Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/272717543

The job satisfaction-job performance relationship for creative tasks: An


empirical investigation of the role of attitude and behavior in job performance
among scientists

Article · January 2013


DOI: 10.5651/jaas.26.201

CITATION READS

1 836

2 authors, including:

Naoko Kato-Nitta
The Institute of Statistical Mathematics
8 PUBLICATIONS   13 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Naoko Kato-Nitta on 01 January 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Japanese Journal of Administrative Science Article
Volume 26, No.3, 2013, 201-214 原 著
Job satisfaction‒job performance relationship for creative tasks
経営行動科学第 26 巻第 3 号,2013,201-214

The job satisfaction‒job performance relationship for creative tasks:


An empirical investigation of the role of attitude and behavior
in job performance among scientists

Naoko KATO-NITTA
(Ibaraki University)
**
Tadahiko MAEDA
(The Institute of Statistical Mathematics /
The Graduate University for Advanced Studies [Sokendai])

This study examines whether job satisfaction–job performance relationship


models vary when creativity considered as a task characteristic. Although previ-
ous studies have pointed out the possibility of a difference in the correlation
strength between these two variables based on job complexity, the evidence is
incomplete. Using data from a survey of Japanese scientists, we evaluated scien-
tists’ performance by categorizing their tasks into two types, creative tasks and
formulaic tasks according to creativity demands, and used publication produc-
tivity as an objective indicator. We then investigated their relationships with job
satisfaction. While the tasks differed in the level of creativity required, they both
were high-complexity tasks. The results showed that a job satisfaction–job per-
formance relationship was observed only for job performance related to creative
tasks. Furthermore, we examined the need for considering the relationship at
both individual and group levels. Our results were consistent for both levels.

Keywords:job satisfaction, job performance, creativity, scientists, public research


institutions, university research administrator (URA)

performance. After Iaffaldano and Muchinsky


1 Introduction
(1985) conducted a meta-analysis and conclud-
The relationship between job satisfaction ed that there was little correlation between the
and job performance has attracted the atten- two, studies exploring this relationship tempo-
tion of many organizational researchers in rarily declined. Judge et al. (2001) conducted a
recent decades. However, gaps in the research meta-analysis and reported a higher correlation
still remain. Contrary to the common intuitive between job satisfaction and job performance,
assumption that happy workers who are satisfied presenting an average correlation coefficient
with their jobs will work harder, previous stud- of 0.30. Encouraged by their findings, research
ies have shown few consistent results regarding interest in this area began to increase once
the correlation between job satisfaction and job again. While many empirical examinations
exploring the relationship between job satisfac-
* Postdoctoral Researcher, Ibaraki University. tion and job performance have been conducted
** The Institute of Statistical Mathematics / Associate
Professor, The Graduate University for Advanced
and have reported a correlation between the
Studies [Sokendai]. two variables, many researchers still hold the

−201−
原 著 経営行動科学第 26 巻第 3 号

view that the correlation is spurious (e.g., Bowl- a group level. However, few empirical examina-
ing, 2007). Considering the significance of the tions have discussed the correlation at both indi-
relationship between “the most focal attitude vidual and group levels within a single empirical
about the job (job satisfaction)” and “the most survey. For this reason, we included both indi-
focal behavior on the job (job performance)” in vidual and group levels in our models to achieve
the field of industrial/organizational psychol- a more comprehensive understanding of the job
ogy (Judge et al., 2001: 388), there is a need satisfaction–job performance relationship.
to re-examine this issue using an alternative
2 Theoretical background and
approach. This study aims to deepen the under-
purpose
standing of the job satisfaction–job perfor-
mance relationship by employing an approach 2.1 Job performance in creative and
from scientometrics for measuring publication formulaic tasks of scientists
productivity as a more objective indicator of job Most literature cited in early studies in the
1
performance . 1950s and 1960s on the relationship between job
Many researchers (e.g., Ivancevich 1979; satisfaction and job performance dealt with sim-
Judge et al., 2001) have cited job complexity ple labor (Brayfield & Crokett, 1955; Latham,
as a moderator between job satisfaction and 2007; Vroom, 1964). However, current social
job performance and found the correlation to and economic structures have greatly changed
be higher in high-complexity jobs than in low- with the advance of science and technology. The
complexity jobs. This hypothesis has indeed quality of jobs is also changing significantly in
been substantially confirmed; however, some the rapidly evolving globalized information soci-
inconsistent evidence has been observed across ety. Job complexity has significantly increased
occupations. Therefore, the current study in every occupation and greater emphasis is
proposes models that assume differences in placed on the creativity of each employee. In
task creativity but similarity in task complexity this context, the importance of examining atti-
for job performance and examines how these tude and behavior in relation to highly skilled
models vary with regard to job satisfaction–job professionals is growing in organizational set-
performance correlation strength. In particular, tings. For this study, focusing on scientists as a
we focused on task differences among the same typical example of highly skilled professionals is
occupation, i.e., scientists engaged in research effective for making future predictions because
in the basic sciences in Japanese public research scientific research is both highly complex and
institutions. creative. In particular, this study focuses on sci-
2
Referring to the ecological fallacy , it has entists engaged in research in the basic sciences
been argued that a job satisfaction–job perfor- at the following five national scientific research
mance relationship is a hypothesis based on institutions in Japan: National Astronomical
group-level analyses (Latham, 2007; Schneider, Observatory of Japan, National Institute for
1985). Ostroff (1992) carried out an organiza- Fusion Science, Institute for Molecular Science,
tional-level analysis stating that most previous Institute of Space and Astronautical Science
empirical examinations discussed the issue at and Japan Aerospace Exploration Agency, and
an individual level and lacked understanding at the High Energy Accelerator Research Organi-

−202−
Job satisfaction‒job performance relationship for creative tasks

zation. These public scientific institutions are butional disparity across occupations observed
recognized globally for being at the forefront in Judge et al.’s (2001) investigation. Thus, we
in the development of cutting-edge physical sci- proposed that these results should be explained
ence research and advanced scientific devices, on the basis of creativity and introduced this
including the Subaru Telescope, the Large Heli- concept to the current study. In particular, we
3
cal Device (LHD), and the KEKB accelerator . categorized scientists’ job performance into two
These national institutions operate many large- types: job performance in creative tasks and job
scale scientific projects and feature scientists’ performance in formulaic tasks. The former is
work. Scientists are expected to contribute quantified by the scientists’ publication produc-
through group performance in projects as well tivity in terms of research articles, patents, and
as individual performance. Current public related work and the latter is quantified by pub-
scientific-research institutions that participate lication productivity in terms of reports submit-
in physical sciences rarely conduct research ted to governmental offices and related work.
solely at the entire organization or individual As highly skilled professionals, scientists are
level. In general, they perform research activi- involved in tasks that are highly complex and
ties at a project or group level. Therefore, from require advanced skills. Both types of tasks pre-
the management point of view, the group level sented above are complex and require highly
should be the most appropriate and important skilled professional knowledge; however, the for-
unit to measure and discuss about achieving mer additionally requires creativity to produce
outcomes for the organizations investigated in novel knowledge while the latter additionally
the present study. Using survey data collected requires the ability to follow specifications to
from these scientists, this study aims to quan- produce formulaic documents. In an interview
4
titatively deepen our understanding of the job conducted as a prestudy for this survey , one
satisfaction–job performance relationship by scientist described the latter task as “the cre-
measuring the two levels of actors in organiza- ation of explanatory materials for bureaucrats”
tions; i.e., group and individual. and the “need to produce them in a certain
Judge et al. (2001) verified the hypothesis manner.” For scientists involved in large-scale
that the correlation between job satisfaction and projects that require managing large-scale bud-
job performance is higher in high-complexity gets, this reporting of research is an important
jobs than in low-complexity jobs by using job task. In the field of technology management,
complexity as a moderator. They revealed that creativity is mostly measured by considering
the highest correlations were observed for scien- patent publication as an indicator. In this study,
tists–engineers and the lowest were observed for our attention is focused on scientists; therefore,
laborers. However, this explanation is incom- publications such as research articles are also
plete because they found a lower correlation for included as indicators of creativity.
nurses and accountants than for clerical work- Some organizational researchers have point-
ers. Furthermore, Argyle (1989) proposed the ed out that achievement without professional
hypothesis that the correlation was higher in growth or improved competence might lead to
white-collar workers than in blue-collar workers. dissatisfaction (Latham and Yukl, 1976; Latham,
This hypothesis still cannot explain the distri- 2007; Parker, 2003). The supposition that the

−203−
原 著 経営行動科学第 26 巻第 3 号

participants in this study, i.e., scientists, experi- Another considerable debate involves directly
ence the greatest growth and improvement in applying task performance scales developed by
their competencies when they exercise their western cultures to eastern cultures (Fisher and
creativity and achieve a scientific breakthrough Hartel, 2004; Ng et al., 2009). We try to solve
leads to the hypothesis below: these issues by applying an approach from scien-
tometrics with the number of publications as an
Hypothesis 1: The relationship between job
objective indicator of the task performance of
performance and job satisfaction is stronger in
scientists to confirm whether the job satisfaction
creative tasks than in formulaic tasks.
–job performance relationship is also observed
Correlation analyses were conducted based through this approach.
on Hypothesis 1.
2.3 Overall job satisfaction and facet
2.2 Measuring job performance job satisfaction
It is important to recognize that performance According to Locke (1976: 1304), job satisfac-
indicates different concepts in empirical exami- tion is defined as a “pleasurable or positive emo-
nations. Typical examples include the two differ- tional state resulting from the appraisal of one’s
ent concepts of task performance and contex- job or job experiences.” This is probably the
tual performance. Motowidlo and Van Scotter most commonly accepted definition of job satis-
(1994) stated that the former is the central task faction. However, this definition is now believed
in a workplace. Borman and Motowidlo (1993, to be lacking in a clear distinction between
1997) suggested that contextual performance overall job satisfaction (satisfaction with one’s
represented a task rather than a support activ- job as a whole) and facet job satisfaction (sat-
ity for employees to promote the central task. isfaction with individual matters such as pay as
Empirical examinations that define organiza- well as specific matters such as a supervisor’s
tional citizenship behavior (Organ, 1988), a management ability). These two types of satis-
concept similar to contextual performance, as faction have often been differentiated in recent
6
performance have been conducted. Some stud- studies (Shimazu, 2010) . For instance, Judge
ies have reported that the correlation between et al. (2001) has pointed out the drawbacks of
job satisfaction and job performance is higher applying the average scores of various facet job
and bidirectional if organizational citizenship satisfaction scales as overall job satisfaction and
behavior is conceptualized as performance that one should use the overall job-satisfaction
(Bateman and Organ, 1983; Schneider, 1985). scale to appropriately estimate the relationship
In this study, however, we define task perfor- between overall job satisfaction and job perfor-
mance as the central task of scientists. mance. Therefore, in this study, we discuss over-
In most previous empirical examinations, all job satisfaction with multiple questionnaire
each individual’s job performance was measured items developed in the Japanese language to
on the basis of supervisor ratings or peer–subor- construct overall job satisfaction.
5
dinate ratings (e.g., Shore and Martin, 1989).
In any case, these ratings are subjective; ratings
by a rater are strongly affected by his/her bias.

−204−
Job satisfaction‒job performance relationship for creative tasks

2.4 Individual-level and group-level both levels. This study discusses the relationship
correlations between the two at both group and individual
Two influential reviews of the job satisfaction levels.
–job performance relationship have discussed Furthermore, we introduce the level of
individual-level analysis (Iaffalno and Muchinski, research activity as a morale-related behav-
1985; Judge et al., 2001). Ostroff (1992) pointed ioral variable. In this study, the frequency of
out that previous studies on the correlation the individual behavior of holding scientific
between the two showed almost no correlation, workshops or seminars within and outside the
and that these studies concentrated on individ- organization is measured as an indicator of the
ual-level analysis and lacked organization-level level of research activity because these behav-
analysis. To measure school performance, she iors are directly related to the level of morale
examined the correlations between the organi- associated with scientific research. Similarly, job
zation (school)-level average of teachers’ job sat- satisfaction is defined as an attitudinal variable
isfaction and the average of multiple variables directly related to the level of morale associ-
such as students’ grades and teachers’ turnover ated with scientific research. The current study
rate at each school. The results revealed that discusses in detail the relationship between job
the school-level job satisfaction of teachers was satisfaction and job performance by contrasting
significantly correlated with school-level perfor- a morale-related behavioral variable, level of
mance. A recent meta-analysis by Whitman et research activity, with a morale-related attitudi-
al. (2010) discussed the relationship between nal variable, job satisfaction, at both group and
unit-level collective job satisfaction and job per- individual levels. Like previous studies that used
formance and reported a correlation between cross-sectional surveys, this study investigates the
them. Considering the group-level correlation correlation between the two variables. However,
between job satisfaction and job performance, on the basis of social–psychological assumption
Latham (2007: 106) stated that ‘when morale that attitude precedes behavior and following
was high, the productivity of the organization previous empirical studies (e.g., Borgogni et al.,
was also high’ and interpreted its meaning as 2010; Fulmer et al., 2003; Harter et al., 2002;
‘individual-level attitudes and behaviors became Nishii et al., 2008; Ostroff, 1992), we investi-
shared and produced an emergent collective gated job satisfaction as an explanatory variable
structure of attitudes, norms, and behaviors that and job performance as an explained variable.
affected organizational outcomes’ (Latham, In particular, we statistically tested the hypoth-
2007: 107). Should the correlation between job esis below with regression analyses.
satisfaction and job performance, then, be dis-
Hypothesis 2: Morale-related variables explain
cussed only at the group level? To confirm that
higher job performance at both group and indi-
a proposition which holds true in a group does
vidual levels.
not always hold true for individuals, it is neces-
sary to analyze the same participants at both This study aims to determine whether job sat-
group and individual levels. To date, few empiri- isfaction–job performance relationship models
cal studies have simultaneously explored the vary with regard to correlation strength when
job satisfaction–job performance relationship at creativity is considered as a task characteristic.

−205−
原 著 経営行動科学第 26 巻第 3 号

Through this examination, we further explore the list of personnel or on group websites.
the influence of job satisfaction on job perfor- The questionnaires were distributed from
mance at both individual and group levels by December 2010 to January 2011. All responses
contrasting scientists’ overall job satisfaction were anonymous. Participation in the study was
with their level of research activity. Finally, we voluntary, and researchers willing to respond
discuss practical implications and future sugges- were asked to mail the self-reported responses
tions derived from the results. Our study applies with a stamped and addressed envelope. A total
an interdisciplinary approach using more objec- of 1,240 questionnaires were distributed, of
tive indicators of task performance to provide a which 494 were returned, giving a response rate
clear and more comprehensive reporting of the of 39.8%. The questionnaires noted that the
results. By this approach, our study seeks to bet- data would be used for academic research, the
ter understand human attitudes and behaviors survey data would be treated statistically, and
in organizational settings. individuals would not be identified.
Of the 494 respondents, 457 were male and
3 Method
37 were female, and the average age was 43
3.1 Survey instruments years (43.6 years for men, 35.5 years for wom-
Questionnaire Questionnaires were distributed en). There were 413 doctoral degree holders,
to all researchers working at the five Japanese accounting for 83.8% of the total respondents.
public scientific research institutions. These Among them, 70.2% had a degree in science,
national institutions are supervised by the Japa- 22.3% in engineering, and 7.5% in other fields.
nese Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Sci-
ence, and Technology. 3.2 Project groups
For distributing the questionnaires, first, The participants were asked to select their
in-person visits were made to the leaders of the research project group or division. They also
research groups. Then, the outline of the survey indicated their tenure in the group or division.
was explained directly by the first author. The This item was necessary to distinguish between
leaders were also asked about the number of their past and current job performances and
researchers in their project groups, and accord- measure the correlation between current job
ingly, they received the appropriate number of satisfaction and current job performance. Fifty-
questionnaires. When a leader was absent on four groups having three or more respondents
the day of the visit, a secretarial staff member were chosen, and we adopted the average values
of the group was asked about the number of in each group as the group-level variables. The
researchers and the member received the ques- number of group members ranged from 3 to 21
tionnaires. The secretarial staff members were with an average of 8.
also requested to transfer the e-mails explaining
the outline of the survey to all research mem- 3.3 Job performance scale
bers in the group. This procedure was necessary In this study, we measured scientists’ job
to obtain more accurate numbers of research- performance by using the number of publica-
active scientists because, sometimes, names of tions produced as the objective indicator. We
part-time or adjunct scientists do not appear in measured publication productivity by consider-

−206−
Job satisfaction‒job performance relationship for creative tasks

8
ing the following seven items partially modified (2004) .
from the UNESCO Survey items (Andrews, 1. Satisfied with current workplace
1979), which were translated into Japanese and 2. Satisfied with current tasks
then revised on the basis of feedback from Japa- 3. Satisfied with current duty position
nese scientists at the examined research insti- 4. My wish is to continue working here for the
tutions: (a) books, (b) scientific or technical next five years
articles published in refereed journals as a first Each item was measured on a five-point scale.
author, (c) scientific or technical articles pub- The sum of the values of the four items was
lished in refereed journals as a coauthor, (d) calculated to construct the overall job satisfac-
scientific or technical articles published in ref- tion scale. The Cronbach alpha coefficient for
ereed conference proceedings as a first author, the four items was 0.826, which indicated a suf-
(e) patents, (f) reports or explanatory materials ficient level of reliability (internal consistency)
to be submitted to government and committees, for this scale.
and (g) internal research reports. The variable
job performance of creative tasks was measured 3.5 Scale of level of research activity
by calculating the total of each of (a)–(e). Prior Few studies have conducted organizational
to calculating the total for the job performance research by constructing scientists’ level of
of creative tasks, (b) scientific or technical research activity with behavioral variables. In the
articles published in refereed journals as a first present study, therefore, a new scale for measur-
7
author, and (e) patents were weighted double . ing the level of research activity was developed
The variable job performance of formulaic tasks by categorizing the frequency of conducting
was measured calculating the total (f) plus (g). scientific workshops or seminars as follows: (a)
By excluding articles published without going within the project group to which the scientists
through refereed systems for (b), (c), and (d), belong, (b) in the organization to which the sci-
we tried to secure the homogeneity of the mea- entists belong, or (c) outside the organization.
surement in the quality of creativity. In terms of Each was measured on a five-point scale. The
the number of produced publications, for sci- Cronbach alpha coefficient for the three items
entists who had worked in the project group for was 0.833, which indicated a sufficient level of
three years or longer, the past three years’ per- reliability (internal consistency) for this scale.
formance was measured, whereas for those who
4 Results
had worked for less than three years, the pos-
sible performance for three years was calculated To examine Hypothesis 1, we statistically ana-
using their period of service and the number of lyzed the correlation between job satisfaction (JS)
publications produced during that period. and both job performance of creative tasks (JP1)
and job performance of formulaic tasks (JP2).
3.4 Job satisfaction scale Table 1 shows the means and standard deviations
Overall job satisfaction was measured using of each variable and the correlations between the
the four items below, which was partially modi- variables at both group and individual levels.
fied from the Japanese language version of the As is shown by the intercorrelations below
job satisfaction scale developed by Shimazu the diagonal in Table 1, JS has a statistically sig-

−207−
原 著 経営行動科学第 26 巻第 3 号

Table 1 Correlation matrix, means and standard deviations of the variables

Measure 1 2 3 4 M SD
_ 0.083 0.419 ** 0.040 14.541 1.566
1. JS
0.095 * _ 0.446** 0.071 7.150 1.225
2. RA
0.105 * 0.264 *** _ 0.100 16.709 11.205
3. JP1
0.034 0.113* 0.260 *** _ 4.771 7.219
4. JP2
M 14.382 7.110 15.517 5.130
SD 3.750 2.800 19.309 17.518

Note: Intercorrelations for group level (n = 54) and individual level (n = 437) are
presented above and below the diagonal, respectively. Means and standard devia-
tions for group and individual levels are presented in the vertical columns and the
horizontal rows, respectively.
JS = Job satisfaction; RA = Level of research activity; JP1 = Job performance of cre-
ative tasks; JP2 = Job performance of formulaic tasks
*** = p < 0.0001, ** = p < 0.001, * = p < 0.005

nificant positive correlation with JP1 at an indi- that affects JP. Employment status (permanent
vidual level, whereas it has no correlation with or temporary) and age were entered in the
9
JP2. In addition, there is a correlation between regression models as control variables . In this
JP1 and JP2. Therefore, we can observe that an research, JS and RA are defined as the attitu-
individual who has a high productivity in JP1 dinal and behavioral variables, respectively,
tends to have a high productivity in JP2. Thus, directly relating to morale. Furthermore, JS and
the result of the correlation analysis supports RA are explanatory variables and JP1 and JP2
Hypothesis 1. As shown by the intercorrelations are explained variables.
above the diagonal in Table 1, JS is also positive-
Hypothesis 2: Morale-related variables explain
ly correlated with JP1 at the group level, where-
higher job performance at both group and indi-
as it has no correlation with JP2. The results
vidual levels.
show that the correlation between JS and JP1 is
Hypothesis 2-1: Higher JS and RA explain high-
higher at the group level than at the individual
er JP1 at the individual level.
level. Thus, Hypothesis 1 is consistent both in
Hypothesis 2-2: Higher JS and RA explain high-
individual and group level analyses. That is,
er JP1 at the group level.
job satisfaction–job performance relationship
Hypothesis 2-3: Higher JS and RA explain high-
models vary with regard to correlation strength
er JP2 in individual level.
when creativity is considered as a task character-
Hypothesis 2-4: Higher JS and RA explain high-
istic both at individual and group levels.
er JP2 at the group level.
In response to the results of the correla-
tion analyses at individual and group levels, Results of regression analyses for Hypotheses
we set the following working hypotheses and 2-1 and 2-2 are shown in Table 2 and results of
performed regression analyses with ordinary regression analyses for Hypotheses 2-3 and 2-4
least squares estimation to further understand are shown in Table 3.
the role of JS. We compared JS with the vari- The individual-level regression model explain-
able representing level of research activity (RA) ing JP1 is significant. Both JS and RA have sig-

−208−
Job satisfaction‒job performance relationship for creative tasks

Table 2 Regression analyses for JP1


JP1 (Job Performance of Creative Tasks)
Individual Group

Variable B 95%CI B 95%CI

Constant -7.013 [-17.344, 3.318] -44.247 [-77.993, -10.500]


JS 0.486* [0.011, 0.961] 0.092 2.684** [1.028, 4.341] 0.375
RA 1.879*** [1.203, 2.556] 0.265 3.939** [1.785, 6.094] 0.431

F (df1, df 2) 10.920*** (4, 445) 9.009*** (3, 50)


2
R 0.089 0.351
2
Adjusted R 0.081 0.312
n 450 54

Note: Employment status (permanent or temporary) and age are controlled for individual level model
and group average age is controlled for group level model
JS = Job satisfaction; RA = Level of research activity; JP1 = Job performance of creative tasks
*** = p < 0.0001, ** = p < 0.001, * = p < 0.005

Table 3 Regression analyses for JP2

JP2 (Job Performance of Formulaic Tasks)

Individual Group

Variable B 95%CI B 95%CI


Constant -2.137 [-9.494, 5.219] 0.876 [-26.019, 27.772]
JS 0.077 [-0.355, 0.509] 0.017 0.142 [-1.178, 1.463] 0.031
RA 0.324 [-0.295, 0.943] 0.052 0.432 [-1.285, 2.149] 0.073

F (df1, df 2) 4.673** (4, 433) 0.112 (3, 50)


2
R 0.041 0.007
Adjusted R 2 0.033 -0.053

n 438 54

Note: Employment status (permanent or temporary) and age are controlled for individual level model
and group average age is controlled for group level model.
JS = Job satisfaction; RA = Level of research activity; JP2 = Job performance of formulaic tasks
*** = p < 0.0001, ** = p < 0.001, * = p < 0.005

nificant coefficients. RA, with a standardized both are statistically significant, the two morale-
coefficient of 0.265, explains JP1 at an individual related variables at the group level explain JP1.
2
level relatively stronger than JS, with a standard- The adjusted R in this regression model is
ized coefficient of 0.092. Both morale-related 0.312, which is relatively higher than that of the
variables explain JP1 at the individual level. individual-level model. Thus, Hypothesis 2-2 is
Thus, Hypothesis 2-1 is supported. The group- supported. Job performance of creative tasks
level regression model that explains JP1 is signif- is affected by the morale-related variables of JS
icant. Because the standardized coefficients of and RA at both group and individual levels.
JS and RA are 0.375 and 0.431, respectively, and As shown in Table 3, although the individual-

−209−
原 著 経営行動科学第 26 巻第 3 号

2
level model has a statistically significant R , to be engaged in higher-complexity tasks, than
neither of the explanatory variables (JS and clerical workers implied that job complexity was
RA) have significant coefficients. Although not the only moderator to be considered. The
a significant simple correlation coefficient is findings of our study may lead to key insights
observed between RA and JP2 in the individual- that support this direction. When exploring the
level correlation analysis, RA has no effect on job satisfaction–job performance relationship,
JP2 after controlling for employment status scientists found greater satisfaction in perform-
and age. Thus, Hypothesis 2-3 is not supported. ing more creative and discretionary tasks than
Consistent with the individual-level analysis, nei- formulaic tasks. Thus, the use of task creativity
ther JS nor RA has a significant coefficient for in our study proved to be advantageous.
JP2 in the group-level regression model. Thus, This study also demonstrated that the
Hypothesis 2-4 is not supported. approach of closely focusing on tasks within the
Results from regression analyses indicate that same occupation, not only across occupations,
JS and RA as morale-related attitudinal and was valid. Focusing on bibliometric information
behavioral variables, respectively, influence JP1 was also effective in facilitating job performance
at both the individual and the group level. In quantification and clear task classification.
contrast, JS and RA have no influence on JP2. To facilitate a deeper understanding of the
These results indicate that morale-related vari- job satisfaction–job performance relationship,
ables affect only the job performance of creative examining both individual and group levels
tasks. simultaneously rather than separately was
required, as performed in our study. According
5 Discussion
to our findings, the correlation between JS and
5.1 Discussion on the key findings JP was higher at the group level. Furthermore,
We investigated the job satisfaction–job per- the individual-level and group-level regression
formance relationship for scientific research analyses indicated that the influence of JS on
as a typical example of a highly complex occu- JP1 was considerably higher at the group level
pation by dividing scientists’ tasks into two than at the individual level. This finding is
variables: creative and formulaic tasks. Our consistent with the observations of researchers
results demonstrate the following. The correla- focused on group-level analyses (Ostroff, 1992;
tions between JS and JP are observed only for Whitman, 2010). Given that there is a statisti-
the job performance of creative tasks for the cally significant relationship between JS and JP1,
scientists. JS explains productivity in the perfor- even after controlling for other variables and
mance of creative tasks at both the individual utilizing objective job performance indicators,
and the group level. In contrast, for the job we can conclude that job satisfaction is related
performance of formulaic tasks, JS does not to job performance at the individual level as
show any correlation at either the individual well. The influence of RA was also stronger at
or the group level. The reverse phenomenon the group level, although it was not as signifi-
observed in Judge et al.’s (2001) meta-analysis cant as that of JS. When members of the group
that the correlation between JS and JP was lower were highly satisfied with their job and higher
in nurses and accountants, who were thought level of research activity, JP1 for the entire

−210−
Job satisfaction‒job performance relationship for creative tasks

group was high. The inherent assumption of measurement of job performance used, which
our results is that morale has a strong effect on possibly integrated tasks that require abilities or
the job performance of creative tasks, particu- efforts with different orientations.
larly at the group level. In this study, the approach of focusing on
two different tasks is the basis of our assertion
5.2 Limitations and suggestions for that task creativity plays a key role in the job
future research satisfaction–job performance relationship even
This study only considered the individual while controlling for task complexity. However,
level and a relatively small group level, with 3– the validity of our hypothesis that differences in
21 team members. In the future, it will be neces- task creativity explain the job satisfaction–job
sary to confirm whether this result is observed performance relationship difference should be
not only at individual and group levels but also examined in other occupations that have a simi-
on a larger scale, that is, at an organizational lar dualistic structure.
level. The present study demonstrated the necessity
In terms of task performance measurement, and effectiveness of considering both individual-
it may be argued that the focal tasks of scientists and group-level relationships within a single
include not only research but also management study. From a statistical point of view, building
and education. Because educational results can a more comprehensive model that reflects the
also result in the creation of publications, the two-level structure of this data employing mul-
aspect of education is reflected in JP1 and the tilevel modeling approach (e.g., Hox, 2010)
aspect of management is reflected in JP2. From is more appropriate. In that case, factors that
these changes, we can conclude that overall are not dealt with in this study should also be
2
task performance was successfully measured incorporated. The relatively small R of the
with two objective variables. One of the original individual-level regression model presented in
contributions of this study is in categorizing task the current paper also indicates the existence
performance into two variables based on the of other explanatory factors, and these factors
concept of creativity. By doing so, our approach would be dealt with in the future researches.
was effective in finding another factor that
influences the job satisfaction–job performance 5.3 Practical implications
relationship, i.e., task creativity, apart from the Claiming that scientists including university
previously reported factors of job complexity faculty members find it difficult to find enough
and job type. In fact, many scientists would feel time for research because of the administrative
uncomfortable with the integration of JP1 and burden, the Ministry of Education, Culture,
JP2 as one variable. Integration of all tasks of an Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT) of
occupation for measuring overall task perfor- Japan is promoting the project of “Building up
mance in one construct may lead to integration research administration system” launched from
10
of tasks that require differently oriented abilities 2011 fiscal year . In this project, MEXT is pro-
or efforts. Inconsistent results for the job satis- posing to facilitate the new positions of “Univer-
faction–job performance correlation presented sity research administrators (URA)” in universi-
by previous studies may be ascribed to the ties. For instance, formulaic tasks investigated in

−211−
原 著 経営行動科学第 26 巻第 3 号

the present study can be one of the options for Acknowledgments


URAs taking over the faculty tasks. Our results This paper is based on a part of doctoral
showed that formulaic tasks did not correlate thesis of the first author (Naoko Kato-Nitta)
with scientists’ job satisfaction even though they and revised with the second author (Tadahiko
are complex and require highly skilled profes- Maeda). The authors thank Nobuo Arimoto,
sional knowledge. Thus, hiring doctoral degree Satoru Ikeuchi, and Yoshio Saito for their assis-
holders with the aptitude for such tasks as URAs tance in conducting the survey. In addition, they
should be a measure for expansion of employ- thank Hiroya Hirakimoto, Hideyuki Izumiura,
ment opportunities for them as well as facilitat- Takashi Nakamura, Koken Ozaki, and Masashi
ing scientific innovation within public scientific Tachikawa who provided insightful comments
research institutions. and suggestions at the various stages of writ-
Our results, showing that job performance ing this paper. They would like to express their
of creative tasks is affected by morale-related gratitude to the scientists who responded the
variables of job satisfaction and level of research questionnaire survey and the public scientific
activity, should be quite beneficial to human research institutions for their cooperation on
resource managers who aim to promote scien- this research project. This study is supported
tists’ and/or organizational creativity. Managers by School of Advanced Sciences, the Graduate
may benefit from such information and try to University for Advanced Studies.
increase their employees’ satisfaction by empha-
sizing the likelihood of an improved work envi- Notes
1
ronment. A project group leader who applies The method of measuring scientists’ performance
effective leadership by encouraging employees using the number of scientific or technical articles
is used not only in scientometrics but also in the
to increase their level of research activity would
sociology of science, and it is not a new approach
also contribute in stimulating scientists’ and/or in itself. The expression “an approach from scien-
organizational creativity. At the same time, to tometrics” refers to the approach of classifying and
enhance future competitiveness, promoting quantifying job performance using bibliographic
information. In other words, we divided various
researcher creativity is a relatively important
types of publication productivity by scientists into
theme for not only public institutions engaged creative tasks and formulaic tasks, which makes this
in the basic sciences but also for the private study original.
2
sector. Exploring the relationship between task The ecological fallacy, or the ecological inference
problem, was originally proposed by Robinson
performance and job satisfaction in the same
(1950), who noted that correlations observed in
occupation may be particularly important for aggregated data at the group level are not always
practitioners interested in creating more imme- observed in individual units of analysis.
3
diate productivity gains. We anticipate that this Subaru Telescope is an 8.2-m optical infrared tele-
scope developed and operated by National Astro-
study will mark the beginning of future empiri-
nomical Observatory of Japan. LHD is a supercon-
cal studies exploring what tasks within occupa- ducting helical device developed and operated by
tions are correlated with the overall job satisfac- the National Institute for Fusion Science of Japan.
tion of employees. KEKB is a particle accelerator with a ring path
approximately 1 km in diameter. It was developed
and is operated by the High Energy Accelerator
Research Organization of Japan. Currently, Subaru

−212−
Job satisfaction‒job performance relationship for creative tasks

is one of the world’s largest optical infrared tele- report lower levels of job satisfaction.
10
scopes, LHD is the world’s largest fusion experi- Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and
ment, and KEKB is the world’s highest luminosity Technology (MEXT) website, http://www.mext.
accelerator. go.jp/a_menu/jinzai/ura/detail/1315871.htm [in
4
The interview was conducted in January 2010 with Japanese, accessed in Feb. 2012.]
a Japanese astronomer in his 30s working on a
large-scale scientific project. References
5
Approximately 89% of the meta-analysis by Judge
Aletraris, L. 2010 How satisfied are they and why? A
et al. (2001) included supervisory or peer ratings.
6 study of job satisfaction, job rewards, gender and
Overall job satisfaction and facet job satisfaction
temporary agency workers in Australia. Human
are differentiated in articles that explore the deter-
Relations, 63, 1129-1155.
minants of job satisfaction, whereas in articles that
Andrews, F. M. 1979 Scientific productivity: The effective-
discuss the relationship between job satisfaction
ness of research groups in six countries. Cambridge:
and job performance, they are ambiguously dif-
Cambridge University Press/UNESCO.
ferentiated in some cases. As Judge et al. (2001) nd
Argyle, M. 1989 The Social psychology of work. 2 edition.
pointed out in their meta-analysis, Iaffaldano and
Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Muchinsky (1985) measured overall job satisfac-
Bateman, T. S. & Organ, D. W. 1983 Job satisfaction
tion using the average correlation between various
and the good soldier: The relationship between
job satisfaction factors and job performance.
7 affect and employee “citizenship”. Academy of
Even in the field of scientometrics or research
Management Journal, 26, 887-895.
evaluation, there is no established, reliable, and
Borgogni, L., Russo, S. D., Petitta, L. & Vecchione, M.
valid way of weighting the types of publication
2010 Predicting job satisfaction and job perfor-
of scientists to date. However, it appears that a
mance in a privatized organization. International
consensus has been established to some extent
Public Management Journal, 13, 275-296.
that the weight of the original papers of the first
Borman, W. C. & Motowidlo, S. J. 1993 Expanding the
author is heavier than that of the second author or
criterion domain to include elements of contex-
later. Also, in the field of engineering, obtaining
tual performance. In N. Schmit & Borman, W. C.
a patent can be considered more important than
(eds) Personnel selection in organizations. New York:
presenting conference proceedings. Therefore, we
Jossey-Bass, 71-98.
doubled the weight of the (b) scientific or techni-
Borman, W. C. & Motowidlo, S. J. 1997 Task perfor-
cal articles published in refereed journals as a first
mance and contextual performance: The mean-
author, and (e) patents for measuring the job per-
ing for personnel selection research. Human
formance and conducted regression analyses. We
Performance, 10, 99-109.
presented the weighted version of performance
Bowling, N. A. 2007 Is the job satisfaction–job per-
indicators in this study; however, we would like to
formance relationship spurious? A meta-analytic
note that the results of regression analyses with the
examination. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 71,
simple summation of (a) through (e) were essen-
167-185.
tially the same as that of the presented results. In
Brayfield, A. H. & Crockett, W. H. 1955 Employee atti-
addition, we confirmed that similar results were
tudes and employee performance. Psychological
also obtained with the regression models excluding
Bulletin, 52, 396-494.
(a) books. This study does not intend to develop a
Fisher, G. B. & Hartel, C. E. J. 2004 Evidence for cross-
new performance measure for scientists’ evaluation
vergence in the perception of task and contextual
or to argue that the weight adopted in this study
performance: A study of western expatriates
should be applied in any context.
8 working in Thailand. Cross Cultural Management,
To lessen the burden on respondents, the number
11, 3-15.
of former questionnaire items of job satisfaction
Fulmer, B., Gerhart, B. & Scott, K. 2003 Are the 100
scale developed by Shimazu (2004) was reduced to
best better? An empirical investigation of the rela-
four items on the basis of the prestudy results.
9 tionship between being a “great place to work”
For example, in her study with Australian workers,
and firm performance. Personnel Psychology, 56,
Aletraris (2010) found that temporary workers

−213−
原 著 経営行動科学第 26 巻第 3 号

965-993. attributions of the “why” of HR practices: Their


Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L. & Hayes, T. L. 2002 Busi- effects on employee attitudes and behaviors, and
ness-unit-level relationships between employee customer satisfaction. Personnel Psychology, 61,
satisfaction, employee engagement, and business 503-545.
outcomes: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psy- Organ, D. W. 1988 Organizational citizenship behavior:
chology, 87, 268-279. The good soldier syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexing-
Hox, J. 2010 Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applica- ton Books.
tions, Second Edition. Routledge Academic. Ostroff, C. 1992 The relationship between satisfaction,
Iaffaldano, M. T. & Muchinsky, P. M. 1985 Job satisfac- attitudes, and performance: An organizational
tion and job performance: A meta-analysis. Psy- level analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77,
chological Bulletin, 97, 251-273. 963-974.
Ivancevic, J. M. 1979 High and low task stimulation Parker, S. K. 2003 Longitudinal effects of lean produc-
jobs: A causal analysis of performance-satisfaction tion on employee outcomes and mediating role
relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 22, of work characteristics. Journal of Applied Psychol-
206-222. ogy, 88, 620-634.
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Thoresen, C. J. & Patton, G. Robinson, W. S. 1950 Ecological correlations and
K. 2001 The job satisfaction–job performance the behavior of individuals. American Sociological
relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Review, 15, 351-357.
Psychological Bulletin, 127, 376-407. Schneider, B. 1985 Organizational behavior. Annual
Latham, G. P. 2007 Work motivation: History, theory, Review of Psychology, 36, 573-611.
research, and practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Shimazu, M. 2004 Job satisfaction and psychological stress
Publications. [in Japanese]. Tokyo: Kazama-Shobo.
Latham, G. P. & Yukl, G. A. 1976 Effects of assigned Shimazu, M. 2010 Shokumu-manzoku to soshiki no
and participative goal setting on performance kasseika. In Sangyo Soshiki Shinri-gaku Fujimori,
and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, T. (ed) Industrial / Organizational Psychology [in
61, 166-171. Japanese]. Tokyo: Fukumura-Shuppan.
Locke, E. A. 1976 The nature and causes of job sat- Shore, L. M. & Martin, H. J. 1989 Job satisfaction and
isfaction. In M. D. Dunnette (ed) Handbook of organizational commitment in relation to work
industrial and organizational psychology. Chicago: performance and turnover intensions. Human
Rand McNally, 1297-1349. Relations, 42, 625-638.
Motowidlo, S. J. & Van Scotter, J. R. 1994 Evidence Vroom, V. H. 1964 Work and motivation. New York:
that task performance should be distinguished John Wiley & Sons.
from contextual performance. Journal of Applied Whitman, D. S., Van Rooy, D. L. & Viswesvaran, C.
Psychology, 79, 475-480. 2010 Satisfaction, citizenship behaviors, and per-
Ng, T. W. H., Sorensen, K. L. & Yim, H. K. 2009 Does formance in work units: A meta-analysis of collec-
the job satisfaction–job performance relationship tive construct relations. Personnel Psychology, 63,
vary across cultures? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy- 41-81.
chology, 40, 761-796.
Nishii, L., Lepak, D. & Schneider, B. 2008 Employee (Received February 24, 2012 Accepted February 16, 2013)

−214−

View publication stats

You might also like