Blocking in Humans

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Psychol Rec

DOI 10.1007/s40732-015-0148-x

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Blocking in Humans: Logical Reasoning Versus Contingency


Learning
Diana Delgado 1,2

# Association for Behavior Analysis International 2015

Abstract This study compared the occurrence of the represents a circumstance in which the prediction of an out-
blocking effect when participants had unlimited and limited come does not depend solely on contiguity relations between
time to respond to a causal learning task. In contrast to the stimuli; but is also influenced by previously established com-
dominant views in human causal learning, the underlying as- peting contingencies (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972). Dominant
sumption is that blocking can be sufficiently explained by the theoretical models of this phenomenon have accounted for
same principles that describe conditioning outcomes in ani- attenuated responses to X when presented alone in terms of
mals, but only when logical reasoning about the experimental its redundancy in the prediction of the outcome (Rescorla &
task is impeded. Experiment 1 compares responses to Wagner, 1972; Mackintosh, 1975).
blocking tests by participants in timed and untimed groups. Although this effect has been sufficiently demonstrated in
As expected, most cases of blocking were observed for par- animal experiments, results have not been as consistent with
ticipants in the timed group. Experiment 2 explores an alter- humans. The most typical procedure in human blocking re-
native procedure in which all information about stimulus- search consists of an experimental task in which stimuli
outcome associations was simultaneously present. Based on (i.e., types of food, drinks, or medicines) are paired with an
this information, participants sorted target and control stimuli outcome (e.g., an illness). Once a history of these pairings has
according to their predicted of outcomes. Very limited evi- been established, a novel stimulus is simultaneously presented
dence of blocking was observed with this procedure. with the first stimulus, and both are followed by the occur-
Findings are discussed in terms of the interference of rule rence of the illness. Blocking is then evaluated by asking
generation processes with direct contingency control. participants to estimate the probability that the second stimu-
lus alone will produce the illness. Because the stimulus–stim-
Keywords Blocking . Contingency learning . Logical ulus pairings are easily interpreted in terms of one or two
reasoning . Causal learning . Pavlovian conditioning . events causing another event (e.g., consumption of food caus-
Interference tasks . Rule generation ing an illness), this body of research is commonly described in
the literature as human causal learning.
The blocking effect occurs when pairings of a stimulus and an Among the studies of cue competition effects in human
outcome prevent subsequent conditioning of a second stimu- causal learning, those focusing on the blocking effect seem
lus, which is presented simultaneously with the first, and to have engendered the most notable and impactful amount of
followed by the same outcome (A+, AX+). This procedure empirical research and theoretical controversies (Delgado &
Medina, 2013). Two main interpretations have accounted for
the variability of research findings regarding the observation
* Diana Delgado of the blocking effect.
diana.delgado@konradlorenz.edu.co; dimadel@gmail.com
A dominant interpretation with respect to the high variabil-
ity of outcomes in human blocking stresses that human causal
1
Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz, Cra 9Bis No 62-43, learning cannot be explained by the same set of contingencies
Bogotá, Colombia described by Pavlovian animal experiments. This interpreta-
2
Present address: Shellers Bend, # 2, State College, PA 16801, USA tion is based on findings from a series of research studies that
Psychol Rec

report greater blocking when the procedures provide specific the task rather than from a weakened conditioning of
information about the predictive function of the target cue X the target stimulus produced by the arranged contingen-
(De Houwer, Beckers, & Vandorpe, 2005; Shanks, 2010; cies (De Houwer & Beckers, 2003; Lovibond et al.,
Penn & Povinelli, 2007; Vandorpe & De Houwer, 2006). 2003).
The blocking effect can be described as an ambiguous cir- The opposing view argues that human causal learning is
cumstance with respect to the causal function of the stimuli in sufficiently explained by associative processes or by the prin-
producing the outcome. Thus, from a cognitive standpoint, ciples that describe stimulus–stimulus contingencies. For ex-
blocking in humans is more likely when the experimental ample, Beckers, Miller, De Houwer, & Urushihara (2006)
preparation provides specific information about the relations demonstrated that equivalent results are obtained when ma-
between each of the stimuli and the outcome. A large body of nipulations similar to those used in human causal learning
empirical evidence supports this assertion. Two of the most tasks are used in studies using animals as subjects.
common procedures used in these studies are (a) those that Furthermore, some studies suggest that reasoning about the
specify that the stimuli in the compound have additive effects task may actually hinder rather than facilitate conditioning
(e.g., Livesey & Boakes, 2004; Lovibond, Been, Mitchell, outcomes. For example, several studies (operant and respon-
Bouton, & Frohardt, 2003) and (b) those that specify a dent) have successfully produced conditioning outcomes
submaximal intensity in the occurrence of the outcome when contingency awareness is impeded by superimposing a
(De Houwer, Beckers, & Glautier, 2002; Beckers, De masking task or a cognitive interference task on the presenta-
Houwer, Pineño, & Miller, 2005). tion of stimulus pairings (see, e.g., Delgado, Medina, & Soto,
A study by Livesey and Boakes (2004) illustrates the first 2011; Karazinov & Boakes, 2007; Tonneau & González,
case. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to identify 2004; Walther & Nagengast, 2006). Restricting time to re-
which of a variety of chemicals would make a newly discov- spond to test trials has also been used to impede reasoning
ered bacteria multiply. In the nonadditive group, presentation and verbalizations about the objectives of the task. It is as-
of both single (P) and compound (PQ) stimuli were followed sumed that such time restrictions will impede rule-generation
by a normal effect (+), that is, bacteria increased twice in processes that may interfere with nonverbal control by the
number as a reaction to the chemical. By contrast, in the ad- contingencies arranged in the experimental preparation
ditive group, single stimulus presentations were followed by a (Karazinov & Boakes, 2007).
normal effect (P+), and compound stimulus presentations Accounts of blocking effects based on logical reasoning
were followed by a strong effect, that is, the number of bac- processes (and not as a result of associative learning process-
teria increased four times as a reaction to the chemical es), seem to vastly outnumber those supporting the contingen-
(PQ++). As expected, the data showed a significant difference cy learning account, however. In sum, research findings show
between groups, with higher blocking effects for the additive that greater blocking is predicted when the experimental task
group. provides information that disambiguates the contribution of
In the second commonly used procedure, the intensity of the target cue on the prediction of the outcome. More specif-
the outcome is specified. When the magnitude is either un- ically, it is argued that manipulations of outcome maximality
specified or maximal—that is, if an illness occurs at its max- and additivity show that logical reasoning and rule based pro-
imum possible intensity—the contribution of each stimulus to cesses may be necessary to produce blocking (De Houwer
the outcome is ambiguous. By contrast, when participants are et al., 2005; Livesey & Boakes, 2004; Vandorpe & De
told that the outcome of both single and compound stimuli Houwer, 2006; Vandorpe, De Houwer, & Beckers, 2005).
occur with a submaximal intensity—for example, at 30% of This interpretation may be questionable, however.
its maximum potential—the redundant role of the target stim- Specifying the environmental elements involved in a contin-
ulus is stressed and, thus, blocking is likely to occur. gency relation may produce a higher accuracy in predicting
For example, Beckers et al. (2005) presented fictitious re- the occurrence of blocking effects in both animals and
cords of patients who presented an allergic reaction after hav- humans. In addition, results from maximality and additivity
ing eaten different foods. For participants in the submaximal studies may be interpreted in terms of differential histories of
condition the allergic reaction was of moderate strength, while responding to the stimuli in the compound either separately
for the other half it was a strong reaction. During blocking (elemental responding), or as a unique stimulus configuration
tests, participants were asked to rate on a scale from 1 to 9 (configural responding; Glautier, 2002; Livesey & Boakes,
how likely each of the foods was to produce the allergic reac- 2004; Melchers, Lachnit, Üngör, & Shanks, 2008).
tion. As it has been systematically shown, blocking was stron- Elemental responding is associated with increased blocking
ger for the group in the submaximal condition (Lovibond effects because it allows for competitor-outcome associations
et al., 2003, Experiment 1; Waldmann & Walker, 2005). (A+) to weaken or impede subsequent compound-outcome
Overall, these findings have led authors to conclude that associations (AX+), and not because of the reasoning process-
blocking results from inferential processes occurring during es involved.
Psychol Rec

Ascertaining whether or not deliberate reasoning produces to the target cue, and a group without this restriction. In the
different blocking outcomes from those observed in the tradi- first phase of this procedure, a card with background color A
tional associative learning literature would entail comparing (the competitor cue) is followed by a payoff. In the following
the results of procedures that facilitate or impede reasoning phase, a card with background color A and figure X
while maintaining unaltered the predictive ambiguity of the (target cue) is also followed by a payoff. In the last phase, a
target cue with respect to the outcome. Karazinov and Boakes card with figure X and a neutral background color is
(2007), for example, included a time restriction for one of two presented, and participants are required to judge whether or
groups exposed to a second order conditioning (SOC) proce- not this card produces a payoff.
dure and examined if differential outcomes were observed. In his study, Glautier (2002) argued that presenting target
The authors found SOC for the group in the time-restricted and competitor cues as part of one same stimulus object may
condition only. be associated with a decreased likelihood of blocking, as this
Contrary to the mainstream view, these authors discuss that stimulus arrangement favors configural responding. However,
when logical reasoning is obstructed, responding occurs on it has been demonstrated that elemental or configural
the basis of associative learning processes. Similar to what responding depends on the participants’ history of exposure
has been demonstrated by operant learning experiments on to one or the other type (Melchers et al., 2005; Melchers et al.,
rule-governed behavior (see Catania, 2013), Karazinov and 2008). Some aspects of the original task were modified in
Boakes (2007) suggested that processes of rule generation order to clarify which cues were irrelevant in terms of their
about the operative contingencies may therefore interfere with outcomes and which cues produced a payoff.
direct contingency control.
The blocking procedure may be construed as an ambiguous
situation with respect to the causal role of the target cue in Experiment 1
producing the outcome, wherein deliberate reasoning may act
to modulate the occurrence of blocking effects. More specif- Method
ically, greater blocking should be expected when reasoning is
impeded. In agreement with Karazinov and Boakes (2007), Experimental Design and Data Analysis
the rationale under this assumption is that rule-generation pro-
cesses may prevent behavioral outcomes from undergoing Most of the experiments conducted in this area evaluate
nonverbal contingency control. blocking effects by statistically comparing group average
In a recent attempt to further examine this hypothesis, probability scores of causal attribution for all the relevant
Delgado (2013) used two different types of questions to in- cues. In contrast, we evaluated blocking effects by com-
duce causal judgments as a result of intuitive reasoning or paring responses to target, competitor, and control cues
logical reasoning (see also García-Retamero & Dieckman, from each participant. The traditional blocking procedure
2006; Todd & Gigerenzer, 2000; Tversky & Kahneman, including an intrasubject control condition was used. In the
1983). These terms are often used in the decision-making first phase of this procedure, an event is paired with an
literature to describe rule-based (logical reasoning) versus outcome. In the second phase, the same event in combina-
nonrule-based responding in a decision-making situation tion with a second event is paired with the outcome. A
(Kahneman, 2003; Sloman, 1996). While the question that novel compound stimulus followed by the outcome serves
was oriented toward logical reasoning requested participants as the intrasubject control condition. Data from partici-
to provide a probability rating of the cue producing the out- pants in both groups (timed and untimed) are presented in
come, the intuitive reasoning question forced a yes/no answer tables showing individual responses to target, competitor
by asking participants whether the target cue alone would and control stimuli.
produce the outcome. Although differences in blocking were
not found, it is possible that the question types did not facili- Participants
tate one or the other type of reasoning as intended. Most likely,
because logical reasoning was not directly obstructed, partic- Participants were 26 undergraduate psychology students.
ipants responded to both questions analyzing the probabilities All of them received course credits for volunteering to
involved given the situation. participate in the experiment, and they were assigned to
The present study attempts to further examine if logical the untimed (n=13) and timed (n=13) groups on the basis
reasoning with respect to the contingencies presented in the of order of arrival to the laboratory. Informed consent was
experimental task hinders or facilitates the occurrence of the obtained from all participants at the beginning of the ex-
blocking effect. The procedure, which is a variation of the card perimental sessions. Data from two participants, who did
game simulation used by Glautier (2002), examines differ- not complete the experimental task for the timed group,
ences in blocking in a group with a time restriction to respond were not included in the analysis.
Psychol Rec

Apparatus and Stimuli Similarly, because a figure stimulus cannot be presented in the
absence of a background, background color M was used as a
The experimental task was presented on IBM-compatible neutral cue. It is rendered neutral because it is associated with
computers with 17-in. screens. All phases of the experiment the prediction of the outcome in MX+ and with its absence in
were designed and implemented using the software LabView, MF-.
2010 for Windows. The experiment included 14 cards Once a card completed four presentations, on the fifth trial
(7.5×11 cm). Each card included two types of stimuli: a black a test was presented for that card only. In the test trial, two
figure on the center of the card and a background color. buttons labeled BPayoff^ and BNo Payoff^ were shown next to
Seven background colors and seven figures were combined the card. A correct response was followed by the presentation
for a total of 14 cards. Background colors were: A = red, of the remaining training trials for the other three cards. An
B = green, D = yellow, E = orange, M = gray with horizontal incorrect response added three extra training trials for that card
stripes, H = purple, and I = checkered gray. Figures were: and an additional test trial. This correction procedure was
N = down arrow, O = clover, P = diamond, Q = heart, repeated until correct outcomes for all cards were selected.
X = crescent, F = circle, and R = two-way vertical arrow. No other consequences were provided for correct or incorrect
Throughout this manuscript, each card will be designated selections.
with two letters corresponding to the background color and
the center symbol, respectively (e.g., AO). For the cards asso-
ciated with a payoff, a 7.5×7.5 picture of a bag of money was Second Training Phase The same procedure used in the first
presented to the right of each card. The same picture, crossed training phase was used to train four additional card–outcome
off with a diagonal red line, indicated no payoff. associations: AO+, BP+, DQ-, and HX+. This phase corre-
sponds to the second phase of the traditional blocking proce-
Procedure dure in which the stimulus presented in the first phase
(i.e., competitor stimulus) is now presented simultaneously
Participants were informed that the objective of the study was with a second stimulus (i.e., the blocking or target stimulus),
to identify how relations among stimuli may characterize and followed with the outcome (payoff). In this phase, AO and
some aspects of human learning. At the start of the experi- HX are the relevant cards. For the figure blocking test, figure
ment, the following instructions were provided on the com- O is now presented with background color A, which reliably
puter screen: predicted the outcome in the first training phase. For the back-
ground blocking test, background color H is now presented
This is a game of cards. The computer will deal one card with figure cue X, which reliably predicted a payoff in the first
at a time. As you will see, only some of them produce a training phase. Card BP+ serves as a control stimulus because
payoff. Eventually you may be asked to indicate wheth- neither of the cues (B or P), were presented previously
er a card produces a payoff or not. Observe carefully. in association with a payoff; and DQ- is a filler cue
You may now begin. (see Table 1). For the timed and untimed groups, these training
phases were identical.
Each training trial consisted on the presentation of a card on Thus, to summarize, the figure blocking procedure includ-
the left side of the screen. After 2 seconds, the picture indicat- ed AN+ (first training phase) and AO+ trials (second training
ing payoff or no payoff appeared 6 cm to the right of the card. phase), where O was the figure target cue and A the compet-
Both stimuli remained on the screen for 3 more seconds. The itor cue. The background blocking procedure included MX+
following trial was preceded by a 1.5 seconds intertrial (first training phase) and HX+ trials (second training phase),
interval. where H was the background target cue and X the competitor.

First Training Phase Four cards and their corresponding con-


sequence (payoff/no payoff) were presented four times, in Table 1 Summary of the design
random order: AN+, EN-, MF-, and MX+. This corresponds Experimental Stage Compound Cues and Outcomes
to the first phase of the blocking procedure in which compet-
itor stimuli are presented separately and followed by the out- Training Phase1 AN+, EN-, MF-, MX+
come. There are two competitor stimuli in this phase, A and X. Training Phase 2 AO+, BP+, DQ-, HX+
In card AN, A is the background competitor stimulus for the Tests MO, HN, BN, MP, IX, AR
figure blocking test, and X in card MX is the figure competitor
Note. The first letter corresponds to the background color of the card and
stimulus for the background blocking test. The function of the second corresponds to its figure. O and H are target cues, A and X are
figure N is rendered null given that it is presented both with their corresponding competitors, and BP is the control stimulus. Payoff is
the outcome in card AN and without the outcome in card EN. indicated by a (+) sign, and no payoff is indicated by a (-) sign.
Psychol Rec

Tests Prior to the beginning of this phase, the untimed group Payoff/no payoff reports for cues O (figure target) and P
received the following instruction on the screen: BYou will (control) show differences in blocking effects between the
now see a new set of cards. The computer will deal one card timed and untimed groups for responses to the figure target
at a time. Please indicate if you think the card pays off or not. cue. In the untimed group (n = 11), only one participant
Now, let’s play!^ For the timed group, the following additional showed blocking of the figure target cue, and three partici-
instruction was included: BYou will have 3 seconds to re- pants showed blocking of the target background cue. For the
spond. Please observe each card carefully and try to respond timed group (n=13), blocking of the figure cue was observed
quickly. Now let’s play!^ for eight participants, and blocking for the background cue
In this phase target, control, and competitor cues, were was observed for six participants. Four participants (3, 7, 8,
tested, by presenting them with neutral background color M and 9 in Table 3) showed blocking for both the figure and the
or with neutral figure N. The card used to evaluate responding background cue.
to the figure target cue was MO. Card HN was used to eval- Tables 4 and 5 summarize these data by showing group
uate responding to the background target cue. Their corre- differences in blocking effects for figure and background cues,
sponding controls were MP and BN, where M and N were respectively. Chi-square values were calculated using Fisher’s
neutral stimuli and P and B were the cues presented as part of exact test to establish if observed differences between groups
the control compound in Training Phase 2 of the blocking were statistically significant. As it is shown in Table 4,
procedure. blocking of the figure cue was observed for a higher number
Thus, blocking would occur if (a) a no-payoff response is of participants in the timed group (8/13) as compared with the
predicted in the presence of the card with the target cue and (b) untimed group (1/11). This difference was statistically signif-
a payoff response is predicted in the presence of the card with icant, with χ2 =6.99, p=.01. A similar finding was observed
one of the stimuli of the control compound. Cards IX and AR for the background cue. Although this difference was not sta-
were used to evaluate predictions with respect to competitor tistically significant χ2 =0.90, p=.42, more cases of blocking
cues X and A. of the background target cue were observed in the timed group
Each card was presented only once to simulate a real card (6/13) than in the untimed group (3/11).
game. On each trial, a card was presented on the left side of the By using a correction procedure during Training Phase 1
screen. Participants were asked to click on one of three buttons and 2, we guaranteed that responses to the target stimulus
located to the right of the card. Buttons were labeled BPays off, occurred after participants had successfully acquired compet-
^ BDoes not payoff,^ or BI don’t know.^ For the timed group, itor–outcome associations. In addition to testing responses to
each test trial was available for 3 seconds. If the participant did the target stimuli we also examined the extent to which the
not respond within this time, the trial was presented again at associative strength of the competitors acquired during phase
the end of the randomized sequence. Table 1 shows the exper- 1 (A+ and X+) and phase 2 (AO+ and HX+) would generalize
imental phases and the cards presented in each phase. to the novel compounds AR and IX in a test trial. For the
untimed group, 9 of 11 participants reported that they were
Results unsure as to whether or not the novel card AR produced a
payoff, one participant responded that it produced a payoff,
In accordance with our hypothesis, blocking effects should and one participant responded that it did not. With respect to
occur more consistently in the timed group. That is to say, the IX compound, six participants responded that it produced
with the time restriction impeding reasoning about the causal a payoff and five responded that they were unsure. For the
relations and probabilities involved in the prediction of a pay- timed group, 9 of 13 participants responded they were unsure
off, responding should come under the direct control of about the outcome of card AR, and four responded that the
Pavlovian contingencies, and blocking should be reliably card did not produce a payoff. For the card IX, seven partic-
observed. ipants responded that it produced a payoff and six participants
Tables 2 and 3 show responses to target (MO, HN) and were unsure about the outcome.
control (BP) cues for the untimed and timed groups, respec-
tively (background cue M and figure N are neutral cues). Discussion
Blocking effects were determined on the basis of the case-
by-case comparison of responses to target and control cues. The data presented in Tables 2 and 3 show greater blocking
More specifically, blocking effects are seen when a no- in the timed group. Approximately half of the participants
payoff response is attributed to target cues while a payoff of this group showed blocking of the target cues. By con-
or a Bdon’t know^ response is attributed to control cues. To trast, only one participant in the untimed group showed
facilitate visual inspection of the data, blocking effects are blocking for the target cue O, and three participants
indicated by a check mark on the fourth and seventh col- showed blocking for target cue H. These differences sug-
umns of Tables 2 and 3. gest that, as expected, when deliberate reasoning about the
Psychol Rec

Table 2 Blocking/no blocking


effects of figure and background Ppts O (figure P (control Blocking H (background B (control Blocking of
target cues in the untimed group target cue) figure) of figure cue target cue) background) background cue

1 − + ✓ + +
2 + + − + ✓
3 + d/k − d/k ✓
4 + + + +
5 + d/k + +
6 + d/k + d/k
7 + + + +
8 + + + d/k
9 + d/k + +
10 + + + +
11 + + − d/k ✓

Note. Payoff is indicated by a (+) sign, and no payoff is indicated by a (-) sign. The letters d/k indicate that
participants were unsure about the outcome of the card.

task is impeded, responding may be more likely to resem- the cues in the compound. This effect may be explained in
ble the patterns observed in animal experiments, that is to terms of a transfer of associative strength between the previ-
say, behavior may be more likely to fall under the control ously exposed cue (competitor), the added element (target),
of the principles of contingency learning. However, the and the outcome (Rescorla & Colwill, 1983).
fact that half of the participants in the timed group did In both groups, the number of Bdon’t know^ or uncommit-
not show blocking shows that additional factors are in- ted responses occurred almost exclusively for control cues.
volved in the prediction of responding with respect to com- While these responses may constitute evidence of elemental
pound stimuli. Configural and elemental histories of responding and cognitive reasoning, they may also be indica-
responding may constitute one such influential factor. tive of weak associative strength acquired by the cues during
On the other hand, the lack of negative predictions by con- the sequence of training trials. To examine the latter possibility
trol cards in the figure blocking test (participants in both we contrasted causal judgments of target, competitor, and con-
groups responded that the control card produced a payoff trol cues, using an alternative procedure. In Experiment 2 all
and a few of them gave a Bdon’t know^ response) shows that the information regarding cues and outcomes was simulta-
previous exposure to only one of the elements of the com- neously present on the screen. Cards from Training Phases 1
pound (in Training Phase 1) does affect causal judgments of and 2 were presented in a payoff and no payoff box,

Table 3 Blocking/no blocking


effects of figure and background Ppts O (figure P (control Blocking H (background B (control Blocking of
target cues in the timed group target cue) figure) of figure cue target cue) background) background cue

1 − + ✓ + +
2 − d/k ✓ d/k d/k
3 − + ✓ − + ✓
4 + d/k d/k d/k
5 + + + +
6 + d/k − d/k ✓
7 − d/k ✓ − + ✓
8 − d/k ✓ − d/k ✓
9 − + ✓ − d/k ✓
10 + + − d/k ✓
11 − + ✓ + d/k
12 + + + +
13 − d/k ✓ + +

Note. Payoff is indicated by a (+) sign, and no payoff is indicated by a (-) sign. The letters d/k indicate that
participants were unsure about the outcome of the card.
Psychol Rec

Table 4 Frequency distribution of blocking effects for the figure cue in were designed and implemented using the software LabView,
the timed and untimed group
2010 for Windows. The experiment included the same cards
Group Blocking of figure cue No blocking of figure cue Total described in Experiment 1. The size of the cards was adjusted
(2.5×3.5 cm) for this procedure so that all the cards could fit
Untimed 1 10 11 into one screen.
Timed 8 5 13
Total 9 15 24 Procedure
χ =6.99, p=.01.
2
As in Experiment 1, participants were informed that the study
examined some of the circumstances under which human
respectively. Based on this classification, participants were learning occurs. The following instructions were provided
asked to place the unclassified test cards into one of the boxes. on the computer screen:

This is a game of cards. You will notice that some cards


Experiment 2 produce a payoff and some cards don’t. Observe care-
fully. On the next screen you will see three boxes labeled
One of the most widely used procedures to study the acquisi- BPayoff,^ BNo Payoff,^ and BI Don’t Know.^ Your job
tion of contingency relations in humans is the discrete trial is to classify the six cards that appear on the top right
procedure. Even though measures are usually taken to ensure corner of the screen into one of the three boxes. You’ll
that the relevant relations are sufficiently trained, in human have to try to identify if the cards produce a pay off or
experiments there is a chance that factors such as attention, not. Using the mouse, drag each card into one of the
motivation, or habituation to the conditions of the task inter- boxes. When you are ready to begin, click on the button
fere with the acquisition of a novel repertoire of arbitrary labeled BStart^ at the bottom of the screen.
relations. By testing an alternative procedure we examined if
differences in blocking were observed when competitor–out- There was no time limit to complete the sorting task. Three
come and target–outcome associations were simultaneously boxes were presented on the computer screen. The box labeled
available than when presented across a limited sequence of BPayoff^ contained the cards AN, AO, BP, MX, and HX.
discrete trials. These were the same cards shown as producing a payoff dur-
ing Training Phases 1 and 2 in Experiment 1. The cards inside
Method the BNo Payoff^ box were EN, DQ, and MF. These were the
same cards that resulted in no payoff in Experiment 1
Participants (see Table 1). The cards classified in these two boxes could
not be moved. The cards used for the test phase in Experiment
Ten undergraduate students between ages 18 and 30 years 1 (MO, HN, BN, MP, IX, AR) were presented outside the
volunteered to participate in this study. All participants re- boxes and on the top right side of the screen. These cards
ceived bonus points in one of their course assignments upon could be placed anywhere on the screen using the computer
completion of the experimental task. mouse. A button labeled BDone^ on the top center of the
screen ended the experiment once the participant had reached
Apparatus and Stimuli a final classification of all test cards.

The experimental task was presented on IBM-compatible Results


computers with 17-in. screens. All phases of the experiment
Table 6 shows classifications of target and control cues ac-
cording to the attributed outcome of the cards. Nine of 10
Table 5 Frequency distribution of blocking effects for the background
cue in the timed and untimed group
participants classified the MO (target) and the MP (control)
card in the Payoff box. Hence, as in the untimed group
Group Blocking of No blocking of Total (Experiment 1), blocking effects were not observed for the
background cue background cue figure target cue in any except one participant. By contrast,
Untimed 3 8 11 only two participants classified the background target cue (H)
Timed 6 7 13
in the Payoff box. One participant classified it in the Don’t
Total 9 15 24
Know box and the seven remaining participants classified it in
the No Payoff box. Differences in responding to target figure
χ2 =0.90, p=.42. and target background cues were statistically significant
Psychol Rec

Table 6 Blocking effect of target


and background cues after Ppts O (figure P (control Blocking of H (background B (control Blocking of
exposure to the sorting task target cue) figure) figure cue target cue) background) background cue

1 + + − −
2 + + − −
3 + + d/k −
4 + + − + ✓
5 + + − −
6 + + − d/k ✓
7 + + + −
8 + + + +
9 + + − −
10 − d/k ✓ − + ✓

Note. Classification in the Payoff box is indicated by a (+) sign, and classification in the No Payoff box is
indicated by a (-) sign. The letters d/k indicate that participants classified the card in the Don’t Know box.

(McNemar p=.01). However, because most participants also According to this interpretation, responding when there is
attributed a negative outcome to the control background cue time to think (even Don’t Know or uncommitted responses)
(B) and a positive outcome to the control figure cue, blocking can be explained in terms of the evaluation of the logical
effects were confirmed only in 3 of 10 participants possibilities implied in the contingencies, rather than in terms
(see Table 6). No significant differences were found between of the strength of the associations.
blocking of target and background cues (McNemar p=.50).
Overall, most participants attributed a positive outcome to
figure cues (target and control), and a negative outcome to General Discussion
background cues (target and control).
In this procedure, competitor–outcome associations AN+, The purpose of this study was to examine if blocking is atten-
AO+ (for background cues) and MX+, HX+ (for target com- uated by deliberate reasoning in a causal learning task. In
petitor cues) were apparent as these cards were presented in- order to do this, causal judgments of target and control cues
side the Payoff box along with their corresponding target cue in participants who had three seconds to respond to the
cards (MO and HN). As in Experiment 1, Experiment 2 ex- blocking test, and in participants who had unlimited time to
amined if participants attributed a positive outcome to novel respond, were compared. Results show notable differences
compounds AR and IX, which included the competitor cues A between the two groups.
and X. Seven participants classified the novel card AR In contrast to Glautier’s (2002) findings, nearly no cases of
(A being the background competitor of the figure target cue) blocking for both target cues (figure and background) in the
in the No Payoff box, and three classified it in the Don’t Know untimed group were observed. Most participants in this group
box. By contrast, all participants classified the IX card (figure associated both cues as well as the control stimuli with a
competitor of background target cue) in the Payoff box. payoff. In contrast, blocking was observed for two thirds of
the participants in the timed group. These results suggest that
Discussion human behavior can be described by the same set of condi-
tioning principles that govern animal behavior, but only when
This procedure produced very weak evidence of blocking. logical reasoning is impeded or highly limited.
There was a notable difference however between responses Most uncommitted or Bdon’t know^ responses correspond
to figure and background cues. In general, while all figure to predictions of the outcome of control cues, and the majority
cues (target, controls, and competitors) seemed to predict a of these responses were given by participants in the timed
payoff, background cues tended to predict no payoff. Hence, group. This may indicate that participants responded separate-
with this procedure, participants seemed to produce a judg- ly to the stimuli in the compound (as they did for the
ment of outcome based on the figure cue and not on the back- competitor-target compound), even though they were present-
ground cue. ed as parts of a single stimulus object. In contrast to target and
Because figure cues were perceptually more salient than competitor cues, neither of the cues in the control card had
background cues, participants may have generalized the infor- been previously associated with the outcome. As a result,
mation from the cards where the figure cue produced an out- participants did not have enough information on which to base
come and derived a rule that was applicable to all cards. a causal judgment with respect to the elements of the control
Psychol Rec

compound card. The same occurred for the evaluation of com- To summarize, results showed greater evidence of
petitor cues with a novel figure or background. A high per- blocking for participants exposed to the procedure in which
centage of Bdon’t know^ responses further suggests elemental cognitive reasoning about the task was impeded. This can be
responding with respect to each of the stimulus in the card. explained in terms of the operation of rule-based processes,
These findings show that despite the fact that target and which may in some cases compete with the control exerted by
competitor cues were presented as part of a single stimulus, the contingencies arranged in the experimental task. Thus,
participants responded elementally with respect to each of the when logical reasoning is taken out of the equation, principles
cues in the compound. In contrast to most views, though, we of contingency learning seem sufficient to describe human
suggest that responding either configurally or elementally behavior in causal learning tasks.
does not necessarily facilitate blocking. What does seem to
account for blocking effects in experimental preparations de-
signed to produce elemental responding (i.e., Beckers et al.,
2005; De Houwer et al., 2005), is that in these procedures, the References
arranged contingencies are such that the null contribution of
the target cue is specified. Beckers, T., De Houwer, J., Pineño, O., & Miller, R. R. (2005). Outcome
In order to rule out if Bdon’t know^ responses were due to additivity and outcome maximality influence cue competition in
human causal learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
insufficient exposure to training trials, we examined responses
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 238–249. doi:10.1037/
to the test cards using a sorting-task procedure. By contrast to 0278-7393.31.2.238.
the traditionally used procedures, in this task, information re- Beckers, T., Miller, R. R., De Houwer, J., & Urushihara, K. (2006).
garding the outcomes of the cards was not presented on a trial- Reasoning rats: Forward blocking in Pavlovian animal conditioning
is sensitive to constraints of causal inference. Journal of Experimental
by-trial basis. In this procedure, the cards were classified into
Psychology: General, 135, 92–102. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.92.
two boxes according to their outcome. Participants in this Catania, C. (2013). Learning (5th ed.). Cornwall-on-Hudson, NY: Sloan.
group responded differently than those in the former two De Houwer, J., & Beckers, T. (2003). Secondary task difficulty modulates
groups (Experiment 1). Very few cases of blocking were ob- forward blocking in human contingency learning. Quarterly Journal
served for both target cues (figure and background). Most of Experimental Psychology, 56B, 345–357.
De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., & Glautier, S. (2002). Outcome and cue
participants attributed payoff outcomes to figure cues, and properties modulate blocking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
no payoff outcomes to background cues. Additionally, in con- Psychology, 55A, 965–985.
trast with the timed and untimed groups, very few cases of De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., & Vandorpe, S. (2005). Evidence for the role
uncommitted responses to the cards were observed. of higher-order reasoning processes in cue competition and other
learning phenomena. Learning & Behavior, 33, 239–249.
Most likely, this experimental arrangement is associated Delgado, D. (2013). Magnitude and valence of outcomes as determinants
with a higher likelihood of self-generated rules with respect of causal judgments. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 45(1),
to the task. Seemingly, these rules produce behavioral out- 9–20. doi:10.14349/rlp.v45i1.1307.
comes different from those produced by the temporal associ- Delgado, D., & Medina, I. F. (2013). Cuando la contigüidad no es
suficiente: Bloqueo en relaciones de equivalencia. Universitas
ations involved in discrete trial contingencies. It would be
Psychologica, 12(2), 613–626.
interesting to examine if limiting the time to sort the test cards Delgado, D., Medina, I. F., & Soto, J. S. (2011). El lenguaje como
would result in a different pattern of responding as in mediador de la transferencia de funciones: Es necesario nominar
Experiment 1. Further research could focus on the comparison para relacionar? Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 37,
31–52. doi:10.5514/rmac.v37.i2.26138.
between the sorting task and the discrete trial procedure. On
García-Retamero, R., & Dieckmann, A. (2006). Una visión crítica del
the other hand, it is possible that the inclusion of two blocking enfoque de los heurísticos rápidos y frugales. Revista
tests (one for a figure cue and one for a background cue) may Latinoamericana de Psicología, 38(3), 509–522.
have increased the overall difficulty of the task. Future repli- Glautier, S. (2002). Spatial separation of target and competitor cues en-
cations of present findings should be conducted using one hances blocking of human causality judgments. The Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55B, 121–135.
blocking procedure only. Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping
This study provides empirical evidence contributing to the bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720.
discussion of whether or not the principles of classical condi- Karazinov, D. M., & Boakes, R. A. (2007). Second order conditioning in
tioning that have been established in the animal laboratory, the human predictive judgments when there is little time to think.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 448–460. doi:
blocking effect in particular, are also descriptive of human 10.1080/17470210601002488.
behavior under similar circumstances of contingency control. Livesey, E. J., & Boakes, R. A. (2004). Outcome additivity, elemental
The effect of verbal processes on nonverbal contingency con- processing and blocking in human causality judgments. The
trol procedures is without doubt crucial in determining the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57B(4), 361–379.
Lovibond, P. F., Been, S. L., Mitchell, C. J., Bouton, M. E., & Frohardt, R.
generality of behavioral processes and outcomes and, in par- (2003). Forward and backward blocking of causal judgment is en-
ticular, in furthering our understanding of human behavior in hanced by additivity of effect magnitude. Memory & Cognition, 31,
light of such processes. 133–142. doi:10.3758/BF03196088.
Psychol Rec

Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning.
associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.
82, 276–298. Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Précis of simple heuristics that
Melchers, K. G., Lachnit, H., Üngör, M., & Shanks, D. R. (2005). Prior make us smart. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 727–780.
experience can influence whether the whole is different from the Tonneau, F., & González, C. (2004). Function transfer in human operant
sum of its parts. Learning and Motivation, 36, 20–41. doi:10. experiments: The role of stimulus pairings. Journal of the
1016/j.lmot.2004.06.002. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 239–255. doi:10.1901/
Melchers, K. G., Shanks, D. R., & Lachnit, H. (2008). Stimulus coding in jeab.2006.49-05.
human associative learning: Flexible representations of parts and Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive rea-
wholes. Behavioural Processes, 77, 413–427. soning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment.
Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). Causal cognition in human and Psychological Review, 90(4), 293–315. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.
non-human animals: A comparative, critical review. Annual Review 90.4.293.
of Psychology, 58, 97–118. doi:10.1146/annurev.pch.58.110405. Vandorpe, S., & De Houwer, J. (2006). People want to see information
085555. that will help them make valid inferences in human causal learning.
Rescorla, R. A., & Colwill, R. M. (1983). Within compound associations Memory & Cognition, 34, 1133–1139. doi:10.3758/BF03193259.
in unblocking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Vandorpe, S., De Houwer, J., & Beckers, T. (2005). Further evidence for
Behavior Processes, 9(4), 390–400. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.9.4.
the role of inferential reasoning in forward blocking. Memory &
390.
Cognition, 33, 1047–1056.
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian condi-
tioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and Waldmann, M. R., & Walker, J. M. (2005). Competence and performance
nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), in causal learning. Learning & Behavior, 33, 211–229.
Classical conditioning II: current research and theory (pp. 64– Walther, E., & Nagengast, B. (2006). Evaluative conditioning and the
99). New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts. awareness issue: Assessing contingency awareness with the four-
Shanks, D. R. (2010). Learning: From association to cognition. Annual picture recognition test. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Review of Psychology, 61, 273–301. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych. Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 454–459.
093008.100519.

You might also like