Rusch2018 PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.

ISSN 0077-8923

A N N A L S O F T H E N E W Y O R K A C A D E M Y O F SC I E N C E S
Special Issue: Annals Reports
REVIEW

The effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep quality: a


systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized
controlled trials
1,2
Heather L. Rusch, Michael Rosario,2 Lisa M. Levison,3 Anlys Olivera,2
Whitney S. Livingston,2 Tianxia Wu,4 and Jessica M. Gill2
1
National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland. 2 National Institute of Nursing Research,
National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland. 3 Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, New York. 4 National
Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland

Address for correspondence: Heather L. Rusch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, 15K North
Drive, Building 15K, Room 115B, Bethesda, MD 20892. heather.rusch@nih.gov

There is a growing interest in the effectiveness of mindfulness meditation for sleep disturbed populations. Our
study sought to evaluate the effect of mindfulness meditation interventions on sleep quality. To assess for relative
efficacy, comparator groups were restricted to specific active controls (such as evidenced-based sleep treatments)
and nonspecific active controls (such as time/attention-matched interventions to control for placebo effects), which
were analyzed separately. From 3303 total records, 18 trials with 1654 participants were included. We determined
the strength of evidence using four domains (risk of bias, directness of outcome measures, consistency of results,
and precision of results). At posttreatment and follow-up, there was low strength of evidence that mindfulness
meditation interventions had no effect on sleep quality compared with specific active controls (ES 0.03 (95% CI
–0.43 to 0.49)) and (ES –0.14 (95% CI –0.62 to 0.34)), respectively. Additionally, there was moderate strength of
evidence that mindfulness meditation interventions significantly improved sleep quality compared with nonspecific
active controls at postintervention (ES 0.33 (95% CI 0.17–0.48)) and at follow-up (ES 0.54 (95% CI 0.24–0.84)).
These preliminary findings suggest that mindfulness meditation may be effective in treating some aspects of sleep
disturbance. Further research is warranted.

Keywords: dose–response; insomnia; MBCT; MBSR; meditation; mindfulness; sleep

Introduction inaccessible.10 While the risks are attenuated with


Sleep disturbance is a common health complaint CBT-I, some of the therapeutic components, such
affecting an estimated 10–25% of the general as intensive sleep restriction, may exacerbate comor-
population.1 Accumulated sleep deficiency can bid psychiatric symptoms and thus compromise
increase the risk for mood and anxiety disorders,2–4 adherence.11–13 Taken together, there is a need
cognitive impairment,5 and a variety of medi- for complementary health interventions, which
cal conditions, including cardiovascular disease6 increase patient choice and may be offered as a
and obesity.7 Pharmaceutical sleep aids remain the second-line treatment option when first-line treat-
first-line treatment for insomnia. While effective, ments are not viable or are intolerable.
they have the potential for abuse, cross-reactivity In recent years, mindfulness meditation has
with other medications, and side effects, includ- gained interest as an alternative treatment for sleep
ing memory loss, abnormal thoughts, behavioral disturbance. Mindfulness means paying attention
changes, and headaches.8,9 Alternatively, behav- in a particular way: on purpose, in the present
ioral treatments, such as cognitive behavioral ther- moment, and nonjudgmentally—this attention is
apy for insomnia (CBT-I), can be expensive and curious and kind.14 Cultivating present moment

doi: 10.1111/nyas.13996
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA 5
Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep Rusch et al.

awareness, in lieu of reinforcing past or future May 2018, with no start date restriction. For
reactivity, may function to transform engrained search terms, two main subject-heading domains
cognitive patterns and subsequent maladaptive were combined with the AND operator: one to
behaviors.15 Mindfulness meditation is hypothe- designate the intervention (meditation, mindful-
sized to target multiple cognitive and emotional ness, mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR),
processes that contribute to poor sleep quality. It mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT), or
has been shown to decrease ruminative thoughts,16 Vipassanā), and the second to designate the outcome
diminish emotional reactivity,17,18 and promote (sleep or insomnia). No language restrictions were
impartial reappraisal of salient experiences, which placed on the search. The bibliography of identi-
together may facilitate sleep.19 fied trials and germane review articles was manually
The effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep searched for additional references.
quality has also been the topic of recent meta-
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
analyses. However, findings were inconsistent and
We included published reports of RCTs in popu-
ranged from no effect to a moderate positive effect
lations with clinically significant sleep disturbance
in favor of mindfulness meditation. Two of the four
that employed a mindfulness meditation inter-
meta-analyses were not restricted to randomized
vention with multiple treatment sessions and
control trials (RCTs).20,21 A third meta-analysis
assessment of baseline and postintervention
restricted its investigation to RCTs;22 however,
sleep quality. Validated sleep measures included
owing to the small number of included trials,
both objective and subjective measurements, for
investigators were unable to analyze the active
example, actigraphy, self-reported sleep quality
control and waitlist control trials separately. This
questionnaires, and diary-reported sleep quality.
made it difficult to parse nonspecific effects (e.g.,
Evidence-based sleep treatments were determined
attention and expectancy bias) from the effect of the
by an American Academy of Sleep Medicine 2006
intervention. A fourth meta-analysis compared the
report25 and updated with a recent meta-analysis
effect of mindfulness meditation to active controls
of 19 trials reporting medium to large effects of
independently;23 however, the small number of
physical activity on subjective measures of sleep.26
included trials limited its generalizability. The
Trials were excluded that compared mindfulness
objective of this meta-analysis is to build on
meditation to an experimental sleep treatment
prior meta-analyses by only including RCTs that
(e.g., transcendental meditation, tai chi, and yoga),
employed a mindfulness meditation intervention
or compared novice meditators to experienced
in populations with clinically significant sleep
meditators. All other populations with clinically
disturbance. Furthermore, to assess for relative
significant sleep disturbance, excluding children
efficacy, comparator groups were restricted to spe-
and adolescents, were eligible. Table 1 includes a
cific active controls (such as evidence-based sleep
detailed summary of the inclusion and exclusion
treatments) and nonspecific active controls (such as
criteria.
time/attention-matched interventions to control for
placebo effects), which were analyzed separately. We Data extraction
aim to examine the following three questions: (1) Two investigators independently screened the title
Does mindfulness meditation improve sleep quality and abstract of each record to assess eligibility.
when compared with specific active controls or non- The full-text article was obtained for all potentially
specific active controls; (2) Does the effect persist eligible trials and screened for inclusion. Of the
long-term; and (3) Is there a dose–response effect. included trials, three investigators independently
extracted data relating to author, publication
Methods
year, population type, sample size, mindfulness
Systematic search meditation intervention, control intervention,
This review was conducted in accordance with control type, intervention weeks, in-class medi-
the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Sys- tation hours, retreat meditation hours, at-home
tematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis) statement.24 meditation hours, criteria for sleep disturbance,
PubMed, EBSCO, Embase, and The Cochrane sleep quality outcome measure, assessment time
Library databases were searched for articles through points, assessment data, and risk of bias criteria.

6 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA
Rusch et al. Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep

Table 1. Detailed inclusion and exclusion summary

Inclusion Exclusion

Population Adult populations with clinically significant sleep disturbance Children, adolescents, and experienced
(i.e., ICD insomnia diagnosis or met symptom severity meditators
threshold defined by sleep quality questionnaires)
Intervention In-person, structured mindfulness meditation (e.g., Mantra-based meditation and
mindfulness-based stress reduction and Vipassanā) movement-based therapies like, tai chi and
yoga, internet administration
Comparator Specific active controls: evidence-based sleep treatments Waitlist or usual care controls
Nonspecific active controls: time/attention-matched
interventions
Outcome Assessment of a preintervention and postintervention validated No validated measure of sleep or only a
subjective or objective measure of sleep baseline measurement
Study design Randomized controlled trials Nonrandomized controlled trials
Other All languages and dates through May 2018 Abstracts, reviews, and nonpublished trials,
as well as duplicate participant samples

Discrepancies in the eligibility and data extrac- prehensive U.S. Agency for Healthcare Research and
tion were resolved through further contact with Quality review of meditative practices.23 Two points
corresponding authors, discussion, and consensus. were given for meeting each major criterion and one
point was given for meeting each minor criterion.
A low risk of bias was assigned to trials with a score
The strength of the body of evidence
between 9 and 12 points. A medium risk of bias
The methods for determining the strength of the
was assigned to trials with a score between 6 and
body of evidence were replicated from our prior
8 points. Any trial with five or fewer points was
meta-analysis.27 Briefly, three investigators graded
assigned a high risk of bias (Table 2). In assessing
the strength of evidence for each outcome, inde-
the directness of measures, both objective and sub-
pendently and then by consensus, using the grading
jective sleep measures were considered direct if they
scheme recommended by the Methods Guide for
were validated to assess a sleep quality dimension.
Conducting Comparative Effectiveness Reviews.28
The consistency of results was evaluated by com-
In assigning evidence grades, we considered four
paring the overall direction of effect. Finally, the
domains: risk of bias, directness of outcome mea-
precision of results was based on the CI range from
sures, consistency of results, and precision of results.
the meta-analysis. If the CI range was wide owing
Evidence was classified into the following four cat-
to a large heterogeneity (which was attributed to
egories: (1) high (indicating high confidence that
the inconsistency of results), the evidence was not
the estimate of effect lies close to the true effect for
scored as imprecise as well.28
this outcome, and further studies would not change
the conclusion); (2) moderate (indicating moderate Outcome measures
confidence that the estimate of effect lies close to the Objective measures of sleep quality included the
true effect for this outcome, and findings are likely actigraphy. Subjective measures with established
to be stable, but some doubt remains); (3) low (indi- validity included the insomnia sleep index (ISI), the
cating limited confidence that the estimate of effect medical outcomes study-sleep scale (MOS-SS), and
lies close to the true effect for this outcome, and that the Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI). Because
additional evidence is needed); and (4) insufficient of the high overlap in content validity between the
(indicating no evidence or inability to estimate an three sleep quality scales, they were pooled in the
effect for this outcome).28 meta-analysis.
Risk of bias scoring was used to evaluate the
methodological quality of the included trials. Four Data synthesis and analysis
major and four minor criteria were determined on Quantitative data were analyzed with the Cochrane
the basis of a system implemented in a prior com- Collaborative Review Manager Software (RevMan

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA 7
Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep Rusch et al.

Table 2. Major and minor criteria in assessing risk of bias

Major criteria Minor criteria

Was the control matched for time and attention by the Was the randomization procedure described?
instructors?
Were evaluators blinded to participant allocation? Was allocation concealed?
Was there a description of withdrawals and dropouts? Was an intent-to-treat analysis used?
Was attrition less than 20% at postintervention assessment? Did the trial evaluate the credibility, and if so, was it
comparable?

5.3).29 All essential data that were not reported in the Characteristics of included trials
original papers were requested and received from the Publication dates ranged from 2010 to 2018. MBSR
trial authors. Since sleep quality measures differed was the most prevalent intervention (9/18), fol-
between trials (e.g., ISI, MOS-SS, and PSQI), the lowed by MBCT (3/18), and mind–body bridging
between-group standardized mean difference was (MBB) (3/18). Weekly in-class meditation sessions
used as the summary effect estimate of sleep qual- ranged from 1 to 2.5 h for 2–16 weeks. At-home
ity and was calculated as Hedges’ g. Two trials used meditation practice was encouraged in all 18 trials;
multiple sleep quality measures (ISI and PSQI).30,31 however, 12 trials recommended a specific daily
In this instance, the PSQI score was included in practice time, which ranged from 15 to 60 minutes.
the meta-analysis since it was the most common Seven trials included a one-day meditation retreat,
measure used across all trials. Outcomes were ana- and one trial offered an in-class booster session at
lyzed on change from baseline to postintervention 2 months postintervention. All 18 trials included
to evaluate between-group percent change and the at least one subjective measure of sleep quality
consistency of results across trials. A meta-analysis and two trials used an objective measure (e.g.,
was used to estimate long-term effects of trials with actigraphy). Detailed characteristics of the 18
a follow-up assessment between 5 and 12 months included trials are presented in Table 3.
from baseline. To test for relative efficacy, all analy-
Quality of included trials
ses were stratified by control type (i.e., specific active
All or most trials included a description of
control or nonspecific active control). Spearman’s
withdrawals and dropouts (18/18), described
correlation was used to examine a dose–response
the randomization procedure (17/18), matched
between in-class meditation hours and standard-
the control group for time and attention to the
ized sleep quality change scores. Heterogeneity was
meditation group (16/18), and reported attrition
evaluated using the I 2 statistic, whereby an I 2 ࣘ
rates less than 20% at postintervention (13/18).
25% was considered low, an I 2 = 50% was consid-
Quality limitations included a failure to evaluate the
ered moderate, and an I 2 ࣙ 75% was considered
intervention credibility (3/18), conceal allocation
high.32 Effect sizes were interpreted on the basis of
(8/18), blind evaluators to participant allocation
Cohen’s recommendation.33 P values of < 0.05 were
(9/18), and include an intent-to-treat analysis
considered significant.
(10/18). The majority of trials had a moderate risk
of bias (10/18), seven had a low risk of bias, and one
Results had a high risk of bias. There were no significant
differences in risk bias scores between the specific
Search results
active control and nonspecific active control groups.
A total of 3303 records were initially identified for
Certified meditation instructors were included in
inclusion in the review. After adjusting for dupli-
16 trials, trait mindfulness was assessed in 11 trials,
cates (n = 1312), another 1912 records were further
and prior meditation was explicitly excluded in
excluded on the basis of title and abstract. A full-text
10 trials.
review of the remaining 79 articles was conducted
and 18 trials with 1654 participants were included Specific active controls
in the final analysis (see CONSORT flow diagram in Seven of the included trials used specific active
Fig. 1). control groups,30,31,34–38 with a total of 716

8 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA
Rusch et al. Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep

Table 3. Study characteristics


Medi-
tation Medi- Follow-
Control Subject Control Meditation Comparison duration tation Scale/V0 Assessment time Postinter- up (5–12 Risk of
a b c d
Ref. type Population (n) (n) intervention intervention (weeks) hours mean point vention months) bias

Adler et al.34 SAC Obesity 100 94 MBSR Progressive 16 42 PSQI 6 months Ø ↑ L (9)
muscle 5.95 12 months
relaxation
Garland SAC Cancer with 64 47 MBSR CBT-I 8 18 PSQI Postintervention – – M (7)
et al.30 insomnia 12.51 5 months
Gross et al.31 SAC Insomnia 20 10 MBSR Drug 8 26 PSQI Postintervention ↑ ↑ M (8)
11.56 5 months
Schmidt SAC Fibromyalgia 53 56 MBSR Progressive 8 27 PSQI Postintervention Ø M (8)
et al.35 syndrome muscle 11.34
relaxation
van der Zwan SAC High stress 27 23 MM Exercise 5 NR PSQI Postintervention ↑ M (7)
et al.36 5.74
Vanhuffel SAC Insomnia 16 13 MM+CBT-I CBT-I 8 14 PSQI Postintervention ↑ H (5)
et al.37 13.07
Wong et al.38 SAC Insomnia 101 95 MBCT-I Sleep psycho- 8 20 ISI Postintervention + Ø L (9)
education with 17.96 8 months
exercise
Black et al.39 NSAC Older adults 24 25 MAPs Sleep hygiene 6 12 PSQI Postintervention + L (11)
with sleep education 10.2
disturbance
Dykens et al.40 NSAC Mothers with 94 108 MBSR Positive adult 6 9 ISI Postintervention + Ø M (8)
e
disabled development 12.34 7.5 months
children
Gross et al.41 NSAC Organ 71 66 MBSR Health 8 26 PSQI Postintervention + + M (7)
transplant education 7.77 12 months
Hoge et al.42 NSAC General anxiety 48 41 MBSR Stress 8 20 PSQI Postintervention + M (8)
disorder management 8.26
education
Johns et al.43 NSAC Breast and 35 36 MBSR Psycho- 8 16 ISI Postintervention Ø Ø L (12)
colorectal education 16.35 6 months
cancer support group
Malarkey NSAC Cardiovascular 93 93 MBSR– Lifestyle 8 10 PSQI Postintervention ↑ L (9)
et al.44 disease risk low dose education 8.55
Nakamura NSAC Post-traumatic 35 28 MBB Sleep hygiene 2 3 MOS-SS Postintervention + M (6)
et al.45 stress disorder education 57.73
with sleep
disturbance
Nakamura NSAC Cancer with 19 18 MBB Sleep hygiene 3 6 MOS-SS Postintervention + M (8)
et al.46 insomnia education 56.52
Nakamura NSAC Gulf war illness 33 27 MBB Sleep hygiene 3 6 MOS-SS Postintervention ↑ L (9)
et al.47 with sleep education 64.2
disturbance
Oken et al.48 NSAC Dementia 10 11 MBCT Caregiver 6 9 PSQI Postintervention Ø M (8)
caregivers education 8.33
Van Gordon NSAC Fibromyalgia 74 74 MAT Cognitive 8 18 PSQI Postintervention + + L (9)
et al.49 syndrome behavioral 14.10 6 months
theory group
a Meditation hours were reported as expected in-class hours per intervention, including the retreat.
b The v0 sleep scale mean (e.g., baseline weighted average) was used to determine that the study cohort had clinically relevant sleep
disturbance on the basis of established cutoff scores.
c Direction of effect is based on the relative difference in change analysis.
d Follow-up findings were reported for studies with a follow-up assessment between 5 and 12 months.
e Inability to obtain the Dykens et al.,40 follow-up data precluded inclusion in the meta-analysis.

CBT-I, cognitive behavioral therapy-insomnia; ISI, insomnia severity index; MAPs, meditation awareness practices; MAT, meditation
awareness training; MBB, mind–body bridging; MBCT, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy; MBCT-I, mindfulness-based cognitive
therapy for insomnia; MBSR, mindfulness-based stress reduction; MM, mindfulness meditation; MOS-SS, medical outcomes study-
sleep scale; NR, mediation was encouraged, but no specific time was reported; NSAC, nonspecific active control; PSQI, Pittsburgh
sleep quality index; RCTs, randomized controlled trials; SAC, specific active control; H, high risk of bias; M, medium risk of bias; L,
low risk of bias; +, favors meditation (>5%) and is statistically significant; –, favors control (<−5%) and is statistically significant; ↑,
favors meditation (>5%) and is not statistically significant; ↓, favors control (<−5%) and is not statistically significant; ø, no effect
(within –5% to 5%).

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA 9
Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep Rusch et al.

Figure 1. Flow diagram from record identification to a final study inclusion.

participants. There was low strength of evidence There was moderate strength of evidence that
that mindfulness meditation interventions had no mindfulness meditation interventions significantly
effect on sleep quality compared with specific active improved sleep quality compared with nonspe-
controls (i.e., evidence-based sleep treatments) at cific active controls (i.e., time/attention-matched
postintervention (ES –0.03 (95% CI –0.49 to 0.43)) controls) at postintervention (ES 0.33 (95% CI
and at a 5- to 12-month follow-up (ES –0.14 (95% 0.17–0.48)) and at a 5- to 12-month follow-up
CI –0.62 to 0.34)). This grading was based on an (ES 0.54 (95% CI 0.24–0.84)). This grading was
overall medium risk of bias, directness of measure, based on an overall medium risk of bias, directness
inconsistency of results (due to high heterogeneity of measure, consistency of results (due to low
at postintervention (I 2 = 88%) and a follow-up heterogeneity at postintervention (I 2 = 0%) and
(I 2 = 84%)), and precision of results (see Fig. 2A follow-up (I 2 = 45%)), and precision of results
and Figs. 3 and 4). (see Fig. 2B and Figs. 3 and 4).

Nonspecific active controls Dose–response effect


Eleven of the included trials used nonspecific active Seventeen trials reported on total in-class med-
control groups,39–49 with a total of 939 participants. itation hours for the intervention, which ranged

10 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA
Rusch et al. Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep

Figure 2. (A and B) Between-group relative percent difference in change score. Author, year, and sleep scale are noted at the
bottom of each cluster bar. Follow-up scores are reported for trials with a follow-up assessment between 5 and 12 months from
baseline. Percent change in sleep score was calculated using the formula: {[(postintervention meancontrol − baseline meancontrol ) −
(postintervention meanmeditation − baseline meanmeditation )]/ (baseline meanmeditation )} × 100. Positive scores should be interpreted
as relative percent change in favor of meditation. For example, a change score of 20% indicates the meditation group had a 20%
higher improvement in sleep quality score compared with the control group. Dotted lines at –5% and 5% demarcate the effect
threshold and do not indicate statistical significance. *The result is statistically significant per manuscript. (*) Overall group effect
is statistically significant; the effect for individual time points was not reported.

from 3 to 42 h (15.6 M, 9.8 SD), including the one- Three trials identified no relationship,31,38,41
day retreat. No significant correlation was found while another three trials identified a significant
between in-class meditation hours and standardized positive correlation.34,36,49 One trial investigated
sleep quality change scores (rs = 0.1, P = 0.704). a long-term dose–response effect and found a
Six trials assessed a dose–response relationship significant positive correlation between continued
between at-home practice minutes and sleep quality at-home practice minutes and additional sleep
improvements from baseline to postintervention. quality improvements at 18-month follow-up.34

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA 11
Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep Rusch et al.

Figure 3. Random effects meta-analysis of the effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep quality at postintervention, stratified
by control type. The standardized mean difference was used as the summary effect estimate and was calculated as Hedges’ g. CI,
confidence interval; IV, inverse variance; SD, standard deviation; Total, number of participants.

Sensitivity analysis (I 2 = 0%) and a similar effect size at a 5- to 12-


We conducted a sensitivity analysis to confirm month follow-up (ES 0.64 (95% CI 0.26–1.02)) (I 2
that our conclusions were not dependent on the = 44%), which remained significant. Finally, a leave-
updated evidenced-based sleep treatment determi- one-out sensitivity analysis determined that the sig-
nation. The specific active control group (minus the nificant heterogeneity in the specific active control
two exercise control trials36,38 ) had similar results at meta-analysis was the result of a single CBT-I trial.30
postintervention (ES –0.14 (95% CI –0.80 to 0.53)) The heterogeneity was reduced from 88% and 84%
(I 2 = 91%) and at a 5- to 12-month follow-up to 0% at postintervention and at a 5- to 12-month
(ES –0.19 (95% CI –0.96 to 0.58)) (I 2 = 89%). The follow-up. Results were similar at postintervention
nonspecific active control group (plus the two exer- (ES 0.15 (95% CI –0.01 to 0.31)) and at a 5- to 12-
cise control trials) also had similar and significant month follow-up (ES 0.10 (95% CI –0.09 to 0.29)),
results at postintervention (ES 0.30 (95% CI 0.17– with a change in direction of effect.
0.43)) (I 2 = 14%) and did not report additional 5- to
12-month follow-up data. A second sensitivity Discussion
analysis was conducted by including trials with The evidence suggests that mindfulness meditation
a PSQI score greater than 10, and an ISI score can improve sleep quality in a variety of clinical pop-
greater than 14, which are indicative of severe sleep ulations with sleep disturbance. While our results
disturbance.50,51 The specific active controls had indicated no effect of mindfulness meditation on
a similar effect size at postintervention (ES –0.12 sleep quality when compared with evidenced-based
(95% CI –0.80 to 0.57)) (I 2 = 91%) and a decreased sleep treatments, the strength of evidence was
effect size at a 5- to 12-month follow-up (ES – low and further studies are needed to elucidate
0.26 (95% CI –0.93 to 0.42)) (I 2 = 85%), which these findings. Results also indicated that mindful-
remained nonsignificant. Meanwhile, the nonspe- ness meditation significantly improved sleep qual-
cific active controls had an increased effect size ity compared with nonspecific active controls. This
at postintervention (ES 0.52 (95% CI 0.32–0.72)) meta-analysis only included RCTs with an active

12 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA
Rusch et al. Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep

Figure 4. Random effects meta-analysis of the effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep quality at a 5- to 12-month follow-up,
stratified by control type. The standardized mean difference was used as the summary effect estimate and was calculated as Hedges’
g. CI, confidence interval; IV, inverse variance; SD, standard deviation; Total, number of participants.

comparator group, so there is greater confidence different patient populations may result in larger
that the reported benefits are not attributed to effects with shorter course durations. Moreover, the
placebo effects commonly observed in usual care nonlinear trajectory of mediation progress is often
and waitlist control trials. misunderstood.57 Traditionally, success is defined by
At a 5- to 12-month follow-up, mindfulness increased awareness and equanimity, whereby pos-
meditation did not differ in effect from evidence- itive states are a byproduct. When symptom change
based sleep treatments and significantly improved over a short period is utilized as a benchmark of
sleep quality compared with nonspecific active success, meditation progress and its potential effect
controls. These findings provide preliminary on well-being may be veiled.
evidence for a long-term effect. The maintenance Of the 10 trials that reported on adverse events,
of intervention effects may be attributed to learned there was no evidence of increased risk of harm.
techniques that reduce sleep-interfering cognitive Two trials reported a worsening of sleep quality in
processes,20 changes in sleep architecture,52 as 3% and 7% of the meditation groups, compared
well as morphometric and connectivity alterations with 24% and 12% in the comparator groups.45,47
in sleep-related brain regions.53,54 Despite these Another trial reported one case of muscle soreness
advances, additional evidence is needed to clarify in the meditation group and one case of sleep dis-
the conditions and mechanisms that drive the ruption in the control group.42 It is not uncommon
maintenance of intervention effects. for symptoms to worsen, particularly in the early
The evidence did not support a dose–response weeks of the intervention.58 Feelings of anger, sad-
relationship between in-class meditation hours and ness, or fear may appear stronger as practice devel-
sleep quality scores. This finding is consistent with a ops since present moment awareness can highlight
meta-analysis of 20 trials that assessed the relation- emotions.59 A history of trauma, mental instabil-
ship between in-class meditation hours and psycho- ity, addiction, or major life changes may heighten
logical distress.55 The link between at-home practice emotional reactivity and require additional clinical
minutes and sleep quality scores was inconclusive monitoring.60
because of the limited number of trials that assessed
this relationship. Dose–response relationships are Limitations
arguably one of the most challenging measures There are several limitations that reduced our abil-
in meditation research. It is difficult to accurately ity to draw robust conclusions from these results. At
assess how mindful (versus mind wandering) an the meta-analysis level, a leave-one-out sensitivity
individual is during meditation practice.56 Studies analysis indicated substantial heterogeneity owing
with tailored curriculums, expert instructors, and to the inclusion of a single CBT-I trial.30 This may be

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA 13
Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep Rusch et al.

attributed to the large positive effects CBT-I is esti- execution of this study. This research did not receive
mated to have on sleep quality when compared with any specific grant from funding agencies in the pub-
other evidenced-based sleep treatments.61 It might lic, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. The work
also be due to the 50% attrition rate in the MBSR was performed at the National Institutes of Health
group (versus 14% in the CBT-I group). Partici- (NIH), Bethesda, MD. The NIH had no role in the
pants who withdrew typically did so within the first study design, collection, analysis, or interpretation
3 weeks and had higher levels of baseline insomnia of the data, or the writing of the manuscript. The
severity, which may have attenuated effects.30 Addi- manuscript received publication clearance by the
tional heterogeneity might have been introduced by NIH.
combining scores from the ISI, MOS-SS, and PSQI
to create a single global sleep quality score. At the Author contributions
study level, the most common drawbacks were a fail- H.L.R. conceived the study, designed the study,
ure to evaluate the intervention credibility and con- acquired the data, analyzed the data, interpreted the
ceal allocation, which may lead to expectation bias. data, and drafted the manuscript; H.L.R. accepts
The lack of comprehensive reporting of treatment responsibility for the integrity of the data analyzed.
adherence and adverse events limited our ability to M.R. designed the study, acquired the data, ana-
rigorously examine the effect of a dose–response and lyzed the data, interpreted the data, and drafted
assess for safety. Moreover, only two trials included the manuscript. L.M.L., A.O., and W.S.L. acquired
an objective measure of total sleep time via actig- the data and drafted the manuscript. T.W. ana-
raphy. One trial identified a statistically significant lyzed the data, interpreted the data, and drafted the
between-group effect in favor of MBSR at a 5-month manuscript. J.M.G. obtained funding, designed the
follow-up, but not at postintervention.30 The other study, and drafted the manuscript. All authors have
trial did not report between-group effects.31 revised the important intellectual content critically
and have read and approved the final manuscript.
Future directions
These findings support continued research explor- Competing interests
ing the clinical application of mindfulness med-
itation and provide a foundation for healthcare The authors declare no competing interests.
providers to consider these interventions in sleep-
disturbed populations. Future research in mind-
fulness meditation would benefit from address- References
ing the outstanding methodological limitations, as 1. Morin, C. & D. Jarrin. 2013. Epidemiology of insomnia:
well as incorporating adherence measures, such as prevalence, course, risk factors, and public health burden.
Sleep Med. Clin. 8: 281–297.
mobile applications, so that participants can eas-
2. Baglioni, C. et al. 2011. Insomnia as a predictor of depression:
ily record at-home practice time. Future research a meta-analytic evaluation of longitudinal epidemiological
should include systematic reporting of adverse studies. J. Affect. Disord. 135: 10–19.
events, which can help identify factors of increased 3. Ritter, P.S., C. Marx, M. Bauer, et al. 2011. The role of dis-
risk. Researchers should use a combination of objec- turbed sleep in the early recognition of bipolar disorder: a
systematic review. Bipolar Disord. 13: 227–237.
tive and subjective sleep outcomes to better under-
4. Rusch, H.L., P. Guardado, T. Baxter, et al. 2015. Improved
stand if improved sleep quality is due to reduced sleep quality is associated with reductions in depression and
sleep onset latency, improved total sleep time, or PTSD arousal symptoms and increases in IGF-1 concentra-
some other factor. The effectiveness of web- and tions. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 11: 615–623.
app-based mindfulness meditation interventions 5. Durmer, J.S. & D.F. Dinges. 2005. Neurocognitive conse-
quences of sleep deprivation. Semin. Neurol. 25: 117–129.
should be investigated to increase accessibility, espe-
6. Sofi, F. et al. 2014. Insomnia and risk of cardiovascular dis-
cially for low-income minorities with poor health ease: a meta-analysis. Eur. J. Prev. Cardiol. 21: 57–64.
and barriers to access. 7. Taheri, S., L. Lin, D. Austin, et al. 2004. Short sleep dura-
tion is associated with reduced leptin, elevated ghrelin, and
Acknowledgments increased body mass index. PLoS Med. 1: e62.
8. Buscemi, N. et al. 2007. The efficacy and safety of drug
We are grateful to Dr. Madhav Goyal for being an treatments for chronic insomnia in adults: a meta-analysis
invaluable asset to the team and to the design and of RCTs. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 22: 1335–1350.

14 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA
Rusch et al. Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep

9. Holbrook, A.M., R. Crowther, A. Lotter, et al. 2000. Meta- 27. Goyal, M. et al. 2014. Meditation programs for psychological
analysis of benzodiazepine use in the treatment of insomnia. stress and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
CMAJ 162: 225–233. JAMA Intern. Med. 174: 357–368.
10. Koffel, E., A.D. Bramoweth & C.S. Ulmer. 2018. Increasing 28. Owens, D.K. et al. 2010. AHRQ series paper 5: grading the
access to and utilization of cognitive behavioral therapy for strength of a body of evidence when comparing medical
insomnia (CBT-I): a narrative review. J. Gen. Intern. Med. interventions—Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality
33: 955–962. and the Effective Health-Care Program. J. Clin. Epidemiol.
11. Ong, J.C., T.F. Kuo & R. Manber. 2008. Who is at risk for 63: 513–523.
dropout from group cognitive-behavior therapy for insom- 29. The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration.
nia? J. Psychosom. Res. 64: 419–425. 2014. Review.
12. Vincent, N., S. Lewycky & H. Finnegan. 2008. Barriers to 30. Garland, S.N. et al. 2014. Mindfulness-based stress reduc-
engagement in sleep restriction and stimulus control in tion compared with cognitive behavioral therapy for the
chronic insomnia. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 76: 820–828. treatment of insomnia comorbid with cancer: a random-
13. Fernandez, E., D. Salem, J.K. Swift & N. Ramtahal. 2015. ized, partially blinded, noninferiority trial. J. Clin. Oncol.
Meta-analysis of dropout from cognitive behavioral therapy: 32: 449–457.
magnitude, timing, and moderators. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 31. Gross, C.R. et al. 2011. Mindfulness-based stress reduc-
83: 1108–1122. tion versus pharmacotherapy for chronic primary insom-
14. Kabat-Zinn, J. 2004. Wherever You Go, There You Are. Hype- nia: a randomized controlled clinical trial. Explore (N.Y.) 7:
rion. 76–87.
15. Davis, J.H. & E. Thompson. 2017. From the five aggregates 32. Higgins, J.P., S.G. Thompson, J.J. Deeks & D.G. Altman.
to phenomenal consciousness: toward a cross-cultural cog- 2003. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327:
nitive science. MindRxiv. https://doi.org/10.31231/osf.io/ 557–560.
bs8af. 33. Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
16. Jain, S. et al. 2007. A randomized controlled trial of mind- sciences. Routledge Academic.
fulness meditation versus relaxation training: effects on dis- 34. Adler, E. et al. 2016. Impact of a mindfulness-based weight-
tress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. loss intervention on sleep quality among adults with obesity:
Ann. Behav. Med. 33: 11–21. data from the SHINE Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Altern.
17. Desbordes, G. et al. 2012. Effects of mindful-attention and Complement. Med. 23: 188–195.
compassion meditation training on amygdala response to 35. Schmidt, S. et al. 2011. Treating fibromyalgia with
emotional stimuli in an ordinary, non-meditative state. mindfulness-based stress reduction: results from a 3-armed
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6: 292. randomized controlled trial. Pain 152: 361–369.
18. van Vugt, M.K., P. Hitchcock, B. Shahar & W. Britton. 2012. 36. van der Zwan, J.E., W. de Vente, A.C. Huizink, et al. 2015.
The effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on affec- Physical activity, mindfulness meditation, or heart rate
tive memory recall dynamics in depression: a mechanistic variability biofeedback for stress reduction: a randomized
model of rumination. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6: 257. controlled trial. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 40: 257–
19. Ong, J.C., C.S. Ulmer & R. Manber. 2012. Improving sleep 268.
with mindfulness and acceptance: a metacognitive model of 37. Vanhuffel, H., M. Rey, I. Lambert, et al. 2018. [Contribution
insomnia. Behav. Res. Ther. 50: 651–660. of mindfulness meditation in cognitive behavioral therapy
20. Winbush, N.Y., C.R. Gross & M.J. Kreitzer. 2007. The effects for insomnia]. Encephale 44: 134–140.
of mindfulness-based stress reduction on sleep disturbance: 38. Wong, S.Y. et al. 2017. Comparing the effects of mindfulness-
a systematic review. Explore (N.Y.) 3: 585–591. based cognitive therapy and sleep psycho-education with
21. Kanen, J.W., R.F. Nazir, K. Sedky & P.K. Basant. 2015. The exercise on chronic insomnia: a randomised controlled trial.
effects of mindfulness-based interventions on sleep distur- Psychother. Psychosom. 86: 241–253.
bance: a meta-analysis. Adolesc. Psychiatry 5: 105–115. 39. Black, D.S., G.A. O’Reilly, R. Olmstead, et al. 2015. Mind-
22. Gong, H. et al. 2016. Mindfulness meditation for insomnia: a fulness meditation and improvement in sleep quality and
meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Psychosom. daytime impairment among older adults with sleep distur-
Res. 89: 1–6. bances: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern. Med. 175:
23. Goyal, M. et al. 2014. Meditation programs for psychological 494–501.
stress and well-being. Comp. Eff. Rev. 124: 1–194. 40. Dykens, E.M., M.H. Fisher, J.L. Taylor, et al. 2014. Reduc-
24. Liberati, A. et al. 2009. The PRISMA statement for reporting ing distress in mothers of children with autism and other
systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evalu- disabilities: a randomized trial. Pediatrics 134: e454–e463.
ate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. 41. Gross, C.R. et al. 2010. Mindfulness-based stress reduction
J. Clin. Epidemiol. 62: e1–e34. for solid organ transplant recipients: a randomized con-
25. Morgenthaler, T. et al. 2006. Practice parameters for the psy- trolled trial. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 16: 30–38.
chological and behavioral treatment of insomnia: an update. 42. Hoge, E.A. et al. 2013. Randomized controlled trial of mind-
An American Academy of sleep medicine report. Sleep 29: fulness meditation for generalized anxiety disorder: effects
1415–1419. on anxiety and stress reactivity. J. Clin. Psychiatry 74: 786–
26. Kredlow, M.A., M.C. Capozzoli, B.A. Hearon, et al. 2015. The 792.
effects of physical activity on sleep: a meta-analytic review. 43. Johns, S.A. et al. 2016. Randomized controlled pilot
J. Behav. Med. 38: 427–449. trial of mindfulness-based stress reduction compared to

Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA 15
Effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep Rusch et al.

psychoeducational support for persistently fatigued breast 52. Nagendra, R.P., N. Maruthai & B.M. Kutty. 2012. Meditation
and colorectal cancer survivors. Support. Care Cancer 24: and its regulatory role on sleep. Front. Neurol. 3: 54.
4085–4096. 53. Fox, K.C. et al. 2014. Is meditation associated with altered
44. Malarkey, W.B., D. Jarjoura & M. Klatt. 2013. Workplace brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of
based mindfulness practice and inflammation: a randomized morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners.
trial. Brain Behav. Immun. 27: 145–154. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 43C: 48–73.
45. Nakamura, Y., D.L. Lipschitz, R. Landward, et al. 2011. 54. Hasenkamp, W. & L.W. Barsalou. 2012. Effects of meditation
Two sessions of sleep-focused mind-body bridging improve experience on functional connectivity of distributed brain
self-reported symptoms of sleep and PTSD in veterans: a networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6: 38.
pilot randomized controlled trial. J. Psychosom. Res. 70: 55. Carmody, J. & R.A. Baer. 2009. How long does a
335–345. mindfulness-based stress reduction program need to be? A
46. Nakamura, Y., D.L. Lipschitz, R. Kuhn, et al. 2013. Investigat- review of class contact hours and effect sizes for psycholog-
ing efficacy of two brief mind-body intervention programs ical distress. J. Clin. Psychol. 65: 627–638.
for managing sleep disturbance in cancer survivors: a pilot 56. Davidson, R.J. & A.W. Kaszniak. 2015. Conceptual and
randomized controlled trial. J. Cancer Surviv. 7: 165–182. methodological issues in research on mindfulness and med-
47. Nakamura, Y. et al. 2017. Investigating clinical benefits of a itation. Am. Psychol. 70: 581–592.
novel sleep-focused mind-body program on Gulf war illness 57. Britton, W.B., J.R. Lindahl, B.R. Cahn, et al. 2014. Awaken-
symptoms: a randomized controlled trial. Psychosom. Med. ing is not a metaphor: the effects of Buddhist meditation
79: 706–718. practices on basic wakefulness. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1307:
48. Oken, B.S. et al. 2010. Pilot controlled trial of mindfulness 64–81.
meditation and education for dementia caregivers. J. Altern. 58. Creswell, J.D. 2017. Mindfulness interventions. Annu. Rev.
Complement. Med. 16: 1031–1038. Psychol. 68: 491–516.
49. Van Gordon, W., E. Shonin, T.J. Dunn, et al. 2017. Med- 59. Ludwig, D.S. & J. Kabat-Zinn. 2008. Mindfulness in
itation awareness training for the treatment of fibromyal- medicine. JAMA 300: 1350–1352.
gia syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. Br. J. Health 60. Crane, R.S. & W. Kuyken. 2013. The implementation of
Psychol. 22: 186–206. mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: learning from the
50. Okun, M.L. et al. 2009. Psychometric evaluation of the UK Health Service Experience. Mindfulness (N.Y.) 4: 246–
Insomnia Symptom Questionnaire: a self-report measure 254.
to identify chronic insomnia. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 5: 41–51. 61. Qaseem, A., S. Kansagara, M.A. Forciea, et al. 2016. Man-
51. Gagnon, C., L. Belanger, H. Ivers & C.M. Morin. 2013. agement of chronic insomnia disorder in adults: a clinical
Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index in primary care. practice guideline from the American College of Physicians.
J. Am. Board Fam. Med. 26: 701–710. Ann. Intern. Med. 165: 125–133.

16 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1445 (2019) 5–16 Published 2018. This article is a U.S. Government work and is in the public domain in the USA

You might also like