Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

T

he Occupational Performance History Inter-


Development and view is designed to gather individual work,
play, and self-care performance histories from
Investigation of the psychiatric and physically disabled populations The
instrument was formulated to be compatible with
Occupational more than one frame of reference. This paper

Performance History presents the instrument, its development, and the re-
sults of a study of its reliability across time and raters.

Interview Review of the Literature


Types ofInterviews
Gary Kielhofner, Alexis D. Henry
Interviews may be characterized on a continuum from
Key Words: interviews. occupational structured to unstructured. The use of the structured
interview in research is based on the ideal of inter-
behavior • test construction view eqUivalence across subjects (Babbie, 1974) The
interviewer follows the wording of questions exactly,
uses only predetermined probes or clarifications, and
This article describes the development of the Occu- records responses exactly as given or on a predeter-
pational Peljormance History Interview, an instru- mined scale. The unstructured interview, used in eth-
ment designed to gather an accurate and clinically nographic or qualitative field studies, is purposely
useful history of an individual's work, play, and open-ended to allow the interviewer to achieve an
self-care performance from psychosocially and/or understanding of the psychological and cultural
physically disabled adolescents, adults, and older viewpoint of the respondent (Edgerton & Langness,
persons. FolloWing the identification and validation
1974; Pelto & Pelto, 1970).
of content and the development offormat, the in-
strument was field-tested and revised. An examina- The clinical interview, drawing on the strengths
tion of reliability across time and raters indicated of both the structured and the unstructured interview,
that although part of the instrument ratings met or often takes on a semistructured format (Clare & Car-
exceeded levels of acceptable stability, further de- iens, 1978; Florey & Michelman, 1982; Gurland,
velopment and testing is indicated. Yorkston, Stone, Frank, & Fleiss, 1972; Moorhead,
1969; Swanson-Fisher & Martin, 1981). Clinical inter-
viewers use their positions as concerned care pro-
viders to elicit accurate responses and achieve em-
pathy with the respondent's point of view, while em-
ploying a clinical frame of reference as a structure for
the interview and its interpretation. Structure in inter-
views is viewed as an aid in eliminating clinical bias
(Fleiss, Spitzer, & Burdock, 1965; Gurland et aI.,
1972; Helzer, Robins, Croughan, & Weiner, 1981;
Helzer et aI., 1977; Strauss, Carpenter, & Nasrallah,
1978) .

Occupational Therapy Historical Interviews


Gary Kielhofner, DrPH, OTR, FAOTA, is Head and Asso- Occupational therapy literature acknowledges the
ciate Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, Col-
importance of interviewing patients/clients (Cynkin,
lege of Associated Health Professions, University of Illi-
nois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60612. 1979; Melvin, 1977; Pedretti, 1981; Smith & Tiffany,
1983; Trombly, 1983) Moorhead (1969) published
Alexis D. Henry, MS, OTR/L, is Rehabilitation COOl-dina- the first historical occupational therapy interview, the
tor, Department of Rehabilitation Services, McLean Hospi- Occupational History, a lengthy, semistructured in-
tal, Belmont, Massachusetts. She is also a doCtoral student
terview designed from an occupational behavior
in therapeutic studies at Sargent College of Allied Health
Professions, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts. frame of reference to gather detailecl qualitative in-
formation concerning functioning in occupational
This article was accepted for publication September 28, 1987 roles.

The American Journal of Occupational Tberapy 489

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Recognizing that the shift from long-term to Purpose
acute care in psychiatry made it imperative for occu-
pational therapists to have a briefer screening tool, The purpose in developing the present assessment
Florey and Michelman (1982) developed and piloted was to create an instrument that would build upon
the Occupational Role History They based the in- previous occupational history interview instruments
strument on Moorhead's original work and on a re- but offer the follOWing: (a) compatibility with more
fined occupational behavior frame of reference. The than a single frame of reference, (b) flexibility in use,
Occupational Role History was designed to provide (c) relevance to differeI1l age and disability groups,
information on occupational roles and the following (d) clinical usability (ie., the instrument should be
components thereof: satisfaction and dissatisfaction easy to learn, have a simple structure to guide the
with interests, people, tasks, and environments; com- interview, and use succinct methods for rating and
petence in simultaneous occupational roles; areas of reporting data from the interview), and (e) evidence
skill; and the degree of balance between work, of reliability.
chores, and leisure. Data from the Occupational Role
History are interpreted and analyzed to determine Method
how well the person fulfills the demands of his or her
Development and Content Validation
various roles in life and how well he or she balances
occupational and leisure activities. Given the criteria noted above, the content of the
interview had to be broad enough to reflect different
Methods of Reducing and Reporting Interview Data conceptual frameworks in occupational therapy and
to address concerns relevant to different disability
Interviews can be used to make a classification deci-
groups and developmental levels. Based on a review
sion (Helzer et al., 1977) Another approach to re-
of the content of existing interviews, preliminary cate-
ducing and reporting interview data is to use a rating
gories of content were identified and defined. These
scale. Most rating scales incorporate either a formal or
were sent to 22 occupational therapists selected for
an implicit theory of adjustment/maladjustment as
their known contributions to theory and practice and
the conceptual backdrop for determining the COI1lent
approved by the AOTA Committee on Standardized
of the rating (Gurland et al., 1972; Shontz & Fink,
Assessments (an AOTA/AOTF ad hoc committee).
1961) Scales may identify fields or areas of adjust-
These experts were asked to comment on the appro-
ment/maladjustment, each consisting of types of
priateness of the content areas, suggest revisions in
questions or items (Gurland et al., 1972)
their definitions or scope, and suggest other content
areas. From the feedback received, an original six
Evaluating Reliability of Historical Interview content areas were collapsed into five and their defi-
Instruments nitions revised.
The reliability of historical interview instruments is Once the content for the interview had been
generally gauged by the stability of (a) the account identified, a set of questions to elicit the appropriate
given by the respondent and (b) the narrative de- data from the interviewee and two complementary
scription and/or rating that an interviewer creates methods of reporting the data were developed. The
from the respondent'S account. Research designs for first method was a scale on which to rate the five
evaluating reliability may be characterized by whether content areas and the second was a brief narrative
they examine one or both of these factors (Carmines reporting form for writing a qualitative account of the
& Zeller, 1979; Helzer et aI., 1977). Agreement interview findings organized into the five content
among raters (interrater reliability) is usually investi- areas. In addition, a user's manual was written to en-
gated by having an interviewer conduct an interview able therapists to teach themselves to administer the
that additional raters observe directly or through vid- interview.
eotape or audiotape. This method is a way of evaluat- The interview and manual were field-tested na-
ing the stability of rating procedures, or more specifi- tionally by 90 therapists chosen to represent a variety
cally, the amount of agreement between independent of treatment settings, patient/client populations, and
raters who apply the same criteria to make a judgment frames of reference. These therapists used the inter-
(Helzer et al., 1977). The test-retest method gener- view and responded to questions concerning the clar-
ally involves two separate interviewers independently ity of the content areas and the clinical relevance and
interviewing and rating the respondent. Technically, usefulness of the questions, the rating scale, and the
this process actually incorporates both test-retest and narrative reporting form. The information received
interrater elements; that is, when ratings are com- from the field test resulted in further revisions in the
pared, both interrater agreement and the effects of definitions of the content areas, the iI1lerview ques-
different interviewers are· simultaneously assessed. tions, and the rating scale.

490 August 1988, Volume 42, Number 8

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Instrument 4. Perceptions ofAbility and Responsibility.- What
The Occupational Performance History Interview is the interviewee's awareness of his or her
consists of 39 recommended questions about the in- own control over everyday life and of his or
terviewee's past and present behavior. The questions her ability to perform behaviors required of
cover five content areas, which are defined as follows:
everyday life?
5. EnlJironmental influences: What is the in.flu-
1. Organization of Daily Living Routines: How ence of the interviewee's present human and
does the interviewee typically spend his or her nonhuman environments on his or her adapta-
time and how does that pattern of time use tion? What was it in the past? What is it likely to
affect his or her functioning? How does he or be in the future?
she balance work, play, and daily living tasks
and how satisfied is he or she with this bal- Although a recommended sequence and format
ance? of questions is proVided, the interviewer is encour-
2. Ltfe Roles: What pattern of role involvement aged to add, delete, and adapt questions as clinically
emerges from the interview and how well indicated during the interview.
does the interviewee fulfill the expectations of A 5-point rating scale (see Figure 1) quamifies
his or her roles? the interviewee's functioning in each of the five con-
3 Interests, Values, and Goals: How well does tent areas. Each content area consists of two items,
the interviewee identify and act on personal which are summed to provide a score for that area.
interests, values, and goals, and to what de- The four content area scores pertaining to the individ-
gree does he or she achieve satisfaction and ual can be summed to obtain a score representing the
enjoyment? person's overall adaptation. All five content area

Instructions: Circle the number that best characterizes the individual's adaptive status for each key item in both past and present
according to the following scale: 5- Totally Adaplive (high level of function); 4-Moderately Adaptive (basic ability to function);
3-Marginal (questionable/at· risk ability to function); 2-Moderately Maladaptive (substantial problems that interfere with
function); 1 - Totally Maladaptive (complete inability to function).
Individual Past Present
Organization of Maintenance of organized
Daily Living functional daily routines 5 4 3 2 5 4 3 2
Routines
Achievement of a balance in work,
play, and daily living tasks 5 4 3 2 5 4 :I 2
Life Roles Maintenance of involvement in
life roles 5 4 3 2 1 5 4 3 2
Fulfillment of expectations of life
roles 5 4 3 2 5 4 3 2
I nterests, Values, Identification of interests, values,
and Goals and goals 5 4 3 2 5 4 3 2
Enactment of interests, values,
and goals 5 4 3 2 ') 4 -~ 2
Perception of Ability Acknowledgment of abilities and
and Responsibility limitations 5 4 3 2 ') 4 3 2
Assumption of responsibility 5 4 3 2 5 4 3 2

Instructions: Circle the number that best characterizes the influences of the individual's environment in both the past and the
present according to the following scale: 5- Totally Supports Adaptation (conditions maximize function); 4-Moderately
Supports Adaptation (conditions give basic support to function); 3-Marginal (questionable/at-risk support to function); 2-
Moderately Interferes With Adaptation (conditions that limit function); 1- Totally Interferes With Adaptation (major obstacles to
function)
Environment Past Present
Environmental Influences of the human
Influences environment 5 4 3 2 ') 4 3 2
Influences of the nonhuman
environment ') 4 3 2 5 4 :I 2

Figure 1. Occupational Performance History Rating Scale

The American/ournal of Occupational 7hl!rapJ' 491

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Table 1
Diagnostic Characteristics of Subjects (N = 153)
Adult Psychiatry Adolescent Psychiatry Physical Disabilities Gerontology
(n = 44) (n = 28) (n = 33) (n = 48)

Depression 19 (43) Depression 14 (50) CVA 7 (21) CVA 13 (27)


Schizophrenia 19 (43) Character 3 (11) Spinal cord 7 (21) Arthritis 6 (13)
disorder injury Depression 6 (13)
Substance 3 (7) Schizophrenia 2 (7) Fractures 5 (16) Amputation 5(11)
abuse
Other 3 (7) Learning 2 (7) Muscle/nerve 3 (9) Fractures 4 (8)
disability injury
Other 7 (25) Pain 3 (9) Parkinson's 3 (6)
disease
Multiple 2 (6) Spinal cord 3 (6)
sclerosis injury
Brain injury/ 2 (6) Multiple sclerosis 2 (4)
head trauma
Other 4 (12) Muscle or nerve 2 (4)
injury
Other 4 (8)
Note Numbers in parentheses are percentages.

scores can be summed to obtain a total score. Separate In addition to the narrative form used to report
ratings and scores are obtained for the past and for the qualitative data, a five-item nominal scale called the
present. To make these ratings the therapist must es- Life History Pattern is llsed to characterize the individ-
tablish a demarcation period between present and ual's overall history. The therapist is asked to identify
past functioning. This is determined individually for which of five life history patterns listed on the scale
each interviewee and is based on such events as onset most characterizes the patient/client. The five life his-
of disability, major change in life roles, or change in tory patterns are (a) history of chronic maladaptation,
living situations. (b) history of adaptation recently interrupted by acute

Table 2
Demographic Characteristics of Subjects (N = 153)
Adult Psychiatry Physical
Psychiatry Adolescent Disabilities Gerontology
(n = 44) (n = 28) (n = 33) (n = 48)

Age
Mean 336 17.0 44.2 75.9
Range 20-58 13-18 23-66 65-93
Occupation
Worker 16 (36) 21 (64) 3 (6)
Unemployed 15 (34) 1 (4) 4 (12)
Student 4 (9) 27 (96) 1(3)
Retired 1 (2) 1(3) 36 (75)
Homemaker 6 (14) 5 (15)
Other 2 (5) 1(3)

living situation
Alone 11 (25) 6 (18) 15 (32)
Head of household 11 (25) 18 (55) 21 (44)
Dependent on family 6 (14) 28 (100) 3 (9) 4 (8)
Institutional 8 (18) 1(3) 3 (6)
Roommate 8 (18) 4 (12) 2 (4)
Other 1 (3) 3 (6)

Education
Less than high school 6 (14) 17 (61) 4 (12) 14 (29)
High school 25 (57) 11 (39) 19 (58) 22 (46)
College 10 (22) 5 (15) 11 (23)
Beyond college 3 (7) 5 (15) 1 (2)

Sex
Male 22 (50) 21 (75) 17 (52) 16 (33)
Female 22 (50) 7 (25) 16 (48) 32 (67)
Note. Numbers in parentheses are percentages.

492 August 1988, Volume 42, Number 8


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Table 3 reliability worked in pairs. The first therapist made
Item Correlations Between Two Administrations of the sure that the patient could respond to an interview,
Occupational Performance History Interview (N = 153)
obtained consent, and interviewed the subject, re-
Item Past Present cording the interview on a tape recorder. When at
Maintenance of organized least 5, but no more than 12 days had passed. the
routines 62 .40 subject was reinterviewed by the second therapiSt.
Acievement of balance in
work, play, and daily Each therapist completed the Rating and Narrative
living tasks .57 .47 Forms immediately after the interview Therapists
Maintenance of were instructed to take turns doing the first interview
involvement in life roles 68 39
Fulfillment of role to counterbalance the effects of order. After the thera-
expectations .65 .33 pists had finished collecting data, they completed a
Identification of interests, feedback/demographic questionnaire giving infor·
values, goals .63 .42
Enactment of interests, mation about themselves and their assessments of the
values, goals 56 35 instru ment.
Acknowledgment of To examine interrater reliability, we duplicated
abilities and limitations .62 .48
Assumption of the taped interviews done by the first therapist during
responsibility 68 .49 the test-retest phase and sent them to two indepen-
Human environment .62 36 dent therapists who listened to the interview once and
Nonhuman environment 55 31
then completed the Rating and Narrative Form. Thes.e
Note. p = .001 for all correlations,
therapists were prOVided with basic demographic in-
formation on the subject. These raters were also asked
to respond to a feedback/demographic questionnaire.
onset of maladaptation, (c) history of variable periods
of both adaptation and maladaptation, (d) history of
adaptation followed by gradual, progressive maladap- Results
tation, and (e) other. To assess test-retest reliability, data across two admin-
Both test-retest and interrater reliability data istrations of the interview were obtained for 153 sub-
were collected by therapists selected from a variety of jects. Interrater data were collected for 129 of these
settings in the United States and Canada. Although the subjects Table 1 presents subjects' diagnoses for each
composition of this sample was determined by the of the four subsamples in the study, Subjects' demo-
therapists' willingness to make a substantial commit- graphic traits are noted in Table 2, A total of 201 thera-
ment of time and effort, we sought to maximize vari- pists helped collect clata or make ratings of the sub-
ability in the types of settings and therapists repre- jects, The therapists' mean age was 32.3 years, and the
sented. Recruitment of therapists was also done so as average number of years of experience was 7.2. Thera-
to assure four categories of subjects adolescent psy- pists' frames of reference were primarily eclectic
chiatry, adult psychiatry, physical dysfunction, and (40%), occupational behavior/model of human occu-
gerontology. Therapists collecting data for test-retest pation (33%), and developmental (10%)

Table 4
Correlations Between Two Administrations of the Interview (N = 153)
Adolescent Physical
Psychiatry Adult Psychiatry Disabil ities Gerontology Total Group
Variable Past Present Past Present Past Present Past Present Past Present
Organization
of routines .55" .44' , .46' , .4}>' .65' , .52" .44" .. 54" ,63'" .49'"
Life roles .57" .25' .46' , .49" .64" .47" .69" .34' • 71**· .39" ,
Interests,
values, goals .66" .45' , .44" .44" .40" .25' .67" .49" .63'" .4 5'"
Perception of
ability and
responsibility .49" .59" .51' , 31* .55' , .55' , .63" .56" 71**· .55" ,
Environmental
influences 33' .18" 18" .49' , ,71' , .37' .55' , .27' .63' " .35" •
Individual total ,64" .51*' .53' , .45' , .63" ,SO" .69" .57' • .73" • .54" ,
Scale total ,60" .47' , .47' , .52' , ,68' , ,47" .69" .54' , ,73" , .53' "
, Not significant,
, p-5. ,05. "'p-5. ,01 ",p-5. 001

The American.!ournal oj'Occupational Tberapy 493


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Table 5
Correlations Between Two Administrations When Therapists' Frames of Reference Were Matched
Matched Frame of Occupational Behavior
Reference (n = 91) EcleClic (n = 38) (n = 24)

Variable Past' , Present' , Past' , Present' , Past' , Present


Organization of
routines 68 45 72 46 75 36'
Life roles 79 34 .84 40 .88 .19'
Interests, values,
goals 71 52 84 57 71 .38'
Perception of ability
and responsibility .81 55 .87 59 87 .66'
Environmental
infiuence 68 41 .74 45 59 .34'
Individual total .82 .51 .86 55 .92 45'
Scale tOtal .82 52 85 53 91 48'
aNot significant.
'p:o;.05."p:o; .01

For each of the 20 items on the rating scale as- compared with 57 (p = ,01) and ,2] (p = nonSignifi-
sessing the individual's past and present adaptive cant) for the subjects with low honesty ratings. Con-
status (see Figure]), the distribution of ratings across tent area and individual item correlations were also
the 5-point scale was about equal with the exception generally lower for the low-honesty subjects,
of the totally maladaptive rating, which was used The effect of therapists's agreement on the de-
about 10% of the time Correlations between past and marcation of past and present was similarly examined,
present item ratings were generally low (ranging Therapists who agreed on demarcation pOints within
from .13 to A1), indicating that the two sets of ratings 3 months of each other yielded total correlations of
were relatively independent. .76 (p =01) for the past and 55 (p = .01) for the
present whereas therapists who disagreed by more
Reliability Across Two Administrations of the than 3 months yielded a total past correlation of .68
Interview (p = .01) and a total present correlation of AO (p =
Table 3 gives Pearson correlations for individual .01), The item and individual toral correlations
items across the two administrations of the interview showed the same pattern of lower correlations when
for the toral group. These ranged from .55 to .68 for therapists did not agree on the demarcation.
past ratings and from .31 to .49 for present ratings.
Overall the correlations were higher for the past than
for the present. Correlations for the content area Table 6
scores and total scores across two administrations of Item Correlations Between Two Interraters for the Total
the assessment are shown in Table 4. As expected, Group (N = 129)
these correlations appear higher than those for the Item Past Present
individual items. Table 4 also presents correlations Maintenance of organized
for ratings across each of the four subgroups in the routines .52 40
study. These correlations are somewhat lower on the Achievement of balance in
work, play, and daily
whole. The past ratings of physical disabilities and Jiving tasks 51 40
gerontology appear more stable than the adolescent Maintenance of involvement
psychiatry and adult psychiatry ratings Although the in life roles .50 38
Fulfillment of role
present ratings are lower overall, there does not ap- expectations .52 46
pear to be the same pattern of the psychiatry rating Identification of intereSts,
being less stable than the other two groups' ratings. values, goals 38 23
Enactment of interests,
The effect of the subject's honesty on stability values, goals 48 40
was examined by dividing the sample into two sub- Acknowledgment of abilities
groups: those whose therapists gave ratings of 3 or and limitations 53 29
Assumption of responSibility 55 42
above on a 5-poinr rating of honesty (n = 132) and Human environment 54 32
those with ratings of 2 or 1 (n = 21), The correlation Nonhuman environment 45 -08
for the total score in the honest group was 75 <p = Note. Interrater data were collected for only 129 of the 153 subjects.
01) for the past and 54 (p = ,01) for the present p:o; .001 for all correlations.

494 August 1988, Votume 42, Number 8


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Table 7
Variable and Total Correlations Between Two Interraters of the Interview for the Four Subgroups (N = 129)
Adolescent Physical
Psychiatry Adult Psychiatry Disabilities Gerontology Total Group
Variable Past Present Past Present Past Present Past Present Past Present
Organization of
routines .46' , .57*' .21' .52' , .27" .12" .61' , .4 5" .56" , .45" ,
Life roles .53' , .2S' 23' .53' , .19' 22' 30' .49'" '" .55" , .44" ,
Interests,
values, goals 39' .54' , .24" .45' , .21' 17" .29' .42' , .4S" , .3S" ,
Perception of
ability and
responSibility .46' , .56' , .19" 12" .4 7" .42' .41' , .48' , .58" , .44" ,
Environmental
influences .30" .04" .40' 11" IS" .07" .30' 02' .57' " .13" •
Individual total .53' • .55' • 23' .54' , .30" .26" .49' , .55" .60'" .50" ,
Scale lOtal .53' , .45' .29' .55' , 32" .21 ' .50' , .52' , .63" , .48" •
Note. Interrater data were collected for only 129 of the 153 subjects.
, Not significant.
'p,,; .05. "p,,;Ol "'p,,; .001

Another potential source of instability was the present rating whereas the present ratings of the three
difference in therapists' frames of reference. Since the other groups were similar
instrument was designed to be compatible with more By eliminating subjects whose raters disagreed
than one frame of reference, it follows that therapists on the event demarcating past from present and who
using different frames of reference might interview had received low ratings on honesty, higher correla-
subjects and interpret data differently Correlations tions were obtained for variable and total scores (see
(see Table 5) obtained for pairs of therapists who had Table 8) When selecting those G1SeS for whom both
matching frames of reference are notably elevated for raters held the same frame of reference (see Table 9),
the past ratings when compared with the total group; there was no increase in stability except for the occu-
present ratings appear unaffected. Correlations for pational be!-Jcwior present rating.
matched pairs of therapists holding an eclectic frame
of reference appeared similar to the total group of Therapists' Assessment of the Interview
matched pairs. Correlations for those who used the Therapists were asked for their assessment of the usa-
occupational behavior frame of reference appeared bility and utility of the instrument. The majority did
higher for the past ratings; correlarions for the present not find the interview, the rating scale, the life history
were somewhat lower in comparison with the others. pattern, or the narrative description difficult to do.
Numbers were too few for comparisons of therapists The entire procedure on the average takes about 1 Vz
who held other frames of reference. hours with an average of 47 minutes for the interview,
21 minutes for the rating, and 28 minutes for the nar-
Reliability Between Two Raters rative report The majority of therapists found the in-
terview helpful in establishing rapport, identifying pa-
Interrater reliability was examined in a fashion similar
to that described above Table 6 shows item correla-
tions between two interraters; they ranged from .38 to
.55 for the past and from a nonsignificant -08 to .46 Table 8
Variable and Total Correlations Between Interraters With
for the present. As shown in Table 7, past content area
Two Potential Sources of Error Eliminated (N = 45)
correlations ranged from .48 toS8; the individual
Variable Past Present
total was .60 and the scale total was .63. The present
correlations ranged from 13 to .45; the individual Organization of routines 67 51
Life roles .66 55
toral was .50 and the scale total was .48. As with reli- Interests, values, goals 74 49
ability ~1CrosS two administrations, present ratings Perception of ability and
were less stable than past ratings. The physical dis- responSibility 69 48
Environmental influences .64 .05"
abilities and adult psychiatry subgroups' past ratings Individual total 75 61
were the least stable whereas the past ratings of the Scale total 77 55
other tWO subgroups were similar The physical dis- Note. p < .0001 unless otherwise nored.
abilities subgroup had the lowest correlations for the , Not significant.

The American journal a/Occupational Therapy 495


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Table 9
Correlations Between Two Interraters When Therapists' Frames of Reference Were Matched
Matched Frame of Occupational Behavior
Reference (11 = 38) Eclectic (n = 22) (n=12)

Variable Past Present Past Present Past Present


Organization of
romines .55' , .63" .25" .63" .57* 79"
Life roles .46' • .40' , 38' .50' , .23' .50'
Interests, val ues.
goals .37' , .37*' .14" 28' .34" .69"
Perception of ability
and responsibility .4 7*· .44' , .28" 38' .26' 60'
Environmemal
influences .23' -.12" .20" -31' .29" .46"
Individual total .53' • .54' , .33' .49" 39' 71*'
Scale total .50' • .49' , .31" .38' .43" .80"
, Not significant.
'p :0;05. "p:o; .01.

tient problems, and setting treatment goals. Only ity. Ratings for the present fell short of reaching this
16.5% of therapists indicated they would not use the minimal criterion
interview, and over 40% indicated they would use it It may be that present ratings were more difficult
regularly or for some patients. Nearly half of the thera· to make because fewer dara were available to raters.
pists said they would use the rating form for the meeii- For example. the recently injured person may not
cal record, and about 80% said they would include it have had enough experience to relate, thus not giving
in the occupmional therapy record therapists a sound basis for a present rating. In a pre-
vious study of reliability in an occupational therapy
Discussion interview, stability of the rating of present adaptation
of patients was also lower than for the past (Kiel-
The conditions under which the interview and rating hofner, Harlan, Bauer, & Maurer, 1986). Another fae
scale were examined in this study yielded a very con- tor that may have made these ratings marc difficult to
servative estimate of stability. When the interview was give was the way in which we instructed therapists to
administered to a patient by twO different therapists, do them (ie., to consider the present in terms of its
several sources of potential differences in scores ex· indications about the future direction of the patient's
isted. (a) respondent reliability from Interview 1 to adaptation) In another study (Kaplan, 1984), stability
Interview 2, (b) differences in therapists' interview· for a rating of future adaptation was also low, indicat-
ing abilities and style, and (c) therapist judgment in ing that this is a more difficult area of clinical judg-
converting interview data to ratings. further differ· ment. Finally, it may be that differences in the present
ences are those creared by the research procedure: rating reflect some actual changes in the patient's
(a) the presence of the audiotape machine in the first status between interviews.
interview but not in the second, (b) inHuence exerted It does appear that the present ratings are based
by the first interview on the information the respon· on different information than the past ratings, as is
dent gave in the second interview, and (c) the artifi- indicated by the low correlations between past and
ciality of being interviewed by a second therapist present ratings. The present ratings bear further at·
using the same interview instrument. Additionally, tention, and some revision of the procedure may be
the large number of interviewers/raters who partici- warranted If it is determined that instability in ratings
pated instead of the few ordinarily used to estimate was due to a lack of information about the present,
stability, all of whom had limited experience ,vith the then the user might be given the option of not making
interview and were self-taught (via the manual) in its the present ratings or of indicating on the form that
use, may have accounted for some differences in they are based on limited information.
scores. Therapists reported tinding it difficult to deter-
Data from the two administrations of the inter· mine the demarcation of past and present on the basis
view indicated that for the total group the variable and of the person's unique life history, and differences
total past scores were fairly stable. All of the correla- between therapists' point of demarcation did have a
tions for past ratings exceeded Benson and Clark's negative effect on stability. Similarly, subjects judged
(1982) minimal criterion of .60 for test-retest reliabil- to be low on honesty had a negative effect on stability.

496 Augus! 1988, Volume 42, Number 8

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Therapists' having the same frame of reference ap- In the present study over a third of the therapists
peared to increase some correlations. If therapists' had never or seldom used interviews in their practice,
frame of reference does influence how they make and all learned to use the Occupational Performance
judgments when doing the ratings, then the question History Interview only by reading a manual. However,
must be raised whether a generic rating scale concor- most did not use it or used it only a few times before
dant with different frames of reference is compatible doing the interviews for this study. Thus, if therapists
with the goal of achieving an optimally stable rating. received organized training and experience in the use
Overall, the correlations between two interraters of the interview, higher reliability coefficients would
were unacceptably low. It was expected that these probably be obtained
correlations would be higher than the correlations Although we made efforts to assure content valid-
across two administrations, since the two administra- ity of the procedure, an empirical examination of the
tions included sources of error not present in the in- validity of the history should be made in the future.
terrating condition (ie., the effects of time and two Specifically, the ability of the history to predict reha-
different interviewers). Since previous research on bilitation participation and success would be of both
psychosocial interviews indicated that audiotaped in- theoretical and clinical interest.
terviews produced ratings as stable as when therapists Finally, direct observation or videotaped inter-
were present at the interviews (Fleiss et al., 1965), we views along with the use of data from other charts may
expected the ratings made from the audiotapes to be prove to be a more effective method of examining
more stable. Several factors may have contributed to interrater agreement than the audiOtape method.
these lower correlations. The semistructured inter- In summary, it appears fair to expect that if the
view permits therapists to use their own style of inter rating scale were revised and developed specific to a
viewing and to construct their own questions. It may frame of reference and if therapists trained in the use
be that therapists condu([ed interviews differently of the Occupational Performance History Interview
from the way raters listening to the tapes would have gave the interview and did the ratings, evidence
and that the information obtained was not what the of acceptable reliability and validity would be
listeners would have obtained to make their judg- forthcoming.
ments. In particular, a therapist listening to an inter-
view conducted by a therapist using another frame of References
reference may have found it difficult to make ratings
from the interview. Babbie, E R. (1974). Sumey research melhods. Bel-
Therapists commented on the difficulty of mak- mont, CA: Wadsworth.
ing ratings from audiotapes. They identified several Benson, j., & Clark, F. (1982) A gUide for instrument
development and validation. American)oumal ofOccupa-
factors that made for difficulty, including the auditory tional Therap)', 36, 789-800.
quality of some tapes, the nature of the interview Carmines, E., & Zeiter, R. (1979) Reliahility and va-
done by the interviewer, and the lack of actual contact lidityassessment Beverly Hills: Sage.
with the patient. A therapist's knowledge of the pa- Clare, A. W, & Cariens, v. E (1978). Design, deveJop-
tient'S personality and abilities (more readily avail- ment and use of a standardized interview to assess social
maladjustment and dysfunction in community studies. Psy-
able from direct observation) may have influenced cbological Medicine, 8, 589-604
ratings, and the lack of this information may have Cynkin, S. (1979) Occupational therapy: Toward
made ratings from audiotapes more prone to error. health tbrougb activities Boston: Little, Brown.
Given the higher stability of the ratings across two Edgerton, R. 13., & Langness, L. L. (1974). Methods and
administrations, it would appear that the interrater st)'les in the study of culture. San Francisco: Chandler &
Sharp
correlations are a result of such problems rather than Fleiss, J. L., Spitzer, R. L., & Burdock, E. 1 (1965).
an accurate reflection of instability in the rating Estimating accuracy of judgment using recorded imerviews
process. Archives of General Ps)'chiatJ}', J2, 562-567.
Florey, L. L., & Michelman, S M. (982) Occupational
role history: A screening tool for psychiatric occupational
Recommendations therapy. journal a/Occupational Tberapy, 36, 301-308.
Gurland, B. J., Yorkston, N. .J., Stone, A. R, Frank, J D,
Given the apparent impact of frame of reference on & Fleiss,J. L. (1972). The structUred and scaled interview to
stability, it appears advisable that rating scales should assess maladjustmem (SSIAM)· 1 Description, rationale,
be based on a frame of reference. By creating rating and development. Archives 0/ General Psychiatr)', 27,
judgments that correspond to how therapists engage 259-263
Helzer, J. E, Robins, L. N, Croughan,.J L., & Weiner,
in clinical reasoning from their frame of reference it
A. (1981) Renard diagnostic interview Its reliability and
should be possible to improve the stability of procedural vailidity with physicians and lay interviewers.
ratings. Archives of General Psychiatr)', 38, 393-398

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 497


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Helzer, ]., Robins, L., Taibleson, M., Woodruff, R., search: The structure of inquiry (2nd ed.). New York: Cam-
Reich, T., & Wish, E. (1977). Reliability of psychiatric diag- bridge University Press.
nosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 129-133. Shontz, F. C, & Fink, S. L. (1961). A method for evalu-
Kaplan, K. (1984). Short term assessment: The need ating psychosocial adjustment of the chronically ill. Ameri-
and a response. Occupational Therapy in Mental Health, 4, can journal of Physical Medicine, 40,63-69.
29-45. Smith, H. D., & Tiffany, E. G (1983) Assessment and
Kieihofner, G., Harlan, B., Bauer, D., & Maurer, P evaluation-An overview. In H. D. Smith & H. L. Hopkins
(1986). The reliability of a historical interview with physi- (Eds.), Willard and Spackman's Occupational Therapy
cally disabled respondents. American journal of Occupa- (6thecl) (pp 143-147) Philadelphia:] B. Lippincott.
tional Therapy, 40, 551-556 Strauss,] S., Carpenter, W. T., & Nasrallah, A. T.
Melvin, ]. (1977) Rheumatic disease: Occupational (1978). How reliable is the psychiatric history' Compre-
therapy and rehabilitation. Philadelphia: F. A. Davis. hensive Psychiatry, 19, 213-219.
Moorhead, L. C. (1969) The occupational history. Swanson-Fisher, R. W., & Martin, C. ]. (1981) Stan-
Americanjournal of Occupational Therapy, 23, 329-334. dardized interview in psychiatry: Issues of reliability. British
Pedretti, L. (1981) Occupational therapy: Practice journal of Psychiatry, 139, 138-143
skills for physical dysfunction. St. Louis: C. V. Mosby. Trombly, C (Ed) (1983) Occupational therapy for
Pelto, P J., & Pelto, G. H. (1970). Anthropological re- physical dysfunction. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.

498 August 1988, Volume 42, Number 8

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 10/22/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms

You might also like