Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2017, 50, 789–804 NUMBER 4 (FALL)

TEACHING CONVERSATIONAL SPEECH TO CHILDREN WITH


AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER USING TEXT-MESSAGE PROMPTING
DENISE GROSBERG
CLAREMONT GRADUATE UNIVERSITY

AND

MARJORIE H. CHARLOP
CLAREMONT MCKENNA COLLEGE

The present study was designed to teach conversational speech using text-message prompts to
children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in home play settings with siblings and peers. A
multiple baseline design across children was used. Children learned conversational speech
through the text-message prompts, and the behavior generalized across peers and settings. Main-
tenance of treatment gains was seen at 1-month follow-up probes. Social validity measures indi-
cated that parents of typically developing children viewed the participants’ conversational speech
as much improved after the intervention. Results are discussed in terms of the efficacy of text-
message prompts as a promising way to improve conversational speech for children with ASD.
Key words: autism spectrum disorder, conversational speech, scripts, technology-based
treatment

Children with autism spectrum disorder individuals to learn verbal (i.e., initiations and
(ASD) tend to exhibit deficits in conversational responses) and nonverbal (e.g., approaching
speech that limit their interaction with others in others) social behavior (McClannahan &
their social environment (Diagnostic and Statisti- Krantz, 2005). Studies have demonstrated the
cal Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; DSM-5; effectiveness of scripts on the emergence of
American Psychiatric Association, 2013). They unscripted verbalizations, phrases that differ
generally do not ask questions or respond to from those specifically written in the scripts
comments or questions from others (Schuler & (Brown, Krantz, McClannahan, & Poulson,
Wolfberg, 2000). These speech deficits persist 2008; Reagon & Higbee, 2009; Sarokoff, Tay-
over time and may become more severe as the lor, & Poulson, 2001). Studies have also shown
children get older (Krasny, Williams, Proven- that conversational statements generalize to
cal, & Ozonoff, 2003). new stimuli (i.e., toys and games), conversa-
One avenue of research that has been partic- tional partners, and settings (e.g., Charlop, Gil-
ularly fruitful in teaching conversational skills more, & Chang, 2008; Charlop-Christy &
to children with ASD has been the use of Kelso, 2003; Sarokoff et al., 2001).
scripts (Krantz & McClannahan, 1993). Scripts Researchers have begun to extend scripts to
prompt individuals with ASD to engage in eve- more naturalistic use. Studies have now
ryday conversations that are important for included parents as trainers and used scripts in
social interaction (Wong et al., 2014). Addi- play groups. Reagon and Higbee (2009)
tionally, they provide an opportunity for demonstrated the effectiveness of training
mothers to use scripts to increase verbal initia-
Correspondence concerning this article should be tions of their children with ASD during play
addressed to Denise Grosberg, Division of Behavioral and activities. Additionally, Erickson-Tomaino
Organizational Sciences, Claremont Graduate University.
E-mail: dbgrosberg@gmail.com. (2011) developed a collaborative (between par-
doi: 10.1002/jaba.403 ent and therapist) parent-training model for
© 2017 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
789
790 DENISE GROSBERG and MARJORIE H. CHARLOP

script development to teach conversation skills situation, faded, and maintained may enable
to children with ASD. Most recently, them to be used by various change agents
Ledbetter-Cho et al. (2015) used a script-fading (therapists, teachers, parents, playmates).
pro[cedure for children with ASD during a play Finally, studies have shown that electronic
group with typically developing peers. screen usage is frequent with children with ASD
Scripts are especially useful because they can (e.g., Mazurek et al., 2012), suggesting that
be adapted for use on an electronic device, these children are likely to interact with this
which may be as or more socially acceptable type of intervention. The present study was
outside of clinical settings than paper-based designed to assess: (1) the efficacy of using text-
scripts (Ramdoss et al., 2010; Raulston et al., message prompts to teach conversational speech
2013). A growing number of studies have to children with ASD; (2) conversational speech
investigated the use of technological applica- during play in the home with typically develop-
tions, which may be more socially acceptable ing siblings or a same-aged peer; (3) generaliza-
learning tools due to their portability and tion of conversational speech across untrained
appeal to both children with ASD and typically community settings with peers and untrained
developing peers (Ayres, Mechling & Sansosti, community settings with unfamiliar peers; and
2013; Macpherson, Charlop, & Miltenberger, (4) the social validity of behavior change.
2015). For example, a nationally representative
sample found that 64% of children with ASD
METHOD
spend more time engaging with electronic
screen media compared to children with other Participants
developmental delays (Mazurek, Shattuck, Six children with ASD between the ages of
Wagner, & Cooper, 2012). Portable devices 7 and 11 participated (see Table 1) in the cur-
may enhance social validity, and technology rent study. All participants were diagnosed by a
such as cell phones, iPads, or laptops may licensed medical or other clinical professional
serve as. Common stimuli that facilitate gener- and met the criteria for ASD as defined in the
alization (Stokes & Baer, 1977). DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association,
One form of electronic screen media is short- 2013). Participants attended a university-based,
messaging communication, or texting. Texting applied behavior analysis (ABA) intervention
is especially popular among preteens, teenagers, program and participated in general behavioral
and young adults (Ling & Baron 2007; Roberts, programming to improve language, communi-
Foehr, & Rideout, 2010). In a large American cation, play, and social skills. Criteria for partici-
sample, 46% of 8- to 18-year-olds reported that pation also included generalized verbal imitation
they typically send text messages daily, and skills and use of phrases of at least three intelligi-
those who text estimated that they devote about ble words for the purpose of engaging in a con-
1.5 hr daily to sending and receiving text mes- versation. Verbal skills were assessed through
sages (Roberts et al., 2010). Delivering scripts two behavioral observations prior to baseline. In
via text messaging may be advantageous. First, addition, the participants needed to read and
cell phones are common in the majority of set- understand simple sentences of at least three
tings in the United States today. Second, scripts words in length to be included in this study.
can be easily texted on a cell phone, which is a Twelve typically developing siblings or peers
socially acceptable device and pervasive part of (aged 4-9) served as conversational partners in the
present-day lives for children, teens, and adults. baseline, testing, and generalization phases of the
Third, the ease with which texts can be typed, study. Typically developing siblings (no more
sent from afar, modified to an ongoing than 6 years older or younger than the child)
TEXT-MESSAGE PROMPTING 791

Table 1
Characteristics of Each Participating Child with ASD

Reading Typically
Chrono- Mean WJ-III Assess- Developing Sibling/ Typically
logical Length VABS Subtest Age ment Peers Age (Year- Developing Peers
Age (Year- Utter- (Year- Equiva-lent Word Month) (Baseline & Age (Year- Month)
Child Ethnicity Month) ance Month) (Year-Month) Length Testing) (General-ization)
Jake Asian 10-3 5 7-4 10-2 6 4-2 6-3
Larry Hispanic- 7-3 4 5-3 6-5 6 8-2 8-10
Caucasian
Landon Caucasian 7-1 4 6-1 6-7 6 6-2* 6-8
Rory Asian 6-1 4 5-2 4-1 3 8-1 7-9
Julie Hispanic 6-2 5 5-6 6-5 6 8-1 8-3
Aaron Indian 6-4 4 5-5 6-0 6 7-7 7-1

Note. * indicates peer conversational partner.

served as conversational partners for participants Verbal behavior observations: Children


who had an eligible sibling. Landon did not have with ASD. The experimenter observed partici-
an eligible sibling. Therefore, a typically develop- pants for two 5-min sessions in the playroom
ing peer who often played with Landon at the of the behavioral treatment center with a typi-
university-based ABA intervention program cally developing conversational partner and
served as his conversational partner. assessed mean length utterance (MLU). MLU
was used to determine if the participants met
the intake language requirement and was used
Assessment of Child Functioning and Verbal to determine the length of text messages for the
Behavior Skills intervention (i.e., children with longer MLUs
Several measures were included as preassess- received longer texts). Vocalizations did not
ments for the participants and their typically need to be directed to another child or stated
developing conversational partners. Assessments with correct grammar, but needed to include at
for the participants included: (1) verbal behav- least three intelligible words. For example, a
ior observations; (2) the Vineland Adaptive child who repeated the syllables “buh buh buh”
Behavior Scales (VABS; Sparrow, Balla, & Cic- would not qualify for participation in the pres-
chetti, 1984); (3) the Letter-Word Identifica- ent study, but a child who said “I want train”
tion and Passage Comprehension subtests of would qualify.
the Woodcock Johnson-III (WWJ-III; Wood- Verbal behavior observations: Typically devel-
cock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001); and (4) a oping conversational partners. Verbal behavior
reading assessment created for this study. These observations of the typically developing conver-
assessments were used to determine eligibility sational partners were conducted to assess how
for inclusion in the study. Eligibility criteria much children, in general, converse with each
included verbal ability of at least three intelligi- other during play activities. Five 5-min play
ble words, communication, social, and adaptive sessions between similar-aged, typically devel-
skills of at least a 36-month-old level, and basic oping siblings of the participants were recorded
reading skills of sentences of at least three to assess the number of conversational
words in length. These assessments were admi- exchanges during play (i.e., board games, pre-
nistered prior to the intervention and are dis- tend play, figurine play). An independent
cussed below. observer scored the videos by averaging the
792 DENISE GROSBERG and MARJORIE H. CHARLOP

number of conversational exchanges made by Only children with ASD who could read at
each peer in the dyad. The overall average least 2 out of the 10 sentences were eligible for
number of conversational exchanges was used participation in the present study. This assess-
as the mastery conversational exchange rate cri- ment was used to determine the length (three
terion for study participants in the present words to six words) of the texts to later be used.
study. On average, the typically developing The participant who read fewer sentences dur-
dyads engaged in eight conversational ing the reading assessment had difficulty read-
exchanges per child. ing phrases beyond three words in length.
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS). Therefore, the texts used for that participant
The VABS is a standardized, semistructured (Rory) consisted of three-word phrases only.
interview used to assess adaptive behavior in
four domains: motor skills, socialization, com- Materials
munication, and daily-living skills. The sociali- The toys and games selected were familiar to
zation and communication domains were the participant so that no additional training
administered in the present study with parents was needed to teach the participant how to
or caregivers familiar with the behavior of the read, play, or understand the activities selected.
individual child. Only children who demon- The parents of the children participating in the
strated adaptive behavior functioning above a study provided cell phones with text-messaging
36-month-old level were eligible for participa- capabilities. A camera was used to record videos
tion in the study. of all phases of the study.
Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities,
Third Edition (WJ-III): Letter–word identifica-
tion and passage comprehension subtests. The Settings
letter–word identification and passage compre- Assessment of child functioning, verbal
hension subtests of the WJ-III were used to behavior skills, and reading assessments were
assess literacy ability for the present study. conducted in a clinic that the participants
Only children that demonstrated letter–word attended regularly. The room contained a small
identification and passage comprehension skills table, two chairs, and several shelves with a
above a 36-month-old level were eligible for variety of toys and teaching materials placed in
participation in the study. containers. All experimental sessions (baseline,
Reading assessment. This measure was devel- training, testing) took place at the home of the
oped by the research team to evaluate the read- participant, typically in a playroom or family
ing skills of participants with items similar to room that included a variety of toys, books,
what would be used for the prompts. The and games based on the participant’s age, func-
measure consisted of 10 play/social phrases, tioning level, and interests. Generalization
including three questions (e.g., “Want to play probes were conducted at an indoor commu-
blocks?”), three comments (e.g., “I like your nity recreation center.
train!”), and four responses (e.g., “Yes, I’ll play
Candyland”) displayed on a cell phone. These Data Collection and Dependent Measures
phrases ranged from three to six words in The dependent measures included the num-
length. Although some words were difficult ber of scripted and unscripted conversational
(e.g., “Candyland”), prior observations and par- phrases for the testing, generalization, and
ent report indicated that the children could follow-up sessions. The primary researcher
read such familiar proper nouns. However, the scored all videotaped sessions, and interobserver
assessment did not control for the reading level. agreement was assessed by comparing a second
TEXT-MESSAGE PROMPTING 793

coder’s scores for 33% of sessions for each con- a text but differed from the text by more than
dition. Coders manually recorded the number conjunctions, articles, prepositions, or pro-
of conversational phrases made by the children. nouns, it was scored as an unscripted, conversa-
A conversational phrase consisted of any con- tional phrase. For example, if the text was, “I
textually appropriate speech including initia- like Candyland,” and the participant said, “I
tions, responses, or comments about the play want to play Candyland,” this was scored as an
items or play activities. Conversational phrases unscripted conversational phrase.
needed to be pertinent to the situation or an
ongoing conversation. For example, if a partici-
pant said, “I like playing with you,” the state- Procedure
ment was coded as a conversational phrase Pre-experiment assessment. All of the partici-
because it was contextually appropriate. If a pants were presented with the assessment mea-
participant said, “I don’t like blue,” and he/she sures discussed above and met eligibility criteria
were playing with various colored blocks, the for the study. The participants’ scores on these
statement was coded as a conversational phrase. assessments can be seen in Table 1.
However, if the participant said, “Turkey hot Baseline. A concurrent, multiple-baseline
dogs!” the statement was not coded at all. If the design across participant was used to assess the
participant repeated the same appropriate number of conversational phrases (questions,
phrase in a row, the phrase was coded only comments, and responses) acquired using the
once. If the participant repeated the same text-message prompt. During baseline, the par-
phrase more than twice, but not necessarily in ticipant and the typically developing conversa-
a row, in the same conversation, the phrase was tional partner were seated across from each
coded twice and then not scored again as cor- other on the floor in the home playroom
rect. In addition, the experimenter kept a rec- (approximately 1 m apart). A variety of toys,
ord of the content of the text messages books, and games were placed on the floor to
delivered to the participants during the training promote conversations between the participant
and testing phases of the study. and the conversational partner. The cell phone
Scripted conversational phrases. Scripted con- was placed on the floor approximately 0.5 m
versational phrases were those that matched the from the participant’s dominant hand. The cell
text messages presented in the relevant training phone’s password was on so the participant
session. Conversational phrases that differed could not use the phone to download a game.
only in the conjunctions, articles, prepositions, If the participant were to pick up the cell
or pronouns were also scored as scripted phone, the participant would be instructed to
phrases (Brown et al., 2008). For example, if place the cell phone down and play with
the text message read, “That game is fun,” and his/her friend. However, this did not happen
the participant said, “That game was fun,” it during the study. Before each baseline session,
would be scored as a scripted phrase. Phrases the experimenter said to the participant,
that differed in any other manner from the pre- “[Child’s Name], it’s time to talk with ____.”
sented text messages were not scored as scripted Data were collected during baseline on partici-
phrases. pants’ frequency of conversational exchanges
Unscripted conversational phrases. Unscripted over a 5-min period. The experimenter pro-
conversational phrases were those that were vided no additional instructions, feedback, or
appropriate and did not match the text mes- prompts. The only instruction provided to the
sages. If a participant constructed a conversa- sibling/peer was that he/she should verbally
tional phrase that was similar to the content of respond with a question or comment that
794 DENISE GROSBERG and MARJORIE H. CHARLOP

makes sense to the situation (i.e., “Say, ‘Yes I’ll for unexpected incoming texts was for the
play trains with you’”; “Say, ‘I like your tower experimenter to merely say to the participant,
too!’”) if the participant spoke to him or her. “That is not your line,” and show the partici-
The sibling or peer could initiate conversation pant that the experimenter’s name is not where
or spontaneously engage the participant, but it typically should be. However, no unexpected
was not explicitly instructed to do so by the texts came in.
experimenter. If the participant did not initially respond to
Text-message-prompt training. Training con- the verbal instruction, the experimenter
sisted of teaching the participant to manipulate repeated the verbal instruction and added a par-
the cell phone, receive the texts, and then to tial physical prompt by placing the participant’s
say the content of the text message during con- hand on the cell phone. All participants in the
versations. This training was conducted twice study were familiar with the function of the
per week at the participant’s home. The experi- text-retrieval button on their mother’s phone
menter conducted two or three 5-min training and did not require explicit instructions to
sessions over a 2-hr period. Breaks were pro- complete this step. The experimenter practiced
vided between each session. During breaks, the this step with the participant in this way until
participant was not involved in any activities the participant pressed the button and read the
related to the present study but was allowed to text message to the experimenter without
eat lunch/snack, play outside, watch TV/iPad, prompting, across two consecutive training ses-
play in his/her room, or take a walk with a par- sions. Two to three training sessions were nec-
ent. The participant did not interact with the essary for each participant to learn how to use
experimenter. The experimenter also used this the phone to read the text messages. The exper-
time to discuss general progress in the imenter then brought in a familiar adult con-
university-based ABA program with the parent. versational partner, usually a therapist, and
Because the participants did not have prior removed herself from the immediate area
experience with texting, they were taught how (approximately 2 m from the participant). The
to access and read texts presented on a cell conversational partner was instructed that
phone. The cell phone for each participant he/she should verbally respond with a question
belonged to the mother, and thus varied in or comment that makes sense to the situation if
model across participants. The participant was the participant spoke to him or her. The adult
seated with the experimenter, playing with pre- conversational partner also was also told not to
ferred toys and games. As in baseline, the cell verbally initiate conversation with the partici-
phone was placed directly next to the partici- pant to provide the participant the opportunity
pant at the start of each session. The experi- to practice this skill. The participant and adult
menter discreetly sent a text message consisting conversational partner were seated across from
of a comment or question about the play activ- each other on the floor in the home playroom
ity to the cell phone. When the cell phone (approximately 0.5 m apart). A variety of toys,
indicated that a text message had been received, books, and games were placed on the floor to
the experimenter verbally instructed the partici- promote conversations between the participant
pant to push the text-retrieval button and read and the conversational partner. The cell phone
the text message to the experimenter. Mothers was placed on the floor approximately 0.25 m
informed the fathers not to text during treat- from the participant’s dominant hand. The
ment sessions. They were also asked to tempo- experimenter told the participant, “[Child’s
rarily block their “favorites” on their phones Name], it’s time to play and talk with
(most phones had this capability). The protocol [Adult’s Name].”
TEXT-MESSAGE PROMPTING 795

During play, the experimenter sent eight eight conversational phrases within two consec-
texts to the participant (consisting of an appro- utive training sessions, the cell phone was
priate social initiation question, comment, or removed and the participant was tested for con-
statement), which the participant read out loud versational skills with his/her typically develop-
to the adult conversational partner. Each play ing sibling or peer.
session started with a verbal-initiation text sent Testing. During testing sessions, the cell
to the participant. After the conversational part- phone was removed from the play setting and
ner’s comment or question, the participant’s sessions followed the same procedure as baseline
phone beeped again with a text message from described above with the sibling or peer. As in
the experimenter with a contextually appropri- baseline, the experimenter said, “[Child’s
ate conversational phrase for the participant to Name], it’s time to talk to [Peer’s Name].” The
read to the partner. A time delay of approxi- sibling or peer was reminded at the beginning of
mately 10 s was embedded during training. each testing session to respond to the participant
This delay consisted of the time it took the as in baseline. Two 5-min testing sessions were
experimenter to text a contextually appropriate conducted, and the criterion of eight conversa-
response to the participant, following an adult tional phrases across these two consecutive ses-
conversational partners’ statement.. Also, this sions was required before generalization was
delay allowed for play with toys or games to assessed. If the participant did not meet this cri-
occur. The adult conversational partner would terion during testing sessions, training was rein-
have verbally prompted (e.g., “Look at your troduced with the adult conversational partner,
phone,” or, “Read the message”) the participant using the cell phone in the same way as
to read the text if no response was made, but described during training. Once the participant
this did not occur. After each 5-min play ses- met criterion with the adult conversational part-
sion, the participant was given praise for gen- ner and cell phone, testing was again conducted.
eral behavior, play, and attention (e.g., “Great If the participant still did not meet the mastery
job sitting so nicely!”). criterion, a script-fading procedure was intro-
The text-message prompts continued until duced with the adult conversational partner.
the participant provided at least eight conversa- Script fading. Script-fading procedures were
tional phrases (prompted or unprompted) to modeled after McClannahan and Krantz’s
the adult conversational partner across two con- (2005) guidelines and consisted of delivering a
secutive 5-min training sessions. Phrases could full sentence, then delivering a sentence with-
include either reading the text-message prompt out the last word, then delivering a sentence
aloud or using independent conversational without the last two words, and so on. Because
phrases. If the participant initiated or each sentence was varied, the script fading
responded appropriately and independently to could consist of the following: “I like your
his/her adult conversational partner, text mes- ball,” then “that’s cool,” then “give me,” then
sages were not sent to the participant’s cell “I,” and, finally, no words, with the phone
phone by the experimenter. However, if the removed completely. Script fading was only
participant did not initiate or respond inde- needed for one participant (Jake).
pendently or appropriately to his/her adult con- Generalization probes. Five-min generaliza-
versational partner within 10 s of the adult’s tion probes were conducted throughout base-
response, the experimenter texted the partici- line and once the participant reached criterion
pant with a contextually appropriate initiation without the cell phone with the sibling or peer.
or response to keep the conversation going. All generalization sessions were conducted at an
When the participant met criterion of saying indoor community recreation center with either
796 DENISE GROSBERG and MARJORIE H. CHARLOP

the sibling or peer who participated in the test- conversational speech during baseline for the six
ing phase or a peer not associated with sessions children ranged from 85%-100%, during text-
in the home environment. Unfamiliar peers message training ranged from 88%-100%, and
included siblings of other children enrolled in during testing ranged from 92%-98%. Further-
the university ABA intervention program. The more, interobserver reliability during generaliza-
community recreation center consisted of a tion and follow-up probes ranged from
series of small and large dining tables on the 89%-97%.
right side of the center, two empty pool tables Social validity. Twenty mothers of school-
on the left side, and a small open area in the aged children unfamiliar with the participants
middle. Generalization sessions took place at and objectives of the study watched 24 video-
the dining tables, but children had access to taped clips of the participants’ sessions. These
other areas of the community center. None of clips were blocked in sets of two 5-min baseline
the participants had been to the recreation cen- and two 5-min testing sessions, randomly
ter prior to the study. Like baseline, the partici- selected from each of the six children’s sessions
pant and the typically developing (totaling 120 min of video). In addition, base-
conversational partner were seated across from line and testing session clips were randomly
each other at a small table located in the corner presented within each block so that parents
of the recreation center. Only the peer conver- could not identify which clips were baseline or
sational partner, the participant, the experi- testing sessions. After each of the twenty-four
menter, and two observing therapists from the 5-min clips, the parents answered a variety of
ABA program were present. A variety of toys, questions regarding the participant’s speech and
books, and games familiar to the participant play behavior that were printed on paper.
were placed on the table to promote conversa- Examples of such questions included, “Does
tion between the peer conversational partner the child demonstrate an interest in having a
and the participant. The procedure for the gen- conversation with his/her peers?” and “Would
eralization sessions was identical to that of base- my child want to talk with this peer?” State-
line, except that the cell phone was not present. ments and questions were rated on a 1-7 Likert
Follow-up. Follow-up probes (using the same scale with 1 being strongly disagree, 4 being
procedures as testing and generalization ses- neutral, and 7 being strongly agree. Social
sions) were conducted with all six participants validity data were taken after generalization
1 month after the last testing session to assess probes were completed for all children. Social
whether the participants had maintained the validity questions can be seen in Table 3.
conversation skills learned during training.
Interobserver agreement. Thirty-three percent
RESULTS
of each of the baseline, training, testing, generali-
zation, and follow-up sessions were coded by a Child Training, Testing, and Follow-up Data
second observer. The primary and secondary Results from Larry, Aaron, and Jake are pre-
observers scored for the occurrence and nonoc- sented in Figure 1. Results for Julie, Landon,
currence of all conversational speech during each and Rory are presented in Figure 2. During
5-min session. Interobserver agreement was cal- baseline, all children demonstrated limited con-
culated by dividing the total number of agree- versational speech with a peer. By the end of the
ments between the two observers by the total study, all participants met criterion of at least
number of agreements plus disagreements and eight conversational phrases following training
then multiplying by 100 to get a percentage. sessions with the familiar adult conversational
Interobserver reliability of appropriate partner. In addition, all participants met testing
TEXT-MESSAGE PROMPTING 797

Figure 1. Baseline, training, testing, and follow-up data for Larry, Aaron, and Jake.*Script Completely Faded.

criterion of at least eight conversational phrases Data for Larry are shown in the upper
with a neurotypical sibling or peer when the text panel of Figure 1. Larry used few conversa-
message prompts were removed. Only one par- tional phrases (one to three phrases) during
ticipant (Jake) required script fading. Generali- baseline. After the first set of training sessions,
zation to untrained peers and settings was also Larry did not meet mastery criteria during
demonstrated for all participants. testing with his sibling. Therefore, text
798 DENISE GROSBERG and MARJORIE H. CHARLOP

Figure 2. Baseline, training, testing, and follow-up data for Julie, Landon, and Rory.

message prompt training was reintroduced, neurotypical sibling. Larry’s conversational


after which Larry exceeded the mastery crite- speech also generalized across a different set-
rion of eight conversational phrases by using ting and peer, and maintained at 1-month
9 and 11 phrases during testing with his follow-up.
TEXT-MESSAGE PROMPTING 799

Data for Aaron are shown in the middle Data for Julie are shown in the top panel of
panel of Figure 1. Aaron demonstrated minimal Figure 2. Julie exhibited between two to five
conversation skills (two to three phrases) during conversational phrases during baseline. How-
baseline. However, he only required three train- ever, she only required two training sessions
ing sessions with the text-message prompts to with the text-message prompts to exceed mas-
exceed mastery criterion for conversational tery criterion for conversational phrases (using
speech during testing (15-16 conversational 15-16 conversational phrases) during testing
phrases) with his sibling without the cell with her sibling without the cell phone. She
phone. Aaron also met the criterion for conver- also demonstrated generalization of conversa-
sational phrases during both generalization ses- tional speech and maintenance of the target
sions and maintained his conversation skills at behavior in both intervention and generaliza-
1-month follow-up. tion sessions at 1-month follow-up.
Data for Jake are shown in the lower panel Data for Landon are shown in the middle
of Figure 1. Jake engaged in the lowest number panel of Figure 2. Landon exhibited one to
of conversational phrases of the participants four conversational phrases during baseline ses-
during baseline (one or two phrases) with his sions. He also only required two training ses-
sibling. Although Jake asked and answered con- sions with the text message prompts to exceed
textually appropriate questions during training mastery criterion for conversational speech
with a familiar adult, he did not independently (using 15-16 conversational phrases) during
meet mastery criteria during testing with his testing with a familiar peer without the cell
phone. In addition, Landon’s conversational
sibling when the cell phone was removed. Dur-
skills generalized across setting and peer and
ing the first two testing phases, the number of
maintained above the mastery criterion at 1-
conversational phrases Jake used improved only
month follow-up.
minimally above baseline levels, ranging from
Data for Rory are shown in the bottom
two to three phrases. Therefore, script fading
panel of Figure 2. Rory engaged in three to
was implemented, during which time the texts
four phrases with his sibling during baseline
were gradually faded word by word until the
sessions. After three training sessions with text
cell phone was removed completely. During his message prompts, he met mastery criterion
third testing phase, Jake met mastery criteria, with his sibling without the cell phone, emit-
using nine conversational phrases for each 5- ting more instances of conversational speech
min testing opportunity with his sibling. This during testing than any of the other partici-
behavior also generalized across a new setting pants (14-18 conversational phrases). The
and conversational partner, with whom he
emitted 12 conversational phrases during the
generalization probe, but did not generalize at Table 2
the criterion level to a new setting with his sib- Percentage of Conversational Phrases Unscripted For
ling, with whom he emitted six conversational Each Phase
phrases during the generalization probe. Follow-
Although the target behavior did not maintain Participant. Training Testing Generalization Up
at criterion level during 1-month follow-up Larry 20% 88% 100% 100%
with his sibling in the intervention or generali- Aaron 25% 80% 100% 100%
Jake 0% 40% 75% 80%
zation setting, the behavior did maintain at Julie 20% 100% 100% 100%
high levels (11 phrases) across an unfamiliar Landon 40% 95% 100% 100%
Rory 50% 95% 100% 100%
peer and setting.
800 DENISE GROSBERG and MARJORIE H. CHARLOP

target behavior also generalized and maintained

2.3 (.5) 4.9 (.6)


1.9 (.6) 6.09 (.7)

2.36 (.5) 4.73 (.8)

2.45 (.5) 4.82 (.6)

2.18 (.8) 5.09 (.6)

4.8 (.8) 2.45 (.7) 5.55 (1.1) 2.36 (.5) 5.91 (.8) 2.45 (.5) 4.81 (.8)
5.1 (.7) 2.82 (.6) 5.55 (1.1) 2.73 (.5) 5.64 (1.1) 2.73 (.5) 5.09 (.7)
M (SD)
Post
at 1-month follow-up with data well above

Larry
baseline levels.

M (SD)
Pre
Scripted and Unscripted Conversational
Phrases

2.2 (.8) 5.9 (.9)


2.4 (.8) 6.27 (.8)

2.64 (.5) 5.55 (.7)

2.36 (.5) 5.27 (.8)

2.45 (.5) 5.54 (.8)


M (SD
Post
Table 2 shows the percentage of conversa-

Landon
tional phrases that were unscripted. Each par-
ticipant engaged in a higher percentage of

M (SD)
Pre
conversational phrases that were unscripted
during testing, generalization and maintenance

2.3 (.5) 5.2 (.4)


1.8 (.8) 6.18 (.8)

4.7 (.8) 2.36 (.5) 4.73 (.8)

4.8 (.6) 2.64 (.7) 4.91 (.9)

5.1 (.6) 2.18 (.8) 5.09 (.9)


M (SD
probes relative to training sessions. Only Jake

Post
used scripted phrases during any generalization

Aaron
and follow-up probes.

M (SD)
Pre
Social Validity of Child Outcomes

5.0 (.6)
Table 3 shows differences between ratings of

6.1 (.7)
M (SD
Post
participants’ pre- and posttreatment conversa-
Participant Social Validity Outcomes

Jake

tional behavior, which were compared through M (SD))


2.3 (.5)
2.0 (.6)

2.4 (.5)

2.4 (.5)

2.2 (.8)

2.4 (.5)
2.7 (.5)
paired samples t-tests on each of the seven
Pre

questionnaire items for each child. All children


were rated higher on every posttreatment item,

5.6 (1.1)
Table 3

M (SD)
6.0 (.9)
6.3 (.8)

5.5 (.7)

5.3 (.8)

5.5 (.8)

5.9 (.8)
Post

with these differences attaining statistical signif-


Rory

icance on six to seven of the seven items for the


M (SD)
2.3 (.8)
2.7 (.8)

2.6 (.5)

2.4 (.5)

2.4 (.5)

2.4 (.5)
2.7 (.5)
individual children. Higher scores indicate
Pre

responses that were more favorable. For five of


the six children (all except Julie), the difference 5.5 (1.1)
5.5 (1.1)
M (SD)
5.2 (.4)
6.2 (.8)

4.7 (.8)

4.9 (.9)

5.1 (.9)
Post

between pretreatment and posttreatment scores


was highest for the second item: “Does the
Julie

child demonstrate an interest in having a con-


M (SD)
2.3 (.5)
1.7 (.8)

2.4 (.5)

2.6 (.7)

2.2 (.8)

2.4 (.7)
2.8 (.6)
Pre

versation with his/her peers?”


2. Does the child demonstrate an interest in having a
1. Does the child spontaneously converse with peers?

3. Does the child make appropriate verbal initiations

5. Does the child look like he/she wants to talk with

7. Does this child socially interact with his/her peer?

DISCUSSION
4. Does the child make appropriate responses to

6. Would my child want to talk with this peer?

The results of this study suggest that the


text-message prompts increased conversational
speech for the children with ASD. Both
conversation with his/her peers?

scripted and, importantly, unscripted conversa-


tional speech increased with relatively few inter-
Social Validity Question

vention sessions for all six children in this


study. Generalization of conversational speech
his/her peers?

occurred to either a new setting or a new set-


to peers?

ting and conversational partner, and mainte-


peers?

nance of conversational speech at 1-month


follow-up was evident. Only one child (Jake)
TEXT-MESSAGE PROMPTING 801

required script fading, which was easily imple- were fluid, based on the present activities and
mented into the text-messaging protocol and the conversational partner’s speech, further
resulting in generalization and maintenance of incorporating variation in prompted speech.
conversational speech. The varied texts were sent to a cell phone
The improvement in conversational speech present in the play setting, with minimal dis-
seen in this study holds important implica- ruption in the setting. This is an important
tions for children with ASD (Charlop et al., departure from hard-copy scripts, which gen-
2008). The majority of verbal children with erally require alteration in physical stimuli
ASD typically have difficulty with reciprocal (e.g., paper or whiteboards, pencils or mar-
conversational speech. Instead, they frequently kers, erasers) and interruption in the conversa-
provide one-word answers to questions and tion and play.
do not ask questions of others During text-message prompting, the child
(e.g., Schreibman, 2005). The present merely looks at the phone to see the conversa-
research addresses this problem by using an tional phrases. As previously discussed, cell
intervention tool that models conversational phones are not stigmatizing, serve as common
phrases that consist of both questions and stimuli, and are perhaps motivating for the
responses. This type of structuring of conver- children (e.g., Ling & Helmersen, 2000;
sational phrases has been demonstrated in Mazurek et al., 2012). Although a bug-in-the-
prior literature to be germane in teaching the ear approach might be affordable, most families
natural flow of conversational speech do not have access to these materials, and many
(e.g., Charlop & Milstein, 1989; Charlop- children with ASD are hypersensitive to sound
Christy & Kelso, 2003; Sherer et al., 2001). or may be annoyed with a device in their ear.
The content analysis of the children’s con- By contrast, cell phones are apparently available
versational speech provided another promising almost everywhere in the natural environment
result of the study in that, over time, the and can be used for a number of different types
number of scripted phrases decreased, and the of treatment for children with ASD. Texting
number of unscripted phrases increased. This can be seen as a natural-setting phenomenon
is important given that for most children with popular with persons of all ages. Cell phones
autism, a stereotypic, robotic style of interac- can make learning for children with ASD
tion dominates their discourse skills (Charlop mobile, are unobtrusive across learning and
et al., 2008). Perseverative speech, preoccupa- therapeutic environments, and are often accessi-
tions or “obsessions” with certain topics ble to parents and service providers (Zickuhr &
(e.g., maps, capitals of states), and routine Smith, 2012). Specialized features of smart-
scripts tend to typify conversations phones (e.g., WiFi connectivity, built-in cam-
(Schreibman, 2005). In the present study, the era) also offer opportunities for more
scripts included response variation, which has innovative instructional interventions, includ-
been shown in past research to replace perse- ing expanding the scope of available communi-
verative speech with appropriate speech cation partners (e.g., a child can have a
(e.g., Charlop et al., 2008; Charlop & Mil- conversation with another peer of similar ability
stein, 1989; Ledbetter-Cho et al., 2015). and age level in another state or country) and
Additionally, the children in the present study providing immediate performance feedback
were texted conversational phrases in real (Rakes, Fields, & Cox, 2006).
time, which allowed the intervention to be Examining the social validity of conversa-
contextually relevant and appropriate to the tional speech as a target behavior is also impor-
play situation. The texted conversational lines tant. Parents of typically developing children
802 DENISE GROSBERG and MARJORIE H. CHARLOP

(not associated with this study) thought the skills for her age, struggling with turn taking,
conversational speech of children with ASD sharing materials, and engaging in a contextu-
was more socially appropriate with peers after ally appropriate conversation. However, despite
intervention than before. This suggests that the these concerns, all the children with ASD in
parents noticed an improvement in conversa- the present study learned from the text message
tional skills that coincided with the prompts.
empirical data. There are many areas to pursue for future
There are several limitations to the current research. First, parents or an older sibling could
study. The initial training occurred with a be the “texter” (e.g., Reagon & Higbee, 2009).
familiar adult as the conversational partner This has implications not only for streamlining
before the child then participated in the play the intervention by removing the teaching
sessions with his sibling or peer. This was done component with therapists, but also for allow-
to explore the feasibility of this new treatment. ing the intervention to be used more consist-
The next step in research would be to assess ently and across a broader range of social
whether incorporating the text-message prompt settings, increasing the likelihood of better
directly into play sessions with children with maintenance and generalization of conversa-
ASD and a peer would also be effective. The tional skills. Second, speech skills could be
reading assessment created for screening partici- taught in other community settings, as texts
pants in the study assessed phrases similar to could be sent to the child to prompt conversa-
the phrases that would be texted to the partici- tional speech at birthday parties, scout meet-
pants, but did not control for the reading level ings, and so on. Additionally, cell phone
of all potential participants, which could limit texting could be used for other target behaviors
its effectiveness in identifying target groups of in addition to conversational speech. Potential
children who could benefit from the applications of this technology could address
intervention. behavioral challenges associated with transitions
In the present study, siblings served as con- and changes in the child’s routine. For exam-
versational partners for the children with ASD ple, a child with ASD could receive texts at
because they had around-the-clock availability school to indicate changes to his or her sched-
and the parents preferred they serve as conver- ule or a text message reminder at the park a
sational partners. However, siblings may not few minutes before the parent has to leave as a
always make the best conversational partner proactive intervention method to ward off chal-
and it might behoove the researcher to have an lenging behaviors (Cihak, Fahrenkrog, Ayers, &
initial screening assessment to identify the Smith, 2010).
appropriateness of the sibling as a conversa- Given that research on mobile phones as
tional partner. In the present study, Jake’s sib- learning tools is still in its infancy, best prac-
ling was younger and had some difficulty when tices and standards for mobile phone learning
engaging in play activities, especially when the still need to be identified. Considerable
activity was nonpreferred. Jake’s sibling often research is required to uncover how to design
protested when Jake chose a game or activity learning materials for delivery on mobile
that was not of interest to him. Often when devices and what is the right mix of technol-
Jake spoke to his sibling, the sibling would not ogy for the most effective interventions. The
respond appropriately and sometimes would blend of technology and treatment, and in
not respond at all, even when verbally this specific case, texting and talking, may be
prompted by the experimenter. In addition, appropriate avenues of research in the field
Larry’s sibling demonstrated impaired play of ASD.
TEXT-MESSAGE PROMPTING 803

REFERENCES of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of


Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 785–799. doi:https://
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and doi.org/10.1002/jaba.240.
statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Ling, R., & Baron, N. S. (2007). Text messaging and IM:
Washington, DC: Author. Linguistic comparison of American college data. Jour-
Ayres, K. M., Mechling, L., & Sansosti, F. J. (2013). The nal of Language and Social Psychology, 26(3), 291–298.
use of mobile technologies to assist with life skills/ doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X06303480.
independence of students with moderate/severe intel- Ling, R., & Helmersen, P. (2000, July). “It must be neces-
lectual disability and/or autism spectrum disorders: sary, it has to cover a need”. The Adoption of Mobile
Considerations for the future of school psychology. Telephony Among Pre-Adolescents and Adolescents. Oslo,
Psychology in Schools, 50, 259–271. https://doi.org/ Norway: Conference on the Social Consequences of
10.1002/pits.21673. Mobile Telephony.
Brown, J. L., Krantz, P. J., McClannahan, L. E., & Macpherson, K., Charlop, M. H., & Miltenberger, C. A.
Poulson, C. L. (2008). Using script fading to pro- (2015). Using portable video modeling technology to
mote natural environment stimulus control of verbal increase the compliment behaviors of children with
interactions among youths with autism. Research in autism during athletic group play. Journal of Autism
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 480–497. https:// and Developmental Disorders, 45(12), 3836–3845.
doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2007.08.006. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2072-3.
Charlop, M. H., Gilmore, L., & Chang, G. T. (2008). Mazurek, M. O., Shattuck, P. T., Wagner, M., &
Using video modeling to increase variation in conver- Cooper, B. P. (2012). Prevalence and correlates of
sations of children with autism. Journal of Special screen-based media use among youths with autism
Education Technology, 23(3), 47–66. doi:https://doi. spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Develop-
org/10.1177/016264340802300305. mental Disorders, 42(8), 1757–1767. doi:https://doi.
Charlop, M. H., & Milstein, J. (1989). Teaching autistic org/10.1007/s10803-011-1413-8.
children conversational speech using video modeling. McClannahan, L. E., & Krantz, P. J. (2005). Teaching
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22(3), 275–285. conversation to children with autism. Bethesda, MD:
doi:https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1989.22-275. Woodbine House.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Kelso, S. E. (2003). Teaching Rakes, G. C., Fields, V. S., & Cox, K. E. (2006). The
children with autism conversational speech using a influence of teachers’ technology use on instructional
cue card/written script program. Education and Treat- practices. Journal of Research on Technology in
ment of Children, 26(2), 108–127. Retrieved from Education, 38(4), 409–424. doi:https://doi.org/10.
http://www.educationandtreatmentofchildren.net/. 1080/15391523.2006.10782467.
Cihak, D., Fahrenkrog, C., Ayres, K. M., & Smith, C. Ramdoss, S., Lang, R., Mulloy, A., Franco, J.,
(2010). The use of video modeling via a video iPod O’Reilly, M., Didden, R., & Lancioni, G. (2010).
and a system of least prompts to improve transitional Use of computer-based interventions to teach com-
behaviors for students with autism spectrum disorders munication in individuals with autism spectrum dis-
in the general education classroom. Journal of Positive orders: A systematic review. Journal of Behavioral
Behavior Interventions, 12(2), 103–115. doi:https:// Education, 20(1), 55–76. doi:https://doi.org/10.
doi.org/10.1177/1098300709332346. 1007/s10864-010-9112-7.
Erickson-Tomaino, M. (2011). Teaching conversation to Raulston, T., Carnett, A., Lang, R., Tostanoski, A.,
children with autism: Assessment of the efficacy of a Lee, A., Machalicek, W.,... Didden, R. (2013).
parent-implemented script procedure (Doctoral disserta- Teaching individuals with autism spectrum disorder
tion). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations & The- to ask questions: A systematic review. Research in
ses. (UMI No. 3449924). Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(7), 866–878. doi:
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1993). Teaching https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.03.008.
children with autism to initiate to peers: Effects of a Reagon, K. A., & Higbee, T. S. (2009). Parent-
script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior implemented script fading to promote play-based ver-
Analysis, 26, 121–132. doi:https://doi.org/10.1901/ bal initiations in children with autism. Journal of
jaba.1993.26-121 Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 659–664. doi:https://
Krasny, L., Williams, B. J., Provencal, S., & Ozonoff, S. doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-659.
(2003). Social skills interventions for the autism spec- Roberts, D. F., Foehr, U. G., & Rideout, V. J. (2010).
trum: Essential ingredients and a model curriculum. Generation M2: Media in the lives of 8-18 year-olds.
Child & Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North Retrieved from the Kaiser Family Foundation web-
America, 12(1), 107–122. Retrieved from http:// site: http://kff.org/other/report/generation-m2-media-
www.childpsych.theclinics.com/. in-the-lives-of-8-to-18-year-olds/.
Ledbetter-Cho, K., Lang, R., Davenport, K., Moore, M., Sarokoff, R. A., Taylor, B. A., & Poulson, C. L. (2001).
Lee, A., Howell, A., ... O’Reilly, M. (2015). Effects Teaching children with autism to engage in conversa-
of script training on the peer-to-peer communication tional exchanges: Script fading with embedded textual
804 DENISE GROSBERG and MARJORIE H. CHARLOP

stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 81– Analysis, 10, 349–367. doi:https://doi.org/10.1901/
84. doi:https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2001.34-81. jaba.1977.10-349.
Schreibman, L. (2005). The science and fiction of autism. Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K., Cox, A. W.,
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. Fettig, A., Kucharczyk, S., ... Schultz, T. R. (2014).
Schuler, A. L., & Wolfberg, P. J. (2000). Promoting peer Evidence-based practices for children, youth, and young
play and socialization: The art of scaffolding. In adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Chapel Hill,
A. M. Wetherby & B. M. Prizant (Eds.), Autism spec- NC: The University of North Carolina, Frank Porter
trum disorders: A transactional developmental perspec- Graham Child Development Institute, Autism
tive (pp. 251–277). Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes. Evidence-Based Review Group.
Sherer, M., Pierce, K.L., Paredes, S., Kisacky, K. L., Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001).
Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2001). Enhancing Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement [Examiner’s
conversation skills in children with autism via video manual]. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
technology. Behavior Modification, 25(1), 140–158. Zickuhr, K., & Smith, A. (2012). Digital differences. Retrieved
doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445501251008. from the Pew Research Center website: http://www.
Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). The pewinternet.org/2012/10/17/digital-differences-2/.
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Survey Form). Cir-
cle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Received December 30, 2014
Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol- Final acceptance August 2, 2016
ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Action Editor, Claire St. Peter

You might also like