Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Contemporary Educational Psychology


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / c e d p s y c h

Teacher interests, mastery goals, and self-efficacy as predictors of


instructional practices and student motivation
Ulrich Schiefele *, Ellen Schaffner
Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: This study addressed the role of elementary school teachers’ motivation as predictors of instructional
Available online 29 June 2015 practices and student motivation. The sample comprised 110 teacher–class pairs (1731 students). The
results showed that teachers’ didactic interest and self-efficacy predicted teacher reports of instruc-
Keywords: tional practices. In contrast, student reports of instruction were significantly associated with teachers’
Teacher motivation educational interest and mastery goals. Moreover, student motivation was only related with student reports
Student motivation
but not teacher reports of instructional practices. In particular, mastery-oriented practices contributed
Interest
strongly to student motivation. Teacher educational interest predicted mastery-oriented practices and
Mastery goals
Self-efficacy also showed a significant direct relation to student motivation.
Instructional practices © 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction these analyses in order to enable a more rigorous test of the pre-
dictive contributions of teacher interests and mastery goals.
There is a growing interest of educational researchers in teacher In the following, we briefly review previous findings related to
motivation and its role in teaching behavior and stress or burnout teachers’ interests, goal orientations, and self-efficacy. The focus of
(Butler, 2007; Fernet, Senécal, Guay, Marsh, & Dowson, 2008; Kunter this review is on the empirical evidence relating these constructs
et al., 2008; Pelletier, Séguin-Lévesque, & Legault, 2002; Retelsdorf, to teachers’ instructional practices and student motivation.
Butler, Streblow, & Schiefele, 2010; Watt & Richardson, 2008).
Whereas past research has mainly focused on teacher self-efficacy
beliefs (e.g., Klassen, Usher, & Bong, 2010; see overviews by Klassen, 1.1. Teacher interests
Tze, Betts, & Gordon, 2011; Tschannen-Moran, Woolfolk Hoy, & Hoy,
1998), recent efforts to conceptualize dimensions of teacher mo- In accordance with recent theories of interest (cf. Hidi &
tivation include goal orientations (Butler, 2007) and intrinsic or self- Renninger, 2006; Hidi, Renninger, & Krapp, 2004; Krapp, 2005, 2007;
determined motivation (Pelletier et al., 2002). In our own previous Renninger & Hidi, 2011; Schiefele, 2009), teacher interests are con-
work (Schiefele, Streblow, & Retelsdorf, 2013), we have proposed ceptualized as individual interests that refer to relatively permanent
teacher interests as potentially relevant determinants of teacher well- attractions to certain topics or domains (e.g., school subjects, spe-
being and instructional practices. The present study is aimed at cific knowledge fields). These attractions are defined as intrinsic
extending that work by taking teacher mastery goals as an addi- valence beliefs which denote cognitively represented relations
tional predictor into account, by assessing instructional practices between a domain (e.g., physics) and both feeling- and value-
not only by teacher ratings but also by student ratings, and by in- related attributes (e.g., excitement, relevance for one’s self; cf. Hidi
cluding students’ self-reported motivation as an outcome variable. & Renninger, 2006; Krapp, 2007; Schiefele, 2009). Accordingly, in-
It was assumed that teacher interests and mastery goals predict dividual interest involves perceptions of positive feelings and
teachers’ instructional practices (as perceived by teachers or stu- personal importance being attached to a given domain.
dents) that in turn contribute to students’ motivation. Because of Schiefele et al. (2013) proposed three dimensions of teacher in-
its demonstrated importance, teacher self-efficacy was included in terest: subject, didactic, and educational interest. Subject interest is
understood as the interest in the subject matter taught (e.g.,
mathematics).1 Didactic interest refers to a teacher’s interest in teach-
ing methods. This includes, for example, a preference for literature
The authors wish to thank Lilian Streblow, Andrea Maczay, Julia Bonin, and on didactics or placing strong personal value on the issue of
Katharina Biermann for their contributions to the collection of data and the devel-
opment of instruments.
* Corresponding author. Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam,
Karl-Liebknecht-Str. 24–25, DE-14476 Potsdam, Germany. Fax: +49 (0)331 977 2091. 1
For a similar concept, see Kunter, Frenzel, Nagy, Baumert, and Pekrun’s (2011)
E-mail address: ulrich.schiefele@uni-potsdam.de (U. Schiefele). definition of “subject enthusiasm”.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.06.005
0361-476X/© 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
160 U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171

effective teaching methods. Educational interest pertains to the in- and cognitive activation). Only a significant association between
terest in the educational or pedagogical aspect of the teaching subject enthusiasm and teacher-rated cognitively activating prac-
profession. This aspect concerns the appropriate pedagogical han- tices was observed.
dling of students in general and problem students in particular.
Whereas didactics or teaching methods focus on students’ learn- 1.2. Teacher mastery goals
ing of subject matter knowledge, educational or pedagogical activities
of the teacher are mostly directed at students’ development of ef- Butler (2007) was among the first to apply achievement goal
ficient work habits, social competencies, and moral values (Van Veen, theory as a framework for conceptualizing teachers’ motivation for
Sleegers, Bergen, & Klaassen, 2001). teaching (see also Papaioannou & Christodoulidis, 2007). She argued
The differentiation into three dimensions of teacher interest is that the classroom constitutes an achievement arena not only for
similar to the components of professional knowledge distin- students but also for teachers. Accordingly, teachers strive to succeed
guished in the literature (e.g., Krauss et al., 2008; Phelps & Schilling, at their job but may differ in the ways they define success and, thus,
2004; Shulman, 1986, 1987). Most notably, Shulman (1987) em- in their achievement goals for teaching. In accordance with goal
phasized two components of teacher knowledge: content knowledge theory referring to students, Butler distinguished between teach-
(domain-specific subject matter knowledge) and pedagogical content ers’ mastery goals, ability-approach, ability-avoidance, and work-
knowledge (the knowledge needed for teaching a specific subject avoidance goals (cf. Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996; Grant & Dweck,
and to make it comprehensible to others). These two dimensions 2003; Nicholls, 1989; Urdan & Maehr, 1995). All forms of goal ori-
seemingly correspond to subject interest and to didactic interest. entations are theorized to be cognitively represented beliefs (e.g.,
In addition, Shulman’s (1987) categories of general pedagogical knowl- “I strive to attain better grades than my classmates”; Elliot, 2005).
edge (which refers to principles and strategies of classroom Mastery-oriented teachers seek to improve their professional com-
management and organization) and knowledge of educational ends, petence. They evaluate their competence relative to task demands
purposes, and values are related to the concept of educational in- or prior outcomes and are likely to show a preference for chal-
terest because both forms of knowledge refer to issues of educating lenge. Teachers with ability-approach goals strive to demonstrate
students (cf. Schiefele et al., 2013). superior teaching ability, whereas teachers with ability-avoidance
Schiefele et al. (2013) provided evidence for the validity of their goals are focused on avoiding the demonstration of inferior teach-
interest concept by means of confirmatory factor analysis, by ana- ing ability. Finally, work-avoidance goals reflect strivings to reduce
lyzing differences in interest between teachers from different school work load and effort. Work-avoidant teachers feel successful when
tracks, and by examining the contributions of the dimensions of in- they get through the day with little effort.
terest to the prediction of teacher reports of burnout symptoms, In the present study, we focused on teachers’ mastery goals
quality of experience in class, and instructional practices. For because they have been found to be significantly related to adap-
example, the authors found that elementary school teachers ex- tive instructional practices. For example, Butler and Shibaz (2008)
hibited higher educational interest than secondary school teachers. reported that teachers with higher levels of mastery goals were per-
This was to be expected because educational aspects of teaching ceived by their students as providing stronger support of question-
are more dominant in elementary teachers’ training and their daily asking and help-seeking, whereas other goal orientations showed
school work (e.g., Brookhart & Freeman, 1992). Moreover, didactic either nonsignificant or negative effects on teacher support.
and educational interest contributed to lower levels of burnout, Retelsdorf et al. (2010) confirmed the hypothesis that teachers with
whereas subject and educational interest were the main predic- stronger mastery goals, who are themselves oriented to learn and
tors of teachers’ positive experience in class. More importantly, acquire competence, are more likely to use mastery-oriented and
didactic and educational interest significantly predicted teacher cognitively activating practices (see also Shim, Cho, & Cassady, 2013).
reports of mastery-oriented practices (e.g., recognizing students’ in- Again, other goal orientations did not positively predict adaptive in-
dividual progress), even when controlling for teacher self-efficacy. structional behavior.
In addition, didactic interest was significantly associated with Previous research on achievement goals also suggests a sub-
cognitively activating practices (e.g., providing challenging and stimu- stantial relationship between classroom-level goal structures
lating tasks), whereas subject interest did not contribute significantly established by the teacher and students’ personal goal orienta-
to instructional practices. tions (e.g., Urdan, 2010; Urdan & Schoenfelder, 2006). Usually,
The reported contributions of didactic interest to instructional classroom goal structures have been assessed by means of stu-
practices were explained by the assumption that interest in teach- dents’ perceptions of teachers’ practices that either reflect an
ing methods enhances the motivation to learn more about efficient emphasis on mastery goals (i.e., promoting learning and under-
instructional practices and, thus, increases the use of mastery- standing) or on performance goals (i.e., promoting social comparison
oriented and cognitively activating practices (Schiefele et al., 2013). and competition). There is ample evidence that mastery goal struc-
Educational interest, however, involves a focus on the pedagogical tures in the classroom predict students’ mastery goals, whereas
handling of students and their individual development. This focus performance goal structures predict students’ performance (or
probably facilitates mastery-oriented practices because these are ability) goals (Meece, Anderman, & Anderman, 2006; Urdan, 2010;
beneficial for students’ competence perceptions and motivation Urdan & Schoenfelder, 2006). Moreover, mastery goal structures con-
(Givens Rolland, 2012). In contrast, cognitively activating prac- tribute positively to students’ competence beliefs, motivational
tices are more closely associated with students’ cognitive learning engagement (e.g., effort), strategy use, and academic achievement
processes (Kunter et al., 2013) and therefore may depend less (Givens Rolland, 2012; Urdan, 2010; Wolters, 2004). In contrast, per-
strongly on educational interest. formance goal structures do not or negatively (depending on the
In contrast to Schiefele et al. (2013), Long and Woolfolk Hoy grade level) contribute to these outcome measures (see also Meece
(2006) demonstrated a substantive relation between teachers’ subject et al., 2006).
interest and their instructional effectiveness (e.g., competence,
clarity). However, the measures of both teachers’ interest and in- 1.3. Teacher self-efficacy
structional effectiveness were based on student ratings. In line with
Schiefele et al., Kunter et al. (2008) found that teachers’ self- Numerous studies have considered possible influences of teacher
reported subject enthusiasm did not predict teachers’ and students’ self-efficacy on the quality of teaching and student motivation.
ratings of various instructional practices (monitoring, social support, Teacher self-efficacy represents teachers’ belief that they are able
U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171 161

to perform those teaching behaviors that bring about student learn- subject, didactic, and educational interest, mastery goals, teaching
ing even when students are difficult or unmotivated (Klassen et al., self-efficacy, and instructional practices. In addition, the students
2011; Ross, 1998; Wolters & Daugherty, 2007). Thereby, previous in the classrooms of the participating teachers should rate their
research has employed both uni- and multidimensional measures teachers’ instructional practices and their own subject interest and
of teacher self-efficacy (cf. Dellinger, Bobbett, Olivier, & Ellett, 2008; mastery goals.
Tschannen-Moran et al., 1998). For example, Tschannen-Moran and As instructional practices, mastery-oriented and cognitively ac-
Woolfolk Hoy (2001) distinguished among teachers’ sense of effi- tivating teaching methods were assessed (cf. Retelsdorf et al., 2010).
cacy with regard to effective instructional strategies, classroom Teachers who endorse mastery-oriented practices emphasize effort,
management, and student engagement. evaluate students’ progress relative to prior attainment, adapt tasks
According to self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1997), teachers’ belief to students’ interests and abilities, and treat student errors as op-
in their teaching ability motivates them to actually use effective portunities for further learning (Meece et al., 2006; Midgley et al.,
teaching practices and to exert effort in preparing their lessons. In 2000). Cognitively activating methods refer to the degree to which
a review of relevant research, Ross (1998; see also Tschannen-Moran teachers provide challenging and stimulating tasks, emphasize crit-
et al., 1998) reported, for example, that self-efficacious teachers apply ical and independent thinking, and embrace unusual ideas (Kunter
more elaborate instruction methods (e.g., activity-based methods), et al., 2008; Retelsdorf et al., 2010). Previous studies have docu-
have a more open attitude toward new methods (e.g., implement- mented that students learn better and are more motivated (a) when
ing a new program), and show either adaptive or supportive teaching emphasis is placed on mastery of the demands of learning and on
behavior (e.g., tolerating mistakes, fostering student autonomy). Ac- individual progress, and not on competing for good grades and (b)
cording to Ross, similar findings were produced by studies using when lessons are cognitively stimulating and encourage cognitive
observations of teachers’ practices and those relying on self- autonomy (e.g., Klusmann, Kunter, Trautwein, Lüdtke, & Baumert,
reported behavior. More recently, Wolters and Daugherty (2007) and 2008; Kunter et al., 2008; Retelsdorf et al., 2010). As a third dimen-
Schiefele et al. (2013) provided evidence for significant relation- sion of teaching quality, not being considered here, classroom
ships between teachers’ sense of efficacy and their self-reported management has been identified (Baumert et al., 2010; Fauth,
mastery-oriented and cognitively activating practices. Finally, sig- Decristan, Rieser, Klieme, & Büttner, 2014).
nificant associations between teacher self-efficacy and indicators An important feature of the present study refers to the assess-
of student motivation (cf. Ross, 1998; Tschannen-Moran et al., 1998), ment of instructional practices by means of teacher and student
in particular student self-efficacy (e.g., Midgley, Feldlaufer, & Eccles, ratings. Previous research has indicated that the agreement between
1989), have been obtained. teacher and student ratings of teachers’ instructional behavior is
Recent evidence suggests that different facets of teacher self- rather low because teachers and students rate instructional events
efficacy may have different effects on outcome variables. In particular, differently (Clausen, 2002; Hamre & Pianta, 2010; Kunter & Baumert,
Wang, Hall, and Rahimi (2015) demonstrated that self-efficacy for 2006). For example, only weak or nonsignificant associations
student engagement proved to be a stronger predictor of teachers’ between teacher and student reports of mastery-oriented prac-
psychological well-being and quitting intentions than self-efficacy tices in the classroom were reported (e.g., Butler, 2012; Deemer,
for classroom management and instructional strategies. More- 2004; Ryan, Gheen, & Midgley, 1998). Both teacher and student
over, Wolters and Daugherty (2007) found significant contributions ratings of instruction have been criticized to be subject to specific
to mastery-oriented practices only for teachers’ efficacy for instruc- biases (Bieg, Backes, & Mittag, 2011; Hamre & Pianta, 2010; Kunter
tional strategies and student engagement but not for teachers’ & Baumert, 2006). In particular, teacher ratings may be influenced
efficacy for classroom management. by teaching ideals or self-serving strategies and thus tend to be more
favorable than student ratings. In contrast, student ratings are pos-
sibly affected by perceived teacher popularity or grading practice.
1.4. The current research Despite these potential biases, evidence on the validity of both
teacher and student ratings of instructional practices has been pro-
Taken together, previous research suggests that teachers’ didac- vided. For example, teacher reports of cognitively activating and
tic and educational interest, mastery goals, and self-efficacy are mastery-oriented practices have been found to be significantly as-
substantially associated with the use of adaptive teaching prac- sociated with relevant student variables such as perceived homework
tices. Notable research deficits pertain to three issues: (a) Teachers’ difficulty, subject interest, goal orientations, and achievement (e.g.,
interests and mastery goals have been only examined separately. Kunter & Baumert, 2006; O’Keefe, Ben-Eliyahu, & Linnenbrink-Garcia,
Moreover, the contributions of mastery goals have not been tested 2013; Urdan & Schoenfelder, 2006). Significant relations with ex-
against those of self-efficacy. Thus, the unique contributions of ternal criteria have been also demonstrated for student reports of
teacher interests and mastery goals to the prediction of instruc- instructional practices (e.g., Kunter et al., 2007; Meece et al., 2006;
tional behavior remain unclear. (b) In research on teacher interests O’Keefe et al., 2013). In particular, Fauth et al. (2014) showed sat-
and mastery goals, the assessment of teachers’ instructional prac- isfying factorial validity for students’ ratings of instructional practices
tices has been mostly based on teacher ratings. Accordingly, we do and confirmed their predictive contributions to student achieve-
not have substantial evidence on whether teacher interests and ment and motivation when controlling for teacher popularity. Taken
mastery goals make a difference in students’ perceptions of instruc- together, teacher and student ratings of instructional practices have
tional behavior. (c) Finally, previous studies on teacher interests and a certain degree of validity although they represent different per-
mastery goals have neglected student outcome variables such as spectives and are subject to specific biases. Thus, it seemed warranted
student motivation and achievement (Butler, 2012). to assess instructional practices by both teacher and student ratings.
In line with the deficits of previous studies, two main goals for Reliance on only a single source of ratings may not adequately rep-
the present research were determined: (a) Direct comparison of the resent the classroom reality.
contributions of teachers’ interests and mastery goals to the pre- We hypothesized that didactic interest, mastery goals, and self-
diction of both teacher- and student-rated instructional practices, efficacy contribute uniquely and positively to the use of mastery-
and (b) analysis of the associations between teachers’ motiva- oriented and cognitively activating instructional practices.
tional characteristics, teacher and student reports of instructional Educational interest should contribute uniquely and positively
practices, and student motivation. In order to address these re- only to mastery-oriented practices, whereas no significant
search goals, we asked elementary school teachers to indicate their contributions to instructional practices were assumed for teacher
162 U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171

subject interest. Moreover, the assumed contributions of didactic female students show greater interest in German and literature (e.g.,
and educational interest, mastery goals, and self-efficacy to instruc- Lee, Lee, & Bong, 2014; Schiefele et al., 1992). Because a variety of
tional practices were expected to be stronger for teacher reports than different subjects was involved in the present study, main effects
for student reports of instruction (cf. Butler, 2012). of student gender on both mastery goals and subject interest did
These hypotheses are based on the theoretical assumptions and not seem likely.
research findings outlined in the preceding sections. Specifically, the
significant role of teachers’ self-efficacy for the implementation of
adaptive instructional practices has been demonstrated by several 2. Method
studies (Ross, 1998; Schiefele et al., 2013; Tschannen-Moran et al.,
1998; Wolters & Daugherty, 2007). Schiefele et al. (2013) reported 2.1. Participants and procedure
that didactic interest predicted mastery-oriented and cognitively
activating practices, whereas educational interest was associated The sample comprised 110 teachers (20 male, 90 female) from
only with mastery-oriented practices. Finally, Retelsdorf et al. (2010; 30 elementary schools and their students in and around a large city
see also Shim et al., 2013) found significant contributions of teach- in Germany. The schools represented both rural and urban areas of
ers’ mastery goals to mastery-oriented and cognitively activating different socioeconomic backgrounds. The average age of the par-
practices. ticipating teachers (M = 47.57, SD = 9.26) was rather close to the
Pertaining to the prediction of students’ subject interest and average age of all German teachers (M = 48.1; Statistisches Bundesamt
mastery goals, we hypothesized only indirect contributions of teacher [Federal Statistical Office], 2008).
motivation that are mediated by teachers’ instructional practices. A total of 35 schools were contacted by telephone and asked to
Specifically, we anticipated that mastery-oriented and cognitively participate. School principals were given detailed information on
activating practices contribute positively to students’ subject in- the goals of the study and the contents of the questionnaires. Thirty
terest and mastery goals. In accordance with previous findings (cf. of these schools (85.7%) agreed to participate. Within the partici-
Meece et al., 2006; Woolfolk Hoy & Weinstein, 2006), we ex- pating schools, only fifth- and sixth-grade classes and one of their
pected the relations between instruction and student motivation major teachers were asked to take part.2 The response rate of classes
to be closer for student- than for teacher-rated instructional across all participating schools was 82.7%. We chose to include stu-
approaches. dents beyond fourth grade because the instruments used in the
The hypothesized associations between instructional practices present study to assess students’ motivation and their percep-
and aspects of student motivation were based on previous consid- tions of instructional practices had not been validated for the early
erations and findings suggesting that mastery-oriented practices elementary grades.
facilitate students’ mastery goals and subject interest (Fauth et al., Elementary school teachers in Germany usually focus their teach-
2014; Meece et al., 2006; Midgley, 2002; Urdan, 2010). Moreover, ing on either German or mathematics. Besides their major subject,
Klusmann et al. (2008) reported a significant association between they teach at least one more subject (e.g., English), which in some
cognitively activating practices and students’ intrinsic motivation instances may become their major teaching subject. Accordingly,
(i.e., experience of competence and autonomy). Due to the multi- reports on the main teaching subject revealed that German (n = 43;
level structure of the student data, the predictive relations between 39.1%) and mathematics (n = 34; 30.9%) were most frequent in the
student reports of instructional practices and student motivation present teacher sample. The remaining teachers (n = 33; 30.0%) were
have to be modeled both at the individual or within-class level and distributed across different subjects (English, geography, social
the between-class level. The present hypotheses referring to the re- science, biology, and music).
lations between students’ perceived instructional practices and their The student sample (N = 1731) included 881 fifth graders (n = 421
motivation were assumed to apply to both the within-class and the female, n = 448 male, n = 12 missing indication of gender) and 850
between-class level. sixth graders (n = 413 female, n = 427 male, n = 10 missing indica-
Finally, gender was included in the present analyses because pre- tion of gender). The number of students per classroom varied
vious studies have reported substantial effects of teacher and student between 6 and 29. The mean age of the student sample was 11.08
gender. Specifically, it has been found that female teachers score (SD = 0.79; age range 9–14; fifth grade: M = 10.58, SD = .61; sixth
higher than male teachers on mastery goals, self-reported mastery- grade: M = 11.59, SD = .59; three students failed to indicate their age).
oriented practices, student-centered educational beliefs, and There were slight but significant age differences between girls
motivation of becoming a teacher (Butler, 2007, 2012; Retelsdorf (M = 11.02, SD = .79) and boys (M = 11.13, SD = .79), F(1,1702) = 14.76,
et al., 2010; Sabbe & Aelterman, 2007). Thus, it was expected that p < .001, d = .14.
female teachers show higher levels of all motivational variables and The teachers and their students were simultaneously pre-
mastery-oriented instruction than male teachers. However, in ac- sented with a questionnaire during a regular lesson. Before working
cordance with Retelsdorf et al. (2010), gender differences in the use on the questionnaire, the teachers were instructed to answer all ques-
of cognitively activating practices were not assumed. tions with respect to their major teaching subject, and the students
Although past research has demonstrated gender effects in re- were asked to respond to the questionnaire with respect to their
lation to students’ mastery goals and subject interests (e.g., Lee, Ning, teacher’s major subject. The largest number of students answered
& Goh, 2014; Meece & Holt, 1993; Schiefele, Krapp, & Winteler, 1992), the questionnaire pertaining to German (n = 685), followed by math-
such effects were not expected in the present study. With respect ematics (n = 522), English (n = 297), geography (n = 105), social
to student mastery goals, previous studies have yielded mixed results science (n = 70), biology (n = 40), and music (n = 12). The distribu-
for gender differences. Whereas some studies have found that girls tion across subjects was highly similar for girls and boys, and for
showed higher scores in mastery goals than boys, other studies have fifth and sixth graders.
reported no differences in students’ endorsement of mastery goals
(Kenney-Benson, Pomerantz, Ryan, & Patrick, 2006; Patrick, Ryan,
& Pintrich, 1999). Additional research (e.g., Anderman & Midgley,
1997) has suggested that the relation between gender and mastery
goals differs by subject. The latter point also applies to the rela- 2 In most states (Bundesländer) in Germany, elementary schools only include four
tion between gender and subject interest. For example, male students grades, whereas in a few states, such as Berlin or Brandenburg, elementary schools
have been found to show greater interest in mathematics, while comprise Grades 1–6.
U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171 163

Table 1 2.2.4. Teacher reports of instructional practices


Mean values, standard deviations, and reliabilities. The assessment of teacher-perceived mastery-oriented prac-
Variable Teacher (N = 110) Student (N = 1731) tices was based on the Pattern of Adaptive Learning Scales (PALS;
M SD α M SD α ICC1 ICC2 Midgley et al., 2000). The German adaption of PALS was devel-
oped and evaluated by Retelsdorf et al. (2010). Sample items of the
Subject interest 3.19 .57 .63 2.65 .71 .75 .139 .72
Didactic interest 3.27 .55 .83
mastery instruction subscale (4 items) are “In my class, I make a
Educational interest 3.31 .47 .74 special effort to recognize students’ individual progress, even if they
Mastery goals 3.10 .53 .68 3.16 .54 .80 .078 .57 are below average” and “In my class, the individual development
Self-efficacy 2.96 .42 .67 of my students is most important to me.”
Mastery-oriented IP 3.44 .38 .67 3.29 .58 .68 .227 .82
The scale measuring cognitively activating practices was taken
Cognitively activating IP 2.92 .40 .74 2.65 .49 .69 .122 .68
from the project Professional Competence of Teachers, Cognitively
IP = instructional practices.
Activating Instruction, and the Development of Students’ Mathe-
matical Literacy (COACTIV; e.g., Kunter et al., 2007). The COACTIV
project was conducted as part of the Programme for International
2.2. Measures Student Assessment (PISA) in Germany. The scale on cognitively ac-
tivating practices describes the level of challenge and autonomy
Items of all scales required responses on 4-point rating scales provided by tasks and instruction (8 items; e.g., “I let my students
ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (very true). All items were coded work on tasks for which they should provide more than one solu-
with a high score indicating a high level on the given construct. tion”; “I change the setting of tasks to find out whether my students
Reliabilities for all teacher and student variables are reported in have really understood the underlying idea”). By means of confir-
Table 1. matory factor analysis, Schiefele et al. (2013) provided evidence that
cognitively activating and mastery-oriented practices represent sep-
2.2.1. Teacher interests arate factors.
Teacher interests were assessed by means of the Teacher Inter-
est Scale (TIS) developed by Schiefele et al. (2013). The TIS involves 2.2.5. Student subject interest
three subscales: subject interest (4 items; e.g. “Being involved with Student subject interest was measured by means of a five-item
my subject puts me in a good mood”; “Dealing frequently with con- scale that is based on interest theory (cf. Schiefele, 2009) and has
tents of my subject is important to me”), didactic interest (4 items; been used in a number of studies on text comprehension in order
e.g. “I like to think about ways of making my teaching more effec- to assess interest in text topics (e.g., Schaffner & Schiefele, 2008).
tive and motivating”; “It is important to me to ensure that my In line with the definition of individual interest (Hidi & Renninger,
teaching methods are always up to date”), and educational inter- 2006; Krapp, 2007; Schiefele, 2009), the scale comprises three items
est (5 items; e.g. “The most interesting aspect of my work pertains referring to feeling-related valence beliefs (e.g., “Being involved with
to the pedagogical handling of students”; “The appropriate han- this subject puts me in a good mood”) and two items referring to
dling of problem students is an important topic for me”). Schiefele value-related valence beliefs (e.g., “The lessons in this subject are
et al. (2013) have used multiple-group confirmatory factor analy- very important to me”).
sis to confirm (a) the three-factor structure of the TIS and (b) the
assumption of scalar measurement invariance (e.g., Wu, Li, & Zumbo, 2.2.6. Student mastery goals
2007) of these factors across teachers from different school tracks We used the German SELLMO scales (Spinath, Stiensmeier-Pelster,
(elementary and both low- and high-achievement secondary Schöne, & Dickhäuser, 2002) for measuring students’ mastery goals
schools). (8 items). The opening stem of this questionnaire was “In German/
mathematics/English/geography/social science/biology/music class,
2.2.2. Teacher mastery goals it is important to me that …”. Sample items are “I learn as much
To capture teacher mastery goals, the Goal Orientations for Teach- as possible” and “I really understand what is being taught”.
ing Scale developed by Butler (2007, Hebrew version) and Retelsdorf
(2006, German version; see also Retelsdorf et al., 2010) was applied. 2.2.7. Student reports of instructional practices
Sample items from the mastery goals subscale (4 items) are: “In my The assessment of students’ perceptions of instructional prac-
work as a teacher, I feel good and successful if …” [opening stem tices was based on the corresponding teacher scales. These scales
for all items] “something that happened in class made me want to were rewritten so that they directly addressed the perspective of
learn more about teaching” and “I saw that I was developing pro- students. For example, the mastery-oriented item “In my class, I make
fessionally and teaching more effectively than in the past.” a special effort to recognize students’ individual progress, even if
they are below average” was changed to “Our teacher makes a special
2.2.3. Teacher self-efficacy effort to recognize progress of all students, even if they are below
In order to measure teachers’ perception of self-efficacy, the Self- average”. Consequently, there was a close correspondence between
Efficacy for Teaching Scale (SETS) was used. This scale represents a the teacher and student scales.
global or unidimensional indicator of teacher self-efficacy and was It should be noted that all items of the student questionnaire
originally developed by Schwarzer and Schmitz (1999; Schwarzer on instructional practices explicitly referred to students’ teachers.
& Hallum, 2008) and slightly modified by Schiefele et al. (2013). The As has been recently demonstrated by Koskey, Karabenick, Woolley,
SETS consists of five items and asks teachers to rate their ability to Bonney, and Dever (2010), survey items used to assess the class-
cope successfully with various demands of the teaching profes- room mastery goal structure show higher degrees of validity when
sion. In accordance with previous definitions of teacher self- they are framed to focus on students’ teachers instead of the class-
efficacy (cf. Klassen et al., 2011; Ross, 1998; Wolters & Daugherty, room context.
2007), these demands refer in particular to finding ways of dealing
with difficult students (e.g., “I am able to communicate well with 2.3. Missing values
rather difficult students if I try”) and to instructional strategies to
facilitate student learning (e.g., “I can teach even the most prob- Missing values were estimated with the expectation-
lematic students what they will need for their exams”). maximization (EM) algorithm computed by means of the statistical
164 U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171

software NORM 2.03 (Schafer, 1999; see also Graham, 2009). On Table 2
average, only 0.6% missing values in the teacher sample and 1.7% Correlations among teacher variables.

missing values in the student sample were observed for individu- Teacher variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
al items. The highest percentage of missing values per item was 2.5% 1. Gendera –
in the teacher sample and 4.3% in the student sample. 2. Subject interest .08 –
3. Didactic interest .37*** .25** –
2.4. Statistical analyses 4. Educational .30** .31** .42*** –
interest
5. Mastery goals .33*** .41*** .53*** .40*** –
In order to examine the hypotheses pertaining to the contribu- 6. Self-efficacy .21* .36*** .34*** .42*** .31** –
tions of teacher motivation and teacher reports of instructional 7. Mastery-oriented IP .39*** .17 .47*** .41*** .21* .42*** –
practices to student reports of instructional practices and student 8. Cognitively .32*** .10 .48*** .26** .30** .43*** .54*** –
activating IP
motivation, we conducted multilevel means-as-outcomes regres-
sion analyses with fixed effects (by using Mplus 7.3; Muthén & N = 110.
IP = instructional practices.
Muthén, 1998–2014). Thus, it was possible to account for the mul- a Scoring of gender: male = 1, female = 2.
tilevel structure of the student data and differentiate between the * p < .05, two-tailed.
portions of variance in the dependent student variables that are due ** p < .01, two-tailed.
to factors at the individual and the class level. *** p < .001, two-tailed.
As a necessary prerequisite of the multilevel analyses, the re-
liability of the aggregated student variables was assessed by means
of the intraclass correlations ICC1 and ICC2 (Bliese, 2000). The ICC1 of subject matter affiliation on teacher variables, significant mean
indicates the proportion of total variance in student ratings that is differences were observed between students referring to mathe-
located between classes, reflecting systematic between-class dif- matics and miscellaneous subjects. Specifically, compared to students
ferences in instructional ratings or student motivation. Values of 5% in classes with miscellaneous subjects, students in mathematics
and above for ICC1 can be regarded as support that there are ade- classes indicated lower levels of subject interest, M = 2.59, SD = .68
quate class-level properties of a variable to warrant aggregation (e.g., versus M = 2.73, SD = .76, p < .01 (Bonferroni post hoc test), and higher
Gavin & Hofmann, 2002). The ICC2 indicates the reliability of the levels of cognitively activating practices, M = 2.70, SD = .47 versus
class mean. It is calculated by applying a Spearman–Brown correc- M = 2.60, SD = .50, p < .01 (Bonferroni post hoc test). These differ-
tion formula (based on the average number of students per class) ences are theoretically plausible but rather small in size, d = .19, for
to the ICC1, thus taking into account that measurement error de- subject interest, and d = .21, for cognitively activating practices.
creases with increasing numbers of raters. Rating of the class mean Student-based ratings of instructional practices differed signifi-
can be considered reliable if student ratings differ systematically cantly from teachers’ self-ratings, t(218) = 3.45, p < .001, d = .47, for
between classes (as measured by the ICC1) and there are many raters mastery-oriented practices, and t(218) = 7.47, p < .001, d = 1.03, for
per class (a fact that informs the ICC2). ICC2 values of approximate- cognitively activating practices (two-tailed tests). Obviously, teach-
ly .70 are considered to indicate that the assessment of the class- ers judged their instructional practices more favorably than did their
level construct is sufficiently reliable (Bliese, 2000). According to students. The largest discrepancy was obtained for cognitively ac-
this criterion, the aggregated scale scores for student subject in- tivating practices (see Table 1).
terest and student reports of mastery-oriented and cognitively
activating instructional practices can be seen as reliable indica- 3.2. Contributions of teacher motivation to teacher reports of
tors of the respective class level in the present study (see Table 1). instructional practices
Although ICC2 for student mastery goals (.57) was somewhat below
the critical value of .70, we decided to retain this variable in the The correlations between teacher interests and instructional prac-
succeeding analyses. tices were in line with the hypotheses (see Table 2). Specifically,
didactic and educational interest were both positively associated
3. Results with mastery-oriented and cognitively activating practices, whereas
subject interest was not significantly related to instructional prac-
3.1. Descriptive statistics and mean differences tices. Moreover, both teacher mastery goals and self-efficacy
positively predicted mastery-oriented and cognitively activating prac-
Mean values and standard deviations of teacher motivation vari- tices. Finally, female teachers scored higher on all motivational
ables, teacher- and student-rated instructional practices, and variables and mastery-oriented practices than male teachers. The
components of student motivation are shown in Table 1. In line with significant association between gender and cognitively activating
previous findings (Butler, 2012; Retelsdorf et al., 2010; Schiefele et al., practices, however, was not expected.3
2013), teachers reported relatively high levels of all three dimen- The hypothesized unique contributions of teacher motivation to
sions of interest, mastery goals, mastery-oriented instruction, and teacher reports of instructional practices were examined by means
cognitively activating practices. Also according to previous re- of multiple regression analyses. Thereby, teacher gender and the di-
search (Spinath et al., 2002), students indicated relatively high levels mensions of teacher motivation were simultaneously entered as
of mastery goals. predictors into the regression models. The findings (see Table 3)
We explored whether subject matter affiliation affected teacher support several of the hypotheses. First, didactic interest and self-
and student motivation as well as teacher and student reports of efficacy contributed significantly to mastery-oriented and cognitively
instructional practices. For that purpose, we compared the mean activating practices. Second, subject interest was not significantly
values of teacher motivation factors and teacher reports of instruc- related to instructional practices. Third, female teachers indicated
tional practices between teachers focusing on German (n = 43), greater use of mastery-oriented practices than male teachers.
mathematics (n = 34), or miscellaneous subjects (n = 33). In addi- However, the observed positive association between educational
tion, differences in student motivation factors and student reports
of instructional practices between students referring to German
(n = 685), mathematics (n = 522), or miscellaneous subjects (n = 524) 3
As is shown in the following section, this association became nonsignificant when
were analyzed. Whereas the findings did not reveal significant effects including other predictors.
U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171 165

Table 3 sequently, most of the hypothesized associations between teacher


Contributions of teacher motivation to teacher reports of instructional practices. motivation and student reports of instruction were not con-
Teacher variable Teacher reports firmed. In particular, the relations between instructional practices
Mastery-oriented IP Cognitively activating IP and both didactic interest and self-efficacy did not attain signifi-
a
cance. In line with the hypotheses, however, significant correlations
Gender .22* (.08) .14 (.07)
Subject interest −.00 (.07) −.11 (.06)
between aggregated student reports of mastery-oriented prac-
Didactic interest .33** (.07) .34** (.06) tices and both teacher mastery goals and educational interest were
Educational interest .18c (.08) −.05 (.07) obtained. Also according to expectations, female teachers were per-
Mastery goals −.19b (.07) .04 (.06) ceived by their students as being more mastery-oriented in their
Self-efficacy .24* (.08) .33*** (.07)
teaching than male teachers.
R2 .38 .34
The correlational findings in Table 4 provide only a first indica-
N = 110. Standardized regression coefficients; standard errors in parentheses.
tion of the contributions of teacher motivation to student reports
IP = instructional practices.
a
Scoring of gender: male = 1, female = 2. of instruction because they do not consider the multilevel struc-
b p = .065. ture of the student data. Therefore, multilevel means-as-outcomes
c p = .053.
regression analyses were performed. These analyses included student
* p < .05. gender and student age as Level 1 predictors and teacher gender
** p < .01.
and the components of teacher motivation as Level 2 predictors (cf.
*** p < .001.
Butler, 2012). Students’ age was included at Level 1 because we theo-
rized that older students exhibit lower achievement levels within
Table 4 classes than younger students and, thus, may rate their teachers’
Correlations between teacher variables and aggregated class-level student variables. instructional practices differently. In particular, older students prob-
ably tend to perceive cognitively activating practices to a higher
Teacher variable Aggregated class-level student variable
degree than younger students. The lower achievement level of older
Mastery- Cognitively Subject Mastery
students is to be explained by grade retention and by retarded school
oriented IP activating IP interest goals
admittance due to developmental deficits.
Gendera .33*** .07 .13 .19 The resulting regression models for student reports of instruc-
Subject interest .05 −.01 .28** .06
Didactic interest .19 .12 .21* .21*
tion are presented in Table 5. First, it should be noted that student
Educational interest .34*** .12 .30** .23* gender did not affect student reports of instructional practices.
Mastery goals .27** .19 .24* .21* However, according to our assumptions, older students reported
Self-efficacy .18 .02 .18 .20* stronger use of cognitively activating practices than younger stu-
Mastery-oriented IP .19* .03 .15 .23*
dents. Moreover, at Level 2, teacher gender showed the expected
Cognitively activating IP .18 .14 .03 .15
positive association with mastery-oriented instruction indicating
NTeachers = 110. NStudents = 1706.
that female teachers were perceived by their students as using
IP = instructional practices.
a
Scoring of gender: male = 1, female = 2.
mastery-oriented practices to a higher degree than male teachers.
* p < .05, two-tailed. This finding is in line with the obtained association between gender
** p < .01, two-tailed. and teacher reports of mastery-oriented practices (see Table 3).
*** p < .001, two-tailed. Interestingly, the results showed that those teacher motivation
factors that predicted teacher reports of instruction, i.e. self-
efficacy and didactic interest, did not significantly contribute to
interest and mastery-oriented practices fell short of attaining student reports of instruction. In contrast, teachers’ educational in-
significance. terest and mastery goals were significantly associated with student
In contrast to expectations, teacher mastery goals failed to predict reports of instruction (see Table 5). Whereas teachers’ educational
instructional practices positively when controlling for other rele- interest predicted student reports of their mastery-oriented prac-
vant predictors. The fact that even a negative regression coefficient tices, teachers’ mastery goals contributed to student reports of their
for mastery goals was obtained might be due to the relatively large cognitively activating practices.
amount of shared variance among mastery goals and the dimen- In the next step of the analyses, we examined the relations
sions of interest.4 Therefore, we tested an additional regression model between teacher and student reports of instructional practices, and
that only included gender, self-efficacy, and mastery goals as pre- whether teacher reports of instruction possibly mediated the re-
dictors of mastery-oriented practices. In this model, a nonsignificant lations between teacher motivation factors and student reports of
regression coefficient (ß = −.00, SE = .06) was obtained for mastery instruction (cf. Butler, 2012). As was found previously (e.g., Bieg et al.,
goals. This finding suggests that teacher mastery goals do not 2011; Butler, 2012; Kunter & Baumert, 2006), the correlations pre-
uniquely contribute to teacher reports of mastery-oriented prac- sented in Table 4 indicate rather low associations between teacher
tices when controlling for other relevant factors. and student reports of instructional practices. In line with Butler’s
(2012) findings, only the correlation between teacher- and student-
3.3. Prediction of student reports of instructional practices rated mastery-oriented practices was significant. The analyzed
multilevel regression models, however, did not reveal any signifi-
As was anticipated, teacher motivation factors mostly showed cant contributions of teacher reports of instruction to student reports
lower correlations with student- than with teacher-rated instruc- (see Table 6). Given these findings, it was not meaningful to further
tional practices (cf. Tables 2 and 4). In addition, none of the explore the role of teacher reports of instruction as mediators of
contributions of teacher motivation to aggregated student reports the relation between teacher motivation and student reports of
of cognitively activating practices was significant (see Table 4). Con- instruction.

3.4. Prediction of student motivation


4 However, indicators of collinearity (tolerance >.60, condition index <26) did not
exceed critical levels (tolerance ≤.20, condition index ≥30) for the regression model As was anticipated, the analysis of bivariate correlations re-
predicting mastery-oriented practices. vealed that student motivation was more closely associated with
166 U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171

Table 5
Contributions of teacher motivation to student reports of instructional practices and student motivation.

Predictor Student variable

Mastery-oriented IP Cognitively activating IP Subject interest Mastery goals

Level 1
Student gendera .03 (.03) .01 (.03) −.01 (.03) −.03 (.03)
Student age .04 (.03) .08* (.04) −.10** (.03) −.09** (.03)
Level 2
Teacher gendera .25* (.12) −.01 (.12) .04 (.12) .17 (.13)
Subject interest −.15 (.11) −.12 (.12) .20 (.12) −.09 (.13)
Didactic interest −.09 (.14) .02 (.15) .09 (.14) .11 (.19)
Educational interest .32** (.12) .07 (.14) .24* (.12) .09 (.14)
Mastery goals .18 (.12) .27* (.13) .04 (.13) .11 (.16)
Self-efficacy .06 (.11) −.05 (.15) .01 (.13) .19 (.13)
R2 (within) .00 .01 .01 .01
R2 (between) .26 .07 .21 .19

NTeachers = 110. NStudents = 1706. Standardized regression coefficients; standard errors in parentheses.
IP = instructional practices.
a Scoring of gender: male = 1, female = 2.

* p < .05.
** p < .01.

student-rated than with teacher-rated instructional practices (see correlational findings, however, need to be interpreted with caution
Tables 4 and 7). Only the correlation between teacher reports of as they do not take into account the nested data structure and si-
mastery-oriented practices and student mastery goals attained sig- multaneous contributions of other predictors.
nificance (see Table 4). In contrast, the hypothesized contributions We first conducted multilevel means-as-outcomes regression
of student-perceived mastery-oriented and cognitively activating analyses in order to analyze the contributions of teacher-rated in-
practices to students’ mastery goals and subject interest were con- struction and teacher motivation to students’ mastery goals and
firmed (see Table 7). subject interest. The results pertaining to the contributions of
Also according to expectations, teacher and student genders were teacher-rated instruction (see Table 6) did not support the hypoth-
not significantly related with student motivation (see Tables 4 and eses and showed nonsignificant (p > .10) regression coefficients for
7). In the Introduction, we have argued that student gender would teacher reports of mastery-oriented and cognitively activating prac-
not affect students’ subject interest and mastery goals in the present tices. However, a significant association between teacher educational
study because such effects are likely to differ by subject area. In ac- interest and student subject interest was obtained (see Table 5). The
cordance with this assumption, bivariate gender–motivation nature of this relation, whether it is direct and/or indirect, will be
correlations indicated a stronger interest of girls in German (r = .10, analyzed in more detail later.
p < .01) and a stronger interest of boys in mathematics (r = −.14, In the next step, we examined the relations between students’
p < .01). Within the group of students referring to miscellaneous sub- perceptions of instruction and their motivation. Thereby, stu-
jects, nonsignificant correlations of gender with interest (r = −.00) dents’ perceptions of instruction were included as predictors at both
and mastery goals (r = −.09) were observed. Levels 1 and 2. The results of these analyses (see Table 8) partially
Moreover, most of the class-level correlations between teacher supported the hypothesized significant contributions of student
motivation factors and aggregated scores of students’ subject in- reports of mastery-oriented and cognitively activating practices to
terest and mastery goals attained significance (see Table 4). These students’ subject interest and mastery goals at the individual and
findings were not explicitly expected because we only assumed in- the class level. Specifically, at the individual level (Level 1), mastery-
direct effects of teacher motivation on student motivation (being oriented practices positively predicted students’ mastery goals and
mediated by instructional practices), and the assumption of an in- subject interest. Moreover, student reports of higher levels of
direct relation between two variables does not require a significant cognitively activating practices were significantly associated with
association between these variables (e.g., Hayes, 2009). All of the higher levels of students’ mastery goals but not subject interest. At

Table 6
Contributions of teacher reports of instructional practices to student reports of instructional practices and student motivation.

Predictor Student variable

Mastery-oriented IP Cognitively activating IP Subject interest Mastery goals

Level 1
Student gendera .03 (.03) .01 (.03) −.01 (.03) −.03 (.03)
Student age .04 (.03) .07* (.04) −.09** (.04) −.09** (.03)
Level 2
Teacher gendera .33** (.12) .06 (.12) .13 (.11) .20 (.11)
Mastery-oriented IP (TR) .07 (.14) −.09 (.13) .18 (.15) .21 (.16)
Cognitively activating IP (TR) .05 (.13) .19 (.14) −.07 (.15) .09 (.12)
R2 (within) .00 .01 .01 .01
R2 (between) .15 .03 .05 .15

NTeachers = 110. NStudents = 1706. Standardized regression coefficients; standard errors in parentheses.
IP = instructional practices. TR = teacher reports.
a Scoring of gender: male = 1, female = 2.

* p < .05.
** p < .01.
U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171 167

Table 7 The focus of the analysis was on the between-class level where
Correlations among student variables. we modeled the relations among teacher educational interest,
Student variable 1 2 3 4 5 teacher gender, student reports of mastery-oriented practices,
1. Gendera – student subject interest, and student mastery goals. In line with our
2. Mastery-oriented IP .02 – initial hypotheses, only indirect paths between teacher and student
3. Cognitively activating IP .02 .38*** – motivation were specified. Accordingly, the between-class model
4. Subject interest −.00 .31*** .13*** – comprised paths (a) from teacher educational interest and teacher
5. Mastery goals −.02 .36*** .29*** .46*** –
gender to student reports of mastery-oriented practices and (b) from
N = 1731. student reports of mastery-oriented practices to students’ subject
IP = instructional practices.
interest and mastery goals. At the within-class level, the model in-
a
Scoring of gender: male = 1, female = 2.
*** p < .001, two-tailed. cluded paths (a) from student gender and student age to student
reports of mastery-oriented practices, student mastery goals, and
student subject interest and (b) from student reports of mastery-
oriented practices to student mastery goals and student subject
the class level (Level 2), strong contributions of student reports of
interest.
mastery-oriented instruction to students’ mastery goals and subject
The overall fit of the model was good, χ 2 = 8.31, df = 4, ns,
interest were obtained, whereas cognitively activating practices were
CFI = .993, TLI = .966, RMSEA = .025. However, modification indices
not significantly associated with student motivation. These find-
suggested a significant direct effect of teacher educational inter-
ings suggest that student-perceived mastery-oriented practices are
est on student subject interest. Therefore, we added that path to
of particular relevance for students’ mastery goals and subject
the between-class model. The adapted model (see Fig. 1) yielded
interest.
the following fit indices: χ2 = 3.03, df = 3, ns, CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00,
In contrast to student gender, student age was significantly related
RMSEA = .002. At the within-class level, the model replicated the
with both students’ mastery goals and subject interest (see Table 8).
findings reported earlier (see Tables 6 and 8). As was expected, sig-
Accordingly, older students within classes reported lower levels of
nificant indirect effects of student gender and student age on student
motivation than younger students. As was argued earlier with respect
motivation were not observed. However, at the between-class level,
to age effects on perceived instruction, this finding may be ex-
significant indirect effects of teacher educational interest on student
plained by the fact that older students within classes tend to be low
subject interest (ß = .19, p < .01) and student mastery goals (ß = .26,
achievers and, thus, not only perceive more cognitive challenge but
p < .001) were obtained. Moreover, teacher gender also showed sig-
also show lower levels of mastery goals and subject interest.
nificant indirect effects on student subject interest (ß = .16, p < .05)
and student mastery goals (ß = .23, p < .05). It is important to note
3.5. Indirect relations between teacher and student motivation that teacher educational interest was both indirectly and directly
mediated by student reports of instructional practices related to student subject interest (see Fig. 1). Thus, the overall re-
lation between teacher educational interest and student subject
We have hypothesized indirect relations between teacher and interest was only partially mediated by student reports of mastery-
student motivation that are mediated by teachers’ instructional prac- oriented instructional practices.
tices. The present findings suggest that only teacher educational
interest and teacher gender are likely to show indirect contribu- 4. Discussion
tions to student motivation. This assumption is based on the obtained
significant class-level contributions of (a) teacher educational in- In extending previous research (e.g., Butler, 2007; Retelsdorf et al.,
terest and teacher gender to student reports of mastery-oriented 2010), the present study (a) directly compared the contributions
practices (see Table 5) and (b) student reports of mastery-oriented of teachers’ interests, mastery goals, and self-efficacy beliefs to teach-
practices to students’ subject interest and mastery goals (see Table 8). ers’ instructional practices, (b) used both teacher and student reports
In order to directly examine the indirect effects of teachers’ edu- to assess instructional practices, and (c) included students’ moti-
cational interest and gender on students’ subject interest and mastery vational characteristics as outcomes of teachers’ motivation and
goals, a multilevel structural equation model was specified. instruction. In the following, we summarize and interpret the find-
ings, address some limitations, and offer directions for future
research.
Table 8
Contributions of student reports of instructional practices to student motivation.
4.1. Teacher motivation and instructional practices
Predictor Student motivation

Subject interest Mastery goals 4.1.1. Teacher reports of instruction


The present study confirmed the hypotheses pertaining to the
Level 1
Student gendera −.02 (.03) −.04 (.02) prediction of teacher-rated instruction by teachers’ interests and self-
Student age −.10** (.03) −.11*** (.03) efficacy. In extending earlier findings (Schiefele et al., 2013), it was
Mastery-oriented IP (SR) .23*** (.02) .23*** (.03) demonstrated that the contributions of didactic and educational in-
Cognitively activating IP (SR) .02 (.03) .19*** (.03) terest remain substantial when controlling for teachers’ mastery goals
Level 2
and gender. However, in contrast to expectations, teacher mastery
Teacher gendera −.09 (.11) .01 (.09)
Mastery-oriented IP (SR) .72*** (.12) .80*** (.13) goals were not significantly related to teacher reports of mastery-
Cognitively activating IP (SR) −.01 (.16) .07 (.12) oriented and cognitively activating practices when controlling for
2
R (within) .06 .13 teachers’ interests, self-efficacy, and gender.
R2 (between) .47 .71
The minor role of teacher mastery goals for the prediction of
NTeachers = 110. NStudents = 1706. Standardized regression coefficients; standard errors mastery-oriented practices has been supported by Butler (2012).
in parentheses. Butler extended her four-factor model of teacher goal orienta-
IP = instructional practices. SR = student reports.
a Scoring of gender: male = 1, female = 2. tions by adding relational goals as a fifth factor. Relational goals
** p < .01. represent “strivings to attain caring personal relationships with stu-
*** p < .001. dents” (Butler, 2012, p. 727). Butler examined the unique
168 U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171

Within-Class Level
-.10**

-.02
Individual
Student
student
gender
.24*** subject interest
.03

Individual student
reports of mastery- .38***
.04 oriented instruction .30***

Individual
Student -.10*** student
age mastery goals

-.04

Between-Class Level

.19*
Teacher Aggregated
educational student
interest .59*** subject interest
.31**

Aggregated student .67***


reports of mastery-
.27** oriented instruction
.83***
Aggregated
Teacher
student
gender
mastery goals

Fig. 1. Multilevel structural equation model for the prediction of student motivation. NTeachers = 110. NStudents = 1706. Standardized path coefficients. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

contributions of teacher goals to various teacher-rated instruc- students’ personal characteristics and development. Mastery-
tional practices and found that relational goals but not mastery goals oriented practices involve encouraging students to master learning
predicted mastery-oriented practices. Although Retelsdorf et al. tasks by attributing their achievement to effort, by focusing on their
(2010) and Shim et al. (2013) demonstrated significant associa- individual progress, and by adapting tasks to their interests and abil-
tions between teacher mastery goals and teacher reports of mastery- ities. These practices fit well with motivational orientations, such
oriented practices, they have not included competing predictors or as educational interest, that refer to students’ individual develop-
relevant control variables. ment. Indirect evidence for these considerations is provided by Butler
and Shibaz (2014) who found that teachers’ relational goals con-
4.1.2. Student reports of instruction tributed to teachers’ social support of students but not to teachers’
Student-based ratings of instructional practices turned out to be cognitively activating practices. Thereby, it is important to note that
significantly less favorable than teacher-based ratings. In addition, the construct of relational goals seems to share some characteris-
correlations between student and teacher ratings were either low tics with the construct of educational interest. Specifically, both
or not significant. These findings were expected (cf. Bieg et al., 2011; constructs involve a focus on students’ personal development and,
Clausen, 2002; Hamre & Pianta, 2010) and converge with Butler thus, imply a caring relationship with students.
(2012). Also in keeping with the hypotheses, teachers’ motiva- Whereas mastery goals did not predict teacher reports of mastery-
tional characteristics were weaker predictors of student-rated than oriented instruction above and beyond teachers’ interests and self-
of teacher-rated practices. This applied in particular to teachers’ self- efficacy, a significant unique contribution of teacher mastery goals
efficacy and didactic interest which significantly predicted teacher to student reports of cognitively activating practices in the class-
reports but not student reports of mastery-oriented and cognitively room was obtained. This result is of particular relevance because
activating instruction. Teachers educational interest, however, proved it shows that teacher mastery goals may be indeed important for
to be the only facet of teacher motivation that contributed posi- a central dimension of teachers’ instructional behavior. In addi-
tively to both teacher and student reports of adaptive instructional tion, this result appears to be conceptually meaningful because
practices. Moreover, educational interest was found to be the only teacher mastery goals and cognitively activating practices share a
significant motivational predictor of student reports of mastery- strong focus on the development of competence. Specifically, teacher
oriented instruction. This finding suggests that mastery-oriented mastery goals entail a striving toward higher levels of teaching com-
instructional practices are more closely associated with features of petence (e.g., by seeking challenging tasks), and cognitively activating
teacher motivation that involve a caring attitude and an interest in practices are directed at fostering students’ competence by means
U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171 169

of providing challenging and stimulating tasks and by emphasiz- the issue of competing or alternative predictors might be crucial
ing critical and independent thinking (Butler, 2007; Kunter et al., when explaining the differences in findings between studies. Al-
2008). Supporting evidence was recently demonstrated by Butler though we obtained a significant bivariate correlation between
and Shibaz (2014) who found that teacher mastery goals pre- individual scores of student-perceived cognitively activating prac-
dicted student reports of cognitively activating practices. tices and student subject interest (see Table 7), this relation became
Although teacher self-efficacy and didactic interest proved to be nonsignificant in a multilevel regression model that included
substantial predictors of teacher reports of instructional practices, mastery-oriented practices as an additional predictor of student
they did not significantly predict student reports. This gap between subject interest (see Table 8). This may suggest that student subject
self-reported and student-reported practices is in line with studies interest depends more strongly on mastery-oriented practices than
showing that teacher ratings may be influenced by self-serving ten- on cognitively activating practices. Thus, the fact that both Fauth
dencies or teaching ideals and thus tend to be more favorable than et al. and Butler and Shibaz have not accounted for mastery-
student ratings (cf. Hamre & Pianta, 2010; Kunter & Baumert, 2006). oriented practices as a predictor of student subject interest may have
In fact, the analysis of mean values in the present study revealed contributed to their findings.
that teacher ratings of instruction were more positive than student Among teacher motivation factors, the present findings suggest
ratings. In our view, it may be assumed that self-efficacy and di- a particularly important role of teacher educational interest for stu-
dactic interest possibly have reinforced the influence of self- dents’ subject interest and mastery goals. Only educational interest
serving tendencies and teaching ideals. Moreover, the tendency to showed significant indirect contributions to students’ mastery goals
appear self-consistent may have led teachers with strong efficacy and subject interest that were mediated by mastery-oriented prac-
beliefs and didactic interest to report high levels of adaptive in- tices. In addition, a significant direct effect of teacher educational
structional practices that were not perceived in the same way by interest on student subject interest was observed. On the one hand,
these teachers’ students. this finding underlines the particular importance of teachers’ edu-
cational interest, which refers to the appropriate pedagogical
4.2. Prediction of student motivation handling of students and to their personal development, for stu-
dents’ subject interest. On the other hand, the obtained direct effect
A novel feature of the present research refers to the inclusion of teacher educational interest on student subject interest points
of student motivation as a possible outcome of teachers’ motiva- to the existence of mediating processes not being considered in our
tion and instructional practices. Overall, closer relations between study. Such mediating processes could involve instructional prac-
instruction and student motivation were expected for student than tices that are directed, for example, at supporting students and
for teacher reports of instructional practices. In accordance with that fostering their experience of autonomy and competence (e.g.,
assumption, the present findings showed that only student reports Klusmann et al., 2008; Kunter et al., 2008).
of instruction, but not teacher reports, did predict student moti-
vation, both at the individual and the class level. This suggests that 4.3. Limitations and future directions
teachers’ self-rated instructional practices possess rather limited va-
lidity with respect to students’ self-reported subject interest and A major limitation of the present study refers to its cross-
mastery goals. sectional character. This makes it impossible to dispel uncertainty
As was expected, the findings revealed that student reports of about the direction of postulated effects. Although it seems rather
mastery-oriented practices contributed significantly to students’ probable that facets of teacher motivation indeed impact on teach-
mastery goals and subject interest at the individual and the class ers’ classroom practices (Butler, 2012; Givens Rolland, 2012; Ross,
level. These contributions were particularly strong at the class level 1998), recent evidence suggests that the association between teacher
suggesting that differences between teachers in their average use motivation and instructional quality might be reciprocal. Specifi-
of mastery-oriented practices (as perceived by students) were more cally, Holzberger, Philipp, and Kunter (2013) conducted a longitudinal
important than individual differences between students within study and found not only effects of teacher self-efficacy on student
classes. This suggests that teachers’ mastery-oriented practices show and teacher reports of instructional practices but also reverse effects.
a relatively close association with students’ motivation that does Reciprocal effects may also apply to the association between stu-
not depend on students’ specific individual experience of their teach- dents’ reports of instructional practices and their motivation. On
ers’ mastery-oriented practices. the one hand, perceived instructional practices are likely to impact
In contrast to mastery-oriented practices, student-reported on students’ motivation. On the other hand, however, highly mo-
cognitively activating practices only predicted student mastery goals tivated students (e.g., with strong mastery goals) probably tend to
at the individual level. Thus, students’ specific individual percep- evaluate their teachers’ methods more positively and, thus, con-
tions but not teachers’ average scores of (student-perceived) tribute to a positive classroom climate that may encourage the
cognitively activating practices made a difference in student mastery teacher to use effective classroom practices. Longitudinal studies are
goals. This suggests that higher levels of cognitively activating prac- needed to explore these considerations.
tices in the classroom only affect students’ mastery goals if students A further limitation concerns the assessment of teachers’ in-
actually perceive their teachers’ practices to be cognitively activat- structional practices by means of teacher and student reports. In
ing. For example, when students perceive their teachers’ cognitively line with previous studies (Butler, 2012; Kunter & Baumert, 2006),
activating practices as too demanding, then their mastery goals will rather low correlations between teacher and student reports were
not be strengthened. found. Accordingly, teachers’ reports of their motivation were more
Contributions of student reports of cognitively activating prac- closely related to their self-rated practices than to student-rated prac-
tices to student subject interest were not observed. This finding tices, whereas students’ motivation was more closely associated with
contrasts with our hypotheses and recent studies by Fauth et al. their perceptions of instructional practices than with teacher-
(2014) and Butler and Shibaz (2014). These studies reported sig- rated practices. It follows that the perspective of an outside observer
nificant contributions of student-reported cognitively activating on teachers’ instructional behavior (e.g., Lerkkanen et al., 2012;
practices to student subject interest at the individual and the class Mashburn, Meyer, Allen, & Pianta, 2014) might be helpful in order
level. However, Fauth et al. found effects of cognitively activating to get a less biased account of the relations among teacher moti-
practices at the individual level only when not controlling for other vation, instructional practices, and student motivation. In addition,
indicators of teaching quality (e.g., supportive climate). In our view, the use of observational methods would potentially contribute to
170 U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171

a better understanding of the nature of teacher and student References


reports of instruction and their discrepancy (Kunter & Baumert,
2006).
Anderman, E. M., & Midgley, C. (1997). Changes in achievement goal orientations,
When evaluating the relevance of the included teacher motiva- perceived academic competence, and grades across the transition to middle-level
tion factors, one should bear in mind that we have not assessed all schools. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 269–298.
of the relevant instructional practices that are possibly affected by Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.
Baumert, J., Kunter, M., Blum, W., Brunner, M., Voss, T., Jordan, A., et al. (2010).
teacher motivation. For example, teacher subject interest might be Teachers’ mathematical knowledge, cognitive activation in the classroom, and
related to teachers’ expression of enthusiasm about their subject student progress. American Educational Research Journal, 47, 133–180.
in the classroom (e.g., talking to students about exciting aspects of Bergin, D. A. (1999). Influences on classroom interest. Educational Psychologist, 34,
87–98.
the subject; Frenzel, Goetz, Lüdtke, Pekrun, & Sutton, 2009) or to Bieg, S., Backes, S., & Mittag, W. (2011). The role of intrinsic motivation for teaching,
teachers’ use of other interest-enhancing methods (e.g., emphasiz- teachers’ care and autonomy support in students’ self-determined motivation.
ing the practical relevance of the subject; Bergin, 1999). Presumably, Journal for Educational Research Online, 3, 122–140.
Bliese, P. (2000). Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability. In K.
teacher subject interest contributes through these processes to the
Klein & S. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multi-level theory, research, and methods in
development of students’ subject interest. Thus, it seems impor- organizations (pp. 349–381). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
tant for future research on the effects of teacher motivation to include Brookhart, S. M., & Freeman, D. J. (1992). Characteristics of entering teacher
forms of instructional practice that were not considered in the candidates. Review of Educational Research, 62, 37–60.
Butler, R. (2007). Teachers’ achievement goal orientations and associations with
present study and that might be potential mediators of the teachers’ help seeking: Examination of a novel approach to teacher motivation.
motivation–instruction relation (see also Butler & Shibaz, 2014; Fauth Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 241–252.
et al., 2014). Butler, R. (2012). Striving to connect: Extending an achievement goal approach to
teacher motivation to include relational goals for teaching. Journal of Educational
It seems also noteworthy that the reliabilities of some of the Psychology, 104, 726–742.
present scales were below the standard minimum of .70 (cf. Table 1). Butler, R., & Shibaz, L. (2008). Achievement goals for teaching as predictors of students’
Thus, it seems desirable to improve these scales by adding new items perceptions of instructional practices and students’ help seeking and cheating.
Learning and Instruction, 18, 453–467.
and/or replacing old items. Improvement of the scales should also Butler, R., & Shibaz, L. (2014). Striving to connect and striving to learn: Influences
take into account the high mean values that we have obtained for of relational and mastery goals for teaching on teacher behaviors and student
the motivation and instruction scales (for similarly high values, see interest and help seeking. International Journal of Educational Research, 65, 41–
53.
Butler, 2012). By creating more “difficult” items, the mean values
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Steca, P., & Malone, P. S. (2006). Teachers’ self-efficacy
could be reduced while enhancing the interindividual variance. As beliefs as determinants of job satisfaction and students’ academic achievement:
a consequence, stronger relations might be observed. A study at the school level. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 473–490.
Clausen, M. (2002). Unterrichtsqualität: Eine Frage der Perspektive [Quality of instruction:
As well as overcoming these limitations, future research should
A matter of perspective]. Münster, Germany: Waxmann.
address possible (indirect) effects of teacher motivation on stu- Deemer, S. A. (2004). Classroom goal orientation in high school classrooms: Revealing
dents’ academic achievement. Studies on the relation between links between teacher beliefs and classroom environments. Educational Research,
teacher motivation (with the exception of teacher self-efficacy; e.g., 46, 73–90.
Dellinger, A. B., Bobbett, J. J., Olivier, D. F., & Ellett, C. D. (2008). Measuring teachers’
Caprara, Barbaranelli, Steca, & Malone, 2006) and academic achieve- self-efficacy beliefs: Development and use of the TEBS-Self. Teaching and Teacher
ment are largely missing. However, in order to corroborate the Education, 24, 751–766.
present findings and to further evaluate the importance of the various Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In A. J.
Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 52–72).
facets of teachers’ motivation and their instructional practices, it New York: Guilford.
seems desirable to examine the relations between teacher moti- Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals
vation and student achievement. For example, it seems likely that and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 70, 461–475.
teacher mastery goals show an indirect relation to student achieve- Fauth, B., Decristan, J., Rieser, S., Klieme, E., & Büttner, G. (2014). Student ratings of
ment that is mediated by cognitively activating practices. This teaching quality in primary school: Dimensions and prediction of student
assumption is supported by (a) the present finding of a significant outcomes. Learning and Instruction, 29, 1–9.
Fernet, C., Senécal, C., Guay, F., Marsh, H., & Dowson, M. (2008). The Work Tasks
relation between teacher mastery goals and student reports of Motivation Scale for Teachers (WTMST). Journal of Career Assessment, 16, 256–
cognitively activating practices and (b) recently demonstrated class- 279.
level effects of cognitively activating practices (assessed by a Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Lüdtke, O., Pekrun, R., & Sutton, R. E. (2009). Emotional
transmission in the classroom: Exploring the relationship between teacher and
combination of teacher and student ratings) on student achieve-
student enjoyment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 705–716.
ment (Kunter et al., 2013). Gavin, M. B., & Hofmann, D. A. (2002). Using hierarchical linear modeling to investigate
Although we have assessed teachers’ self-efficacy, future re- the moderating influence of leadership climate. The Leadership Quarterly, 13,
search should also include more direct or objective indicators of 15–33.
Givens Rolland, R. (2012). Synthesizing the evidence on classroom goal structures
teacher competence. Teachers’ classroom behavior does not only in middle and secondary schools: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Review
depend on their motivation but also on their instructional compe- of Educational Research, 82, 396–435.
tence. For example, Baumert et al. (2010; see also Kunter et al., Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annual
Review of Psychology, 60, 549–576.
2013) provided evidence for a positive effect of mathematics teach- Grant, H., & Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact. Journal
ers’ pedagogical content knowledge (e.g., knowledge of tasks as of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 541–553.
instructional tools and recognition of students’ misconceptions) Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2010). Classroom environments and developmental
processes: Conceptualization, measurement, and improvement. In J. L. Meece
on students’ learning gains over one school year that was medi- & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Handbook of research on schools, schooling and human
ated by teachers’ cognitively activating practices and learning development (pp. 25–41). New York: Routledge.
support. Thus, it would be desirable for future studies to examine Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the
new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420.
whether teacher motivation contributes to the use of instruc- Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development.
tional practices when controlling for teacher knowledge. However, Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127.
teacher knowledge could also be considered as a possible media- Hidi, S., Renninger, K. A., & Krapp, A. (2004). Interest, a motivational variable that
combines affective and cognitive functioning. In D. Y. Dai & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.),
tor of the relation between teacher motivation and instructional
Motivation, emotion, and cognition (pp. 89–115). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
practices. For example, educational interest might facilitate the ac- Holzberger, D., Philipp, A., & Kunter, M. (2013). How teachers’ self-efficacy is related
quisition of pedagogical content knowledge that, in turn, leads to to instructional quality: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology,
the use of appropriate teaching methods (e.g., mastery-oriented 105, 774–786.
Kenney-Benson, G. A., Pomerantz, E. M., Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2006). Sex
practices). Hence, here as well, a longitudinal design could gener- differences in math performance: The role of children’s approach to schoolwork.
ate important findings. Developmental Psychology, 42, 11–26.
U. Schiefele, E. Schaffner/Contemporary Educational Psychology 42 (2015) 159–171 171

Klassen, R. M., Tze, V. M. C., Betts, S. M., & Gordon, K. A. (2011). Teacher efficacy Retelsdorf, J. (2006). Zielorientierung und Unterrichtsverhalten bei Lehrerinnen und
research 1998–2009: Signs of progress or unfulfilled promise? Educational Lehrern [Teachers’ goal orientations and instructional practices] (Unpublished
Psychology Review, 23, 21–43. diploma thesis). University of Bielefeld.
Klassen, R. M., Usher, E. L., & Bong, M. (2010). Teacher’s collective efficacy, job Retelsdorf, J., Butler, R., Streblow, L., & Schiefele, U. (2010). Teachers’ goal orientations
satisfaction, and job stress in cross-cultural context. The Journal of Experimental for teaching: Associations with instructional practices, interest in teaching, and
Education, 78, 464–486. burnout. Learning and Instruction, 20, 30–46.
Klusmann, U., Kunter, M., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., & Baumert, J. (2008). Teachers’ Ross, J. A. (1998). The antecedents and consequences of teacher efficacy. In J. Brophy
occupational well-being and quality of instruction: The important role of (Ed.), Advances in research on teaching (Vol. 7, pp. 49–73). Greenwich, CT:
self-regulatory patterns. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 702–715. JAI Press.
Koskey, K. L., Karabenick, S. A., Woolley, M. E., Bonney, C. R., & Dever, B. V. (2010). Ryan, A. M., Gheen, M. H., & Midgley, C. (1998). Why do some students avoid asking
Cognitive validity of students’ self-reports of classroom mastery goal structure: for help? An examination of the interplay among students’ academic efficacy,
What students are thinking and why it matters. Contemporary Educational teachers’ social-emotional role, and the classroom goal structure. Journal of
Psychology, 35, 254–263. Educational Psychology, 90, 528–535.
Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of interest and intrinsic Sabbe, E., & Aelterman, A. (2007). Gender in teaching: A literature review. Teachers
motivational orientations. Learning and Instruction, 15, 381–395. and Teaching: Theory and Practice, 13, 521–538.
Krapp, A. (2007). An educational-psychological conceptualisation of interest. Schafer, J. L. (1999). NORM: Multiple imputation of incomplete multivariate data
International Journal of Educational and Vocational Guidance, 7, 5–21. under a normal model, version 2.03 for Windows 95/98/NT [Computer Software].
Krauss, S., Brunner, M., Kunter, M., Baumert, J., Blum, W., Neubrand, M., et al. (2008). <http://www.stat.psu.edu/~jls/misoftwa.html> Retrieved 11.01.09.
Pedagogical content knowledge and content knowledge of secondary Schaffner, E., & Schiefele, U. (2008). Familiäre und individuelle Bedingungen des
mathematics teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 716–725. Textlernens [Familial and individual conditions of text learning]. Psychologie in
Kunter, M., & Baumert, J. (2006). Who is the expert? Construct and criteria validity Erziehung und Unterricht, 55, 238–252.
of student and teacher ratings of instruction. Learning Environments Research, Schiefele, U. (2009). Situational and individual interest. In A. Wigfield & K. Wentzel
9, 231–251. (Eds.), Handbook on motivation at school (pp. 197–222). New York/London:
Kunter, M., Frenzel, A., Nagy, G., Baumert, J., & Pekrun, R. (2011). Teacher enthusiasm: Routledge.
Dimensionality and context specificity. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, Schiefele, U., Krapp, A., & Winteler, A. (1992). Interest and academic achievement:
289–301. A meta-analysis of research. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.),
Kunter, M., Klusmann, U., Baumert, J., Richter, D., Voss, T., & Hachfeld, A. (2013). The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 183–212). Hillsdale, NJ:
Professional competence of teachers: Effects on instructional quality and student Erlbaum.
development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 805–820. Schiefele, U., Streblow, L., & Retelsdorf, J. (2013). Dimensions of teacher interest and
Kunter, M., Klusmann, U., Dubberke, T., Baumert, J., Blum, W., Brunner, M., et al. (2007). their relations to occupational well-being and instructional practices. Journal for
Linking aspects of teacher competence to their instruction. Results from the Educational Research Online, 5, 7–37.
COACTIV project. In M. Prenzel (Ed.), Studies on the educational quality of schools. Schwarzer, R., & Hallum, S. (2008). Perceived teacher self-efficacy as a predictor of
The final report on the DFG Priority Programme (pp. 32–52). Münster: Waxmann. job stress and burnout: Mediation analyses. Applied Psychology: An International
Kunter, M., Tsai, Y.-M., Klusmann, U., Brunner, M., Krauss, S., & Baumert, J. (2008). Review, 57, 152–171.
Students’ and mathematics teachers’ perceptions of teacher enthusiasm and Schwarzer, R., & Schmitz, G. S. (1999). Skala zur Lehrer-Selbstwirksamkeitserwartung
instruction. Learning and Instruction, 18, 468–482. [Teacher self-efficacy scale]. In R. Schwarzer & M. Jerusalem (Eds.), Skalen zur
Lee, K., Ning, F., & Goh, H. C. (2014). Interaction between cognitive and non-cognitive Erfassung von Lehrer- und Schülermerkmalen (pp. 61–62). Berlin, Germany: Freie
factors: The influences of academic goal orientation and working memory on Universität Berlin.
mathematical performance. Educational Psychology, 34, 73–91. Shim, S. S., Cho, Y., & Cassady, J. (2013). Goal structures: The role of teachers’
Lee, W., Lee, M.-J., & Bong, M. (2014). Testing interest and self-efficacy as predictors achievement goals and theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Education,
of academic self-regulation and achievement. Contemporary Educational 81, 84–104.
Psychology, 39, 86–99. Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching.
Lerkkanen, M.-K., Kiuru, N., Pakarinen, E., Viljaranta, J., Poikkeus, A.-M., Educational Researcher, 15, 4–14.
Rasku-Puttonen, H., et al. (2012). The role of teaching practices in the Shulman, L. S. (1987). Knowledge and teaching: Foundations of the new reform.
development of children’s interest in reading and mathematics in kindergarten. Harvard Educational Review, 57, 1–22.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 37, 266–279. Spinath, B., Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Schöne, C., & Dickhäuser, O. (2002). SELLMO: Skalen
Long, J. F., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2006). Interested instructors: A composite portrait zur Erfassung der Lern- und Leistungsmotivation [Scales for the assessment of
of individual differences and effectiveness. Teaching and Teacher Education, 22, achievement motivation]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
303–314. Statistisches Bundesamt [Federal Statistical Office] (2008). Datenreport 2008. Ein
Mashburn, A. J., Meyer, J. P., Allen, J. P., & Pianta, R. C. (2014). The effect of observation Sozialbericht für die Bundesrepublik Deutschland [Data report 2008. A social report
length and presentation order on the reliability and validity of an observational for the Federal Republic of Germany]. Bonn: Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung.
measure of teaching quality. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 74, Tschannen-Moran, M., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2001). Teacher efficacy: Capturing an
400–422. elusive construct. Teaching and Teacher Education, 17, 783–805.
Meece, J. L., Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2006). Classroom goal structure, Tschannen-Moran, M., Woolfolk Hoy, A., & Hoy, W. K. (1998). Teacher efficacy: Its
student motivation, and academic achievement. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, meaning and measure. Review of Educational Research, 68, 202–248.
487–503. Urdan, T. C. (2010). The challenges and promise of research on classroom goal
Meece, J. L., & Holt, K. (1993). A pattern analysis of students’ achievement goals. structures. In J. L. Meece & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Handbook of research on schools,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 582–590. schooling, and human development (pp. 92–108). New York: Routledge.
Midgley, C. (Ed.), (2002). Goals, goal structures, and patterns of adaptive learning. Urdan, T. C., & Maehr, M. L. (1995). Beyond a two-goal theory of motivation and
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. achievement: A case for social goals. Review of Educational Research, 65, 213–243.
Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., & Eccles, J. S. (1989). Change in teacher efficacy and student Urdan, T. C., & Schoenfelder, E. (2006). Classroom effects on student motivation: Goal
self- and task-related beliefs in mathematics during the transition to junior high structures, social relationships, and competence beliefs. Journal of School
school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 247–258. Psychology, 44, 331–349.
Midgley, C., Maehr, M. L., Hruda, L. Z., Anderman, E. M., Anderman, L. H., & Freeman, Van Veen, K., Sleegers, P., Bergen, T., & Klaassen, C. (2001). Professional orientations
K. E. (2000). PALS—Manual for the pattern of adaptive learning scales of secondary school teachers towards their work. Teaching and Teacher Education,
(Unpublished manuscript). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan. 17, 175–194.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2014). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Wang, H., Hall, N. C., & Rahimi, S. (2015). Self-efficacy and causal attributions in
Muthén & Muthén. teachers: Effects on burnout, job satisfaction, illness, and quitting intentions.
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA: Teaching and Teacher Education, 47, 120–130.
Harvard University Press. Watt, H. M. G., & Richardson, P. W. (Eds.) (2008). Motivation for teaching. Learning
O’Keefe, P. A., Ben-Eliyahu, A., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2013). Shaping achievement and Instruction, 18(5).
goal orientations in a mastery-structured environment and concomitant changes Wolters, C. A. (2004). Advancing achievement goal theory: Using goal structures and
in related contingencies of self-worth. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 50–64. goal orientations to predict students’ motivation, cognition, and achievement.
Papaioannou, A., & Christodoulidis, T. (2007). A measure of teachers’ achievement Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 236–250.
goals. Educational Psychology, 27, 349–361. Wolters, C. A., & Daugherty, S. G. (2007). Goal structures and teachers’ sense of
Patrick, H., Ryan, A. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (1999). The differential impact of extrinsic efficacy: Their relation and association to teaching experience and academic level.
and mastery goal orientations on males’ and females’ self-regulated learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 181–193.
Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 153–171. Woolfolk Hoy, A., & Weinstein, C. S. (2006). Student and teacher perspectives on
Pelletier, L. G., Séguin-Lévesque, C., & Legault, L. (2002). Pressure from above and classroom management. In C. M. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook of
pressure from below as determinants of teachers’ motivation and teaching classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues (pp. 181–219).
behaviors. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 186–196. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Phelps, G., & Schilling, S. (2004). Developing measures of content knowledge for Wu, A. D., Li, Z., & Zumbo, B. D. (2007). Decoding the meaning of factorial invariance
teaching reading. Elementary School Journal, 105, 31–48. and updating the practice of multi-group confirmatory factor analysis: A
Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. (2011). Revisiting the conceptualization, measurement, demonstration with TIMSS data. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 12,
and generation of interest. Educational Psychologist, 46, 168–184. 1–26.

You might also like