. Each participant viewed and rated Post-hoc analyses of each of the scales
either the nine typefaces or the nine logo using an ANOVA revealed significant dif-
symbols. Note that the scales and emotion ferences for Carrefour, Samsung and Man-
list were presented in Spanish. antial. None of the other brands displayed a

640 © 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649
Love for logos

Table 1: Mean scores of the six shape symbolism scales by the nine brands presented to the participants

Brand Brand component Maluma–Takete Bouba–Kiki (image) High pitch–low pitch

M SD M SD M SD

Carrefour Name 19.2 57.4 −5.3 62.7 −3.8 59.2


Symbol 19.0 59.1 −38.8 60.8 −16.3 62.3
Coca-Cola Name −18.8 66.9 −33.9 65.8 −2.4 68.4
Symbol −9.4 72.9 −51.5 59.7 −3.7 63.9
Davivienda Name −11.4 59.6 −47.4 48.3 −8.4 54.5
Symbol −31.3 55.6 −51.6 54.4 −13.4 61.9
Falabella Name −7.0 64.5 −1.2 64.0 5.1 59.0
Symbol −1.9 65.0 −9.9 64.7 34.7 57.9
Fanta Name −30.5 67.4 −70.9 44.6 −0.8 71.6
Symbol −14.2 67.6 −49.2 44.1 −12.4 61.2
Homecenter Name 35.9 59.0 21.4 66.2 −12.4 60.1
Symbol 23.3 63.8 30.0 54.7 −26.4 60.7
Manantial Name −2.3 66.7 6.5 71.1 13.6 62.0
Symbol −40.6 56.1 −59.6 36.3 9.0 63.9
Samsung Name 13.3 61.5 23.7 53.0 −6.4 57.6
Symbol −16.3 65.3 −51.2 47.0 8.5 68.3
Trident Name 5.3 68.8 −16.4 69.6 4.4 65.1
Symbol −14.9 65.8 −38.3 60.4 15.2 63.0

Brand Brand component Curve–triangle Bouba–Kiki Circle–square

M SD M SD M SD

Carrefour Name −2.3 61.2 −5.8 62.2 11.6 60.5


Symbol −11.1 69.7 −12.4 63.1 −44.7 47.0
Coca-Cola Name −67.0 49.1 −18.8 64.9 −58.3 42.5
Symbol −69.4 44.4 3.2 77.2 −59.8 39.3
Davivienda Name −24.1 60.9 −27.5 58.8 −27.9 56.2
Symbol −8.0 77.5 −33.3 65.3 −1.8 74.8
Falabella Name −25.5 64.6 17.1 58.4 −2.2 61.4
Symbol −45.3 61.4 −8.9 60.8 −14.6 61.9
Fanta Name −62.4 48.5 −28.2 73.2 −66.6 41.5
Symbol −64.8 42.6 −31.8 65.1 −50.1 52.9
Homecenter Name 48.5 50.6 6.7 65.8 62.3 37.8
Symbol 46.4 59.6 10.9 58.4 70.2 41.5
Manantial Name −14.2 74.6 12.7 68.9 16.7 71.0
Symbol −70.4 37.9 −29.2 61.2 −46.9 50.0
Samsung Name 21.1 59.6 0.1 58.2 21.2 68.0
Symbol −52.2 48.4 −40.0 59.5 −62.2 43.9
Trident Name −38.0 62.3 0.0 73.8 −10.8 71.7
Symbol −64.4 48.9 −3.8 66.2 −37.9 59.9

Note: M = Mean, SD = Standard deviation.

significant difference in the shape symbo- intermediate emotional valence difference


lism scales (see Figure 2 and Table 1). score (see Table 3).
Small significant correlations were found The only brands presenting a significant
between roundness and higher values in the difference when comparing typeface and
emotional valence difference; except for logo symbol were Carrefour F(6, 72) = 4.48,
Manantial, which presented incongruent P < 0.01, Manantial F(6, 72) = 5.48, P < 0.01
scores in the shape symbolism scales and an and Samsung F(6, 72) = 11.2, P < 0.01.

© 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649 641
Salgado-Montejo et al

Circle-square Bouba-kiki image Circle-triangle


Bouba-kiki text Maluma-takete text Low pitch-high pitch
1.2

0.8

Effect size
0.6

0.4

0.2

Figure 2: Effect sizes of each scale depicting the magnitude of the difference (in standard deviations) between typeface and logo symbol
scores for each brand on each scale. Effect sizes are order by level of congruency. Brands that are rated as significantly incongruent are
presented on the left side of the grey line.

Table 2: Frequency of the emotion-related words that were chosen by the participants across trials and for each brand

Emotion-related words Coca-cola Trident Fanta Davivienda Manantial Falabella Homecenter Samsung Carrefour

Pride/Zest 67 35 29 60 12 25 52 29 18
Pleasure/Enjoyment 197 171 167 72 164 142 86 90 78
Nostalgia/Longing 36 26 29 34 46 20 27 25 23
Relief/Calm 45 55 46 44 92 27 44 20 27
Interest/Enthusiasm 119 83 72 119 75 144 124 125 120
Fun/Laughter 147 167 154 108 41 58 35 60 26
Joy/Happiness 223 146 140 109 74 64 73 59 33
Tenderness/Love 56 32 51 20 30 17 40 10 13
Admiration/Awe 146 46 45 122 68 69 78 86 46
Surprise/Wonder 67 62 39 50 43 57 47 88 53
Contempt/Scorn 15 4 11 21 9 12 19 16 27
Envy/Jealousy 5 9 3 3 7 17 3 12 4
Sadness/Desperation 10 3 17 4 7 2 2 13 7
Fear/Preoccupation 4 4 7 7 13 2 8 26 21
Shame/Embarrassment 7 5 10 18 11 5 7 28 25
Regret/Disappointment 2 6 2 5 6 2 2 8 5
Revulsion/Disgust 9 2 9 6 4 9 17 6 12
Guilt/Remorse 10 5 3 22 10 6 9 7 23
Anger/Irritation 30 9 8 11 7 6 5 6 7
Compassion/Pity 3 3 11 30 11 19 15 14 20
Total positively valenced (+) 1103 823 772 738 645 623 606 592 437
Total negatively valenced (−) 95 50 81 127 85 80 87 136 151
Valence ratio (+/−) 11.61 16.46 9.53 5.81 7.59 7.79 6.97 4.35 2.89

642 © 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649
Love for logos

The differences between the logo symbol Table 3: Average ranks for the emotional valence
and typeface for the Carrefour brand were difference for each of the brands
generated by scores in the circle–square scale, Brand Average ranks
F(1, 77) = 21.6, P < 0.001, d = 0.735 and the
Bouba–Kiki image scale F(1, 77) = 5.8, Carrefour 254.9
Samsung 308.5
P = 0.02, d = 0.383. Lack of congruency for Homecenter 327.1
Samsung was found in five of the scales: Falabella 336.7
Maluma–Takete F(1, 77) = 4.3, P < 0.05, Manatial 341.5
Davidienda 364.4
d = 0.33; circle–square, F(1, 77) = 42.9, Fanta 385.4
P < 0.001, d = 1.031, Bouba–Kiki (image) Trident 415.7
F(1, 77) = 44.3, P < 0.001, d = 1.057, Coca-Cola 462.7
Bouba–Kiki (text) F(1, 77) = 9.2, P < 0.01, Note: Higher scores indicate a higher frequency of positive
d = 0.48 and curve–triangle F(1, 77) = 36.3, emotions
P < 0.001, d = 0.955. Lack of congruency for
Manantial was found in the Maluma–Takete were ‘Admiration/Awe’. Thus suggesting
F(1, 77) = 7.5, P < 0.01, d = 0.439; Bouba– that each brand has a different emotional
Kiki image F(1, 77) = 16.8, P < 0.001, profile and that consumers associate differ-
d = 0.828; curve–triangle F(1, 77) = 17.1, ent emotion concepts to them.
P < 0.001, d = 0.671; and Bouba–Kiki text The Emotional valence difference was
scales F(1, 77) = 8.1, P < 0.001, d = 0.455. evaluated with a Kruskall–Wallis test and
No significant differences were found for any was positive for all of the brands tested in
of the brands when it came to the high pitch– the present study. This suggests that there
low pitch scale. was a higher frequency of positive emotions
The choices made by the participants in when compared with the frequency of
the emotion-related list demonstrate that, negative emotions (Medianemotions per stimulus
overall, there was a higher frequency of = 3, SD = 3.39, median was chosen over
positively valenced emotion-related words mean because of high kurtosis). The results
associated with each of the nine brands. were significant χ2(8) = 56.086, P < 0.001.
However, there are differences in the over- Further analyses revealed that there was less
all frequency of both positively and nega- of a difference between the frequency of
tively valenced emotion-related words as positive and negative emotions, and a greater
can be seen in the valence ratio. Trident difference in terms of the overall frequency
presented the highest number of positively of positive emotions (that is, participants
valenced emotions for every negatively paired some brands with a greater number
valenced emotion (16.46:1) while Carre- of positive emotions). Carrefour and
four presented the lowest (2.89:1). There Samsung displayed the lowest emotional
were also differences in the specific words valence difference (see Table 3). Post-hoc
that were related to each brand, for exam- Mann–Whitney U tests revealed that
ple, Coca-Cola presents a high frequency of Carrefour had the lowest emotional valence
associations with the words ‘Pleasure/ difference and was significantly different
Enjoyment’ and ‘Joy/Happiness’, which is when compared with all of the other brands
consistent with their campaigns and adver- tested in the present study, with the excep-
tisements. On the other hand Trident pre- tion of Samsung. The emotional valence
sents a higher number of choices in the difference was significantly lower for
words ‘Pleasure/Enjoyment’ but also in Samsung than for both Trident (U = 2133.5,
‘Fun/Laughter’. The words that were more P = 0.001) and Coca-Cola (U = 1881.5,
easily associated with the bank, Davivienda, P < 0.001).

© 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649 643
Salgado-Montejo et al

Table 4: Pearson’s correlations between each scale and the brands that have been successful in building
emotional valence difference scores their reputation and market share must have
Maluma–Takete Pearson’s correlation −0.053 a minimum level of emotional engagement;
Sig. (bilateral) 0.160 it is likely that brands that are associated
N 709 with emotions that are perceived by con-
Bouba–Kiki image Pearson’s correlation −0.113**
Sig. (bilateral) 0.002
sumers as negative and that would reduce
N 709 approach motivation would not survive in a
Low pitch–high pitch Pearson’s correlation −0.054 competitive marketplace. However, the clo-
Sig. (bilateral) 0.148
N 709
seness or strength of consumer–brand rela-
Curve–triangle Pearson’s correlation −0.181** tionships may well-depend not only on the
Sig. (bilateral) 0.000 emotional valence difference, but also on the
N 709 intensity of the emotion and on the parti-
Bouba–Kiki text Pearson’s correlation 0.011
Sig. (bilateral) 0.768 cular emotions that a brand generates, or is
N 709 associated with. On this last point, further
Circle–square Pearson’s correlation −0.162** research is undoubtedly needed in order to
Sig. (bilateral) 0.000
N 709 determine whether, and how, distinct visual
design elements can communicate particular
**P < 0.01 emotions. There is also a need to address
the question of whether visual elements
Pearson’s correlations between emo- influence consumers’ perception, trust and
tional valence differences and the scales loyalty towards a brand or product (Amine,
were performed in order to determine 1998; Chaudhuri and Holbrook, 2001;
whether those brands with higher scores Hwang and Kandampully, 2012; Spanjaard
(indicating roundness) also presented a and Freeman, 2012).
higher emotional valence difference (see The congruency of the different logo
Table 4). Small significant negative correla- elements, in terms of the shape symbolism
tions were observed between the emotional scales, may lead to more positive emotions.
valence difference and Bouba-Kiki image This could be the result of increased pro-
(r = −0.113, P = 0.002), curve-triangle cessing fluency (Labroo et al, 2008); since
(r = −0.181, P < 0.001) and circle-square higher congruency could mean that more
scales (r = −0.162, P < 0.001); thus showing elements are providing consistent informa-
that there was a higher frequency of positive tion. Reber et al (2004) have suggested that
emotions for brands that were evaluated as visual or semantic priming can have an
rounder in the visual analogue scales. influence on processing fluency and that this
can enhance aesthetic judgments. Previous
research by Lee and Labroo (2004) found
DISCUSSION that advertising can facilitate processing flu-
In the present study, we tested brands taken ency and generate more positive attitudes
from several different categories (from both towards the brand. Congruency between
the service and product sectors). We were brand typeface and logo symbols could
interested in evaluating whether the con- increase the effect of both semantic and
gruency of the various elements that com- visual priming and thus enhance processing
pose a brand’s visual identity would fluency. The Manantial brand, however,
influence people’s affective judgments. The seems to provide an exception to this rule
results revealed that the emotional valence (see Table 2 and Figure 2) and indicates that
difference scores were positive for all of the other factors, aside from shape symbolism,
brands tested here, thus suggesting that can also influence processing fluency for

644 © 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649
Love for logos

typefaces and logo symbols. More research is has control over the experience, leading to a
needed in order to explore this phenomenon. classification of emotions in terms of ‘high’
Particular emotions associated with and ‘low control’. In the context of brand
brands may also convey information about management, these categories (high/low
which contexts and experiences are more control and conducive/obstructive) could
suitable for a brand. This has important help explain how the consumer relates to
implications for product placement, adver- each brand. It could also reflect the type of
tising and media campaigns in general. communication, behaviours and interactions
Pearson’s correlations between the scales that the consumer associates or expects when
and the emotional valence difference sug- interacting with a brand (or its products/ser-
gest that brands that were classified as vices). Words such as interest/enthusiasm,
‘rounder’ (that is, circle, Bouba and curve) pleasure/enjoyment, fun/laughter and joy/
actually had a higher frequency of positive happiness are classified as being related to low
emotions associated with them. This is control conducive emotions. These four
consistent with the results of previous categories were the most chosen in most of
research showing that rounder shapes are the brands that presented a high degree of
associated with positive emotions and are congruency between logo type and logo
preferred in different contexts (Bar and symbol. The brands that presented lower
Neta, 2006, 2007; Westerman et al, 2012; congruency scores had more dispersed choi-
Vartanian et al, 2013). ces in the emotion-related list, with interest/
Research by both Parise and Spence enthusiasm being the emotion that presented
(2012) and by Velasco et al (2014) suggests the highest frequency.
that packaging shapes can be associated with An interesting finding here is that extreme
particular product attributes and consumer values on the shape symbolism scales
experiences (see also Spence and Piqueras- (no matter in which direction) might be
Fiszman, 2012, for a review). The results of correlated to a higher change on emotional
the present study further demonstrate that valence. This would suggest on the one hand
brand logos could also have underlying that consumers are more likely to associate
associations that might influence a con- designs that present congruent elements with
sumer’s experiences. positively valenced emotions. On the other
The specific emotion-related words cho- hand, these findings also show that less con-
sen by our participants for each brand also gruent elements are more likely to be asso-
reveal an interesting opportunity to study ciated with negatively valenced emotions.
consumers’ emotional associations towards With the latter in mind, an important
specific brands. The choices made by the guideline for brand design could be to match
participants in this particular task also reflect design elements in terms of congruency (and
that complexity of consumers’ attitudes not only aesthetics). It is also interesting to
towards brands and support the notion that note that brands that contain more round
there can be different layers of attitudes elements (that is, curves, lines, typefaces),
towards brands, products and experiences such as Trident, Coca-Cola, Fanta and
(see Olney, Holbrook and Batra, 1991; Davivienda displayed the highest emotional
Wilson et al, 2000; Kugler et al, 2012). valence difference. As a side note, it is inter-
As Scherer (2005) has pointed out, some esting that two of the previously mentioned
emotions are more conducive, while others brands presented human-like shapes as part
are more obstructive of potential goals, of their designs. In the case of Trident we can
actions or motives. Emotions can also be observe a smile-like line under the typeface
understood in terms how much the person and in the case of Davivienda the logo

© 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649 645
Salgado-Montejo et al

symbol is a house that resembles a face. psychology and neuroscience, suggest that
Already, there is evidence that simple design emotion can be understood as information
elements can be associated with emotional and that it is part of the thought process.
categories (Landwehr et al, 2011, but see It also shows that sensory information can
Puzakova et al, 2013). There is also evidence influence emotion and therefore they
that rounder body movements are associated should always be thought of as a joint sys-
with positively valenced emotions and tem. Further research is needed in order to
angular movements with negatively valenced address the link between crossmodal corre-
emotions. It is possible that curves, such as spondences (that is, the tendency to match
the ones displayed by the Coca-Cola logo information across the senses, see Spence,
could be associated with rounder move- 2011, 2012 for reviews; see also Connolly,
ments and that this, in turn, communicates 2014) and logo design.
positively valenced emotions. Finally, since a consumer’s explicit
Another key element that is relevant to reports can help shape brand reputation, it is
this study is the fact that we used known important to understand how design ele-
brands. While this may have the effect of ments influence word-of-mouth and can
generating noise within our measurements ultimately alter purchase behaviour. We are
from other information that the consumer convinced that the use of methods and
may possess about the brand (that is, as a result techniques from the field of experimental
of being exposed to advertisements, market- psychology to answer design questions
ing campaigns and interactions with products could become a powerful tool with which
or services). Brand identity and perception is to support the development of brand value
not only shaped by logo design (Alden et al, across niches and cultures. These types of
1999; Schmitt, 1999; Underwood, 2003). tests could also be used to diagnose how
In reality, a variety of information and inter- well a brand is performing in a given market
actions influence consumers perception of a and can provide marketers, brand managers
brand. Thus, suggesting that an ecologically and media agencies with relevant informa-
valid measurement of the perception that a tion as to consumers’ impressions about a
consumer has of a brand should account for brand. Certainly, combining design with
the overall impression that has been formed the analytical tools provided by the field of
via different channels. That said, future experimental psychology could potentially
research should also consider how to isolate open up a whole new range of possibilities
brand components to avoid the intrusion of as to how to create trust between brands and
other information. Research that utilizes consumers and also suggests that design may
shape symbolism scales and other tools to be a vehicle to create attitudes and percep-
study brand identity and logo design should tions towards brands that increase brand
take into account whether they are interested value.
in the design itself (for example, in the case
of a new brand or design) or if they are
interested in understanding the underlying NOTE
attitudes and emotions that consumers have 1 Note that the visual presentation of the brands may
change over time. The brands tested here were from the
towards a brand. end of 2013, beginning of 2014.
The present study also addresses a key
issue in brand management. So far most
research has separated emotional, cognitive REFERENCES
and sensory aspects into different categories Aaker, D. (2010) Marketing challenges in the next
or intervention strategies. Evidence from decade. Journal of Brand Management 17(5): 315–316.

646 © 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649
Love for logos

Alden, D.L., Steenkamp, J.-B.E.M. and Batra, R. (1999) Ghazanfar, A.A. and Schroeder, C.E. (2006) Is neocortex
Brand positioning through advertising in Asia, North essentially multisensory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences
America, and Europe: The role of global consumer 10(6): 278–285.
culture. Journal of Marketing 63(1): 75–87. Henderson, P.W. and Cote, J.A. (1998) Guidelines for
Amine, A. (1998) Consumers’ true brand loyalty: The selecting or modifying logos. Journal of Marketing
central role of commitment. Journal of Strategic 62(2): 14–30.
Marketing 6(4): 305–319. Henderson, P.W., Giese, J.L. and Cote, J.A. (2004)
Ariely, D. and Berns, G.S. (2010) Neuromarketing: The Impression management using typeface design.
hope and hype of neuroimaging in business. Nature Journal of Marketing 68(4): 60–72.
Reviews Neuroscience 11(4): 284–292. Hirschman, E.C. and Holbrook, M.B. (1982) Hedonic
Bar, M. and Neta, M. (2006) Humans prefer curved consumption: Emerging concepts, methods
visual objects. Psychological Science 17(8): 645–648. and propositions. Journal of Marketing 46(3):
Bar, M. and Neta, M. (2007) Visual elements of 92–101.
subjective preference modulate amygdala activation. Hultén, B. (2011) Sensory marketing: The multi‐sensory
Neuropsychologia 45(10): 2191–2200. brand‐experience concept. European Business Review
Bartholmé, R.H. and Melewar, T.C. (2009) Adding 23(3): 256–273.
new dimensions to corporate identity management Hynes, N. (2009) Colour and meaning in corporate
and corporate communication: Exploring the sensory logos: An empirical study. Journal of Brand
perspective. The Marketing Review 9(2): 155–169. Management 16(8): 545–555.
Becker, L., van Rompay, T.J.L., Schifferstein, H.N.J. Hwang, J. and Kandampully, J. (2012) The role of
and Galetzka, M. (2011) Tough package, strong emotional aspects in younger consumer-brand
taste: The influence of packaging design on taste relationships. Journal of Product and Brand Management
impressions and product evaluations. Food Quality 21(2): 98–108.
and Preference 22(1): 17–23. Jadva, V., Hines, M. and Golombok, S. (2010) Infants’
Bremner, A.J., Caparos, S., Davidoff, J., de Fockert, J., preferences for toys, colors, and shapes: Sex
Linnell, K.J. and Spence, C. (2013) ‘Bouba’ and differences and similarities. Archives of Sexual Behavior
‘Kiki’ in Namibia? A remote culture make similar 39(6): 1261–1273.
shape-sound matches, but different shape-taste Jansson-Boyd, C.V. (2011) Designing aesthetic concepts:
matches to Westerners. Cognition 126(2): 165–172. Can it be done? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and
Calvert, G., Spence, C. and Stein, B.E. (eds.) (2004) The the Arts 5(3): 279–290.
Handbook of Multisensory Processing. Cambridge, MA: Karaosmanoglu, E. and Melewar, T.C. (2006)
MIT Press. Corporate communications, identity and image:
Chaudhuri, A. and Holbrook, M.B. (2001) The chain of A research agenda. Journal of Brand Management
effects from brand trust and brand affect to brand 14(1/2): 196–206.
performance: The role of brand loyalty. Journal of Kugler, T., Connolly, T. and Ordóñez, L.D. (2012)
Marketing 65(2): 81–93. Emotion, decision, and risk: Betting on gambles
Cheskin, L. (1981) Research design and analysis in the versus betting on people. Journal of Behavioral
testing of symbols. In: W. Stern (ed.) Handbook Decision Making 25(2): 123–134.
of Package Design Research. New York: Wiley Labrecque, vor dem Esche, J., Mathwick, C., Novak, T.P.
Interscience, pp. 211–220. and Hofacker, C.F. (2013) Consumer power:
Connolly, K. (2014) Multisensory perception as an Evolution in the digital age. Journal of Interactive
associative learning process. Frontiers in Psychology 5: Marketing 27(4): 257–269.
1095. Labroo, A.A., Dhar, R. and Schwarz, N. (2008) Of frog
Dazkir, S.S. and Read, M.A. (2011) Furniture forms and wines and frowning watches: Semantic priming,
their influence on our emotional responses toward perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. Journal of
interior environments. Environment and Behavior Consumer Research 34(6): 819–831.
44(5): 722–732. Landwehr, J.R., McGill, A.L. and Herrmann, A. (2011)
Deonna, J.A. and Teroni, F. (2014) In what sense are It’s got the look: The effect of friendly and aggressive
emotions evaluations? In: S. Roeser and C. Todd ‘facial’ expressions on product liking and sales. Journal
(eds.) Emotion and Value. Oxford: Oxford University of Marketing 75(3): 132–146.
Press, pp. 15–31. Larson, C.L., Aronoff, J. and Steuer, E.L. (2011)
Deroy, O. and Spence, C. (2013) Why we are not all Simple geometric shapes are implicitly associated
synesthetes (not even weakly so). Psychonomic Bulletin with affective value. Motivation and Emotion 36(3):
& Review 20(4): 643–664. 404–413.
Doyle, J.R. and Bottomley, P.A. (2006) Dressed for the Leder, H. and Carbon, C. (2005) Dimensions in
occasion: Font-product congruity in the perception appreciation of car interior design. Applied Cognitive
of logotype. Journal of Consumer Psychology 16(2): Psychology 19(5): 603–618.
112–123. Lee, A.Y. and Labroo, A.A. (2004) The effect of
Friese, M., Wänke, M. and Plessner, H. (2006) Implicit conceptual and perceptual fluency on brand
consumer preferences and their influence on product evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research 41(2):
choice. Psychology and Marketing 23(9): 727–740. 151–165.

© 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649 647
Salgado-Montejo et al

Madden, T.J., Hewett, K. and Roth, M.S. (2000) management: An introduction to the special issue.
Managing images in different cultures: A cross- Journal of Marketing Research 31(4): 149–158.
national study of color meanings and preferences. Silvia, P.J. and Barona, C.M. (2009) Do people
Journal of International Marketing 8(4): 90–107. prefer lined objects? Angularity, expertise, and
Marks, L.E. and Mulvenna, C.M. (2013) Synesthesia, at aesthetic preference. Empirical Studies of the Arts
and near its borders. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 651. 27(1): 25–42.
Ngo, M.K., Piqueras-Fiszman, B. and Spence, C. (2012) Spanjaard, D. and Freeman, L. (2012) The hidden
On the colour and shape of still and sparkling water: agenda: Emotions in grocery shopping. The Inter-
Insights from online and laboratory-based testing. national Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer
Food Quality and Preference 24(2): 260–268. Research 22(5): 439–457.
Ngo, M.K., Velasco, C., Salgado, A. and Spence, C. Spence, C. (2011) Crossmodal correspondences: A
(2013) Assessing crossmodal correspondences in tutorial review. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics
exotic fruit juices: The case of shape and sound 73(4): 971–995.
symbolism. Food Quality and Preference 28(1): Spence, C. (2012) Managing sensory expectations
361–369. concerning products and brands: Capitalizing on the
Noble, C.H. and Kumar, M. (2010) Exploring the potential of sound and shape symbolism. Journal of
appeal of product design: A grounded, value-based Consumer Psychology 22(1): 37–54.
model of key design elements and relationships. Spence, C. and Deroy, O. (2013) How automatic are
Journal of Product Innovation Management 27(5): crossmodal correspondences? Consciousness and
640–657. Cognition 22(1): 245–260.
Olney, T.J., Holbrook, M.R. and Batra, R. (1991) Spence, C. and Gallace, A. (2011) Tasting shapes
Consumer responses to advertising: The effects of ad and words. Food Quality and Preference 22(3):
content, emotions, and attitude toward the ad on 290–295.
viewing time. Journal of Consumer Research 17(4): Spence, C. and Parise, C.V. (2012) The cognitive neuro-
440–453. science of crossmodal correspondences. i-Perception
Parise, C.V. and Spence, C. (2012) Assessing the 3(7): 410–412.
associations between brand packaging and brand Spence, C. and Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2012) The
attributes using an indirect performance measure. multisensory packaging of beverages. In: M.G.
Food Quality and Preference 24(1): 17–23. Kontominas (ed.) Food Packaging: Procedures,
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Velasco, C. and Spence, C. Management and Trends. Hauppauge, NY: Nova
(2012) Exploring implicit and explicit crossmodal Publishers, pp. 187–233.
colour-flavour correspondences in product packaging. Spence, C., Puccinelli, N.M., Grewal, D. and Roggeveen,
Food Quality and Preference 25(2): 148–155. A.L. (2014) Store atmospherics: A multisensory
Puzakova, M., Kwak, H. and Rocereto, J.F. (2013) When perspective. Psychology & Marketing 31(7): 472–488.
humanizing brands goes wrong: The detrimental effect Underwood, R.L. (2003) The communicative power of
of brand anthropomorphization amid product product packaging: Creating brand identity via lived
wrongdoings. Journal of Marketing 77(3): 81–100. and mediated experience. Journal of Marketing Theory
Ragunathan, R. and Pham, M.T. (1999) All negative and Practice 11(1): 62–76.
moods are not created equal: Motivational influences Vartanian, O. et al. (2013) Impact of contour on
of anxiety and sadness on decision making. aesthetic judgments and approach–Avoidance
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process decisions in architecture. Proceedings of the National
79(1): 56–77. Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
Ragunathan, R., Pham, M.T. and Corfman, K.P. (2006) 110(2): 10446–10453.
Informational properties of anxiety and sadness, and Velasco, C., Jones, R., King, S. and Spence, C. (2013)
displaced coping. Journal of Consumer Research 32(4): Assessing the influence of the multisensory
596–601. environment on the whisky drinking experience.
Reber, R., Schwarz, N. and Winkielman, P. (2004) Flavour 2(1): 23.
Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in Velasco, C., Salgado-Montejo, A., Marmolejo-Ramos,
the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and F. and Spence, C. (2014) Predictive packaging
Social Psychology Review 8(4): 364–382. design: Tasting shapes, typefaces, names, and
Scherer, K.R. (2005) What are emotions? And how can sounds. Food Quality and Preference 34: 88–95.
they be measured? Social Science Information 44(4): Watson, D.G., Blagrove, E. and Selwood, S. (2011)
695–729. Emotional triangles: A test of emotion – Based
Schmitt, B. (1999) Experiential marketing. Journal of attentional capture by simple geometric shapes.
Marketing Management 15(1–3): 53–67. Cognition and Emotion 25(7): 1149–1164.
Shapiro, S. and Krishnan, H.S. (2001) Memory-based Westerman et al. (2012) Product design: Preference for
measures for assessing advertising effects: A rounded versus angular design elements. Psychology
comparison of explicit and implicit memory effects. and Marketing 29(8): 595–605.
Journal of Advertising 30(3): 1–13. Westerman, S.J. et al. (2013) The design of consumer
Shocker, A.D., Srivastava, R.K. and Ruekert, R.W. packaging: Effects of manipulations of shape,
(1994) Challenges and opportunities facing brand orientation, and alignment of graphical forms on

648 © 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649
Love for logos

consumers’ assessments. Food Quality and Preference APPENDIX


27(1): 8–17.
Wilson, T.D., Lindsey, S. and Schooler, T.Y. (2000)
A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review Table A1: List of emotions presented to participants
107(1): 101–126.
Wu, S.-C. and Fang, W. (2010) The effect of consumer- Positive emotional valence Negative emotional valence
to-consumer interactions on idea generation in
virtual brand community relationships. Technovation Spanish
30(11–12): 570–581. Orgullo/Júbilo Desprecio/Desdén
Yoon, C. et al. (2012) Decision neuroscience and Placer/Disfrute Envidia/Celos
consumer decision making. Marketing Letters 23(2): Nostalgia/Anhelo Tristeza/Desesperación
473–485. Alivio/Descargado Miedo/Preocupación
Interés/Involucramiento Vergüenza/Pena
Diversión/Risa Arrepentimiento/Descepción
Alegría/Felicidad Repulsión/Asco
WEBLINKS Ternura/Amor
Admiración/Respeto
Culpa/Remordimiento
Ira/Irritacion
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Carrefour; Sorpresa/Asombro Compasión/Lástima
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Coca-Cola;
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Davivienda; English
Pride/Zest Contempt/Scorn
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/S.A.C.I._ Pleasure/Enjoyment Envy/Jealousy
Falabella; http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/ Nostalgia/Longing Sadness/Desperation
Fanta; http://es.wikipedia.org/wiki/Home Relief/Calm Fear/Preoccupation
Interest/Enthusiasm Shame/Embarrassment
center_Sodimac; http://www.revistapym. Fun/Laughter Regret/Disappointment
com.co/noticias/rediseno/agua-manantial- Joy/Happiness Revulsion/Disgust
renueva-su-imagen; http://en.wikipedia Tenderness/Love Guilt/Remorse
.org/wiki/Samsung; http://en.wikipedia Admiration/Awe Anger/Irritation
Surprise/Wonder Compassion/Pity
.org/wiki/Trident_(gum)

© 2014 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-231X Journal of Brand Management Vol. 21, 7/8, 635–649 649

You might also like