Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

The Get Ready Mind-Set: How Gearing Up for

Later Impacts Effort Allocation Now


ANICK BOSMANS
RIK PIETERS
HANS BAUMGARTNER

People need to allocate their limited cognitive resources to current and future tasks.
We provide evidence that anticipating the resource demands of a future task creates
a “get ready mind-set” that mobilizes these resources. However, the mobilized re-
sources for the future task can carry over to unrelated current tasks. This implies
the counterintuitive notion that anticipating difficult tasks in the future leads to greater
effort expenditure on unrelated tasks in the present. We also demonstrate that re-
source carryover is particularly likely when consumers’ ability to separate tasks is
low, whereas resource conservation is more likely when ability to separate is high.

T his research examines if and how consumers’ expec-


tations about the demands that future tasks may impose
on their cognitive resources influence the allocation of these
bilization of cognitive resources, because people want to get
ready for the future task. Intuitively, one may expect that
this “get ready mind-set” will lead to attempts to expend
resources to current tasks. That is, we explore how much fewer resources on current tasks in an effort to conserve
effort consumers will allocate to a task that they engage in resources for the difficult future task (Muraven, Shmueli,
right now, knowing that it will be followed by a demanding and Burkley 2006). However, in the present research we
or simple task later. Recent studies have shown how the propose that getting ready for a future task may actually
actual effort allocated to current tasks influences perfor- increase rather than decrease effort expenditure on current
mance on later tasks (Vohs and Faber 2007), but it remains tasks, because the resources mobilized for future task de-
unclear how anticipated future task demands affect perfor- mands may inadvertently carry over to unrelated tasks in
mance on unrelated current tasks. This is surprising in view the present. More specifically, the main purpose of this ar-
of the fact that people consecutively pursue different tasks ticle is twofold: (1) to provide evidence in support of the
all the time and need to consider how much effort to expend operation of the get ready mind-set and (2) to show that the
on a current task, knowing that it will be followed by later resources marshaled in anticipation of difficult future tasks
tasks: thinking about the weekly grocery list before pre- can indeed carry over to unrelated current tasks.
paring lunch, searching the Internet for a family holiday We do not assume that resource carryover will always be
location before making a decision about a home improve- the norm and allow for the possibility that in certain situ-
ment loan, or trying to come up with a creative birthday ations resource conservation will occur. Our second purpose
gift before choosing a restaurant. in this article is to examine a potential moderator of the
We propose that anticipating resource demands imposed predicted effects, namely, a person’s ability to keep tasks
by a difficult future task may encourage the immediate mo- separate. That is, we propose that carryover of mobilized
resources will be more likely when people find it difficult
Anick Bosmans (abosmans@uvt.nl) is assistant professor of marketing, to separate tasks, either because of situational reasons (the
and Rik Pieters (pieters@uvt.nl) is professor of marketing, in the Marketing tasks are similar) or personal reasons (people are intrinsi-
Department, Tilburg University, Warandelaan 2, P.O. Box 90153, 5000 LE cally bad at keeping things separate), whereas conservation
Tilburg, The Netherlands. Hans Baumgartner ( jxb14@psu.edu) is professor is more likely when ability to separate is high (because tasks
of marketing and Charles and Lillian Binder Faculty Fellow in the Smeal
College of Business, Pennsylvania State University, 482 Business Building, are dissimilar or people are intrinsically good at keeping
University Park, PA 16802. The authors thank Kathleen Vohs, the editors, things separate).
and three reviewers for insightful comments on previous drafts of this
article.
GETTING READY FOR FUTURE
John Deighton served as editor and Laura Peracchio served as associate PERFORMANCE
editor for this article.
Electronically published October 20, 2009
We propose that difficult future tasks that are expected
to be resource demanding promote the activation of a get
98

䉷 2009 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc. ● Vol. 37 ● June 2010


All rights reserved. 0093-5301/2010/3701-0020$10.00. DOI: 10.1086/648520
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706
by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
THE GET READY MIND-SET 99

ready mind-set in which people mobilize the resources re- that the mere prospect of engaging in a difficult future task
quired for the execution of the future task. However, once makes people gear up by mobilizing the required resources.
activated, these resources are not necessarily reserved for
the future task and instead may carry over to unrelated CARRYOVER OF MOBILIZED
current tasks, which leads to the somewhat counterintuitive RESOURCES
prediction that the anticipation of a difficult task in the future
leads to greater effort expenditure on unrelated tasks in the How does the proposed get ready mind-set, which pre-
present. sumably prepares people for future task demands, influence
Although different in focus, research on the implemen- the allocation of cognitive resources to current tasks? At
tation mind-set is broadly consistent with the proposed pro- first sight, you might think that expecting a difficult (vs.
cess. According to Gollwitzer and his colleagues (Gollwitzer easy) future task should activate attempts to conserve (vs.
1990; Gollwitzer, Heckhausen, and Steller 1990), the im- expend) resources during unrelated current tasks. However,
plementation of a chosen goal is associated with a partic- several lines of research suggest that cognitive, affective,
ular cognitive orientation called an “implementation mind- and motivational states evoked by one context can readily
set,” which creates a special preparedness to efficiently carry over to other, unrelated contexts. Then, expecting a
plan and execute the future task. Specifically, these authors difficult future task should lead to working harder on an
show that the prospect of implementing a future task makes unrelated current task. The previously cited research by
people more selective with respect to task-related versus Gollwitzer et al. (1990) on the transfer of implementation
task-unrelated information and increases their sensitivity to thoughts to an unrelated storytelling task provides the most
direct support for this. Indirect support for our hypothesis
implementation-related procedures such as how, when, and
comes from research on the effects of residual arousal in-
where to execute the intended task. Importantly, the acti-
duced by earlier tasks on subsequent tasks. In that literature,
vated procedural knowledge associated with an implemen-
it is usually found that the excitation felt after physiological
tation mind-set has been found to transfer from one task to exercises transfers to subsequent activities and may result
another. For example, relative to participants in a control in increased aggressive behavior (Zillmann, Johnson, and
group, those who were asked to plan the execution of a Day 1974) or sexual excitement (Cantor, Zillmann, and Bry-
future project (e.g., moving from home or terminating one’s ant 1975). In a consumer behavior context, residual phys-
college education) reported more implementation-related iological arousal was found to affect information-processing
thoughts in an immediate, unrelated storytelling task (e.g., strategies (Sanbonmatsu and Kardes 1988). In a related,
“The king asked himself how he could find a trusted person albeit more general, vein, transfer effects have also been
who would stay home and protect his daughter”; Gollwitzer reported in the mood literature, where moods experienced
et al. 1990, 1127). in one situation may carry over to other contexts and influ-
Of course, the effective implementation of tasks requires ence decision making in that context (Pham 1998; Schwarz
not only the activation of procedural knowledge involved and Clore 1983). The implication of these various streams
in planning and executing a chosen goal but also the allo- of research is that consumers may actually expend more
cation of sufficient cognitive resources that allow successful rather than less effort on unrelated current tasks when a
task completion (Locke and Latham 2006; Wright 1996). difficult (vs. easy) future task is anticipated, because re-
We therefore specify analogously to the operation of an sources mobilized in preparation for a later difficult task
implementation mind-set the operation of a get ready mind- inadvertently carry over to earlier tasks.
set. Although the idea that anticipating a future task will The first three studies report evidence that anticipating a
activate a get ready mind-set is new and has not been directly difficult future task generates a get ready mind-set, in which
tested, to our knowledge, several findings in the literature required cognitive resources are mobilized that are subse-
support the plausibility of such a mind-set. For instance, quently allocated to an unrelated current task. The final two
when people were instructed that they would have to engage studies examine boundary conditions.
in a demanding task, they exhibited higher levels of systolic
blood pressure and had higher heart rates than people who STUDY 1
were told that an easy task would follow (Contrada, Wright,
This study tests the basic idea that more effort is allocated
and Glass 1984). Also, neuroimaging research indicates that to a current task (henceforth called the focal task) when a
anticipating a future task activates brain regions in the pre- difficult rather than easy future task is anticipated.
frontal cortex that are associated with executive control and
preparation of motor activity (Brass and von Cramon 2002).
In particular, it has been found that after task instructions Method
(but before actual task execution) there is increased acti- Thirty-four undergraduate students participated in this
vation of the anterior prefrontal cortex, and this activation two-group experiment in return for monetary compensation
appears to be independent of the specific domain of the of i7. They were told that they would engage in two dif-
anticipated task (e.g., verbal vs. spatial; Sakai and Pas- ferent tasks. First, concern with future task performance was
singham 2003). These findings are consistent with the idea primed by asking participants to write a short essay about

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
100 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

a personal (easy or difficult) task that they planned to carry This is desirable because the information was identical, and
out in the near future. In the Easy Future Task condition, stimuli were not constructed so that exposure to more in-
participants wrote about a simple and routine task. In the formation would increase evaluation.
Difficult Future Task condition, they wrote about a difficult In support of our prediction, when participants wrote about
and complex task. The instructions were (Difficult Future a difficult future task, they later consulted (in an unrelated
Task instructions are in brackets): task) more information for purposes of evaluating the new
product compared to when they first wrote about an easy
We are currently investigating how people experience routine future task: Mdif f p 6.65 versus Measy p 3.59 (F(1, 32)
[complex] tasks. Routine tasks are tasks that are relatively p 9.64, p ! .01, h 2 p .23). These results support the op-
simple, require no or little energy, and are relatively effort- eration of a get ready mind-set and carryover of resources
less. You do not become tired after performing a routine task. to an unrelated current task: the more difficult the future
[Complex tasks are tasks that are difficult, require a sub- task was anticipated to be, the more effort participants ex-
stantial amount of energy, and are relatively effortful. You pended on the current task.
become tired after performing a complex task.] In this task
you are asked to describe—in as much detail as possible—a
routine [complex] task that you plan to carry out in the near
STUDY 2
future (i.e., in a few moments or in a couple of hours). Think This study had two primary purposes. First, we wanted
about a task that you expect to be easy and effortless [difficult to generalize the effects to a qualitatively different focal
and effortful]. task. Study 1 used performance on a convergent, analytical
thinking task (Guilford 1967) as the measure of effort in
Instructions asked to write as elaborately and detailed as the focal task. Participants evaluated a new product based
possible to ensure mind-set activation: on various information about the product, and the number
of information links consulted was used as the effort mea-
In the space below please describe as elaborately and in as sure. In study 2 we chose an effort measure that reflects a
much detail as possible an easy, routine task [a difficult, more divergent, creative way of thinking, namely, idea gen-
complex task] that you anticipate to carry out in the near eration about how to lose body weight. The number of sep-
future. Also, discuss the reasons why you anticipate this task arate ideas produced was taken as the measure of cognitive
to be easy [difficult] and why the task will require little or effort.
no [a lot of] energy. Second, study 2 tested the idea that a get ready mind-set
influences not only quantitative but also qualitative aspects
After completing the first study, participants were thanked of cognitive effort in task performance. Gollwitzer et al.
and asked to engage in a presumably unrelated second study, (1990) observed that people in an implementation mind-set
which contained the focal task. In this focal task, participants produced more thoughts about when and how a fictional
evaluated a new (fictitious) diet drink. The new product was character in a storytelling task would act. Building on this,
presented on a computer screen, and participants were in- we predicted that, in case of an anticipated difficult future
structed to consult as much information as necessary to task, participants would not only generate a greater number
arrive at a good evaluation of the new product (adapted of ideas in the focal task but also produce more effort-related
from Martin et al. 1993). They could do so by clicking on (strength and energy) ideas and that these ideas would require
several “links” (eight in total) that contained information more effort to come up with as rated by external judges.
about ingredients and manufacturer and consumer testi-
monials. Participants were instructed to click the “OK” but-
ton when they felt they had sufficient information to evaluate Method
the product. The number of information links consulted was Seventy-seven undergraduate students participated in this
the measure of effort allocation. After completion, partici- two-group experiment in return for monetary compensation
pants evaluated the new drink on four bipolar attitude items of i7. They were informed that they would engage in two
(I like it/I dislike it, good/bad, positive/negative, desirable/ different tasks and were randomly assigned to either the
undesirable; alpha p .87), using response scales ranging Easy or Difficult Future Task condition. Future Task Dif-
from 1 to 9. ficulty was manipulated by instructing participants that one
of the tasks dealt with creativity in buying small (i.e., i1)
Results gifts. Participants were told that they would have to come
up with as many gifts as possible that one can buy for i1
Two one-way ANOVAs were performed with evaluation for various occasions (birthday, Easter, etc.). In the Easy
of the new drink and the number of information links con- Future Task condition, the task was described as rather easy
sulted as dependent variables. We report h2 values as a mea- (most people had little difficulty coming up with several
sure of effect size when F-values are significant, both here ideas), whereas in the Difficult Future Task condition, the
and in subsequent studies. Participants evaluated the new task was described as difficult (most people found it hard
product equally favorably in the two Future Task Difficulty to come up with different ideas). After having received this
conditions (F(1, 32) ! 1; Mdif f p 4.12 vs. Measy p 3.74). instruction, participants were told, “before we turn to this

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
THE GET READY MIND-SET 101

gift-giving task, you will be asked to participate in another pated a difficult future task generated more dieting ideas than
study. More specifically, you will be asked to list ideas about participants who anticipated an easy future task: F(1, 75) p
how to lose weight in a time span of 1 month.” 6.61, p ! .05, h 2 p .08 (Mdiff p 3.97 vs. Measy p 3.08).
To gauge the effectiveness of the Future Task Difficulty Also, participants in the Difficult Future Task condition had
manipulation, participants indicated the expected difficulty more high effort-related thoughts than participants in the
of the first (focal) task and second (future) task (“how dif- Easy Future Task condition: F(1, 75) p 7.65, p ! .01, h2 p
ficult do you expect the first weight loss task to be?” and .20 (Mdiff p .49 vs. Measy p ⫺.10). Furthermore, the inde-
“how difficult do you expect the second gift-listing task to pendent judges indicated that participants in the Difficult Fu-
be?” respectively; both rated on 9-point scales ranging from ture Task condition, on average, had put more effort into
“not at all difficult” [1] to “very difficult” [9]). generating their ideas than participants in the Easy Future
Next, participants engaged in the “weight loss” idea gen- Task condition: F(1, 75) p 13.74, p ! .001, h 2 p .16 (Mdiff
eration task and were instructed as follows: p 2.36 vs. Measy p 1.84).
These results demonstrate the hypothesized carryover of
Imagine the following scenario: 1 month from now, you are resources on both quantitative and qualitative measures of
invited to a high school reunion. You haven’t seen your class- cognitive effort in a task that requires divergent thinking.
mates for years, and you want to make a good impression. Moreover, people in the easy and difficult future task con-
One of your goals is to lose 6 pounds of weight by the time ditions did not differ in their judgments of anticipated effort
you meet your former classmates. required for the focal (current) task, but they did differ sig-
In the space below, please describe in as much detail as nificantly in the amount of effort that they actually expended
possible how you could lose 6 pounds in a time span of 1 on the focal task. While gearing up for a difficult future
month. Please list as many ideas as possible that would enable task, they worked harder on the current task.
you to do so. Although our studies show that resources mobilized for
a difficult later task can carry over to an unrelated earlier
The number of separate weight loss ideas was the quan- task, they do not provide direct evidence about the process
titative effort measure. The qualitative effort measures were, hypothesized to account for the effect. Study 3 aims to pro-
respectively, the number of effort-related ideas (i.e., explicit vide such evidence.
mentions of effort and energy) and independent observer
ratings of effort (i.e., effort required to generate the ideas
as rated by independent judges). Specifically, the first mea-
STUDY 3
sure of qualitative effort was the difference between the We have argued that making people aware of a difficult
number of high-effort ideas (e.g., hard work, costs a lot of future task makes them gear up for this future task and
energy, is effortful, is difficult) and low-effort ideas (e.g., promotes a state of readiness to act. Alternatively, one might
costs little energy, effortless, easy). The second measure was argue that making people aware of a difficult later task cre-
the average score of 12 judges (trained PhD students) who ates an expectation that they would also have to work hard
rated the effort that participants had put into formulating each on unrelated earlier tasks, thus increasing their motivation
idea (10-point scale ranging from “very easy to come up with” to do well on these tasks. According to such an account,
[1] to “very difficult to come up with” [10]; alpha p .94). the higher performance on the current task is due to raised
performance standards (Carver and Scheier 1998) rather
Results than resource carryover. A get ready mind-set would be
supported if information about the difficult future task pro-
A mixed between-/within-subjects ANOVA with Future moted a state of action readiness, even though their standards
Task Difficulty as the between-subjects factor and Antici- of performance with respect to the current, focal task re-
pated Difficulty level of the focal and future tasks as the mained unaffected.
within-subjects factor tested the success of our manipulation.
In general, participants expected the future task to be more Method
difficult than the focal task (Mfut p 4.62 vs. Mfoc p 4.18;
F(1, 75) p 4.04, p ! .05). More important, and confirming Forty undergraduate students participated in a two-group
the success of the manipulation, a significant interaction ef- study in return for a monetary compensation of i7. They
fect was observed between our Future Task Difficulty ma- were informed that they would be participating in two (sup-
nipulation and the Anticipated Difficulty measures: F(1, 75) posedly) unrelated studies that were briefly described at the
p 21.39, p ! .001. Participants in the Difficult Future Task beginning of the experimental session. Participants were told
condition expected that the future task would be more dif- that the first study would investigate participants’ ideas
ficult than participants in the Easy Future Task condition about losing weight and that this would be followed by an
(Mdiff p 5.84 vs. Measy p 3.40; F(1, 75) p 44.94, p ! .001, anagram study. Future Task Difficulty was manipulated by
h 2 p .38). In contrast and as expected, there was no dif- telling participants that the second study would be a difficult
ference in the expected difficulty level of the focal (first) and complex (vs. simple and easy) task and that in an earlier
task (Mdiff p 4.38 vs. Measy p 3.98; F(1, 75) p 1.06, NS). study only 10% (vs. 90%) of the participants were able to
Consistent with our predictions, participants who antici- recognize half of the anagrams. After having read the in-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
102 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

structions, participants were asked several questions about focal task (Mdiff p 5.14 vs. Measy p 5.86; F(1, 38) p 3.38,
the studies. p ! .07, h 2 p .08).
First, the effectiveness of the Future Task Difficulty ma- Together, the first three studies provide evidence that an-
nipulation was assessed as in the previous study (i.e., “how ticipating a difficult future task activates a get ready mind-
difficult do you expect the first weight loss task to be?” and set that prepares people to act (study 3) and that the resources
“how difficult do you expect the second word recognition mobilized can carry over to a current, focal task (studies 1
task to be?”; both rated on 9-point scales ranging from “not and 2). These findings are not due to the fact that the an-
at all difficult” [1] to “very difficult” [9]). ticipated difficulty level of the future task informs people
In addition, participants’ readiness for the future task was about the difficulty level of the current task or that expecting
assessed with three items: “I feel ready to start working on a difficult later task establishes a higher standard of per-
the future anagram task,” “I am well prepared for the future formance that is applied to all tasks, including the earlier
anagram study,” and “I almost can’t wait to start working on ones (study 3).
the future anagram task,” rated on 9-point scales ranging from
“totally disagree” (1) to “totally agree” (9) (alpha p .73).
Also, participants’ standard of performance concerning CARRYOVER VERSUS CONSERVATION
the current task was measured with two items: “how much OF MOBILIZED RESOURCES
effort do you think you will have to put into the first weight
loss study?” and “how hard do you plan to work on the first We presented evidence that cognitive resources mobilized
weight loss study?” assessed with 9-point scales ranging in preparation for difficult future tasks readily carry over to
from “very little” (1) to “very much” (9) ( r p .25). unrelated current tasks. Although one may argue that re-
After completing the questions, participants were told source carryover is a default, it does not necessarily occur.
that, due to unexpected conditions, there was no need to In fact, if mental effort is regarded as a scarce resource, it
participate in the studies. They were debriefed and thanked. would seem to be more economical to conserve resources
on a current task when a difficult future task is anticipated.
The question thus arises when carryover of cognitive re-
Results sources does not occur and conservation is more likely.
Successful manipulation of Future Task Difficulty should Carryover effects are believed to occur because people
result in higher ratings of anticipated difficulty of the future generally have difficulty separating the experiences asso-
task when this task was presented as difficult rather than ciated with different events (Pham 1998), that is, to keep
easy, whereas no effect on the expected difficulty level of things separate. As a result, experiences due to one event
the first (focal) task should be observed. This is indeed what are sometimes misattributed to another, unrelated event. One
we found. A mixed between-/within-subjects ANOVA on determinant of ability to separate is the distinctiveness of
the manipulation checks, with Future Task Difficulty as a the situations involved. That is, the likelihood that experi-
between-subjects factor and Anticipated Difficulty level of ences carry over should decrease as the perceived distinc-
the focal and future tasks as a within-subjects factor, indi- tiveness of situations increases. To illustrate, Schwarz and
cated a main effect of both the Future Task Difficulty ma- Clore (1983) observed that the effects of temporary mood
nipulation (F(1, 38) p 5.64, p ! .05; Mdiff p 5.59 vs. Measy states induced by current weather conditions on judgments
p 4.73; h 2 p .13) and the two measures of Anticipated of life satisfaction disappeared when people were made
Difficulty level (F(1, 38) p 7.68, p ! .01; Mfut p 5.73 vs. aware that their mood might be caused by these weather
Mfoc p 4.65; h 2 p .17). More interesting, and supporting the conditions. In the present context, one may expect that the
successful manipulation of Future Task Difficulty, there was distinctiveness of the current and future tasks will determine
a significant interaction effect: F(1, 38) p 11.20, p ! .01, whether carryover or conservation is more likely. By default
h 2 p .23. Participants in the Difficult Future Task condition (as shown in the first three studies), and particularly when
expected the future (second) task to be more difficult than the two tasks are similar, resources mobilized for a difficult
participants in the Easy Future Task condition did (Mdiff p future task should transfer to unrelated intervening tasks.
6.71 vs. Measy p 4.63; F(1, 38) p 19.69, p ! .001), whereas However, when people are specifically alerted to the fact
there was no difference in the expected difficulty level of the that the current and future tasks are dissimilar, they should
focal (first) task (Mdif f p 4.48 vs. Measy p 4.84; F(1, 38) ! be more conscious of the fact that the resources were actually
1, NS). mobilized for the future task, and they should thus be better
Importantly, and supporting the proposed get ready mind- able at conserving marshaled resources for future use. There-
set account, participants who anticipated a difficult future fore, anticipating a difficult future task should lead to de-
task felt more ready to start working on this task (Mdiff p creased performance on a current task when the current and
6.76 vs. Measy p 5.85; F(1, 38) p 5.22, p ! .05, h 2 p .12). future tasks are dissimilar. Task similarity is a situational
In contrast, the prospect of engaging in a difficult future factor that determines how difficult future tasks influence
task did not raise participants’ standards of performance for effort expenditure on current tasks.
working on the focal task. In fact, if anything, the opposite A personal factor with comparable effects is people’s in-
result was obtained, with participants in the Difficult Future trinsic ability to keep things separate. Whereas some people
Task condition planning to work somewhat less hard on the tend to look at information in an interdependent, holistic

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
THE GET READY MIND-SET 103

way by focusing on the possible connections between var- You will participate in a number of consumer studies that
ious components and features, others are inclined to process are all unrelated to each other. There will be a word recog-
information in a more independent, analytic way by focusing nition study that aims to investigate consumers’ attentional
on the separate components instead of the whole (Nisbett processes. There will also be a dieting study investigating
et al. 2001; Riding and Cheema 1991). Interdependent ver- how creative consumers are when losing weight. These stud-
sus independent processing styles have been found to affect ies draw on different psychological processes.
performance in a wide range of domains, including percep-
tion, cognition, and interpersonal behavior. For example, Future Task Difficulty was manipulated by instructing
independents are more likely to ignore contextual informa- participants that the upcoming consumer task would be fol-
tion (Kitayama et al. 2003) and are less likely to make lowed by an anagram task that was experienced as either
connections between various elements of the environment difficult (in the Difficult Future Task condition) or easy (in
(Monga and John 2007). Given interdependents’ tendency the Easy Future Task condition) by most participants in an
to process information in a holistic, global way, they should earlier study. Participants received an anagram training task
be particularly susceptible to carryover effects of mobilized that allowed them to practice the anticipated future task for
resources, because they are less able to discriminate between a time period of 5 minutes. This was done to help people
tasks. In contrast, since independents tend to process infor- mentally simulate the future task (Jeannerod and Decety
mation in an analytic, discrete way, they should be better 1995) and to encourage activation of the get ready mind-
at discriminating between resources available for future ver- set, consistent with Gollwitzer’s idea that mind-set effects
sus present task demands. Therefore, and comparable to the are more likely to occur after a training task (Brandstätter
situation with dissimilar tasks, carryover effects are less and Frank 2002; Gollwitzer et al. 1990).
likely, and independents may, in fact, conserve resources
for future tasks, which means that performance on earlier The consumer tasks you will be participating in will be fol-
tasks will be lower. lowed by an anagram task, where we investigate how con-
Study 4 tests the role of task similarity in determining sumers convert meaningless letters into meaningful words.
whether carryover or conservation of resources will occur This task is difficult [easy]. An earlier study indicated that
in intervening tasks, and study 5 investigates the moderating only 10% of the participants were able to recognize half of
influence of individual differences in interdependent versus the anagrams [90% of the participants were able to recognize
independent processing styles. half of the anagrams].
For organizational reasons, we will conduct this task at the
end of the session. However, in order to give you an idea of
STUDY 4 what this task will be like, we will present you with a number
of anagram problems right now. This will give you an op-
This study tests the hypothesis that resource carryover portunity to practice the upcoming anagram task.
occurs when the future and current tasks are perceived to
be similar, and resource conservation occurs when the future
Participants were instructed that if an anagram was too
and current tasks are perceived to be dissimilar.
difficult, they could press the “enter” button on the keyboard
to proceed to the next one. Anagrams were presented on
Method the computer one by one from a list of 40 anagrams. Par-
ticipants’ performance on the practice anagram task was
Fifty-nine undergraduate students participated in this 2 measured by counting the number of correctly solved an-
(Future Task Difficulty: easy vs. difficult) # 2 (Perceived agrams. Absence of performance differences in the practice
Task Similarity: similar vs. dissimilar) full factorial between- task reduces the likelihood that our future task instructions
subjects design for a monetary compensation of i7. may have activated different performance standards (Carver
Perceived Task Similarity was manipulated by instructing and Scheier 1998).
participants that both tasks that they would engage in were Directly after the practice anagram task, participants were
similar or dissimilar in terms of psychological processes and given the same focal weight loss task as in study 2. The
resource demands. Participants in the Similar Task condition number of separate ideas generated (thoughts) was taken as
read the following: the measure of cognitive effort. Participants did not perform
the future (anagram) task and were told that due to time
You will participate in a number of consumer studies that constraints there was insufficient time for this task.
are all related to each other. There will be a word recognition
study that aims to investigate how creative consumers are
with letters and words. Also, there will be a dieting study,
Pretest
investigating how creative consumers are when losing weight. The success of the Perceived Task Similarity manipulation
All studies draw on similar psychological processes. was assessed in a pretest among 34 undergraduate students.
Half of them received the instructions from the High Per-
In the Dissimilar Task condition, participants read the fol- ceived Similarity condition, and the other half received the
lowing: instructions from the Low Perceived Similarity condition.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
104 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

They reported to what extent the anagram and weight loss FIGURE 1
tasks were similar (using “both tasks are similar” and “both
MEAN NUMBER OF DIETING IDEAS AS A FUNCTION OF
tasks draw on similar psychological processes”; 9-point FUTURE TASK DIFFICULTY AND PERCEIVED
scales, alpha p .72). As expected, participants who received TASK SIMILARITY (STUDY 4)
the similar task instructions rated both tasks as more similar
than participants who received the dissimilar task instruc-
tions (Msim p 4.90 vs. Mdis p 3.68; F(1, 32) p 6.52, p !
.05, h 2 p .17).

Results
Neither the main effects of Future Task Difficulty and
Perceived Task Similarity nor their interaction affected the
number of correctly solved or total attempted anagrams dur-
ing the practice task (all p’s 1 .20; Mcor p 10.51, Mtotal p
15.80). This argues against the possibility that Future Task
Difficulty instructions activated different performance stan-
dards.
There was a significant main effect of Perceived Task
Similarity on the number of dieting ideas: when the focal
and future tasks were presented as being dissimilar, partic- NOTE.—Error bars indicate +/⫺ one standard error of the mean.
ipants listed more dieting ideas than when the tasks were
presented as being similar (F(1, 55) p 4.78 , p ! .05, h2 p their resources in a more economical fashion, they should
.08). The main effect of Future Task Difficulty was not conserve resources when the anticipated difficulty of the
significant ( F(1, 55) ! 1). More important, and consistent future task is high. Study 5 tested this prediction.
with expectations, there was a significant interaction effect
between Future Task Difficulty and Perceived Task Similarity Method
(F(1, 55) p 12.36, p ! .001, h 2 p .18). Figure 1 shows that
when tasks were presumably similar, participants formulated Sixty-seven undergraduate students participated in this 2
more unique dieting ideas during the focal task when they (Future Task Difficulty: easy vs. difficult) # 2 (Processing
anticipated a difficult rather than easy future task: F(1, 55) Style: independents vs. interdependents) full factorial be-
p 3.99, p p .05 (Mdiff p 4.00 vs. Measy p 3.00). Con- tween-subjects design for a monetary compensation of i7.
versely, when tasks were presumably dissimilar, participants Future Task Difficulty was manipulated as in study 4 by
allocated more effort to the focal task and listed more dieting informing participants that the upcoming consumer study
ideas when they anticipated an easy compared to a difficult would be followed by an anagram task that was presented
future task: F(1, 55) p 8.90, p ! .01 (Mdiff p 3.53 vs. Measy as easy or difficult. As in the previous study, participants
p 5.00). engaged in a training task in which they were asked to
When the focal and future tasks were supposedly similar, practice on a number of anagrams for 5 minutes.
performance on the focal task was higher when a difficult Immediately after the training anagram task, participants
as compared to an easy future task was anticipated, which received the same focal product evaluation task as in study
is consistent with resource carryover. In contrast, when the 1, with the number of information links consulted as the
focal and future tasks were supposedly dissimilar, perfor- dependent variable. Again, participants did not perform the
mance on the focal task was lower when a difficult as com- future (anagram) task, supposedly because there was not
pared to an easy future task was anticipated, which is con- enough time left.
sistent with resource conservation. The evaluation task was followed by a number of filler
tasks, and, at the end of the experimental session, individual
STUDY 5 differences in interdependent versus independent Processing
Style were assessed. Independents are characterized by
Study 5 aimed to replicate the pattern of results of study higher levels of perceptual flexibility than interdependents
4 using an alternative measure of cognitive effort and a and are better able to separate relevant from irrelevant per-
different operationalization of ability to separate tasks, ceptual information. Therefore, independents are more pro-
namely, individual differences in independent versus inter- ficient at separating figure from ground, have a greater ten-
dependent processing styles. Given independents’ tendency dency to focus on details instead of the whole, and are better
to process information in a discrete, analytic way, we ex- able to ignore irrelevant perceptual information (Kitayama
pected them to be better able to discriminate between re- et al. 2003). Therefore, we assessed independent versus in-
sources available for future versus present task demands. terdependent Processing Style using three visual reaction
As a result, resource carryover should be less likely for time (RT) tasks, in which participants had to separate rel-
independents. In fact, if independents are able to manage evant from irrelevant stimulus information. The RTs in the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
THE GET READY MIND-SET 105

three tasks served as measure of independent (low RTs) predictions. The number of information links consulted was
versus interdependent (high RTs) Processing Style (as in the dependent variable. The main effects for Processing
Witkin 1950). Style (b p .08, t p .67, p p .51) and Future Task Difficulty
In the first RT task, participants saw a black-and-white (b p .06, t p .45, p p .65) were insignificant, but the pre-
map of the Mediterranean Sea. Contrary to what people dicted interaction effect was significant and with the right
expect to see when looking at geographical images, the black sign (b p .38, t p 2.34, p p .02; model R 2 p .09). Spot-
areas represented the water, and the white areas represented light analyses at ⫺/+ 1 SD from the median score on Pro-
the land masses. Because people are used to seeing fore- cessing Style revealed that for independents the effect of Fu-
grounds (the land mass) in black and backgrounds (the wa- ture Task Difficulty was in the predicted direction but not
ter) in white, interpretation of this map requires efficient significant (b p ⫺.80, t p ⫺1.35, p p .18; means of Measy
figure-ground switching. Participants were instructed to in- p 4.85 and M diff p 3.26), whereas for interdependents the
terpret the image as quickly as possible and to indicate their difference was significant and in the predicted direction
answer on a 10-category response scale (with the categories (b p 1.17, t p 1.98, p ! .05; means of Measy p 2.32 and
cow, mountains, dog, tree, Mediterranean Sea, naked woman, Mdiff p 4.67). Figure 2 shows that the more interdependent
man playing the sax, two men playing guitar, face of a woman, people’s processing style is, the more likely they are to work
two people in love). more on the intermediate task when the future task is difficult
In the second RT task, participants saw an ambiguous and to work less when the future task is easy. Independents
image that can be interpreted as either a woman’s face or did not exhibit this resource carryover and used their re-
a saxophone player. One of the two interpretations is always sources in a more economical fashion, showing signs of
dominant. Again, participants were instructed to recognize resource conservation.
as quickly as possible the image shown in the picture, using
the 10-category scale of the first task. After participants had
recognized one of the two images (i.e., either the sax player GENERAL DISCUSSION
or the woman’s face), they were asked to take another look Many tasks are knowingly executed sequentially, such
at the picture in order to try to find the other image. This that consumers may already be thinking about a task to be
not only forces them to switch back and forth between figure done in the future but in the meanwhile have to engage in
and ground but also requires them to ignore the perceptual some other task in the present. Consumers make a list of
features that were informative for recognizing the first im- ways to diet before checking out a video to watch with the
age. Only the latter RT was incorporated into the interde- kids, or evaluate products online prior to generating ideas
pendent-independent Processing Style measure. for a birthday gift to buy, and so on. The question, then, is
The third RT task used Currier and Ives’s The Puzzled if and how the anticipated resource demands of the future
Fox, which displays a fox (foreground) in a forest (back- task influence effort expenditure and performance on the
ground). A number of other animals are hidden in the forest, current task.
which can only be recognized when focusing on the details Building on work about implementation mind-sets and
of the background. Participants were instructed to try to find task preparation, we proposed that when anticipating diffi-
the image of a hidden horse as quickly as possible. Fast cult future tasks, people mobilize resources in preparation.
identification of the horse requires separation of, and switch- An interesting and at first counterintuitive implication of
ing between, figure (fox) and ground (forest) to focus on this get ready mind-set is that the resources marshaled in
the details of the background and to look beyond the figure
as a whole.
FIGURE 2
The sum of the weighted RTs on the three tasks indicates
interdependent versus independent Processing Style, using MEAN NUMBER OF INFORMATION LINKS CONSULTED
the first principal factor from a principal components anal- AS A FUNCTION OF FUTURE TASK DIFFICULTY AND
ysis (Woltman-Elpers, Wedel, and Pieters 2003). Lower PROCESSING STYLE (STUDY 5)
scores (fast reactions) on the measure reflect a more inde-
pendent Processing Style and higher scores (slow reactions)
a more interdependent Processing Style, hereafter simply
labeled Processing Style.

Results
As in study 4, neither the main effect of Future Task
Difficulty and Processing Style nor their interaction affected
the number of correctly solved anagrams during the practice
task (all p’s 1 .27; Mcor p 14.15, Mtotal p 19.69).
Processing Style (continuous), Future Task Difficulty (⫺1
p easy, 1 p difficult), and their product were the three in-
dependent variables in a regression analysis that tested our

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
106 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

preparation for difficult future tasks may carry over to un- what happened, because the manipulation checks in studies
related current tasks. We observed and replicated this re- 2 and 3 (i.e., the expected difficulty levels of the focal and
source carryover effect across a variety of manipulations of future tasks) showed that our Future Task Difficulty manip-
future task difficulty (anticipating simple or complex future ulation only affected the expected difficulty level of the future
personal tasks; generating ideas for i1 gifts, described to task, not the expected difficulty level of the focal task. Second,
be easy or difficult; anticipating working on easy or difficult a standard of performance account would predict that per-
anagrams) and effort measures (amount of product infor- formance on all tasks should be affected by the standard
mation consulted, quantity, and quality of idea generation). established by the anticipated difficulty of the future task.
The present research contributes to the literature in several In particular, performance on the practice task that came
ways. It is, to our knowledge, the first to show that antic- after participants were informed about the difficulty of the
ipatory responses to impending tasks can systematically in- future task but before they performed the focal task should
fluence the execution of unrelated intervening tasks. Our also have been affected by the Future Task Difficulty ma-
results indicate that difficult future tasks encourage the mo- nipulation. However, this was not the case (studies 4 and
bilization of resources, which can carry over to unrelated 5). Finally, a standard of performance account would predict
tasks in the present. Furthermore, just as cognitive proce- that—when explicitly asked—participants should indicate
dures associated with an implementation mind-set may that they would allocate more resources to an intervening
transfer to unrelated tasks, the mobilized resources promoted task when the future task was anticipated to be difficult
by the get ready mind-set can carry over to earlier tasks. rather than easy. Yet, in study 3, participants stated that they
Our results also demonstrate that the carryover of resources were willing to invest less effort in the current task when
is moderated by people’s ability to separate tasks because the future task was anticipated to be difficult compared to
it vanishes when the perceived distinctiveness between tasks easy but mobilized resources later on (unconsciously) car-
is high or when people are naturally more proficient at keep- ried over to the current task when the tasks were perceived
ing tasks separate. The present research also contributes to to be similar.
the misattribution literature in that it suggests that people One interesting avenue for future research concerns the
may misattribute their experiences to the wrong situation implications of the get ready mind-set for performance on
not only on a post hoc basis but also on an ex ante basis. the future task. We speculate that the effect critically depends
That is, whereas the misattribution literature has examined on the extent to which the intervening (focal) task is draining
how current experiences affect the evaluation of unrelated resources. Increased expenditure of effort on the focal task
later events (e.g., “I feel aroused, so the situation must be might use up resources that are subsequently unavailable
exciting”), we focus on how anticipated future activities for the future task, so that performance on the future task
affect the execution of unrelated current events (e.g., “I feel suffers as a consequence. On the other hand, if engagement
ready, so I am going to work hard”). in the intervening task is not draining resources and per-
Finally, by investigating the moderating impact of indi- formance on the future task does not suffer (or even im-
viduals’ ability to separate tasks, our findings shed new light proves due to a kind of “warming-up” effect), this could be
on the seemingly incompatible finding that people some- taken as further evidence for the get ready mind-set. Al-
times try to conserve resources for future tasks in an effort though it was not the focus of the present study, we explored
to deal with the limited nature of their scarce resources. In the effects of the get ready mind-set on performance in the
general, but particularly under conditions where people were future task in study 2. After the focal consumer task, par-
not able to separate tasks, performance on the intervening ticipants in this study actually engaged in the future task
task was better, not worse, when people anticipated a de- and listed as many Christmas gifts for i1 as they could think
manding future task. On the other hand, we observed results of. The gift ideas were assessed with regard to their creativity
consistent with a conservation effect when people were able by 10 independent judges (based on a 10-point scale ranging
to keep things separate due to situational or personal condi- from “not creative at all” to “very creative”; alpha p .83).
tions. Our Future Task Difficulty instructions did not affect the
Could it be that, as suggested by a reviewer, our observed overall number of gifts listed (F(1, 75) ! 1) nor did they
effects are due to the fact that information about the diffi- affect the average creativity of the gifts listed per participant
culty of the future task established different standards of (F(1, 75) ! 1). Interestingly, however, the first two top-of-
performance and that participants monitored their perfor- mind gifts generated by participants were judged to be sig-
mance on the focal task against this standard of performance nificantly more creative in the Difficult, compared to the Easy,
(i.e., “the future task is difficult, so I’ll have to work hard Future Task condition (F(1, 75) p 4.78, p ! .05, h 2 p .06;
on all tasks”)? A number of observations argue against this Mdif f p 8.04 vs. Measy p 6.91). This suggests that perfor-
possibility that participants employed such a conversational mance in the future task did not suffer as a consequence of
logic. First, if anticipated future task demands had created the stepped-up effort in the earlier, focal task and even im-
a standard of performance for all tasks, we should have proved early on. Future research may examine in more detail
observed that participants expected the focal task to be dif- under which conditions a get ready mind-set, after raising
ficult when the future task was presented as difficult and performance on the intervening task, either increases or de-
easy when the future task was presented as easy. This is not creases future task performance.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018
THE GET READY MIND-SET 107

In sum, and returning to the main idea and finding, con- the Motivational Implications of Their Moods,” Journal of
sumers who anticipate having to perform a difficult future Personality and Social Psychology, 64 (March), 317–26.
task may gear up and already start working hard on a current Monga, Alokparna Basy and Deborah Roedder John (2007), “Cul-
task, particularly when they do not keep tasks separate due tural Differences in Brand Extension Evaluation: The Influ-
ence of Analytic versus Holistic Thinking,” Journal of Con-
to situational contingencies or personal characteristics. This
sumer Research, 33 (March), 529–36.
reveals that when getting ready for a later challenge, people Muraven, Mark, Dikla Shmueli, and Edward Burkley (2006),
may already be set to get going now. “Conserving Self-Control Strength,” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 91 (September), 524–37.
Nisbett, Richard E., Kaiping Peng, Incheol Choi, and Ara Noren-
REFERENCES zayan (2001), “Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic ver-
sus Analytic Cognition,” Psychological Review, 108 (April),
Brandstätter, Veronika and Elisabeth Frank (2002), “Effects of De- 291–310.
liberative and Implemental Mindsets on Persistence in Goal- Pham, Michel Tuan (1998), “Representativeness, Relevance, and
Directed Behavior,” Personality and Social Psychological the Use of Feelings in Decision Making,” Journal of Con-
Bulletin, 28 (October), 1366–78. sumer Research, 25 (September), 144–59.
Brass, Marcel and D. Yves von Cramon (2002), “The Role of the Riding, Richard and Indra Cheema (1991), “Cognitive Styles: An
Frontal Cortex in Task Preparation,” Cerebral Cortex, 12 (9), Overview and Integration,” Educational Psychology, 11
908–14. (3–4), 193–215.
Cantor, Joanne R., Dolf Zillmann, and Jennings Bryant (1975), Sakai, Katsuyuki and Richard E. Passingham (2003), “Prefrontal
“Enhancement of Experienced Sexual Arousal in Response Interactions Reflect Future Task Operations,” Nature Neuro-
to Erotic Stimuli through Misattribution of Unrelated Residual science, 6 (January), 75–81.
Excitation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Sanbonmatsu David M. and Frank R. Kardes (1988), “The Effects
32 (July), 69–75. of Physiological Arousal on Information Processing and Per-
Carver, Charles S. and Michael F. Scheier (1998), On the Self- suasion,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (December),
Regulation of Behavior, New York: Cambridge University 379–85.
Press. Schwarz, Norbert and Gerald L. Clore (1983), “Mood, Misattri-
Contrada, Richard J., Rex A. Wright, and David C. Glass (1984), bution, and Judgments of Well-Being: Informative and Di-
“Task Difficulty, Type A Behavior Pattern, and Cardiovas- rective Functions of Affective States,” Journal of Personality
cular Response,” Psychophysiology, 21 (November), 638–46. and Social Psychology, 45 (September), 513–23.
Gollwitzer, Peter M. (1990), “Action Phases and Mind-Sets,” in
Vohs, Kathleen D. and Ronald J. Faber (2007), “Spent Resources:
Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of So-
Self-Regulatory Resource Availability Affects Impulse Buy-
cial Behavior, Vol. 2, ed. E. Tory Higgins, New York: Guil-
ing,” Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (March), 537–47.
ford, 53–92.
Gollwitzer, Peter M., Heinz Heckhausen, and Birgit Steller (1990), Witkin, Herman A. (1950), “Individual Differences in Ease of
“Deliberative and Implemental Mind-Sets: Cognitive Tuning Perception of Embedded Figures,” Journal of Personality, 19,
toward Congruous Thoughts and Information,” Journal of 1–15.
Personality and Social Psychology, 59 (December), 1119–27. Woltman-Elpers, Josephine, Michel Wedel, and Rik Pieters (2003),
Guilford, J. Paul (1967), The Nature of Human Intelligence, New “Why Do Consumers Stop Viewing Television Commercials?
York: McGraw-Hill. Two Experiments on the Influence of Moment-to-Moment
Jeannerod, Marc and Jean Decety (1995), “Mental Motor Imagery: Entertainment and Information Value,” Journal of Marketing
A Window into the Representational Stages of Action,” Cur- Research, 40 (November), 437–53.
rent Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 727–32. Wright, Rex A. (1996), “Brehm’s Theory of Motivation as a Model
Kitayama, Shinobu, Sean Duffy, Tadashi Kawamura, and Jeff T. of Effort and Cardiovascular Response,” in The Psychology
Larsen (2003), “Perceiving an Object and Its Context in Dif- of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behavior, ed.
ferent Cultures: A Cultural Look at New Look,” Psycholog- Peter M. Gollwitzer and John A. Bargh, New York: Guilford,
ical Science, 14 (May), 210–16. 424–53.
Locke, Edwin A. and Gary P. Latham (2006), “New Directions in Zillmann, Dolf, Rolland C. Johnson, and Kenneth D. Day (1974),
Goal-Setting Theory,” Current Directions in Psychological “Attribution of Apparent Arousal and Proficiency of Recovery
Science, 15 (October), 265–68. from Sympathetic Activation Affecting Excitation Transfer to
Martin, Leonard L., David W. Ward, John W. Achee, and Robert Aggressive Behavior,” Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
S. Wyer (1993), “Mood as Input: People Have to Interpret chology, 10 (November), 503–15.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article-abstract/37/1/98/1820706


by University of Florida user
on 09 July 2018

You might also like