Dr. Michael W. Eysenck (Auth.) - Attention and Arousal - Cognition and Performance-Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg (1982) PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 218

Michael W.

Eysenck

Attention and Arousal


Cognition and Performance

With 56 Figures

Springer-Verlag
Berlin Heidelberg N ew York 1982
Dr. Michael W. Eysenck
Birkbeck College
University of London
Department of Psychology
Malet Street
London, WCIE 7HX, England

ISBN-13 :978-3-642-68392-3

Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data


Eysenck, Michael W.
Attention and arousal. Cognition and performance.
Bibliography: p.
Includes index.
I. Cognition. 2. Performance. 3. Motivation (Psychology) 4. Selectivity (Psychology) 5. Atten·
tion. 6. Arousal (Physiology) I. Title.
BF311.E93 153 81-21240
ISBN-13 :978-3-642-68392-3 e- ISBN-13 :978-3-642-68390-9
DO!: 10.1007/978-3-642-68390-9
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved, whether the whole or part of the
material is concerned, specifically those of translation, reprinting, re-use of illustrations,
broadcasting, reproduction by photocopying machine or similar means, and storage in data
banks. Under § 54 of the German Copyright Law where copies are made for other tban
private use a fee is payable to 'Verwertungsgeselischaft Wort", Munich.

c Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1982


Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1st edition 1982

The use of general descriptive names, trade marks, etc. in this pUblication, even if the former
are not especially identified, is not to be taken as a sign that such names, as understood by the
Trade Marks an Merchandise Marks Act, may accordingly by used freely by anyone.

Typesetting, Printing and Bookbinding: Rombach+Co., Freiburg


2126/3140-543210
This book is dedicated
to my mother
for everything
Preface

The thinker who has a mortal fear of being wrong


will give all that is valuable in himself to that little ambition.
Walter Lippmann (1914)

Psychology has always been plagued by passing fads and fan-


cies to a greater extent than is seemly in a scientific discipline.
Over the past few years the Zeitgeist can be summed up by the
two words 'cognitive psychology'. Indeed, a recent poll of
academic psychologists in American indicated that over 80%
of them regarded themselves as cognitive psychologists!
Cognitive psychology is in the ascendant, but it has never
been clear to me that it has addressed all of the appropriate is-
sues. In particular, information processing in the real world
(and even in the laboratory) occurs within a motivational and
emotional context, but cognitive psychologists usually main-
tain the convenient fiction that cognition can fruitfully be stud-
ied in isolation. The main reason for writing this book was to at-
tempt to demonstrate that there can be a useful cross-fertiliza-
tion between cognitive and motivational-emotional psycholo-
gy and that there are already tantalizing glimpses of the poten-
tial advantages of such inter-disciplinary research.
The ideas of Donald Broadbent and his associates have exer-
cised a formative influence during the writing of this book.
They discovered some years ago that there are intriguing simi-
larities (as well as differences) in the effects on performance
of such apparently quite disparate factors as white noise, time of
day, introversion-extraversion and incentive. They argued that
all of these factors (as well as others) increased arousal and
supported their argument with physiological and behavioural
evidence. This attempt to explain a great variety of findings by
means of a fairly simple arousal model was surprisingly suc-
cessful, and one of my main aims has been to assess this whole
approach in the light of recent research.
I would like to express my thanks to several people. I had an
enjoyable time at the University of South Florida in Tampa
during the initial stages of writing this book, and I am very grate-
ful especially to Doug Nelson but also to Jim Anker, Tramm
Neill and Cathy McEvoy for providing a friendly and helpful
atmosphere and for interesting discussions of ideas. In addi-
tion, the weather in Florida lived up to all expectations.
I would like to thank my students, especially Kathy Cohen,
Anna Eliatamby, Jackie Laws, Chris Morris and Jan Graydon,
for their contribution to my education - I only hope this has
been a two-way process! I have also learned much from a num-
ber of my professional colleagues and friends, including Bob
VIII Preface

Hockey, Simon Folkard, Tony Gale, Dylan Jones and Tim


Monk. A debt of gratitude is also owed to the Social Science
Research Council for providing generous financial support for
my research over the past several years.
Finally, I am especially indebted to my family. This book is
appropriately dedicated to my mother, and my wife Christine
has provided me with limitless personal and professional sup-
port. Without her help and that of our daughter Fleur the writ-
ing of this book would have taken less time but would also have
been less pleasurable.

Michael W. Eysenck
Table of Contents

1 Introduction .....

2 Theories of Attention . 8
2.1 Early Theories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
2.2 Posner and Snyder: Automatic Activation and
Conscious Attention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
2.3 Shiffrin and Schneider: Controlled and Automatic
Processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18
2.4 Treisman and Gelade: Features and Objects. 24
2.5 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27

3 Theories of Processing Resources 28


3.1 Baddeley and Hitch: Working Memory . 28
3.2 Norman and Bobrow: Data-Limited and
Resource-Limited Processes . . . . . . . 33
3.3 Johnston and Heinz: Multi-Mode Theory 35
3.4 Navon and Gopher: Multiple Resources. 39
3.5 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43

4 Theories of Arousal and Performance 47


4.1 The Yerkes-Dodson Law . . . . 47
4.2 Easterbrook's Hypothesis . . . . 49
4.3 Broadbent: Decision and Stress. 54
4.4 NiHitiinen's Theory. . . . . ... 56
4.5 Kahneman: Attention and Effort 57
4.6 Thayer: Self-Reported Arousal . . 60
4.7 Hasher and Zacks: Automatic and Effortful Processes 63
4.8 Summary and Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66

5 Incentives and Motivation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 67


5.1 Reinforcement and Incentives . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
5.2 Learning and Memory: Atkinson and Wickens (l971) 68
5.3 Short-Term Memory: Weiner. 73
5.4 Task Characteristics . . . . . . . . 74
5.5 Performance Efficiency . . . . . . 83
5.6 Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 84
5.7 A Theoretical Overview . 87

6 Anxiety and Performance 95


6.1 Worry and Emotionality. 97
6.2 Working Memory Capacity 99
X Table of Contents

6.3 Spence and Spence (1966): Task Difficulty 101


6.4 Anxiety and Motivation . . . . . . . . . . . 108
6.5 Failure and Shock . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112
6.6 Failure and Success: A Cognitive Approach . 113
6.7 Depth and Elaboration of Processing 115
6.8 Theoretical Positions . . . . . . . . . . 120

7 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal . . 124


7.1 Introversion - Extraversion . . . 124
7.2, Time of Day ... 138
7.3 Sleep Deprivation . . . . . . . . 146

8 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise. . . . . 156


8.1 Vigilance Task Performance . . . . . . . . . . . . 156
8.2 Continuous Tasks: Serial Reaction and Tracking. 157
8.3 Learning and Memory . . 159
8.4 Attentional Selectivity. . 164
8.5 Theoretical Formulation 167
8.6 Summary . . . . . . . . . 172

9 Conclusions and Speculations 173


9.1 The Yerkes-Dodson Law . 173
9.2 Stages of Processing . 178
9.3 Two Arousal Systems . . . 182

10 References . 187

11 Author Index . 201

12 Subject Index . . 205


1 Introduction

It may truly be said that theories in the area multifaceted criteria. For example, an acad-
of cognitive psychology are in many ways emic may make writing a book one of his
like rabbits in a hutch: they multiply rapidly goals, but he will often have ancillary goals
but all manage to look remarkably similar. such as making the book readable and enter-
This similarity applies both to what they say taining, advancing his career and perhaps
and, perhaps even more strongly, to what even earning a few bob in the process! In a
they do not say. Closer examination of this similar way, even when the participant in
conspiracy of silence suggests that the most psychological research is apparently en-
striking omission from the great majority of grossed in an experimental task, he or she
information-processing theories is any seri- will usually continue to have as an addition-
ous consideration of the dynamic interrela- al goal the task of monitoring the environ-
tionships among motivation, emotion and ment for important sources of information.
cognition. There is plenty of evidence that some at-
This rather narrow approach to theory tentional capacity is invested in non-task ac-
construction has a number of important dis- tivities, except perhaps under hypnosis.
advantages. It has, for example, encouraged Consider for example an experimental task
the simplistic notion that humans possess which requires the subject to learn a list of
considerable single-mindedness and unity words. Subsequent retention tests almost in-
of purpose. At the theoretical level, this has variably indicate that some task-irrelevant
led to computer simulations of human information (e.g. the colours in which the
thinking based on the assumption that only words are printed, the spatial locations of
a single goal (e.g. satisfactory task comple- the words and the nature of additional sti-
tion) is operative at any given point in time. muli presented alongside the to-be-remem-
A hierarchy of subprograms or subroutines bered words) has been attended to (see Eas-
is often postulated, with all information pro- terbrook 1959 for a review).
cessing being done in the service of the high- Why do we habitually assign some of our
est level executive program. As Simon spare processing capacity to the task of
(1967) pertinently remarked, "This appar- monitoring the environment? The reason
ently single-minded, single-purpose behav- appears to be that this monitoring allows us
iour of most existing simulations of infor- to handle any sudden emergencies that may
mation-processing systems provides a stri- arise by alerting the processing system to im-
king contrast with human behaviour" portant changes occurring in the immediate
(p.32). environment. On those occasions that the
Any attempt to understand human cogni- monitoring system does detect the existence
tion and behaviour must recognize that peo- of a new need of high priority, the ongoing
ple are often responsive to several goals si- information processing is interrupted, and
multaneously rather than to just one. In the previous goals are replaced by goals
other words, most goals are not unitary; more appropriate to the changed circum-
rather, the achievement of a goal typically stances.
requires a pattern of behaviour that fulfills In contrast to the rapidly changing and
2 Introduction

unpredictable conditions that frequently postulated the existence of an interrupt sys-


prevail in the real world, there is the drasti- tem based on continuous monitoring of the
cally restricted environment of the typical environment. When the monitoring system
laboratory experiment in psychology. It is detects needs of high priority, new goals
unusual in such an environment for any dra- which are appropriate to handling these
matic changes in the subject's goals to be re- needs replace those previously in operation
quired as the experimental session pro- (i.e. the interrupt system is used). As a con-
gresses. sequence, the attentional focus shifts to
We have emphasized the point so far that those stimuli which are relevant to the new
there are considerable changes over time in goals. According to Simon, the interrupt sys-
the goal systems which control behaviour. It tem can be triggered by sudden and intense
seems intuitively obvious that goals playa stimuli, by physiological needs or by emo-
major role in determining the direction of tionally laden cognitive associations. It is ty-
attention at any given moment. As a conse- pically the case that interrupting stimuli of
quence, most of the effects of goals on be- all kinds produce arousal of the autonomic
haviour are mediated by attentional me- nervous system and feelings of emotion,
chanisms. This line of argument can be tak- leading Simon (1967) to the following con-
en further. It is one of the major contentions clusion: "All the evidence points to a close
of this book that there is an intimate rela- connection between the operation of the in-
tionship between motivational and atten- terrupt system and much of what is usually
tional processes. In geneAl terms, motiva- called emotional behaviour" (p. 36).
tional states affect attentional selectivity, i.e.
they determine the contents of attention. If we are prepared to assume that motiva-
This theoretical position is not original; in- tional and emotional processes both have
deed, it has been espoused by several re- important effects on attentional mechan-
searchers and was given expression in the isms, then the next issue is to indicate pre-
following terms by Simon (1967): "We can cisely what those effects are likely to be. The
use the term motivation . .. simply to desig- most influential attempt to address this is-
nate that which controls attention at any giv- sue was made by Easterbrook (1959); his
en time. The motivation may be directed quintessential theoretical assumption was
toward a single goal, or, more commonly, that motivation and emotion influence at-
toward multiple goals" (p. 34). tentional selectivity by narrowing attention
It may seem fairly obvious that motiva- and thereby reducing the attention paid to
tional forces affect the allocation of atten- non-task sources of stimulation.
tional resources within the information-pro- While there is impressive empirical sup-
cessing system. However, many researchers port for Easterbrook's (1959) viewpoint,
apparently fail to find the connection obvi- there are other possible effects of motiva-
ous at all. As I am writing these words, I tion and emotion on attentional processes
have in front of me two books on motiva- that deserve to be considered. For example,
tion: Theories of Motivation by Bernard it might be the case that attentional capacity
Weiner and Motivation: An Experimental is affected systematically by motivational
Approach by Eva Dreikurs Ferguson. The and emotional factors. Alternatively, the
former book does not even have 'attention' rate of change of the contents of attention
as a heading in its subject index, and the lat- (cf. mental tempo) might be affected; an ap-
ter book contains only one reference to the propriate analogy here might be with a cine
concept. film, which looks very different when pro-
It seems probable that emotional states al- jected at a number of different speeds. A fi-
so affect behaviour via attentional pro- nal major aspect of attention is the extent to
cesses. A plausible way in which this might which one is able to prevent it from being di-
happen was outlined by Simon (1967). He verted to irrelevant sources of information;
Introduction 3

in other words, motivational and emotional mon effects on behaviour of several appar-
factors may influence distractibility. ently disparate factors such as incentives,
In sum, there are a variety of ways in failure feedback, ego threat, intense noise,
which motivation and emotion might affect time of day, sleep deprivation, introversion-
attention; these include the selectivity and extraversion and a number of stimulant and
capacity of attention, the rate of change of depressant drugs.
attention and susceptibility to distraction. The notion that there may be partial over-
Evidence is presented in later chapters lap in the effects on performance of each of
which indicates that all four aspects of at- the above factors may be referred to as the
tention are affected by motivational and 'weak' arousal theory. According to the
emotional factors. 'strong' arousal theory, virtually all of the ef-
Several attempts have been made over the fects of these stressors and arousing agents
years (e.g. Broadbent 1971, Duffy 1962, Eas- on behaviour are mediated by a single
terbrook 1959, Schachter and Singer 1962) arousal system. This strong theory can be
to relate motivation and emotion to arousal, evaluated in terms of a number of criteria.
which has usually been thought of in phy- The first criterion is that of equivalence,
siological terms. It has been claimed that i.e. the various arousing agents should have
there is a continuum of arousal ranging broadly comparable behavioural conse-
from deep sleep or coma at one extreme to quences. Perhaps the most important aspect
panic-stricken terror or great excitement at of the equivalence criterion is the assump-
the other extreme. tion that different arousing agents should
In many ways, the concept of 'arousal' is produce a similar pattern of improvement
an amorphous one; furthermore, it has been and impairment of performance across a
used in several different senses. Duffy variety of tasks.
(1962) defined arousal or activation in terms The second criterion is that two different
of metabolic activity in the tissues: "The arousing agents or stressors should tend to
construct energy mobilization, or activation, interact with each other if they are, in fact,
derives from and emphasizes the fact that a striking at the same mechanism in the same
living organism is characteristically an ener- way. On the other hand, if two arousing
gy-system ... The level of activation of the agents affect different mechanisms, then
organism may be defined as the extent of the they should exert independent effects on
release of potential energy, stored in the tis- performance. Consider as an example two
sues of the organism, as this is shown in ac- stressors, A and B. On a particular task,
tivity or response" (p. 17). A very different stressor A produces a 15 % decrement in per-
approach was adopted by Broadbent (1971), formance when applied on its own, and
who argued as follows: "The physiological stressor B has exactly the same effect. If the
concept of arousal is certainly of interest two stressors affect different mechanisms,
and of ultimate relevance to the one we have then there should be a decrement of approx-
found from behaviour, but at this stage the imately 30% in performance when both
connection of any suggested physiological stressors are applied together. In contrast,
measure and the psychological state is too the combined effects of the two stressors
remote to make it practical to attach one might be considerably in excess of a 30 % de-
concept directly to the other" (p. 4\3). crement if they both affect a common me-
The position taken in this book is that chanism of arousal. This point is developed
there are important but indirect relation- at greater length by Broadbent (1971).
ships between arousal defined psychologi- If arousal is thought of in physiological
cally and arousal construed in physiologi- terms, it is reasonable to propose a third cri-
cal terms. The emphasis is primarily on a terion, namely, that the effects of different
psychological concept of arousal; as a first arousing agents on physiological response
approximation, arousal refers to the com- patterns should be comparable. One of the
4 Introduction

major difficulties in attempting to use this specific factors (e. g. verbal and spatial) also
criterion is the lack of any satisfactory mea- need to be postulated. These specific factors
sure of the level of arousal. It is typically the account for the finding that all of the tests
case that different physiological measures within a small group of, say, spatial tests in-
of arousal (e.g. skin conductance, EEG, tercorrelate highly, but none of these tests
heart rate and pupil dilation) only correlate shows a sizeable correlation with other tests.
approximately + .2 to + .3 with each other. In other words, a hierarchical set of factors
Moreover, as Lacey (1967) pointed out, ranging from the very general to the fairly
there are pronounced individual differences specific can account for intelligence test
in terms of which physiological indexes of performance. It may well be that several
arousal are most affected by stress. arousing agents all affect a general arousal
In a number of situations, some physio- system but that differences among them
logical measures indicate increased arousal must be explained in terms of more specific
at the same time as other measures suggest mechanisms and processes (e.g. effort and
the opposite. Lacey (1967) introduced the stress).
term 'directional fractionation' to refer to
Further justification for the postulation
such discrepant patterns. Perhaps the most
of more than one arousal-related mechan-
thoroughly investigated example of direc-
ism is available when one considers the
tional fractionation occurs during the time
complexities of the performance data in a
interval between an alerting signal and the
little more detail. The best-known generali-
stimulus in reaction time studies (reviewed
zation concerning the effects of arousal on
by Kahneman 1973). While some of the
performance was offered by Yerkes and
components of the EEG indicate height-
Dodson (1908). On the basis of a series of
ened arousal, there is typically a reduction
experiments involving hungry rats learning
in heart rate (i.e. cardiac deceleration). In
mazes, they argued that there is a curvilinear
general, directional fractionation and card-
relationship between arousal or motivation-
iac deceleration both occur under condi-
allevel (determined by the hours of food de-
tions of waiting for an expected event.
privation) and performance; in other words,
Much space in later chapters is devoted to
performance was best at a moderate level of
comparing and contrasting the effects of dif-
motivation and suffered if motivation were
ferent arousing agents and stressors. How-
either very low or very high. They also as-
ever, it is worth noting here that while there
sumed that the optimal level of motivation
is some evidence that fulfills the three crite-
or arousal was lower for difficult tasks than
ria discussed previously, there are other
for easy ones.
findings apparently at variance with each of
them. The obvious conclusion is that the Perhaps the most convincing evidence in
various arousing agents operate in part favour of the two major assertions made by
through a common arousal mechanism and Yerkes and Dodson (1908) was obtained by
in part through rather more specific me- Broadhurst (1957). Rats were trained to
chanisms. swim in an underwater maze in the shape of
Eysenck (1917) drew an analogy between a y. One arm of the Y was lighted and gave
the pattern of results in the areas of intelli- access to the open air, whereas the other was
gence and arousal research. He pointed out a dark cul-de-sac. Four levels of motivation
that different kinds of intelligence test items were created by submerging the rats for 0, 2,
are typically positively intercorrelated but 4 or 8 s immediately before releasing them
that these correlations are often rather directly into the maze. Task difficulty was
small. The usual interpretation of these find- manipulated by using three different light
ings is that there is a general factor of intelli- intensities in the illuminated part of the
gence (which explains the preponderance of maze. These were bright, moderate and very
positive intercorrelations) but that more dull; in the last condition, it was hard to dis-
Introduction 5

85 n
~~
80 r:o
fTlfTl
75 ~ ~
70 ~--f
Z:o
Glj;
65 ~r
(/l

Fig. 1.1. Efficiency of learning as a function of


task difficulty and degree of motivation. (Broad-
hurst 1957) o

criminate between the illuminated and the terrelationship between anxiety and drive in
dark arms of the maze. the formula A = EC, with C representing
the amount of frustration and complication
The main findings obtained by Broad- producing conflict. The two formulae can
hurst (1957) are shown in Fig. 1.1. It is clear be amalgamated into a third formula as fol-
that the findings are broadly consistent with lows:
the theoretical assumptions of Yerkes and P = aE- b.EC.
Dodson (1908). However, Yerkes and Dod- They then went on to claim that, at least in
son failed to indicate precisely why increas- the pursuit of complex goals, C is usually a
ing levels of motivation or arousal should function of drive (E). As a consequence,
first improve performance and then impair they reached the following conclusion with
it. This rather paradoxical effect can be ex- respect to the third formula: "The second
plained in terms of the operation of a single term has really E2 in it, and a parabolic rela-
arousal system but is perhaps more readily tion of performance P to ergic drive
accounted for in terms of the workings of strength, E, ensues, explaining the Yerkes-
two or more partially separate systems. Dodson curve without requiring the Yerkes-
A speculative attempt to explain the cur- Dodson law's paradoxical quality" (p. 117).
vilinear relationship between arousal and It is undoubtedly the case that the theoret-
performance was made by Cattell and Child ical proposals of Cattell and Child (1975) re-
(1975). They argued that it was not really ac- present a gross over-simplification of a com-
curate to state that very high levels of moti- plex reality. Nevertheless, the notion that
vation result in poor performance, because motivation per se typically facilitates per-
this overlooks the fact that motivation and formance, whereas emotional reactions
anxiety are positively correlated in most si- such as frustration and anxiety tend to im-
tuations. According to Cattell and child, in- pair performance, is interesting and seems
creased motivation or drive improves per- to capture an important truth.
formance, whereas anxiety worsens it. This Humphreys et al. (1980) also suggested
led them to the following equation, in which that many of the effects of arousal on per-
P stands for performance, E for drive and A formance could be explained by assuming
for anxiety: that there are two separate activation states:
P = aE - bA (a and b being constants). arousal and on-task effort. According to
Cattell and Child specified the precise in- their model, the arousal mechanism is af-
6 Introduction

fected by several factors, including anxiety, al. (1969). Three levels of difficulty of a digit
caffeine and impulsivity. Increased arousal transformation task were used. On each tri-
reduces the capacity of short-term memory al, 0, I or 3 had to be added to each of four
but improves attention. In contrast, on-task serially presented digits, with the trans-
effort is reduced under high anxiety but is formed series being read out by the subject
unaffected by impulsivity and caffeine; in- after a 2-s pause. Three physiological mea-
creased on-task effort improves attention sures (i.e. pupil diameter, heart rate and skin
but has no effect on short-term memory ca- resistance) were recorded continuously. The
pacity. While the precise effects on perform- same pattern of activity was observed for all
ance of changes in either of the two activa- three measures: an increase during the input
tion states depend in part on the demands of and processing of the information, followed
the task being performed, the general as- by a decrease during the report stage. The
sumption is that performance is a positive pupillary diameter data are shown in
function of both attention and the capacity Fig. 1.2. Pupil diameter grew steadily as the
of short-term memory. task load (i.e. number of digits presented)
Most of the research investigating arousal increased, and pupil dilation was also re-
and performance has concentrated on the sponsive to the difficulty of the required
effects of arousal on performance. How- transformation.
ever, it may very well prove just as impor- What significance should be attached to
tant to consider the impact of task perform- the existence of such a close correspon-
ance on arousal. As Kahneman (1973) has dence between processing demands on the
pointed out, there are at least two well-docu- one hand and physiological measures of
mented effects of task processing on a num- arousal on the other? Since most cognitive
ber of physiological variables: (I) the grea- theories fail to discuss this correspondence
ter the processing demands imposed by a at all, it is apparently the case that most the-
task, the larger are the effects on physiologi- orists regard it either as an epiphenomenon
cal measures of various kinds and (2) phy- or as a relatively trivial finding. In contrast,
siological indexes often reflect with great the view taken in this book is that an ade-
sensitivity moment-by-moment fluctuations quate understanding of human cognition
in the processing demands of a task. must involve some consideration of the pre-
These intimate interrelationships be- cisely modulated physiological changes as-
tween processing activities and arousal were sociated with processing activity.
demonstrated very clearly by Kahneman et The basic themes and assumptions ex-

.1-0
~

E
E
z

0:
w .0-5
f-
W
~
«
0
..J
Q.
~ BASELINE

-0-2
Fig. 1.2. Effects of memory
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 (TIME IN SEeS) load (digit presentation)
DIGIT P RESPOND
PRESENTATION C and task complexity (add
factor) on pupillary dila-
S
E tion. (Kahneman et al. 1969)
Introduction 7

plored in this book can now be summarized. Chap. 4 looks at the effects of arousal on
Firstly, it is assumed that information pro- performance from the perspective of the
cessing and cognition generally are affected most influential theories in the area. Many
by the prevailing motivational and emotion- of these theories assume that changes in a
al conditions. Secondly, the assumption that unitary arousal system (however produced)
a large variety of motivational, emotional have predictable consequences for perform-
and other factors all affect performance via ance. In contrast, Chaps. 5 to 8 consider the
a unitary arousal system is discussed at possibility that there may be relatively spe-
some length. The notion that there is a single cific effects on performance of applying any
arousal system is clearly an over-simplifica- particular arousing agent. Motivational ef-
tion; it is probably more fruitful to distin- fects on performance are explored in
guish a number of sub-components of the Chap. 5, with the emphasis primarily on the
arousal system. It is further assumed that ways in which incentive manipulations af-
many of the performance effects of arousing fect information processing. Chap. 6 deals
agents (e.g. incentives, ego threat, white with the effects on performance of anxiety,
noise and stimulant drugs) are mediated by whether construed as an important person-
attentional mechanisms. There are several ality dimension or produced by an external
ways in which stressors or arousing agents stressor. The fact that incentives and anxiety
might affect attentional processes; these in- have somewhat dissimilar effects on behav-
clude changes in attentional selectivity, at- iour suggests the value of distinguishing
tentional capacity, speed of functioning of among components of arousal.
attentional mechanisms and susceptibility
A number of other factors thought to in-
to attentional distraction.
volve the arousal system are considered in
The structure of the book is determined in
Chaps. 7 and 8. These include such dispa-
large measure by the requirement to provide
rate factors as white noise, time of day, sleep
adequate coverage of these main themes.
deprivation and introversion-extraversion.
Chaps. 2 and 3 deal with contemporary the-
ories of attention and processing resources, Finally, in Chap. 9 an attempt is made to
together with a discussion of the earlier sem- produce a Hegelian synthesis out of the ear-
inal views of Broadbent, Treisman and lier thesis and antithesis. In contemporary
Deutsch and Deutsch. The hypothetical jargon, a 'framework' or 'sketch-map' is
constructs examined in these two chapters supplied rather than a fully fledged and
provide a suitable basis for considering the quantitatively precise hypothetico-deduc-
effects of arousal on attention. tive theory.
2 Theories of Attention

The concept of attention has led a rather ture distinguishing the cognitive approach
chequered existence during the history of from the earlier Behaviourist conceptualiza-
psychology. It was highly regarded by the tion.
introspectionists and armchair theorizers of
the nineteenth century, some of whom had
important things to say about attention. For
example, the following quotation from Wil- 2.1 Early Theories
liam James (1980) anticipates a number of
later theoretical developments: "If, then, by
the original question, how many ideas or Major theories were put forward in the late
things can we attend to at once, be meant 1950s and early 1960s by Broadbent (1958),
how many entirely disconnected systems or Treisman (1964) and Deutsch and Deutsch
processes of conception can go on simul- (1963). While there were important differ-
taneously, the answer is, not easily more ences among them, these three theories had
than one, unless the processes are very habi- in common the notion that there was a bot-
tual; but then two, or even three, without tleneck at some point in the course of infor-
very much oscillation of the mind from one mation processing. They differed primarily
to the next, and no consequent gain of time" with respect to the putative location of the
(p.409). bottleneck. Broadbent (1958) argued that it
The arrival of Watson and the start of the occurred early in processing. According to
Behaviourist revolution soon put a stop to him, when two stimuli or messages occur to-
such idle speculation. It was deemed fruit- gether, they are entered in parallel to a sen-
less to speculate on the processes interven- sory buffer. In the absence of peripheral
ing beween stimulus and response, and so masking, no interference occurs at this
all consideration of attention (an interven- stage. One message is then allowed through
ing process par excellence) virtually came to a filter on the basis of its physical character-
an end for some 40 years or more. As recent- istics, while the other message is held in the
ly as 1953, Osgood published a renowned buffer for later processing. The filter pre-
and massive 800 page textbook on experi- vents overloading of a limited-capacity me-
mental psychology and failed to refer to at- chanism that translates the stimulus to some
tention at all. other code stored in long-term memory. In
It was against this background that contrast, Deutsch and Deutsch (1963) sug-
Broadbent's (1958) book appeared. In it, he gested that all incoming stimuli receive full
developed the notion that attention was of perceptual analysis and that the selection
central importance in accounting for human shown in attentive behaviour (i.e. its bottle-
functioning and in many ways set the scene neck) occurs thereafter when it comes to the
for the subsequent burgeoning of cognitive selection of a response.
psychology. Indeed, the concept of atten- Treisman (1964) preferred a modification
tion lies at the very core of much contempor- of Broadbent's (1958) theory in which unat-
ary thinking and may even be the main fea- tended information is attenuated rather
Early Theories 9

than completely rejected and in which the versed speech, the subjects did not usually
location of the bottleneck is somewhat flexi- notice anything unusual. On the other hand,
ble. She described a hierarchy of analyses physical changes in the non-shadowed mes-
based on physical cues, syllabic pattern, sage (e.g. the introduction of a different
specific sounds, individual words, grammat- voice or a pure tone) were nearly always de-
ical structure and meaning. Tests are carried tected.
out in order, starting at the bottom of the Thus early work with the shadowing task
hierarchy and working up. If there is inade- seemed to indicate that Broadbent was right
quate capacity to process all of a message, to assume that there was extremely limited
tests low in the hierarchy can be completed, processing of unattended information. One
providing information about, say, its physi- of the more striking findings was obtained
cal characteristics and syllabic pattern, but by Moray (1959), who found that subjects
tests higher up the hierarchy have to be completely failed to remember words pre-
omitted. In addition, Treisman argued that sented 35 times each on the unattended mes-
if a particular word was expected, all of the sage.
tests relevant to selecting that word might be With the benefit of hindsight, it is easy to
pre-biased or pre-sensitized towards it. see that the shadowing task may in many
Some of the initial empirical evidence fit- ways be unrepresentative of normal atten-
ted in well with Broadbent's notion that the tional functioning. Inter alia, the subjects
selective filter rejects information from un- used in the early experiments had no previ-
wanted sources after a preliminary analysis ous experience of shadowing messages, and
of its physical characteristics. Of particular so the shadowing requirement may have
importance was the work of Cherry (1953). used most of their available attentional ca-
True to his English background, Cherry got pacity. If that was the case, it might explain
interested in the 'cocktail party' problem, the subjects' relative inability to process the
i.e. how are we able to follow just one con- non-shadowed message. In addition, people
versation when several people are talking at seem to be better at performing two activi-
the same time? The answer to the problem is ties at once when the activities are dissimilar
that people use physical differences be- (e.g. driving a car and holding a conversa-
tween auditory messages to sort them out: if tion), whereas shadowing experiments typi-
two messages are in different voices or pre- cally require two extremely similar activities
sented one to each ear, there is no problem. to be performed (i.e. processing each of two
However, if both messages are presented in auditorily presented messages). The notion
the same voice to both ears, it is extremely that people ought to be able to perform two
difficult to separate out the messages on the simple and dissimilar activities together
basis of meaning alone. These findings presumably lies behind the remark made by
clearly suggest that efficient selective atten- former American President Lyndon
tion depends on the filter being able to reject B. Johnson about Gerald Ford viz., that he
unwanted information at an early stage on was so dumb that he couldn't fart and chew
the basis of simple physical features such as gum at the same time!
voice or spatial location. The importance of practice was demon-
Cherry (1953) obtained even more stri- strated very clearly by Underwood (1974).
king findings in other experiments in which He compared the performance on a shad-
the auditory message in one ear had to be owing task of naive subjects and of Neville
shadowed (i.e. repeated back aloud) while Moray, an eminent researcher who has
another message was played to the other spent hundreds or thousands of hours con-
ear. He found that his subjects were ama- ducting shadowing. experiments. The sub-
zingly poor at extracting information from jects shadowed one list ofletters while a sec-
the non-shadowed message. Even when that ond list was being presented, and their task
message was in a foreign language or in re- was to detect a single digit appearing in eith-
10 Theories of Attention

er list. Detection rates were uniformly high . lowed by an electric shock. Later on, two
when the digit appeared on the shadowed lists of words were presented, one in each
list. However, there was a huge difference ear, and the subjects shadowed one ear and
between Moray and the other subjects when ignored the other. When the previously
it came to detecting digits in the non-sha- shocked word (or its synonym or homonym)
dowed list, especially when the digits were appeared in the ignored list, there was a dis-
spoken in the same voice as the letters: the cernible galvanic skin response, which is
naive subjects detected 8 % of such digits, part of the typical response to shock.
whereas Moray detected 67 %. Moray thus Since the subjects were not consciously
has some. claim to be the world champion aware that any of these words had been pre-
shadowing task expert, and his extremely sented, the inadequacy of relying entirely on
high level of performance presumably oc- introspective awareness is obvious. In addi-
curs because he can shadow with minimal tion, of course, the results go against the ear-
use of attentional capacity. ly filter notion, since they suggest that infor-
The importance of the similarity between mation on the ignored ear is not blocked pri-
two concurrent tasks was shown by Allport or to semantic analysis in long-term memo-
et al. (1972). The task of shadowing pas- ry. However, it should be noted that Ward-
sages taken from George Orwell's Selected law and Kroll (1976) were unable to repli-
Essays was combined with the presentation cate the galvanic skin response findings of
of a learning task. When the learning task Von Wright et al.
consisted of a list of auditorily presented The history of work on the shadowing
words, subsequent recognition memory for task demonstrates that the amount of pro-
the words was at chance level. However, cessing on the non-shadowed message of
when the learning task involved a series of which the subject is aware can vary between
pictures, 90% of them were correctly recog- practically nothing (Cherry 1953) and virtu-
nized. Obviously, the limitations on the pro- ally everything (Allport et al. 1972), depend-
cessing of two concurrent inputs that were ing on various factors such as the stage of
stressed by Broadbent (1958) can be largely practice and the degree of similarity be-
overcome if the inputs are sufficiently dissi- tween the shadowed and non-shadowed in-
milar. This conclusion was strengthened by puts. In a nutshell, the problem with Broad-
the results of a further experiment carried bent's (1958) theory is that it postulates a rel-
out by Allport et al. in which expert pianists atively rigid system of selective attention
were able to shadow an auditorily presented that cannot cope with the great flexibility
passage and play the piano using sight read- actually observed. This problem can be fur-
ing at the same time, with no discernible im- ther illustrated by considering the dichotic
pairment in either task compared to its per- listening task, to which we now turn.
formance when done alone. Broadbent (1958) invented a task in
A general problem with early theoretical which three pairs of digits were presented
and experimental approaches was the impli- dichotically, i.e. one set of three digits
cit assumption that people are aware of the read serially was heard at one ear at the
processing which they do or at least of the same time as a second set of three digits was
products of such processing. There is increa- heard at the other ear. After all of the digits
sing evidence that this assumption is errone- had been presented, subjects had to recall
ous and that substantial amounts of process- them. The key finding was that subjects pre-
ing occur below the level of consciousness. ferred to recall the digits ear by ear, rather
As an example, consider a study by on than in terms of the temporal order in which
Wright et al. (1975). They presented a long they had heard the digits. According to
list of words to the attended ear, and the Broadbent (1958), this indicated that the fil-
Finnish word for 'suitable' occurred six ter selected information on the basis ofphy-
times. On two of those occasions, it was fol- sical features (i.e. ear of arrival).
Early Theories II
Gray and Wedderburn (1960) rejected the tended message over the unattended mes-
notion that selective attention necessarily sage in terms of the targets detected (87 %
operates on the basis of the physical charac- versus 8%). However, Deutsch and Deutsch
teristics of sensory channels. They used a (1967) were disinclined to take the results at
dichotic task in which one ear might re- face value. They pointed out that while their
ceive "Who 7 there" while the other ear theory claimed that all inputs were analysed
was hearing "2 goes 3". The order of report completely, it also stated that only import-
was usually semantically determined (e.g. ant inputs led to a response. Since subjects
"Who goes there" followed by "2 7 3"). In had to make two different responses to tar-
other words, attentional selection can occur gets in the shadowed message (i.e. shadow
after the processing of information from and tap) but only had to make one response
both ears rather than before as predicted by to non-shadowed targets (i.e. tap), it could
Broadbent (1958). be argued that the shadowed targets were in
In spite of the intuitive plausibility of some sense more important than the non-
Broadbent's notion of a selective filter that shadowed targets.
acts to minimize the amount of processing
Treisman and Riley (1969) returned to the
that must be performed by more complex,
fray with an experiment designed to obviate
higher level processes, it is clear that there is
this problem. Subjects were told to stop
frequently extensive processing of unat-
shadowing at once and to tap whenever they
tended sources of information. The theories
heard a target in either message, and they
of Treisman (1964) and Deutsch and
were explicitly instructed not to shadow the
Deutsch (1963) both allowed for this, so
target word if it occurred on the shadowed
there was a search during the 1960s for an
message. This ensured that the response was
experimentum crucis to decide between
precisely the same to targets in either mes-
them. The two most impressive attempts
sage. The targets were either in the same
were those of Treisman and Geffen (1967) voice as the rest of the message or in an op-
and Treisman and Riley (1969).
posite-sexed voice. Since the difference in
Treisman and Geffen asked their subjects voice would not affect the relative import-
to shadow one of two dichotic messages ance of shadowed message and non-sha-
and at the same time to monitor both mes-
dowed message targets, it should not affect
sages so that they could tap whenever they performance according to Deutsch and
heard a particular target word in either ear. Deutsch's theory.
They argued that since the tapping response
and its stimulus were identical for both mes- The results of this experiment are shown
sages, any difference in the efficiency of de- in Fig. 2.1. They clearly fail to support the
tecting targets in the two messages must be approach of Deutsch and Deutsch (1963).
due to a failure to perceive targets in the The targets were detected far more often
non-shadowed message. If perceptual anal- when they were presented in a voice differ-
ysis of both messages is complete, as sug- ent to the rest of the input than when they
gested by Deutsch and Deutsch (1963), were presented in the same voice. Further-
there should be no difference in target de- more, same voice targets were detected al-
tections between the two messages. In con- most twice as often on the shadowed ear as
trast, Treisman's notion that perceptual an- on the non-shadowed ear.
alysis of the non-shadowed message is at- There are some problems of interpreta-
tenuated leads to the prediction that fewer tion with the findings of Treisman and Gef-
targets would be detected on the non-sha- fen (1967) and Treisman and Riley (1969)
dowed message. which have not always been recognized. The
Who was right? On the face of it, Treis- shadowing requirement may have meant
man won hands down: there was an enor- that the speech presented to the non-sha-
mous advantage of the shadowed or at- dowed ear had to be actively prevented from
12 Theories of Attention

100 DIFFERENT
VOICE

z 90
Q 80
f-
U
w 70 SAME
f- VOICE
W
0 60
f-
Z
w 50
U
tr
w 40
0..
30 Fig. 2.1. Target detection in the shadowed
and non-shadowed messages when the tar-
I
gets were presented in the same voice as,
NON-SHADOWED SHADOWED or a different voice to, the non-target sti-
MESSAGE muli. (Treisman and Riley 1969)

commanding the shadowing response rath- able to do these two tasks together extreme-
er than merely remaining passively unat- ly well and could read as quickly and with as
tended. In addition, the differential detec- much comprehension when taking dictation
tion rates on the two ears may reflect the de- as when only reading, and their handwriting
mands of selective shadowing rather than had improved.
general capacity limitations. When there is At this stage, they did not appear to be
no shadowing requirement, it has some- processing the dictated words at all thor-
times been found that target detection is oughly. Diane and John were only able to
equivalent, whether one ear or both ears recall 35 out of several thousand words
must be monitored (e.g. Moray et al. 1976). which they had written down. When 20 suc-
As we have already mentioned, the early cessive dictated words all belonged to the
theories of attention all assumed that there same semantic category (e.g. articles of fur-
was a bottleneck somewhere in the stream of niture) or formed a sentence, they were un-
information processing that made it ex- aware of the fact. With more training, they
tremely difficult, or even impossible, for were able to detect rhyming words, semanti-
people to perform two activities efficiently cally related words and sentences among
at the same time. In recent years, however, the dictated words while reading a story
there has been accumulating evidence to with full comprehension. Eventually they
suggest the possibility that there is no bottle- learned to write down the names of the cate-
neck at all. gories to which the dictated words be-
Perhaps the most striking support for longed, rather than the words themselves,
such a viewpoint was obtained by Spelke et while maintaining normal reading speed
al. (1976). They gave two students (Diane and comprehension.
and John) 5 h of training per week for 17 If you have any doubts about the achieve-
weeks on a variety of tasks. At first, they ment of these two students, just try to read a
asked the students to read short stories se- book with full comprehension while writing
lected from collections of works by Ameri- down dictated words! Spelke et al. (1976)
can, English and translated European wri- drew the following conclusions from the re-
ters and at the same time to write down markable results obtained from John and
words at dictation. The students found it Diane: "They understood both the text they
very difficult to do these two tasks together: were reading and the words they were copy-
their reading speed decreased substantially ing. In at least this limited sense, they
and their handwriting deteriorated. After achieved a true division of attention: they
six weeks of training, however, they were were able to extract meaning simultaneous-
Posner and Snyder: Automatic Activation and Conscious Attention 13

ly from what they read and from what they needed to capture the cross-situational var-
heard ... People's ability to develop skills iability that is so noticeable in the function-
in specialized situations is so great that it ing of attentional mechanisms. Some of the
may never be possible to define general li- major current attempts to provide such con-
mits on cognitive capacity" (p. 229). ceptualizations are discussed in the follow-
The data of Spelke et al. are consistent ing pages.
with the notion that there is no fixed atten-
tional capacity. However, alternative inter-
pretations are possible. Perhaps the writing 2.2 Posner and Snyder: Automatic
task became automatic and so did not re- Activation and Conscious Attention
quire any attentional capacity, or there
might have been a rapid alternation of atten-
tion between reading and writing. Hirst et An interesting new theory of attention was
al. (1980) attempted to adjudicate among proposed by Posner and Snyder (1975a, b)
these possibilities. They rejected the hypo- in which they develofJed a conceptual dis-
thesis that writing to dictation was done au- tinction between automatic processes and
tomatically on the basis that subjects clearly conscious attention. They suggested that
understood what they were writing (e.g. they there were three useful operational criteria
made fewer copying errors with real sen- that had to be satisfied in order for a process
tences than with random words). They fur- to be classified as automatic: (1) it must oc-
ther argued that successful performance of cur without intention, (2) it should not give
the two tasks with rapid alternation could rise to any conscious awareness and (3) it
only readily be accomplished when the ma- should not interfere with any other ongoing
terial to be read was highly redundant. mental activity.
However, most subjects who were trained to Posner (1978) related automatic pro-
copy words while reading very redundant cesses to the notion of a "psychological
material were able to cope extremely well pathway', which was defined as "the set of
when switched to less redundant reading internal codes and their connections that
matter. This would not have been possible if are activated automatically by the presenta-
attentional capacity were being alternated tion of a stimulus" (p. 90). It was further as-
between the two tasks, according to Hirst et sumed that automatic activation occurs as a
al. result of prior learning.
Hirst et al. (1980) came to a conclusion In contrast to the relative invariance of
dramatically at variance with the theoretical automatic processes, conscious attention
positions adopted by the early attention the- can be used in an extremely flexible way at
orists: "The ability to divide attention is several different stages of information pro-
constrained primarily by the individual's le- cessing. As Kerr (1973) demonstrated in a
vel of skill, not by the size of a fixed pool of review of many of the relevant studies, con-
resources" (p. 98). An equally plausible in- scious attention can be applied to the mak-
terpretation of their data is simply that high- ing of an overt response, to retrieval from
ly practised skills require less attentional ca- memory, to stimulus transformation or to
pacity than newly acquired ones. hypothesis formation. It can ev~n be ap-
In sum, the greatest inadequacy of the pi- plied to processes that are normally auto-
oneering theories of attention was that they matic, although not always with success.
all under-estimated the flexibility of atten- For example, if you think too deeply about
tional mechanisms and processes. It may be the leg movements involved in walking
correct to say that some people in some situ- down a flight of stairs, you may well finish
ations behave in the way specified by each up in a heap at the bottom of those stairs!
of the three theories dealt with in this sec- According to Posner (1978), the most im-
tion, but broader conceptualizations are portant characteristic of conscious attention
14 Theories of Attention

is its limited capacity. Two fairly obvious to shift from its focus on the expected event
manifestations of this capacity limitation to the event which has actually occurred,
are the narrow range of conscious experi- and this shift is time consuming. If no spe-
ence at any given moment (James 1890) and cific expectations can reasonably be
the decrements in performance usually formed, conscious attention may have little
found when people attempt to perform two effect on performance.
tasks at the same time (Kerr 1973).
Some of the main conditions used by Pos-
Can these various theoretical ideas be
ner and Snyder are shown in Table 2.1. In
tested experimentally? Posner and Snyder
order to evaluate their predictions about the
(1975a) attempted to show that they could,
coasts and benefits associated with the use
using a letter-matching task. A priming sti-
of automatic activation and conscious atten-
mulus (either a plus sign or a letter) was fol-
tion, they had to have a control condition to
lowed by a letter pair, and the subject had to
use as a baseline. This was provided by us-
decide whether the two letters in the pair
ing " + " as a prime. As can be seen in Table
were the same as quickly as possible. Posner
2.1, the results were in good accord with the
and Snyder made two basic predictions:
theoretical predictions and suggested fairly
1. The priming stimulus automatically acti-
strongly that at least two different processes
vates its own representation in memory; this
were determining performance.
facilitates performance whenever the letter
pair is the same as the prime (e.g. "A" fol- The proper interpretation of the finding
lowed by "AA") and has no effect on per- that expected events were responded to fas-
formance when the letter pair is different ter than unexpected events merits further
from the prime. consideration. Posner and Snyder claimed
2. Conscious attention responds to the prob- that limited-capacity attention was allocated
abilities of different events, speeding up de- preferentially to expected events, which
cision making when an expected event oc- seems intuitively reasonable. However, the
curs, but slowing it down when the unex- use of primes or cues to indicate the proba-
pected happens. It takes a long time to han- bilities of different subsequent events has
dle unexpected events because attention has been considered to influence the response

Table 2.1. Speed of deciding that the two letters in a pair are the same as a function of automatic activa-
tion and conscious attention. Times are in milliseconds. (Posner and Snyder 1975a)

Positive trials

Prob. that Control Expected Unexpected Benefit Cost


prime matches (e.g. + AA) (e.g. A AA) (e.g. BAA)
letter pair

80% 414 329 450 85 36

Automatic activation and conscious attention facilitated performance when the letter pair was expect-
ed, but conscious attention slowed performance when it was unexpected.

Prob. that Control Match Mismatch Benefit Cost


prime matches (e.g. + AA) (e.g. A AA) (e.g. BAA)
letter pair

20% 439 408 439 31 o


Automatic activation facilitated performance when the letter pair matched the prime, but there was no
cost because no specific expectations were formed.
Posner and Snyder: Automatic Activation and Conscious Attention 15

bias parameter of signal detection theory. It In this experiment, Posner and Snyder
is thus not clear that the data necessarily re- (1975a) predicted that inhibitory effects
veal the functioning of a limited capacity at- (due entirely to conscious attention) should
tentional mechanism. take longer to appear than facilitatory ef-
Why was such an elaborate experiment fects (due to a combination of automatic ac-
necessary? Part of the answer is that in tivation and conscious attention). The suc-
many instances it is extremely difficult to cess of their prediction can be seen in
separate out the processes at work. By refer- Fig. 2.2.
ence to Table 2.1, you can see that if 'A' is The most noteworthy application of the
followed by the expected letter pair 'AA', basic theoretical ideas of Posner and Snyder
there are two quite different reasons why has been to work on lexical decision, in
performance should be facilitated: (1) con- which subjects are typically asked to decide
scious attention is prepared for the letter rapidly whether or not a letter string is a
pair before it is presented and (2) automatic word. One of the fundamental findings is
activation allows more rapid processing of that the amount of time taken to decide that
the letter pair. Several other conditions were a particular letter string (e.g. 'NURSE') is a
required to decide on the relative import- word is shorter if the preceding item or
ance of those two factors. prime is a semantically related word (e.g.
An important theoretical point to note is 'DOCTO R') than if it is a semantically unre-
that, accord~ng to Posner and Snyder, facili- lated word (e.g. 'BUTTER'). In terms of
tation of performance can be due either to Posner and Snyder's approach, the main is-
conscious attention or to automatic activa- sue is whether this so-called semantic-pri-
tion, whereas impairment of performance is ming effect is due to automatic activation, to
due only to conscious attention. Posner and conscious attention or to a combination of
Snyder (1975a) made use of these theoreti- the two.
cal assumptions in a further experiment One obvious way of attempting to show
based only on the conditions from the top the role played by automatic activation un-
half of Table 2.1. They tested the prediction confounded by conscious attention is to
that an important difference between auto- present the priming word so rapidly that
matic activation and conscious attention is subjects have no awareness of which word
that automatic activation, as a result of ex- was presented. Fischler and Goodman
tended practice, operates at greater speed. (1978) presented a priming word for only
They did this by varying the time interval be- 40 ms, followed immediately by a letter
tween the prime and the letter pair. string for lexical decision. The usual seman-

z~
60
Ou ()o---------~ INHIBITION
-w
f-", I
~l: 40
,/
FACILITATION
:::i~
-z
U o
it;:
LLiii
o-J:
f-z
20
,fio...., /
I
"
z-
:>Q:
00
0
JI "'/

l:
«
Fig. 2.2. The differential time course of the 0 100 200 300 400 500
development offacilitatory and inhibitory PRIME TO LETTER PAIR
effects. (Posner and Snyder 1975a) INTERVAL (MSEC)
16 Theories of Attention

tic-priming effect was obtained: the letter task. The priming word was the name of a
string was identified as a word more quickly semantic category (e.g. 'Bird'), and it was
when the prime was an associated word followed by a letter string requiring a word-
than when it was unrelated. nonword decision. In the crucial condition,
In spite of the fact that subjects had been subjects were led to expect that a category
instructed to attempt to recall the prime name would usually be followed by a mem-
word immediately after each lexical deci- ber of a different, pre-specified category
sion, only 2 % of the primes were recalled. (e.g. 'Bird' followed by part of a building).
This suggests that there was little conscious In this condition, there were two important
awareness of the prime words and that the kinds of trials:
semantic-priming effect obtained was prob- 1. The category name followed by a member
ably due to a rapid automatic activation of of the expected category (e.g. 'Bird-Door').
the representation of the prime word In this case, conscious attention can facili-
spreading out to nearby locations of seman- tate performance, but automatic activation
tically associated words. This presumably is irrelevant, since it only activates semantic
happens as a result of many years of using associates of 'bird'.
language. 2. The category name followed unexpected-
The approach of Fischler and Goodman ly by a member of the same category (e.g.
was taken to its logical conclusion by Mar- 'Bird-Robin'). In this case, automatic activa-
cel. He presented the priming word very tion will facilitate performance (because the
briefly and followed it immediately with a words are semantically related), but con-
pattern mask that inhibited any further pro- scious attention will impair performance
cessing. Initial testing was done to discover (because the event is unexpected).
the time between onset of the prime and the The rule of thumb to use in understanding
mask at which each subject individually was the findings is that conscious attention facil-
no longer able to detect the presence of a itates performance when the word requiring
word at above chance level. lexical decision belongs to the expected
Although his subjects apparently did not category and impairs performance when it
even realize that a priming word had been belongs to an unexpected category; auto-
presented, Marcel nevertheless obtained a matic activation improves performance
fairly substantial semantic-priming effect! when the word is semantically related to the
Since this effect depends on the congruence priming category name (i.e. same category)
of meaning between the prime and the sub- and has no effect when there is no semantic
sequent word, the implication is that auto- association between the prime and the tar-
matic processes can reach that level of anal- get word. There is one important qualifica-
ysis. tion to the above predictions: since con-
These findings may seem counter-intui- scious attention requires some time to devel-
tive or even downright unbelievable, but the op an expectation, little or no effect of con-
reason for this is that we have a natural tend- scious attention on performance is discerni-
ency to exaggerate the role of consciousness ble at the shortest interval between the cate-
in our everyday lives. A commonplace ex- gory prime and the target word. Amazingly
ample of automatic processing occurs some- enough, virtually all of these detailed pre-
times when you turn over the pages of a dictions were supported by the data, as can
book and start to read the top line, but find be seen in Fig. 2.3.
that an unattended word near the bottom of Posner (1978) provided more extensive
the page seems to 'catch your eye'. support for some of the ideas put forward by
Neely (1977) used an ingenious technique Posner and Snyder (1975a, b). He pointed
to disentangle the effects of automatic out that there is evidence from other situa-
spreading activation and conscious 'atten- tions suggesting the importance of automat-
tion on performance of the lexical decision ic activation. One example is the Stroop ef-
Posner and Snyder: Automatic Activation and Conscious Attention 17

+60
'+
~u

ZW +40
01/)
-~
I-
«Z + 20 1:.. .....
1--
' .......
:ii
u~ 0 .... .......,
--------~~------------
~~ ..........
IJ..-
Ot: - 20 ............
...........
f-~
zI
..... ....-6
:::>z -40
0-
~a::
«0 -60
Fig. 2.3. The time course of fa-
cilitatory and inhibitory effects 250 350 450 550 650 750
as a function of whether or not PRIME TO TARGET INTERVAL (IN MSEC)
the target word was semantical-
ly related to the prime word and 0---0 EXPECTED, SEMANTICALLY RELATED
of whether or not the target _ EXPECTED; SEMANTICALLY UNRELATED
word belonged to the expected !:r'-'-6. UNEXPECTED; SEMANTICALLY RELATED
category. (Neely 1977) ---.. UNEXPECTED; SEMANTICALLY UNRELATED

fect in which people find it extremely diffi- al. (1974) replicated some features of Le-
cult and stressful to name the colours in wis's experiment; they found that the se-
which colour words are printed (e.g. the mantic relationship between simultaneous
word 'Red' printed in blue). While the pre- shadowed and non-shadowed words had a
cise processes involved in the Stroop effect modest effect on the time taken to shadow
are unknown, it is clear that subjects cannot early in the list, but the effect disappeared
choose to avoid processing aspects of an in- by the seventh item shadowed.
put that they wish to ignore. Posner (1978) pointed out that conscious
There is also evidence for automatic pro- attention should be regarded as a discrete
cessing of unattended inputs in the shadow- event that plays a specific role in the stream
ing 'task. In a study discussed earlier in this of information processing. Its presence in
chapter, Von Wright et al. (1975) found that that stream gives rise to a number of indi-
an unattended word produced a galvanic cants at the behavioural and physiological
skin response if it was synonymous with a levels. For example, as we have seen, Posner
word previously associated with electric and Snyder (1975a) argued that the occur-
shock. This indicates that there can be sub- rence of a performance impairment when
stantial processing of unattended informa- responding to unexpected events is one
tion, presumably via automatic processes. measure of the involvement of conscious at-
A related finding was obtained by Lewis tention.
(1970), who found that the reaction time to An alternative method is to present a
shadow a word on the attended ear was probe stimulus at some point during the per-
slowed down slightly when a synonymous formance of a main task and observe the
word was presented simultaneously to the reaction time to that stimulus. The basic ar-
unattended ear. Since the subjects reported gument is that reaction time to the probe
no 3lwareness of the content of the unat- will be increased when conscious attention
tended words, this suggests that automatic is most committed to the main task. In one
processes may have been responsible. of Posner's studies, the subjects carried out
The effect observed by Lewis may be a letter-matching task on two letters separat-
more limited than he implied. Treisman et ed by a short interval of time, and the probe
18 Theories of Attention

stimulus was presented during that interval. fairly convincingly that there is a valid con-
The point at which the probe stimulus pro- ceptual distinction between automatic and
duced the slowest reaction time moved pro- conscious processes, and Posner and Sny-
gressively further away from the presenta- der (197Sa, b) have provided a plausible ac-
tion of the first letter as the time interval be- count of the salient differences between the
tween the two letters was increased. In this two kinds of processes. Perhaps the weakest
case, conscious attention was deployed flex- theoretical assumption is that automatic ac-
ibly, with subjects using their knowledge tivation cannot impair other ongoing men-
about the time between letters in such a way tal processes. One of the few studies to in,
that maximal attention to the first letter was vestigate this assumption directly was that
delayed until shortly before the presenta- of Neely (1977), and his data seemed to sup-
tion of the second letter. port Posner and Snyder. However, while
At the physiological level, Posner (1978) there was no impairment in reaction time
argued that the P 300 component of the aver- when automatic activation was irrelevant to
age evoked potential may reflect conscious the target word, there did appear to be some
attention. It is referred to as P300 because it "cost" in terms of increased error rate. Since
is a positive wave occurring approximately Antos (1979) found evidence of a substan-
300 ms after the presentation of a stimulus, tial speed-accuracy trade-off on the lexical
although it may occur as early as ISO ms or decision task (i.e. a small increase in error
as late as 600 ms after input. The evidence rate was associated with a large increase in
suggests that P 300 occurs whenever a signal response speed), Neely's data cannot be
or a time interval can reasonably be said to considered to be definitive.
demand close attention in a particular task. There may also be some problems with
It does not appear to represent merely some the assumption that the delay in responding
invariant response to a stimulus, because it to an unexpected event occurs simply be-
can be obtained when an event is omitted af- cause of the time required to switch atten-
ter a repetitive series of stimuli. This, of tion. The expectation from this point of
course, relates P 300 to the subject's expecta- view is that the time to switch attention will
tions, and we have seen in the behavioural depend on the semantic dissimilarity or dis-
data of Posner and Snyder (l97Sa) that the tance between the actual and expected
formation of expectations is one of the func- events. Neill (1979) found that it was some-
tions of conscious attention. times more difficult to switch attention to an
Posner et al. (1973) looked at the average unexpected event in the same category as
evoked potential in a letter-matching task in the expected event than to an unexpected
which the first letter was followed after I s event in a different category, which is diam-
by a letter that either matched it or did not. etrically opposed to the theoretical predic-
Their main interest was in the evoked poten- tion. As Neill suggested, the time to switch
tial to the second item as a function of attention may be determined by several fac-
whether there was a match or mismatch. At- tors, including the time taken to signal the
tention was manipulated by asking the sub- switching mechanism, the time to disengage
jects to count only matches or only mis- attention from the expected event, the trans-
matches. The instructional manipulation it time between the expected event and the
had a considerable effect on P300, with its actual, unexpected event and the time to en-
magnitude being greater for matches than gage attention with the unexpected event.
mismatches when matches had to be count-
ed (i.e. attended to) and the opposite occur- 2.3 Shiffrin and Schneider: Controlled
ring when mismatches had to be counted. It and Automatic Processes
seems probable that these effects on P300
reflect the use of conscious attention. Schneider and Shiffrin (1977) and Shif-
The empirical evidence seems to indicate frin and Schneider (1977) proposed an am-
Shiffrin and Schneider: Controlled and Automatic Processes 19

bitious theory that attempted to integrate turn to each item of the memory set at a rate
work and theorizing in the related areas of of 40 ms per comparison.
selective attention, short-term memory Very similar results have been obtained in
search and visual search. While their basic studies of visual search. In a typical study
theoretical position resembles that of Pos- (Atkinson et al. 1969), a single memory item
ner and Snyder (197 Sa, b), they have carried was placed in short-term memory, and then
out very different kinds of experiments. the items comprising the visual display were
Since the paradigm which they used repres- presented simultaneously. The task was to
ents an amalgam of standard memory decide as quickly as possible whether or not
search and visual search situations, we shall the memory set item was present in the dis-
begin by considering these component play. Mirabile dictu, there was a linear rela-
tasks. tionship between the number of items in the
Sternberg (1966, 1975) introduced a sim- visual display and reaction, and there was a
ple method for investigating short-term regular increase of about 40 ms per item.
memory search. In essence, a small set of Furthermore, the functions for positive and
items (typically one to six in number) known negative functions were parallel. It is obvi-
as the memory set is presented and entered ously tempting to apply Sternberg's theory
into short-term memory. Very shortly there- to these data.
after a single input item (or probe) is pre- A closer examination of studies of short-
sented, and the subject reacts as rapidly as term search and visual search indicates that
possible with one response if the input item an attention-demanding, serial comparison
is in the memory set (a positive response) process cannot possibly account for all of
and with a different response if it is not (a the data. In particular, if the memory set re-
negative response). mains unchanged throughout extended
The typical finding is that the time taken practice in the Sternberg paradigm, the
to respond is a linear function of memory function relating number of memory-set
set size, with reaction time being increased items to reaction time increases much more
by about 40 ms for each additional item in slowly than 40 ms per item and is curvilinear
the memory set. In addition, the functions rather than linear. Similar results have been
based on positive responses and on negative observed in studies of visual search.
responses are usually parallel (see Fig. 2.4). Shiffrin and Schneider argued that these
Sternberg explained these findings by pro- and other findings suggest a distinction be-
posing that the input item was compared in tween two basic kinds of processing: con-

u 650
w
If) NEGATIVE TRIALS
~ 600 ,,0 POSITIVE TRIALS
~ ",,""

>- 550
u ,P"
z (f' "
W
« 500
t- ",,'

...J "P'"
w ,/
z 450
If)
1:1'
~ 0""'..., '
...

w 400
If)

a:
z
<{
w
~
1 2 3 4 5 6
NUMBER OF ITEMS IN MEMORY SET

Fig. 2.4. The effects of memory-set size on response speed in the Sternberg paradigm (illustrative data)
20 Theories of Attention
1200
U
W 1100 P NEGATIVE
1/1 / TRIALS
:I 1000
Z ,//// VARIED
900 MAPPING
w
:I 800 / ~ POSITIVE
i= / ",'-- TRIALS

)
J ",'"
z 700 ,.....0 tfI'-'
0
i= 600 ...... """,,",,"
'" --0 NEGATIVE
U 0--------0- ......¥ TRIALS
« o 0 .,.> ... 0--=- CONSISTENT
500 MAPPING

-------
W • POSITIVE
0: • _-------...
400 ... - - - - - - - - - - TRIALS

I I I I
MEM.SET= 1 MEM.SET.1 MEM.SET.4 MEM.SET.4
0ISP.SET.1 DISP.SET.4 DISP.SET.1 DISP.SET.4

Fig. 2.S. The effects of memory-set size, display- speed of response in a detection task. (Schneider
set size and consistent versus varied mapping on and Shiffrin 1977)

trolled and automatic. Controlled processes items (as in visual search tasks), and they
are of limited capacity and require atten- were asked to decide whether anyone of the
tion; their constraints are those of short- visual display items was the same as anyone
term memory (such as the limited compari- of the memory set items. The task could thus
son rate and the limited amount of informa- be made extremely easy (one item to mem-
tion that can be stored without loss), and orize and one item in the visual display) or
they can be used flexibly in changing cir- quite difficult (four items in memory and
cumstances. The typical pattern of findings four items in the display).
in studies of visual search and short-term Some subjects were run urider consistent
memory search is attributable to controlled mapping conditions, in which only numbers
processes making serial comparisons at a were used as items in the memory set and
rate of 40 ms per comparison. only consonants were used as distractor
Automatic processes, on the other hand, items in the visual display (or vice versa).
are not hindered by the capacity limitations Consider someone who was given only
of short-term memory and do not require at- numbers as members of each memory set. If
tention. They require considerable training a number was seen in the visual display, it
to develop and are extremely difficult to had to be a member of the current memory
modify once they have been learned. The set. In other words, the simple strategy of
finding from visual and memory search looking for any member of the number cate-
studies that extended practice can greatly gory could be used, with a 'yes' response be-
reduce the effects on reaction time of increa- ing appropriate if one was discovered.
sing the number of items can be attributed to Other subjects received varied mapping
the gradual development of automatic pro- conditions in which numbers and conso-
cesses. nants were used both as items in the memo-
Shiffrin and Schneider (1977) and ry set and as distractors .
Schneider and Shiffrin (1977) attempted to .What do you think the results of this ex-
clarify the distinction between these two periment looked like? While you may have
kinds of processes in a task in which sub- guessed that the consistent mapping condi-
jects were given one, two or four items as tion would produce better performance
members of the memory set (as in Stern- than the varied mapping condition, you may
berg's paradigm). They were then shown be surprised at the size of the difference (see
visual displays containing one, two or four Fig. 2.5). Under varied mapping conditions,
Shiffrin and Schneider: Controlled and Automatic Processes 21

there were large effects of both memory-set formance. Are there any disadvantages as-
size and display-set size, and the effects sociated with the use of automatic pro-
were approximately twice as large on nega- cesses? Theoretically, since they are inflexi-
tive trials (i.e. when there was no match be- ble and difficult to modify, they should be a
tween any of the memory items and any of handicap when circumstances change. In
the items in the visual display). According to the study just described, the 2100 trials of
Shiffrin and Schneider, subjects used a con- consistent mapping were followed by an-
trolled search process in these conditions. other 2400 trials of consistent mapping but
with everything reversed. In other words, if
Why does controlled search take longer
the items in the memory set had previously
when there are more items to be considered?
been drawn from the first half of the alpha-
The answer is that each item in the visual
bet, they were subsequently always taken
display needs to be compared against each
from the second half of the alphabet, and
item in the memory set in the search for a
vice versa.
match, and these comparisons can only be
After this reversal, detection accuracy fell
made serially and at a limited rate.
to about 30%, a level well below that seen at
The picture is completely different for the the start of training when the subjects were
consistent mapping conditions, in which completely unpractised. Indeed, it took al-
there was almost no effect of either memory most 1000 trials under reversal conditions
set size or visual display size on reaction before performance recovered to its initial
time. Performance in these conditions was level! Automatic processes were continuing
attributed to the use of automatic processes to detect items in the visual display that had
operating independently and in parallel. been targets in the first part of the experi-
These automatic processes have developed ment, but were no longer, and this interfered
as a result of a lifetime's practice in distin- with the performance of the task.
guishing between letters and numbers. A similar interfering effect was noticed
If automatic processes occur as a result of informally by subjects who were exposed to
practice, it should be possible to demon- hundreds of trials of consistent mapping
strate in a laboratory setting the way in with the memory-set items always being
which they develop. Shiffrin and Schneider drawn from a limited number of conso-
used consistent mapping, with the conso- nants. They reported that these memory-set
nants B to L forming one set and the conso- items seemed to 'jump out' from the page
nants Q to Z forming the other set. Items during normal reading. One subject found
from only one set were used in the memory this effect so distracting that he would not
set, and all of the distractors in the visual attempt to read for an hour or more after an
display came from the other set. Several vis- experimental session, which is certainly a
ual displays were presented very rapidly, novel excuse for not studying.
and the main measure of performance was In contrast, the effects of altering the con-
the accuracy of detection. ditions for subjects exposed to varied map-
After 2100 trials, detection accuracy had ping were relatively small, indicating that
climbed from an initial 55 % to over 80 %, controlled processes can be modified much
and performance seemed to be determined more readily than automatic processes. The
by automatic processes. It thus appears that same conclusion was suggested by further
practice is an extremely important factor in experiments in which consistent or varied
producing automaticity and that really sub- mapping was followed by a modification in
stantial amounts of training may be re- which subjects were told to look for targets
quired for automatic processing to develop in only one part of the visual display and to
fully. ignore targets in the remaining part of the
We have seen that automatic processes display. It was much harder for subjects to
can lead to greatly enhanced levels of per- ignore part of the visual display when they
22 Theories of Attention

had developed automatic search processes mitations on the input, the complete analy-
(consistent mapping conditions) than when sis of all inputs is followed by almost imme-
they had been using controlled search pro- diate forgetting unless the relevant informa-
cesses (varied mapping conditions). tion receives attention.
The main findings of Shiffrin and The notion that all stimuli are fully ana-
Schneider will now be summarized. In do- lysed in an automatic fashion is similar to
ing so, it will be useful to distinguish be- theoretical ideas put forward by Deutsch
tween divided attention situations, in which and Deutsch (1963). While it is difficult to
attention must be paid to all the stimuli in know how we can distinguish empirically
the visual display, and focussed attention si- between full analysis followed immediately
tuations, in which attention is to be paid to by forgetting, on the one hand, and a total
some stimuli but not to others. In essence, lack of analysis, on the other, the evidence
subjects can divide attention among several reviewed in this chapter broadly supports
items without impairing performance when the complete analysis position. However,
automatic processing is utilized but cannot since there is an almost infinite number of
divide attention without worsening per- potential ways of encoding a complex sti-
formance when controlled search is used. mulus array, it seems doubtful that the
In contrast, there is a substantial decre- whole range of possible encodings could all
ment in performance on a focussed-atten- be processed automatically. If 'full analysis'
tion task from automatic responses to to-be- simply means that all stimuli contact their
ignored items but only a slight decrement representations in long-term memory, then
from controlled processing of such items, that is a defensible position, except perhaps
since the subjects' control is usually suffi- for stimuli which have only rarely been en-
cient to prevent unwanted processing. In a countered. If it means more than that, then
nutshell, automatic processes function rap- the available data cannot be taken to indi-
idly and in parallel but suffer from inflexi- cate the existence of an automatic full analy-
bility; controlled processes are flexible and sis in that broader sense.
versatile but operate relatively slowly and in While the approach adopted by Shiffrin
a serial fashion. and Schneider has much to recommend it,
On the basis of their various findings, they made some dubious theoretical as-
Shiffrin and Schneider proposed a general sumptions. For example, they claimed that
theory of memory and attention. The long- control processes are "limited-capacity pro-
term store was regarded as an elaborate col- cesses requiring attention. . . these limita-
lection of interrelated information, most of tions prevent multiple control processes
which is passive at any given moment. The from occurring simultaneously" (p. 160),
short-term store consists of that subset of in- and they regarded rote rehearsal, serial
formation in the long-term store which is search, various decisions and strategies and
currently active. In contrast to the multi- long-term memory search as examples of
store approach (e.g. Atkinson and Shiffrin, control processes. Despite their claim, there
1968), which treated the short- and long- is in fact strong evidence that rote rehearsal
term memory stores as separate, a flow chart can occur concurrently with other control
of Shiffrin and Schneider's theory would processes. Hitch and Baddeley (1976) found
show the short-term store as a small box that the speed and accuracy with which ver-
contained completely within the larger box bal reasoning problems were solved were
of the long-term store. hardly affected by concurrent overt rehear-
Automatic processes are assumed to op- sal of the word 'the' or by rehearsal of the se-
erate at input, with all features of incoming quence 'one-two-three-four-five-six'. In
stimuli being analysed and represented other studies, it has been found that rote re-
briefly as active elements within the long- hearsal has essentially no effect on normal
term store. While there are no attentionalli- silent reading comprehension.
Shiffrin and Schneider: Controlled and Automatic Processes 23

Consideration of the operation of control responses) were required to eliminate the


processes leads naturally to the issue of the difference between four-choice and two-
mechanism that controls the control pro- choice reaction times (Le. to produce a slope
cesses. Shiffrin and Schneider (1977) pro- of zero).
vided their answer in a brief discussion of It is interesting to note that Shiffrin and
the ways in which control processes are Schneider themselves did not in practice ad-
used to retrieve information from the long- here too rigidly to their own criterion. They
term store: "On each cycle, (a) the subject argued that their consistent mapping condi-
generates probe information and places it in tions produced automaticity, and yet in vir-
STS ... (c) the subject searches the STS set; tually every experiment they failed to de-
(d) the subject decides whether the approp- monstrate a slope of zero as a consequence
riate information has been found" (p. 159). of consistent mapping! They typical finding
The notion that it is 'the subject' who con- was that increases in memory-set size and
trols his or her own control processes lacks visual display size led to increases of ap-
something in terms of scientific respectabili- proximately 15 % in reaction time under
ty and seems to hark back to very primitive consistent mapping conditions.
homunculus theories. Their use of the zero-slope technique is
Shiffrin and Schneider were perhaps based on the assumption that there is only
more successful in elucidating some of the one capacity (Le. controlled attention) limit-
ways in which automatic processes can de- ing performance. On this assumption, a
velop. They showed convincingly that ex- non-zero slope indicates that controlled at-
tended practice under conditions in which tentional processing is being used. How-
any given stimulus is always associated with ever, it is entirely possible that there are
the same response can lead to automaticity. other capacity limitations, in which case a
However, it is not altogether clear from their non-zero slope would not necessarily mean
studies exactly what aspects or features of that attention was being paid to the task in
the stimuli were being detected, i.e. they did question.
not specify the nature of the information Some important experimental evidence
which comes to be processed automatically relating to these issues was obtained by Lo-
as a result of practice. This issue is of central gan (1979). Subjects performed a multiple-
interest to the theory of attention recently choice reaction time task with two, four, or
put forward by Treisman and Gelade eight stimulus-response alternatives. The
(1980), which is dicussed later in this chap- stimulus-response mappings either re-
ter. mained unchanged from day to day (con-
There are some problems with the method sistent mapping) or were altered every ses-
by which Shiffrin and Schneider decided sion (varied mapping). This task was either
whether processing was automatic or con- performed by itself or during the retention
trolled. In essence, they identified automati- interval of a memory task making consider-
city with a slope of zero in the function relat- able demands on attentional capacity (or-
ing reaction time to the memory-set size or dered recall of eight digits). The memory
to the visual display size, and they identified task would be expected to impair perform-
the degree of involvement of conscious at- ance on the multiple-choice task to the ex-
tention with the amount by which the slope tent that the multiple-choice task demands
was greater than zero. This is a rather strin- attention. Furthermore, if the attentional de-
gent criterion, and very prolonged practIce mands of the multiple-choice task vary di-
is sometimes needed to achieve a slope of rectly with the number of stimulus-response
zero. In a multiple-choice task, Mowbray alternatives, then there should be an interac-
and Rhoades (1959) found that several hun- tion, with the detrimental effect of memory
dred sessions of practice with same sti- load being greatest with eight alternatives
mUlus-response mappings (a total of 45 000 and least with two alternatives. As can be
24 Theories of Attention

CONSISTENT MAPPING
1800 DAY 1 DAY 6
"D

Y
1500 ////

1200
u 900
w
c:f"" ....
~
\J1 600
CY'''
~ 300
z
l' l' I I
2 4 6 2 4 6
w
NUMBER OF S - R ALTERNATIVES
~

I- VARIED MAPPING
1800 DAY 1 DAY 6
Z
1500 ",0
0 ",'
",/J
- , ",'" ","

::/
1200

V
","
I-
"
u 900
« 600 cr"
w
Cl:: 300
Fig. 2.6. Choice reaction time as a
l' I I I I I
1 I
function of the number of stimulus-
response alternatives, concurrent
2 4 8 2 4 8
memory load, amount of practice

-
NUMBER OF S -R ALTERNATIVES
and consistent versus varied map-
0---0 8 DIGIT LOAD NO DIGIT LOAD ping. (Logan 1979)

seen in Fig. 2.6, these predictions were con- At the most mundane level, the implica-
firmed on the 1st day of practice for both tion is that different techniques for measur-
consistent and varied mapping conditions. ing automaticity cannot be considered to be
As would be predicted by Shiffrin and equivalent. More interestingly, Logan may
Schneider, the effects of extended practice have shown that a set of tasks can be per-
on the performance of those subjects receiv- formed with minimal reliance on attentional
ing varied mappings were slight, presu- capacity (as indexed by memory load inter-
mably because changing the stimulus-re- ference) without producing a slope of zero.
sponse mappings precludes the develop- If so, then the dichotomy of controlled and
ment of automatic and attention-free pro- automatic processes may represent a sub-
cessing. In contrast, several days of practice stantial over-simplification.
with a consistent stimulus-response map-
ping led to a considerable improvement in
reaction time, and there was a marked re-
duction in the effects of the memory load 2.4 Treisman and Gelade: Features and
(see Fig. 2.6). Objects
Do these data indicate the development
of automaticity? According to Logan, the
fact that the attention-demanding memory ·Treisman and Gelade's (1980) theory of at-
task has no more effect on the eight-alterna- tention resembles that of Posner and Sny-
tive condition than on the two-alternative der (l975a, b) and Shiffrin and Schneider
condition after 6 days of practice indicates (1977) in that it emphasizes the conceptual
automaticity. In terms ofShiffrin and Schnei- distinction between attention-demanding
der's zero-slope criterion, of course, auto- and automatic processes. They contended
maticity has by no means been demonstrated. that there are very important differences be-
Treisman and Gelade: Features and Objects 25

tween the ways in which features and ob- an'S' (either target could be presented on
jects are perceived, using the term 'feature' any trial). The basic assumption was that fo-
to refer to a particular value on a dimension cal attention would be necessary to detect
which is analysed by some functionally in- the target in the former condition (because
dependent perceptual sub-system. For ex- the target was defined by a combination of
ample, colour is a perceptual dimension, features) but would not be required in the
and blue is a feature on that dimension. latter condition. While the two task condi-
According to Treisman and Gelade's fea- tions may appear to be of equivalent diffi-
ture integration theory of attention, features culty (and reaction time was the same in
are registered automatically and in parallel both conditions with a one-item display),
within the visual field, whereas objects com- the overall pattern of results was very differ-
prising conjunctions of features are identi- ent in the two conditions (see Fig. 2.7).
fied as the result of a serial process which in- The data for the condition in which the
volves focussed attention. Why is focussed target was defined by a conjunction of fea-
attention necessary? Treisman and Gelade tures are most straightforwardly explained
argued that attention ensures that the fea- by assuming that focal attention was in-
tures of an object are appropriately com- volved and that a self-terminating serial
bined in perception; what happens is that search was employed. A self-terminating
all of the features simultaneously present search means that the search ends as soon as
during focal attention are integrated to form a target is detected; on average, this will re-
a perceptual object. In other words, focal at- quire searching through half of the items
tention provides the 'glue' which combines when a target is present, as against all of the
features into unitary objects. items when there is no target. It thus follows
Perhaps the strongest support for the the- that the negative slope should be approxi-
ory was obtained in an experiment in which mately twice as steep as the positive slope,
Treisman and Gelade asked subjects to and this was what happened.
search for a specified target in a visual dis- In contrast, there was very little effect of
play of between I and 30 items; a target was display size on reaction times on positive tri-
presented on half of the trials. In one condi- als when the targets were defined by single
tion, the target was a green letter 'T'; in the features. These findings point to a parallel
other condition, it was either a blue letter or search that operates automatically. How-

2400 : CONJUNCTIVE NEGATIVE

U
W
TARGETS
","
""A TRIALS

If) 2000 - - - - - : SINGLE FEATURE ",'"


~
TARGETS ,,'"
",'"
z 1600 ",'"
/h POSITIVE
//
-_.._..
w .
TRIALS

....
/
~ 1200 // NEGATIVE

........
~ TRIALS
/
//
Z

-
0 800 tI' --,'
l-
U
v'
POSITIVE
<f
w 400 TRIALS
0:

5 15 30
DISPLAY SIZE
Fig. 2.7. Performance speed on a detection task as single·feature) and display size. (Treisman and
a function of target definition (conjunctive versus Gelade 1980)
26 Theories of Attention

ever, the existence of a substantial effect of specified letters, provided that they received
display size on speed of responding on ne- protracted practice and that targets and dis-
gative trials suggests that there is some serial tractors never interchanged their roles.
searching of the display if no target is pres- Treisman and Gelade offer a more detailed
ent. explanation of the conditions under which
Shiffrin and Schneider (1977) have parallel search can occur: the prime necessi-
stressed the role played by practice in pro- ty is that the targets can be discriminated
ducing the switch from attentionally de- from non-targets on the basis of a single fea-
manding to automatic processing. In con- ture. Practice might achieve this state of af-
trast, Treisman and Gelade found that fairly fairs in at least two different ways: (I) each
prolonged practice with the target defined target is processed as a unit, i.e. as a single
by a combination of features did not pro- functional feature or (2) subjects learn to
duce evidence for automatic processing in identify a single feature that differentiates a
the form of parallel search. target from all of the distractors (e.g. with
The feature integration theory of atten- 'N' as the target and 'HOTU' as distractors,
tion leads to a further rather striking predic- the target is the only letter with a diagonal
tion. If focussed attention to particular ob- line in it).
jects is prevented for any reason, then the A final interesting finding obtained by
features of different objects are processed Treisman and Gelade concerned the ability
but remain 'unglued'. This opens up the pos- to locate correctly perceived targets. In
sibility of 'illusory conjunctions', in which a those conditions in which focal attention
feature from one object is erroneously com- was supposedly utilized, it transpired that
bined with a feature from a different object approximately accurate information about
to form a perceptual object. Treisman (1977) target location was both necessary and suffi-
found that in the absence of focussed atten- cient for target identification. In contrast,
tion, subjects often reported these illusory when processing was automatic and in par-
conjunctions, combining the shape of one allel, the target was identified correctly on
object with the colour of a second. Of approximately 40 % of the trials on which
course, such illusions are much less likely to the location was totally misjudged. It seems
occur when we possess relevant knowledge intuitively reasonable that one of the advan-
about objects: we rarely perceive green dogs tages offocussed attention is that it provides
or black and white traffic lights! accurate information about the spatialloca-
Illusory conjunctions can also occur tions of any objects to which attention is di-
when the subject is attempting to detect a rected.
target letter among distractor letters con- Treisman and Gelade have shown be-
taining features which can combine to form yond peradventure that relatively slow, seri-
the target letter. For example, if the target al, attentional processes can fulfil the vital
letter is 'R', then distractor letters 'P' and 'Q' function of combining feature information
can combine to form the target letter if the to form integrated percepts. However, one
diagonal of the 'Q' links up with the 'P'. Ac- of the problems with their theory is whether
cording to the theory, the way to avoid such unattended stimuli are invariably processed
illusory conjunctions is to resort to serial only at the feature level. There is accumulat-
processing involving focal attention. As ex- ing evidence (e.g. Lewis 1970, Treisman et
pected, the search through the visual display al. 1974) that the meaning of unattended
was serial when illusory conjunctions words is registered on occasion without
among distractor letters could occur, but it reaching conscious awareness. This is prob-
was largely parallel otherwise. lematical for the theory because it seems im-
These results can be compared to those of probable that meaning is represented as a
Shiffrin and Schneider (1977), who found single feature. In other words, it is possible
that subjects could search in parallel for that there can be some integration of fea-
Summary 27

tures even for unattended stimuli; a resolu- More recent theories of attention, such as
tion of this issue awaits a more precise con- those of Posner and Snyder (1975a, b), Shif-
ceptualization of the notion of a 'feature' at frin and Schneider (1977), and Treisman
the semantic level. and Gelade (1980) have emphasized the
The feature integration theory of atten- conceptual distinction between attention-
tion is obviously somewhat circumscribed demanding and automatic processes. While
in a number of ways. While it is a theory of this distinction has been proposed by sever-
attention, it is concerned exclusively with al theorists over the years, it is only compar-
the role played by attention in visual percep- atively recently that convincing empirical
tion. None of the other possible functions of support for such a distinction has been ob-
attention is considered by Treisman and Ge- tained. It is to the credit of all these investi-
lade. gators that they have demonstrated the exis-
tence of important qualitative differences
between attention-demanding and automat-
2.5 Summary ic processes. It is as a result of such research
that a more precise concept of 'attention' can
Six major theories of attention have been be attained. However, there is a pressing
considered in this chapter. The early the- need for greater differentiation among dif-
ories of attention (Le. those of Broadbent ferent kinds of automatic processes, since
1958, Deutsch and Deutsch 1963, and Treis- otherwise automatic processes tend to be
man 1964) are often thought to represent defined solely in terms of a lack of attention-
very different ways of regarding attentional al involvement. It may be profitable to dis-
phenomena. In fact, all three theories as- tinguish between automatic processes hav-
sumed that there was a bottleneck some- ing direct effects on attention and/or overt
where in the various stages of processing responding and those that do not. Only the
which tended to preclude successful concur- former type of automatic process should
rent performance; the dissimilarities among have the potential ability to disrupt per-
these theories revolved around the putative formance when environmental conditions
locus of the bottleneck. change.
3 Theories of Processing Resources

We saw in the previous chapter that one of regard attention and short-term retention as
the major recent developments in the area of being closely related. A comparatively re-
attention has been the distinction between cent example is the attempt by Baddeley
attention-demanding processes and auto- and Hitch (1974) to integrate thinking on
matic processes that do not require atten- problems of attention and short-term mem-
tion. There have been at least two other sign- ory. Their starting point was the concept of
ificant shifts in thinking. One is that the ori- the short-term store, which was an integral
ginal notion of attention as a bottleneck has part of several multi-store theories of memo-
been replaced by a conceptualization in ry put forward during the 1960s (e.g. Atkin-
which attention is regarded as a limited son and Shiffrin 1968, Waugh and Norman
power supply. The basic idea is that atten- 1965).
tion represents a general I urpose limited ca- In essence, Baddeley and Hitch (1974)
pacity that can be flexibl J allocated in many suggested replacing the concept of 'short-
different ways in response to task demands. term store' with that of 'working memory';
The second shift in the status quo anteis that one of the main differences is that the no-
attention is increasingly regarded as consti- tion of working memory is broader than that
tuting only one out of a number of different of short-term store and is relevant to a much
processing resources. greater variety of situations. Working mem-
The approaches dealt with in this chapter ory is a processing mechanism comprising
exemplify contemporary thinking. As will the two separable components of a modali-
become apparent, there are substantial the- ty-free central processor of limited capacity
oretical disagreements despite a measure of and an articulatory loop. The central pro-
consensus on the broad framework. Some cessor is similar to, or even the same as, an
theorists (e.g. Baddeley and Hitch 1974) attentional processing mechanism and is al-
choose to regard attentional mechanisms as most invariably used in a wide range of cog-
being intimately linked to the processes in- nitve tasks. In contrast, use of the articulato-
volved in short-term storage, whereas others ry loop is largely optional; its function is
(e.g. Navon and Gopher 1979) believe that it primarily to permit transient storage of a li-
is more profitable to postulate the existence mited amount of information in a phonemic
of a number of relatively specific processing code at relatively low 'cost' to the process-
resources. At the end of this chapter, an at- ing system.
tempt is made to identify the significant ad- The potential advantages of this concept-
vances in our understanding achieved by ualization over that of the multi-store ap-
the various theorists discussed. proach, with its emphasis on the distinction
between the short-term and long-term
3.1 Baddeley and Hitch: stores, can be seen in terms of the memory
Working Memory deficits exhibited by the patient K. F. He
had a digit span of only two items but
At least from the time of James (1890), it showed no apparent deficit in either com-
has been popular among psychologists to prehension or long-term learning. This is
Baddeley and Hitch: Working Memory 29

rather mysterious from the perspective of This problem was obviated by Hitch and
multi-store models, because they claimed Baddeley (1976), who utilized a concurrent
that long-term memories could only be laid load technique in which a random sequence
down as a result of extensive processing in of six digits was repeated out loud by the
the short-term, store. If the short-term store subject at high speed during the perform-
were impaired (as it appeared to be in this ance of a verbal reasoning task. This digit
case), normal long-term learning ought to be task produced a substantial detrimental ef-
impossible. fect on the task of verbal reasoning, in con-
Baddeley and Hitch (1974) suggested that trast to the minimal effect of a digit task in-
K. F. was suffering from a deficient articula- volving only the articulatory loop (e.g. cycli-
tory loop, but that his central processor was cal repetition of 'one-two-three-four-five-
intact. Since long-term learning and com- six'). These data suggest that the concurrent
prehension depend primarily on the central load of six digits impaired performance be-
processor, the mystery is solved. They justi- cause of the involvement of the central pro-
fied their theoretical formulation in a series cessor rather than simply due to use of the
of experiments using a pre-loading para- articulatory loop.
digm: subjects were presented ('loaded') Have these findings definitely shown that
with up to six random digits, which were fol- the central processor is a modality-free sys-
lowed by a short interpolated verbal task tem possessing the characteristics of a gen-
and then serial recall of the digit string. The eral attentional capacity? Not really, be-
verbal tasks used included reasoning, com- cause all of the tasks we have considered
prehension and free recall learning. have involved verbal processing, so that
As anticipated, all of the interpolated ver- the central processor might merely be a ver-
bal tasks were performed more poorly when bal coding system. However, other data sug-
as many as six digits had to be remembered. gest that the functions of the central proces-
A more interesting finding from the theoret- sor may be fairly general. For example, Wat-
ical point of view was that the various verbal kins et al. (1973) found that memory for ver-
tasks were performed equally well whether bal information in short-term store was se-
three digits or no digits (control condition) verely disrupted by performing a visual
had to be retained. Why did remembering tracking task on the pursuit rotor during the
three digits not interfere with the verbal retention interval.
tasks? According to Baddeley and Hitch In similar fashion, Reitman (1974) found
(1974), performance of the verbal tasks de- that verbal short-term memory was reduced
pended mainly on the central processor, when subjects detected pure tones in a back-
whereas three digits were within the capaci- ground of white noise during the retention
ty of the articulatory loop. Since the central interval. The fact that short-term memory is
processor was only substantially involved in interfered with by completely non-verbal
the digit-string task when more than three tasks, although less than it is by verbal tasks,
digits had to be retained, it was only with suggests that verbal short-term memory re-
such digit strings that the two tasks could lies on a specific capacity for verbal coding
not be combined satisfactorily. (the articulatory loop) as well as a general
Baddeley and Hitch (1974) concluded attentional capacity (central processor).
that the central processor and the articulato- The characteristics of the articulatory
ry loop could be used concurrently and in loop were explored by Baddeley et al.
parallel. It has to be pointed out, however, (1975). They found that word span (i.e. the
that there is a fairly serious methodological number of words that can be recalled imme-
problem with the pre-load technique, since diately in their correct order) was inversely
we do not know whether the subjects are ac- related to the number of syllables per word;
tively processing the digits at all during the more specifically, they discovered that sub-
performance of the interpolated verbal task. jects recalled approximately as many words
30 Theories of Processing Resources

as they could read in 2 s. Obviously, fewer during the middle part of the problem.
multi-syllable than one-syllable words can When a short-term memory task was intro-
be read in that time, and the word span was duced half way through the presentation of
correspondingly less. These findings ap- problems of the latter type it was more dis-
peared to be due to the articulatory loop, be- ruptive of both subtraction accuracy and
cause the word length effect disappeared short-term memory than when it was placed
when articulatory suppression (i.e. rapid in the middle of problems of the former
repetition of a single sound by the subject) type. The reason for this difference is that
accompanied word presentation. Thus the problems of the latter type place greater de-
articulatory loop is a time-based system op- mands on working memory and thus com-
erating at the phonemic level; it is plausible pete more strongly with the short-term
to assume that rehearsal is its primary func- memory task for the limited resources of
tion. working memory.
The most interesting implication of the The importance of transient demands on
approach proposed by Baddeley and Hitch memory during the solving of addition
(1974) is that working memory is a resource problems was shown by Hitch (1978). One
that plays a major role in a great diversity of approach which he used was to present an
situations. Mental tasks of all kinds appear addition problem auditorily (e.g. 348 + 27)
to make temporary demands upon working and to ask the subjects to work through the
memory. Such demands arise whenever in- problem starting with the units column, then
formation becomes available at one point in moving on to the tens column, and finally
the solution to a problem and must be main- on to the hundreds column. The demands
tained for use at a subsequent point. These on memory were minimized when the solu-
considerations suggest the intriguing pos- tion was written down in the order units,
sibility that many of our intellectuallimita- tens and hundreds, because partial results
tions may actually be memorial limitations. could be written down as they were calculat-
A very simple problem-solving task was ed, whereas they were maximized when the
used by Wanner and Shiner (1976) involving normal order (hundreds, tens and units) was
auditorily presented subtraction problems used. As can be seen in Fig. 3.1, there were
of the following type: (5 -4) -1. Nearly almost 50% more errors when the normal
everyone reports remembering only one di- order was used with its greater demands on
git at each successive stage of problem solu- working memory.
tion (first 5, then 1 and then 0). There were Hitch (1978) obtained even more direct
other subtraction problems that were super- evidence of the role played by working
ficially similar, such as 5 - (4 - 1), but memory when he combined auditory pres-
which necessitate remembering two digits entation of addition problems with visual

25

I/)
cr 20
0 NORMAL
cr
cr OUTPUT
w 15 "A,, ORDER
I-
z "
w
u
10 " " ",
cr
w
!l. 5
if ""''0 REVERSE
OUTPUT
ORDER Fig. 3.1. Error rate on a addition
task as a function of the order in
'HUNDREDS' 'TENS' 'UNITS' which the answer is given. (Hitch
FIGURE FIGURE FIGURE 1978)
Baddeley and Hitch: Working Memory 31

aids varying between the entire problem and two sub-systems of working memory: (1) a
nothing. The error rate increased from only relatively passive input register which holds
3 % with complete visual information to information about recent verbal inputs and
22 % with no visual information. It may well resembles Waugh and Norman's (1965) con-
be that the initial information and the re- cept of primary memory and (2) an output
sults of interim calculations are both held in system which holds information about po-
the articulatory loop, where they are subject tential verbal responses and which is used in
to some form of decay. Complete visual in- articulatory rehearsal. The latter corre-
formation greatly eases the storage problem sponds to the articulatory loop component
in the articulatory loop. The central process- of working memory postulated by Baddeley
or performs various functions, including se- and Hitch (1974).
lecting information from the articulatory Since Hitch (1980) retained the notion of
loop (e.g. 4 + 5), transforming it on the basis a limited-capacity central executive, the ma-
of information in the long-term store (e.g. jor alteration from the position adopted by
4 + 5 = 9) and putting the result back into Baddeley and Hitch (1974) is the postula-
the articulatory loop (e.g. 9). tion of a passive input register. This register
The available evidence is consistent with can be investigated in studies of free recall
the notion that working memory is one of in which the subject is presented with a list
the most useful processing resources. Un- of words and then has to recall as many as
like most attention theorists, Baddeley and possible in any order. It is usually found
Hitch (1974) have explicitly suggested that that the subject recalls well from the begin-
short-term storage and attention are so ning (the primacy effect) and the end (the
closely related that they should be consid- recency effect) of the list. If a short distract-
ered together at a theoretical level. Another ing task is interpolated between the end of
advantage of their approach is that working list presentation and recall, the recency ef-
memory is viewed mainly as a process; this fect is eliminated, but there is little effect on
contrasts with multi-store theories (e.g. At- recall from the rest of the list (Glanzer and
kinson and Shiffrin 1968), in which the Cunitz 1966). This result, which is illustrated
short-term store was conceptualized in in Fig. 3.2, suggests that most of the items are
structural terms as a box with arrows lead- in a stable long-term store but that those
ing to and away from it. from the end of the list are in a rather fra-
Hitch (1980) has recently proposed a gile short-term store which can hold approxi-
modification of some of the views expressed mately three items. Hitch (1978) argued that
by Baddeley and Hitch (1974). He argued the recency effect in free recall reflected the
that it was important to distinguish between workings of the input register.

BO
..J 70
..J
«
u 60 NO DELAY
w
a: 50
I-
z 40 10 - SEC
w p-~-o
U
a: 30 '~ INTERPOLATED
w TASK
11. 20

1" I I
5 10 15
SERIAL POSITION
Fig. 3.2. Free recall with no delay and with a 10 s interpolated task. (Glanzer and Cunitz 1966)
32 Theories of Processing Resources

There is some evidence to support the third trial. The repeated string was better
notion that the input register is passive and remembered than non-repeated strings, in-
thus does not depend on active processing dicating that there is some long-term storage
strategies (e.g. rehearsal). The recency effect of information in span studies. Subsequent
is not reduced when subjects are given a work has indicated that there is greatest
concurrent task (e.g. mental arithmetic) to long-term storage of the initial part of the
perform during the presentation of a free re- string, possibly because that is the part re-
call list, whereas recall from the rest of the ceiving the most rehearsal (Corballis 1969).
list is impaired. If memory span is based both on working
The distinction between the input register memory and on long-term storage, then
and the articulatory loop becomes clearer span measures provide an inexact estimate
when one considers language comprehen- of working memory capacity.
sion and production. On the one hand, the The general theoretical approach has
input register holds a parsed representation proved successful in explaining a number of
of the most recent words of spoken lan- puzzling findings. For example, Baddeley
guage, and paves the way for comprehen- and Hitch (1974) used an experimental par-
sion; on the other hand, the articulatory adigm in which a digit span task was per-
loop acts as an output buffer in the produc- formed simultaneously with the presenta-
tion of overt speech. It seems likely that the tion of a free recall list. As the number of
articulatory loop may be involved in Spoon- digits increased from one to six, free recall
erisms, which typically involve the acciden- of most of the list items was impaired, but
tal transposition of the initial letters of two the recency effect was left intact. If, as has
or more words (e.g. "He has just received a sometimes been assumed, memory span and
blushing crow"). the recency effect in free recall depend on
One of the greatest weaknesses of the the same limited-capacity, short-term store,
working memory model is that it is not clear then it is very surprising that there is no per-
that there is any good measure of its capaci- formance decrement when the two tasks are
ty. Memory span (i.e. the limit on the num- combined.
ber of items that can be recalled immediate- Hitch (1980) explained this apparently
ly in the correct order) undoubtedly de- anomalous finding by arguing that memory
pends in part on working memory, but it span depends primarily on the articulatory
seems to involve other factors as well. This loop whereas the recency effect depends on
is suggested by a closer examination of the the input register. The basic logical argu-
data of Baddeley and Hitch (1974). Even ment is that non-interference between two
when they used a near span memory load of concurrent activities indicates that they in-
six digits, subjects were still able to perform volve separate mechanisms; on this argu-
a variety of concurrent verbal tasks reason- ment, the postulation of four different com-
ably efficiently. If memory span really re- ponents of working memory (input register,
quired all of the resources of working mem- articulatory loop, short-term visuo-spatial
ory, a more precipitous fall in performance store and central executive) can perhaps be
of a concurrent verbal task might have been justified.
expected. However, there is still no strong evidence
More direct evidence on the issue of for a clear distinction between the two ma-
whether memory span is a good measure of jor components of working memory (i.e. the
transient storage capacity was obtained by articulatory loop and the central executive).
Hebb (1961), who discovered what has Matters would be improved if a double dis-
come to be known as the "Hebb effect". He sociation could be demonstrated, i.e. if
presented subjects with nine-digit strings for some tasks could be found that relied exclu-
immediate serial recall and surreptitiously sively on the central executive whereas
repeated precisely the same string on every other tasks only involved the articulatory
Norman and Bobrow: Data-Limited and Resource-Limited Processes 33

loop. In practice, most of the tasks that have task depends on the amount of resources ap-
been used actually involve both compo- plied to that task; more specifically, they
nents. For example, Hitch and Baddeley postulated that the function relating per-
(1976) argued that the verbal reasoning task formance to resource allocation was mono-
which they used in a number of experiments tonically non-decreasing.
primarily involved the central executive. According to Norman and Bobrow
However, speed of reasoning was slowed (1975), it is necessary to distinguish between
down when there was concurrent overt re- two possible reasons for a task being per-
hearsal of the word "the", which suggests formed relatively poorly. On the one hand,
some involvement of the articulatory loop. performance may be resource limited,
Baddeley and Hitch (1974) suggested that meaning that an increase in the amount of
rehearsal of random items would typically resources invested in the task would result
involve the articulatory loop but that the in an improvement in performance efficien-
central processor would only be used when cy. On the other hand, performance may be
there were several items requiring rehearsal. data limited, in which case the further allo-
However, there is no compelling evidence cation of processing resources would not af-
for a qualitative shift in processing of this fect performance. Data limitations can re-
kind. Furthermore, even if a second process- sult either from the poor quality of the pre-
ing resource is increasingly employed as the sented stimuli or the inadequacy of memori-
number of items requiring rehearsal is in- al information. In practice, task perform-
creased, it is not clear that a general purpose ance will typically be resource limited up to
central processor constitutes this second re- the point at which all of the possible pro-
source. cessing has been done, after which it will be
In general terms, Baddeley and Hitch data limited.
have clarified the functioning of the articu- This theoretical approach has most rele-
latory loop much more than that of the cen- vance to the prediction of performance
tral processor. They have argued that the when two or more tasks are being performed
central processor is of limited capacity, but at the same time. In general terms, different
they have not been able to assess that capac- tasks should not interfere with each other
ity; in addition, the central processor is alle- unless the total processing resources which
gedly "modality free" and used in numerous they require exceed the upper limit on avail-
processing operations, but the precise con- able resources. Furthermore, only tasks that
straints on its functioning are by no means are being performed within their resource-
clear. limited region are susceptible to interfer-
ence. In many situations, the main or high-
priority task will be data limited, whereas
3.2 Norman and Bobrow: Data-Limited the subsidiary task will be resource limited.
and Resource-Limited Processes In such cases, the interference effect will be
asymmetrical, with the main task interfering
with the subsidiary task, but not vice versa.
A theoretical viewpoint bearing some Norman and Bobrow (1975) argued that
similarity to that of Baddeley and Hitch the degree of practice which a skill receives
(1974), and which also regards general ca- has important implications for the alloca-
pacity limitations as important, was pro- tion of processing resources. The learning
posed by Norman and Bobrow (1975). In that results form practice means that there is
particular, they argued that resources are enhanced performance for a given resource
limited, using the term 'resources' to apply allocation and also that a given level of per-
to processing effort, the various kinds of formance requires fewer resources.
memory capacity and communication chan- The latter point can be illustrated with
nels. Performance of any complex cognitive reference to data obtained by LaBerge
34 Theories of Processing Resources

(1973) in a study involving the processing of it was possible to predict the degree of per-
perceptual figures. When the subjects were formance efficiency for each of two concur-
attending fully to the stimulus, they typical- rent tasks on the basis of the principle of
ly showed equivalent levels of performance complementarity, according to which an in-
on newly learned and well-learned patterns, crease in the use of resources in one task re-
although more resources were presumably sults in a commensurate decrease in the re-
needed to process the newly learned pat- sources available for the processing of the
terns. This difference in resource expendi- second task. One method of testing this
ture was revealed when subjects were ex- principle is by manipulating the allocation
posed to attentional distraction requiring of resources by using instructions to vary the
some of the available resources: under those relative importance to be attached to the
circumstances, performance on the newly performance of each of two tasks and then
learned patterns was more detrimentally af- using the data obtained to construct per-
fected than performance on the well- formance operating characteristics.
learned patterns. These performance operating characteris-
An even more striking demonstration of tics involve plotting the performance level
the way in which practice can reduce the de- of one task against the concurrent perform-
mand for resources was provided by Logan ance level of a second task (see Fig. 3.3). In
(1979). A multiple-choice reaction time task regions where one process is data limited,
was performed during the retention interval but the other is not, the function will be eith-
of a memory task making substantial de- er a vertical or a horizontal line. One of the
mands on attentional capacity. Initially, putative advantages of such performance
performance on the reaction time task was operating characteristics is that they permit
very adversely affected by the concurrent one to identify the extent to which perform-
memory task, indicating that the reaction ance is either data limited or resource limit-
time task required considerable processing ed at several points along the resource ex-
resources. However, after 6 days of practice penditure continuum.
on the same version of the reaction time A useful clarification of the circum-
task, the interfering effect of the memory stances under which a single performance
task was greatly reduced, and reaction times operating characteristic may appropriately
were much faster. In this case, practice pro- be constructed was offered by Norman and
duced improved performance combined Bobrow (1976). They pointed out that the
with a reduced allocation of resources. tasks performed by the subject must remain
Norman and Bobrow (1975) claimed that completely unchanged; all that is manipu-

HORIZONTAL LINE:
/ Only (1) is Data Limited
'-'-'-'-\,
'\. / " (1) and (2) are both
w ", Resource Limited
u
z "
~ "
1: ""
a:: ...................
~
a:: •... ·...i/VERTICAL LINE.

I
w . Only (2) is
Q.
Data Limited
Fig. 3.3. A hypothetical per-
i formance operating charac-
teristic for performance on
two tasks. (Norman and Bo-
PERFORMANCE (2)
brow 1975)
Johnston and Heinz: Multi-Mode Theory 35

lated should be the division of resources, 3.3 Johnston and Heinz: Multi-Mode
perhaps by means of instructions about the Theory
relative weighting to be placed on each task.
If there are obvious qualitative changes in
the demands of the tasks being performed
concurrently, it is not surprising that the re- While several theorists have argued that
sultant data cannot be accounted for by there are important capacity limitations as-
means of a single performance operating sociated with attention and information
characteristic. processing, there has been much controver-
sy concerning the particular stage of pro-
The cornerstone of the Norman and
cessing at which the major capacity limita-
Bobrow approach is the principle of com-
tion or bottleneck is postulated to occur. In
plementarity, according to which all avail-
the most general terms, theories of attention
able resources can be allocated in a flexible
have typically argued for the existence of
way to two concurrent tasks. However, the
three stages of perceptual processing.
system probably does not work as efficiently
Stage I constructs sensory representations
as is implied by this principle. It is likely
of the inputs to the system, Stage 2 con-
that there are many occasions on which the
structs semantic representations of these
subject has unused resources which he does
sensory representations and Stage 3 admits
not know how to utilize to improve the per-
the products of the earlier stages into con-
formance of either of two concurrent tasks.
sciousness.
It may also be an over-simplification to sug-
Selective attention, in the sense of the dif-
gest that the total available resources remain
ferentiation between targets and non-tar-
constant under different instructional con-
gets, could conceivably occur at any or all of
ditions; in this connection, Kahneman
the three stages. Over the years, most of the
(1973) made a strong case that the total
various possibilities have been favoured by
available processing capacity changes as a
one or more theorists. At the two extremes,
function of task demands.
Broadbent (1958) argued that selection oc-
There is a fundamental problem at the curred only at Stage 1, whereas Deutsch and
heart of the theoretical analysis put forward Deutsch (1963) claimed that selection oc-
by Norman and Bobrow (1975). On the one curred only at Stage 3. Various intermediate
hand, they identified several different re- positions have been adopted by other theo-
sources, including processing effort, memo- rists; for example, Broadbent (1971) and
ry and communication channels. On the Treisman (1964) argued that selection could
other hand, their principle of complemen- occur either at Stage I or Stage 2, and Nor-
tarity is based on the assumption that two man (1968) proposed that Stages 2 and 3
concurrent tasks compete for a single gen- were the ones involved in selection.
eral pool of resources. It follows that when A major inadequacy of some of the earlier
performance on one of two concurrent tasks theories is that they assumed that the atten-
is unaffected by varying performance on the tional system could only operate in a single,
other task, performance of the first task is invariant way. The assumption that the at-
said to be data limited. It would be equally tentional system is highly flexible in its
persuasive to argue that the two tasks are mode of operation seems more realistic and
competing for different specific resources. suggests a re-appraisal of the issue of the
Norman and Bobrow's failure to indicate stage or stages of processing at which selec-
exactly how we can distinguish between tion occurs. Theorists such as Kahneman
competition for a general pool of resources (1973) and Johnston and Heinz (1978) have
and competition for a specific resource proposed that selective attention is possible
greatly limits the usefulness of their con- at all three stages. According to Johnston
ceptualization. and Heinz, selective attention consumes
36 Theories of Processing Resources

processing capacity, and the amount con- tion-time task. The rationale was that the
sumed increases as one moves through the more resources or expended processing ca-
various attentional stages or modes. While pacity that were required to perform the lis-
the system may on occasion delay selection tening task, the less residual capacity would·
until Stage 3, as Deutsch and Deutsch be available for monitoring the visual stimu-
(1963) claimed, it will not normally operate lus, and thus the slower the reaction time
in this way because of the very substantial would be.
cost in terms of processing capacity. Johnston and Heinz (1978) confirmed
Johnston and Heinz (1978) suggested that previous findings by obtaining better per-
the essential properties of Broadbent's formance on the listening task when only
(1958) theory could be simulated if subjects sensory cues were available than when only
attempted to differentiate between targets semantic cues were available. Of greater the-
and non-targets at Stage 1, whereas the es- oretical interest, they consistently found
sential properties of Deutsch and Deutsch's that subsidiary reaction time was shorter
(1963) theory could be simulated if selection when sensory cues were available than
occurred at Stage 3. What determines the when semantic cues were available. These
stage or mode of analysis chosen for selec- findings suggest that selective attention is
tion? According to Johnston (1978), the na- less effortful or demanding when two audi-
ture of the prevailing circumstances is im- tory inputs can be discriminated on the ba-
portant: "Late modes should become more sis of sensory or Stage 1 analysis than when
optimal as the importance of breadth of at- semantic or Stage 2 analysis is necessitated.
tention approaches or exceeds that of selec- Johnston and Heinz (1978) pointed out
tion efficacy; for example, during a war- that there was an alternative interpretation
time night patrol in an enemy-infested area, of their data. In line with the theoretical
or during a boring conversation at a cocktail views of Shiffrin and Schneider (1977), it
party" (p. 41). could be argued that selection always oc-
One of the major predictions flowing curs at Stage 3, but that it is more efficiently
from these theoretical assumptions is that guided by sensory cues than by semantic
selection based on semantic information cues. This suggested a further experiment to
(Stage 2 analysis) requires more processing Johnston and Heinz, using the same basic
capacity (i.e. the limited pool of energy, re- paradigm as before but varying the instruc-
sources or effort by means of which some tions. If subjects were told to extract as
cognitive operations are mobilized or main- much information as possible from both
tained) than selection based on sensory in- messages, this would lead them to use a late
formation (Stage 1 analysis). Johnston and mode or stage of processing in all condi-
Heinz (1978) asked subjects to pay attention tions.
to one of two concurrently presented binau- What effect should these new instructions
ral messages. The target message was distin- have on the difference in expended process-
guished from the non-target passage by a ing capacity between the sensory cue and se-
sensory cue (different voices) in one condi- mantic cue conditions? According to Shif-
tion, by a semantic or meaning cue in a sec- frin and Schneider (1977), both messages
ond condition and by both sensory and se- are always processed up to the Stage 3 level
mantic cues in a third condition. irrespective of instructions, so the previous-
In addition, there was a subsidiary task in ly obtained difference should still occur. Ac-
every condition, which required rapid reac- cording to Johnston and Heinz (1978), on
tion to a visual stimulus. The subjects were the other hand, the difference in expended
instructed to devote as much mental energy processing capacity should only occur when
as required in order to perform the listening an earlier mode or stage of selection is uti-
task as well as possible, with any surplus en- lized in the sensory cue than the semantic
ergy or resources being allocated to the reac- cue condition. Since the new instructions re-
Johnston and Heinz: Multi-Mode Theory 37

quired the subjects to use an equally late is necessary to discriminate between target
mode or stage of processing in both cue con- and non-target information. High sensory
ditions, the difference in expended process- discriminability permits a substantial reduc-
ing capacity should be eliminated. The data tion in the processing of non-targets, thus
were entirely consistent with this latter pre- reducing the consumption of processing ca-
diction and were thus apparently at var- pacity, the memorability of non-target infor-
iance with late selection models of the type mation and the extent to which non-targets
favoured by Deutsch and Deutsch (1963) are processed semantically.
and by Shiffrin and Schneider (1977). The notion that different levels or stages
Some of these important theoretical is- of analysis may make varying demands on
sues were considered further by Johnston processing capacity has been explored in a
and Heinz (1979). Subjects shadowed (i.e. very different paradigm by M. W. Eysenck
repeated aloud) target words that were pre- and M. C. Eysenck (1979a). Visually pre-
sented binaurally at the same time as non- sented words were preceded either by a
target words. Both sets of words were spo- physical (e.g. 'letter R 1') or a semantic (e.g.
ken in the same male voice in the low senso- 'solid 1') question and required a decision
ry discriminability condition; under condi- whether the word contained the specified
tions of high sensory discriminability, target letter or was appropriately described by the
words were spoken in a male voice and non- given adjective, respectively. Elaboration
target words in a female voice. According to was introduced at the physical level by pro-
late selection models (e.g. Shiffrin and viding two letters (e.g. 'letters R,DT), and at
Schneider 1977), there should be essentially the semantic level by giving two adjectives
complete perceptual processing of non-tar- (e.g. 'solid, edibleT). Expended processing
get words, irrespective of the degree of sen- capacity was indexed by means of a subsidi-
sory discriminability between targets and ary reaction time task.
non-targets.
In contrast, the multi-mode theory of As is shown in Fig. 3.4, semantic analysis
Johnston and Heinz (1978) predicts that required more processing capacity than
more processing of non-target words is ne- physical analysis, and elaborate processing
cessary under conditions of low sensory dis- was more demanding than non-elaborate
criminability, since Stage 1 analysis is insuf- processing. Thus the data confirmed the no-
ficient to discriminate between targets and tion that Stage 2 processing requires more
non-targets in those circumstances. In other expended processing capacity than Stage 1
words, the thoroughness of non-target pro- processing. While it is, of course, undoub-
cessing should be consistently high accord- tedly true that factors such as degree of
ing to late selection theories but should be practice substantially affect the demands of
inversely related to sensory discriminability different task activities on processing capac-
according to multi-mode theory. ity, it may nevertheless be an important gen-
The actual findings were that recall of eralization that semantic processing re-
non-target words was better with low than quires greater processing capacity than does
with high sensory discriminability, ex- physical processing.
pended processing capacity was greater M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck
with low sensory discriminability and the (1979 a) carried out a further experiment in
existence of a semantic relationship be- which they investigated the hypothesis that
tween targets and non-targets only disrupt- retrieval from long-term storage requires
ed the efficiency of shadowing performance more expended processing capacity than re-
when there was low sensory discriminabili- trieval from short-term storage. It was as-
ty. The implication of all of these findings is sumed that events in short-term storage are
that analysis of non-target information is those being attended to or rehearsed at any
flexible and is typically only as thorough as given time; they are thus readily accessible
38 Theories of Processing Resources

550

500 ELABORATE
Cl
z~
-u 450
"'w
"'", NON-ELABORATE
tl~
Oz 400
0:-
n.~

wt:: 350
0>-
Ou
z~
wn. 300
n.~
xu
w
250

f I I
Fig. 3.4. Expended processing ca-
pacity as a function of elaboration
PHYSICAL SEMANTIC and depth of processing. (Eysenck
PROCESSING TASK and Eysenck 1979a)

and can be retrieved with a relatively mod- make small demands on processing capacity
est use of processing capacity. Performance will be detected. The expectation is that se-
on an simple reaction time task that was car- mantically defined targets will require more
ried out at the same time as the retrieval task processing capacity than physically defined
was worse when subjects were retrieving targets (M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck
from long-term storage than from short- 1979 a). This would explain Lawson's
term storage, indicating that more process- (1966) finding of virtually perfect detection
ing capacity is needed for retrieval from of targets on the non-shadowed ear when
long-term storage. The various findings the targets consisted of auditory pips super-
from this study indicate that there are im- imposed on verbal messages.
portant differences in processing demands Treisman and Riley (1969) directly com-
among several of the processing activities pared the ability to detect targets as a func-
involved in learning. tion of the processing which they required.
Some of the major findings within the The targets in their study were letters pre-
shadowing paradigm may be explicable by sented among digits. When the target letters
reference to the theoretical views advanced were spoken in the same voice as the digits,
by M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck so that semantic analysis was needed in or-
(1979 a) and by Johnston and Heinz (1978). der to identify them, the detection rate for
In this paradigm, one of two concurrent au- targets in the non-shadowed message was
ditory inputs must be shadowed or repeated 33 %. However, when the target letters were
out loud, and at the same time target items in a different voice from the digits, so that
presented in either the shadowed or the only physical analysis was required for tar-
non-shadowed message must be detected. get detection, the detection rate on the non-
The usual finding is that target detection is shadowed message was 99 %. While these
much better in the shadowed message (e.g. data are consistent with the hypothesis that
Treisman and Geffen 1967, Treisman and semantic analysis consumes more process-
Riley 1969); part of the reason for this is that ing capacity than physical analysis, it could
shadowing is a demanding activity that also be argued that the improvement in tar-
seems to 'lock' attention on to the message get detection in the different voice condition
being shadowed. was due to the use of two differentiating cri-
If shadowing uses up most of the avail- teria for targets (i.e. voice and type of item)
able processing capacity, then only those rather than only one (i.e. type of item).
targets on the non-shadowed message that The analysis of shadowing task data in
Navon and Gopher: Multiple Resources 39

terms of processing capacity considerations the potential danger of confounding the


implies that the detection of targets on the level at which selection occurs (sensory ver-
non-shadowed message should depend on sus semantic) with ease of discriminability.
the capacity demands of the shadowing Johnston and Heinz seemed to assume
task. Sullivan (1976) manipulated shadow- that processing at any given level involves
ing demands by varying the overall redun- either automatic or attentional processes,
dancy of the message that was shadowed. but not both. In fact, while their data imply
As expected, fewer targets were detected on that different kinds of attentional selection
the non-shadowed message when the shad- make varying demands on attentional ca-
owed message was less redundant and so pacity, it is still entirely possible that both of
made more demands on processing capaci- the stimulus inputs in their experiments
ty. In addition, target detection was much were always thoroughly processed in an au-
higher when a single word was used as the tomatic fashion. In other words, their data
target (thus permitting detection on a pho- do not eliminate the possibility that full au-
nemic basis) than when any word from a tomatic processing of all inputs occured.
particular category could be used as the tar-
get (thus necessitating semantic analysis).
The greatest problem with the work of
Johnston and Heinz is that their experimen- 3.4 Navon and Gopher:
tal data fail to provide unequivocal support Multiple Resources
for their theoretical position. They claimed
that selection based on semantic informa-
tion requires more processing capacity than Navon and Gopher (1979) proposed a
selection based on sensory information. new theoretical perspective based on the no-
However, it is surely the case that ease of se- tion of multiple resources, an approach
lection must depend on the discriminability which they contrasted with central capacity
of two inputs (T. Neill, personal communi- interference models (e.g. Baddeley and
cation). Would attentional selection based Hitch 1974, Johnston and Heinz 1978,
on sensory information still require very Kahneman 1973, Norman and Bobrow
little capacity if subjects had to select a mes- 1975). Since their theory of multiple re-
sage spoken by one identical twin while re- sources is in large part an attempt to over-
jecting the same message spoken by the come the alleged deficiencies of the central
other identical twin? It is clear that atten- capacity approach, it is useful to start by
tional selection based purely on sensory in- considering the merits and demerits of that
formation can require considerable process- approach.
ing in some circumstances; similarly, selec- In essence, central capacity interference
tion based on the semantic difference be- models claim that there are finite resources
tween two messages would presumably re- (sometimes referred to as effort, capacity, or
quire relatively little processing capacity if attention) which can be flexibly deployed
that difference were sufficiently great. across a wide range of activities. It is usually
In spite of this complication, the findings assumed that performance of a task is posi-
of Johnston and Heinz (1978), M. W. Ey- tively related to the amount of available re-
senck and M. C. Eysenck (1979 a), Treis- sources. The processing system supplies re-
man and Riley (1969) and Sullivan (1976) all sources to meet the demands associated
suggest that attentional selection can usual- with the intended level of performance to
ly be achieved more readily on the basis of the extent that they are available.
physical than of semantic features. As Capacity models have most often been
M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck pointed applied to dual-task performance. The most
out, that still seems like a reasonable gen- natural assumption is that the demand for
eralization, even though one must beware of resources of two tasks when performed to-
40 Theories of Processing Resources

gether should equal the sum of the demands tendency to start tapping your stomach
for the same tasks when performed sepa- and/ or rubbing your head.
rately. This assumption that there is additiv- The simpler versions of the capacity-mod-
ity of demands is often erroneous, usually el approach assume that there is fixed ca-
because the apparent demands on resources pacity and availability of resources. How-
are increased by having to do the two tasks ever, Kahneman (1973) suggested that ca-
together. Why should this be so? A probable pacity might be elastic to some extent; more
reason is that the process of co-ordinating specifically, he proposed that increasing
resources between two tasks itself requires processing load leads to a rise in arousal,
resources. which in turn permits the system to mobilize
Capacity models have typically made the resources that were not available with a low-
simplifying assumption that tasks preserve er load. This notion is plausible but obvious-
their separate identities while being per- ly makes it more difficult to predict what is
formed together. However, there is increas- going to happen when two tasks are com-
ing evidence that new or emergent processes bined.
come into operation when certain tasks are Although it is not usually stated explicit-
combined (as the Gestaltists used to tell us, ly, most central capacity interference mod-
the whole is more than the sum of its parts). els assume that processing resources are at
Duncan (1979) illustrated this Gestaltist the disposal of the system to be allocated as
principle in an experiment on the psycho- required. In other words, the system can se-
logical refractory period. Subjects had to re- lect any combination of performance levels
spond as rapidly as possible to two closely on each of two tasks, provided only that ca-
successive stimuli, one requiring response pacity limits are not strained. In practice, of
from the left hand and the other requiring course, the system is often not perfectly free
response from the right hand. The relation- to decide how to allocate its resources.
ship or mapping between the first stimulus A very clear example of this is the Stroop
and response was either corresponding (i.e. effect, in which people name the colour in
leftmost stimulus location requiring re- which a colour name is printed (e.g. the
sponse of the leftmost finger, and rightmost word 'red' printed in blue). Even with exten-
stimulus location requiring response of the sive practice, there is interference from the
rightmost finger) or it was crossed (i.e. left- printed word, indicating that resources are
most stimulus location requiring response applied to it, in spite of all attempts to pre-
of the rightmost finger). The mapping of the vent this happening. A second example is
second stimulus and response could also be the way in which intense, novel or surprising
corresponding or crossed. stimuli attract attention to themselves and
The most interesting finding was that per- produce orienting responses. There is thus
formance was especially poor when one an important distinction between mandato-
corresponding mapping and one crossed ry and optional processes that has not been
mapping were used. The new or Gestaltist emphasized by central capacity theorists.
factor here is the element of choice and un- Central capacity theories often assume
certainty over which mapping to use. This that all of the available fixed capacity is al-
interpretation of the data was supported by located to the two tasks in dual-task situa-
an analysis of the error data; when the two tions. For instance, Norman and Bobrow's
mappings differed, errors tended to be those (1975) principle of complementarity
expected from selection of the inappropri- claimed that the sum of the resources ap-
ate mapping, something that did not occur plied to two concurrent tasks was equal to
when the two mappings were the same. You the sum ofthe available resources. There are
can obtain a similar effect by attempting to several reasons why the resources invested
rub your stomach and tap your head at the in the two tasks might fall short of the total
same time - there is an almost irresistible pool of resources. Some resources might be
Navon and Gopher: Multiple Resources 41

't:J 2·25 VISUAL


IMAGERY
>-
l-
S; 2·00
i=
Vi
z
~ ',75 AUDITORY
IMAGERY
...J
<{
:::l
I-
0.. 1'50
w
u
Fig. 3.5. Sensitivity of signal detection as a
function of signal modality and modality
~ 1r~_______I'-__________'I________
of concurrent imagery. (Segal and Fusella VISUAL AUDITORY
1970) SIGNAL

invested in other activities, or the joint de- with the detection of auditory signals than
mands of the two tasks might be well within with the detection of visual signals. Central
the capacity of the system. Another possibil- capacity theories would interpret this to
ity is that people might choose not to exert mean that the detection of auditory signals
themselves maximally (how often does any- makes greater demands on precessing re-
one work 'flat out' ?). sources than does the detection of visual
signals and would predict that the same pat-
N avon and Gopher (1979) concluded that
tern of results should be obtained when the
central capacity interference theories have
visual image task was combined with each
only been able to account for the experi-
of the signal detection tasks. In fact, exactly
mental data by adding several extra explan-
the opposite result was obtained.
atory principles to the basic formulation.
The general position is that central capac-
These include the notion of elastic capacity,
ity interference theories are embarrassed
Gestalt-like properties of combining tasks,
when the performance of a given task is dis-
data-limited processes and automatic pro-
rupted more than the performance of an-
cessing. The present state of affairs is sum-
other one by pairing either of them with a
marized by Navon and Gopher in the fol-
third task but is disrupted less by a fourth
lowing way: "The most parsimonious view
task. The natural inclination in such cases is
of the field (i.e., the central capacity interfer-
to assume the existence of relatively specific
ence theory) seems to have proved inade-
processing mechanisms, rather than a single
quate; the remaining alternatives are either
general capacity. With respect to the data of
to augment, patch and hedge that view so
Segal and Fusella (1970), the most plausible
that it barely resembles its original form, or
explanation is that visual perception and
to substitute it altogether with a broader,
visual imagery share common structures
and necessarily more complex, view" (pp.
and mechanisms, as do auditory perception
247-248).
and auditory imagery.
One of their reasons for preferring the lat- One of the reasons for the popularity of
ter alternative was that there are several central capacity interference models is that
studies (e.g. Segal and Fusella 1970, Treis- they usually provide a simple explanation of
man and Davies 1973) obtaining results that dual-task performance. Whenever two con-
are completely inconsistent with the central current tasks interfere with each other, we
capacity notion. Segal and Fusella (1970) can argue that this is due to competition for
combined the tasks of image construction a limited general-purpose central capacity
(visual or auditory) and signal detection or attention. In contrast, if two tasks are per-
(visual or auditory). As can be seen in Fig. 3.5, formed together without disruption, then
the auditory image task interfered more the conclusion is that one or both of the
42 Theories of Processing Resources

Fig. 3.6. The four kinds of resources in-


volved in the concurrent performance of
two tasks x and y. (Navon and Gopher
1979)

tasks must be data limited or automatic. It is The basic notions of their theory were ex-
only when additional task combinations are pressed in the following words by Navon
considered (as in the work of Segal and Fu- and Gopher (1979): "Not only can the pro-
sella 1970, or Treisman and Davies 1973) cessing system as a whole be involved in
that data obviously incompatible with the several activities in variable~proportions but
central capacity approach can be obtained. a specific mechanism or modality is not ne-
It has too often been assumed by propo- cessarily dominated by one process exclu-
nents of central capacity interference mod- sively but instead can accommodate more
els that questions regarding the limitations than one process at the expense of quality or
of performance of two or more concurrent speed of performance ... Each channel may
tasks are essentially the same as questions have its own capacity (which is, roughly, the
concerned with the limitations of attention. limit on the amount of information that can
The only measure of limited attentional ca- be stored, transmitted, or processed by the
pacity is often mutual interference between channel at a unit of time)" (p. 233).
two tasks, with this interference being 'ex- According to multiple resource theory,
plained' in terms of limited attentional ca- the concurrent performance of two tasks
pacity! Some of the problems associated (x and y) involves four categories of re-
with the use of the dual-task paradigm as a sources (see Fig. 3.6):
way of demonstrating capacity limitations 1. resources that can be used by task x but
are discussed in detail by Duncan (1980). not by task y;
Specific interference effects have led 2. those resources that can be used by both
some theorists (e.g. Allport et al. 1972) to tasks;
postulate the existence of mUltiple channels 3. resources that can be used by task y but
or mechanisms. However, a strict model of not by task x;
this type seems inadequate in view of the 4. resources that are irrelevant to both tasks.
fact that processes using the same mecha- If either or b9th of the tasks can be per-
nisms often interfere with each other but sel- formed with various combinations of re-
dom block each other completely. Navon sources, the optimal strategy is to minimize
and Gopher (1979) proposed instead a mul- the extent to which the same resources are
tiple capacity or resource theory. According involved in the two tasks. Indeed, if there is
to this theory, auditory imagery interferes no overlap in the resources required by each
more with auditory signal detection than task, then it should be possible to perform
with visual signal detection because the for- the tasks in parallel.
mer pair of tasks has more types of required Gopher and Navon (1979) argued that
resources in common than the latter pair. their theory led to an alternative interpreta-
Conclusions 43

tion of observations suggesting that capaci- propriate, are discussed in the remainder of
ty is elastic and grows as the processing load this chapter.
increases. It may simply be that a new task
added to an old one is often able to utilize
formerly unused resources because of dis- 3.5.1 Dual-Task Performance:
similarities in the resources required by the Non-Specific Interference
two tasks. The high arousal typically asso-
ciated with a heavy processing load may The basic notion that two inputs sharing
reflect the stress produced by the processing the same limited capacity system might in-
requirements rather than an increase in pro- terfere with each other was initially put for-
cessing capacity. ward by the biologist Loeb and then tested
It is obvious that the theorizing of Navon empirically by his student Jeannette Welch
and Gopher (1979) is still at a very early (1898). She used a standard secondary task
stage of development. While they made a (maximal hand grip) and combined it with a
strong case for the general multiple resource variety of primary tasks such as calculation
approach, their failure to specify the num- and reading. Her argument was that one
ber and nature of the various resources could use the degree of interference with the
means that their theory is not logically falsi- secondary task as an index of the attentional
fiable. However, they suggested that this demands of the primary task.
omission could be rectified by pairing each In the years since then, this basic tech-
of a number of tasks with each other. Pro- nique has been applied extensively in sever-
vided that a sufficiently variegated set of al different ways. One application has been
tasks were used, it could be assumed that to work on the psychological refractory pe-
every type of resource would be competed riod in which two stimuli occurring very
for by at least two tasks. Interference data closely together in time require separate re-
from all of the task combinations could then sponses. Another example is the paradigm
be used to identify the major resources used used by Johnston and Heinz (1978), involv-
in information processing. ing a simple reaction time subsidiary task to
ascertain expended processing capacity on
the primary task.
One of the most impressive findings from
a theoretical point of view is the extent to
3.5 Conclusions which apparently dissimilar activities inter-
fere with each other. Welch, for example,
found that performance of the physical
There has been a tremendous explosion in hand grip task was impaired when it was
the number of theories of attention and combined with purely mental activities. In
processing resources proposed in recent similar vein, Earl Hunt (personal communi-
years. In this chapter, and the previous one, cation) has used a hand-steadiness task in
we have considered ten of them and that which the subject is simply told to keep his
number could have been doubled without or her hand still between two metal plates.
any difficulty. Fortunately enough, in terms When this task is combined with items taken
of not overtaxing the reader's attentional ca- from an intelligence test, it is performed
pacity, there does seem to be a substantial progressively worse as the test items become
overlap of ideas among these theories. In more difficult. A final example is the study
addition, there are only a limited number of by Watkins et al. (1973) discussed earlier in
key findings that need to be extracted from the chapter; they found that visual tracking.
the plethora of experimental data. Four of interfered with verbal short-term memory.
these crucial findings, together with the the- The implication of such findings is that
oretical explanation which seems most ap- there is some non-specific resource of limit-
44 Theories of Processing Resources

ed capacity that is importantly involved in (1970). Treisman and Davies (1973) report-
the performance of an enormous variety of ed that monitoring tasks interfered much
tasks and in the co-ordination of two con- more with each other when stimuli were pre-
current tasks. This resource has been de- sented in the same sense modality (visual or
scribed in a number of ways; Posner (1978) auditory) than when they were presented in
called it 'conscious attention', Baddeley and different modalities. A number of other rele-
Hitch (1974) referred to it as a 'modality- vant studies are discussed by Navon and
free central processor', Shiffrin and Gopher (1979).
Schneider (1977) described it as 'controlled Is mixed modality presentation always
processing' and Johnston and Heinz (1978) superior to single modality presentation
referred to 'expended processing capacity'. when it comes to performing two concurrent
tasks? Not necessarily, according to the
views expressed by Treisman and Davies
(1973). They argued that separate visual and
3.5.2 Dual-Task Peiformance: Specific
auditory processing capacity exists for phys-
Inteiference
ical analysis of inputs but that only shared
capacity was available for semantic analy-
Some theorists have used the data from sis. According to this viewpoint, recall of si-
dual-task studies as strong evidence for the multaneously presented pairs of words
existence of a general processing resource should be superior when the words within
(i.e. attention). However, other theorists each pair are presented in different modali-
have quite correctly pointed out that the ties (i.e. one visually and one auditorily)
same data also indicate that this view must than when they are in the same modality (i.e.
be regarded as a substantial over-simplifica- both presented visually or auditorily), pro-
tion. vided that the words are analysed at the
The kind of result that is especially em- physical level. If the words are processed se-
barrassing for a theory postulating only a mantically, then recall should be the same
single non-specific resource of limited ca- irrespective of whether or not the same or
pacity can be illustrated most simply by con- different modalities are used. In fact, Mar-
sidering two primary task (P 1 and P2) and tin (1980) discovered that there was indeed a
two secondary tasks (S 1 and S 2). Initially, larger advantage of mixed-modality presen-
we pair P 1 with S 1 and P2 with S 1, and we tation for physical analysis than for seman-
discover that P 1 interferes more with the tic analysis (see Fig. 3.7), but there was still
performance of S 1 than P2 does. Central some advantage for semantic analysis.
capacity theorists would interpret this result These findings suggest that at least some
as meaning that P 1 requires more of the central processing capacity is modality spe-
limited attentional capacity than does P2. cific in nature.
That conclusion leads to the prediction that Theoretically, these findings obviously
PI should also interfere with the perform- indicate the necessity for postulating a num-
ance of S 2 to a greater extent than P 2. If it ber of specific resources or mechanisms,
turns out that PI interferes significantly less over and above a non-specific attentional
than P 2 with the performance of S 2, then a resource. As Navon and Gopher (1979)
simple central capacity theory cannot ex- pointed out, the available data are quite in-
plain the data. sufficient to permit accurate identification
So far, we have simply described a hypo- of these specific resources. However, it is
thetical state of affairs. In fact, there are fairly clear that there are a number of mo-
now several studies in the literature demon- dality-specific processing resources. It may
strating precisely this kind of interaction in- well be that the articulatory loop (a tempo-
volving two pairs of tasks. We have already rary holding mechanism for verbal informa-
discussed the study by Segal and Fusella tion) exemplifies a specific processing re-
Conclusions 45

70 PHYSICAL
ANALYSIS
z
0 65
~
z
w 60 SEMANTIC
~
W ANALYSIS
D:
~ 55
z
w
U
w 50
D:
11.

Fig. 3.7. Recall as a function of depth of


analysis and of presentation modality.
L I I
SINGLE MIXED
(Martin 1980) PRESENTATION MODALITY

source, and there may be analogous holding tively general and non-automatic processes
mechanisms for other kinds ofinformation. appear towards the top of the hierarchy, and
specific, automatic processes occur at the
bottom. As a rule of thumb, the location in
3.5.3 The Effects of Practice the hierarchy of the processes involved in
the performance of a task can be assessed by
Professional psychologists and laymen have a series of dual-task experiments in which
long known that practice can produce the task is paired with several others: high-
large effects on performance, and the thou- er-level processes will more consistently
sands of hours of training undergone by out- produce interference than will low-level
standing sportsmen and wom~n in order to processes.
perfect their skills bear testimony to the ac- Since higher-level processes are more
curacy of that viewpoint. However, it is true widely deployed than lower-level processes,
to say that the recent work of Posner and and since they are also more susceptible to
Snyder (1975a), Shiffrin and Schneider interference, they can be regarded as espe-
(1977) and others has demonstrated that the cially useful and valuable. Practice plays a
effects of practice on information process- vital role in reducing the congestion at the
ing are much more dramatic and profound higher levels of the hierarchy. It accom-
than !lad previously been supposed. plishes this by permitting tasks to be carried
We have seen that there is some ambigui- out increasingly by lower-level processing
ty about the appropriate criteria for decid- systems as training proceeds.
ing that automaticity has occurred, but there
is little doubt that fairly complicated pro-
cesses can occur entirely outside of con- 3.5.4 The Central Processor:
scious awareness. A plausible conceptuali- Rigidity Versus Flexibility
zation of the organization of the attentional
system involves the postulation of a hierar- The pioneering attention theorists such as
chical structure of processes, with the cen- Broadbent, Deutsch and Deutsch and Treis-
tral processor at the top of the hierarchy and man were all in some sense attempting to lo-
more specific processing resources below it. cate the bottleneck in information process-
The position of any processing system with- ing. It was assumed that a limited capacity
in the hierarchy is determined by two major attentional system was responsible for the
criteria: (1) the generality-specificity of the bottleneck and that it was valuable to track
processing system and (2) the degree of au- down the precise point at which the central
tomaticity of the processing system. Rela- processor was located.
46 Theories of Processing Resources

Subsequent research has indicated that this 3.5.5 A Sketch-Map of the Processing
was too rigid a conceptualization. Bothle- System
necks in different tasks do not necessarily
occur at the same point in the stream of in-
formation processing. Several contempo- The basic theoretical assumption is that
rary theorists of otherwise very different there is a hierarchical system of processes or
persuasions agree that the central processor capacities involved in information process-
is used in a remarkably flexible way and can ing. Higher-level processes are general and
be employed to facilitate processing at virtu- non-automatic, whereas lower-level pro-
ally any stage of information processing. cesses are relatively specific and automatic.
The central processor resembles a trou- Practice enables a task to be performed by
ble-shooter in a number of respects. It ap- processes further down the hierarchy than is
pears to be responsive to indications that possible without training.
some lower-level process is experiencing There is a central processor at the top of
difficulty. For example, walking is usually the hierarchy. This central processor is used
largely controlled by lower-level processes. in a very flexible way to assist lower-level
However, if walking poses unusual prob- processes, and it also has its own special
lems (e.g. because of a limp or while des- functions (e.g. transformation of informa-
cending very steep steps), then the central tion and executive decision making). It is
processor can be used to facilitate walking characterized by limited capacity, and its
by attending closely to the taking of each functional capacity at any given time, is de-
step. termined by the demands placed on it.
The central processor possesses a number This theoretical position is to be contrast-
of important characteristics in addition to ed with the notion that only one processing
flexibility. As virtually everyone has as- resource (i.e. attentional capacity) needs to
sumed, the central processor appears to be postulated. That notion is refuted by the
have relatively limited capacity, and this as- evidence for specific interference effects.
sumption is buttressed by the widespread Another theoretical possibility is that there
interference effects obtained in the dual- are many capacities limiting performance
task paradigm. As Kahneman (1973) has ar- but that none of them can be identified un-
gued, the full resources of the central pro- iquely with attention. Such a system is hetar-
cessor are rarely used in everyday life. The chic, with different capacities assuming ex-
central processor is responsive to the de- ecutive control in different tasks (Treisman
mands made on it and supplies attentional 1969). The substantial evidence from dual-
resources commensurate with the demands. task studies that a single resource plays a
It is only when substantial demands for at- dominant part in a vast range of processing
tentional capacity are imposed by a task that tasks seems to favour a hierarchical system
its total resources will be used. rather than one based on hetarchic control.
4 Theories of Arousal and Performance

The emphasis in contemporary ap- 4.1 The Yerkes-Dodson Law


proaches to attention and performance, as
we saw in the previous two chapters, is very The first attempt to provide a simple gen-
much on theoretical constructs of a cogni- eralization that would be of wide applicabil-
tive nature. However, it seems probable, ity to the very complex and apparently in-
both in terms of everyday experience and consistent findings on arousal and perform-
the relevant experimental evidence, that the ance was made by Yerkes and Dodson as far
physiological state of the organism while back as 1908. They proposed what has since
engaged in information processing is im- been referred to as the Yerkes-Dodson law,
portant and ought to be considered. consisting of two crucial assumptions. First-
Early theoretical positions emphasized ly, it was assumed that there was an invert-
only one side of the interrelationship be- ed- U relationship between the level of ten-
tween physiological state and task perform- sion, motivation or arousal on the one hand
ance, namely, the effect of a greater or lesser and performance on the other, with per-
level of physiological arousal on an individ- formance efficiency being optimal at some
ual's ability to perform efficiently. How- moderate level of arousal; secondly,
ever, it is equally important to consider the strength of motivation or arousal was as-
effects of performing a task on physiologi- sumed to interact with task difficulty in such
cal activity. If you look into a mirror while a way that the optimal level of arousal was
attempting to solve a difficult problem in inversely related to task difficulty. The pre-
mental arithmetic (e.g. multiplying 69 by dicted relationships among arousal, task
17), you will find that your pupil will dilate difficulty and efficiency of performance are
and there will be several other less ob- shown in Fig. 4.1.
servable physiological effects. It is only In their 1908 study, Yerkes and Dodson
comparatively recently that serious attempts used mice as subjects, aversive drive was
have been made to account for such effects provided by electric shock and task diffi-
in a systematic way. culty was defined in terms of ease of discri-
This chapter considers some of the main mination along a brightness dimension. The
theoretical approaches that have attempted pattern of results which they obtained re-
to explain the bi-directional effects of phys- sembles that shown in Fig. 4.1 and has been
iological states on information processing, replicated in most respects in several other
and vice versa. The relevant theories are dis- studies manipulating arousal and task diffi-
cussed more or less in their actual historical culty in very variegated ways (see Broad-
sequence; the main reason for doing this is hurst 1959).
that it facilitates an understanding of the The strength of the experimental support
ways in which ideas in this area have dev- for the Yerkes- Dodson law can be evaluated
eloped over the years. most straightforwardly by taking the two as-
48 Theories of Arousal and Performance

>-
u
""'--', \
,
/
z I \
w /
Fig. 4.1. The anticipated
U TASK / \
,, relationship among
,
LL /
LL / arousal, task difficulty
W /

;~ /
/ """ and performance effi-
ciency according to the
~, " Yerkes-Dodson law
AROUSAL (1908)

sumptions of the law separately. The main significant and in line with the Yerkes-Dod-
problem with the first assumption is that it is son law in 22 experiments, and the 'reverse'
quite difficult to disprove the predicted in- interaction was significant in two experi-
verted-U relationship between arousal and ments.
performance. If, as in many studies, three It is probably true to say that there has
levels of arousal are compared, there are six been more support for the Yerkes-Dodson
possible orderings of these three levels with law when arousal has been produced by
respect to performance. Only two of these aversive stimulation rather than by incen-
orderings are inconsistent with the Yerkes- tives. The usual finding with incentives is
Dodson law (the medium level of arousal that there is no interaction between incen-
cannot be associated with the worst level of tive conditions and task difficulty (M. W.
performance). In other words, two-thirds of Eysenck, in preparation; Wilkinson et al.
studies investigating this assumption of the 1972; Willett 1964a).
Yerkes-Dodson law with three arousal lev- This apparent discrepancy in the effects
els would obtain supportive evidence by of anxiety and incentives on performance
chance alone! highlights one of the problems with the
The position is much better if four differ- Yerkes- Dodson law. It is difficult to define
ent points along the arousal continuum are 'arousal' in an unequivocal way, and it is
sampled; there are 24 possible orderings of probably very misleading to assume that
four conditions with respect to perform- such diverse methods of producing arousal
ance, 16 of which are inconsistent with the as monetary incentives and threat of electric
inverted-U function. Unfortunately, very shock affect performance in comparable
few studies have used four or more different ways. Some of the evidence supporting this
levels of arousal. viewpoint will be discussed in later chap-
There is rather more convincing evidence ters.
that the optimal level of arousal is inversely A further problem with the Yerkes-Dod-
related to task difficulty. A thorough exami- son law is that it is not entirely clear how the
nation of the anxiety literature by one of my dimension of task difficulty can best be con-
students, Kathy Cohen, uncovered approxi- ceptualized. Kahneman (1973) has suggest-
mately 50 studies looking at the effects of ed that difficult tasks require greater mental
anxiety on two or more tasks of varying dif- effort or attentional capacity than easy
ficulty (usually paired-associate learning). tasks. He reviewed a number of studies in
The Yerkes-Dodson law would tend to be which physiological measurements (possi-
supported if anxiety interacted with task dif- bly reflecting mental effort) were taken dur-
ficulty in such a Way that high anxiety had a ing the performance of tasks of varying
greater detrimental effect on hard than on complexity. The consistent finding was that
easy tasks. This interaction was not statisti- more effort or arousal appeared to be invest-
cally significant in 30 experiments; it was ed in the performance of relatively difficult
Easterbrook's Hypothesis 49

tasks. Some of this evidence is discussed in tive with respect to the underlying mechan-
Chap. I. isms and processes.
The Yerkes-Dodson law indicates that for
successful performance an individual's lev-
el of arousal should become progressively
lower the more difficult and demanding the 4.2 Easterbrook's Hypothesis
task involved. However, the findings re-
ferred to in the previous paragraph indicate
that people in fact exert more effort and be- One of the most prevalent views concern-
come more aroused as task difficulty in- ing the effects of arousal on performance
creases. There is an apparent paradox here, has been that they are mediated by atten-
in that the typical reaction to increased task tional mechanisms. More specifically, it has
complexity ought to be counter-productive often been assumed that heightened arousal
according to the Yerkes-Dodson law. How- has a substantial effect on attentional selec-
ever, this paradox disappears if it is argued tivity. The best-known exponent of this po-
that effort differs from other manifestations sition is Easterbrook, who argued in 1959
of arousal in usually being positively related that states of high emotionality, arousal and
to the quality of performance. anxiety all produce comparable effects on
A further inadequacy with the Yerkes- cue utilization. There is a progressive reduc-
Dodson law is the assumption that perform- tion in the range of cues used as arousal in-
ance efficiency can be assessed properly by creases, which "will reduce the proportion
a single, global, behavioural measure. In of irrelevant cues employed, and so improve
many cases, it is relatively meaningless to performance. When all irrelevant cues have
say that arousal has increased or decreased been excluded, however, ... further reduc-
performance efficiency; this becomes clear tion in the number of cues employed can on-
when two or more behavioural measures are ly affect relevant cues, and proficiency will
calculated, and a complex pattern emerges. fall" (p. 193).
An example of such a pattern was obtained One of the advantages of Easterbrook's
by Hamilton et al. (1977). They found that formulation is that it provides a potential
intense white noise increased the speed with explanation for the inverse relationship be-
which simple transformations could be tween the optimal level of arousal and task
made on a letter transformation task but re- difficulty. If difficult tasks involve a greater
duced the ability to retain the products of number of relevant cues than do easy tasks,
previous processing at the same time. The then attentional narrowing under high
utilization of any global estimate of per- arousal will disrupt the performance of dif-
formance efficiency would clearly represent ficult tasks more readily than the perform-
an over-simplification, since noise led to in- ance of simple tasks.
creased efficiency for certain aspects of the Walley and Weiden (1973,1974) attempt-
task but to decreased efficiency for other ed to develop some of Easterbrook's ideas,
aspects. and they proposed a speculative mechanism
Finally, it is worth noting that the Yerkes- whereby arousal might produce a reduction
Dodson law merely refers to predicted in- in cue utilization. They claimed that pattern
terrelationships among arousal, task diffi- recognition was accomplished via a hierar-
culty and performance efficiency. While the chical network of feature analysers and that
law has received some empirical support, it encoding represented the function of the
provides no indication of the appropriate highest level of the pattern recognition net-
theoretical explanation of the various in- work. Encoding is potentially a parallel pro-
terrelationships. Accordingly, the Yerkes- cess, so that more than one input can be en-
Dodson law at best describes certain pre- coded concurrently.
dicted patterns of results but is uninforma- In practice, however, the encoding of one
50 Theories of Arousal and Performance

input tends to interfere with the concurrent hypothesis is that high arousal will increase
encoding of any additional inputs; they re- the attentional bias towards the primary
ferred to this interference effect as 'cogni- task at the expense of the secondary task
tive masking'. They argued that cognitive in- and thus impair performance on the secon-
terference was due to inhibitory interactions dary task. This result was obtained in many
among cortical neurons at the highest levels of the studies reviewed by Easterbrook
of the hierarchical pattern recognizers. One (1959).
of the major determinants of cognitive In most of these studies, it is not clear
masking is the degree of similarity of the whether the performance decrement under
processes involved in encoding the various high arousal is due to an actual attenuation
inputs: the greater the similarity, the more of sensitivity of the processing system or
the cognitive masking that occurs. Of more whether it reflects an increase in the re-
immediate relevance, Walley and Wei den sponse criterion (Le. greater cautiousness of
also argued that increments in arousal pro- responding). Bacon (1974) asked subjects to
duce increased cognitive masking, the rea- perform a pursuit rotor tracking task and an
son being that the degree of lateral inhibi- auditory signal-detection task at the same
tion in the cortex is related to arousal. As a time. Arousal was manipulated by electric
consequence, while shared or parallel pro- shocks, and performance on the two tasks
cessing would be possible at low levels of was analyzed by the measures of signal de-
arousal, it would become decreasingly so as tection theory. As is implied by Easter-
arousal increased. brook's hypothesis, the decrement in per-
At the physiological level, Walley and formance on the subsidiary task under high
Weiden claimed that arousal produced an arousal resulted from a reduction in sensi-
amalgam of excitatory and inhibitory ef- tivity rather than from an increase in the re-
fects. Steriade (1970) discussed a number of sponse criterion.
studies dealing with the responsiveness of Bacon (1974) also found that high arousal
the cortex and the thalamus during states of only impaired performance on the subsidi-
high arousal. He concluded that although ary task when there was a delay of a few sec-
there is some facilitation of thalamic excit- onds between stimulus presentation and re-
ability during arousal, a state of tonic arous- port. This indicates that there is a reduction
al is associated with reduced cortical re- in the ability to 'hold' information in states
sponsiveness. of high arousal and suggests that arousal
Demetrescu et al. (1965) found that arous- may reduce working memory capacity.
al based on activity in the brain stem reticu- One unfortunate feature of Easterbrook's
lar formation produced a combination offa- (1959) hypothesis is that there are in fact
cilitatory and inhibitory effects on the re- three rather different patterns of results
sponsiveness of the cortex. There was some which are all consistent with the notion that
evidence for a diffuse ascending inhibitory arousal produces increased attentional se-
system originating in the ventral pontine re- lectivity. An improvement in performance
ticular formation. This system activates an of the main task coupled with an impair-
intracortical inhibitory network which is ex- ment of performance on the subsidiary task
cited by specific input to the cortex and under high arousal clearly suggests a re-al-
which may be responsible for cognitive location of attention, as does improved
masking. main-task performance combined with no
The crucial assumption that high arousal effect on the subsidiary task or no effect on
produces attentional narrowing has usally the main task in conjunction with impaired
been tested by using a paradigm in which a performance on the subsidiary task. Overall,
main or primary task and a secondary or in- there are nine possible combinations of
cidental task are performed concurrently. main-task and subsidiary-task performance,
The natural prediction from Easterbrook's only three of which are clearly incompatible
Easterbrook's Hypothesis 51
with Easterbrook's hypothesis; in those subsidiary-task performance. Anxiety and
cases, arousal either has a less detrimental shock both produced a significantly detri-
effect or a greater enhancing effect on the mental effect on the secondary task in be-
subsidiary task than on the main task. tween 70% and 80% of the experiments in
A summary of the various findings from which they were used; for incentive and
experimental tests of Easterbrook's hypothe- noise the figure is under 50%.
sis is given in Table 4.1. It can be seen that well Thus the modal finding with respect to
over half of the published studies produced anxiety and electric shock is no effect of
patterns of results in line with the hypothesis. these factors on the main task combined
However, it is clear that there are interesting with impairment of the subsidiary task. In
differences among the various methods of contrast, incentive often leads to improved
producing arousal. Ifwe consider main-task performance on the main task combined
performance on its own, then electric shock with no effect or a performance decrement
enhanced performance in 0% of the studies on subsidiary task performance.
(0 out of 10), and anxiety improved per- At the very least, these data suggest that it
formance in 10 % (lout of 10). In contrast, may be unwise to follow Easterbrook in em-
incentives improved main-task perform- phasizing the similarity of the effects on du-
ance 40% of the time (6 out of 15) and noise al-task performance produced by various
enhanced performance 30% of the time arousing or stressful factors. One way of
(5 out of 17). conceptualizing the different behavioural
The various methods of manipulating patterns induced by these various arousal
arousal also have rather variable effects on agents is to argue that high anxiety and elec-

Table 4.1. Effects of arousal on dual-task performance

Shock

Effect of
arousal
Researchers Main task Other task Main Other

Bacon (1974) Tracking Signal detection N.S. X


Bacon (1974) Signal detection Tracking N.S. N.S.
Kohn(l954) Relevant recall Irrelevant recall N.S. X
McNamara and Fisch (1964) Serial learning Irrelevant recall N.S. X
Reeves and Bergum (1972) Light detection Light detection N.S. N.S.
Silverman (1954) Line discrimination Incidental learning X X
Silverman and Blitz (1956) Intentional learning Incidental learning N.S. N.S.
Tecce and Tamell (1965) MotorR.T. RetumR.T. N.S. X
Thornton and Powell (1974) PAleaming Incidental learning N.S. X
Wachtel (1968) Tracking R.T. N.S. X

Anxiety

Effect of arousal
Researchers Main task Other task Scale Main Other

Eysenck and Eysenck Intentional learning Incidental learning EPI-N N.S. X


(unpuhl.) EPI-E
Kausler, Trapp and Brewer Serial learning Incidental learning MAS + X
(1959)
Markowitz (1969) Intentional learning Incidental learning R-S N.S. N.S.
Miller and Dost(1964) Word sorting Incidental learning MAS N.S. X
Silverman and Blitz (1956) Intentional learning Incidental learning MAS N.S. X
52 Theories of Arousal and Performance

Table 4.1 (Continued)


Effect of
arousal
Researchers Main task Other task Main Other

Wachtel (1968) Tracking R.T. TAS N.S. N.S.


Weltman and Egstrom Detection Detection Diving N.S. X
(1966)
Weltman, Smith and Visual acuity Detection Sim.press. N.S. X
Egstrom (1971)
Wolk and DuCette (1974) Typographical error Content recall L.c. X X
detection
Wolk and DuCette (1974) Intentional learning Incidental learning L.C. X X

Incentive

Effect of
arousal
Researchers Main task Other task Main Other

Bahrick (1954) Serial learning Colour learning + X


Bahrick, Fitts and Rankin Tracking Light detection + X
(1952)
C~hen, Telegdy, Laroche Concept classification Concept classification N.S. N.S.
arid Getz (1973)
Davies and Jones (1975) Serial learning Location lea~ing + N.S.
(incidental)
Dixon and Cameron (1976) Serial learning Colour learning N.S. +
Dornbush (1965) Intentional learning Incidental learning + N.S.
Dornbush (1965) Intentional recall Incidental recognition N.S. N.S.
Johnson and Thomson Intentional recall Incidental recognition N.S. X
(1962)
Johnson and Thomson Intentional learning Incidental recognition N.S. N.S.
(1962)
Kausler, Laughlin and Serial learning Colour learning + +
Trapp (1963)
Kausler and Trapp (1962) Serial learning Incidental learning N.S. N.S.
McNamara and Fisch (1964) Serial learning Incidental recall N.S. N.S.
Rubin, Shantz and Smock Form retention Form retention + X
(1962)
Wolk and DuCette (1974) Typographical error Content recall N.S. +
detection
Wolk and DuCette (1974) Typographical error Content recall N.S. X
detection
Noise

Effect of
arousal
Researchers Main task Other task Main Other

Bell (1978) Tracking Number detection N.S. X


Boggs and Simon (1968) Reaction time Number detection N.S. X
Cohen and Lezak (1977) Free recall Incidental learning N.S. X
Davies and Jones (1975) Serial learning Location learning N.S. X
(incidental)
Finkelman and Glass Tracking Short-term memory N.S. N.S.
(1970; predictable noise)
Finkelman and Glass Tracking Short-term memory N.S. X
(1970; unpredictable noise)
Easterbrook's Hypothesis 53
Table4.1. (Continued)
Effect of
arousal
Researchers Main task Other task Main Other

Forster and Grierson Tracking Light detection N.S. N.S.


(1978; 4 exps.)
Hartley (1981) Tracking Light detection X N.S.
Hockey (1970a) Tracking Light detection + N.S.
Hockey (1970b) Tracking Light detection + X
(unbiased probs)
Hockey (1970c) Tracking Light detection + N.S.
(biased probs)
Hockey, Dornicand Intentional learning Incidental learning + N.S.
Hamilton (unpubl.)
Hockey and Hamilton Serial learning Location learning + X
(1970) (incidental)
O'Malley and Poplawsky Serial learning Incidental learning N.S. X
(1971)

EPI = Eysenck Personality Inventory; MAS = Manifest Anxiety Scale; TAS = Test Anxiety Scale;
Sim. press. = simulated pressure; L.c. = Locus of Control; R-S = Repression-Sensitization Scale;
R.T. = reaction time; PA = paired-associate; + = arouser significantly improved performance;
N.S. = arouser had no significant effect on performance; X = arouser significantly impaired perform-
ance.

tric shock both lead to increased attentional because they could not process them due to
selectivity and reduced attentional capacity limited attentional capacity. Subjects must
with respect to the performance tasks, be instructed to attempt to perform the sub-
whereas incentive increases attentional se- sidiary task in order to make the interpreta-
lectivity without necessarily reducing atten- tion of the data unequivocal.
tional capacity. Easterbrook's notion that heightened
It should be pointed out that several of arousal produces greater attentional selec-
the studies summarized in Table 4.1 may tivity provides a valuable point of depar-
not in fact be as relevant to the issue of the ture. However, it leaves certain key issues
effects of arousal on attentional selectivity unresolved. Why does arousal have this ef-
as their authors suggested. The subsidiary fect on attention? Easterbrook (1959) re-
task often involved incidental learning; garded the basic phenomenon of attentional
since the test for incidental learning typical- narrowing under high arousal as reflecting
ly occurred several seconds or minutes after the operation of a relatively passive and au-
the relevant stimulus material had been pre- tomatic process. However, it is more reason-
sented, there can be no unequivocal inter- able to view attentional narrowing as an ac-
pretation of the data. Reduced incidental tive coping response, i.e. under those cir-
learning is clearly consistent with increased cumstances in which the total information-
attentional selectivity favouring the main processing demands cannot be handled by
task, but storage, consolidation, and retrie- the available processing capacity, people
val processes may all be implicated. may decide to restrict attention voluntarily
There is a further amlJiguity about these to a small number of information sources.
findings. It is important to know whether In other words, increased attentional selec-
the poor incidental learning under high tivity may be only a probable, rather than an
arousal occurred because aroused subjects inevitable, consequence of heightened
chose not to process the irrelevant stimuli or aro·usal.
54 Theories of Arousal and Performance

A detailed consideration of Easterbrook's creased attentional selectivity, but several


hypothesis indicates that it makes a number additional theoretical assumptions need to
of somewhat bizarre predictions. The hypo- be made in order to account for the data.
thesis implies that tired, bored and listless
people in a state of low arousal who are
slumped in front of a television set are suf-
fering from an excessive openness to experi- 4.3 Broadbent: Decision and Stress
ence! It is surely necessary to consider the
effects of arousal not only on attentional se-
lectivity but also on attentional capacity; it Broadbent's (1971) theory has several im-
makes intuitive sense to argue that people portant things to say about the effects of
who are bored and listless are operating un- arousal on performance, so that it will only
der conditions of reduced attentional capac- be possible to discuss a few of the main
ity. points here. He argued that it was valuable
Another curious prediction concerns the to carry out studies in which two stressors or
effects of anxiety on attention. Easterbrook arousing agents are applied singly and to-
(1959) implies that high anxiety impairs per- gether. If the combined effects on perform-
formance because it leads to intense concen- ance of the two stressors substantially ex-
tration on some, rather than all, of the task ceed pure additivity, one can conclude that
elements. Common sense, as well as much both stressors are affecting the same me-
of the available evidence, indicates that the chanism in the same way. In other words,
effect of high anxiety is exactly the oppo- predictable interactions between stressors
site: anxious individuals find it unusually can strengthen the argument that they are
difficult to concentrate on the task in hand. affecting the processing system in the same
Anxious subjects report that they spend a way.
smaller percentage of the available time This strategy fairly often suggests the ex-
than non-anxious subjects attending to the istence of a general factor of arousal, but
task (Deffenbacher 1978), and they also there are sufficient exceptions to indicate
seem to engage in more off-task glancing that more than one arousal mechanism must
(Nottelman & Hill 1977). be postulated. For example, Wilkinson and
A second strange prediction concerning Colquhoun (1968) used the five-choice seri-
the effects of anxiety on attentional pro- al reaction task, and there were two stressors
cesses is that high-anxiety subjects should (incentives provided by knowledge of re-
be less distractible than low-anxiety sub- sults and alcohol). Incentives are known to
jects. While the avialable data are rather in- increase arousal, whereas alcohol is a corti-
consistent, there is evidence in some studies cal depressant. It was accordingly predicted
(e.g. Dornic 1977; Pallak et al. 1975) that that any impairment in performance under
anxiety can lead to increased rather than de- alcohol would be due to suboptimal arousal
creased distractibility. Pallak et al. found and thus would be reduced by incentives. In
that anxious subjects performed the Stroop fact, exactly the opposite result was ob-
test (a naming task in which each colour tained, with incentives increasing the ad-
name is printed in ink of a different colour, verse effect of alcohol.
such as 'red' printed in green) better than The implication of this kind of finding is
non-anxious subjects under low distraction that a single arousal mechanism is probably
but performed it wor~e under high distrac- insufficient. This is also suggested by a com-
tion. parison of some of the effects of noise,
Easterbrook's hypothesis has been ex- which increases arousal, and sleeplessness,
tremely influential and has been used as the which decreases arousal. They are similar in
basis for several subsequent theories. In es- that prolonged work is necessary before
sence, arousal usually does lead to in- they show their effects but dissimilar in that
Broadbent: Decision and Stress 55

UPPER MECHANISM
which compensates
tor sub - or supra- ~
optimal activity of
lower mechanism

~
Fig. 4.2. Two hypothetical interrelated LOWER MECHANISM
arousal mechanisms: upper and lower. (Affected by noise
INPUT and sleeplessness) OUTPUT
(Broadbent 1971)

the effects are quite different when they do lure of either noise or sleeplessness to affect
appear. performance at the start of the work period
Further puzzling findings concern the ef- is attributable to the efficient state of the up-
fects of individual differences in introver- per mechanism at that time. Mter prolonged
sion-extraversion on performance. Intro- monotonous work, the opposite effects of
verts are usually regarded as more cortically noise and sleeplessness on performance be-
aroused than extraverts (H. J. Eysenck gin to appear because the developif).g ineffi-
1967), and the effects of introversion-extra- ciency of the upper mechanism prevents ap-
version appear to be closely related to the ef- propriate compensatory activity occurring.
fects of time of day and circadian rhythms The upper mechanism can compensate
of arousal. However, the effects of introver- much more readily for inefficiency of the
sion-extraversion are only tenuously related lower mechanism on unpaced tasks, where
to those of sleeplessness. the subject can make up for brief periods of
Broadbent (1971) suggested that many of slow performance, than on paced tasks,
these anomalous findings could be account- where he cannot do this. Mirsky and Ros-
ed for by postulating two interrelated arous- void (1960) found that barbiturates (depres-
al mechanisms (see Fig. 4.2). The 'lower' me- sant drugs which first affect cortical centres)
chanism is concerned with the execution of had more effect on the unpaced than on the
well-established decision processes, and it is paced version of a task. This suggests that
this mechanism which is affected by noise barbiturates influence the upper mecha-
and by sleeplessness. Sleeplessness, which is nism.
a low arousal state, produces very cautious In contrast, Mirksy and Rosvold found
and unreactive decision criteria, whereas the that tranquiliziers, which lower activity in
arousing agent noise leads to a very risky the brain stem, mainly affected the paced
and hyper-reactive state. version, and the same was true of sleepless-
A crucial assumption is that the conse- ness. It may be that the effects of sleepless-
quences of inefficiency of the lower me- ness and of tranquilizers are mediated by
chanism will not become manifest in per- the lower mechanism.
formance provided that the 'upper' mecha- One of the most interesting implications
nism remains in an efficient state. The task of the theory proposed by Broadbent (al-
of the upper mechanism is to monitor and though not emphasized by him) is that
alter the parameters of the lower mechanism behavioural measures can be inadequate.
in order to maintain a given standard of per- Precisely the same level of performance can
formance. Introversion-extraversion and al- be achieved in two very different ways: effi-
cohol may both affect the upper mechanism cient functioning of the lower mechanism
rather than the lower mechanism. combined with modest use of the upper me-
This theory can account for the relative chanism or inefficient functioning of the
independence of the effects of introversion- lower mechanism which is compensated for
extraversion and sleeplessness: the former by extensive involvement of the upper me-
affects the upper mechanism, whereas the chanism.
latter affects the lower mechanism. The fai- Broadbent (1971) agreed with Easter-
56 Theories of Arousal and Performance

brook (1959) that one of the major effects of away from that of the task at hand as 'activa-
arousal is on attentional selectivity: "The tion' increases under conditions in which
aroused system devotes a higher proportion deteriorating effect has been observed"
of its time to the intake of information from (p. 160).
dominant sources and less from relatively What kinds offactors produce this shift in
minor ones" (p. 433). He also proposed that the focus of behaviour away from the main
noise, and perhaps other stressors, made task? Niiiitiinen pointed out that arousal is
perceptual selection more difficult: "The often produced by using a non-task source
difficulty in noise is one of selecting one of of stimulation (e.g. intense white noise,
the stimuli present for reaction and ignoring hand dynamometer) which may distract at-
another, and not one of changes in response tention. Anxiety reactions, intentional at-
bias causing performance to be inefficient" tempts to try harder and divided attention
(p.431). (Le. the subject simultaneously performing
The importance of perceptual selection the task and observing himself performing
was suggested by the results of a number of the task) are all more likely to occur in states
experiments. In one study, Broadbent and of high activation; all of these factors will
Gregory (1965) presented a mixture of red interfere with task performance.
and white digits and asked for recall of the Mandler's (1975) explanation of the detri-
digits presented in one of the colours. Noise mental effects of high arousal on perform-
produced a deterioration in performance, ance similarly argued that distraction
but this was reversed when subjects were played a major role. He claimed that arousal
asked to recall the digits irrespective of col- generated internal cues. As arousal in-
our. creases, more internal cues are created, and
Obviously, Broadbent hs made a number there is an increasing tendency for the sub-
of important contributions to our under- ject to pay attention to his own arousal cues
standing of the effects of arousal on per- rather than to the task.
formance. His distinction between upper Niiiitiinen (1973) reported some empirical
and lower mechanisms of arousal is espe- findings which provided support for his the-
cially interesting, because it sheds some oretical viewpoint. Two activities had to be
light on the often surprisingly small detri- performed at the same time: riding a bicycle
mental effects of stressors on performance. with an ergometer set at different power
levels and a simple reaction-time task. The
various cycling conditions produced sub-
stantial differences in arousal (as measured
4.4 Naatanen's Theory
by heart rate), with reaction-time lengthen-
ing considerably under the more arousing
Niiiitiinen (1973) addressed himself primari- conditions.
ly to the assertion that there is an opti- There are two possible kinds of explana-
mal level of activation or arousal for any tion of this result: (1) supra-optimal arousal
task, above which there is an impairment of directly impaired performance or (2) the
performance (Yerkes and Dodson 1908). It more demanding cycling conditions led sub-
has often been assumed that this perform- jects to direct more attention to the cycling
ance decrement at high levels of arousal is task and away from the reaction-time task,
directly attributable to the level of arousal producing an increasing divergence be-
per se. Niiiitiinen argued for a theoretical tween the actual patterning of activation
position in which over-arousal is regarded and that required by the reaction-time task.
as impairing performance in a rather more Niiiitiinen attempted to distinguish be-
indirect fashion. In his own words, the over- tween these two explanations in a further
arousal effect is "an artifact of the change of experiment. This time the cycling activity
direction in behaviour farther and farther immediately preceded the reaction-time
Kahneman: Attention and Effort 57
task but did not overlap with it in time. In tive thought. Another difficulty with
this experiment, there was no relationship Niiiitiinen's views is their relative untestabil-
between arousal (heart rate) and reaction ity; it is unclear how one determines either a
time, in spite of the fact that heart rate had subject's qualitative patterning of activation
been varied almost as much as possible in or the optimal qualitative patterning for any
the waking subject. Thus the impairment of given task.
performance in the previous experiment
was probably due to the distracting effect of
the cycling task rather than to the intensity
of arousal. 4.5 Kahneman: Attention and Effort
Niiiitiinen (1973) concluded that there
had been far too much emphasis in previous
theories on the notion of arousal as a uni-di- Kahneman (1973) proposed an interesting
mensional measure varying only in intensi- theoretical framework which dealt with
ty. There are important qualitative differ- some of the major interrelationships among
ences among activation states, so that an ex- motivational, attentional and arousal pro-
clusive focus on quantitative variations in cesses. He assumed that theories of atten-
arousal is doomed to failure. Performance tion must consider both the selectivity and
of a task ultimately depends on the appropi- the intensity or capacity of attention and
rateness of the activation pattern of the or- that "the intensive aspect of attention corre-
ganism with respect to that task's demands. sponds to effort" (p. 4). The amount of ca-
Niiiitiinen is right to argue that the detri- pacity or effort supplied to the task is deter-
mental effects of high arousal on perform- mined primarily by the evaluation of de-
ance have often been interpreted in grossly mands at a cognitive level. Task difficulty
over-simplified ways. There are undoubted- will typically play the main role in determin-
ly many different reasons why high arousal ing effort, but incentive or motivational fac-
impairs performance, and he has delineated tors may also be involved.
some of the main factors. Furthermore, he is Some important aspects of Kahneman's
probably correct in proposing that the quali- theorizing are shown in Fig. 4.3. Effort in-
tative patterning of arousal is a more conse- creases fairly steadily as the processing
quential determinant of performance than is demands of the main or primary task
the quantitative or intensive aspect of arous- increase due to greater task difficulty or
al. complexity; however, as these demands
On the other hand, it is not immediately increase, the discrepancy between the effort
obvious that there are no detrimental effects or capacity required and the effort that is
of high arousal per se on the performance of actually supplied becomes greater. There
many tasks; for example, high arousal is an inverse relationship between the
seems incompatible with quiet and reflec- effort supplied to the main task and the
/~ SUPPLY = DEMAND
/
o /
/
W /
-l /
/
a.. /
a.. // _._._._.-.- TOTAL CAPACITY
:::>
til ,/-'-'
_.--.-; _ _ CAPACITY SUPPLIED
>-
I- -' // TO PRIMARY TASK
/
U /
<{ /
a.. /
<{
U

DEMAND OF PRIMARY TASK


Fig. 4.3. Supply of capacity in response to the demands of the primary task. (Kahneman 1973)
58 Theories of Arousal and Performance

spare capacity or effort that is available for task decrements were obtained when the tar-
processing subsidiary tasks or simply moni- get stimulus was sighted through an artifi-
toring the external environment. cial pupil.
In answer to the question of how one can In addition to using pupillary dilation as
index the amount of effort invested in task an index of intra-task effort, there have been
performance, Kahneman (1973) argued that attempts to investigate the viability of pupil-
increased effort would tend to produce lary dilation as a measure of inter-task dif-
physiological arousal. However, most mea- ferences in difficulty or processing de-
sures of arousal are also sensitive to factors mands. A recent study by Beatty and Wag-
other than effort. He proposed pupillary di- oner (1978) used a task devised by Posner
lation as the most generally suitable and (1978) in which subjects have to decide
sensitive measure, in spite of the fact that whether or not two letters belong to the
pupil dilation occurs in response to more same category (vowels or consonants).
than 20 different factors. 'Same' judgements can sometimes be based
Reasonable evidence that pupillary dila- purely on feature analysis (e.g. 'AA' or 'aa'),
tion reflects intra-task fluctuations in effort sometimes on name code extraction (e.g.
was obtained in a series of studies by 'Aa' or 'aA') or may require evaluation of
Kahneman and his co-workers (Kahneman category membership (e.g. 'AE' or 'ui').
1970; Kahneman et al. 1967; Kahneman et Posner (1978) suggested that subjects per-
al. 1969). The main task was a digit transfor- formed a hierarchical series of processes,
mation task in which subjects listened to a beginning with feature analysis and pro-
series of four digits (e.g. '4826') and re- ceeding to name code extraction and finally
sponded by adding one to each digit (e.g. to category membership. If a match is ob-
'5937'). During list presentation, there was a tained at any point, processing is halted,
subsidiary task which involved either moni- and a response is made. Beatty and Wagon-
toring a display for a specified letter or re- er (1978) obtained the greatest pupillary di-
taining a single letter that was presented lation in the most difficult condition, in
briefly. which all three analyses must be performed
On the assumption that task demands in- (i.e. match only at the category level), and
creased progressively as each additional the least dilation in the easiest condition, in
digit was presented, the predicted results which only feature analysis is necessary (i.e.
can be deduced by reference to Fig. 4.3. As match at the level offeature analysis). While
expected, pupil dilation increased steadily the extent of pupillary dilation corre-
during digit presentation, whereas perform- sponded to apparent task difficulty, there is
ance on the subsidiary tasks declined pro- a confounding between difficulty and pro-
gressively the further into the digit presenta- cessing time, with the most difficult task tak-
tion trial that the critical stimulus occurred. ing the longest time.
The latter finding follows from the postulat- If pupillary dilation does genuinely re-
ed inverse relationship between effort or ca- flect processing effort, then it follows that
pacity expenditure on the main task and attempts to manipulate effort by monetary
performance on the subsidiary task, which incentives should affect dilation in predict-
depends on the amount of spare processing able ways. Kahneman and Peavler (1969)
capacity. varied the monetary incentives within a
There is an alternative interpretation of paired associate learning task, and obtained
this finding based on the notion that the greater pupillary dilation for high-reward
worsening performance on the subsidiary pairs than for low-reward pairs.
task is due to decreased perceptual sensitivi- There are alternative ways of measuring
ty when the pupil is dilated. Disconfirming effort. The simplest method of all is to use
evidence was obtained by Kahneman self-report scales on which subjects provide
(1970), who found that similar subsidiary- an estimate of perceived effort during task
Kahneman: Attention and Effort 59
performance (Dornic 1977). A further meth- category of theories of attention which em-
od is based upon the assumption that spare phasize the importance of capacity limita-
processing capacity is inversely related to tions on man's ability to perform mental
the effort invested in the primary task; this work. It differs from previous theories of the
spare processing capacity can be measured same general type in two main ways:
by adding a concurrent processing task I. Rather than assuming that capacity
which is performed only when it is possible limitations or 'bottlenecks' occur almost ex-
to do so without disrupting performance of clusively at a particular stage of information
the primary task. processing, as Broadbent (1958) and
While Kahneman (1973) emphasized the Deutsch and Deutsch (1963) did, Kahne-
concept of effort, he incorporated it within a man assumed that a flexible allocation of
more general framework, of which the two limited capacity occurs, so that the source of
central aspects are the allocation policy and any bottleneck can be quite variable.
the evaluation of demands on capacity. The 2. The assumption is made that the mo-
evaluation of demands causes effort or ca- mentarily available processing capacity va-
pacity to be supplied in order to accomplish ries substantially over time, rather than be-
the activities that the allocation policy has ing fixed; such factors as task demands and
selected. The allocation policy itself is in effort expenditure partially determine avail-
turn controlled by four factors: able capacity.
1. Enduring dispositions which are based A controversial prediction from Kahne-
on modes of functioning of involuntary man's general theory is that the ability to
processes (e.g. attention tends to be paid perform two tasks together in a divided at-
to novel, surprising or incongruous stimu- tention paradigm depends primarily on the
li). attentional or effort demands of the tasks
2. Momentary intentions (e.g. look for a between which attention must be divided.
white cat). This contrasts with an alternative viewpoint
3. Evaluation of demands, such that one ac- (McLeod 1977, 1978), according to which
tivity is carried through to completion if the ability to perform two concurrent tasks
two or more activities require in toto more depends on the extent to which the two tasks
capacity than is available. are similar and use the same processing me-
4. Effects of arousal produced by external chanisms. In fairness to Kahneman, he does
stressors, including an increased tenden- acknowledge the existence of such specific
cy to focus on a few relevant cues, a re- interference effects; however, his main em-
duced ability to discriminate betweefl phasis is on general capacity limitations.
relevant and irrelevant cues and in- The evidence on this important issue was
creased attentionallability. discussed in the previous chapter; broadly
The above considerations can be applied speaking, there is good evidence in favour
to the. curvilinear relationship between of both general capacity limitations and spe-
arousal and performance (Yerkes and Dod- cific interference effects.
son 1908), in which moderate levels of In many ways, Kahneman's theory re-
arousal are associated with the optimal level presents an impressive extension of earlier
of performance. In essence, Kahneman hypotheses, and it is plausible to assume
(1973) argued that poor task performance that there is an important distinction be-
under low levels of arousal is typically attri- tween arousal as produced by an individu-
butable to insufficient effort, whereas inferi- al's active involvement in task performance
or performance under high arousal is due to and arousal generated by intense or stressful
an amalgam of attentional narrowing, external stimulation. The notion that the ef-
heightened lability of attention and im- fective processing capacity varies as a func-
paired attentional discrimination. tion of effort expenditure is an attractive
Kahneman's theory belongs to the broad one, and the idea that performance decre-
60 Theories of Arousal and Performance

ments under high arousal may have several sights, and so he devised the Activation
different causes is more realistic than previ- Deactivation Adjective Check List (AD-
ous approaches. ACL). Individuals are asked to rate each of
The key concept of 'effort' is not defined a series of arousal-related adjectives on a
as precisely as one would like. At times it four-point scale according to how it de-
seems to be synonymous with 'concentra- scribes their feelings at the moment the test
tion', but at other times it is used in a rather is taken.
broader sense, as when Kahneman refers to On the basis of a factor analysis with an
"a nonspecific input, which may be various- orthogonal rotation, Thayer (1967) con-
ly labeled 'effort', 'capacity', or 'attention'." cluded that there were four arousal or acti-
Dornic (1977) made the reasonable point vation factors. These factors (with high-
that rather than equating the concepts of'ef- loading adjectives in brackets) were as fol-
fort' and 'attentional capacity', it is prefer- lows: General Activation (lively, active, full
able to distinguish the two concepts and to of pep, energetic, peppy, vigorous, activat-
argue that effort is the cause of increased at- ed), Deactivation-Sleep (sleepy, tired, drow-
tention. For Dornic, 'effort' denotes "a con- sy), High Activation (clutched up, jittery,
sciously and deliberately initiated activa- stirred up, fearful, intense), and General
tion of a person's information processing Deactivation (at rest, still, leisurely, quies-
power, which results in a certain degree of cent, quiet, calm, placid).
attention being focussed on a specific task" Thayer (1970) related these four arousal
(p.2). factors to physiological arousal in a study in
It is often difficult to use Kahneman's which skin conductance, heart rate, muscle
theory to make accurate predictions of be- action potentials and finger blood volume
haviour. On the one hand, increased effort were recorded while the subjects were sit-
should generally improve performance; on ting quietly or while they performed a men-
the other hand, increased arousal produced tal arithmetic task in the presence of noise.
by an external stres~or will often reduce per- The subjects completed an AD-ACL at the
formance because of changes in the alloca- end of each phase of the experiment.
tion policy (e.g. heightened lability of atten- As is frequently the case, the various
tion and attentional narrowing). What on physiological measures did not intercorre-
earth is going to happen when subjects are late highly; indeed, none of the correlations
exposed to factors (e.g. failure feedback) among the physiological variables exceeded
which produce increased effort and in- + 0.15. In contrast, an amalgam based on
creased stress? In such circumstances, there information from all four variables correlat-
is increased attentional capacity coupled ed + 0.47 with the general activation factor
with impaired attentional selectivity, so that of the AD-ACL and + 0.34 with the high ac-
performance might improve, worsen or re- tivation factor. This pattern of results clear-
main unaffected. ly suggests that self-report measures of
arousal have some important advantages
over physiological measures. As Thayer
(1970) concluded, "These results lend evi-
4.6 Thayer: Self-Reported Arousal dence to the contention that self report may
be an integrative variable more representa-
tive of general states of bodily activation
Thayer's (1967, 1970) approach to theoreti- than any single psychophysiological var-
cal issues surrounding the concept of iable" (p. 93).
arousal has been rather different from any Some of Thayer's contentions have been
of the other approaches discussed in this disputed. In particular, the notion that there
chapter. He has consistently claimed that are four separate arousal or activation fac-
self-report data can provide valuable in- tors has not met with universal approbation.
Thayer: Self-Reported Arousal 61
Mackay et al. (1978), using an Anglicized plete the AD-ACL, together with seven
version of the AD-ACL, obtained only two other scales constructed a priori to measure
bipolar factors rather than four monopolar pleasure, displeasure, arousal, sleepiness,
ones; they identified these factors as arousal dominance, submissiveness and depression.
and stress. The arousal factor combined Russell (1979) discovered that degree of
Thayer's general activation and deactiva- arousal and pleasure-displeasure were or-
tion-sleep factors, whereas the stress factor thogonal factors which formed a two-di-
was a synthesis of Thayer's high activation mensional space accounting for nearly all of
and general deactivation factors. the reliable variance. The way in which
Thayer (1978a, b) has independently ar- Thayer's arousal factors related to this two-
rived at an essentially identical solution to dimensional space is shown in Fig. 4.4. The
that of Mackay et al. on the basis of further main implication of these findings is that
factor analytic studies. Thayer now distin- there may not be a number of different
guishes between Activation Dimension A arousal dimensions; rather, Thayer's arous-
(ranging from energetic and vigorous to al factors may simply reflect different loca-
sleepy and tired) and Activation Dimen- tions on the pleasure-displeasure continu-
sion B (ranging from tense to placid and um in combination with variations on a
still). single arousal dimension.
An alternative conceptualization of Thay- Thayer (1978a) reviewed the most inter-
er's arousal factors that seems quite plausi- esting research which has been done to vali-
ble was offered by Russell (1979). His study date the claim that there are two separate ac-
took as its starting point the factor analytic tivation dimensions. It has sometimes been
data of Russell and Mehrabian (1977), who assumed that a variety of manipulations
found that all emotions could be accurately such as time of day, sleep deprivation, noise,
described on the basis of a three-dimension- incentive and stimulant and depressant
al emotional space consisting of the follow- drugs all affect the same arousal dimension.
ing dimensions: degree of arousal, pleasure- In contrast, Thayer's theoretical stance al-
displeasure and dominance-submissive- lows for the possibility that many of these
ness. In an attempt to relate Thayer's AD- arousing agents affect one of the two activa-
ACL factors to this three-dimensional tion dimensions more than the other.
space, Russell (1979) asked subjects to com- Consider as an example the effects of
HIGH
AROUSAL

GENERAL
• ACTIVATION

• HIGH
ACTIVATION

DISPLEASURE PLEASURE

Fig. 4.4. The location of Thayer's DEACTIVATION- • • GENERAL


arousal factors within a two-dimen- SLEEP DEACTIVATION
sional space comprising the ortho-
gonal factors of arousal and plea- LOW
sure-displeasure. (Russell 1979) AROUSAL
62 Theories of Arousal and Performance

time of day on self-reported arousal. Thayer periments, but Dimension A activation was
(1978a) asked students to complete the AD- unaffected. Lopes (1971) used even more in-
ACL immediately upon awaking in the tense noise stimulation (85 - 88 dB) and
morning and just before retiring at night, found that noise had differential effects on
and every 2 h in between. Peak arousal on the two activation dimensions: Dimen-
both activation dimensions occurred at sion B activation was increased, whereas
around mid-day for most subjects, and the Dimension A activation was decreased. It
overall pattern of time of day effects was thus appears that noise consistently in-
similar for both dimensions. However, diur- creases Dimension B activation, but it has
nal variation was considerably greater with smaller and more variable effects on Dimen-
respect to Activation Dimension A (energy- sion A activation.
sleep) than to Activation Dimension B (ten- An additional manipulation which has
sion-placidity). been found to influence Dimension B acti-
In a second study, Thayer (1978a) investi- vation more than Dimension A activation
gated the effects of physical exercise on self- was investigated by Thayer and Moore
reported arousal. Students completed the (1972). They produced three levels of anxie-
AD-ACL on two occasions, once after they ty through experimental manipulations
had been sitting quietly and once immedi- while subjects filled in the AD-ACL. Di-
ately after completing a moderately brisk mension B activation increased progressive-
walk around the university campus. This ly as the level of experimentally induced
physical exercise produced a substantial in- anxiety increased, whereas Dimension A
crease in self-reported arousal on Activa- activation increased from low to moderate
tion Dimension A but had a very small ef- anxiety conditions but decreased as manip-
fect on Activation Dimension B. ulated anxiety went from moderate to high.
Thus far, we have seen that time of day The discovery that a number of arousing
and physical exercise have more pro- agents have differential effects on Thayer's
nounced effects on Activation Dimension A two activation dimensions is of potential im-
than on Activation Dimension B. Are there portance. As Thayer (1978a) pointed out,
other arousal manipulations that affect the experiments in the area of information pro-
B dimension (i.e. tension-placidity) more cessing, "have incorporated a variety of
than the A dimension? Thayer (1978a) has arousal manipulations including time of
provided an affirmative answer to this ques- day, sleep deprivation, noise, incentive, and
tion by extending the range of arousing various drugs (Broadbent 1971)... It is risky
agents investigated. For example, students to assume that all these manipulations af-
who completed the AD-ACL just before a fect a single activation dimension by either
college examination showed greatly elevat- increasing or decreasing arousal as a linear
ed scores on Dimension B, but 'examination function of different intensities of stimula-
nerves' had essentially no effect on Dimen- tion. Activation Dimension A probably me-
sion A scores. diates time of day, sleep deprivation, and in-
What about the effects of white noise on centive where no extra anxiety-inducing
self-reported arousal? Thayer et al. (1970) treatments are employed. On the other
found that the main effect of 75-dB white hand, noise, and possibly certain kinds of
noise presented continuously for 20 min drugs (e.g., tranquilizers) could be expected
was to produce a significant increase in Di- to primarily influence Dimension B activa-
mension B activation. A somewhat more in- tion" (p. 31).
tense level of noise (85 dB) was used over a Although the two activation dimensions
period of approximately 20 min in two other are claimed to be conceptually distinct,
experiments (Patterson 1974, Thayer and Thayer (1978a) hypothesized that they were
Carey 1974). Reliable increases in Dimen- positively correlated at moderate levels of
sion B activation were obtained in both ex- energy expenditure but negatively correlat-
Hasher and Zacks: Automatic and Effortful Processes 63
ed at high levels (i.e. high tension is associat- one end of this continuum there are auto-
ed with low energy and vigour, and high en- matic processes, and at the other end
ergy and vigour is associated with low ten- there are effortful processes.
sion). This inverse relationship was demon- 2. Attentional capacity varies both within
strated in one of the studies already dis- and between individuals.
cussed (Thayer and Moore 1972). It was al- The first assumption follows from the
so shown in an unpublished study by Thay- work of Posner and Snyder (1975a), Shiffrin
er and Wettler. They found that females and Schneider (1977) and others, with the
when feeling very depressed reported great minor exception that a continuum of mental
tension (high Dimension B activation) com- operations is postulated rather than the
bined with low energy and vigour (low Di- more usual dichotomy into conscious or
mension A activation). controlled and automatic. Automatic pro-
Since Activation Dimension A appears to cesses are seen as requiring minimal energy
be related to motivational factors, whereas from the limited-capacity attentional me-
Activation Dimension B is similar to an chanism. They occur without intention,
anxiety dimension, it might be anticipated without necessarily giving rise to awareness
that Dimension A activation would typical- and without interfering with other process-
ly facilitate performance, whereas Dimen- ing, and they do not benefit from practice.
sion B activation would impair perform- In contrast, effortful operations require con-
ance. In fact, Dimension A activation has siderable attentional capacity and so tend to
complex effects on cognitive tasks, and Di- interfere with other cognitive activities also
mension B activation often has no effect at requiring capacity. The efficiency of effort-
all (for a review see Thayer 1978a). This may ful operations increases with practice, and
be due in part to the fact that high levels of their use is voluntary, often occurring only
tension or anxiety are unlikely to occur nat- with specific instructions.
urally during the course of an experiment. The second assumption, that attentional
It is a shortcoming of Thayer's approach capacity is variable rather than fixed, fol-
that he has as yet shed little light on the ef- lows from the research and theorizing of
fects on performance of arousal occurring Kahneman (1973). Hasher and Zacks (1979)
along each of the activation dimensions. related the notion of variable attentional ca-
However, his work represents a promising pacity to arousal in the following theoretical
first step in the task of unravelling some of statement: "At low levels of arousal, capaci-
the complexities of arousal. ty seems to increase with increasing levels of
arousal. At high levels, a reduction in func-
tional attentional capacity occurs" (p. 364).
They pointed out that the reduced available
4.7 Hasher and Zacks: Automatic and capacity in states of high arousal could oc-
Effortful Processes cur either because of a straightforward re-
duction in total attentional capacity or be-
cause capacity has been taken from the
Hasher and Zacks (1979) have proposed cognitive system and applied elsewhere in
an ambitious theory to account for the ef- the total physical system. In either case, the
fects of age differences, depression and predictions from their theory are the same.
arousal on memory, but the focus here will The upshot of these various theoretical
be largely on that part of their theory con- assumptions is that the reduced attentional
cerned with arousal. The general theory in- capacity characteristic of high arousal will
corporates two basic assumptions as fol- impair those processes requiring attentional
lows: capacity (i.e. effortful processes) but will
1. There is a continuum of attentional re- have no effect on those processes not requir-
quirements among encoding processes; at ing attentional capacity (i.e. automatic pro-
64 Theories of Arousal and Performance

cesses). Hasher and Zacks argued that this ies were discussed by Eysenck (1977). It has
general viewpoint is supported by the fact been found on tests of imagery that intro-
that trainers of divers, pilots, firemen, para- verts (allegedly chronically aroused) re-
chutists and other groups of people exposed port less vivid imagery than extraverts. Voi-
to high levels of stress place great emphasis cu and Vranceanu (1975) investigated the
on the need for high levels of overlearning learning of lists of pictures and words in
of emergency procedures. conditions of quiet or of noise, with the par-
What kinds of automatic and effortful ticipants being divided into physiologically
processes are involved in the main area of hyper-reactive and hypo-reactive groups.
research discussed by Hasher and Zacks High arousal (hyper-reactive subjects ex-
(1979), namely learning and memory? They posed to noise) impaired picture learning
claimed that automatic processes encode but had no effect on word learning, suggest-
the fundamental aspects of the flow of in for- ing that imaginal processes may be detri-
mation, i.e. spatial, temporal and frequency- mentally affected by high arousal.
of-occurrence information. As a result of ex- Hasher and Zacks (1979) claimed quite
tended practice, word meanings are also au- erroneously that there were no relevant data
tomatically activated. There is, indeed, rea- with respect to their prediction that high
sonable evidence that these different kinds arousal should impair rehearsal processes.
of information are processed automatically, It has been known for some time that sub-
at least in terms of the criteria for automati- jects exposed to arousing stimulation from
city used by Hasher and Zacks. The effortful white noise tend to remember more infor-
orocesses involved in learning and memory mation about the order of presentation of
include imagery, rehearsal, organization stimuli (Hamilton et al. 1972), and it seems
and mnemonic techniques. likely that a major function of rehearsal is to
Does high arousal impair the use of ef- preserve order information.
fortful organizational processes? Hasher More recently, Wilding and Mohindra
and Zacks (1979) argued that there were on- (unpublished) compared the effects of 65
ly two relevant studies (Hormann and Oster- and 85 dbC white noise on ordered recall of
kamp 1966, Schwartz 1975) and that both letter strings. High arousal (85 dbC noise)
had obtained confirmatory evidence. When was associated with superior recall under
a categorized word list is presented (i.e. control conditions, but this superiority dis-
several words belonging to each of a num- appeared when rehearsal was prevented by
ber of categories), the subsequent free recall engaging the subject in an articulatory sup-
tends to be organized in terms of the catego- pression task (i.e. rapid repetition of the
ries in the list. This organizational tendency word 'the') during list presentation. This
(often referred to as 'categorical clustering') suggests that high arousal in the form of in-
presumably requires effortful processing, tense white noise leads to greater use of re-
and both Hormann and Osterkamp (1966) hearsal processes, which seems opposed to
and Schwartz (1975) found that high arousal the prediction of Hasher and Zacks (1979).
significantly reduced categorical clustering. Why does not high arousal consistently
A similar finding was obtained by Ey- reduce the utilization of rehearsal processes
senck (1974a), who asked subjects to think as predicted by Hasher and Zacks (1979)? A
of as many words as possible belonging to plausible way of salvaging their theory is
five semantic categories. He found that suggested by reference to the working mem-
there was reduced categorical clustering ory model of Baddeley and Hitch (1974),
among the most aroused subjects. which comprises a modality-free central
Hasher and Zacks (1979) also predicted processor and an articulatory loop. Hasher
that high arousal would inhibit the use of and Zacks appear to be suggesting that high
imagery but claimed that there was no rele- arousal reduces the capacity of the modali-
vant evidence. In fact, some pertinent stud- ty-free central processor while leaving the
Hasher and Zacks: Automatic and Effortful Processes 65

articulatory loop intact. If so, they could fectively put forward two closely related hy-
reasonably argue that high arousal impairs potheses concerning the effects of high
only those rehearsal processes that involve arousal on task performance: (1) high
the use of the central processor but does not arousal impairs all processes making de-
reduce usage of the relatively effort-free re- mands on attentional capacity and (2) high
hearsal processes of the articulatory loop. arousal has no effect on automatic pro-
According to Hasher and Zacks (1979), cesses. The evidence relating to the first hy-
high arousal should have no effect on auto- pothesis is broadly supportive, with the ex-
matic processes such as the activation of ception of data on the effects of arousal on
word meaning. They argued that the most rehearsal processes. At the very least, the
persuasive evidence that meaning is auto- theory should distinguish between rehearsal
matically derived from printed words comes processes solely dependent on the articula-
from the Stroop phenomenon. This refers to tory loop and those involving the central
the difficulty which most people experience processor.
in naming the ink colour when a colour There is as yet very limited evidence relat-
word is printed in ink of a different colour ing directly to the second hypothesis. How-
(e.g. 'red' printed in green). ever, such evidence as is available suggests
While Hasher and Zacks (1979) claimed that it is not true that high arousal has no ef-
that the available evidence supported their fect on automatic processes; rather, high
prediction that high arousal would not af- arousal seems to make it easier to inhibit the
fect the Stroop effect, this is not strictly ac- operation of automatic processes when this
curate. Indeed, intense white noise im- is desirable for efficient task performance.
proves the speed with which the ink colour At a more general level, it seems unlikely
of colour words can be named (Agnew and that reality can be quite as straightforward
Agnew 1963, Callaway and Stone 1960, as Hasher and Zacks (1979) implied. For ex-
Houston 1969, Houston and Jones 1967). ample, there are several studies in which the
This improvement in performance seems to effects of arousal on two concurrently per-
be due to an enhanced ability to inhibit ir- formed effortful tasks have been assessed,
relevant responses under intense noise; some of which are reviewed by Easterbrook
when there is no need to inhibit such re- (1959). While Hasher and Zacks would pres-
sponses (as when subjects have to name the umably predict that high arousal would im-
colours of non-word stimuli), then perform- pair the performance of both tasks, the typi-
ance is actually worse in noise than in quiet cal finding is that performance on the main
(Houston 1969, Houston and Jones 1967). task is maintained or improved, whereas
Intense noise is not the only arousing subsidiary task performance is impaired. It
agent that has been found to influence per- is unclear how Hasher and Zacks (1979)
formance on the Stroop test. Folkard and would account for this pattern of perform-
Greeman (1974) looked at the speed with ance.
which stimuli could be sorted either on the According to Hasher and Zacks (1979),
basis of colour name or in terms of ink co- high arousal must impair the performance
lour and found that sorting was faster on the of all effortful tasks. In fact, high arousal
basis of colour name. Of more immediate in- has sometimes been found to enhance the
terest, high arousal in the form of induced performance of attentionally demanding
muscle tension led to increased speed of tasks. For example, Hamilton et al. (1977)
sorting on the basis of ink colour but to re- found that intense white noise improved
duced sorting speed of colour names. Fol- performance on the non-automatic task of
kard and Greeman interpreted their data by transforming single letters (e.g. 'C + 3 = 1';
suggesting that muscle tension inhibits sup- answer = 'F').
posedly automatic naming responses. One puzzling aspect of the theory put for-
In sum, Hasher and Zacks (1979) have ef- ward by Hasher and Zacks concerns the as-
66 Theories of Arousal and Performance

sumption that high arousal reduces the use Until fairly recently most theories in this
of imagery, rehearsal, organizational pro- area incorporated a unitary concept of
cesses and mnemonic techniques. Since all arousal. This simplistic approach was justi-
four forms of processing have been found to fied by two discoveries: (1) different arousal
enhance long-term memory, the natural pre- manipulations often produced similar beha-
diction is that high arousal should lead to re- vioural patterns and (2) arousers frequently
duced long-term retention of information. interacted with one another in theoretically
In fact, one of the most robust findings in predictable ways. However, as a result of an
the arousal literature is that high arousal in- increasing number of anomalous findings,
creases long-term retention (for a review, see there has been a growing tendency to postu-
Eysenck 1976a). late two or more partially independent
The greatest weakness in the theoretical arousal systems. The theories of Broadbent,
position of Hasher and Zacks concerns their Kahneman and Thayer all fall into that cate-
assumption that increasing arousal leads gory.
first to an increase in functional attentional If there are a number of arousal systems,
capacity, followed by a decrease. In prac- what do they look like? One possibility is
tice, we rarely have any independent mea- that there is one arousal system which is af-
sure of attentional capacity, so that there is a fected primarily by intense or stressful ex-
natural temptation to argue backwards from ternal stimulation or by various drugs. This
the behavioural data. Thus we can 'explain' arousal system corresponds closely to the
good performance in terms of increased at- Lower arousal mechanism postulated by
tentional capacity, and poor performance in Broadbent (1971) and may resemble Thay-
terms of decreased attentional capacity. er's Activation Dimension B (a tension-
Even if the assumption about the effects of placidity dimension). The arousal level in
arousal on attentional capacity turns out to this system will be more or less appropriate
be correct, we are still left with the puzzle of for performance depending on task de-
trying to understand why arousal should mands. If the arousal level is non-optimal,
have these effects. the second arousal system comes into opera-
tion. This second system attempts to comp-
ensate for performance decrements pro-
duced by the first arousal system by re-allo-
4.8 Summary and Conclusions cating resources in response to task de-
mands. This arousal system is similar in
In spite of the fact that an attempt is made in some ways to Broadbent's Upper arousal
Chap. 9 to integrate theory and research on system and shares common features with
arousal and performance, a few general Thayer's Activation Dimension A (an ener-
comments deserve mention at this point. In gy-sleep dimension).
the first place, several theorists have argued A very striking finding is that arousal ma-
that many of the effects of arousal on per- nipUlations often have much smaller effects
formance are mediated by attentional mech- on performance than one would expect.
anisms. While different theorists have One of the advantages of postulating a se-
tended to emphasize different possible ef- cond arousal system of a compensatory na-
fects of arousal on attention, there is reason- ture is that it provides a potential explana-
able support for the notion that high arousal tion for this finding (as well as many others).
produces increased attentional selectivity; These theoretical issues are discussed at
in addition, there are indications that arou- greater length in Chap. 9, after we have
sal also increases attentional lability and considered in detail the effects on perform-
makes attentional processes more suscepti- ance of some of the major arousal manipu-
ble to distraction. lations.
5 Incentives and Motivation

In very general terms, motivation depends gard drives as more susceptible than in-
on an amalgam of internal and external fac- stincts to environmental influence, and the
tors. Thus, for example, the motivational concept of 'drive' is still to be found in the
state known as hunger is clearly influenced contemporary literature.
by internal conditions related to the number In contrast to the prominence assigned to
ofhoursoffood deprivation and by external internal determinants of motivation by ad-
stimuli providing visual and olfactory infor- vocates of drive or instinct theories, other
mation about food. It is a meaningless ques- theorists have focussed on the part played
tion to ask whether motivational states are by external factors such as incentives and
more affected by external or by internal fac- reinforcing stimuli. The first systematic ex-
tors, since the relative importance of each position of such a viewpoint was made by
set of factors can vary within extremely wide Thorndike (1913), who formulated the well-
limits. In addition, of course, internal and known Law of Effect. This law states that
external factors typically interact with each when a particular stimUlus-response bond is
other: the incentive value of a bowl of rice is followed by a satisfying state of affairs (re-
much greater for a starving peasant in Asia ward or positive reinforcement in modem
than forabloated member ofWestern society. terminology), the strength of that bond in-
Thinking. on motivation has moved from creases. On the other hand, when a particu-
an emphasis on internal factors to a growing lar stimulus-response bond is followed by
recognition of the major role played by ex- an annoying state of affairs (negative rein-
ternal factors. One hundred years ago, moti- forcement), the strength of that bond is
vation was conceptualized largely in terms weakened.
of instinct, in keeping with the biological Thorndike recognized that there was an
and phylogenetic approach to the study of element of circularity in his formulation of
behaviour pioneered by Darwin. As time the Law of Effect: when a stimulus-re-
passed, some theorists went to great lengths sponse bond is strengthened, it is fatally
to account for behaviour by postulating an easy to claim that the response must have
enormous variety of instincts. For example, been followed by a satisfying state of affairs.
McDougall (1923) argued that there was a What is needed is some independent mea-
social (gregariousness) instinct, an assertion sure of annoyers and satisfiers, and Thorn-
instinct, a parental instinct, and so on. This dike attempted to supply this. He defined a
type of theorizing fell into disfavour when it satisfier as something the organism would
was realized that claiming that assertive be- approach, whereas an annoyer was some-
haviour was caused by an assertion instinct thing the organism would not approach and
did not constitute a proper explanation of would actively avoid. Several years later,
the behaviour in question. Thorndike abandoned the notion that an an-
The concept of 'instinct' was replaced by noyer decreases the strength of a stimulus-
the notion of 'drive', which resembled in- response bond, primarily because much of
stinct in emphasizing the role of hereditary the experimental evidence failed to provide
factors. However, most theorists seem to re- support.
68 Incentives and Motivation

This chapter addresses certain limited has been refuted many times. In the classic
questions within the field of motivation. study by Macfarlane (1930), rats initially
The emphasis is on the effects of incentive learned to run through a maze for reinforce-
manipulations on performance and on pos- ment. When the maze was then filled with
sible mechanisms mediating these effects. It water, the rats were able to swim through the
is interesting to consider whether the effects maze successfully, in spite of the fact that
of incentives on behaviour are the same as, the specific responses produced differed
or similar to, the effects of other motivation-considerably from those used previously.
al factors but that issue is beyond the scope In his theory of motivation, Bindra (1969)
of this chapter. argued that a combination of internal phy-
siological conditions and external incentive
stimulation produces a central motive state
which persists for some time. Within this
5.1 Reinforcement and Incentives framework, it is worth considering whether
initially neutral stimuli (e.g. money) which
acquire motivational properties do so by be-
It has usually been argued that there are im- coming substitutes for the organismic state
portant similarities between the behavioural component or for the incentive component
effects of incentives and of positive reinfor- of the central motive state. He has obtained
cers or rewards. Indeed, incentive motiva- evidence (Bindra and Palfai 1967) that such
tion is often defined as the energizing effect stimuli assume certain properties of the in-
on behaviour of the anticipation of reinfor- centive but do not reinstate the organismic
cing events. However, some apparently im- state.
portant differences have been identified. In sum, the process of reinforcement does
For example, the process of reinforcement not seem to be intrinsically different from
appears to work 'backwards' on responses - the process of incentive motivation. Motiva-
that precede the reinforcing event, whereas tional states are importantly determined by
inaentives work 'forwards' on events which the presentation or removal of incentive ob-
follow the motivating event. A second ap- jects or reinforcing events, together with an
parent difference between reinforcers and internal organismic state.
incentives is that reinforcers have a highly
specific effect on behaviour (i.e. directly
strengthening reinforced responses), wher-
eas motivational factors such as incentive 5.2 Learning and Memory: Atkinson
stimuli have more general energizing ef- and Wickens (1971)
fects.
Bindra (1974) has persuasively argued
that these apparent differences are illusory There has been considerable research inter-
and that the process of reinforcement est in the effects of incentive on learning
should be subsumed under the more general and memory. The major theory in this area
process of incentive motivation. He pointed was proposed by Atkinson and Wickens
out that both the 'backward-working' rein- (1971). They attempted to explain the role
forcement effect and the 'forward-looking' played by reward or incentive in storage and
incentive effect are actually inferred from retrieval along lines suggested by the Atkin-
subsequent behaviour. As a consequence, son and Shiffrin (1968) model. This model
the data do not clearly demonstrate that assumed that the memory system was di-
reinforcers and incentives operate in oppo- vided into three components: sensory regis-
site temporal directions. ters (e.g. iconic store, echoic store) which re-
The notion that reinforcement always has ceive information from the various sense or-
highly specific effects on response emission gans, a long-term store of essentially unli-
Learning and Memory: Atkinson and Wickens (1971) 69
mited capacity, and a short-term store of lues associated with each item may facilitate
strictly limited capacity which temporarily the search process. A second possibility is
holds information that has arrived either that subjects will be prepared to engage in
from one of the sensory registers or from the more extensive retrieval activity for high-in-
long-term store. centive than for low-incentive items, which
According to Atkinson and Shiffrin would be expected to enhance the retrieval
(1968), these three components are so organ- of high-incentive items.
ized that in order for information to be Experimentation on the effects of incen-
stored in the long-term store, it must initially tives (usually of a monetary nature) on
pass from the sensory register to the short- learning of word lists has revealed an im-
term store, from which it is then transferred portant difference between the results ob-
or 'copied' into the long-term store. This tained from unmixed lists (i.e. all items high
transfer of information typically occurs as a incentive or all low incentive) and from
result of rehearsal activity in the short-term mixed lists (i.e. half the items high-incentive
store, although it can also occur as a conse- and half low-incentive). The typical finding
quence of other processing activities (e.g. (e.g. Harley 1965a, b; Nelson 1976) is that
imaginal coding) taking place in the short- high-incentive items are significantly better
term store. During the time that information learned than low-incentive items with
is being rehearsed in the short-term store, it mixed lists, but there is no incentive effect
is passed on to the long-term store; when the with unmixed lists.
rehearsal activity ceases, information in the In the study by Nelson (1976), 40 com-
short-term store starts to decay. In contrast, mon words were presented for immediate
information that is stored in the long-term free recall in a mixed or unmixed list. The
store is not subject to decay and remains in results are shown in Fig. 5.1. The incentive
store indefinitely. However, subsequent at- manipulation had a substantial effect on
tempts to retrieve the stored information word recall in the mixed-list condition but a
may fail if an inappropirate retrieval strate- non-significant effect in the unmixed-list
gy is employed. condition.
Atkinson and Wickens (1971) proposed The theoretical approach of Atkinson and
that incentive or reinforcement plays two Wickens (1971) can account for the incen-
separate roles in determining the storage of tive effect in mixed lists in a straightforward
information: an informational role and an manner. If the learning task leads subjects
attentional role. On the one hand, reinforce- to establish a limited-capacity rehearsal buf-
ment provides the subject with feedback fer in the short-term store, then the probabil-
about the correctness of his responses. On ity of an item entering the buffer and being
the other hand, reinforcement causes the rehearsed will be greater for high-incentive
subject to attend to certain items or situa- than for low-incentive items. Since the the-
tional stimuli in preference to others. The ory also states that long-term storage de-
implication of this theoretical position is pends on the amount of rehearsal activity in
that the effects of incentive on learning and the short-term store, it follows that high-in-
memory are dependent on the way in which centive material will be better learned and
information about to-be-remembered mate- remembered than low-incentive material.
rial is processed in the short-term store. There is no particular reason for assuming
The final theoretical assumptions con- that differential rehearsal of high- and low-
cern two rather different ways in which in- incentive items will occur when unmixed
centive can affect retrieval. If information lists are used, however, and consequently
concerning the incentive values of to-be- there is no effect of incentive on recall.
remembered events is stored in memory at The differential effectiveness of incentive
input, then knowledge given at the time of manipulations on learning in mixed and un-
test regarding the appropriate incentive va- mixed lists is elegantly accounted for in
70 Incentives and Motivation

65 HIGH
INCENTIVE
60
...J
...J

u 55

-
~

-
(l:

I- 50 LOW
z INCENTIVE
w
u 45
(l:
w
Q.
40

L I Fig. 5. 1. Word recall as a function of mone-


tary incentive and nature of list (mixed versus
UNMIXED MIXED
NATURE OF LIST unmixed). (Nelson 1976)

terms of the influence of incentive on atten- found that high-incentive words were better
tional selectivity and patterns of rehearsal. recalled in a mixed list than an unmixed list,
However, the findings are rather more mys- whereas low-incentive words showed com-
terious from other theoretical perspectives. parable recall in both kinds of list.
Broadbent (1971) and others have argued The findings discussed so far do not show
that incentives increase the level of arousal conclusively that differential rehearsal ac-
and that the memorial consequences of tivity is the process responsible for the ob-
varying incentive:; can be predicted from the served incentive effects. A somewhat more
amount of arousal increment produced. direct attempt to study differential attention
Since high-incentive items should produce to high- and low-incentive stimuli was made
more arousal than low-incentive items by Loftus (1972). Subjects looked at pairs of
whether mixed or unmixed lists are used, it pictures and attention was measured by
is by no means clear how an arousal theory monitoring their eye movements.
would account for the absence of any incen- As expected, incentive was effective in
tive effect with unmixed lists. improving recognition memory. The notion
Further predictions can be generated that high-incentive stimuli receive more at-
from the Atkinson-Wickens position if we tention than low-incentive stimuli was sup-
compare recall of high-incentive items in ported by the key finding that the number of
mixed and unmixed lists and then perform eye fixations was much greater for high-in-
the same comparison for low-incentive centive than for low-incentive pictures. In
items. High- incentive items should receive addition, recognition memory in the various
more rehearsal (and so be better recalled) in incentive conditions was essentially iden-
mixed than in unmixed lists, whereas the op- tical in those cases where the number of fi-
posite should be the case for low-incentive xations was the same.
items. While the combined data of Harley Cuvo (1974) presented high- and low-in-
(1965a) and Harley (1965b) provided sup- centive words for free recall in a mixed list.
port for this prediction, later work by Harley Some of the subjects were asked to rehearse
(1968) indicated that the incentive effect overtly during list presentation, whereas
with mixed lists was due primarily to re- others were required to count backwards by
duced recall of the low-incentive items rath- threes between the presentation of each
er than to enhanced recall of the high-incen- word in order to minimize the amount of re-
tive items. The picture was further confused hearsal activity. There was a strong incen-
by the data of Nelson (1976; see Fig. 5.1); he tive effect on recall, but this was reduced un-
Learning and Memory: Atkinson and Wickens (1971) 71
der conditions of rehearsal prevention. Of hanced retention. While this has been dis-
greatest interest, however, was the finding confirmed, it still makes sense to think of re-
that the number of overt rehearsals was sig- hearsal activities as falling along a continu-
nificantly greater for high-incentive than for um ranging from mindless rote rehearsal to
low-incentive words. active constructive rehearsal.
One of the potential problems with both Some of these issues were addressed by
the Atkinson and Wickens (1971) theoreti- M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck (1980).
cal approach and the data of Cuvo (1974) They presented lists of words for cued re-
and Loftus (1972) is that the emphasis is call, with half of the words in each list being
very much on possible quantitative effects of associated with a high monetary incentive
incentive on attention and rehearsal activi- and half with a low incentive. They used the
ty. Thus Atkinson and Wickens postulated overt rehearsal technique and discovered
that incentive effects are due to differential that high-incentive words were rehearsed
amounts of rehearsal, Loftus obtained evi- more frequently than low-incentive words
dence that the number of eye fixations was and also showed much better cued recall.
affected by incentive and Cuvo found that However, the major focus of interest was on
incentives affected the number of rehear- possible differential effects of incentive on
sals. However, it is entirely possible that in- recall to the various kinds of retrieval cues.
centive manipulations may affect process- Cues were either semantic (e.g. 'CHAIR as-
ing in a qualitative manner in addition to the sociated with ---') or phonemic (e.g.
purely quantitative. 'CABLE rhymes with ---'), and the cues
As learning proceeds, it has been found were either strongly or weakly associated
that the extent to which rehearsal is semanti- with their respective list words.
cally organized becomes an increasingly po- The cued recall findings are shown in Fig.
tent determinant of recall, whereas the op- 5.2. Incentive had no effect on the kind of
posite is true of the amount of rehearsal processing, since the effects of incentive
(Weist 1972). At the theoretical level, Craik conditions were very similar for phonemic
and Lockhart (1972) contrasted mainte- and semantic retrieval cues. This suggests that
nance rehearsal (i.e. mere repetition of pro- incentive did not produce qualitative chan-
cessing activity) and elaboration rehearsal ges in processing. In contrast, there was a sub-
(i.e. rehearsal that extends the variety of sti- stantial interaction between incentive con-
mulus processing). They hypothesized that ditions and cue strength, in which high in-
it was only elaboration rehearsal that en- centive improved recall to weak retrieval

65
~,
60
...J
...J55 ""
<{
u 50
w
"

~
Il:
~
45
z
w 40
u
Il: 35
w
Q.
30

Fig. 5.2. Recall as a func-


tion of monetary incen-
1"
tive, depth of retrieval PHONEMIC SEMANTIC WEAK STRONG
cue and strength of re- RECALL CUES RECALL CUES
trieval cue. (Eysenck and
Eysenck 1980) - LOW INCENTIVE 0---0 HIGH INCENTIVE
72 Incentives and Motivation

cues but had no effect with strong retrieval at all for other subjects, incentives did not
cues. This interaction suggests that incen- affect recall.
tive increases the extensiveness or elabora- Two studies (Weiner 1966b, Wickens and
tion of encoding; in other words, low-incen- Simpson 1968) have considered the effects
tive words were mainly processed in terms of incentives on retrieval in a modified
of the most obvious and readily accessible Brown-Peterson paradigm. Subjects re-
features, whereas high-incentive words ceived to-be-remembered information, per-
were processed in terms of both readily ac- formed an interpolated task and then tried
cessible and less accessible features. It may to recall the items. Information about the
well be that this increase in the number of relevant incentive condition was only pro-
encoded features or attributes under high- vided at the time of recall. Neither study
incentive conditions is produced by the dis- found any effect of the incentive manipUla-
proportionate amount of the available pro- tion on retention test performance.
cessing time that subjects spend rehearsing One obvious possibility is that incentives
high-incentive stimuli. might reduce the criterion for response in a
In sum, Atkinson and Wickens (1971) as- retrieval situation. Katz (1969) compared a
sumed that the main reason for the effects of non-risky incentive condition in which cor-
incentive on learning and memory was the rect responses were financially rewarded
greater amount of rehearsal accorded to and incorrect responses were ignored with
high-incentive than to low-incentive stimuli. a second, risky condition in which correct
They also seem to have assumed that this ex- responses were rewarded and incorrect re-
tra rehearsal resulted in more elaborate or sponses were penalized. Free recall per-
extensive processing and 'stronger' memory formance was better under the non-risky in-
traces rather than in the utilization of more centive condition, but this was largely at the
efficient learning strategies. As we have expense of a substantial increase in errors.
seen, the data consistently support these In other words, the manipulation of incen-
various theoretical assumption. tives can affect the location of the response
In some ways, it is rather puzzling that criterion.
high-incentive stimuli are not processed in a Loftus and Wickens (1970) looked at the
qualitatively superior manner to low-incen- effects of incentive on retrieval in a paired
tive stimuli. An intriguing explanation is associate task. When information about the
that people are largely unaware of the dif- incentive value of a pair was provided only
ferential effectiveness of various processing at the time of test, high-incentive items were
activities and so fail to maximize learning of better recalled than low-incentive items.
high-incentive stimuli. For example, it has However, there was a corresponding in-
been found that people underestimate the crease in the response time, suggesting that
memorial advantage usually associated with incentive improved retention simply be-
semantic processing (Cutting 1975). cause subjects spent more time searching
It will be remembered that Atkinson and through memory.
Wickens (1971) suggested that incentives All in all, the basic contentions of Atkin-
might affect retrieval either by supplying son and Wickens (1971) appear to be well
useful information to the search process or founded. A major effect of incentive manip-
by inducing subjects to prolong the search ulations is to cause a re-allocation of atten-
process. However, attempts to demonstrate tion in which certain stimuli (i.e. those asso-
effects of incentives on retrieval have usual- ciated with high incentives) receive a dispro-
ly been unsuccessful. Wasserman et al. portionate amount of attention at the ex-
(1968) gave subjects paced and free recall of pense of other stimuli (i.e. those associated
a list of paired associates. In spite of the fact with low incentives). In the case oflearning
that some subjects were offered 50 cents for tasks, allocation of attention can be roughly
each item correctly recalled against nothing indexed by rehearsal activity. The extra at-
Short-Term Memory: Weiner 73
tention and rehearsal accorded to high-in- on retention, with shock and 5 cent
centive stimuli simply lead to extra process- reward being associated with better re-
ing of the same kind as that given to low-in- call than one cent reward or no incentive.
centive stimuli, rather than to processing of Incentive conditions interacted with reten-
a different kind. tion interval, with no effects of incentive be-
ing apparent with short retention intervals.
However, there appears to be a 'ceiling' ef-
fect, with recall being nearly perfect in all
conditions at the shortest retention interval.
5.3 Short-Term Memory: Weiner
Weiner and Walker (1966) argued that the
incentive effect might be due to motivation-
al factors at storage reducing the suscepti-
A rather different approach to incentive and bility of the memory trace to pre- and post-
memory was pursued in a series of studies perceptual interference, which is a some-
by Weiner (e.g. Kernoff et el. 1966, Weiner what amorphous notion. Weiner (1967) sug-
1966b, Weiner and Walker 1966, Weiner gested as an alternative explanation that the
1967). They used a modification of the heightened arousal produced by incentives
Brown-Peterson paradigm in which three or might produce a prolonged period of re-
four consonants were presented for approx- verberation or consolidation.
imately 1 s. Following stimulus presenta- That suggestion was based on Walker's
tion, subjects read digits in time to a metro- (1958) action-decrement theory. Walker ar-
nome and then attempted to recall the con- gued that there was a temporary inhibition
sonants after up to 17 s of the interpolated of retrieval (or action decrement) during the
activity. process of consolidation which preserves
In the initial studies (Kernoff et al. 1966, the memory trace and protects it from dis-
Weiner and Walker 1966), incentives were ruption. Since such action decrement lasts
manipulated by means of different coloured longer under high arousal, it would be ex-
backgrounds on the stimulus slide. Accord- pected that incentives would reduce recall at
ing to a pre-arranged system, a particular short retention intervals, but that result has
colour might indicate a shockforincorrect re- not been obtained. There is thus no reason
call, 1cent reward for correct recall, five cents for supposing that Walker's theory can ac-
for recall, or no incentive. The results from count for the data. In any case, Walker's the-
both studies were similar, and those of Ker- ory has been criticized on several grounds
noff et al. are shown in Fig. 5.3. There were (Eysenck 1977), notably because of the very
substantial effects of incentive conditions narrow range of data that supports it and be-
...J100
...J
3 90
w
Q: 60
I-
U 70 SHOCK
W
Q:
60 5-CENT
Q:
0 INCENTIVE
U 50
I- 1,CENT INCENTIVE
Z 40 NO INCENTIVE
w
U
Q: 30
W

l'
!l.

I I I
2·60 9·35 17·00
RETENTION INTERVAL (IN SECS)

Fig. 5.3. Recall as a function of incentive conditions and retention interval. (Kernoff et al. 1966)
74 Incentives and Motivation

cause high arousal often fails to produce the aware of the relevant incentive condition
predicted reduction in short-term retention. while performing the interplated task, there
The hypothesis that subjects engage in was more counting backwards under the no-
more covert rehearsal of high-incentive than incentive condition than under the incentive
of low-incentive items, in spite of the use of conditions. The most natural interpretation
an interpolated task designed to prevent re- of the various findings is that high incen-
hearsal, was suggested by Tarpy and tives lead to increased recall in the Brown-
Glucksberg (1966). It provides an extremely Peterson paradigm because they encourage
straightforward explanation of most of the extra covert rehearsal of the to-be-remem-
available data. Wickens and Simpson (1968) bered material. It is interesting to note that
explored this hypothesis. Their paradigm this explanation is in line with the theoreti-
was very similar to that used by Weiner, ex- cal emphasis of Atkinson and Wickens
cept that their subjects had to count back- (1971) on attentional and rehearsal mechan-
wards by threes from a three-digit number isms to account for incentive effects.
during the retention interval. Information
about the incentive condition was provided
with the to-be-remembered information,
with the three-digit number or at recall. 5.4 Task Characteristics
The findings of Wickens and Simpson
(1968) are shown in Fig. 5.4. High incentive
only enhanced recall when information At a more general level, there has been a cer-
about the incentive condition was pre- tain amount of controversy over the issue of
sented at the same time as the to-be-re- the primary effects of incentive or reward on
membered item. This result should be inter- performance. On the one hand, Skinner has
preted in the light of the data concerning for many years argued that a reward or rein-
the speed with which the interpolated task forcer is a stimulus event that follows a re-
was performed. When the subjects were sponse and thereby increases the probabili-

60
,6
..J
..J
55 ,,",,"
« 50 A- __ ~._._ ...t:J.~"''''''
U

~
W
II:: 45
>-
z 40
w
u
II:: 35
w
Il.
30

I
1:
ci'"
zlJ) lJ,. __ ....
;:::~ 1700

'8°°1
- ................. .../j. _._0
Zo

~
=>w 1600
0>-

IL..J 1500
00
>-Il.
zll::
=>w l' I I I
0>-
NO MONETARY SHOCK
Fig. 5.4. Top: the effects of incentive con-
~~ ditions and the time at which incentive in-
« INCENTIVE INCENTIVE IF WRONG
INCENTIVE CONDITIONS formations was provided on recall; bot-
tom: the effects of incentive conditions
INCENTIVE INFORMATION PROVIDED: and the time at which incentive informa-
- WITH TO-BE-REMEMBERED ITEMS tion was provided on performance of the
6---fj. WITH INTERPOLATED DIGIT SLIDE subsidiary task. (Wickens and Simpson
0-'-0 AT RETRIEVAL 1968)
Task Characteristics 75

ty of occurrence of that response. The impli- cluding a book of matches and a box of
cation of this position is that rewards or drawing pins, are available. Problem solu-
reinforcers can be relied on to facilitate per- tion involves using the box as a platform for
formance. the candle. The problem is said to involve
On the other hand, Condry (1977) is an in- functional fixedness because a box is usual-
fluential anti-reward psychologist: "Com- ly regarded as a container rather than as a
pared to non-rewarded subjects, subjects of- platform.
fered a task-extrinsic incentive choose eas- Glucksberg used two versions of the
ier tasks, are less efficient in using the infor- problem, one in which the box was initially
mation available to solve novel problems, full of drawing pins and matches and the
and tend to be answer oriented and more il- other in which the box was presented empty.
logical in their problem-solving strategies. The former version makes problem solution
They seem to work harder and produce more difficult, because it emphasizes the
more activity, but the activity is of a lower normal use of a box as a container. When
quality, contains more errors, and is more the problem was presented in its easier ver-
stereotyped and less creative than the work sion, there was no effect of monetary incen-
of comparable nonrewarded subjects work- tive on speed of solution. In contrast, the in-
ing on the same problems" (pp. 471-472). centivized subjects required approximately
The antecedents of Condry's position are to 3.5 min more than the non-incentivized sub-
be found in the Holy Bible rather than in the jects to solve the difficult verion of the prob-
collected works of Fred Skinner; in particu- lem.
lar, one is reminded of the hypothesis ex- Related findings were reported by Glucks-
pounded in the first Epistle of Paul to Timo- berg (1964) using a different functional fix-
thy: "Money is the root of all evil". edness problem which involved employing
The simplest way of reconciling the ap- a screwdriver to complete a circuit when the
parently opposed viewpoints of Skinner and available wires proved to be too short. Fi-
Condry is to consider the types of task each nancial incentive increased the time taken to
of them has used in his work. Skinner has ty- find the solution from 6 to 13 min on aver-
pically used extremely simple and repeti- age when the relevance of the screwdriver
tious tasks such as lever pressing for food re- was de-emphasized, but there was no effect
ward in which the emphasis is on the per- of incentive when the task was presented in
formance of over-learned patterns ofbehav- such a way that it was reasonably clear that
iour rather than on the acquisition of new the screwdriver was involved in solving the
skills. In contrast, Condry has tended to problem.
look at the effects of incentive on relatively The difficult versions of these two func-
complicated problem-solving or concept-at- tional fixedness problems are designed to
tainment tasks. Thus we can fairly neatly ac- demonstrate the fact that habitual ways of
count for the differing theoretical perspec- regarding an object tend to prevent the in-
tives by proposing that incentive or reward sight that the object can be used in some
augments performance on simple and unde- novel fashion that is appropriate to the task
manding tasks but worsens performance on in hand. This analysis suggests that incen-
tasks of great cognitive complexity. tive increases the tendency to persist with es-
A more detailed examination of the evi- tablished modes of thinking even when
dence indicates that matters are actually these turn out to be counter-productive; in
rather more complicated than this. Let us other words, incentive can reduce the flexi-
begin by considering whether incentive and bility of thought.
reward consistently impair performance on A further study pointing to the same con-
difficult tasks. Glucksberg (1962) used a clusion was reported by McGraw and
problem in which a candle has to be mount- McCullers (1979). They made use of water-
ed on a vertical screen; various objects, in- jar problems in which subjects have to im-
76 Incentives and Motivation

agine pouring liquid from one jar to another improve test performance above that of the
in order to finish up with a designated quan- control group.
tity of liquid in one of the jars. The first nine Some investigators (e.g. Nichols 1959,
problems had the same three-jar solution, Sarason and Minard 1962) have attempted
thus leading the subjects to establish a men- to motivate their subjects by means of ego-
tal set. The ease with which this mental set involving or achievement-oriented instruc-
could be broken was assessed by using a tions; once again, motivation had no overall
tenth and final problem which only had a influence on test performance. In contrast,
simple two-jar solution. knowledge of results in the form of feed-
Monetary incentive did not affect solu- back about the correctness of responses has
tion speed on the first nine problems but in- been found to improve performance (Bass
creased the mean time taken to solve the fi- & Ninias 1974, Willis and Ehrlick 1975).
nal problem by approximately 60 %. Thus However, in view of the fact that incentive
once again we can see that incentive has had per se had no effect on performance in eith-
the effect of reducing cognitive flexibility; er of these studies, it was probably the infor-
more specifically, it has produced a negative mative rather than the motivational compo-
transfer effect, in which previously acquired nent of knowledge of results that produced
skills were applied inappropriately to a new the observed improvement.
situation. The various findings clearly support
More evidence that incentive can have a Spearman's (1927) contention that perform-
disruptive effect on the performance of ance on cognitive tests is relatively inde-
cognitively demanding tasks was obtained pendent of conation or motivation. How-
by Condry and Chambers (1978) in a series ever, Maller and Zubin (1932) obtained the
of studies on concept attainment. They sum- following findings: "The incentive of rivalry
marized the disadvantages of incentive in failed to bring about a significant increase
the following way: "Intrinsically motivated in score on an intelligence test, although the
subjects attend to and utilize a wider array subjects worked faster when motivated by
of information; they are focused on the way rivalry. The increase in speed resulted only
to solve the problem rather than the solu- in an increased number of items tried and a
tion. They are, in general, more careful, logi- corresponding increase in error, leaving the
cal, and coherent in their problem-solving final scores practically unchanged" (p. 149).
strategies than comparable subjects offered Maller and Zubin also found that motiva-
a reward to solve the same problems" tion produced enhanced performance on a
(p.69). speed test (Comparison of Numbers) but
A different kind of cognitively demand- impaired performance on a power test (An-
ing task is the standard intelligence test. The alogies). Related findings were obtained by
typical finding, perhaps surprisingly, is that Dickstein and Ayers (1973). The found that
incentives of various kinds have little or no monetary incentive improved performance
effect on the results of most intelligence on the performance sub-tests of the Wechs-
tests. Monetary incentives were found to ler Adult Intelligence Scale; however, on
have no effect on intelligence-test scores by the Advanced Progessive Matrices, which
Ferguson (1937), Maller and Zubin (1932) is an extremely difficult power test deve-
and Willis and Ehrlick (1975) with normal loped for people of high intelligence, there
groups and by Eysenck (1944) with neurotic was no incentive effect.
patients. Benton (1936) gave the Otis test to There is some evidence that the precise ef-
seventh and eighth grade children in two fects of motivational factors on intelligence-
matched groups and found that even an am- test performance depend on individual dif-
algam of incentives (including praise, strong ferences. Those high in ability are more like-
encouragement, knowledge of results and ly than those low in ability to benefit from
the promise of a prize) was insufficient to increased motivation (Maller and Zubin
Task Characteristics 77

1932), and those low in anxiety are more sing et al. 1959). In addition, there is a slow
likely to respond positively to incentives negative drift in the EEG which is known in
than those high in anxiety (Sarason and Mi- the trade as contingent negative variation
nard 1962). (CNV). This starts some 100-200 ms after
In contrast to the predominantly negative the warning stimulus and approaches its
effects of incentive on cognitively demand- maximum amplitude at a rate that is a func-
ing tasks requiring flexibility of thought, in- tion of the warning interval. The CNV ap-
centive has fairly consistently been found to pears to consist of two phases, the first of
enhance performance on tasks where the which is an alertness to stimuli, and the sec-
emphasis is on speed of response. The su- ond of which is a motor pre-programming
preme example of such a task is lever press- phase (Posner 1978).
ing for food, which is beloved of those inter- Posner (1978) argued that alertness or
ested in operant conditioning. A second ex- preparation affected the processes involved
ample is the reaction-time task, on which in reaction-time performance in the follow-
knowledge of results (which has frequently ing way: "The effects of alertness seem to be
been regarded as an incentive) has been best described as reducing the time for some
found to produce faster reaction times (e.g. central mechanism to respond to the build-
Blowers and Ongley 1975). up of information about the signal. The cen-
In order to understand more clearly the trals systems of an alert organism react more
effects of incentive on reaction time, we quickly. If the task requires detection of a
must consider the reaction-time task in transient signal, a warning may improve all
some detail. One of the major determinants aspects of performance. If the signal is
of reaction time is the interval between the steady-state, a speed-accuracy tradeoff is
occurrence of a warning stimulus and the usually found" (p. 131).
signal requiring response; this is known as In many studies, the error rates at differ-
the foreperiod. The optimal foreperiod for ent foreperiods are suggestive of a speed-ac-
fast reaction is typically between 0.5 and 2 s. curacy trade-off, but the overall number of
The function relating reaction time to the errors is so small that no significant differ-
length of the foreperiod depends import- ences in error rate were found. An exception
antly on the individual's state of prepara- is an experiment reported by Posner et al.
tion or alertness. (1973), in which the subjects were given in-
Gottsdanker (1975) proposed the follow- structions that emphasized the importance
ing hypothesis concerning this state: "Pre- of speed. There was clear evidence of a
paration is an aversive. state and as such is speed-accuracy trade-off, with fast respond-
entered into only when profitable, but other- ing at foreperiods in the 100-400 ms range
wise avoided" (p. 34). He found that it took being accompanied by high error rates.
approximately 300 ms to attain full prepara- If incentive leads to increased alertness or
tion and that the state of high-level prepara- preparation, then it follows that the major
tion was rarely maintained in full for more characteristics of the alert state should be
than a relatively short period of time, presu- produced by incentive. As a consequence,
mably because of its aversive nature. we can make the following predictions:
Common sense suggests that incentive 1. Incentive should increase the amplitude
would produce a prolonged state of prepar- oftheCNV.
ation or alertness. However, this notion can 2. Since the alert state is difficult to sustain
only be evaluated properly after considering over long periods of time, incentive mani-
what is involved in preparation or alertness. pulations should have their maximal effect
The occurrence of a warning stimulus is ty- with long foreperiods (i.e. when alertness
pically followed by a temporary blocking of would usually be low). In other words,
rhythmical alpha activity of the EEG and there should be an interaction between in-
then by fast, desynchronized activity (Lan- centive conditions and foreperiod length.
78 Incentives and Motivation

3. Provided that a reasonable number of er- used (see Fig. 5.5). On the other hand, there
rors are made, incentive should lead to a was no interaction when the length of the
speed-accuracy trade-off. foreperiod varied randomly; instead, there
Some support for the first prediction was was simply a general improvement in speed
obtained by Blowers and Ongley (1975) in of reaction under incentive conditions.
an study already mentioned. Incentive in The third prediction is that the increased
the form of knowledge of results not only speed of responding by incentivized sub-
produced faster responding; it also led to in- jects should be accompanied by a greater er-
creased CNV amplitude. Similarly, Otto ror rate. While this seems plausible in view
(1971) discovered that knowledge of results of the effects of incentive on other kinds of
in the form of red or green lights indicating tasks, no directly relevant evidence seems to
whether or not a given reaction-time criteri- be available.
on had been exceeded led to a greater CNV Another way of looking at the effects of
area than no feedback. In contrast, there incentive on performance speed is to use the
was no effect of knowledge of results on five-choice serial reaction task or some var-
CNV amplitude in a study carried out by iant of it. The basic apparatus consists of
Rebert (1972). However, Rebert failed to ob- five lights with a response key immediately
tain any effect offeedback on reaction time, below each one. When a light comes on, it is
so that it is possible that feedback simply switched off by pressing its corresponding
failed to incentivize his subjects. key, which simultaneously illuminates an-
There is limited evidence relevant to the other light at random. The task is usually
second prediction. Elliott et al. (1976) used performed for relatively long periods of
blocks of trials with preparatory intervals or time, and the detrimental effects of various
foreperiods of 4 and 8 s; incentive reduced stressors are most pronounced towards the
reaction times more at the longer interval. end of the task period.
They also observed that incentive led to The effects of motivation on the serial
greater heart rate deceleration during the reaction task depend in large measure on
foreperiod, which is consistent with the no- the strength of motivation which is created.
tion that incentive increases alertness. Very strong motivation produces a decre-
In an earlier study, Elliott (1970) had ment in performance, whereas less intense
found that reaction times were interactively motivation improves performance. In a
determined by incentive conditions and by study by Willett (1964), unusually powerful
length of the foreperiod when blocks of tri- motivation was created by using as subjects
als with constant length of foreperiod were the candidates for a highly desirable ap-

240 LOW
INCENTIVE

w 220
~
!-

Zu 200 HIGH
Qw INCENTIVE
!-I/I
u~ 180
~Z
0:-
Z 160
<{
w
~ 140

L I
Fig. 5.5 Mean reaction time as a function
of monetary incentive and length of the
2 3 4 5
LENGTH OF FOREPERIOD foreperiod in children between the ages of
(IN SECS) 11 and 13. (Elliott 1970)
Task Characteristics 79

prenticeship who thought that their per- difficult, because it may provide useful in-
formance on the serial reaction task was formation over and above any incentive ef-
relevant to their chances of being accepted. fect.
The control group consisted of those previ- If incentive is regarded as an arouser
ously accepted for the apprenticeship who (Broadbent, 1971), then the pattern of re-
knew that the data being collected had no sults on the serial reaction task is in line with
bearing on their standing in the training the Yerkes-Dodson law (discussed in
school. The highly motivated subjects pro- Chap. 4). According to Yerkes and Dodson
duced fewer correct responses and approxi- (1908), there is a curvilinear relationship be-
mately twice as many errors on the five- tween arousal and performance, with inter-
choice serial reaction task. mediate arousal levels being optimal for
Motivation was manipulated in the same performance. Unfortunately Yerkes and
fashion by Eysenck and Warwick (1964). Dodson described rather than explained
They used a very complicated multiple- their data, so that the reasons for this curvili-
choice reaction task with responses being near relationship remain obscure.
produced by one finger, by one foot or by The cancellation task seems conceptually
the whole hand. In contrast to the serial similar to the serial reaction task in that it re-
reaction task, this task was experimenter quires self-paced responses performed in a
paced rather than subject paced. High in- continuous manner. The task involves cross-
centive motivation reduced the number of ing out certain target stimuli (e.g. all the 'e's)
correct responses by approximately 30%. on a sheet of paper containing target and
Very different results have been obtained non-target stimuli.
with less intense incentive manipulations. Dey and Kaur (1965) used a letter cancel-
Wilkinson et al. (1972) incentivized some of lation task and found that subjects who
their subjects by the fairly mild expedient of were assigned hard output goals performed
offering payment by results and providing better than those who were assigned easy
information about errors and unduly long output goals. Feldman (1964) compared the
response times. They used both an easy and performance of high-incentive subjects
a difficult version of the serial reaction task (those seeking a desired apprenticeship)
(four- and ten-choice, respectively). Speed and low-incentive subjects (those previous-
of responding was increased by incentive, ly accepted) on a digit cancellation task,
but there was no effect of incentive on accu- which was given in both an easy and a diffi-
racy of performance. The beneficial effects cult version. Incentive increased the number
of incentive were comparable on both ver- of correct responses on the easy task but did
sions of the task. not affect correct responding on the difficult
In earlier work with the five-choice serial task. Incentivized subjects made many more
reaction task (Wilkinson 1963), knowledge errors than non-incentivized subjects, espe-
of results reduced the number of errors and cially on the easy task.
the number of gaps (i.e. unusually long re- Corcoran (1962b) found that incentive in
sponses) and did so to a greater extent in the form of fake feedback improved the
quiet than in noise. Wilkinson and Colqu- speed with which a letter cancellation task
houn (1968) used the same task and dis- was performed. Further analysis revealed
covered that knowledge of results led to that this enhancement effect was almost en-
more correct responses and to a reduction in tirely attributable to the extraverted sub-
the number of errors and gaps; the overall jects: their speed of performance increased
advantage of knowledge of results was re- by approximately 80% under incentive con-
duced (but probably not eliminated) among ditions, whereas that of introverts was unaf-
subjects who had consumed a large amount fected.
of rum. Of course, unequivocal interpreta- Another task on which the effects of in-
tion of the effects of knowledge of results is centive are usually positive is vigilance.
80 Incentives and Motivation

Systematic scientific investigation of vigi- group of subjects that good performance on


lance was initiated by Mackworth (1950), the vigilance task might lead to the offer of a
who simulated the task of maintaining radar desired part-time employment at the rate of
watch for submarines by using a clock £4-an hour. The highly motivated group de-
pointer which moved on in a series of steps. tected more auditory signals; in addition,
The subjects watched the pointer and re- there was a vigilance decrement for the non-
ported the relatively infrequent occasions incentivized subjects, but not for the high-
on which the pointer gave a double jump. motivation subjects. Related findings were
Most of the subsequent researchers in the vi- obtained by Lucaccini, Freedy and Lyman
gilance area have followed Mackworth's (1968).
lead and used tasks requiring the observer to Nachreiner (1977) noted that the vigi-
keep watch for inconspicuous signals (eith- lance decrement occurred far more fre-
er visual or auditory) over long periods of quently in the laboratory than in 'real-life'
time of I h or more. The most important situations and suggested that this difference
finding is the so-called vigilance decrement, was due to the much greater likelihood of
i.e. the probability of signal detection tends motivation diminishing over time in the la-
to decrease over time. boratory.
As Broadbent (1971) pointed out, it is not When incentives are offered for correct
sufficient merely to consider the detection detections, the effect is to increase the gain
data, since information about false alarms associated with correct detections and so
(i.e. the reported detections of signals when presumably to lower the response criterion.
none is presented) is of great importance. In other words, it is conceivable that the en-
tire motivational effect is on the response
Information about detections and false
alarms can be used by signal detection the- criterion rather than on sensitivity. In one
orists (e.g. Swets 1977) to provide independ- study of auditory vigilance (Levine 1966),
the financial gain associated with signal de-
ent measures of observer sensitivity and of
tection and the financial cost associated
his decision criterion (i.e. his bias towards
with a missed signal or a false alarm were
one or other of the response alternatives).
manipulated. Increased costs for either mis-
According to the theory, the decision criteri-
ses or false alarms led to poorer detection
on is determined jointly by the probabilities performance, whereas changes in the gains
of the stimulus alternatives (signal present for detections failed to influence perform-
versus signal absent) and by the payoff ma- ance. Sensitivity was invariant across condi-
trix. The payoff matrix is determined by the tions, so that the performance changes were
values or gains associated with correct deci- attributed to shifts in the response criterion.
sions ('yes' when the signal is present and Very similar results were obtained by
'no' when the signal is absent) and the costs Davenport (1968) for an auditory vigilance
of errors ('no' when the signal is present and task and by Davenport (1969) for vibro-tac-
'yes' when the signal is absent). tile vigilance. The value placed on detec-
The vigilance decrement has occasionally tions had little or no effect, whereas increa-
been found to be due to a decline in sensitiv- sing costs for missed signals and false
ity, especially if there is a rapid rate of sti- alarms produced poorer detection perform-
mulus presentation. However, the typical ance because of greater cautiousness about
finding is that it is primarily attributable to a responding.
progressive rise in the strictness of the re- It would not necessarily be appropriate to
sponse criterion. conclude that incentive has no effect on sen-
There have been various attempts to elu- sitivity or d'. The reason is that most of the
cidate the effects of motivational factors on relevant studies failed to use a control group
vigilance performance. Nachreiner (1977) receiving no incentive. When such a control
produced high motivation by informing one group has been used, the various incentive
Task Characteristics 81
conditions have been found to increase sen- It is clear that incentives and knowledge
sitivity or d' in an auditory vigilance situa- of results (which seems to act as an incen-
tion (Sostek 1978). A group encouraged to tive) both enhance vigilance performance,
be cautious (loss of 9 cents for each false but there have been relatively few attempts
alarm) adopted a more stringent response to explain why this should be so. The most
criterion than the control group, whereas promising lead is that motivational manipu-
subjects threatened with loss of 9 cents for lations sometimes affect the response crite-
every miss (i.e. failure of detection) chose a rion or cautiousness of responding, but it
rather low response criterion. certainly cannot be assumed that there are
In his seminal research, Mackworth no effects on sensitivity. The vigilance para-
(1950) discovered that the vigilance decre- digm was originally devised because it
ment could be prevented by providing seemed to capture some of the salient char-
knowledge of results when a signal was eith- acteristics of certain work conditions, and it
er missed or correctly detected. This result is obviously useful in that context to know
has been obtained several times subsequent- that added incentives can prevent the vigi-
ly. The central interpretative problem is that lance decrement. From a more academic
the effects of knowledge of results may be perspective, however, it is rather difficult to
mediated by anyone of several different me- conceptualize the psychological mechan-
chanisms, including enhanced motivation, isms and processes involved in vigilance
increased arousal, information provision or performance. This may help to account for
indications of signal schedules. the relative lack of headway in explaining
Some clarification of what is happening exactly why motivation improves vigilance
has been obtained by considering the effects performance.
of simulated or 'false' knowledge of results. In sum, we have seen that the effects of in-
Several researchers, including Loeb and centive depend greatly on the precise nature
Schmidt (1963), have discovered that false of the task which is used. Incentive typically
knowledge of results improves vigilance has a positive effect on reaction time, can-
performance. Thus part of the effect of cellation, lever pressing and vigilance tasks
knowledge of results on performance is not and no effect on intelligence test perform-
due to any specific information which it pro- ance; it often has a negative effect on prob-
vides. It is worth noting that Loeb and lem-solving and concept attainment tasks.
Schmidt also found that genuine knowledge What is the meaning of this collection of
of results was more effective than false findings at the theoretical level? One at-
knowledge of results. However, the evi- tempt to provide an answer was made by
dence on this point is equivocal, since it has McGraw (1978). He claimed in essence that
sometimes been found (e.g. Warm et al. incentive would have a negative effect on
1974) that genuine and false feedback or task performance only when two crucial
knowledge of results produce comparable pre-conditions were met: (I) the task is suf-
enhancement of vigilance performance. ficiently intrinsically interesting that extrin-
In an interesting study, Warm et al. (1972) sic incentive or reward provides a redun-
noted that simply asking their subjects to es- dant source of motivation and (2) the task
timate their performance efficiency after solution is relatively open ended, with the
each response produced as great an im- necessary steps involved in that solution be-
provement in performance of a visual vigi- ing non-obvious. McGraw elucidated the
lance task as knowledge of results, despite second point by drawing a conceptual dis-
the fact that the accuracy of the estimates tinction between algorithmic and heuristic
did not exceed chance. They concluded that tasks; in algorithmic tasks, the path to solu-
the motivational effects of knowledge of re- tion is clear and straightforward, whereas
sults stem from the subject's self-evalua- the first essential requirement with heuristic
tions in relation to performance standards. tasks is to develop an algorithm.
82 Incentives and Motivation

This theoretical analysis suggests that of tasks adversely affected by incentive will
tasks can be classified with respect to their be greater with large rather than small in-
location on an attractive-aversive continu- centives. It is strange that McGraw failed to
um and in terms of an algorithmic-heuristic include amount of incentive as one of the
dimension. While this is a somewhat conjec- factors determining the behavioural effects
tural exercise, it is nevertheless clear that the of incentive.
tasks which have been used in incentive re- It has also been found that incentive
search do not represent anything like a ran- sometimes fails to harm performance on
dom selection from within this two-dimen- tasks which are both interesting and heuris-
sional space. In fact, a disproportionate tic. The most obvious example of this is the
number of the tasks used have been aversive frequent finding that incentive has no effect
and algorithmic. Since the beneficial effects at all on intelligence test performance. A
of incentive are maximal with such tasks, it more instructive example is the finding that
is probable that this bias in task selection incentive did not affect performance speed
has led many researchers into the erroneous on the initial water jar problems in the study
assumption that incentive nearly always by McGraw and McCullers (1979), in spite
augments performance. of the non-obvious nature of the appro-
The studies by Glucksberg (1962, 1964) priate algorithm. However, incentive did
can be used to illustrate the potential useful- disrupt performance on the last problem in
ness of the algorithmic-heuristic distinction. the series, which required a different algo-
In these terms, he discovered that incentive rithm to the preceding problems. This sug-
disrupted problem-solving activities when gests that the critical factor may be the pres-
the problem was presented in a heuristic ence of an accessible but inappropriate al-
form but failed to do so when the relevant gonthm rather than (as McGraw claimed)
algorithm was made more obvious. Why merely the absence of obvious steps to task
should incentive interfere with the cognitive solution.
processes used on heuristic tasks? Accord- It was surely right of McGraw to empha-
ing to McGraw (1978) the answer can be size task attractiveness, since it is impossible
found in the fairly reliable finding (which is to predict the effects of incentive without
discussed later) that incentive reduces inci- some knowledge of the extent to which non-
dental learning. McGraw expressed the incentivized subjects are intrinsically moti-
point in the following way: "Reward sub- vated to perform the task. However, it is ac-
jects might do less well on concept forma- tually rather difficult to measure task attrac-
tion, problem solving, and those other tasks tiveness, which is interactively determined
on which we have seen reward subjects to be by the personal characteristics of the subject
inferior precisely because of their inferiority (e.g. intelligence, personality) and by the in-
at incidental learning ... The heuristic pro- trinsic characteristics of the task.
cess needed for some intentional-task solu- The greatest limitation of McGraw's the-
tions can be said to feed on incidental oretical stance may well be the implicit as-
thoughts and perceptions" (p. 55). sumption that incentive improves or impairs
While McGraw's suggestions are plausi- performance efficiency in a readily deter-
ble, there are some problems with his posi- minable way. A very common finding is that
tion. Incentive has sometimes harmed per- incentive speeds up performance, but de-
formance on tasks which are neither intrin- creases the quality of that performance by
sically interesting nor heuristic (e.g. Willett increasing the error rate. In such circum-
1964, discovered that performance on the stances it is very difficult (or even impossi-
serial reaction task was impaired by incen- ble) to decide whether incentive has en-
tive). Willett used an extremely powerful in- hanced or reduced performance efficiency;
centive (i.e. a desired apprenticeship), and it indeed, such findings make one wonder
seems reasonable to assume that the number whether the emphasis on assessing the over-
Performance Efficiency 83
all efficiency of performance should be re- centive increased speed of responding, but
placed by a stress on the patterning of per- reduced the accuracy of performance.
formance. This issue is explored at greater There is some evidence that incentive can
length in the next section. more readily affect the speed of perform-
ance than its quality. Adam (1972) used a
task that involved collating information
from data processing unit records cards.
5.5 Performance Efficiency The initial instructions emphasized either
quantity or quality of output, and incentive
was offered for following the instructions.
There would presumably be almost com- When there was an unannounced shift from
plete agreement that the use of two or more rewarding quantity to rewarding quality,
measures of performance is likely to provide subjects did not alter their behaviour; how-
a more complete picture of the conse- ever, a shift in the opposite direction led to
quences of incentive than can be obtained increased quantity and reduced quality.
by using only a single dependent variable. Why is it that the quantitative aspects of
However, it is the norm rather than the ex- performance are more easily improved by
ception for only one dependent measure to incentive than are the qualitative aspects?
be used. This not only reduces the amount Part of the answer may be that effort is more
of information that is available; it can also directly related to performance quantity
mislead researchers into erroneous conclu- than to its quality (Terborg and Miller
sions. 1978). A further point is that incentive is an
Locke (1968) discussed some research arouser (Broadbent 1971, Wilkinson et al.
that provides a case in point. The task used 1972), and high arousal tends to be associat-
was listing a specified number of objects ed with rapid, non-reflective responding.
meeting various criteria (e.g. 'heavy, rough At the theoretical level, the most interest-
and square'). The number of objects actual- ing example of incentive producing differ-
ly listed was largely determined by the goal ential effects on two aspects of performance
level (i.e. the specified number of objects), occurs when two concurrent tasks are used.
leading Locke to the conclusion that per- There are at least 12 relevant studies; Bah-
formance efficiency is a direct function of rick 1954, Bahrick et al. 1952, Cohen et al.
goal level. 1973, Davies and Jones 1975, Dixon and
This conclusion was challenged by Bave- Cameron 1976, Dornbush 1965, Johnson
las and Lee (1978) in a study in which they and Thomson 1962, Kausler et al. 1963,
replicated the finding that the number of ob- Kausler and Trapp 1962, McNamara and
jects written down is positively related to Fisch 1964, Rubin et al. 1962, Wolk and Du-
goal level. When they evaluated the quality Cette 1974. The main task used in several of
of the responses in terms of the extent to these studies involved intentional learning
which they matched up with each of the of a list of items (words, nonsense syllables
three criteria, they discovered that the quali- or geometrical forms), with the subsidiary
ty of performance was inversely related to task consisting of the incidental learning of
goal level! In other words, high goal level other stimuli (additional words or colours)
led to increased quantity but to reduced which were presented at the same time as
quality of responding. With such findings the main-task stimuli.
the notion of 'performance efficiency' More than one experiment was reported
seems almost meaningless. in some of the above studies, so that there
We have already discussed other studies are 15 sets of relevant data altogeth·er. In-
in which incentive produced a similar centive improved main-task performance in
speed-accuracy trade-off. Feldman (1964) six experiments and had no effect in the
and Maller and Zubin (1932) found that in- other nine. In contrast, incentive was found
84 Incentives and Motivation

to improve subsidiary-task performance in ter transformation task actually increases


only three of the experiments, it impaired distractibility, at least when the distracting
performance in five cases and it had no ef- stimuli are very similar to the task stimuli.
fect in the other seven. Incentive apparently This result can be explained by assuming
produces a re-allocation of attentional re- that incentive reduces the spare attentional
sources, with extra resources being invested capacity available for discriminating be-
in the main task (thus enhancing main-task tween task and non-task stimuli.
performance) and fewer resources being as- A simple generalization that describes
signed to the subsidiary task (so impairing many of the results discussed above is that
subsidiary-task performance). incentive enhances performance on those
The notion that incentive increases atten- aspects of performance emphasized in the
tional selectivity provides a very attractive task instructions at the cost of impaired per-
explanation of these findings and may well formance on other aspects. In other words,
be the major reason why incentive often im- incentivized subjects follow a 'minimax'
proves performance. However, it must be principle: they limit their performance to
admitted that there are major methodologi- those aspects minimally required for the de-
cal deficiencies in the dual-task studies re- livery of maximal rewards.
viewed above. It is difficult to be sure that
incentive is affecting attentional processes
when the only data collected are based on
retention test performance. Strictly speak- 5.6 Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation
ing, we cannot conclusively attribute the dif-
ferential effects of incentive on main and Most of the research discussed so far in this
subsidiary tasks to task importance, since chapter has been based on observing the ef-
rather different stimuli were used in the two fects of various external or extrinsic incen-
tasks in nearly all of the studies. Finally, the tives (e.g. food, money, praise) on perform-
use of incidental learning as the subsidiary ance. These incentives or rewards are extrin-
task provides an indication of what people sic in the sense that they provide satisfaction
choose to do rather than what they are able that is independent of the activity itself, and
to do. they are controlled by someone other than
Easterbrook (1959) used the dual-task da- the subject. Deci (1975) has proposed a con-
ta to argue that high arousal, whether pro- ceptual distinction between extrinsic and in-
duced by incentive or in some other way, trinsic motivation, with the latter term being
leads to a narrowing of cue utilization. This aplied to activities for which there is no ap-
emphasis on attentional selectivity is ap- parent reward except the activity itself. The
propriate; however, Easterbrook argued intrinsic motivation to perform a task will be
that the narrowing of cue utilization was a present if engaging in that task enhances a
fairly automatic consequence of the arousal person's feelings of competence and self-de-
produced by incentive, whereas it seems termination.
preferable to regard it as an active coping re- If there are any important differences be-
sponse to an altered payoff matrix. tween intrinsic and extrinsic motivation,
One way of attempting to distinguish be- then a rather biased and fragmentary view
tween the two viewpoints is to consider sus- of motivation is bound to emerge if only ex-
ceptibility to distraction. According to Eas- trinsic sources of motivation such as finan-
terbrook, incentive increases the degree of cial incentives are considered. For example,
concentration on the main task and thus it has been suspected for some time that the
presumably reduces distractibility. In some provision of extrinsic rewards mainly af-
recent unpublished work, we have dis- fects behaviour during the time that such re-
covered that the offer of large financial in- wards are available but may have surpris-
centives for superior performance on a let- ingly little long-term effect.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 85

One of the main practical uses of extrinsic onistic is because they involve different per-
rewards has been in token economies, in ceived loci of causality.
which groups of various kinds (e.g. juvenile According to Deci, extrinsic rewards do
delinquents, criminals or mental patients) not only have a controlling function which
have been offered tokens for designated de- initiates the change in perceived locus of
sirable modes of behaviour; these tokens causality; they also provide information
can subsequently be exchanged for various about competence and self-determination.
goods or privileges. These token economies If incentive or reward strengthens these feel-
often suffer from one major drawback, as ings of competence, then it will enhance in-
was pointed out by Kazdin and Bootzin trinsic motivation; if it weakenes these feel-
(1972) in their review of the literature: ings, then it decreases intrinsic motivation.
"There are numerous reports of token pro- So much for theory - how much empirical
grammes showing behaviour change only support is there for these various hypo-
while contingent token reinforcement is be- theses? The notion that the introduction of
ing delivered. Generally, removal of token extrinsic rewards will reduce intrinsic moti-
reinforcement results in undesirable re- vation has been investigated many times.
sponses and a return to baseline or near Two major measures of strength of intrinsic
baseline levels of performance" (p. 359). It motivation have been utilized: (1) the
is possible that the tokens produce a reduc- amount of time spent working on a specific
tion in intrinsic motivation by causing a per- activity in a free-choice situation in which
son to lose his feelings of self-determina- there are other things to do and no external
tion, which could help to explain the tran- rewards are available and (2) task satisfac-
sient nature of the benefits of the token tion or willingness to volunteer for a similar
economy. experiment as revealed by questionnaires.
It has typically been assumed that the ef- Both measures of intrinsic motivation
fects of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation were used in a classic study by Deci (1971).
are independent and additive. This view was He used a task called SOMA, which com-
challenged by Deci (1975). He claimed that prises a number of blocks that can be ar-
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are not in- ranged to form a variety of patterns speci-
dependent, that they do not sum and that fied by the experimenter. The participants
one consequence of increasing extrinsic mo- in this study attended for three sessions and
tivation may be to reduce intrinsic motiva- received no payment for the first and third
tion. Since intrinsically motivated behav- sessions. For the second session, however,
iour is behaviour which allows a person to one group of subjects was paid $ 1 for each
feel competent and self-determining (Deci problem solved, and a second group re-
1975), it follows that there are two ways in ceived no payment.
which extrinsic rewards can affect intrinsic In the middle of each session, the experi-
motivation: (1) a change in the perceived lo- menter went out of the room for 8 min, leav-
cus of causality and (2) a change in an indi- ing the subject alone with more puzzles as
vidual's feelings of competence. well as recent magazines and other distrac-
When someone is intrinsically motivated, tions. The effects of the extrinsic rewards on
the perceived locus of causality is within intrinsic motivation were indexed by the
that person. However, if people perceive amount of time spent in problem-solving ac-
that they are engaging in a particular activi- tivity during the middle of the third session.
ty primarily for an extrinsic reward, then the As predicted, those who had received finan-
perceived locus of causality changes to the cial rewards in the second session showed
external reward or incentive, leaving them less intrinsic motivation than those who had
with reduced intrinsic motivation. Thus a not. While it was also expected that extrinsic
major reason why extrinsic and intrinsic rewards would affect attitudes towards the
motivation are sometimes mutually antag- task, the two groups did not differ in terms
86 Incentives and Motivation

of rated task satisfaction. The phenomenon the latter by varying the interest value of the
of extrinsic rewards reducing subsequent in- task (jigsaw-type puzzles that were either
trinsic motivation has been replicated sever- blank or consisted of interesting pictures
al times (see Notz 1975). such as Playboy centrefolds). Performance
The phenomenon can be accounted for in level was effectively controlled, since with
other ways than that favoured by Deci only five pieces per puzzle no errors were
(1975). For example, extrinsic rewards may made.
lead subjects to set higher performance As can be seen in Fig. 5.6, there was an in-
goals, so that extra effort is exerted and the teraction between intrinsic and extrinsic
probability of goal attainment is reduced. A motivation on a measure of task satisfac-
subsequent dimunition in intrinsic motiva- tion, and a similar pattern of results was ob-
tion as a consequence of fatigue and failure tained for time volunteered by subjects for
experiences might be anticipated. future experiments of a similar nature. It
This alternative explanation was rejected may well be that extrinsic rewards reduce in-
by Kruglanski et al. (1972). Children re- trinsic motivation for relatively interesting
ceived a reward after they had participated tasks, such as those used by Deci or the pic-
in a series of games. They were erroneously ture puzzles used by Calder and Staw
led to believe that they had been promised (1975), but actually increase intrinsic moti-
the reward before they took part in the var- vation for uninteresting tasks such as the
ious games. In this case, extrinsic rewards blank puzzles.
could not have affected their behaviour Extrinsic rewards do not always reduce
while they were joining in the games. In intrinsic motivation for interesting tasks,
spite of this, their intrinsic motivation for however. When children unexpectedly re-
the games was reduced, presumably be- ceived an extrinsic reward (a good player
cause they attributed their involvement in award consisting of a gold star and a red rib-
the games to extrinsic motivation. bon) after performing the interesting task of
One of the few studies to look for an inter- playing with magic markers, their intrinsic
action between intrinsic and extrinsic moti- motivation increased (Lepper et al. 1973). It
vation was reported by Calder and Staw may be that rewards only reduce intrinsic
(1975). They manipulated both extrinsic and motivation when they are conspicuous or
intrinsic motivation, the former by contrast- salient. Ross (1975) found that simply offer-
ing non-payment with a monetary incentive ing and giving a reward for task perform-
that was not contingent on performance and ance did not affect intrinsic motivation,

3
w
...J
al 2
<{
>-
0..., BLANK
z
(!)W
z
i=
<{
0 '"
-----------7'--
/ '" PICTURE
a: w
...J / '"
al
'"
cI'"'"
~
0..., 2
z
w
z
:::> 3
Fig. 5.6. Ratings of the enjoyableness of
the task as a function of extrinsic motiva-
LOW(NOMONEY) HIGH(MONEY) tion and intrinsic motivation (blanks ver-
EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION sus pictures). (Calder and Staw 1975)
A Theoretical Overview 87

whereas having the anticipated reward visi- the attributed cause of a pattern of beha-
ble in front of the subject while the experi- viour is congruent with its objective cause;
mental task was being done led to reduced this is an assumption requiring further in-
intrinsic motivation. vestigation.
We have so far concentrated on the no-
tion that the introduction of extrinsic re-
wards will reduce intrinsic motivation. The
general theoretical position adopted by Oe- 5.7 A Theoretical Overview
ci (1975) suggests the further prediction that
the motivation to perform a task undertaken
originally to obtain an extrinsic reward may While no satisfactory theory of the effects
increase if the reward is subsequently with- of incentive on performance is currently
drawn. This paradoxical effect should occur available, we can at least identify some of
theoretically because the withdrawal of ex- the major factors which are involved. As Ey-
trinsic rewards changes the perceived locus senck (in press) argues, "The effects of in-
of causality from an external to an internal centive on task performance can usefuUy be
locus. This sounds like an employer's pana- regarded as depending on four classes of
cea for cash flow problems, but there must variables: (l) the nature of the incentive,
be some doubts as to whether trade union (2) the processes required by the task,
leaders would agree that pay cuts would (3) the aspects of performance selected for
motivate their members to work harder! measurement and (4) individual differences
Some support for the putative benefits of in mood or state and in semi-permanent per-
reward with drawal was obtained by Weick sonality characteristics." Such a statement is
(1964). Students were invited to participate so non-controversial as to be almost banal,
in an experiment for which they would re- yet its implications have been almost totally
ceive credit towards a course in psychology. ignored. So far as the author is aware, there
When the students arrived for the experi- has never been an empirical study in which
ment, some of them were told that they all four factors were manipulated simul-
would not receive the credit they had been taneously; in fact, it is usual for only one or
promised and were treated rather brusquely. two of them to be considered within the con-
Ten different measures including expended fines of a single experiment.
effort, task accomplishment, goal setting Let us consider each of these factors in
and interest in the task aU indicated stronger turn, beginning with the nature of the incen-
motivation for those subjects whose pro- tive. Incentive conditions can obviously
mised extrinsic reward had been withdrawn vary as a function of incentive magnitude,
than for those who received course credit. and it has often been assumed that an indi-
There are obviously some interesting and vidual's level of motivation will increase pa-
surprising interactions between extrinsic ri passu with the amount of incentive of-
and intrinsic motivational factors. However, fered for successful performance. However,
the proper interpretation of the data is in the subjective probability of performing the
some doubt. Oeci (1975) claimed that extrin- task well enough to obtain the incentive also
sic rewards reduce intrinsic motivation by plays a part in determining an individual's
changing the perceived locus of causality, level of motivation: very high incentive
but he presented no empirical evidence to combined with a near-zero probability of
support the proposed mechanism. A further success may lead to a rather low level of mo-
difficulty with Oeci's theoretical position is tivation. According to Revelle and Michaels
that he seems to believe that extrinsic re- (1976), tasks with subjective probabilities of
wards decrease the perception of intrinsic success between .1 and .5 are highly moti-
motivation, with this in turn decreasing ac- vating ("When the going gets tough, the
tual intrinsic motivation. It is assumed that tough get going"). In contrast, there is very
88 Incentives and Motivation

low motivation when the probability of suc- Despite these findings, matters are not al-
cess is less than .1, because "There is no ways as straightforward as Locke suggested.
point in banging your head against a brick Certainly very few professional sportsmen
wall." would agree with the notion that their per-
It has usually been assumed that factors formance always improves steadily as they
such as the amount of incentive and the sub- set themselves more and more challenging
jective probability of success jointly deter- performance goals. In a more experimental
mine some internal motivational state. For context, adverse effects of very high motiva-
Locke (1968), this internal state was repre- tion on performance have been reported in a
sented by goal setting, with the term 'goal' number of studies in which a desired ap-
meaning "what the individual is consciously prenticeship was believed to be at stake (e.g.
trying to do'; (p. 159). Locke regarded goal- Eysenck and Warwick 1964, Willett 1964).
setting intention as one of five sequential We have already seen that some of the
stages involved in human action and per- studies allegedly supporting Locke's theo-
formance (see Fig. 5.7). Initially a specific retical position are suspect. Bavelas and Lee
environmental event (e.g. monetary incen- (1978) replicated one of Locke's findings in
tive) produces cognitive and evaluative pro- a study in which they found that the number
cesses which jointly determine the goal-set- of objects listed which were 'hard, white and
ting intention; the goal then determines per- edible' was directly related to goal level.
formance. However, further analysis of their data re-
Locke claimed that a simple, linear rela- vealed a problem for Locke: the rated quali-
tionship had been established between goal ty of the responses varied inversely with
difficulty and performance: "The results are goal level (see Fig. 5.8).
unequivocal: the harder the goal the higher While it is clear that high goal setting is
the level of performance" (p. 162). While not always followed by a superior level of
Locke did not discuss in any detail exactly performance, it is still possible that high
why harder goals should lead to enhanced goal setting is a necessary, if not sufficient,
performance, he implied that there is greater pre-condition for performance enhance-
effort and utilization of resources when dif- ment. A related hypothesis was applied to
ficult goals are set. He referred to 12 studies incentives by Locke et al. (1968): "Incen-
concerned with the relationship between tives such as money should affect action on-
performance and the level or difficulty of ly if and to the extent that they affect the in-
the subject's goal. dividual's goals and intentions" (p. 104). In
A large variety of tasks was used in these line with their hypothesis, they found that
studies, including uses for objects, complex monetary incentives did not affect either
computation, addition, perceptual speed, goal setting or performance on a uses-for-
toy construction, reaction time and grade objects task. However, subjects who were
achievement in college. Goal difficulty was assigned goals set higher goals than subjects
determined by the percentage of trials on who set their own goals and also produced
which subjects attained their goal, and the more responses.
overall rank-order correlation coefficient It is possible that the monetary incentives
between performance and goal difficulty offered by Locke et al. (1968) were too low.
was a highly significant + .78. Farr (1976) used larger financial incentives

ENVIRONMENTAL
GOAL-SETTING
EVENT COGNITION EVALUATION PERFORMANCI
INTENTION
(EG. INCENTIVE)

(1 ) ( 2) ( 3) (4) (5 )

Fig. 5.7. The role of goal setting in a theory of action and performance. (Locke 1968)
A Theoretical Overview 89

18

I
~
16

14 6·32 1I
I/l
>-1- 6·30
I-U 12 ~
-w
f-, 6·28 >- ..
zID -z
I-~

~O 10 6·26 ...J<
OLL <w
:::>0
0 6·24 0 -
a:: 8 ...J
w 6·22 U
ID :::>
::E w
Fig. 5.S. The effects of :::> 6 6·20
z
goal level on the quali-
ty and quantity of per- l I I I
1
formance on an object- 5 10 15 20
listing task. (Bavelas GOAL LEVEL
and Lee 1978) ( RESPONSES)

and discovered that such incentives had a Terborg (1976) also found that goal level
substantial effect on goal setting on a task alone could not account for performance ef-
involving sorting cards into eight categories. fects. In his study, monetary incentives af-
Farr also found that incentives improved fected both goal setting and performance,
sorting speed; this was not due to a trade-off but incentives still improved performance
between speed and accuracy, because all er- even with goal setting partialed out.
rors had to be rectified. Since it has frequently been suggested
Pritchard and Curts (1973) obtained evi- (e.g. Broadbent 1971) that knowledge of re-
dence somewhat inconsistent with the posi- sults or feedback operates as an incentive, it
tion of Locke et al. (1968). After practice on is of interest to consider whether feedback
a card-sorting task, subjects were asked to has the same effects on goal setting and per-
set a goal representing a 30 per cent im- formance as incentive. Locke and Bryan
provement in performance. All of the sub- (1968) found that subjects given knowledge
jects agreed to do this, with different groups of results set higher goals and produced bet-
being offered nothing, 50 cents, or $ 3 for ter performance than those not given feed-
goal attainment. back. Moreover, when the effects of goal
According to Locke et al. (1968), perform- setting were partialed out statistically, there
ance should have been comparable in all was no residual relationship ~between
three groups because they all adopted the knowledge of results and performance. Un-
same goal. In fact, while all three groups fortunately the goal-setting data were ob-
showed more improvement than a control tained at the end of the experiment, so that it
group which had no specific goal, the $ 3 in- is possible that performance affected goal
centive led to significantly more improve- setting rather than goal setting affecting per-
ment than any other condition. It could be formance. It was subsequently found that
argued in a post hoc fashion that high-incen- performance level and knowledge of results
tive subjects actually adopted a higher goal both playa part in determining goal setting
than the other subjects or perhaps that they (Cummings et al. 1971).
were more committed to the goal. However, One of the inadequacies with Locke's
post-experimental questioning indicated (1968) theoretical approach is that he virtu-
that the high-incentive subjects had been ally ignored the important issue of the me-
slightly less committed to the goal than chanism or process mediating the effects of
those in the no-incentive condition. goal setting on performance. Terborg (1976)
90 Incentives and Motivation

made the reasonable suggestion that high ing and improved paired associate learning
goal setting is likely to lead to a greater in- and serial learning. In spite of the exiguous
vestment of effort and resources than low nature of the evidence, the likelihood is that
goal setting. The participants in his study incentive strength (determined by an inter-
spent 5 h a day for a week working on pro- action between incentive magnitude and the
grammed texts designed to teach introduc- probability of attaining it) is related to per-
tory principles of electricity. Effort was in- formance by a curvilinear function, with the
dexed by the percentage of the available point of inflexion depending on task re-
time that they spent working at the material. quirements. It may be necessary for incen-
Goal setting was directly related both to ef- tive to affect goal level if performance is to
fort expenditure and overall performance. be improved, but Locke's (1968) notion that
With effort partialed out, there was no effect it is both necessary and sufficient must be
of goal setting on performance. Thus goal rejected.
setting may not reliably predict perform- It is a truism that the effects of incentive
ance unless harder goals become translated on performance depend critically on the na-
into increased effort. ture of the task and its processing require-
Locke (1968) regarded goal level as equi- ments. At the most general level, it is clear
valent to conscious intention; it is like a dial that complex tasks are more likely than sim-
which can be turned up or down to regulate ple tasks to be adversely affected by incen-
motivational intensity. However, there is no tive. However, there have been remarkably
good reason for supposing that motivation- few systematic attempts to identify the cru-
al forces are invariably accessible to con- cial aspects of task complexity. One tempt-
sciousness. A related difficulty is that goal ing hypothesis is that incentive resembles
setting has typically been assessed by means other arousers in reducing the extent to
of post-experimental questioning, which which parallel or shared processing is possi-
may provide a very imprecise measure. ble (cf. Walley and Weiden 1973). This
In sum, Locke (1968) provided an ex- would explain the findings, since we would
tremely simple theoretical account of the obviously expect complex tasks to necessi-
way in which incentive conditions influence tate more parallel processing than easy
performance, according to which incentive tasks.
must affect goal level in order to affect per- As we saw earlier in this chapter, cogni-
formance. More specifically, performance tive flexibility is sometimes reduced by in-
was assumed to be a direct function of goal centive. This is shown by the increased tend-
level or motivational intensity. This view- ency to persist with ways of thinking which
point can be contrasted with the notion have proved successful either pre-experi-
(Yerkes and Dodson 1908) that an interme- mentally or during the initial stages of an ex-
diate level of motivation is optimal for per- periment, even when they are manifestly in-
formance. It would be unwise to be dogmat- appropriate to a new situation or problem.
ic on this issue, because relatively few stud- What lies behind this cognitive inflexibility?
ies have used more than two incentive con- It is an intriguing idea that the reduction in
ditions. Furthermore, any given incentive parallel processing under incentive condi-
has widely varying effects on different tasks. tions that was discussed previously is in-
As an example, consider the research into volved: perhaps it is essential for apparently
the impact on performance of the powerful irrelevant aspects of the environment to be
incentive of a desired apprenticeship dis- attended to in order to break a mental set.
cussed earlier in this chapter. This incentive Those researchers who have examined
worsened performance on the serial reac- the effects of incentive on two or more
tion task and the Tsai-Partington Number aspects of performance have typically
Tracing Test, had no effect on forward and found that incentive does not have a uni-
backward digit span or pursuit rotor learn- form effect. It is the rule rather than the ex-
A Theoretical Overview 91
ception for incentive to improve certain contrast, Gray proposed that individual dif-
aspects of performance while at the same ferences in susceptibility to punishment are
time impairing other aspects. One important related to the personality dimension of anx-
implication of such findings is that we must iety, with those low in anxiety (i.e. stable ex-
abandon over-simplified issues such as traverts) being considerably less susceptible
whether or not incentive improves overall to punishment than those high in anxiety
performance efficiency. (i.e., neurotic introverts).
If they are properly interpreted, the pat- It can be predicted from this theoretical
terns of performance improvement and im- analysis that those individuals showing the
pairment produced by incentive can prove greatest improvement in performance when
to be of great importance in increasing our incentives are available should be extraverts
understanding. Firstly, they indicate that in- (especially neurotic extraverts), whereas
centive leads to a re-allocation of resources those most likely to be adversely affected by
in which some processing activities are fa- incentives are those high in anxiety.
voured at the expense of others. Secondly, There is some empirical support for both
close examination of the positive and nega- of these predictions. Corcoran (1962b) used
tive effects of incentive should provide us a letter cancellation task under low or high
with some idea of the characteristic shift in motivation conditions. With low motivation
the balance of processing resources induced there was an extremely high correlation of
by incentive. Presumably the direction in + .90 between introversion and the speed
which task performance is affected by in- with which the cancellation task was per-
centive depends in part on the extent of the formed; with high motivation, the correla-
match between the requirements of that task tion became negative. High motivation
and the nature of the incentive effect. failed to affect introverts but improved the
The most ignored determinant of incen- performance of extraverts by approximately
tive effects is the internal state of the indi- 80%.
vidual, which is affected by semi-permanent Corcoran (1962b) also considered the
personality characteristics and by situation- five-choice serial reaction task. He found
al factors. What are the most crucial person- that improvement in performance speed
ality characteristics in this connection? One produced by knowledge of results correlat-
obvious contender is the extent to which an ed + .76 with extraversion. In a further ex-
individual is motivated by incentive. How- periment using the same task, incentive was
ever, there is another side to incentive: the provided by telling subjects that they could
incentivized subject has more to lose than stop doing the task when they obtained a
the non-incentivized subject ifhe or she per- specified number of correct responses. Ex-
forms poorly and is thus more likely to expe- traverts were more affected than introverts
rience frustration. Gray (1973) has argued by the incentive; under sleep-deprived con-
persuasively that the feeling of frustration ditions there was a correlation of +.59 be-
induced by non-reward resembles the anxie- tween extraversion and the incentive effect.
ty produced by punishment and is likely to A more detailed discussion of the differen-
impair performance on many tasks. tial effects of incentive on introverts and ex-
Gray (1973) put forward a theoretical traverts can be found in Eysenck (1981).
model which extended these ideas. He sug- In the next chapter we consider the pre-
gested that individual differences in suscep- diction that incentive is more likely to im-
tibility to reward depend upon the personal- pair performance for high-anxiety than for
ity dimension of impulsivity, with those low low-anxiety individuals. It has typically
in impulsivity (i.e. stable introverts) being been found that motivational factors disrupt
relatively unaffected by reward whereas the performance of those high in anxiety but
those high in impulsivity (i.e. neurotic extra- have no effect or even augment the perform-
verts) have great susceptibility to reward. In ance of low-anxiety people. Not surprising-
92 Incentives and Motivation

ly, anxious individuals tend to be more sus- produced by incentive is attenuated among
ceptible to punishment than non-anxious people exposed to intense noise, because
individuals; for example, they are much noise has an alerting and arousing function
more likely to be adversely affected by fake (Broadbent 1971).
failure feedback. If incentive is, in fact, an arouser, then its
So far, we have seen that the effects of in- behavioural effects should be equivalent to
centive on performance can only be accu- those of other arousers. Broadbent (1978)
rately predicted when some account is taken claimed that such equivalence has been de-
of individual differences in personality. In monstrated: "One of the interesting aspects
general terms, the likelihood of incentive of noise is the similarity of its effects to
improving performance is greater among those of other conditions, such as financial
extraverts than among introverts; more spe- incentives" (p. 1960). While it is reasonably
cifically, neurotic extraverts should be the certain that noise and incentive both in-
individuals most likely to benefit from the crease attentional selectivity and lead to
introduction of incentive, whereas those rapid, inaccurate responding, their equiva-
least likely to benefit from incentive should lence is still a matter of controversy.
be neurotic introverts. Davies and Jones (1975) considered the
It has also been established that relatively immediate serial recall of eight words (in-
transient states or moods help to determine tentional learning) and recall of the spatial
the exact impact of incentive on perform- location at which each word was presented
ance. In this connection, Broadbent (1971) (incidentallearning). The results are shown in
has argued that incentive is an arouser that Fig. 5.9. Intentionallearningwasimproved by
affects the same internal arousal mechanism monetary incentives, but there was no effect
as other factors such as noise and sleepless- of white noise. In contrast, incidental learn-
ness. He cited as evidence for this conten- ing was unaffected by incentives but was re-
tion the fact that incentive often interacts duced by noise. Davies and Jones drew the
with these other factors. For example, incen- following conclusion from their data: "In-
tive usually augments performance to a centives permit increased selectivity to take
greater extent for sleep-deprived than for place without reducing attentional capacity,
non-deprived individuals, presumably be- whereas in noise increased selectivity takes
cause sleep deprivation produces sub-opti- place as a result of reduced attentional ca-
mal arousal (see Chap. 7). On the other pacity" (p. 67).
hand, any improvement in performance Fowler and Wilding (1979) found that in-

80

70

j 60
«
u ",.I:::. INCENTIVE
~ 50 //
~
,/
~ 40 - /'" ,,0 NOISE
u /' ",' -,. CONTROL
a: fj,/ ",'"
~ 30 ",'"
",'"
c:J''''
20

I I Fig. 5.9. The effects of monetary incentive


INCIDENTAL INTENTIONAL and noise on intentional and incidental
LEARNING learning. (Davies and Jones 1975)
A Theoretical Overview 93
centive improved incidental learning, which is accumulating in some short-term
whereas white noise reduced it. This led storage system. There is some evidence that
them to emphasize the dissimilarities be- performance on the one-letter task is pri-
tween the effects of noise and incentive: marily affected by transformation speed,
"Noise appears to reduce attentional capac- whereas the efficiency of short-term storage
ity whilst incentives appear to increase it ... becomes more and more important as the
Distinctions between arousers exist and number of letters requiring transformation
general explanations in terms of 'arousal' increases.
are not adequate" (p. 153). However, it is The effects of white noise on this task
rather dubious to assume that incidental have been assessed (Hamilton et al. 1977) as
learning represents a direct measure of at- have the effects of monetary incentives
tentional capacity, because incidental learn- (M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck). The
ing is more a reflection of what the subject very different effects of these two variables
chooses to do than of what he or she can do. can be seen in Fig. 5.10. White noise ap-
A rather different approach to the same pears to increase the speed of letter transfor-
issue was adopted by M. W. Eysenck and mation but to reduce the ability to hold in-
M. C. Eysenck in unpublished work. They formation in short-term storage, whereas in-
used a letter transformation task in which centives increase both transformation speed
the requirement is to add a given number of and the efficiency of short-term storage.
letters to each of between one and four let- While some of these findings suggest that
ters. Thus the answer to 'SEG + 2', is 'UGI', incentive and noise do not have equivalent
and the answer to 'SNDG + 4' is 'WRHK'. effects on performance, an alternative ex-
There appear to be three major factors in- planation is tenable. If there is a curvilinear
volved in the performance of this task: relationship between arousal and perform-
(1) the time taken to access long-term mem- ance (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908), and if the
ory and locate the appropriate part of the al- fairly small financial incentives typically
phabet, (2) the speed of transformation and used were less arousing than the intense
(3) the time to store and organize the answer noise, then equivalence might be demon-

120
0
0
J).------7), EXPERIMENTAL =
110
X " NOISE
w
~ 100 -----------7~~~-----------
t::r----- _..IX"
j:: W
...J ~ 90
i=
~
z ...J
W 0
0::
~ I-
80
0:: Z
W 0
Q.
X
U 70
~ -0-
W EXPERIMENTAL =
--V
60 INCENTIVE
0
I-
<l:
0:: L I I I
2 3 4
LETTERS REQUIRING TRANSFORMATION
Fig. 5.10. The effects of incentive and white noise senck unpublished, noise data from Hamilton et
on speed of performance of a letter transforma- al. 1977)
tion task. (Incentive data from Eysenck and Ey-
94 Incentives and Motivation

strable if incentive and noise were both vi- 2. Incentive characteristically speeds up in-
gorously manipulated. Against that, we ternal processing and external response
have recently discovered that larger finan- rate but does so at the cost of reduced per-
cial incentives (up to £ 50) enhance per- formance quality.
formance on the four-letter version of the 3. Incentive usually affects an internal moti-
letter transformation task. vational state but can also produce frus-
In spite of some inconsistencies in the da- tration and/ or anxiety.
ta, it is reasonable to argue that an incenti- 4. Incentive often has adverse long-term ef-
vized individual behaves as if he or she were fects on performance, because it reduces
in a state of high arousal. Over and above or 'undermines' intrinsic motivation.
the behavioural findings, there are some in- 5. Powerful incentive reduces parallel or
teresting physiological findings reported by shared processing.
Wilkinson et al. (1972); they discovered that 6. Incentive increases arousal.
incentive led to changes in respiration rate, 7. Incentive is related to performance by
skin conductance level and pulse rate which means of a curvilinear relationship.
were indicative of increased arousal. This 8. Incentive may increase distractibility.
does not necessarily mean that incentive op- It is obviously tempting to look for in-
erates in precisely the same way as noise; terrelationships among these various ef-
for example, noise increases physiological fects, and perhaps it will do no harm to fol-
arousal in a direct manner, whereas the low Oscar Wilde in resisting everything but
arousing effect of incentive is more indirect, temptation. Part of the reason why incentive
being mediated by alterations in processing often reduces performance quality may be
strategies. because it restricts parallel processing and
According to the theoretical position tak- thus limits the information available at any
en in this chapter, it is important to consider given moment. The curvilinear function re-
the nature of the incentive, of the task, of the lating incentive to performance may be ex-
dependent variable and of the subject's state plicable in the following way: (1) the initial
of arousal and personality in investigating improvement in performance as incentive
incentive effects. Indeed, one is almost cer- increases may reflect increased attentional
tainly justified in drawing the rather strong- selectivity and (2) the subsequent perform-
er conclusion that task performance can on- ance decrement with strong incentive may
ly be predicted with accuracy when all of be due to an increased likelihood of frustra-
these variables are considered jointly. tion and anxiety combined with reduced
At present, we have relatively little idea of parallel processing. These speculations may
the ways in which these different variables not prove to be accurate; however, the for-
interact with each other. However, certain mulation of a coherent theoretical account
crucial effects of incentive do seem to pro- on the basis of these effects of incentive is an
vide the basis for a general understanding: important task for the future.
1. Incentive changes the priorities accorded
to environmental events, producing in-
creased attentional selectivity.
6 Anxiety and Performance

The concept of anxiety has been used in a help to explain the relatively weak evidence
bewildering variety of ways by different the- for increased physiological arousal in anx-
orists. While some regard anxiety as a moti- ious individuals (reviewed by Eysenck
vational state, others consider it to be an 1977).
emotion, and still others claim that it com- It will come as a surprise to some readers
bines motivational and emotional compo- to learn that degree of arousal and pleasure-
nents. In spite of these differences of opin- )displeasure are independent factors. After
ion, there would probably be reasonable all, a number of investigators (e.g. Berlyne
agreement with the following definition of 1960) have suggested that pleasure is an in-
the anxiety state that was proposed by Spiel- verted U shaped function of arousal level.
berger (1972): "Unpleasant, consciously- According to such a theoretical position, un-
perceived feelings of tension and apprehen- pleasant emotional states such as anxiety
sion, with associated activation or arousal are regarded as an inevitable consequence
of the autonomic nervous system" (p. 29). of high arousal. In fact, there was a small
A major attempt to provide a relatively but significant curvilinear relationship be-
precise analysis of all emotional states (in- tween pleasure and arousal, but it account-
cluding anxiety) was made by Russell and ed for rather less than 5 % of the variance in
Mehrabian (1977). They used 200 emotion- the pleasure-displeasure scores. The data
eliciting situations, each of which was de- were more consistent with the notion that
scribed in a short paragraph. For each situa- high arousal has an augmentation or en-
tion, the subject was asked to imagine that hancement effect: for an unpleasant emo-
he was actually there and to rate his emo- tion, high arousal tended to increase the ex-
tional state on 42 verbal report emotion 'perienced displeasure, whereas it enhanced
scales. Analysis of the data indicated that the feelings of pleasure for a pleasant emo-
the three independent and bipolar dimen- tion.
sions of pleasure-displeasure, degree of It seems reasonable to argue that the
arousal and dominance-submissiveness strength of an individual's anxiety state de-
were both necessary and sufficient to define pends primarily on two factors: (I) the de-
any emotion. In other words, all emotional gree of external threat or stress and (2) that
states can apparently be described as re- individual's susceptibility to anxiety, or
gions within a three-dimensional space. anxiety proneness. These two factors were
How does anxiety fit into this theoretical incorporated into a general theory of anxie-
framework? Russell and Mehrabian (1977), ty by Spielberger (1966,1972). According to
on the basis of results obtained with six Spielberger, there is a valid distinction be-
scales measuring fear or anxiety, claimed tween trait anxiety (defined by Spielberger
that easily the major component of anxiety et al. 1970, p. 3, as "relatively stable individ-
is displeasure, followed by high arousal and ual differences in anxiety proneness") and
submissiveness. The fact that self-reported state anxiety (defined by Spielberger et al.
anxiety is far more closely associated with 1970, p.3, as "characterized by subjective,
displeasure than with high arousal may well consciously perceived feelings of tension
96 Anxiety and Performance

and apprehension, and heightened auto- Spielberger's state-trait conceptualiza-


nomic nervous system activity"). tion leads to the additional prediction that
The basic interrelationships among trait there should be only a small difference in
anxiety, state anxiety, environmental stres- the level of state anxiety between those high
sors and performance are shown in Fig. 6.1. and low in trait anxiety under non-stressful
It is assumed that the degree of anxiety ex- conditions but that there should be a much
perienced (state anxiety) is interactively de- greater difference under conditions of
termined by the individual's susceptibility stress. The anticipated interaction between
to anxiety (trait anxiety) and by the amount trait anxiety and degree of environmental
of stress in the situation. stress has been obtained several times (for a
According to this state-trait conceptuali- review see Shedletsky and Endler 1974).
zation, state anxiety is affected by the de- An important complicating factor was
gree of environmental stress, whereas trait discovered by Hodges (1968). As can be
anxiety is not. It follows that trait anxiety seen in Fig. 6.2, he obtained the predicted
should be more consistent or stable than interaction when stress involved threat to
state anxiety across situations of varied self-esteem via failure threat, but not when
stressfulness, and this appears to be the case stress involved physical danger via threat of
(Allen 1970, Martuza and Kallstrom 1974). electric shock. A very similar pattern of re-
What accounts for the relative consisten- sults was obtained by Glanzmann and Laux
cy of trait anxiety? The answer appears to be (1978). These findings suggest that most
that hereditary influences playa crucial role questionnaire measures of trait anxiety are
(for a review see M. W. Eysenck and H. J. primarily concerned with reactions to ego
Eysenck 1980). A major study by Shields threat rather than to physical danger.
(1962) looked at hereditary effects on neu- Some relevant evidence was obtained by
roticism, a personality dimension that corre- Endler et al. (1976). They factor analysed
lates approximately + .6- +.8 with trait data from Spielberger's State-Trait Anxiety
anxiety. He used a questionnaire resembling Inventory, the Behavioural Reactions Ques-
the Maudsley Personality Inventory and ob- tionnaire and the S-R Inventory of Anxious-
tained intra-pair correlations of + .53 for ness, obtaining two major factors which
monozygotic or 'identical' twins reared they labelled interpersonal threat and physi-
apart, of + .38 for monozygotic twins reared cal danger. While the trait anxiety measure
togehter, and of only + .11 for dyzygotic of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory loaded
twins reared together. This pattern of results approximately .80 on the interpersonal
is obviously indicative of some hereditary threat factor, it only loaded .25 on the physi-
determination of neuroticism or anxiety. cal danger factor.
When Jinks and Fulker (1970) applied the Several theorists have claimed that anxie-
biometrical method of analysis to the corre- ty should be thought of as comprising two or
lations obtained by Shields they emerged more major components. For example, Lie-
with a heritability estimate of 54% for neu- bert and Morris (1967) suggested that test
roticism. anxiety is a complex amalgam of worry and

ENVIRON-
MENTAL PROCESSING
PERFORMANCE
STRESS ACTIVITIES

Fig. 6.1. State anxiety is determined jointly by en- anxiety rather than trait anxiety which has a di-
vironmental stress and trait anxiety, and it is state rect effect on processing activities
Worry and Emotionality 97
14 (0) HIGH performance on tests and (2) apparent con-
,,",,'"
"I:)
TRAIT
ANXIETY sequences of this intense worry, including
,," SUBJECTS interference with effective cognitive func-
W
12 ,,"" tioning and a variety of physical and emo-
(f "
a:: tional consequences" (p. 6).
10 LOW
They interpreted their second factor as
~
0
TRAIT
U ANXIETY reflecting "a distinct, coherent set of atti-
til SUBJECTS
8 tudes and negative emotional reactions to
>- tests" (p. 7). Since Richardson et al. found
r-
W
1: I I
that worry and emotionality items both
NONE FAILURE loaded on the same factors, they concluded
X
STRESS that their results did not support the Libert
14
Z (b) and Morris two-factor conceptualization of
HIGH
« TRAIT test anxiety. However, the relevance of their
12 ANXIETY findings to the Liebert-Morris position is re-
w ............-0 SUBJECTS
duced by their failure to use marker var-

~
I-
LOW iables for either worry or emotionality.
« 10 TRAIT
I- ANXIETY In sum, there is evidence from the field of
SUBJECTS
til psychometrics to sustain a distinction be-
8 tween state and trait anxiety. There is also
support for a distinction between the worry
l' I I and emotionality components of anxiety. As
NONE SHOCK we will see, some of the work on the effects
STRESS
of anxiety on performance has utilized these
Fig. 6.2. a State anxiety as a function of trait anx- conceptual distinctions in an attempt to
iety and failure stress. b State anxiety as a func- clarify the processes involved.
tion of trait anxiety and shock stress. (Hodges
\968)

6.1 Worry and Emotionality


emotionality. Worry is "primarily cognitive
concern about the consequences of failure"
(p.975), whereas emotionality involves Anxiety typically impairs the performance
changes in physiological functioning, and of most tasks. An interesting issue is wheth-
accompanying unpleasant feeling states of er this anxiety-induced decrement is attri-
uneasiness, tension and nervousness. butable primarily to the worry or to the emo-
The evidence supporting a distinction be- tionality component of anxiety. The preva-
tween the physiological (i.e. emotionality) lent view (e.g. Morris et al. 1977, Wine 1971)
and the cognitive (i.e. worry) components of is that worry and other task-irrelevant cogni-
anxiety was discussed by Morris et al. tive activities associated with high anxiety
(1977). They noted that several factor ana- are the most important factors involved.
lyses of Mandler and Sarason's (1952) Test This view was endorsed by Sarason (1975),
Anxiety Questionnaire have produced con- who expressed it in the following way: "The
ceptually distinct worry and emotionality highly test anxious person is one who is pro-
factors. However, the findings are equivo- ne to emit selfcentred interfering responses
cal. Richardson et al. (1977) factored the when confronted with evaluative condi-
Test Anxiety Scale and reported the results tions. Two response components have been
of a two-factor solution. According to them, emphasized by writers who espouse this
"Factor I is clearly defined by (1) cognitive view. One is emotional and autonomic -
concern and worry about oneself and one's sweating, accelerated heart rate, etc. The
98 Anxiety and Performance

other concerns cognitive events - e.g., say- notion that worry impairs performance. It is
ing to oneself while taking a test, 'I am stu- probable that worry and performance exert
pid', 'Maybe I won't pass'." (p. 175). bi-directional influences on each other.
Doctor and Altman (1969) carried out one Thus the negative relationship between wor-
of the earliest studies to evaluate the relative ry and performance might reflect the influ-
importance of these major components of ence of anticipated or actual poor perform-
anxiety in determining the level of perform- ance on worry rather than the effects ofwor-
ance. They asked students to answer worry ry on performance. Strictly speaking, of
and emotionality items from the Test Anxie- course, the correlational data which have
ty Questionnaire in terms of their feelings been obtained do not even definitely esta-
immediately prior to an important examina- blish that there is any causal relationship be-
tion. They found that both emotionality and tween worry and performance.
worry were negatively correlated with per- Deffenbacher (1978) argued that there
formance on the examination, but worry were three classes of distraction that should
was the stronger determinant of poor per- be distinguished: in addition to worry, peo-
formance. In similar fashion, Morris and ple may direct attention away from the task
Liebert (1970) found that correlations be- either to heightened autonomic arousal
tween worry scores and final examination (emotionality) or to competing response
grades, with emotionality partialed out, tendencies generated by the task. In his
were negative and significant. In contrast, study, Deffenbacher asked high and low
correlations between emotionality scores scorers on the Test Anxiety Scale to solve
and grades, with worry partialed out, were difficult anagrams under conditions of high
non-significant. or low stress. The worst level of perform-
In a recent study, Spielberger et al. (1978) ance was shown by the high-anxiety sub-
related academic performance to scores on jects run under high stress, and it was also in
the Test Anxiety Inventory, a test which this condition that self-reported anxiety was
contains separate worry and emotionality greatest. According to post-test questioning,
sub-scales. For male students, worry corre- this group of subjects experienced more in-
lated -.47 with grade point average (a mea- terference than any of the other groups dur-
sure of academic achievement), whereas ing anagram solution from emotionality,
emotionality correlated only -.13 with the worry and the task, with the effects being
same measure. For female students, the re- greatest with respect to worry. These
spective correlations were -.35 and .00. stressed high-anxiety subjects claimed that,
Further suggestive evidence that worry as a result, they had spent only 60% of the
plays a major role in producing anxiety-in- available time actually engaged in the task,
duced decrements in examination perform- compared to approximately 80 per cent in
ance was obtained by Houston (1977). He each of the other conditions.
discovered that high scorers on Debilitating We have already seen that there is plenty
Anxiety as measured by the Achievement of indirect evidence to support the notion
Anxiety scale were inferior to those scoring that anxious individuals tend to engage in
low on an examination. Close questioning more task-irrelevant processing activities
of the students revealed that the only signifi- (including worry) than non-anxious individ-
cant difference between the two groups in uals. There is somewhat more direct evi-
the coping strategies used during the exami- dence of a behavioural nature available in
nation was that those high in Debilitating the work of Ganzer (1968) and of Nottelman
Anxiety were more likely than low scorers to and Hill (1977). Ganzer looked at the joint
have experienced worry symptoms. effects of test anxiety and audience pres-
It will not have escaped the eagle-eyed ence on serial learning, and at the same time
reader's notice that none of these studies he recorded any task-irrelevant comments
really provides unequivocal support for the made by the subjects while they were en-
Working Memory Capacity 99

gaged in the learning task. He found that relevant cognitive activities associated with
high scorers on the Test Anxiety Scale, espe- anxiety always impair the quality of per-
cially if they performed the learning task in formance because the task-irrelevant infor-
the presence of an audience, emitted more mation involved in worry and cognitive self-
task-irrelevant comments than the other concern competes with task-relevant infor-
groups of subjects; most of these comments mation for space in the processing system.
were pf an apologetic or self-evaluative na- As a consequence, highly anxious subjects
ture. are in effect in a dual-task or divided atten-
N ottelman and Hill (1977) used the direc- tion situation, in contrast to non-anxious
tion of gaze as a measure of task-irrelevant subjects who primarily process task-rele-
processing in a study of anagram solving vant information.
among children obtaining high and low M. W. Eysenck (1979a) assumed that the
scores on the Test Anxiety Scale for Child- part of the processing system most heavily
ren. High-anxiety children did less well than implicated in concurrent processing of task-
low-anxiety children on the anagram task; relevant and task-irrelevant information is
this may have been due to task-irrelevant working memory. As was pointed out in
processing, since they were observed to en- Chap. 3, Baddeley and Hitch (1974) argued
gage in substantially more off-task glancing. that working memory comprises a limited
In spite of the fact that the empirical un- capacity central processing space and a sup-
derpinning for the hypothesis that anxiety plementary articulatory rehearsal loop with
impairs performance because it leads to a capacity of approximately three items. Ac-
task-irrelevant processing (e.g. worry) is cording to M. W. Eysenck (l979a), "Since
considerably less than overwhelming, it is task-irrelevant cognitive activities such as
nevertheless the case that most of the data worry preempt some of the limited capacity
are at least consistent with it. The issue of of working memory, it is clear that they will
the exact mechanism by which anxiety-in- produce decrements in the quality of per-
duced decrements in performance occur is formance. However, the extent to which
dealt with in the next section of the chapter. such decrements occur should depend on
However, it is highly probable at the most the demands placed on the capacity of
general level that attentional processes are working memmory by task-relevant infor-
implicated. It has been argued that anxious mation" (p. 365).
individuals effectively divide their attention It is of considerable theoretical signific-
between task requirements and various ance to establish the relationship between
task-irrelevant cognitive activities such as anxiety and the capacity limitations of
worry and self-criticism. This theoretical po- working memory. The reason for this is that
sition was expressed succinctly by Wine working memory is crucially involved in
(1971) in the following way: "The highly the processing and temporary holding of in-
test-anxious person responds to evaluative formation, so that any anxiety-induced re-
testing conditions with ruminative, self- duction in its capacity would inevitably
evaluative worry, and, thus, cannot direct have wide-ranging repercussions on the per-
adequate attention to task-relevant var- formance of a great variety of cognitive
iables" (p. 99). tasks. Since Baddeley and Hitch (1974) ar-
gued that working memory is involved in the
digit-span task, there are some grounds for
considering the effects of anxiety on digit-
6.2 Working Memory Capacity span performance to be relevant.
The experimental evidence has been dis-
cussed in some detail by Eysenck (1979a,
According to the theoretical position adopt- 1981). From the perspective of the state-trait
ed in this chapter, worry and other task-ir- approach discussed earlier in the chapter,
100 Anxiety and Performance

there should be more pronounced effects of The position is much clearer with respect
state anxiety than of trait anxiety on per- to the effects of situational stress and state
formance. In fact, most of the experimental anxiety on digit-span performance. Eysenck
findings are consistent with this prediction. (l979a) discussed seven studies in which
It has typically been found that trait anxiety stress was negatively related to perform-
has little or no effect on digit span, with a to- ance, together with a further four studies in
tal of nine different studies obtaining a non- which state anxiety reduced performance.
significant effect. The picture that emerges An interpretative difficulty with some of the
from studies obtaining significant results is studies looking at the effects of state anxiety
confusing, since digit span was negatively on working memory capacity is that the sub-
related to trait anxiety in three studies but jects were only asked after performing the
was positively related in two further studies. span task whether they had experienced
Rather more complex relationships be- anxiety during testing; this raises the possi-
tween trait anxiety and digit-span perform- bility that poor performance caused anxie-
ance were uncovered by Knox and Grippal- ty, rather than the reverse.
di (1970) and by Hodges and Durham Eysenck (1979a) concluded his review of
(1972). Knox and Grippaldi obtained a cur- this literature in the following words: "In
vilinear relationship between anxiety and sum, II out of 12 studies reporting a signifi-
digit span, with the level of performance be- cant effect of state anxiety or situational
ing better among those of intermediate trait stress on working memory capacity found
anxiety than among those high or low in that high anxiety reduced its capacity. In
anxiety. Hodges and Durham found that view of the fact that working memory (espe-
anxiety was positively related to digit-span cially the modality-free central processing
performance among those of high intelli- component) is vitally involved in many in-
gence, but anxiety was negatively related to formation-processing tasks, this is an im-
performance among those of less intelli- portant finding" (p. 370).
gence. It is somewhat unfortunate that only very
An alternative method of assessing the ef- limited experimental data are available in
fects of trait anxiety on short-term storage this area. For example, while it would clear-
capacity has been used in a few studies. It is ly be useful to have information about the
based on the free recall paradigm and in- effects of anxiety on each of the two princi-
volves the assumption that any item is re- pal components of the working memory sys-
called from short-term store or working tem (i.e. the modality-free central processor
memory provided that fewer than an arbit- and the articulatory loop), researchers in
rary number of input-output events (e.g. se- this area have almost invariably used only
ven) intervene between presentation and re- digit-span measures of short-term storage
call. Mueller and Overcast (1976) employed capacity rather than any more refined index.
this method and discovered that highly anx- An exception is the recent unpublished
ious subjects were able to recall more items work carried out by one of my students
from short-term store than non-anxious sub- named Anna Eliatamby. She found that
jects. However, no effects of trait anxiety on high-anxiety subjects showed inferior digit
short-term capacity as indexed by this meth- span to low-anxiety subjects under normal
od were obtained in two additional studies testing conditions. However, there was no
(Brower and Mueller 1978, Mueller 1976). A effect of anxiety on digit span when subjects
difficulty with this method of measuring the were required to perform a simple articula-
capacity of working memory is that the ap- tory suppression task (i.e. saying rapidly
parent capacity is affected by the extent to 'ABCDEF') during presentation of the di-
which subjects adopt the strategy of starting gits. Suppression tasks of this kind use the
their free recall with the most recently pre- resources of the articulatory loop but make
sented items. minimal demands on the central processor.
Spence and Spence (1966): Task Difficulty 101
If these results can be replicated, the impli- tained even more striking evidence that anx-
cation is that anxiety affects the articulatory iety pre-empts some of the available atten-
loop component of the working memory tional capacity. Subjects high and low in ax-
system more than the central processor. iety solved anagrams while counting back-
The theoretical assumption that anxiety wards by threes (memory-load condition) or
(or, more precisely, the worry component of while rehearsing over-learned material (arti-
anxiety) reduces the capacity of working culatory suppression condition); in the con-
memory can also be explored in a rather dif- trol condition, there was no additional task.
ferent way. We have assumed that worry The results for those subjects who were in-
pre-empts some of the available capacity of structed that their intellectual ability was be-
working memory, and so places the anxious ing tested are shown in Fig. 6.3. Anxiety
subject in a divided attention situation (i.e. produced a very large performance decre-
attention has to be shared between task sti- ment in the memory-load condition, presu-
muli and anxiety-related information such mably because the attentional demands of
as worry). This would appear to resemble the digit task when combined with the re-
the state of affairs when a non-anxious sub- duced attentional capacity associated with
ject is required to divide attention between a anxiety meant that very few resources were
main task and a concurrent attention-de- available for the anagram task.
manding subsidiary task (e.g. remembering
six random digits in the correct order).
In both cases, of course, the expectation is
that the additional task (Le. attending to 6.3 Spence and Spence (1966):
worry or to the subsidiary task) will worsen Task Difficulty
performance. There is recent interesting
work suggesting that the resemblance is
even closer. Hitch and Baddeley (1976) The notion that anxiety reduces the avail-
found that complexity of a reasoning task able capacity of working memory may be of
interacted with the presence or absence of a some relevance in accounting for the results
subsidiary task (Le. retaining six random di- of a substantial body of research on paired
gits); the detrimental effects of concurrent associate learning carried out mainly in the
performance of the subsidiary task on rea- 1950s and 1960s. The fact that dozens of re-
soning speed were greater on the more com- searchers carried out experiments amazing-
plex reasoning problems. ly similar to one another, busying them-
Anna Eliatamby used the same reasoning selves with comparing the effects of anxiety
task as Hitch and Baddeley (1976) and ob- on 'easy' and 'hard' paired associates, is ex-
tained a very similar interaction; however, plicable only as a tribute to the pre-emi-
she used anxiety as a factor rather than pres- nence at that time of the theoretical views of
ence or absence of a subsidiary task and Kenneth Spence. His theoretical position,
found that high anxiety had a much greater which reached its apotheosis in a chapter by
detrimental effect on the more difficult Spence and Spence in 1966, will be consid-
problems. The implication is that anxiety ered at length in order to provide some his-
operates in a similar fashion to a subsidiary torical perspective. An important additional
task, utilizing some of the available atten- reason is the fact that Spence and Spence
tional resources. Not surprisingly, the detri- (1966) made one of the very few attempts to
mental effects of either anxiety or a subsidi- provide agenaral theory of anxiety and
ary task on main-task performance are learning. Those interested in a fuller analy-
greater when the main task makes substan- sis of Spence's work are referred to Eysenck
tial demands on processing resources (e.g. (1977).
complex versions of the reasoning task). Spence and spence (1966) used as their
Very recently Anna Eliatamby has ob- starting point the fundamental Hullian as-
102 Anxiety and Performance

VI
60
u 55 HIGH-ANXIETY
w 0 SUBJECTS
VI I
z 50 I
;::
45 /
w /
~ 40 I
l- I
z 35 I
0 30 I
l- I
::> 25 I
...J
0 I
VI 20 I
z 15 I
« LOW-ANXIETY
0 10 __ II
w SUBJECTS Fig. 6.3. The effects of trait
-_ I
~ 5 --0 anxiety and concurrent task
on speed of solution of five-
CONTROL ARTICULATORY MEMORY letter anagrams. (Eliatamby
SUPPRESSION unpublished)

sumption that habit strength (a measure of tioning and reported that 21 out of 25 exper·
the degree of learning) multiplied by drive imental comparisons of high and low scor·
(a motivational measure) produces excitato- ers on the Manifest Anxiety Scale supported
ry potential. It was assumed that the habit of the prediction.
greatest strength in any given situation The position is quite different with more
would manifest itself in performance, pro- complex tasks in which the correct response
vided only that the level of excitatory poten- has to be discriminated from other compet-
tial produced by that habit when mulitplied ing responses which are stronger than the
by drive exceeded the response threshold. correct response. When such tasks are used,
Anxiety entered the picture in two rather any increase in anxiety makes the incorrect
different ways: (1) an individual's emotion- responses still stronger relative to the cor-
al responsiveness (typically assessed by rect response, and this will produce a per-
means of the Manifest Anxiety Scale) par- formance decrement.
tially determines the level of drive and The various predictions of the Spence-
(2) the emotional response associated with Spence theory have typically been tested by
anxiety produces drive stimuli which often comparing the effects of anxiety on the
lead to task-irrelevant behaviour. learning of easy (non-competitive) and hard
This theory predicts that anxiety may (competitive) paired associates. Competi-
either facilitate or impair performance, de- tive paired associates can be formed in a
pending on the precise nature of the task variety of ways, but one popular method is
which is to be performed. It follows from to take highly associated pairs of words (e.g.
the postulated multiplicative relationship man-woman, day-night and gold-silver) and
between drive and habit strength that an in- to re-arrange them into new paired asso-
crease in drive produced by anxiety will in- ciates (e.g. man-night, day-silver, gold-
crease the difference in probability of two woman). This method guarantees that there
responses differing in habit strength. Anxie- is at least one strong incorrect competing re-
ty should thus cause the stronger of two sponse for each paired associate.
competing responses to become still strong- Spence and Spence (1966) predicted that
er, so that anxiety should lead to improved anxiety would facilitate the acquisition of
performance on tasks such as simple condi- non-competitive pairs but impair the learn-
tioning, in which the correct response has ing of competitive paired associates; this
no effective competitor. Spence (1964) re- implies that anxiety should interact with
viewed the literature on anxiety and condi- task difficulty as indexed by the presence or
Spence and Spence (1966): Task Difficulty 103

I/)
w
a:
oU LOW-ANXIETY
I/) SUBJECTS
(!)
z
z
a: HIGH-ANXIETY
« SUBJECTS
w
..J

Fig. 6.4. The predicated interaction be-


tween trait anxiety and competitive
versus non-competitive paired asso-
NON-COMPETITIVE COMPETITIVE ciates on the basis of Spence's theory.
PAIRED ASSOCIATES (Spence and Spence 1966).

absence of strong competing responses (see tions of the Yerkes-Dodson Law, namely
Fig. 6.4). However, there is an important that the optimal level of arousal (or anxiety)
qualification that must be added to the pre- is inversely related to task difficulty.
diction that anxiety will reduce the speed In spite of the apparent success of the the-
with which competitive pairs are learned. ory put forward by Spence and Spence
As learning progresses and the correct re- (1966), it suffers from a number of intrac-
sponses begin to become stronger than the table problems. For example, there are cir-
competing incorrect responses, high-anxie- cumstances in which the theory must make
ty subjects should start to learn faster than exactly the opposite predictions from those
low-anxiety subjects. discussed so far. If an easy task has a domi-
Kathy Cohen, one of my students, has nant correct response and a weak competing
very kindly scoured the literature for studies response, it may well be the case under low
testing for an interaction between anxiety anxiety that only the dominant response ex-
and task difficulty. She uncovered a total of ceeds the response threshold. This ensures
54 relevant experiments; in 30 cases, the in- that there will be perfect performance with
teraction was non-significant, but in 22 the low anxiety. In contrast, the excitatory po-
predicted interaction between anxiety and tential of the weak competing response may
task difficulty was obtained. In the two re- exceed the response threshold under condi-
maining experiments (Weiner, 1966a, Wein- tions of high anxiety, because the excitatory
er & Schneider, 1971), the reverse interac- potential of all responses is increased by
tion was obtained; these studies are dis- anxiety. As a consequence, the incorrect re-
cussed in more detail later. Of the 22 experi- sponse has at least a small probability of be-
ments reporting the predicted interaction, ing produced, so that the theoretical expec-
high anxiety significantly facilitated per- tation must be that anxiety will reduce per-
formance on the 'easy' task in eight experi- formance on easy tasks of this type.
ments and significantly impaired it in three While Spence and Spence (1966) predict-
experiments. On the 'hard' task, high anxie- ed that anxiety would worsen performance
ty improved performance in two experi- on difficult tasks where the correct response
ments and worsened it in six. was not dominant, exactly the opposite re-
In general terms, it is clear that the data sult has to be predicted if the correct re-
largely support the theoretical predictions sponse is below the response threshold for
of Spence and Spence (1966). Furthermore, the low-anxiety subjects. In short, the
their theory appears to provide a potential Spences can only make definite predictions
underpinning for one of the key assump- about the effects of anxiety on performance
104 Anxiety and Performance

provided that detailed information is avail- tive list that was actually rather more diffi-
able about the number and relative cult than a competitive list. Anxiety had no
strengths of all of the relevant responses and effect on the learning of the easy competi-
about the location of the response thresh- tive list but was inversely related to speed of
old. In practice, there is typically no way of learning on the difficult non-competitive list
obtaining the necessary information. (see Fig. 6.5). The results from this study sug-
Spence and Spence (1966) were careful to gest that the important interaction is between
point out that the effects of anxiety on per- anxiety and task difficulty rather than be-
formance depended on the amount of intra- tween anxiety and degree of response
task response competition rather than on competition.
task difficulty per se. It is unfortunate that There are even more crucial problems
these two conceptually distinct factors have with the views of Spence and Spence; for ex-
usually been confounded in experimental ample, their theory assumes that the effects
studies, thus rendering any interpretation of anxiety are centred exclusively on retriev-
equivocal. al processes and that anxiety does not affect
One of the few attempts to unconfound other aspects of information processing
response competition and task difficulty such as encoding and attentional processes
was made by Saltz and Hoehn (1957). In one or the response threshold.
experiment, subjects engaged in serial learn- With the benefit of hindsight, it seems al-
ing of nonsense syllables; one list consisted most incredible that there is practically no
of familiar syllables with a high level of in- serious attempt anywhere in the voluminous
tra-list competitiveness produced by letter literature based on the theories of Spence
duplication, and a second list consisted of (1958) and of Spence and Spence (1966) to
relatively unfamiliar nonsense syllables examine the major theoretical assumption
with a low level of intra-list competitive- that anxiety affects retrieval but has no ef-
ness. While the two lists were of comparable fect at all on learning and storage. Unfortu-
difficulty for low-anxiety subjects, the the- nately, most of the research in this area was
ory put forward by Spence and Spence designed in such a way that it is quite impos-
(1966) would predict that high-anxiety sub- sible to separate out the effects of anxiety on
jects should take longer to learn the com pet- storage and on retrieval.
titive list than the non-competitive list. In One of the few exceptions is a study by
fact, the difference in learning times was Straughan and Dufort (1969). High- and
non-significantly in the opposite direction. low-anxiety subjects were given relaxation
Saltz and Hoehn obtained rather more instructions either shortly before learning a
convincing data in their second experiment. list or just prior to recall. Analysis of the da-
They managed to produce a non-competi- ta on speed of correct responding indicated

z 45 HIGH-ANXIETY
0 SUBJECTS
0::
IU
.... 40
0::
U

....0 35
V)
~ LOW-ANXIETY
-t 30 SUBJECTS
....0::

I I Fig. 6.5. Speed of learning as a func-


EASY DIFFICULT tion of trait anxiety and type of list.
COMPETING NON-COMPETING (Saltz and Hoehn 1957)
Spence and Spence (1966): Task Difficulty 105
a significant interaction b~tween anxiety the area of anxiety and performance. Even
and relaxation in which low-anxiety sub- if their explanation of the interaction be-
jects were slowed by relaxation instructions, tween anxiety and task difficulty (or intra-
whereas high-anxiety subjects performed . task response competition) must be reject-
more rapidly when relaxed. As can be seen ed, the empirical data remain and have to be
in Fig. 6.6, relaxation prior to learning was accounted for. A useful starting point is to
more effective than relaxation prior to recall examine the concept of 'task difficulty'.
in altering the response speeds of the two While there are undoubtedly many ways in
anxiety groups. The implication is that the which one task can be more difficult than
effects on learning of anxiety manipulations another, it is typically the case that 'hard'
are as strong as, or even stronger than, those tasks make greater demands on working
on retrieval. memory capacity than do 'easy' tasks. We
It is possible to demonstrate very simply have already seen that high-anxiety subjects
that some of the theoretical assumptions have less available working memory capaci-
made by Spence and Spence (1966) are er- ty than low-anxiety subjects. It thus follows
roneous. They assumed that anxiety does that the detrimental effects of high anxiety
not affect the response threshold and that should be greater with difficult tasks than
high drive in the form of anxiety increases with easy ones, and the data are in line with
the excitatory potential of all responses, that expectation. Of course, additional ex-
thus raising additional responses above the planatory principles are needed to account
response threshold. The natural expectation for the finding that anxiety sometimes leads
from these two assumptions is that high- to improved performance on easy tasks.
anxiety subjects should recall more items Is it in fact justified to assume that the
than low-anxiety subjects on a test of free re- learning of competitive or difficult paired
call. In fact, the data do not support this ex- associates makes more substantial demands
pectation. Rogers and Battig (1972) and Mu- on working memory than does the learning
eller (1976) found that anxiety had no effect of non-competitive or easy paired asso-
on free recall, and Mueller (1977) actually ciates? An initial endeavour to answer this
discovered that high-anxiety subjects had question was made in unpublished work by
significantly inferior free recall to low-anx- M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck. They
iety subjects. formed easy paired associates by pairing
In spite of its manifold deficiencies, the strongly associated words and difficult
approach of Spence and Spence (1966) is paired associates by re-arranging the pairs
still one of the most impressive theories in so that the words within each pair were very

600

~iil'550
~1;1
<>-_-------0
HIGH-ANXIETY
D..1Il SUBJECTS
~ ~
a:~
'500 LOw-ANXIETY
SUBJECTS
...Jill
<l:W
guz ·450
a:
D..~
u<l:
~...J '400

Fig. 6.6. Speed of retrieval as a func- I i i


NO RELAXATION RELAXATION
tion of trait anxiety and relaxatior. con- RELAXATION AT AT RECALL
ditions. (Straughan and Dufort 1969) ACQUISITION
106 Anxiety and Performance

weakly associated and there was a strong they make on working memory capacity,
competing response from another pair. For they undoubtedly differ in other ways as
example, one pair might be 'good-radio' well. It seems intuitively reasonable that
and a second pair 'tower-bad', with 'bad' people are more likely to experience feel-
acting as a strong competing response when ings of failure when attempting an extreme-
subjects were tested on the first pair (i.e. ly demanding task than when attempting a
'good- 1'). straightforward task, because progress is
The easy and difficult lists were presented slower and the number of errors is greater
for learning under three different condi- on the more demanding task.
tions: (I) no concurrent task, (2) the concur- The emotional reactions to different tasks
rent task of rapid overt rehearsal of the was investigated by Tennyson and Wooley
digits I - 6 in order (to utilize the articulato- (1971), who used concept acquisition self-
ry loop) and (3) the concurrent task of rapid instruction tasks with easy and difficult
overt rehearsal of six random digits in a problems. The average state-anxiety score
specified order (to utilize both the articula- on the State Trait Anxiety Inventory was
tory loop and the modality-free central pro- much higher immediately after performing
cessor). On the assumption that the difficult the difficult task than after the easy task.
paired associates require more of the re-· Spielberger et al. (1972) also found that dif-
sources of working memory than do the ficult learning tasks produced a much great-
easy paired associates, it follows that the er increase than easy tasks in the level of
concurrent rehearsal of six random digits state anxiety, particularly during the early
should be more disruptive of the learning of stages of learning.
the hard paired associates. Weiner (1966a) argued that subjects may
Reference to Fig. 6.7 indicates that the re- find that easy tasks provide feelings of suc-
sults were disappointingly ambiguous. The cess, whereas hard tasks provide feelings of
percentage reduction in the number of pairs failure. The normal interaction between
recalled under the random digit concurrent anxiety and task difficulty can then be inter-
task was indeed greater for the hard paired preted as indicating simply that high-anxie-
associates than for the easy ones. However, ty individuals learn poorly when they expe-
exactly the opposite result was obtained rience failure (i.e. on hard tasks) but learn
when the absolute reduction in the number well when they experience success (i.e. on
of pairs recalled was considered. easy tasks). This hypothesis was investigat-
While easy and difficult tasks almost cer- ed by telling subjects who were learning a
tainly differ in terms of the demands which list of difficult paired associates that they

90

....J
75
....J
«
uw 60
EASY
cr ITEMS
t- 45
z
w
U
cr 30
w
a. HARD
15 ITEMS

NONE ARTICULATORY MEMORY Fig. 6.7. Recall as a function of task


SUPPRESSION LOAD difficulty and nature of the concurrent
CONCURRENT TASK task. (Eysenck unpublished)
Spence and Spence (1966): Task Difficulty 107
were doing well relative to others, thus pro- versus failure). However, the appropriate
ducing a success experience on the difficult design was used by Weiner and Schneider
task. In contrast, those learning a list of easy (1971), and their findings are shown in Fig.
paired associates were told that they were 6.8. It should be noted that those subjects
performing poorly relative to others, in or- classified as high in resultant achievement
der to create a failure experience. scored high on need for achievement on
The results obtained by Weiner were the the Thematic Apperception Test and low on
exact opposite of those typically obtained: anxiety on the Test Anxiety Questionnaire,
high-anxiety subjects learned the difficult whereas those low in resultant achievement-
paired associates more rapidly than low- motivation scored low on need for achieve-
anxiety subjects but took longer than low- ment and high on anxiety.
anxiety subjects to learn the easy paired as- It is interesting to see that high-anxiety
sociates. This suggests that high-anxiety in- subjects actually showed faster learning
dividuals thrive on success but shrivel under than low-anxiety subjects when success
failure conditions. Weiner concluded by re- feedback was given. It is rather doubtful
lating his findings to Spence's drive theory: whether any of the subjects receiving suc-
"It is erroneous to cite prior research in this cess feedback experienced much state anx-
area as validating evidence for drive theory. iety, since one of the most reliable conse-
The important determinants of behaviour in quences of success is to reduce the level of
this situation are the cognitive and motiva- state anxiety (Gaudry 1977). The results un-
tional consequences resulting from success der success feedback are similar to those of
or failure at the task, rather than the individ- Sarason (1972), who looked at serial learn-
ual's drive level interacting with the struc- ing under several different instructional
ture of the task" (p. 342). conditions: control, achievement orienta-
Boor and Harmon (1971) quite properly tion, reassurance, motivating task orienta-
pointed out that Weiner's (1966a) experi- tion and task orientation. The low-anxiety
mental design was inadequate because it subjects learned more rapidly than the high-
does not permit one to disentangle the ef- anxiety subjects with achievement orienta-
fects of task difficulty and feedback (success tion instructions, but the opposite pattern of

SUCCESS FEEDBACK FAILURE FEEDBACK


36
z
o ,,l
f5
I-
30 I
I
I

I
a:: I
U I

224
Vl ///
Y
...J
<t:
ii:
I- 18
1" I I I
EASY DIFFICULT EASY DIFFICULT
LEARNING TASK

000----0 HIGH RESULTANT ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION


___I LOW RESULTANT ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION

Fig. 6.8. Speed ofiearning as a function of task difficulty, success versus failure feedback and result-
ant achievement motivation. (Weiner and Schneider 1971)
108 Anxiety and Performance

results was obtained under reassurance and usually reduces the quality of performance,
task orientation conditions. should be associated with increased effort
Under failure feedback conditions, Wein- and motivation. Indeed, the simplest expla-
er and Schneider (1971) found that there nation of anxiety-induced impairments of
was a non-significant interaction between performance is that anxiety leads to de-
anxiety and task difficulty. The implication creased motivation and to a withdrawal of
is that the usual interaction between those processing resources from task perform-
variables only occurs when there is no prop- ance.
er control over subjects' feelings of success How can the notion that anxiety leads to
and failure. increased motivation be justified? Miller
In this section, we have considered the (1948) argued that anxiety was an aversive
very robust finding that anxiety interacts state having motivational properties and
with task difficulty. Three main attempts that anxiety reduction was positively rein-
have been made to account for this interac- forcing. In a classic demonstration, he in-
tion, but the explanation offered by Spence itially administered electric shocks to rats in
and Spence (1966) is not generally satisfac- the white compartment of a test apparatuse
tory. It is likely that both of the remaining and allowed them to escape through a door
theories have something to recommend into the adjacent black compartment. This
them, that is to say, that anxiety has a great- procedure paired the cues of the white com-
er detrimental effect on difficult than on partment with the painful electric shocks
easy tasks both because difficult tasks make and presumably produced a conditoned
more substantial demands than easy tasks fear response.
on the capacity of working memory and be- Did this conditioned fear have drive or
cause difficult tasks are more likely to be as- motivational properties? Miller attempted
sociated with failure experiences leading to to show that it did by seeing whether the rats
enhanced state anxiety. would learn a new response in order to es-
cape from the white compartment in spite of
the fact that they did not receive any further
shocks. The argument was that if such a re-
6.4 Anxiety and Motivation sponse could be learned, it would have to be
on the basis of the motivational properties
Several theorists (e.g. Sarason 1975, Wine of conditioned fear rather than of the pain
1971) have argued that the major way in produced by shock. Miller was able to show
which anxiety affects performance is by convincingly that rats could learn the new
leading to task-irrelevant processing activi- response of turning a wheel when reinforce-
ties. If the further assumption is made that ment consisted of the fear reduction asso-
these task-irrelevant activities pre-empt ciated with escape into the black compart-
some of the processing resources of working ment.
memory, it follows that anxiety will produce A further reason why anxiety might lead
a performance decrement on all tasks de- to enhanced use of resources is suggested by
pendent on working memory. In actual fact, the theoretical analysis offered by Kahne-
anxiety has often been found to facilitate man (1973). One of his basic contentions
performance on easy learning tasks, thus was that the most potent determinant of the
suggesting that anxiety must also affect the amount of effort expended by an individual
processing system in other ways. was the evaluation of task demands. It is
Many theorists, including Miller (1948) fairly clear that the effective processing de-
and Spence and Spence (1966), have argued mands faced by anxious individuals are
that anxiety produces an increase in drive or greater than those faced by non-anxious in-
motivation. At first glance, it seems strange dividuals. This is because anxious people
and counter-intuitive that anxiety which engage in much task-irrelevant processing
Anxiety and Motivation 109
(e.g. the retrieval and analysis of anxiety-re- Revelle and Michaels (1976) have deve-
lated information and the preparation of loped a theory about the ways in which mo-
adequate cognitive coping strategies) over tivation is affected by the subjective proba-
and above the processing demanded by the bility of task success. They proposed that
task itself. In essence, the anxious individu- the following relationship exists between
al attempts to compensate for the perform- these two variables: "Moderately difficult
ance decrements produced by task-irrele- problems or situations (with probalilities of
vant processing (e.g. worry) by means of an success ranging from .5 to .I) should be ex-
increase in attentional resources. tremely motivating ('When the going gets
Does anxiety always lead to an increase tough, the tough get going'). On the other
in motivation? Common sense suggests that hand, very difficult or impossible tasks
the answer to that question is no. For exam- (probabilities of success less than .1) should
ple, if an anxious person believes that the lead to extremely low levels of motivation
probability of reducing anxiety through suc- ('Wise people do not beat their heads
cessful task performance is very low, then against brick walls')" (p. 402).
anxiety will lead to a low investment of ef- There is an further consideration which is
fort in the task. usually ignored but almost certainly plays
This combination of high anxiety and an important part in determining whether or
little effort is illustrated by the phenomenon not anxiety is associated with a high level of
of learned helplessness. In a classic study, motivation. This is the perceived instrumen-
Maier et al. (1969) placed dogs in a shuttle tality of effort expenditure in leading to a re-
box. A warning signal was presented for lOs duction in the level of anxiety. Anxiety
and was followed by 50 s of electric shock should lead to increased effort if the source
unless the dog avoided or escaped the shock of the anxiety is intrinsic to the task, i.e.
by jumping over a hurdle. Naive dogs rapid- caused by fear of the consequences of inade-
ly learned to avoid the shock, but dogs quate task performance. In contrast, there is
which had previously been subjected to a no particular reason to expect that anxiety
series of brief but very intense inescapable will be associated with increased effort if the
electric shocks showed learned helpless- cause of the anxiety is extrinsic to the task,
ness; in the words of Maier et aI., "A dog i.e. originating in events unrelated to the
which has experienced inescapable shocks task.
prior to avoidance training soon stops run- A study by Capretta and Berkun (1962)
ning and howling and remains silent until provides a clear example of task-extrinsic
shock terminates. The dog does not cross anxiety. Their subjects performed a digit-
the barrier and escape the shock. Rather it span task while walking across a swaying
seems to give up and passively accept the rope bridge over a deep ravine. Since per-
shock. On succeeding trials, the dog conti- formance on the digit-span task could not
nues to fail to make escape movements and be instrumental in reducing the anxiety
will take as much shock as the experimenter . caused by being in a dangerous environ-
chooses to give" (p. 311). ment, there is no good reason for supposing
The phenomenon of learned helplessness that Capretta and Berkun's anxious subjects
probably provides a good example of the were highly motivated to repeat the digits in
precipitous drop in motivation that can oc- the correct order!
cur when the chances of success appear mi- We have seen that any statement about
nimal. If, as seems likely, high-anxiety people the motivational consequences of high lev-
tend to be more pessimistic than low-anx- els of anxiety must be hedged with qualifica-
iety people about the probability of perfor- tions. However, it is likely that task-intrinsic
ming a task successfully, there could well anxiety typically produces increased moti-
be circumstances in which motivational level vation. There are three kinds of data that
is actually inversely related to anxiety level. provide at least indirect evidence concern-
110 Anxiety and Performance

ing the effects of anxiety on use of re- significant performance differences be-
sources: (l) self-reports of perceived effort, tween the two personality groups. In con-
(2) motivational manipulations and (3) sub- trast, there was a significant triple interac-
sidiary task performance in dual-task situa- tion involving task load, environmental load
tions. and anxiety when the perceived effort data
Dornic (1977) reported an interesting se- are considered. In this interaction, the high-
ries of experiments in which effort was mea- anxiety subjects (i.e. the neurotic introverts)
sured by means of self-report question- expended more effort than the low-anxiety
naires. In one experiment, neurotic and nor- subjects (i.e. the stable extraverts), especial-
mal subjects showed comparable levels of ly in the more demanding task conditions
performance on various versions of a high- (i.e. high task load and/or high environmen-
load task involving time stress and storage tal load).
load. On the more demanding versions of A second method of ascertaining the ef-
the task, the neurotic subjects perceived the fects of anxiety on effort expenditure in-
task as more difficult than the normal con- volves the rather more direct technique of
trols, perhaps because they exerted more ef- using instructions to manipulate the level of
fort than the normal subjects. motivation. On the assumption that high-
Rather more convincing evidence was ob- anxiety subjects are typically more motivat-
tained by Dornic (1977) in a second experi- ed than low-anxiety subjects, it should be
ment. He compared stable extroverts and more difficult to improve their performance
neurotic introverts, who can reasonably be by means of instructions designed to in-
considered as low-anxiety and high-anxiety crease motivation. This should be reflected
groups, respectively. Several versions of a in an interaction between anxiety and moti-
closed-system-thinking task were used, with vational instructions.
task difficulty being manipulated in two dif- The most popular strategy has been to
ferent ways: (1) alteration of the number of compare the effects of ego-involving (e.g.
information sources (task load) and (2) var- "This is a test of intelligence") and of task-
iation in the distraction power of semi-ver- involving (e.g. "I want to see how well the
bal extra-task stimulation (environmental apparatus works") instructions. It has usual-
load). ly been assumed that ego-involving instruc-
The key findings are presented in Fig. 6.9. tions will produce a higher level of motiva-
The first point to note is that there were no tion than task-involving instructions. While

15 8
x

...._---
l-
g6
W
e
z ~.= ._._--0 U.
10 -0
..
U.
W W
U
z -----~ e 4
« w
~ ~
0:
5
w
0 u
U. 0:
w W 2
Q. Q.

Fig. 6.9. The effects of


anxiety (stable extraver-
LOW HIGH LOW HIGH sion versus neurotic in-
TASK LOAD TASK LOAD troversion), task load
0----0 STABLE EXTRAVERTS; LOW EXTRA-TASK LOAD and extra-task load on
STABLE EXTRAVERTS; HIGH EXTRA-TASK LOAD performance and per-
0-._._.-.<) NEUROTIC INTROVERTS; LOW EXTRA-TASK LOAD ceived effort. (Dornic
----- NEUROTIC INTROVERTS; HIGH EXTRA-TASK LOAD 1977)
Anxiety and Motivation 111
ego-involving instructions are generally mo- task; there was no experiment in which anx-
tivating, they undoubtedly have other ef- iety improved subsidiary task performance.
fects as well. For example, they may contain In terms of the theoretical position of
information about task difficulty (e.g. "Few Kahneman (1973), the implication is that
people ever obtain high scores"), and they anxious subjects invest more effort and re-
may well cause anxiety by emphasizing the sources than non-anxious subjects in the
importance of the task. main task. However, this extra effort had
While the effects of this instructional ma- little or no impact on performance of the
nipulation have sometimes been disappoint- main task: there was a non-significant effect
ingly weak or inconsistent, the predicted in- of anxiety on the main task in 16 experi-
teraction between anxiety and instructional ments and a beneficial effect in only one ex-
conditions has been obtained in a number of periment.
studies of learning (e.g. Nicholson 1958, An unfortunate characteristic of most of
Sarason 1956, 1957a). Ego-involving or mo- these dual-task studies is that the main task
tivating instructions lead to improved per- was intentional learning and the subsidiary
formance only for low-anxiety subjects; in- task was incidental learning. There are as a
deed, ego-involving instructions have fairly consequence at least two possible reasons
consistently been found to impair the per- for the poor subsidiary task performance
formance of high-anxiety subjects. shown in the high-anxiety conditions:
The evidence from instructional manipu- (1) high anxiety reduced the spare process-
lations of motivation and from self-reports ing capacity available for handling the sub-
of effort expenditure is consistent in indicat- sidiary task information or (2) high-anxiety
ing that high anxiety is associated with a subjects simply decided not to process the
greater expenditure of effort. A third meth- apparently irrelevant information from the
od of measuring effort expenditure is sug- subsidiary task, although they could have
gested by the theoretical analysis offered by processed it if they chosen to.
Kahneman (1973) and discussed at greater The best evidence in favour of the first alt-
length in Chap. 4. Kahneman argued that ernative was obtained by Hamilton (1978).
spare processing capacity is inversely relat- The main task was to retain up to seven di-
ed to the amount of effort that an individual gits in the correct order for a few seconds,
devotes to a primary or main task. It is possi- and a subsidiary reaction time task was in-
ble to measure this spare processing capaci- terpolated between presentation of the digit
ty by looking at performance on a secondary string and its subsequent recall. Perform-
or subsidiary task that is carried out at the ance on the reaction time task was consid-
same time as the primary task. The general ered only on those trials on which the digit
expectation is that performance on the sub- string was recalled correctly. When the digit
sidiary task will be inversely related to effort string task was maximally demanding (i.e.
expenditure on the primary task (together seven digits), high-anxiety subjects had
with any task-irrelevant processing associat- significantly longer reaction times than low-
ed with worry). anxiety subjects. Since the subjects had
As was pointed out in Chap. 4, the typical been instructed to devote all of their spare
finding is that anxiety is associated with re- processing capacity to the subsidiary task,
duced performance on the subsidiary or in- the implication is that more effort and pro-
cidental task. There are 20 experimental cessing resources are expended on the main
comparisons in which the effects of anxiety task under high anxiety.
(either via experimental manipulation of, In sum, there is a gallimaufry of empirical
e.g., shock or as a personality variable) have data indicating that high-anxiety subjects
been considered in dual-task situations. In are more motivated and exert more effort
15 of these experiments, anxiety significant- than low-anxiety subjects. Since this extra
ly impaired performance on the subsidiary effort usually fails to translate itself into a
112 Anxiety and Performance

performance superiority for anxious sub- whereas low-anxiety subjects were either
jects, it is interesting to speculate on this ap- unaffected by failure or actually improved
parent disparity between effort investment their level of performance.
and performance level. A plausible explana- The effects of electric shock, or the
tion is that much of the extra effort exerted threat of it, are quite different. Eysenck
by anxious subjects is devoted to task-irrele- (1979a) has summarized the data from
vant processing activities such as worry and studies reporting a total of 14 experimental
cognitive self-concern. comparisons of the effects of shock on the
speed of learning of people with different
levels of trait anxiety. The threat of shock
improved the performance of high-anxiety
6.5 Failure and Shock subjects in nine cases and impaired it five
times; in contrast, shock only improved
learning for low-anxiety subjects in one ex-
Spence and Spence (1966) assumed that a periment and reduced it on the remaining 13
variety of stressors (e.g. ego-involving in- occasions. A more direct comparison of the
structions, failure feedback and electric effects of shock on the performance of high-
shock) all produced broadly comparable ef- and low-anxiety subjects revealed that in 13
fects on anxiety and performance. This intu- out of 14 cases the performance of high-an x-
itively reasonable assumption was contro- iety subjects was either less impaired or
verted by Saltz (1970), who contrasted the more improved by the threat of shock.
effects of failure feedback and electric Saltz (1970) argued that the differential
shock on the learning displayed by high- effects of failure and shock could be ex-
anxiety and low-anxiety subjects. He plained by assuming that failure is much
claimed that the research literature showed more likely to produce feelings of anxiety in
that high-anxiety subjects were more sus- high-anxiety subjects than in low-anxiety
ceptible than low-anxiety subjects to disrup- subjects, whereas shock produces greater
tion of learning under failure or threat of feelings of anxiety in low-anxiety subjects.
failure but that low-anxiety subjects were The evidence is only partially supportive of
more susceptible than high-anxiety subjects this hypothesis. Reference back to Fig. 6.2
to impaired learning under shock or threat will remind the reader that Hodges (1968)
of shock conditions. As a result, Saltz ar- discovered that failure did produce a larger
gued that it would make more sense to refer increase in state anxiety for those high in
to 'failure-disrupted' and 'pain-disrupted' trait anxiety than for those of low trait anx-
people instead of continuing to use the lab- iety, and this is in line with Saltz's position.
els 'high-anxiety' and 'low-anxiety'. Furthermore, very similar effects of failure
The empirical data certainly provide on state anxiety were obtained by Glanz-
some support for Saltz's position. People of mann and Laux (1978).
high anxiety fairly consistently show greater In contrast, there is practically no evi-
adverse effects of failure feedback than dence to indicate that high- and low-anxiety
those of low anxiety across a wide range of subjects are differentially sensitive to shock.
learning tasks. This differential sensitivity Hodges (1968) and Glanzmann and Laux
has been reflected in significant interactions (1978) found that threat of shock produced
between the level of anxiety and the type of comparable increases in state anxiety for
feedback (failure versus neutral) in a num- both anxiety groups, and the latter authors
ber of studies (Gordon and Berlyne 1954, also reported that the same result had been
Krugman 1958, Lucas 1952, Sarason 1957b, obtained in two unpublished Ph. D. theses.
Walker 1961). In these studies, high-anxiety Thus there is an apparent discrepancy be-
subjects showed a consistent impairment of tween the state anxiety and performance da-
performance as a result of failure feedback, ta: threat of shock impairs the learning of
Failure and Success: A Cognitive Approach 113

low-anxiety subjects much more than that 6.6 Failure and Success: A Cognitive
of high-anxiety subjects, in spite of the fact Approach
that shock has no differential effect on the
state anxiety experienced by the two groups.
Both findings were obtained in the same ex- A totally different approach to the effects of
periment by Glover and Cravens (1974). failure on performance has been put for-
They found that threat of shock only had a ward by Weiner (1972). Following the lead
detrimental effect on the learning perform- to Heider (1958) and other attribution theo-
ance of low-anxiety subjects, but the threat rists, Weiner claimed that the cognitively
of shock had no effect on the level of state perceived causes of failure (and of success)
anxiety of either high- or low-anxiety sub- vitally affect performance. Four major per-
jects. ceived causes of success and failure on
It is abundantly clear by now that, while achievement tasks were identified: ability,
failure feedback and electric shock may effort, task difficulty and luck. According to
both appropriately be described as stres- Weiner, these four factors can be considered
sors, they do not influence the processing within a two-dimensional framework, with
system in comparable ways. However, it has the two dimensions being the locus of con-
proved surprisingly difficult to account for trol (internal versus external) and stability
the differences between them. The most pro- over time (fixed versus variable).
mising lead was discovered by Morris and As can be seen in Table 6.1, ability and
Liebert (1973). They compared the effects of effort are both internal determinants of per-
threat of failure and threat of shock on the formance, whereas task difficulty and luck
two major components of state anxiety (i.e. are external determinants. In terms of the
emotionality and worry). Threat of failure stability dimension, which refers to relative
produced an increase in worry but not in stability over time, perceptions of ability
emotionality; on the other hand, threat of and task difficulty are less subject to change
shock increased emotionality but not worry. than effort and luck. Of course, as Weiner
These findings allow us to make sense of (1972) himself admitted, this conceptualiza-
some of the data. For example, failure feed- tion is inadequate in some respects. For ex-
back is usually more effective than shock in ample, while effort is alleged to be an un-
reducing performance for high-anxiety sub- stable factor, some people perceive them-
jects, and failure is apparently more likely selves to be diligent. With respect to ability,
than shock to produce worry. Since we have perceptions of one's general ability may be
argued that anxiety-produced decrements stable, whereas perceptions of specific abili-
in performance are largely attributable to ties can be substantially modified by experi-
worry and other forms of task-irrelevant ence.
processing, this set of results is in line with Most people seem to demonstrate self-
expectation. We will have to leave it to some serving biases when attributing causality to
Einstein of the future to account for the fact their own behaviour (for a review see Brad-
that people of low anxiety produce exactly ley 1978). That is to say, they accept respon-
the opposite behavioural pattern! sibility for successful performance (e.g. by

Table 6.1. The perceived causes of success and failure incorporated into a two-dimensional framework.
(Weiner 1972)

Locus of control
Internal External

Stability Fixed Ability Task difficulty


Variable Effort Luck
114 Anxiety and Performance

attributing it to high ability) but deny re- jects are asked to account for success and
sponsibility for failure (e.g. by attributing it failure outcomes (a sample question:
to bad luck). Obviously, such self-serving bi- "When you do well at school, is it more like-
ases can be useful in protecting or enhan- ly to be (a) because you studied for it or
cing an individual's self-esteem. (b) because the test was especially easy?").
Are there any important individual differ- Low-anxiety subjects (i.e. those of high re-
ences in the perceived causes of success and sultant achievement motivation) were more
failure? Some of the research suggests that inclined than high-anxiety subjects to
there are and that low-anxiety individuals ascribe success to ability and to effort; high-
are more likely than those of high anxiety to anxiety subjects were more likely than those
manifest self-serving biases. In the litera- of low anxiety to attribute failure to low
ture, a frequently employed measure of indi- ability and less likely to attribute failure to a
vidual differences is resultant achievement lack of effort.
motivation, which is typically based on a Similar results were obtained by Meyer
combination of scores on need for achieve- (1970). His study has the advantage over
ment (derived from the Thematic Appercep- that of Weiner and Potekan (1970) ofinves-
tion Test) and on anxiety (derived from the tigating success and failure in an actual task
Test Anxiety Questionnaire). If an individu- situation rather than in hypothetical situa-
al scores high on the Thematic Appercep- tions. He gave children a series of I5-digit
tion Test and low on the Test Anxiety Ques- progressions to solve, accompanied by fake
tionnaire, he or she is classified as high in re- success or failure feedback. As can be seen
sultant achievement motivation. Those scor- in Fig. 6.10, low-anxiety subjects (i.e. those
ing low on the Thematic Apperception Test high in resultant achievement motivation)
and high on the Test Anxiety Questionnaire tended to ascribe success to high ability and
are deemed to be low in resultant achieve- failure to bad luck, whereas high-anxiety sub-
ment motivation. jects (i.e. those low in resultant achievement
Weiner and Potekan (1970) investigated motivation) attributed success to good luck
the relationship between resultant achieve- and failure to a lack of ability. In other
ment motivation and self-ascriptions of words, those low in anxiety felt personally
causality on the Intellectual Achievement responsible for success but not for fai-
Responsibility Scale. On this latter test, sub- lure; those high in anxiety made them-

w 4
0:::":
OU
z l:3 3
0
I- >-w 2 SUCCESS
:;;) 1-0::
::::io FEEDBACK
~
0:: ail:
l- e(
l-
e( 0
1L. w>-
0 o::!: - 1
J: O..J
I- l:1!l
~ :..:e( -2
z uz
w
0:: :;;)e(
I- ..JJ: -3
Vl I-
FAILURE Fig. 6.10. Relative strengths
-4 FEEDBACK of attributions to luck and
ability as a function of type
of feedback (success versus
LOW HIGH failure) and resultant
RESULTANT ACHIEVEMENT achievement motivation.
MOTIVATION (Meyer 1970)
Depth and Elaboration of Processing 115
selves miserable by feeling responsible for proved their speed of responding, whereas
failure but not for success. Meyer also those who reacted to failure by lowering
found that the emotional reactions of pride their expectations failed to show improved
in success and shame in failure were usually performance.
more intense when the performance out- In sum, it appears that failure experiences
come was attributed to the internal factor of have differential cognitive consequences for
ability rather than to the external factor of individuals high and low in anxiety, and the
luck. same is true of success experiences. Part of
In a further experiment, Meyer (1970) ex- the reason why high-anxiety individuals ex-
posed subjects to five repeated failures on a perience more state anxiety than low-anxie-
digit-symbol substitution task. Mter each ty individuals after failure feedback is that
trial, the subjects ascribed the failure feed- they are more inclined to feel personally re-
back which they received to low ability, bad sponsible for the failure. In addition, high-
luck, lack of effort or task difficulty and also anxiety individuals seem to respond to fai-
estimated the probability of performing suc- lure by attributing it to factors which they
cessfully on the next trial. Individuals high cannot readily control such as task difficulty
in resultant achievement motivation (involv- and ability. Since these are relatively un-
ing low anxiety) were more likely than those changing factors, failure is seen as being
low in resultant achievement motivation to predictive of subsequent failure, and so
attribute failure to lack of effort and bad high-anxiety individuals show a reduced
luck and less likely to attribute failure to low subjective probability of success after fai-
ability and high task difficulty. lure. This in tum may have a detrimental ef-
Not surprisingly, there was a general re- fect on performance (Zajonc and Brickman
duction in the subjective estimates of the 1969).
probability of success with increasing num- Research on the perceived causes of fai-
bers offailure trials, but the extent of this re- lure seems to further our understanding of
duction was greatly affected by exactly the reasons why the task performance of
which causal factor was perceived as the high-anxiety people is more detrimentally
main determinant of failure. When failure affected than that of low-anxiety people by
was attributed to either of the stable factors failure feedback. However, this approach
(i.e. task difficulty or ability), there was a has proved very limited in several ways, and
much greater decline in the subjective prob- major questions have been ignored. For ex-
ability of success than when failure was ample, what determines the selection of a
ascribed to one of the variable factors (i.e. particular causal determinant of achieve-
effort and luck). As would be expected on ment behaviour? Is the selection of a causal
the basis of the data already presented, high factor veridical? How exactly does the per-
resultant achievement motivation (involv- ception that performance is determined by a
ing low anxiety) was associated with a much given causal factor affect subsequent behav-
smaller decrement over trials in the subjec- iour? Are workers in this area investigating
tive probability of success than was low re- epiphenomena rather than the true causes
sultant achievement motivation. of behaviour?
What are the consequences of a reduced
subjective probability of success for per-
formance? Zajonc and Brickman (1969)
answered this question in the context of a 6.7 Depth and Elaboration of Processing
study in which fake feedback was intro-
duced on a simple visual reaction time task.
They found that those who resisted lowering One of the most immediately obvious short-
their expectations concerning subsequent comings of most of the work on anxiety and
performance after failure feedback im- performance has been the failure to capital-
116 Anxiety and Performance

ize on relevant theoretical advances that An equally damaging attack on the Craik-
have occurred in other areas. A partial ex- Lockhart formulation was made by Stein
ception to this is some of the recent research (1978). A list of words was presented with a
on anxiety and memory which has been single letter capitalized in each word (e.g.
based on the stimulating theoretical frame- 'cHair'), and either a semantic or a shallow
work for memory provided by Craik and task had to be performed on each \yord. The
Lockhart (1972). Craik and Lockhart's basic semantic task involved deciding whether
contentions were that the memory trace is a each word fitted appropriately into a sen-
residue of attentional processes and that tence frame, whereas the shallow task re-
there are an almost limitless number of ways quired decisions about whether each word
in which a stimulus can be processed. Sti- had a particular word capitalized. An unex-
mulus encodings were seen as varying in pected recognition test was subsequently
terms of the amount of meaningfulness ex- aqministered; this either required the sub-
tracted from the stimulus or, in their pre- ject to pick out the list word from among a
ferred terminology, the 'depth' of processing. number of non-list words (normal test) or to
Craik and Lockhart (1972) regarded se- select the exact stimulus presented from a
mantic analysis as involving deep process- set of stimuli that only differed from the pre-
ing, whereas physical or phonemic analysis sented stimulus in terms of the capitalized
only necessitates shallow processing. The letter (a sample of this case-oriented test is
crucial prediction that follows from this the- as follows: Chair, cHair, chAir, chaIr).
oretical analysis is that retention should be The results are shown in Fig. 6.11. They
positively related to the depth of processing. indicate very clearly that retention does not
There is much empirical support for this depend exclusively on the depth of process-
prediction (see Lockhart et al. 1976, for a re- ing at input; rather, it depends on the relev-
view). In the usual paradigm, subjects per- ance of the encoded information to the re-
form an orienting task on each of several quirements of the retention test. Thus the
words and are then unexpectedly asked for only information that is relevant to selecting
free recall. Depth of processing is manipu- the input stimulus from the set 'Chair cHair
lated by means of a variety of orienting chAir chaIr' is information about which let-
tasks, ranging from the semantic (e.g. think ter was capitalized: the shallow task led to
of an adjective to go with each word) to the the storage of that information whereas the
physical (e.g. decide whether each word is semantic task did not, and so shallow pro-
printed in upper or lower case). The typical cessing led to better retention than deep pro-
finding is that free recall is much higher for cessing.
semantically processed words than for phy- A final difficulty with the position advo-
sically or phonemically encoded words. cated by Craik and Lockhart (1972) is that
This theoretical approach to memory has they did not explain in any detail precisely
been criticized in a variety of ways. An im- why deep encodings should be better re-
portant inadequacy is the lack of any inde- membered than shallow encodings. It has
pendent measure of the depth of processing, been pointed out many times (e.g. Eysenck
so that it is often difficult to know whether a 1979b) that semantic encodings will tend to
particular form of processing is shallow or be more elaborate or extensive than phon-
deep. Eysenck (1978) pointed out with refer- emic encodings due to the fact that the num-
ence to actual examples "the danger of us- ber of potentially encodable semantic fea-
ing retention-test performance to provide tures of a word is considerably greater than
information about the depth of processing, the potential phonemic word attributes.
and then using the putative depth of pro- In related work, Jacoby (1974) and Ey-
cessing to 'explain' the retention-test per- senck (1979b) have discovered that the se-
formance, a self-defeating exercise in circu- mantic encoding of a word tends to be more
larity" (p. 36). distinctive or unique than the phonemic en-
Depth and Elaboration of Processing 117
100
NORMAL TEST
90
z
0 80
....
z 70
(!)
0 60
u
w
Q: 50
....
z 40
w
u
Q: 30
CASE-ORIENTED
W TEST
Q.
20
Fig. 6.11. Recognition performance as a
function of type of orienting task (case ver- 1: I i
sus semantic) and the nature of the recog- SHALLOW SEMANTIC
nition test (normal versus case-oriented). (CASE)
(Stein 1978) ORIENTING TASK

coding. The reason for the distinctiveness of verts are low in anxiety (Gray 1973), we will
semantic encodings is that the semantic en- hereafter refer to the dimension investigated
coding of a word in a particular context is by Schwartz as one of anxiety.
different from the semantic encodings of Schwartz's (1975) first experiment in-
the same word when presented in other volved a paired-associate learning task in
contexts, so that one semantic encoding which the response words were either all
of a word is discriminable from other se- phonemically or semantically similar. Since
mantic encodings of the same word. Trace response similarity typically interferes with
discriminability is probably much lower in learning, it would be auticipated that se-
the case of phonemic en co dings. mantic response similarity would impair
Since the depth of processing is frequently performance if the subjects processed the
confounded with the elaboration or distinc- words semantically; in similar fashion,
tiveness of encoding, it is important for re- phonemic response similarity will interfere
search to attempt to separate out the effects of with learning provided that phonemic pro-
each ofthesefactors. In practice,studies ofthe cessing occurs.
effects ofanxiety on memory have not usually The main feature of the data was a highly
been responsive to the increasing theoreti- significant three-way interaction among neu-
cal complexity in this area of research. roticism, extraversion and list type (see Fig.
The first major attempt to consider possi- 6.12. High-arousal (or high-anxiety) subjects
ble inter-relationships between personality did not show any adverse effects of semantic
and depth of processing was made by similarity, perhaps because they were focuss-
Schwartz (1975). On the basis of previous re- ing on the physical attributes of the present-
search, he proposed the following hypothe- ed material, and low-arousal (or low-anxie-
sis: "Arousal facilitates recall of the actual ty) subjects were unaffected by phonemic si-
physical properties of verbal stimuli but ad- milarity because they concentrated on se-
versely affects memory for semantic fea- mantic processing.
tures" (p. 2). In the two experiments which In a second experiment, Schwartz (1975)
he reported, Schwartz assigned people to considered the effects of arousal or anxiety
the various arousal groups on the basis of on free recall of a categorized word list pre-
their neuroticism and extraversion scores on sented in a random order. High-arousal (or
the Eysenck Personality Inventory. The high-anxiety) subjects recalled the list in a
highest arousal group consisted of neurotic less semantically organized way than sub-
introverts and the lowest arousal group con- jects with lower levels of arousal or anxiety
sisted of stable extraverts; since neurotic in- and tended to recall the words in the order
troverts are high in anxiety and stable extra- in which they were presented. These results
118 Anxiety and Performance

15
(!)
z
Z PHONEM ICALLY
Q: SIMILAR
<{ 12
;:r------o LIST
w
..J /
(!) /
/
Z 9 /
Q:
:::> , /
/

""
0 SEMANTI CALLY
III
6 d SIMILAR
Q:
0 LIST
Q:
Q:
W
z 3
<{
w
~
Fig. 6.12. Learning errors on
STABLE
phonemically and semantically
EXTRAVERTS EXTRAVERTS similar lists as a function of
STABLE NEUROTIC introversion-extraversion and
INTROVERTS INTROVERTS neuroticism. (Schwartz 1975)

are consistent with the notion that high terminology usually employed, anxiety will
arousal or anxiety leads to reduced semantic reduce the extensiveness or elaboration of
processing but to increased physical pro- encoding.
cessing. This elaboration hypothesis has been in-
A failure to replicate some of Schwartz's vestigated in several studies by Mueller and
key findings was reported by Craig et al. his associates (Miller et al. 1978; Mueller
(1979). They considered paired-associate 1976, 1977, 1978, unpubl.; Mueller et al.
learning with either semantically or phon- 1978, Mueller et al. 1977). In the majority of
emically similar stimuli; neurotic extraverts these studies, a free recall paradigm was
showed better learning than any of the other used in which the list words could be organ-
groups (i.e. stable extraverts, stable intro- ized along shallow (physical) or semantic
verts and neurotic introverts), which is in- lines.
consistent with Schwartz's hypothesis. Fur- Anxiety significantly impaired retention
thermore, when subjects were given the test performance in 6 out of 11 experimental
learning task after the administration of caf- comparisons and had no effect in the other
feine (a known arousing agent), it was pri- five comparisons. There was no support for
marily the low-arousal (stable extraverts) the notion that anxiety impairs semantic
and high-arousal (neurotic introverts) processing more than shallow processing:
groups that showed improved performance. in eight out of nine comparisons, there were
This pattern of results is rather mysterious equivalent effects of anxiety on retention of
from the perspective of a simple arousal semantic and shallow features or attributes.
model and suggests that the four personality In the remaining experiment (Mueller un-
groups may not vary in arousal level in the published), anxiety significantly reduced
way proposed by Schwartz. retention of phonemic information but had
A rather more plausible hypothesis has no effect on retention of semantic informa-
been proposed and tested by Mueller in a tion; this result is the exact opposite of that
series of publications. As Mueller (1979) predicted by Schwartz (1975).
pointed out, if anxiety leads to reduced or Further evidence that anxiety reduces ela-
restricted processing, then the natural ex- boration of encoding was obtained by M. C.
pectation is that fewer features or attributes Eysenck and M. W. Eysenck in unpublished
will be encoded under high anxiety. In the work. A list of to-be-remembered words was
Depth and Elaboration of Processing 119

followed by a test of cued recall. Each retrie- high arousal reduces short-term rentention.
val cue was either semantically related (e.g. Since states of high anxiety tend to be ac-
'BAT associated with -') or phonemically companied by high arousal (see Eysenck,
related (e.g. 'HALL rhymes with -') to one 1977a, for a review), it can be predicted from
of the list words. In addition, the strength of Walker's action decrement theory that the
the relationship between the word used as anxiety-produced decrements in perform-
the retrieval cue and the list word was vari- ance at the relatively short retention inter-
ed, on the assumption that more elaborate vals used in the majority of the published
encoding would be required for recall if studies should be reversed at longer renten-
there were a weak relationship between the tion intervals.
two words rather than a strong one. As is In fact, the available evidence does not in-
shown in Fig. 6.13, there was a highly signi- dicate that anxiety leads to superior long-
ficant interaction between anxiety and cue term retention. Pagano and Katahn (1967)
strength, with high anxiety greatly reducing compared recall for high and low scorers on
recall with the weak retrieval cues but hav- the Test Anxiety Questionnaire. They con-
ing little effect on recall with the strong re- trolled for original learning and found that
trieval cues. These findings were interpreted anxiety had no effect on recall at retention
as reflecting reduced elaboration of encod- intervals of 24 hand 7 days. Mueller (1978
ing under high anxiety; it remains unclear unpUblished) has consistently obtained a si-
whether this narrowness of encoding occurs milar pattern of results. Ray et al. (1971)
at input, at output or at both output and in- looked at memory for a complex verbal task
put. 2 days after learning. Their results showed
The basic finding that retention is re- that high-anxiety subjects had forgotten
duced under high anxiety can also be ex- more than low-anxiety subjects.
plained by Walker's (1958) action decre- In essence, it has been discovered that
ment theory. This theory states that high anxiety reduces retention test performance
arousal leads to a longer lasting active mem- for deep and shallow features or attributes
ory trace than does low arousal, and this and at all retention intervals. While these
produces superior long-term retention. It is findings are inconsistent with the predic-
further assumed that there is an inhibition tions of Walker's (1958) action decrement
of retrieval (or 'action decrement') during theory and of Schwartz's (1975) hypothesis,
the time that the memory trace remains ac- they are reasonably in line with the expecta-
tive. It follows from this assumption that tions of the elaboration hypothesis favoured

60

III 5·0
w
cr STRONG
0 CUES
U 4·0
III
...J
...J
<t 3·0
U
W
cr 20
z WEAK
<t CUES
W
2 1·0

Fig. 6.13. Recall as function of strength NEUROTIC


EXTRAVERTS
of the retrieval cues, introversion-ex- STABLE NEUROTIC
traversion and neuroticism. (Eysenck INTROVERTS INTROVERTS
and Eysenck unpublished) ( ANXIETY)
120 Anxiety and Performance

by M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck and subjects attending to task-relevant informa-


by Mueller (1979). tion. Nottelman and Hill (1977) found that
high-anxiety children performing an ana-
gram task engaged in substantially more off-
task glancing than low-anxiety children,
6.8 Theoretical Positions which seems diametrically opposed to the
expectation from Easterbrook's hypothesis.
Easterbrook's hypothesis also appears to
A theoretical approach which can plausibly generate the counter-intuitive prediction
account for many of the major effects of that high anxiety is associated with a re-
anxiety is Easterbrook's (1959) hypothesis, duced susceptibility to distraction. This pre-
which was discussed at some length in diction received some support in studies by
Chap. 4. Easterbrook's fundamental con- Bruning et al. (1968) and by Zaffy and Brun-
tention is that states of high emotionality, ing (1966). Learning occurred in the pres-
arousal and anxiety all produce a restriction ence of no cues, task-relevant cues or task-
in the range of cue utilization. In somewhat irrelevant cues; the beneficial effect of rele-
more contemporary terminology, the basic vant cues and the adverse effect of irrelevant
idea is that arousal and anxiety lead to a nar- cues were smaller for high-anxiety than for
rowing of attention. low-anxiety subjects, indicating a reduced
Evidence consistent with Easterbrook's susceptibility to distraction under high anx-
hypothesis has been obtained in dual-task iety.
situations involving a main and a subsidiary On the other hand, the opposite pattern of
task. The key finding is that anxiety impairs results has been obtained in other studies.
performance on the subsidiary task more Pallak et al. (1975) looked at performance
than on the main task, which suggests that on the Stroop test under conditions of high
anxiety increases attentional selectivity. and low conflict or distraction. Anxious
If one assumes that difficult tasks tend to subjects performed the task better than non-
involve more cues than easy tasks, then the anxious subjects under conditions of low
narrowing of attention under high anxiety distraction, but performed it worse under
should have a greater detrimental effect on conditions of high distraction. Dornic
the performance of difficult tasks. As we (1977) also obtained evidence that distrac-
saw earlier in the chapter, one of the more tibility was positively related to anxiety.
robust findings in the area of anxiety and Some final inadequacies of Easterbrook's
performance is the interaction between anx- formulation as it applies to anxiety remain
iety and task difficulty, with high-anxiety to be considered. The first problem is that
subjects performing much worse than low- Easterbrook regarded anxiety as a unitary
anxiety subjects on hard tasks, but not on construct, whereas there is fairly convincing
easy ones. evidence that anxiety comprises the two
The least plausible assumption made by components of worry and emotionality. The
Easterbrook (1959) was that anxiety impairs finding that worry and emotionality have
performance on difficult tasks by producing rather different effects on performance can-
great concentration on only a few task ele- not readily be accounted for by Easter-
ments. On this assumption, high-anxiety brook. Related to this problem is the puzzle
subjects spend a greater proportion of the of interpreting the differential effects on
time processing task-relevant information performance of electric shock and failure
than low-anxiety subjects, whose broader feedback, since Easterbrook's hypothesis
range of cue utilization may well include implies that both stressors should have
non-task elements. In fact, Deffenbacher equivalent behavioural consequences.
(1978) discovered that high-anxiety subjects Finally, even if attentional narrowing un-
actually spent far less time than low-anxiety der high anxiety does occur, there are alter-
Theoretical Positions 121

native ways of conceptualizing the process. verse effect of anxiety on performance.


While Easterbrook regarded attentional However, it is unclear how he would explain
narrowing as a relatively passive and auto- the existence of performance improvements
matic process, it may well prove more fruit- under high anxiety, and he largely ignores
ful to regard it as an active coping response the motivational implications of anxiety ma-
when the processing system is in danger of nipulations.
becoming overloaded. A theoretical conceptualization resem-
A interesting theoretical analysis was of- bling those of Easterbrook (1959) and Ham-
fered by Hamilton (1975). He argued that ilton (1975) has been put forward by Ey-
the major effect of anxiety was to give rise to senck (1979a, 1981). One of the central
complex cognitive processing: "Anxiety can points which he made was that the level of
be defined as internally generated cycles of performance achieved by an individual only
connotative signals elicited by external sti- provides an indirect and fallible indication
muli, which a central interpreting or apprai- of the way in which the processing system is
sal process codes as requiring avoidance, functioning. A concrete example will make
and as indicating physical danger, injury to this clear. If you went on a long run with an
self-esteem, rejection, and loss of affection outstanding athlete such as Sebastian Coe,
in valued social settings" (p. 50). Such task- he might be kind enough to run at the same
irrelevant processing competes with task- speed as you, with the result that your per-
relevant information in a processing system formance in terms of the number of seconds
having limited capacity. taken to complete the run would be the
According to Hamilton, the exact effects same. It would obviously be invalid to con-
of anxiety on task performance can be pre- clude that you were equally gifted runners.
dicted only in the light of the available pro- While the run would leave you in a state of
cessing capacity. The various theoretical collapse gasping for air, Sebastian Coe
ideas were incorporated into a formula stat- would be breathing normally and would
ing that a task will be performed successful- scarcely have broken sweat. The relevance
ly when average processing capacity plus of this analogy to the effects of anxiety is
spare processing capacity are equal to or that anxious individuals may perform as ef-
greater than internally generated task-irrele- ficiently as non-anxious individuals but only
vant information (produced by anxiety) at greater 'subjective cost' to the system.
plus internally generated task-relevant in- It may prove useful to distinguish be-
formation plus externally generated task- tween performance efficiency (which is sim-
relevant information. ply a measure of the quality of performance)
Any tendency for the available informa- and processing effectiveness (which refers
tion to exceed the available processing re- to the relationship between the quality of
sources will lead to a decrement in main- performance and the effort or processing re-
task performance. This will frequently hap- sources invested in it). More specifically,
pen under conditions of high anxiety, be- the relationship between processing effec-
cause anxiety usually produces internally tiveness and performance efficiency can be
generated task-irrelevant information. expressed in terms of the following formula:
When the total information-processing de- . . efficiency
processmg effectiveness = ff
mands cannot be handled by the available e ort
processing capacity, a probable reaction is The separate effects of anxiety on process-
to restrict attention voluntarily to a small ing effectiveness and on performance effi-
number of information sources. This will ciency were spelled out by Eysenck (1979a):
lead to the attentional narrowing predicted "Since a major component of anxiety is
by Easterbrook (1959). task-irrelevant cognitive activity or worry,
Hamilton's views certainly provide a which reduces the working memory capaci-
plausible explanation for the typical ad- ty available for processing task information,
122 Anxiety and Performance

it follows that anxiety will reduce process- ty has thus been found to be far more likely
ing effectiveness with respect to any task to reduce than to enhance performance of a
making demands on working memory. subsidiary task (P < .001).
However, ... the extent to which anxiety At the most general level, these results in-
either facilitates or impairs performance is dicate the advisability of using dual-task
determined by the extent to which high-anx- paradigms to investigate the effects of anx-
iety subjects compensate for reduced pro- iety on performance. More intriguingly,
cessing effectiveness by enhanced effort" they suggest that the typical non-significant
(p.365). effect of anxiety on performance efficiency
In other words, it is expected that anxiety of the main task masks the adverse effects of
will influence processing effectiveness more anxiety on processing effectiveness, which
than performance efficiency. The great ma- only becomes manifest when subsidiary
jority of studies have produced weak anxie- task performance is considered.
ty effects, perhaps because they were de- While Eysenck (1979a, 1981) emphasized
signed so that only information about per- the importance of the conceptual distinc-
formance efficiency was obtained. Perform- tion between processing effectiveness and
ance efficiency under high and low anxiety performance efficiency, he also made a var-
can only be used as a direct indication of iety of theoretical assumptions. For exam-
processing effectiveness provided that one ple, he assumed that the main reason that
assumes that effort or resource expenditure anxiety reduced processing effectiveness
is comparable under conditions of low and was because of additional task-irrelevant
high anxiety. However, as the evidence re- processing necessitated by worry and cogni-
viewed earlier in this chapter indicated, ef- tive self-concern. This task-irrelevant pro-
fort expenditure is usually greater under cessing pre-empts some of the limited ca-
high anxiety, so that the quality of perf9rm- pacity of working memory, so that there is
ance as a function of anxiety does not accu- reduced working memory capacity to pro-
rately reflect the effects of anxiety on pro- cess task-relevant information.
cessing effectiveness. Since anxious individuals have to process
The clearest evidence that a distinction both task-relevant and task-irrelevant infor-
between processing effectiveness and per- mation, whereas non-anxious individuals
formance efficiency is necessary is available only need to process task-relevant informa-
in the dual-task studies discussed earlier in tion, the effective task demands are greater
the chapter. It will be recalled that anxiety for anxious individuals. In line with Kahne-
had a non-significant effect on performance man's (1973) theoretical analysis of effort, it
of the main task in 16 out of 20 experimen- is hypothesized that anxious individuals
tal comparisons; in other words, anxiety react to these increased task demands with
characteristically had no effect on perform- greater effort expenditure. These various
ance efficiency. It would be extremely mis- theoretical assumptions generate a number
leading, however, to conclude that anxiety of testable predictions, including the follow-
had no effect at all on the processing system. ing:
The analysis of subsidiary task data for 1. Performance is impaired by the worry
those 16 experimental comparisons in component of anxiety rather than by the
which there was no effect of anxiety on emotionality component.
main task performance is instructive in this 2. The worry component of anxiety reduces
regard. Of the 16 subsidiary task comparis- the working memory capacity available
ons, five indicated a non-significant effect for task processing.
of anxiety on performance, none revealed a 3. Given that anxiety reduces working mem-
facilitatory effect of anxiety and II pro- ory capacity, anxiety will interact with
duced a significant impairment of perform- task demands, with the detrimental ef-
ance as a function of anxiety. Overall, anxie- fects of anxiety increasing directly with
Theoretical Positions 123
the demands that tasks place on the ca- 7. If high- and low-anxiety subjects are in-
pacity of working memory. duced to utilize near maximal effort on a
4. The reduced working memory capacity task (e.g. via incentives or motivational
associated with high anxiety means inter instructions), then the reduced processing
alia that elaboration of encoding will vary effectiveness under high anxiety will be
inversely with anxiety. reflected directly in task performance.
5. With certain qualifications, anxiety leads There are numerous experimental studies
to increased effort expenditure. providing empirical support for each of
6. Anxiety will always reduce processing ef- these seven predictions. However, as we
fectiveness but will not impair perform- have indicated throughout the chapter,
ance efficiency if there is sufficient effort much of the research in this area has been ill
expenditure. This means that anxiety will conceived and methodologically unsound.
usually have a greater detrimental effect A satisfactory theory of anxiety and per-
on the subsidiary task than on the main formance still lies some way off in the
task in dual-task situations. future.
7 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

In this chapter we consider a variety of inter- 7. Introversion-Extraversion


nal or endogenous factors contributing to-
wards an individual's level of arousal. It has
been claimed that even in the absence of a 7.1.1 Theoretical Background
stimulating environment, some individuals
will characteristically be in a more aroused The most complete theory of the personality
state than others. The dimension of individ- dimension of introversion-extraversion was
ual differences most frequently referred to put forward by H. J. Eysenck (1967). He ar-
in this connection is introversion-extraver- gued in favour of a three-dimensional per-
sion (H. J. Eysenck 1967), the usual assump- sonality space comprising the three ortho-
tion being that introverts are chronically gonal dimensions of introversion-extraver-
more aroused than extraverts. sion, neuroticism and psychoticism. Each of
A second endogenous factor affecting the these personality dimensions was regarded
level of arousal is the t; me of day. It has as having a substantial hereditary compo-
been found that there are circadian rhythms nent and as having an identifiable physio-
in a number of physiological and behaviour- logical basis. Differences in introversion-ex-
al measures, and some of the physiological traversion were thought to depend upon
measures suggest that the level of arousal cortical arousal stemming from the ascend-
fluctuates between its nadir during the mid- ing reticular activating system, whereas dif-
dle of the night to its apogee at some point ferences along the neuroticism-stability di-
between mid-day and the early evening. mension are based on activation of the vis-
There is an interesting link between time-of- ceral brain, i.e. the hippocampus, amygdala,
day effects and introversion-extraversion, cingulum, septum and hypothalamus.
because there is accumulating evidence (e.g. The most crucial assertion of this theory
Blake 1967b, Revelle et al. 1980) that intro- for present purposes is that introverts are
verts and extraverts differ with respect to the more cortically aroused than extraverts. In
timing of some of their circadian rhythms. this connection, H. J. Eysenck (1967) sug-
A third factor that relates to time-of-day gested that "skin conductance and alpha ac-
effects is sleep deprivation. Broadbent tivity are measures of extraversion" (p. 170),
(1971) has reviewed the research in this area but the physiological evidence is equivocal.
and concluded that one of the typical effects Gale (1973) has provided an excellent re-
of sleep deprivation is to lower the level of view of the EEG findings, following the
arousal. A consideration of the physiologi- usual procedure of regarding low amplitude
cal and performance characteristics of the and high frequency of alpha as indicative of
sleep-deprived state, together with those of high arousal. Of the 16 studies discussed by
introversion-extraversion and time of day, Gale, seven supported the hypothesis that
constitutes the subject matter of this chap- introverts are more aroused than extraverts,
ter. three found that extraverts were more
aroused than introverts and the remaining
studies obtained non-significant EEG dif-
Introversion-Extraversion 125
ferences between introverts and extraverts. arousal earlier in the day than extraverts.
It should be noted that there is a methodo- There are additional physiological and be-
logical problem with some of the studies, in havioural data indicative of a phase differ-
that the subjects were simply instructed to ence, and it has recently been claimed that
'do nothing': such instructions obviously this is attributable primarily to the impulsiv-
fail to provide adequate control over the ity component of extraversion rather than to
subject's cognitive processes and behaviour. the sociability component (Revelle et al.
Gale attempted to reconcile the discrepant 1980).
findings in a post hoc manner by claiming H. J. Eysenck (1967) incorporated the as-
that most of the data were consistent with sumption that introverts are more cortically
the notion that extraverts are more aroused aroused than extraverts into a broader theo-
than introverts when the experimental retical framework. He argued that this grea-
procedure is either very interesting or un- ter cortical arousal of introverts made them
remittently tedious, with introverts being more conditionable than extraverts; as a
more aroused than extraverts in moderately consequence, introverts show greater fear
arousing situations. conditioning than extraverts and are thus
Somewhat more convincing evidence has more law abiding and also more likely to de-
been obtained from drug studies. In particu- velop dysthymic symptoms (phobias, obes-
lar, if introverts are chronically more sions, anxiety states and reactive depres-
aroused than extraverts, then there are cer- sion). In contrast, extraverts condition poor-
tain predictable consequences of adminis- ly, and their low level of fear conditioning
tering stimulant and depressant drugs. As can lead to criminal and psychopathic be-
can be observed any night at the local pub, haviour.
the depressant drug alcohol frequently has Gray (1970, 1972, 1973) has proposed a
an extraverting effect, presumably because theory of introversion-extraversion that
it reduces the level of arousal. More formal bears a close resemblance to that of H. J. Ey-
evidence was obtained by Laverty (1958). senck (1967) but with a number of specific
He found that injections of sodium amytal differences. He disputed the notion that in-
(a depressant) produced a significant shift troverts are, in general, more readily condi-
towards greater extraversion on a personali- tioned than extraverts and pointed out that
ty questionnaire as well as associated beha- the opposite is true under some circum-
vioural changes (e.g. increased talkativeness stances (e.g. Eysenck & Levey 1972). If in-
and sociability). In addition, the amount of troverts are only more conditionable than
sodium amytal required to produce slurred extraverts under certain conditions, then it
speech (the so-called sedation threshold) is implausible to attribute the greater law-
was greater for introverts than for extra- abidingness of introverts to their superior
verts. conditionability. Gray (1972) suggested a
We have seen already that introverts are simpler reason for the presence of more con-
only more aroused than extraverts under ditioned fear in introverts than in extraverts:
certain circumstances. A potentially import- "We may regard the dimension of introver-
ant example of this was discovered by Blake sion-extraversion as a dimension of suscep-
(1967b), who found that the body tempera- tibility to punishment and non-reward: the
ture of introverts was higher than that of ex- greater the degree of introversion, the grea-
traverts during the morning and early after- ter is this susceptibility" (p. 194).
noon, whereas the opposite was the case At the physiological level, Gray (1973)
during the evening. To the extent that body argued that detailed examination of the
temperature reflects the prevailing level of extraverting effects of barbiturate drugs
arousal, the implication is that there is a (especially sodium amytal) and of alcohol
phase difference in the circadian rhythm of suggested a modification of Eysenck's
arousal, with introverts attaining peak hypothesis that differences in intro-
126 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

version-extraversion represent differences orbitofrontal-septo-hippocampal stop sys-


solely in the activity of the ascending reticu- tem. Individual differences in susceptibility
lar activating system. As an alternative, Gray to reward, on the other hand, are primarily
proposed that physiological differences be- associated with the diagonal from stable in-
tween introverts and extraverts depend up- troversion to neurotic extraversion; this ap-
on a feedback loop comprising the ascend- proximates to the personality trait of impul-
ing reticular activating system, the frontal sivity and derives from the degree of activity
cortex, the septal area and the hippo- in a physiological approach system involv-
campus. ing the medial forebrain bundle and the la-
While Gray (1973) agreed with H. J. Ey- teral hypothalamus.
senck (1967) that the two-dimensional space Gray (1973) suggested that his anxiety
formed by the introversion-extraversion and impulsivity dimensions are located 45°
and neuroticism-stability dimensions was of away from Eysenck's dimensions of neurot-
major importance, he claimed that Eysenck icism and introversion-extraversion. How-
had not identified the primary lines of caus- ever, it may be preferable to locate anxiety
al influence. Work on animal learning and closer to the neuroticism dimension than to
physiology has uncovered two major sys- introversion-extraversion; the reason is that
tems characterized by the emotional reac- measures of anxiety such as the Manifest
tions to signals of reward or non-delivery of Anxiety Scale typically correlate approxi-
anticipated punishment, on the one hand, mately + 0.7 with neuroticism compared
and to signals of punishment or frustrative with only about -0.3 with extraversion. If
non-reward, on the other hand. Gray (1973) the anxiety dimension were re-located as
concluded that these dimensions of suscep- suggested, and the impulsivity dimension
tibility to reward and susceptibility to pun- remained orthogonal to it, then impulsivity
ishment could be located fairly precisely would be closer to the dimension of intro-
within Eysenckian two-dimensional space version-extraversion than to neuroticism.
(see Fig. 7.1). Questionnaire data reported by Eysenck
Individual differences in susceptibility to and Eysenck (1977) are consistent with this
punishment are represented by the diagonal expectation.
running from stable extraversion to neurotic The theoretical positions of H. J. Eysenck
introversion; this corresponds roughly to and Gray are sufficiently similar to make it
the anxiety dimension as measured by the difficult to adjudicate between them. For
Manifest Anxiety Scale and has as its phy- example, both theorists predict stronger
siological basis the degree of activity in the conditioning in introverts than extraverts

NEUROTICISM

ANXIETY IMPULSIVITY
(Dimension of, / (Dimension of
/ ...
susceptibility',
to punishment) " // s~~c~~~~:~t)y
" ,, //
/

,,
, /
/
/
/

/ ,,
INTROVERSION /
,, EXTRAVERSION
,
/
/
/
/

/
/
/ " ,,
" ,,,
/
/
/
/
/
,,
/
/
/
, Fig. 7.1. Gray's proposed re-Iocation of the major
/
" dimensions within H. 1. Eysenck's two-dimen-
STABILITY sional space. (Gray 1973)
Introversion-Extraversion 127
when aversive unconditioned stimuli are 7.1.2 Reward and Punishment
used (although for somewhat different rea-
sons). However, Eysenck's theory continues A number of researchers have looked at the
to predict greater conditioning of introverts effects ofreward and punishment on the be-
when appetitive unconditioned stimuli are haviour of introverts and extraverts. Nichol-
used, but Gray's theory predicts the oppo- son and Gray 1972) reported some encou-
site on the basis of the extravert's greater raging early findings. They assumed that
susceptibility to reward. In general, the evi- non-delivery of anticipated reward (i.e. frus-
dence on this point is more in line with trative non-reward) was punishing and ar-
Gray's prediction. gued that high anxiety people (i.e. neurotic
A pertinent question is whether the causal introverts) should be more affected than low
lines of influence on behaviour correspond anxiety people (i.e. stable extraverts) by
more closely to the dimensions proposed by such punishment. In this context, they con-
Gray or to those put forward by Eysenck. As sidered two phenomena that have been dis-
yet, there is little relevant evidence. It has covered in operant conditioning situations:
been found that the salivary response to behavioural contrast and peak shift. Both
lemon juice relates more strongly to intro- phenomena refer to the impact of a negative
version-extraversion than to impulsivity stimulus (signalling no reward or reduced
(Eysenck and Eysenck 1969). However, eye- reward for responding) on responding to
blink conditioning (Eysenck and Levey other stimuli, and it was expected that this
1972) and the behavioural effects of caffeine impact would be greater for those high in
(Revelle et al. 1980) both relate more closely anxiety. In line with this expectation, there
to impUlsivity than to introversion-extraver- was a greater peak shift among high-anxiety
sion. It is, of course, entirely possible that subjects than low-anxiety subjects, and the
there are causal influences operating at same was true (albeit non-significantly) for
several different points within the two-di- measures of behavioural contrast. In addi-
mensional personality space postulated by tion, sensitivity to reward in the form of gen-
Eysenck and Gray. eralization of responding to stimuli resem-
There are at least two additional attrac- bling previously rewarded stimuli was grea-
tive features of Gray's theory: (1) individual ter among high than low impulsives, which
differences are linked securely to well-re- was also in accord with theoretical expecta-
searched reward and punishment systems in tion.
the brain; and (2) situations that will pro- There is other evidence that extraverts are
duce differential effects on introverts and more susceptible than introverts to reward.
extraverts are easy to devise, since it is rela- Corcoran (l962b, 1965) has reported a num-
tively straightforward to manipulate reward ber of studies on the effects of incentives on
and/or punishment contingencies in the en- tasks such as the five-choice serial reaction
vironment. task and a letter-cancellation task. The typi-
While Gray's assumption that introverts cal finding was that extraverts responded to
are more sensitive than extraverts to signals incentives with improved performance,
of punishment and non-reward presumably whereas the performance of introverts was
has implications for individual differences unaffected.
in arousal, his is not primarily an arousal Gupta (1976) investigated Gray's theory
theory. In contrast, Eysenck has empha- in a study of verbal operant conditioning in
sized the higher cortical arousal of intro- which he used various conditions of positive
verts than of extraverts. This has the great ad- and negative reinforcement. Introverts con-
vantages of relating together a diverse set of sistently conditioned better than extraverts
findings and of readily generating testable with negative reinforcement, but extraverts
predictions. were more affected than introverts by the
provision of reward.
128 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

Gupta and Nagpal (1978) essentially re- to make mistakes and become confused as a
plicated those findings and additionally car- result of thinking about too much at once. If
ried out separate analyses on the two major these reports are veridical, then they are
components of extraversion (i.e. impulsivity consistent with the hypothesis that intro-
and sociability). Gray's theory implies that verts have a narrower range of cue utiliza-
the effects of positive reinforcement should tion than extraverts.
be greater for impulsivity than for sociabili- A rather more interesting study was carri-
ty (unsociability = high anxiety), whereas ed out by Amelang et al. (1977). The main
the effects of negative reinforcement should task was to remember a string of eight con-
be greater for sociability than for impulsivi- sonants for up to 25 seconds, and there was
ty. In fact, the data indicated that the effects a subsidiary visual reaction time task during
of the various reinforcement conditions the retention interval. Introverts performed
were very similar on both components of ex- worse than extraverts on the subsidiary task,
traversion. especially early in the retention interval.
However, it is not clear that this indicates
7.1.3 Attention: Selectivity and greater attentional selectivity on the part of
Distractibility introverts, because they also showed poorer
memory performance at the shorter reten-
Following from the work of Easterbrook tion intervals.
(1959), it has often been assumed that high M. W. Eysenck and M. C. Eysenck
arousal leads to a reduced range of cue utili- (1979b) investigated the hypothesis that in-
zation together with increased attentional troverts are less able than extraverts to en-
selectivity. The most direct method of test- gage in parallel or shared processing. They
ing these assumptions is to use a dual-task used the Sternberg paradigm, in which sub-
paradigm: high arousal should be more like- jects have to decide as rapidly as possible
ly to impair performance of the subsidiary whether or not a probe stimulus matches
or secondary task than of the main or pri- any of the items in a short memorized list. In
mary task. the single task conditions, subjects either
There are a few studies relating Easter- had to look for an exact match between the
brook's theoretical ideas to the performance probe and one of the memorized items or for
of introverts and extraverts. In one of the a semantic match in which the probe exem-
earliest studies, Imam (1974) presented his plified one of the category names in the
subjects with nonsense syllables enclosed in memorized list. In the dual-task condition
geometrical figures, having instructed them subjects had to look for both kinds of match
to learn the syllables. The obvious predic- at the same time. Broadly speaking, there
tion that introverts would show less inciden- were only small effects of introversion-ex-
tal learning than extraverts of which shapes traversion on speed of responding in the
went with which syllables was not support- single-task conditions, but introverts were
ed, since there was no difference in inciden- slower than extraverts in the dual-task con-
tal learning between introverts and extra- dition in which parallel processing was re-
verts. quired. The findings were generally consist-
Some indirectly relevant evidence was ob- ent with the hypothesis that high arousal re-
tained by Nideffer (1976), who used a self- duces the capacity for parallel processing.
report inventory called the Test of Atten- However, arousal was also manipulated in
tional and Interpersonal Style. Extraverts this study by using conditions of quiet and
perceived themselves as being able to inte- of 85 db. white noise, and this manipulation
grate many stimuli, to use information or had no effect on performance. This suggests
ideas from several different areas effectively that an interpretation of the findings in
and to process a great deal of information. terms of arousal may not be valid.
In contrast, introverts reported a tendency Anderson and Revelle (unpublished) con-
Introversion-Extraversion 129

sidered the effects of impulsivity (one of the discovered that the learning of extraverts
major components of introversion-extraver- was less detrimentally affected than that of
sion) and caffeine (a stimulant drug that in- introverts by the introduction of extraneous
creases arousal) on proofreading of a pas- visual interference or distraction. In a subse-
sage. The passage containede non-contextual quent study (Howarth 1969), subjects were
errors (which could be detected by examin- asked to learn serial lists of numbers, with
ing single words) and contextual errors distraction consisting of additional irrele-
(which could only be detected by consider- vant numbers. There was no effect of intro-
ing groups of words). On the assumption version-extraversion on learning when there
that arousal leads to a reduction in the range was no distraction, but extraverts outper-
of cue utilization, they predicted that formed introverts under the distraction con-
aroused subjects should be less sensitive than ditions.
non-aroused subjects to contextual errors. Some of the most convincing results were
There was no effect of either impulsivity obtained by Morgenstern et al. (1974). The
or drug condition (caffeine versus placebo) subjects' task was to learn auditorily pre-
on detection of non-contextual errors. How- sented words for recall; distraction consist-
ever, impulsivity interacted with drug condi- ed of additional auditorily presented words,
tion with respect to detection of contextual a German prose passage or an English prose
errors. As predicted, subjects in the highest passage. Recall was not affected by introver-
arousal condition (i.e. low impulsives given sion-extraversion in the absence of distrac-
caffeine) performed poorly and so did high tion, but distraction impaired the recall of
impulsives receiving the placebo. introverts much more than extraverts.
It is unfortunate that most of the research When we consider the various findings on
has failed to use the appropriate experimen- the effects of introversion-extraversion on
tal design for testing the hypothesis that in- attentional selectivity and distractibility, we
troverts display greater attentional selectivi- are confronted by an apparent paradox. The
ty than extraverts. Such a design would in- data on attentional selectivity indicate that
volve two concurrent tasks (one of which introverts have a more restricted range of
was clearly designated as the primary task), cue utilization than extraverts, and this nar-
together with recording of the level of per- rowed attention leads to poor processing of
formance on each task. These are necessary stimuli that are not of direct relevance to the
requirements, because changes in attention- primary task. If this is so, why should intro-
al selectivity can only be assessed by com- verts' performance suffer more from distrac-
paring performance on at least two concur- tion caused by irrelevant stimulation?
rent activities. Nevertheless, even in the ab- One resolution of the paradox would be
sence of any convincing experimentation, it to argue that introverts allocate a greater
still seems likely that introverts usually man- proportion of their available attentional re-
ifest greater attentional selectivity than ex- sources than extraverts to the performance
traverts. of the primary task. This produces poor sub-
At the very least, we can say that most of sidiary task performance by introverts and
the evidence reviewed so far in this section also means that they have fewer processing
is consistent with Easterbrook's (1959) hy- resources available to cope with the rejec-
pothesis. However, that hypothesis seems to tion demands posed by irrelevant environ-
imply that high arousal (e.g. introversion) is mental stimuli, or to differentiate clearly be-
associated with intense concentration on a tween task and non-task stimuli.
few of the task-relevant stimuli and thus
presumably with low distractibility. What
are the effects of introversion-extraversion
7.1.4 Vigilance Task Performance
on distractibility? In the first study on this While the effects of introversion-extraver-
issue, Shanmugan and Santhanam (1964) sion on dozens of performance tasks have
130 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

been considered over the years (see H. J. Ey- statistical significance. Carr (1971) dis-
senck 1967), most of the findings are rather covered that introverts not only made fewer
fragmentary and inconsistent. One of the false alarms than extraverts but also detect-
major exceptions is work on vigilance, ed more signals. Tune (1966) used the mea-
where there is a reasonably clear picture. sures of signal detection theory and found
In a typical vigilance task, people are that extraverts made many more false
asked to detect the occasional and unpredic- alarms than introverts and thus had a lower
table occurrence of an auditory or visual sig- beta.
nal. The task often lasts for an hour or more, Perhaps the most thorough investigation
and the most important single finding is that was by Harkins and Geen (1975). They used
performance usually declines over time (the a 40-min visual vigilance task and found
so-called vigilance decrement). Broadbent that introverts had much greater sensitivity
(1971) has reviewed the literature on vigil- (d') than extraverts. They also found that in-
ance and concluded that the primary reason troverts set a higher criterion for reporting
for the vigilance decrement is a progressive signals than extraverts.
increase in the cautiousness of responding It is rather surprising that there have been
over time. very few attempts to account for the greater
In those studies that have assessed the ef- cautiousness in responding of introverts.
fects of introversion-extraversion on vigil- However, Eysenck (1981) pointed out that
ance performance, the modal finding (Ba- the setting of the response criterion is
kan 1959, Bakan et al. 1963, Davies and known to be affected by the subjective gains
Hockey 1966, Harkins and Geen 1975, Keis- associated with correct responding as well
ter and McLaughlin 1972) is that introverts as by the subjective costs associated with in-
show superior performance to extraverts. In correct responding (i.e. false alarms). If, as
addition, extraverts show a more pro- Gray (1973) has claimed, introverts are
nounced vigilance decrement than intro- more susceptible to punishment than extra-
verts (i.e. the superior performance of intro- verts but are less susceptible to reward, then
verts is more apparent late in the experimen- it follows that introverts may attach relative-
tal session). ly more importance than extraverts to the
It is possible to arrive at a more precise potential costs of false alarms, whereas ex-
description of the effects of introversion-ex- traverts may ascribe more importance than
traversion by using the approach of signal introverts to the potential gains of correct re-
detection theory (e.g. Green and Swets sponding. As a consequence, introverts
1966), which makes use of the two parame- would tend to set a higher criterion point for
ters d' and beta. Beta is a measure of the their responses than extraverts.
amount of information required for re- How are we to account theoretically for
sponse emission (i.e. it reflects the cautious- the crucial finding that extraverts exhibit a
ness of responding), whereas d' is a measure more marked vigilance decrement than in-
of the subject's sensitivity to signals. In gen- troverts? As a starting point, it may well be
eral terms, the greater the tendency to say relevant that the deterioration of vigilance
that a signal has occurred when it has not performance over time is usually accompan-
(i.e. the higher the false alarm rate), the low- ied by a progressive decrease in physiologi-
er will be the resultant value for beta (indi- cal arousal (measured by EEG alpha, skin
cating incautious responding). conductance, etc.). If extraverts are chroni-
Most of the available evidence suggests cally less cortically aroused than introverts
that introverts adopt a more stringent re- (H. J. Eysenck 1967), then their declining
sponse criterion than extraverts. Krupski et performance may be due to the develop-
al. (1971) found that the positive correlation ment of a sub-optimal level of arousal in the
between false alarms and extraversion on an boring and monotonous conditions of the
auditory vigilance task just failed to attain typical vigilance task.
Introversion-Extraversion 131
The most obvious way of testing this hy- ey (1966), who observed the effects of in-
pothesis is to increase the level of arousal tense (95 db) white noise on performance of
experienced by extraverts while performing a visual vigilance task. Their key finding
a vigilance task; the prediction must be that was that intense noise eliminated the vigil-
their performance decrement can thereby be ance decrement in extraverts but had very
attenuated or eliminated. This research little effect on introverts (see Fig. 7.2 A).
strategy was first used by Davies and Hock- A conceptually similar approach was

PANEL A PANEL B
12 65
C/l
Z
Q
10 ~ 60
0
~
...,l
~
/ "

.';loo.....
I- . ,-,,"'6,
u
w
I-
8 ~ 55
W
W I-
a w
I- 6 a 50
u I-
W Z
a:: 4 w 45
a:: u
0 a::
u w
z 2 a.. 40
<f

l' l'
W
~
I I I I

- ..
2 3 4 1 2 3
10·min periods of task 16·min . periods of task
EXTRAVERTS; QUIET _ EXTRAVERTS; NO DRUG

.---
~ EXTRAVERTS
INTROVERTS;
f:r._.-f:, INTROVERTS;
NOISE
QUIET
NOISE
0----0 EXTRAVERTS, CAFFEINE
6---" INTROVERTS; NO DRUG
tr·_·-t,. INTROVERTS; CAFFEINE

PANEL C. PANEL D.
a 27 12
w
I- C/l
I- 25 Z
0 10
~ 23 f=
0 u
C/l W
...J 2 1 I- 8
<f W
z 19 a
~ I-
u 6
C/l 17

~
w
>-
a:: 15
<f
,.n...... ,,/.A
a::
a::
0
4 b/
~ ·-·a· u
a:a.. 13
z 2
6 <f
Z 1 W
<f
w Z I I I I
~

~
2 3 4 5 2 3 4
16-min.periods of task 10min.perlods of task
_ EXTRAVERTS, PRIMARY ONLY _ EXTRAVERTS, FREQUENT SIGNALS
0----0 EXTRAVERTS, TWO TASKS o--<l EXTRAVERTS; INFREQUENT SIGNALS
!:r._.-t,.INTROVERTS, PRIMARY ONLY . - - -.. INTROVERTS; FREQUENTS SIGNALS
. - - - . INTROVERTS; TWO TASKS !:r.-.~ INTROVERTS; I NFREQUENT SIGNALS

Fig. 7.2. A Vigilance performance as a function of formance as a function of presence versus ab-
noise and introversion-extraversion. (Davies and sence of concurrent task and introversion-extra-
Hockey 1966) B Vigilance performace as a func- version. (Bakan 1959) D Vigilance performance
tion of caffeine and introversion-extraversion. as a function of signal frequency and introver-
(Keister and McLaughlin 1972) C Vigilance per- sion-extraversion. (Davies and Hockey 1966)
132 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

adopted by Keister and McLaughlin (1972). is some central control system that attempts
They considered the effects of caffeine (a to compensate for the adverse effects of sup-
drug increasing arousal) on auditory vigi- ra-optimal or sub-optimal levels of arousal
lance. In essence, they found that caffeine and that this control system is more exten-
eliminated, and even reversed, the normally sively used by introverts.
inferior vigilance performance of extraverts,
whereas the performance of introverts was
unaffected (see Fig. 7.2 B). Further analysis 7.1.5 Learning and Memory
revealed that the only subjects producing a
vigilance decrement were the extraverts not The literature on the effects of introversion-
receiving caffeine. Keister and McLaughlin extraversion on learning and memory has
drew the following conclusion: "It is theo- been summarized in various places (Ey-
rized that the effect of caffeine was to in- senck 1976b, 1977, 1981). Accordingly, the
crease the cortical arousal of the extraverts emphasis here will be on the major conclu-
and make them equal in performance to the sions that can appropriately be drawn.
introverts who characteristically function at One of the most robust findings in the re-
a high arousal level" (p. 10). search on arousal and memory is that high
The notion that extraverts perform poorly arousal impairs short-term retention (up to
on vigilance tasks because they are insuffi- approximately 20 min after acquisition) but
ciently aroused can also be applied to an facilitates long-term retention. This pattern
earlier study by Bakan (1959). He looked at of performance has been found several
performance of a primary auditory vigi- times when arousal has been manipulated
lance task, which was either performed on by means of white noise or time of day. It
its own or in conjunction with a secondary has also been obtained when the effects of
task; Extraverts were more likely than intro- retention interval on the memory of intro-
verts to improve their performance of the verts and extraverts have been assessed. The
main task when the secondary task was in- modal finding is that extraverts have better
cluded (see Fig. 7.2 C); this may be due to short-term recall than introverts but that this
increased arousal produced by the greater is reversed at longer retention intervals. The
demands imposed on the system when two striking results obtained by Howarth and
tasks must be performed at the same time. Eysenck (1968) are shown in Fig. 7.3. It is
Fig. 7.2 D shows the differential effect of worth noting, however, that there are some
time on task on the vigilance performance of studies in which no interaction between in-
introverts and extraverts; the data are those troversion-extraversion and retention inter-
of Davies and Hockey (1966). The four pan- val was obtained (Fuller 1978, McLaughlin
els together reveal an interesting phenomen- 1968, McLaughlin and Kary 1972, Schneller
on: three arousing factors (i.e. white noise, and Garske 1976).
caffeine and additional task) and one de- The interaction between introversion-ex-
arousing factor (i.e. time on task) all pro- traversion and retention interval has usually
duce substantial changes in the perform- been explained in terms of Walker's (1958)
ance of extraverts, but have no effect on in- action decrement theory. According to this
troverts. In contrast, there is evidence (H. J. theory, high arousal produces a longer last-
Eysenck 1967) that introverts are more af- ing active memory trace, leading to en-
fected physiologically than extraverts by a hanced consolidation and long-term memo-
stimulus of standard intensity. How do in- ry. During the consolidation period, there is
troverts manage to maintain a relatively in- a transient inhibition of retrieval (called 'ac-
variant level of performance when their le- tion decrement') which protects the trace
vel of physiological arousal changes sub- from disruption. This theory is unsatisfacto-
stantially? One possibility, which is dis- ry for a number of reasons, the main one be-
cussed at greater length later on, is that there ing that some of the available data are in-
Introversion-Extraversion 133

12
'\
11 \ INTROVERTS
o \\
ij 10 \
--J \
<{ \
\
~
a::
(/)
9 b----_-o.
,
o 8
a::
o
3:
z 7 EXTRAVERTS
<{
w
~ 6
Fig. 7.3. Recall as a func-
tion of introversion-ex-
traversion and retention 1 I I I I
interval. (Howarth and Omin 1min 5min 30min 1day
Eysenck 1968) RETENTION INTERVAL

consistent with it. For example, while high complexity is that tasks involving response
arousal does typically impair short-term re- competition (e.g. certain retroactive inter-
tention in paried associate learning, it actu- ference paradigms) are more difficult and
ally enhances short-term free recall and re- demanding than those not involving re-
cognition (Eysenck 1976a). It must be con- sponse competition. In other words, Jen-
cluded that no satisfactory explanation of sen's findings are consistent with the
the interaction between introversion-extra- Yerkes-Dodson law. In addition, there are
version and length of the retention interval at least three other studies (reviewed by Ey-
has as yet been offered. senck 1981) in which extraverts were less af-
Another factor which has received some fected than introverts by response competi-
attention is task difficulty. It derives its the- tion.
oretical relevance from the idea (Yerkes and Of course, the Yerkes-Dodson law merely
Dodson 1908) that the optimal level of describes the predicted relationship be-
arousal is inversely related to task difficulty. tween arousal and task difficulty and does
On the assumption that introverts are more not explain it. Eysenck (1975a) discovered
cortically aroused than extraverts, it follows that the poor performance of introverts on
that introversion-extraversion should inter- paired associate lists involving reponse
act with task difficulty and that extraverts competition was largely attributable to their
should cope relatively better than introverts slow retrieval of the relevant information.
with very demanding tasks. He argued that high arousal increased the
Jensen (1964) carried out a large-scale tendency to retrieve readily accessible infor-
factor-analytic study that involved some mation, which is counter-productive when
consideration of task difficulty and came to there are very accessible but incorrect re-
the following conclusion: "Extraverts per- sponses available (e.g. in conditions of re-
form better than introverts, especially in sponse competition).
serial learning, and extraversion had a load- There is an interpretative problem with
ing of + .41 on the general learning ability much of the literature on introversion-extra-
factor. Extraversion seemed to correlate version and memory, since it is usually un-
mainly with resistance to response competi- clear whether observed differences in reten-
tion" (pp.7-8). The relevance of Jensen's tion are due to effects at the time of initial
findings to the issue of the effects of intro- perception, attention, rehearsal, consolida-
version-extraversion on tasks of varying tion, retrieval or response emission. One of
134 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

my concerns in conducting research in this some time on the task. Since more common
area has been to utilize paradigms that per- category exemplars tend to be emitted in-
mit investigation of a single aspect of pro- itially, the retrieval difficulty of introverts
cessing. This can be achieved by, for exam- seemed to centre on retrieval of relatively
ple, studying the effects of introversion-ex- rare and non-dominant items.
traversion on the retrieval of well-learned This serendipitous finding was explored
information from permanent storage. Vo- in several subsequent studies (Eysenck
cabulary tests can be used to ensure that 1974b, 1975a, b, c). It was consistently
there has been equivalent storage of rele- found in a variety of paradigms that intro-
vant information by introverts and extra- verts were primarily inferior to extraverts
verts, thus permitting the conclusion that when retrieving relatively inaccessible kinds
performance differences reflect the effects of information, with the opposite happen-
of introversion-extraversion on the process ing when accessible information had to be
of retrieval itself. retrieved. Eysenck (1976a) proposed the fol-
The most popular task has been a test of lowing hypothesis that was consistent with
verbal fluency in which the subject must most of the available data: "High arousal
write down as many words as possible with- has the effect of biasing the subject's search
in a given time period fulfilling some speci- process towards readily accessible, or func-
fied criterion (e.g. words starting with the tionally dominant, stored information more
letter R or animal names). The typical find- than is the case with lower levels of arousal"
ing is that extraverts think of more words (p. 461). While this hypothesis remains an
than introverts, indicating that they have a attractive one in some ways, it is rather un-
more efficient retrieval process. clear how the hypothesis can explain why
Similar results have been obtained with incentives (which are typically arousing) do
tasks assumed to measure creativity or di- not produce impaired efficiency of retrieval
vergent thinking ability. For example, White (see Chap. 5).
(1968) found that extraverts produced many A related approach to the issue of the ef-
more responses than introverts on two di- fects of introversion-extraversion on speed
vergent thinking tasks (the Alternate Uses of retrieval was taken by M. W. Eysenck and
test and the Consequences test), and he cit- M. C. Eysenck (1979b) in a study already
ed other work in which similar findings had discussed. They used a modified version of
been obtained. the Sternberg paradigm, with up to four
Recent work has clarified some of the fac- words (the memorized set) being followed
tors involved in the effects of introversion- by a probe word. Subjects had to decide
extraversion on retrieval. Eysenck (1974a) either whether there was a physical match
obtained the usual retrieval advantage for between the probe and one of the memor-
extraverts over introverts when self-report- ized words or whether there was a semantic
ed arousal or activation was high, but there match. As is shown in Fig. 7.4, there was no
was no difference in retrieval between intro- effect of introversion-extraversion on scan-
verts and extraverts who reported them- ning speed in the physical match condition,
selves to be low in arousal. This suggests but extraverts scanned faster than introverts
that the normal retrieval deficit in introverts under semantic matching conditions. It is
is due in some way to excessively high interesting to note that the typical inferiority
arousal. of introverts to extraverts in the efficiency of
In the study by Eysenck (1974a), subjects retrieval found in earlier research always in-
were asked to think of as many words as volved semantically based retrieval.
possible belonging to five semantic catego- Schwartz (1979) presented two words si-
ries. Detailed analysis of the data indicated multaneously under one of three instruc-
that differences in recall between introverts tional conditions: physical identity, requir-
and extraverts only became apparent after ing a decision based on whether or not the
Introversion-Extraversion 135
1300
If) 1200
u
w 1100
If)
~
1000
~
900
w
~ 800
~
z 700
0
~ 600
u
-c{
w 500
a:.
l' I I i
2 3 4
Fig. 7.4. Reaction time as a function of MEMORY-SET SIZE
memory set size, introversion-extra- f:r..-.-t:. INTROVERTS; PHYSICAL MATCH CONDiTiON
version and type of match (physical ver- A---. INTROVERTS; SEMANTIC MATCH CONDITiON
sus semantic). (Eysenck and Eysenck ~ EXTRAVERTS; PHYSICAL MATCH CONDITION
1979b) - EXTRAVERTS; SEMANTIC MATCH CONDITION

two words were identical; homophone verts adopt a more stringent response crite-
identity, requiring a decision on identicality rion than extraverts in vigilance tasks, and
of pronunciation (e.g. 'deer-dear'); and tax- the same seems to be true of memory tasks.
onomic category identity, involving a deci- McLaughlin and Kary (1972) found that ex-
sion whether the two words belonged to the traverts made more correct responses and
same semantic category. While introverts more errors or fals alarms than introverts in
and extraverts responded at equivalent a recognition test. Gillespie and Eysenck
speed on physical identity trials (801 versus (1980) analysed the data from a continuous
780 ms, respectively), introverts were much recognition memory task in terms of the
slower than extraverts on homophone trials measures of signal detection theory and
(1095 versus 953 ms) and taxonomic catego- found that introverts adopted a significantly
ry trials (1205 versus 1058 ms). more stringent response criterion than ex-
These findings are mostly in line with the traverts; there was no effect of introversion-
notion that introverts experience greater dif- extraversion on sensitivity or d'.
ficulty than extraverts in accessing relatively In spite of the relatively small volume of
inaccessible deep or semantic information research on the effects of introversion-extra-
from long-term store but that there is no ef- version on learning and memory, there are
fect of introversion-extraversion on the re- various fairly robust findings, the more im-
trieval of shallow or physical information. portant of which are as follows:
Unfortunately for this hypothesis, the find- 1. The learning of introverts is more disrupt-
ing that introverts are slower than extraverts ed than that of extraverts by distracting
at making phonemic decisions in the homo- stimulation.
phone condition of Schwartz's study poses 2. The learning of introverts is more disrupt-
problems for it.It may be preferable to sup- ed than that of extraverts by response
pose that introverts take longer than extra- competition.
verts to retrieve information from perma- 3. Introverts take longer than extraverts to
nent storage, whether that information is se- retrieve information from long-term or
mantic or phonemic in nature. permanent storage, especially non-domi-
A further aspect of retrieval concerns the nant information.
response criterion or cautiousness of re- 4. Introverts typically adopt a more cautious
sponding. We have already seen that intro- response criterion than extraverts.
136 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

5. Extraverts tend to show superior memory The most complete attempt to relate intro-
to introverts at short retention intervals, version-extraversion to Furneaux's three
but the opposite occurs at long retention factors was made by Goh and Farley (1977).
intervals. They investigated performance on the Nuf-
It is by no means clear that any simple ferno tests under stressed conditions (sub-
theoretical integration of these findings is jects told to work as rapidly as possible, with
possible. However, it does seem to be the their solution time per item being recorded)
case that, for whatever reason, introverts ty- or unstressed conditions (subjects instruct-
pically have fewer processing resources ed to work at their own rate). Introver-
available than extraverts. As a consequence, sion-extraversion did not affect accuracy
introverts are less able than extraverts to or persistence, but extraverts performed
cope effectively with increased processing faster than introverts in the unstressed con-
demands (e.g. from distracting stimulation, dition.
from response competition or from complex In sum, extraverts consistently handle in-
retrieval tasks). telligence test items more rapidly than intro-
verts, but there is typically no effect of intro-
version-extraversion on accuracy. However,
somewhat different findings have been ob-
7.1.6 Speed-Accuracy Trade-Off
tained with different paradigms. For exam-
ple, Brebner and Flavel (1978) gave their
States of high arousal seem to be naturally subjects a total of 700 reaction-time trials
associated with fast and relatively inaccu- and included large numbers of catch trials
rate performance. Much of the noise litera- on which the appropriate signal for re-
ture is consistent with that notion, and the sponse was not presented. The performance
same is true of research on incentives. In of extraverts was much less accurate than
contrast, work on introversion-extraversion that of introverts, with extraverts being
seems to point to the opposite conclusion. more likely to respond on catch trials and to
H. J. Eysenck (1967) summarized the find- make anticipatory responses. In vigilance
ings as follows: "The studies ... are remar- and memory tasks, as we have seen, extra-
kably unanimous in showing introverts to be verts tend to be considerably more error
slower and more careful and accurate than prone than introverts in terms of false
extraverts" (p. 162). alarms (Carr 1971, Gillespie and Eysenck
The theoretical ideas of Furneaux (1961) 1980, Harkins and Geen 1975, Krupski et al.
have influenced research in this area. He 1971, Tune 1966).
claimed that mental speed, accuracy (or er- While the findings discussed so far sug-
ror-checking mechanism), and persistence gest that introverts perform more slowly but
were three independent determinants of more accurately than extraverts, there are
performance on tests of intelligence. Jensen numerous exceptions to this generalization.
(1966) obtained a correlation of + .44 be- One important factor is that while extra-
tween extraversion and the speed of solu- verts often respond more rapidly than intro-
tion of the Matrices test of intelligence. Ex- verts at the beginning of an experimental
traverts made more errors than introverts, task, introverts often catch up and even sur-
but not significantly so. Farley (1966) gave pass the performance speed of extraverts.
the Nufferno speed test to introverts and ex- This has been found with a tapping task
traverts; this test involves letter-series prob- (Wilson et al. 1971), with a continuous serial
lems, in each of which a recurring pattern of reaction task (Thackray et al. 1974) and with
letters must be identified and the next letter a prolonged reaction-time task (Brebner
in the series written down. Introversion-ex- and Cooper 1974).
traversion did not affect accuracy, but extra- The findings of Corcoran (1972) make
verts were much faster than introverts. matters still more confusing. He reported
Introversion-Extraversion 137

that introverts responded much more quick- terms of a simple unidimensional construct
ly and made many more errors than extra- of arousal is further supported by the var-
verts on the five-choice serial reaction task. ious interactions that have been obtained
On a letter cancellation task run under rela- between introversion-extraversion and
tively unmotivated conditions, introversion arousing or de-arousing factors (e.g. white
correlated + .90 with speed of performance. noise, time of day, caffeine, time on task,
Finally, Corcoran considered performance sleep deprivation and incentives). In addi-
on a card-sorting task. After normal sleep, tion, the differential effects of introversion-
introverts made many more errors than ex- extraversion on short- and long-term reten-
traverts. These various findings led Corcor- tion are very similar to the effects of other
an to a conclusion that is diametrically op- arousing agents, and suggest the existence
posed to that of H. J. Eysenck (1967): "In- of a common arousal system.
troverts tend to be speedier and less accu- In spite of the obvious success of a simple
rate than extraverts" (p. 277). arousal-based explanation of the effects on
What are we to make of these apparently performance of introversion-extraversion, it
inconsistent data? In general terms, extra- is becoming increasingly clear that rather
verts tend to respond faster than introverts more complex formulations are needed.
under relatively arousing conditions, in- One complicating factor is the accumulating
volving tasks that are intrinsically interest- physiological and behavioural evidence
ing and/or of relatively short duration. In (e.g. Blake 1967b, Eysenck and Folkard
contrast, introverts perform faster than ex- 1980, Revelle et al. 1980) that introverts are
traverts under unstimulating conditions more aroused than extraverts at some times
(e.g. when long and monotonous tasks are of day, but less aroused at others. The notion
used). Perhaps as a result of greater cogni- that introverts are chronically more aroused
tive control of performance, the response than extraverts needs to be re-thought in or-
speed of introverts is relatively unaffected der to incorporate the time-of-day findings.
by the prevailing level of arousal, whereas A second complicating factor stems from
that of extraverts varies directly with the the assumption that introverts are usually
arousal level. more arousable than extraverts (i.e. they
produce a greater increment in physiologi-
cal arousal to a stimulus of standard intensi-
7.1. 7 Conclusions ty). Evidence supporting this assumption
was discussed by H. J. Eysenck (1967).
Several of the effects of introversion-extra- If it is correct that introverts are more af-
version on performance tasks can be ex- fected physiologically than extraverts by
plained straightforwardly by assuming that arousing stimuli, and if the prevailing state
introverts are chronically more aroused of arousal is an important determinant of
than extraverts. That assumption, when behaviour, then the most natural prediction
combined with the notion (Yerkes and Dod- is that the arousing stimuli will affect the
son 1908) that the optimal level of arousal is task performance of introverts to a greater
inversely related to task difficulty, leads to extent than that of extraverts. However, as
the prediction that extraverts should cope our brief review of the literature has made
relatively better than introverts with com- clear, there seems to be a partial dissocia-
plex tasks. This has, indeed, been found in a tion between the effects of arousing stimuli
number of memory studies in which task on physiological arousal and their effects on
complexity was manipulated by varying performance. A variety of arousing factors,
either the degree of response competition or including white noise, caffeine, time of day,
the accessibility of the correct response. the presence of an additional task and
The interpretation of behavioural differ- knowledge of results and other incentives,
ences between introverts and extraverts in all influence the performance of extraverts
138 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

to a greater extent than introverts. Indeed, this book that the effects of arousing agents
introverts often manifest no performance on performance are often mediated by at-
change at all when one of these arousing tentional mechanisms. There are tantalizing
factors is introduced. Furthermore, there is suggestions that this may also be true of the
evidence that de-arousing factors such as behavioural effects of introversion-extraver-
sleep deprivation and prolonged perform- sion. Introverts appear to show greater at-
ance of a task also tend to influence the be- tentional selectivity than extraverts, i.e. a
haviour of extraverts rather more than intro- greater proportion of their total attentional
verts. resources are allocated to the main or pri-
While such empirical complexities clearly mary task. As a result, introverts have less
indicate that some addition to a single attentional capacity available for processing
arousal mechanism needs to be postulated, additional stimuli than extraverts and so ex-
the nature of the appropriate conceptualiza- perience more difficulty in rejecting dis-
tion is unclear. At a speCUlative level, it is tracting stimuli which resemble task stimuli.
tempting to relate these findings to the two They also find more difficulty in performing
arousal-related mechanisms postulated by a secondary task at the same time as the
Broadbent (1971). He argued that high or main task.
low arousal in the lower mechanism would
only impair performance provided that the
controlling upper mechanism was not func-
tioning efficiently. The basic findings can be 7.2 Time of Day
accounted for by assuming that the cogni-
tive control exerted by the upper mechan-
ism is much greater in introverts than in ex- 7.2.1 Basic Findings
traverts. Thus the strongly functioning up-
per mechanism in introverts tends to pre- It has been reasonably well established (see
vent changing levels of arousal from mani- Hockey and Colquhoun 1972, for a review)
festing themselves in performance, whereas that circadian (i.e. approximately 24 h
the weakly functioning upper mechanism in rhythms exist with respect to human per-
extraverts means that their performance is formance on a wide range of tasks. These
fairly directly affected by the prevailing le- circadian rhythms are large enough to be of
vel of arousal in the lower mechanism. practical and theoretical significance, often
A third complicating factor concerns the amounting to a 10% variation in perform-
effect of introversion-extraversion on ance during the course of the waking day. In
speed-accuracy trade-off. Since the typical addition, almost every physiological mea-
effect of high arousal is to produce fast, in- sure in man demonstrates a circadian rhyth-
accurate performance, a simple arousal the- micity. Both performance and physiological
ory must predict that introverts will respond rhythms will be discussed, since there is a
more quickly and inaccurately than extra- natural tendency to assume that the per-
verts. In fact, the more usual finding is that formance variations at different times of
introverts are slower and more accurate day are determined, at least in part, by the
than extraverts, which may reflect intro- physiological changes.
verts' greater susceptibility to punishment. The most extensive body of research on
A fourth complicating factor is that beha- diurnal rhythms in performance was report-
vioural differences between introverts and ed by Blake (1967a, 1971). He asked naval
extraverts cannot be adequately explained ratings to perform a wide range of tasks at
in terms of the level of arousal per se; the various times of day (0800, 1030, 1300, 1530
processes or mechanisms mediating the ef- and 2100). He discovered that performance
fects of arousal on performance must be on several of the tasks (including five-choice
specified. We have seen at various points in serial reaction, auditory vigilance, card sort-
Time of Day 139

ing, letter cancellation and simple arithmet- handicap of being almost completely er-
ic) manifested a fairly consistent improve- roneous. The first difficulty is that it is by no
ment over the day, with the best level of per- means certain that the level of arousal does
formance being achieved at 2100. in fact increase throughout the waking day
Blake also recorded body temperature on up to the evening. In some studies, scores on
a number of occasions during the day. Intri- the general activation scale of Thayer's Acti-
guingly, there was a similar trend in the vation-Deactivation Adjective Check List (a
body temperature and performance data, measure of self-reported arousal) were ob-
with both increasing during the course of tained at different times of day. Thayer
the day. Body temperature reached a peak (1967) found that self-reported activation
between 2000 and 2100 and a nadir between reached a peak at approximately 1230, and
0400 and 0500. There was a rapid rise in Thayer (l978a) and Clements et al. (1976)
temperature between 0800 and 1100, fol- confirmed that self-reported activation was
lowed by a more gradual increase over maximal in the late morning or very early af-
the subsequent several hours and a rapid ternoon.
decline during the late evening and night There are other kinds of data supporting
period. the conclusion that arousal level peaks
There was one major exception to the par- several hours earlier than is suggested by the
allel trends for performance and tempera- body temperature findings. Klein et al.
ture: performance on a test of short-term (1977) reported that adrenalin secretion
memory (digit span) tended to deteriorate reached its peak at approximately midday,
throughout most of the day. Largely on the and Akerstedt (1977) found that the urinary
basis of Blake's findings, Hockey and Cal- excretion of both adrenalin and noradrenal-
quhoun (1972) drew the following conclu- in reached a maximum at about noon, as did
sions from their survey of the literature: subjective ratings of 'alertness'.
"Tasks with a large memory load compo- The discrepancy between the diurnal
nent may perhaps be expected to show an rhythm for temperature and those for the
inverse relation to temperature, while those catecholamines (adrenalin and noradrenal-
demanding a more immediate processing in) and self-reported arousal may simply
(or 'throughput') of information will follow mean that body temperature is an unsatis-
the more typical direct relationship" (p. 15). factory measure of arousal. Alternatively,
How can we account theoretically for there may be two or more arousal systems,
these findings? A straightforward explana- with each of the various physiological mea-
tion can be constructed based on the follow- sures reflecting activity in one of those sys-
ing two assumptions: (1) body temperature tems.
is an indicant of arousal level and (2) the A further problem is that some of the ef-
optimal level of arousal varies inversely as a fects of time of day on performance may not
function of task difficulty (Yerkes & Dod- be mediated by diurnal rhythms in arousal
son, 1908). From these assumptions, it fol- at all. Barton and Cattell (1974) adminis-
lows that simple tasks such as those involv- tered the Seven-State Questionnaire to
ing immediate processing should be better school children either early in the morning
performed as the day progresses. In con- or in the afternoon. Self-reported arousal
trast, the performance of difficult tasks such was lower at the later testing time, but there
as those imposing substantial demands on were also significant changes in anxiety,
memory might be expected to be disrupted stress, regression and depression. Thayer
by the high arousal associated with the end (1978a) found that self-reported energy
of the working day. and vigour were greatest at midday, and
While this explanation has a beguiling there was a similar pattern for self-reported
simplicity which has appealed to a number tension.
of theorists, it suffers from the rather severe All in all, it would be foolhardy to main-
140 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

tain the position that there is a single diurnal ers who were required to work at irregular
rhythm for arousal which reaches a peak in times of day; as a consequence, they did not
the middle of the evening. A more plausible shift their hours of sleeping to a new and
conclusion is that there is a major diurnal consistent period during the 24 h day. These
rhythm for arousal that peaks at around workers maintained the usual rhythm of
midday and that can be measured either by body temperature, and performance corre-
self-reported arousal or by catecholamine sponded to it. There was a rather different
output. In addition, there may be a minor di- pattern when these shift workers were as-
urnal rhythm for arousal that is indexed by signed work which allowed them to sleep
body temperature; however, it is preferable during the same part of the 24 h cycle on
to regard body temperature as an indirect each successive day but at a time that dif-
measure of basic metabolic processes rather fered from the conventional one. Under
than of arousal. these circumstances, their circadian rhythm
As all textbooks of statistics carefully of body temperature shifted to the new cy-
point out, one cannot properly infer a causal cle, and the rhythm of their performance
relationship between two variables on the shifted correspondingly. Such parallelism is
basis of correlational data. Thus the similar obviously suggestive of some kind of causal
trends for performance and body tempera- relationship linking body temperature and
ture obtained by Blake (l967a, 1971) do not performance.
provide definitive evidence that the arousal Evidence that is rather difficult to recon-
(or other) system measured by body temper- cile with the notion that performance and
ature is instrumental in producing the diur- body temperature are causally related was
nal rhythm in performance. Furthermore, obtained by Horne et al. (1980). They used a
close inspection of Blake's data indicates self-assessment questionnaire to classify
that performance and body temperature did their subjects as morning or evening types
not always co-vary: at 1300, there was a and then gave them the task of sitting by a
marked reduction in performance (the so- conveyor belt attempting to reject faulty
called post-lunch dip) that was unaccompan- items. The morning types ('larks') showed a
ied by any reduction in temperature. steeper rise in body temperature during the
In spite of the difficulty of establishing a morning than evening types ('owls') and
causal relationship between the system mea- reached peak body temperature approxi-
sured by body temperature and perform- mately I h earlier than evening types (1900
ance, potentially relevant information can versus 2000). However, as is shown in
be obtained from a study of shift work. Col- Fig. 7.5, there was a much more pronounced
quhoun et al. (1968) investigated shift work- difference between morning and evening

115
t~'O EVENING
i\ / TYPES
~ 110 p \ ..d
Q b-
I-
~
I-
105
!oJ
o
tJ
!oJ
100 MORNING
TYPES
a::
g
u 95

f I I I I i I I I
Fig. 7.5. Detection performance at
various times of day among those
08·00 10·00 12·00 140016·00 18-00 2000 22·00 classified as morning or evening
TIME OF DAY types. (Horne et al. 1980)
Time of Day 141

types in performance accuracy; evening a number of simple processing tasks such as


types showed an essentially continuous im- reaction time and card dealing occurred
provement throughout the day, whereas around noon or the early afternoon. In con-
morning types performed progressively trast, Blake (l967a) found that performance
worse as the day proceeded. on the five-choice serial reaction task was
More specifically, there was a high posi- indicative of a continuous improvement
tive correlation coefficient of + 0.89 be- during the day, with the best performance at
tween body temperature and number of cor- 2100 and the second-best performance at
rect detections for the evening types but a 1530.
high negative correlation of -0.81 for the A search through the literature reveals
morning types. Such findings obviously that peak performance on most tasks is
make it hazardous to assume that tempera- reached around midday and that it is only
ture and performance are causally related. on tasks involving simple visual search
Indeed, it is hard to disagree with the conclu- (such as letter cancellation tasks) that per-
sions arrived at by Horne et a.: "There may formance definitely attains its maximal le-
well be no causal relationship between the vel in the early evening. The general superi-
circadian rhythms of body temperature and ority of midday performance seems intui-
performance ... these rhythms are concom- tively reasonable and may be related to the
itant, being driven by independent oscilla- fact that subjective ratings of alertness tend
tors, often influenced by the same zeitgeb- to be highest at that time.
ers" (p. 35).
Additional doubts about the assumption
that performance levels are determined by 7.2.2 Short- Term and Long-Term
sytems linked to body temperature were Memory
raised by Rutenfranz et al. (1972). They
correlated body temperature with perform- There has been considerable theoretical in-
ance on a multiple-choice reaction-time terest in the effects of time of day on short-
task, holding the two circadian rhythms term (up to approximately 20 minutes after
constant by statistical means. Reaction time learning) and long-term retention. Much of
did not correlate with temperature, either this interest stems from related work indi-
between or within subjects. cating that high arousal has differential ef-
It has often been assumed that perform- fects on short-term and long-term retention
ance on tasks involving immediate process- (for a review, see Eysenck 1976a). In a clas-
ing with little memory load shows an effec- sic experiment, Kleinsmith and Kaplan
tively continuous improvement throughout (1963) compared recall of high-arousal and
the day. This assumption relies heavily on low-arousal items as defined by recordings
the data of Blake (1967a, 1971), who found of skin resistance at the time of presenta-
that peak performance on several perform- tion. Their results, which are shown in
ance tasks was achieved at the last testing Fig. 7.6, demonstrated a marked interaction
time used (i.e. 2100). The assumption of a between item arousal and retention interval:
continuous through-the-day improvement low-arousal items showed considerable for-
also implies that the second-best level of getting over time, whereas high-arousal
performance should have been obtained at items exhibited a strong reminiscence effect.
the penultimate testing time. However, for Although these findings have been repli-
auditory vigilance, two- and eight-category cated several times, their interpretation is
card sorting, letter cancellation and simple equivocal. It is obvious that items differing
arithmetical additions, the second-best level in terms of the physiological responses
of performance was reached at 1030 rather which they produce may also differ in mea-
than 1300 or 1530. In similar fashion, Kleit- ningfulness, familiarity or other important
man (1963) found that peak performance on characteristics. However, when this prob-
142 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

50
Q
,, HIGH-AROUSAL
ITEMS

,,
40 \
\
,,
...J
...J
<{
u 30
w
!r
f-
Z
\
\ ,,
w 20
u
!r
W
[l.

10
Fig. 7.6. Recall of high-
LOW-AROUSAL arousal and low-arousal
ITEMS
items at different re-
2 20 45 1440 10080 tention intervals. (Klein-
RETENTION INTERVAL (I N MINS) smith and Kaplan 1963)

lem is obviated by the expedient of assign- long-term memory measure indicated a


ing items to high- and low-arousal catego- slight, but non-significant, afternoon superi-
ries by pairing some of them at random with ority. However, it should be noted that the
white noise on the presentation trial, the enhancement of long-term memory by
same basic pattern of results continues to be arousal has usually only been found after
obtained (e.g. Berlyne et al. 1965). delays of 20 min or more, which is much
Most researchers have assumed that there longer than the time intervals considered by
is a progressive increase in physiological Baddeley et al.
arousal during most of the working day and Folkard et al. (1977) presented children
have accordingly predicted that short-term with a story at either 0900 or 1500, followed
memory is best relatively early in the day, by recall either immediately or after 1 week.
whereas long-term memory is greatest fol- As would be expected on a simple arousal
lowing late afternoon or early evening pres- model, immediate recall was better in the
entation. There is some support for this pre- morning presentation, whereas delayed re-
diction in Blake's (1967a) finding that short- call was superior following afternoon pres-
term memory as measured by digit span be- entation.
came progressively worse during the greater The long-term recall data indicate that
part of the day. Many years previously, learning was in some sense better in the af-
Winch (1912a, b) had also found a small but ternoon than in the morning. Why then was
consistent advantage of the morning over immediate recall worse with afternoon pres-
the afternoon in immediate memory. entation? Folkard (1980) provided an
A few studies have dealt with the effects answer to this question by re-analyzing
of time of day on both short-term and long- some of the data of Folkard et al. (1977). For
term memory. Baddeley et al. (1970) used immediate recall, morning presentation led
the technique of surreptitiously repeating to equivalent recall of important and trivial
the same sequence of digits on every third story information, whereas afternoon pres-
trial in a memory test involving immediate entation was followed predominantly by re-
recall of sequences of nine random digits. call of the important details. This finding
The superiority of recall on repeated over links up well with the notion (Easterbrook
non-repeated trials was assumed to reflect 1959) that attentional selectivity is greater
long-term memory. There was the typical under high arousal (i.e. in the afternoon).
morning superiority for short-term memory Alternatively, the morning subjects may not
(i.e. the non-repeated lists), whereas the have processed the meaning of the story and
Time of Day 143
thus could not be influenced by the import- semantic processing in the afternoon than in
ance of the information. Since all subjects the morning. Of course, the same explana-
tended to recall mainly the important details tion will not readily account for the oppo-
at the delayed retention test, the implication site pattern of results for short-term reten-
of the results is that the afternoon subjects tion. However, many of the demonstrations
processed less of the passage than morning of a morning superiority in short-term reten-
subjects, but what they did process was tion have involved serial recall of syntacti-
more thoroughly learned. cally unstructured lists of words or digits,
While it is tempting to explain the various which may well depend upon non-semantic
findings by means of a simple arousal mod- rote rehearsal for successful performance.
el, the temptation should probably be resist- Folkard (1979b) tested some of these
ed. In the first place, the assumption that the ideas by comparing serial recall of unrelated
participants in these studies were more words with recall of phonemically similar or
aroused in the afternoon that in the morning semantically similar words. The impairment
depends quite heavily on the circadian of serial recall produced by using phonemi-
rhythm of body temperature. If self-report- cally similar words was greater for those
ed arousal or adrenalin is preferred as an in- tested in the morning than for those tested in
dicant of arousal, then the picture becomes the evening, suggesting that the amount of
much less clear. Secondly, some of the ef- phonemic processing declines during the
fects seem to be rather fragile. Folkard day. The reduction in serial recall produced
(1979a) found that the normal superiority of by using semantically similar words was on-
digit span in the morning to the evening dis- ly significant when subjects were tested in
appeared when the initial instructions in- the evening, indicating increased semantic
cluded suggestions about how to process the processing later in the day. There thus ap-
digit string (e.g. group and rehearsal instruc- pears to be a noticeable shift in processing
tions). strategy as a function of time of day.
Perhaps the strongest evidence against a
simple arousal-based theory of the memory
data was obtained by Folkard and Monk 7.2.3 Interactions with Arousers
(1978). They tested memory for the factual
details of a 10 min film presented at 0400 or One of the more obvious strategies for eluci-
2030 both immediately and after 28 days. dating the factors underlying time-of-day
The subjects were night nurses on either effects is to introduce experimental manipu-
their first or second night of a night shift, lations at different times of day in an at-
and Folkard and Monk discovered that tempt to eliminate or accentuate the effects
short-term memory adjusted more rapidly of time of day on performance. It is a matter
than long-term memory to the shift in the of some surprise that this strategy has been
sleep-wake cycle. If there were only a single used so rarely. However, Blake (1971) did so
circadian rhythm of arousal underlying in a study in which he looked at the effects
time-of-day effects on short-term and long- of knowledge of results on performance of a
term memory, then it is difficult to account letter-cancellation task. As is shown in Fig.
for this differential rate of adjustment. 7.7, knowledge of results increased output
There may be some mileage in relating the by 10.1 % at 0800 but by only 0.2 % at 2100;
key findings to the theoretical position es- of most theoretical importance, knowledge
poused by Craik and Lockhart (1972). They of results completely eleminated the time-
argued that deep or semantic levels of pro- of-day effect. This suggests that poor per-
cessing led to better retention than shallow formance early in the day is attributable to
or non-semantic forms of processing. If one insufficient motivation and effort or to sub-
considers the long-term retention data, the optimal arousal.
implication is that there is deeper or more Arousal-based interpretations of the ef-
144 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

1750

1700
~...
, ' - -..........'0""'.............
,0-----___--'""0
------ OF
KNOWLEDGE
RESULTS

o NO KNOWLEDGE
w OF RESULTS
~ 1650
W
:t
U 1600
III
W
~ 1550

1550 Fig. 7.7. Performance on a letter


cancellation task at various times
I I I I I I I I of day as a function of the presence
0&00 10-00 12-00 14·00 16·00 1600 20{)0 22·00 or absence of knowledge of results.
TIME OF DAY (Blake 1971)

fects oftime of day on performance have re- a positive correlation between introversion
ceived support in other studies in which and efficiency, whereas the afternoon ex-
time of day has been found to interact with periments gave a negative correlation. This
white noise, a known arouser. Mullin and suggests that introverts tend to be morning
Corcoran (1977) discovered that auditory vi- types and extraverts evening types.
gilance was much better at 2030 than at Blake (1967b, 1971) provided an explana-
0830. The introduction of intense white tion for these findings. He discovered that
noise eliminate the time-of-day effect, large- introverts' rapid rise in temperature oc-
ly by improving performance at 0830. Blake curred a little earlier in the day than that of
(1971) compared performance of a letter extraverts. This finding that introverts were
cancellation task at 0800 and 1030 under phase advanced with respect to the circadi-
conditions of 70-db or 100-db noise. The an rhythm of arousal suggests that extra-
more intense noise produced a 6 % increase verts may be sub-optimally aroused in the
in output at 0800, but had no effect at 1030. morning, with introverts being supra-opti-
Of especial significance in the light of other mally aroused in the afternoon. However, it
findings to be discussed shortly, the benefi- is somewhat implausible to claim that rather
cial effects of intense noise on early morn- small differences in body temperature be-
ing performance were much greater for ex- tween introverts and extraverts are responsi-
traverts than for introverts. ble (even indirectly) for large performance
So far we have concentrated very largely differences.
on general trends in time-of-day effects. Revelle et al. (1980) argued that extraver-
However, individual differences are import- sion should be divided into its two main
ant, and there appears to be a valid distinc- components (i.e. impulsivity and sociabili-
tion between people who prefer to work in ty). They discovered in several experiments
the morning ('morning types') and people that interactions between introversion-ex-
who work better in the evening ('evening traversion and time of day were due to im-
types'). The personality dimension that has pUlsivity rather than to sociability. Fairly
most frequently been explored in this con- demanding tasks (e.g. analogies, antonyms
text is that of introversion-extraversion. In- and sentence completions) were performed
terest in the relationship of this personality either in the morning (0900 or 1000) or in the
dimension to time-of-day effects was evening (1900) and after ingestion of caf-
sparked off by Colquhoun (1960). He ana- feine (an arousing drug) or a placebo.
lysed the results from 17 vigilance studies The averaged results across a total of se-
carried out at various times of day and dis- ven experiments are shown in Fig. 7.8. Caf-
covered that the morning experiments gave feine had its greatest beneficial effect on
Time of Day 145

MORNING EVENING
+3
lLI~ ,./J
u lll
zlLl
<0::
+2
,.,."
~O
o::~
+1
,.,." ___o..~~~
______ _
0 ~--------
fro
0::0::
......
-1 "",
lLI<
0.0
Zz -2
<:!
0 111 -3 - PLACEBO
lLIZ
~:: -4 0---0 CAFFEINE

LOW HIGH LOW HIGH


IMPULSIVITY IMPULSIVITY

Fig.7.8. Performance on relatively demanding and impulsivity. (Revelle, Humphreys et al. 1980)
verbal tasks as a function of time of day, caffeine

high impulsives (i.e. extraverts) tested in the nounced as Revelle et al. claimed it should
morning, indicating that extraverts are sub- have been.
optimally aroused at that time of day. In Revelle et al. (1980) argued that introver-
contrast, the modest effect of caffeine on sion-extraversion, time of day, and caffeine
low impulsives (i.e. introverts) tested in the all affected the same arousal system, but this
morning suggests that they are close to the is rather simplistic. For example, it may be
optimal arousal level at that time. The oppo- misleading to claim that introversion-extra-
site pattern of results was obtained at the version and time of day affect the same un-
evening testing session, but the interaction derlying process. The ability to retrieve in-
between drug conditions and impulsivity is formation from long-term or semantic mem-
weak. These data suggest that high impul- ory is affected by introversion-extraversion
sives are over-aroused in the evening, (Eysenck 1974a, 1975a) but is not affected
whereas low impulsives are under-aroused. by time of day (Folkard and Monk 1980).
Revelle et al. (1980) argued that low im- Blake (1971) found that digit span declined
pulsives were more aroused than high im- throughout most of the day (i.e. was appar-
pulsives in the morning but that the oppo- ently inversely related to arousal level). He
site was true in the evening, and they also found that introverts had a greater digit
claimed that this was due to a phase differ- span than extraverts at 0800, at which time
ence of several hours between introverts and they appear to be more aroused than extra-
extraverts in the circadian rhythm of arous- verts. These performance data indicate the
al. Eysenck and Folkard (1980) pointed out need for a more complex conceptualization
that there was no evidence in the literature involving a number of qualitatively distinct
to support the notion of such a large phase activation states.
difference in the circadian rhythm of arous- Humphreys et al. (1980) agreed with Ey-
al. However, they reported some new body senck and Folkard (1980) that a more com-
temperature data suggesting that as Revelle plex formulation was required and pro-
et al. claimed, the phase difference between posed a model incorporating two different
introverts and extraverts was due more to activation states (on-task effort and arous-
the impulsivity component of extraversion al), together with two information-process-
than to the sociability component. However, ing constructs (attention and short-term
the phase difference which Eysenck and memory). Their model is shown in Fig. 7.9.
Folkard obtained was by no means as pro- It assumes that arousal and on-task effort
146 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

ANXIETY ~------------. ATTENTION


L..-_ _----'

PERFORMANCE

IMPULSIVITY

Fig. 7.9. An arousal model postulating two activa- lines imply monotonic decreasing functions.
tion states (on-task effort and arousal). Solid lines (Humphreys et al. 1980)
imply monotonic increasing relations and dashed

both have a positive effect on attention, al place the time of peak arousal anywhere
whereas arousal has a negative effect on between approximately 1100 and 2100
short-term memory. Humphreys et al. also hours. It is probably best to assume that
assumed that performance was a positive there is more than one diurnal rhythm for
function of both short-term memory and at- arousal.
tention. According to this formulation, sub- It is interesting that many tasks are best
optimal arousal impairs performance be- performed at approximately midday, and
cause insufficient attention is paid to the this is the time at which self-reported alert-
task, whereas supra-optimal arousal leads ness and arousal are usually greatest. Re-
to a performance decrement via reduced cent data suggest that the earlier view that
short-term memory capacity. peak performance of most tasks occurs in
It is unfortunate that Humphreys et al. the middle of the evening (always rather
failed to discuss the concepts of short-term counter-intuitive) is not correct. A compli-
memory and attention at greater length. In cating factor concerns the differential ef-
normal usage, they would be regarded as in- fects of time of day on short-term and long-
terdependent constructs rather than as be- term memory: short-term memory is best in
ing totally separate. In any case, the model the morning and long-term memory in the
could only be tested by looking at the effects late afternoon. This appears to be due to a
of arousal and anxiety on tasks varying shift away from maintenance processing
systematically in their attentional and short- and towards semantic processing as the day
term memory demands, and this has not as proceeds.
yet been done.

7.2.4 Conclusions 7.3 Sleep Deprivation

It seems clear that there are systematic


changes in arousal level during the course of There are various kinds of sleep depriva-
the waking day. This has been demonstrated tion. While the most common procedure has
by the fact that several arousers such as been to deprive subjects of all sleep on one
knowledge of results, white noise, introver- or more successive nights, partial sleep de-
sion-extraversion (or its impulsivity compo- privation probably has more social rele-
nent) and caffeine all have different effects vance. When people are only deprived of
on performance at different points during some of their normal sleep, it is important to
the day. However, more precise statements ascertain whether it is predominantly REM
are difficult to make because various phy- or non-REM sleep that has been lost. The
siological and self-report measures of arous- former is characterized by rapid eye move-
Sleep Deprivation 147
ments, with the EEG manifesting a pattern stimulation this deprivation leads to in-
of fast asynchronous low voltage changes. creased activation" (p. 17). Unfortunately
In contrast, non-REM sleep typically pro- the precise meaning of 'sufficient stimula-
duces an EEG showing rather more pro- tion' was not made clear by Malmo and Sur-
nounced changes from the waking pattern. willo, but they did report data apparently
However, the best known difference is that supporting their hypothesis. They deprived
subjects awakened from REM sleep are three subjects of sleep for approximately
considerably more likely than those awak- 55 h and took physiological recordings from
ened from non-REM sleep to report that them during the performance of a tracking
they have just been dreaming. task. Strong environmental stimulation was
It is already clear that the behavioural provided by exposing the subjects to painful
consequences of REM and non-REM sleep heat when their performance fell below a
loss are rather different. For example, it ap- certain level. While there were some incon-
pears that REM sleep is more important sistencies in the data, Malmo and Surwillo
than non-REM sleep in stimulating flexible (1960) concluded that "sleep deprivation
divergent thinking (e.g. Glaubman 1978). had the effect of increasing the level of acti-
Even though the precise functional signifi- vation" (p. 22).
cance of the major kinds of sleep remains As a rule of thumb, it appears to be the
unclear, it is likely that the behavioural con- case that sleep deprivation leads to reduced
sequences of total sleep loss represent an arousal when the environment is relatively
amalgam of relatively specific effects. unstimulating or monotonous, whereas
Many theorists have ignored the above more inconsistent effects are observed when
caveat and assumed with some justification the situation is demanding or stressful. This
that the major effect of sleep loss is to pro- means that we cannot accurately predict
duce a general lowering of arousal. Much of someone's arousal level solely on the basis
the relevant experimental evidence has been of knowing that he has been deprived of
reviewed by Kjellberg (1977a). While the ef- sleep. Arousal level is determined interac-
fects of sleep deprivation on measures of au- tively by sleep deprivation and environmen-
tonomic activation are rather inconsistent, tal stimulation.
sleep loss generally produces decreased al- There is reasonable evidence that sleep
pha abundance and/or a lowering of alpha deprivation potentiates the de-arousing in-
amplitude. Both of these effects on the EEG fluence of monotonous environments, but it
are conventionally regarded as indicative of is less clear what mechanism mediates this
de-arousal. effect. A promising lead was provided by
Subjective measures also indicate that a Bohlin (1971). She presented a series of
major effect of sleep deprivation is to lower identical tones and found that habituation
the level of arousal. The most common find- of the orienting response occurred more
ing is both earth shattering and mind bog- rapidly when the tones were presented in
gling: people who are deprived of sleep re- fairly quick succession. Since sleep onset
port increased sleepiness! However, irrita- was also faster with more frequent tone
tion is reported almost as frequently as slee- presentations, it seems possible that sleep
piness, and anxiety reactions are also expe- loss increases the rate of habituation. How-
rienced some of the time. ever, Bohlin (1973) discovered that sleep-
In spite of the accumulating evidence that deprived subjects did not show faster habi-
sleep loss lowers the level of arousal, Mal- tuation than non-deprived subjects, al-
mo and Surwillo (1960) proposed an alter- though they did fall asleep faster when ex-
native formulation. According to them, posed to the habituation procedure. In spite
"Depriving an organism of sleep appears to of these disappointing results, it may still be
be like depriving it of other basic needs. Un- worth exploring the notion that sleep loss af-
der conditions of sufficient environmental fects some aspect of habituation to stimuli.
148 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

7.3.1 Performance Effects a substantial deterioration on performance


of a 40-min radar-watching task. The ad-
It has been found, with very few exceptions, ministration of painful electric shocks at
that any effects of sleep loss on a task are in irregular intervals improved the detection
the direction of impaired performance. Al- performance of sleep-deprived subjects, but
though there has been some disagreement most of the improvement occurred during
on the details, it has been accepted almost the first 15 min of the task. An arousal-
universally that such performance decre- based interpretation is supported by their
ments are attributable to a sub-optimal level finding that sleep loss reduced heart rate,
of arousal in the sleep-deprived state. The whereas the stress produced by the shocks
most direct method of testing this hypothe- increased it.
sis is to observe the effects on performance Collins (1977) investigated the effects of
of increasing the arousal level of sleep-de- administration of d-amphetamine on per-
prived subjects in some way; the prediction formance of a visual tracking task. He found
is that adverse effects of sleep deprivation that this arousing drug produced a substan-
per se on performance should be attenuated tial improvement in tracking accuracy
by arousal manipulations. among subjects who had been deprived of
Most of the available evidence supports sleep for 55 h; however, their performance
the above prediction. In some early work, was still lower than that of subjects who had
Wilkinson (1961) found that sleep depriva- slept normally.
tion produced widespread impairment on In sum, it has been clearly established
the five-choice serial reaction task, includ- that each of a large variety of arousing
ing slower overall responding, more gaps agents (including electric shocks, noise,
and more errors. Incentive in the form of knowledge of results and d-amphetamine) is
knowledge of results largely eliminated able to counteract the adverse effects of
these effects. In similar vein, Wilkinson sleep loss on performance. The notion that
(1963) found that the adverse effects of sleep loss leads to a level of arousal below
sleep loss on the five-choice serial reaction that which is optimal for the performance of
task were much reduced by the administra- most tasks can also be tested by considering
tion of white noise. task performance in more detail. For exam-
Corcoran (1962a) found that noise had a ple, it has often been found that arousal lev-
modestly beneficial effect on the visual vig- el declines over time when subjects have to
ilance performance of sleep-deprived sub- perform relatively monotonous tasks for a
jects, and the same was true for perform- prolonged period. If the arousal level of
ance of the five-choice serial reaction task. sleep-deprived subjects is sub-optimal even
The findings led Corcoran to the following at the start of task performance, then this de-
conclusion: "The effect of noise in these ex- arousal over time might be expected to lead
periments was clearly to reduce the decline to progressively poorer performance under
during the test which is normally associated sleep deprivation as the time on-task in-
with loss of sleep. This result is in accord- creases.
ance with a simple arousal theory, which By and large, it does seem that tasks re-
would maintain that noise is arousing and vealing effects of sleep deprivation on per-
loss of sleep de-arousing, so that their com- formance do so to a greater extent after the
bination results in levels of arousal between task has been performed for some time. Wil-
these extremes" (p. 181). kinson (1960) found that sleep loss impair-
In more recent work, the effects of other ed performance on a 40-min visual vigi-
arousers on the performance of sleep-de- lance task, with most of the impairment ocur-
prived subjects have been assessed. ring during the last 20 min (see Fig. 7.1 0). Sim-
Bergstrom, Gillberg and Amberg (1973) ilar results have also been obtained with
found that two nights of sleep loss produced the five-choice serial reaction task (Wilkin-
Sleep Deprivation 149
100
o
w
~
U ............. _ SLEEP
~ 75 o..............
~
~
w ......
o "'-0....
"'...
"'...' ...
I/)
...J
~ 50
(!)
'0.,
I/)
~
' .........
z NO SLEEP
w 25 ......"'0
U
Q:
W
Il.

Fig.. 7.10. Visual vigilance perform-


ance after sleep or no sleep. (Wil- 2 3 4
kinson 1960) 10-MIN PERIODS OF TEST

son 1961). There were 6 weeks of testing, they had liked best and found that vigilance
and the adverse effects of sleep were greater was the least popular, followed in order by
towards the end of each session, especially serial choice reaction, rote learning, card
in the later weeks. Most striking of all are sorting and then the games. A comparison
the data obtained by Wilkinson (1964) with of the performance and preference data led
a 30-min visual vigilance task. On the 3rd Wilkinson to the following conclusion:
day without sleep, the sleep-deprived sub- "The least attractive types of work are those
jects performed at approximately 60 % of most likely to suffer a loss of efficiency
the level of non-deprived subjects during when sleep has been lost" (p. 180).
the first half of the task; subsequently, their It is probable, of course, that the various
performance declined catastrophically to tasks differed in important ways other than
only 5 % of that of the control subjects. merely attractiveness. In general, one might
Another way of testing the notion that expect attractive tasks to provide more vari-
sleep loss characteristically produces de- ety, novelty and unpredictability and to be
arousal is to manipulate the intrinsic inter- more complex. However, there is some indi-
est or stimulation provided by the experi- cation in the literature that complexity per
mental task: the expectation is that the more se increases rather than decreases the ad-
interesting and stimulating a task is, the verse effects of sleeplessness. For example,
more resistant it will be to the effects of Williams and Lubin (1967) considered per-
sleep loss. The most thorough investigation formance on two experimenter-paced tasks;
of this issue was by Wilkinson (1964), who the less complex task simply required ad-
compared the effects of sleep loss on eight ding two pairs of digits, whereas the more
different tasks. Sleep loss severely disrupted complex task also involved adding eight to
performance on the choice serial reaction each sum. The more complex task was more
task and visual vigilance, reduced the speed detrimentally affected by sleep loss. Similar
of sorting cards into both four and ten cate- results with a different task were obtained
gories, reduced the ability to play chess, but by Williams et al. (1965). They discovered
did not have any effect on rote serial learn- that sleep loss produced a larger increase in
ing, table tennis or darts. errors of omission on a visual vigilance task
Any interpretation of this pattern of re- when signals were unpredictable (high com-
sults is greatly complicated by the heteroge- plexity) than when they were totally predict-
neous nature of the tasks used. However, able (low complexity).
Wilkinson asked the subjects which tasks At least some of the above studies have
150 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

obtained an interaction between sleep loss produce ordered recall of the most recently
and task difficulty or complexity in which presented items. The running memory scan
performance on a difficult task is more im- task seems to require a rather unusual strate-
paired by sleep loss than is performance on gy. As one of the subjects put it, "To learn to
an easy task. This appears to be inconsistent do this test one must learn not to listen"
with the assumption of Yerkes and Dodson (p. 109). In other words, behaving passively
(1908) that the optimal level of arousal is in- may be more successful than active process-
versely related to task difficulty. Beyond ing of each digit when performing this task.
that, it is clear that the crucial task charac- Very different results were obtained by
teristics mediating the effects of sleep loss Gieseking et al. (1957). They found that im-
are still only partially understood. However, mediate recall of general information items
an interesting speculation was offered by was impaired by sleeplessness, and the same
Kjellberg (1977b): "The influence of sleep was true of delayed recall after 24 h. How-
deprivation appears to be accentuated as a ever, relative to immediate recall, long-term
function of how optimal the conditions are recall showed no significant sleep-loss de-
for habituation to occur" (p. 146). crement up to 50 h of wakefulness. Similar
A further aspect of the effects of sleepless- results were obtained by Williams et al.
ness on performance deserves mention at (1959).
this point. When subjects are at liberty to Williams et al. (1966) wondered whether
perform a task at their chosen rate, there are their previous findings might have been due
very characteristic effects of sleep loss a- to a failure on the part of sleep-deprived
cross a wide variety of tasks: the sleep- subjects to perceive some of the to-be-re-
deprived subject performs relatively slowly membered words. Accordingly, they present-
but maintains a high level of accuracy. Pos- ed word lists in which the subjects were re-
sible explanations for the differential effects quired to write down each word immediate-
of sleeplessness on speed and accuracy will ly after it had been presented. In spite of
be discussed later. this, one night of sleep loss was still associat-
ed with reduced immediate recall. In a sec-
ond experiment, Williams et al. found that
7.3.2 Short-Term and Long-Term long-term retention of pictorial information
Memory was impaired when pictures were presented
to sleep-deprived subjects and the sub-
A well-established finding in the arousal lit- sequent recognition test was given after a re-
erature (see Eysenck, 1976a, for a review) is covery sleep. Since there was no impairment
that high arousal has differential effects on when presentation was given in the non-de-
short-term and long-term memory: it im- prived state but the retention test was given
pairs or has no effect on short-term memory in the sleep-deprived state, sleep loss may
but enhances long-term memory. If we may affect initial encoding rather than subse-
assume that sleep loss typically reduces the quent retrieval. .
level of arousal, then the presumption must More evidence that sleep loss can reduce
be that sleep loss will improve short-term short-term memory was collected by Polzel-
memory but impair long-term memory. la (1975). Up to 13 items were presented, fol-
The prediction that sleep loss should en- lowed by a probe. Subjects had to decide
hance short-term memory is of interest part- whether or not the probe item corresponded
ly because it is counter-intuitive. Hamilton to any of the items presented initially. Sleep
et al. (1972) found that partial sleep loss did loss significantly impaired performance on
indeed improve performance on running this recognition test; since there were com-
digit span. This task involves the presenta- parable effects of sleep loss at all retention
tion of an unpredictable number of digits; intervals, the performance impairment un-
when the series ends, the subject attempts to der sleep loss probably reflects inefficient
Sleep Deprivation 151

encoding (perhaps due to attentional nals. However, if the stimulus was long and
lapses) rather than an increased speed of complex, then a lapse might coincide with
forgetting or decay of trace information. only part of the stimulus, leading to an inac-
In sum, sleep loss disrupts both short- curate rather than an omitted response.
term and long-term memory. While a com- Williams et al. (1959) began their search
parison of the findings obtained with other for evidence of lapses by considering per-
arousal manipulations indicates that sleep formance on a reaction-time task. After 78 h
loss should reduce long-term retention, the of sleep deprivation, mean reaction time
fact that sleep deprivation impairs short- was twice as long as under non-deprived
term memory is somewhat inconsistent with conditions. Of more interest, this deteriora-
the expectations of a simple arousal model. tion of performance was largely attributable
The most straightforward explanation of the to the fact that the slowest 14% of responses
data is that sleeplessness produces greater averaged approximately 2.50 s after three
difficulty in focussing attention on the to- nights' sleep loss compared to only 0.50 s
be-remembered stimuli; such attentional under control conditions. In contrast, the
problems would presumably impair both fastest 14% of responses were only slightly
short-term and long-term memory. affected by sleep loss. It was assumed that
the incidence of lapses increased as sleep
loss continued and that such lapses led to
7.3.3 The Lapse Hypothesis unusually slow response latencies.
A potential problem with this analysis is
The first thorough theoretical account of the that it is likely that response speed was fas-
effects of sleep loss was put forward by Wil- ter at the beginning of each session, so that
liams et al. (1959). In essence, they claimed there is some confounding between reaction
that sleep deprivation leads to an increased time and time on-task. Lisper and Kjellberg
frequency of short periods of unresponsive- (1972) overcame this problem by calculating
ness or reduced sensitivity to stimulation; the reaction times falling at the 25th and
such periods they referred to as 'lapses'. 75th percentiles in each period of 1 min dur-
Lapses were regarded as instances of low- ing a 10-min task. Sleep deprivation primar-
ered arousal level, and it was predicted that ily lengthened the slowest reactions but also
the performance of any task would be dis- had a discernible effect on the fastest re-
rupted during a lapse. They appear to have sponses. Similar results were obtained by
been influenced by the work of Bjerner Tharp (1978); he also found that sleep de-
(1949), who defined a lapse as "a transient privation slowed the fastest responses more
condition of the same nature as sleep" when there was stimUlus-response incompa-
(p.73). tibility, suggesting that sleep loss increases the
The most fundamental prediction of the duration of the response selection process.
lapse hypothesis is that sleeplessness leads With respect to a further prediction of the
to more uneven performance rather than to lapse hypothesis that sleeplessness leads to
a general deterioration. In order to make slow but accurate performance on subject-
more specific predictions, they distin- paced tasks, there is no doubt at all that
guished between subject-paced and experi- most of the available data provide strong
menter-paced tasks. In subject-paced tasks, support (for a review, see Kjellberg
it was assumed that lapses would lead the 1977a, b, c). However, this differential ef-
sleep-deprived subject to proceed more fect of sleep loss on speed and accuracy can
slowly but without sacrificing accuracy. In equally well be explained in other ways. For
contrast, on experimenter-paced tasks they example, if sleeplessness produces a general
anticipated that lapses would lead to errors reduction in processing efficiency, then sub-
of omission, i.e. the subject would simply jects may simply adopt the strategy of sacri-
fail to respond to some of the critical sig- ficing speed in order to maintain accuracy.
152 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

There is also experimental support for the phasis in research designed to evaluate
prediction that sleeplessness produces er- Easterbrook's hypothesis has been on the
rors of omission on experimenter-paced effects of high arousal on attentional pro-
tasks. Williams et al. (1959) performed a cesses, the hypothesis implies that lowered
series of vigilance experiments using visual, arousal should lead to decreased attentional
auditory or tactile signals and discovered selectivity.
that omission errors were more affected Hockey (1970c) investigated the effects of
than commission errors by sleep loss irre- sleep loss on attention in a situation in
spective of signal modality. In subsequent which subjects had to perform a tracking
work, they recorded the EEG during per- task while at the same time responding to
formance of an auditory vigilance task. Of light signals. To begin with, both sleep-de-
especial interest, errors of omission consist- prived and non-deprived subjects re-
ently exhibited less alpha activity than cor- sponded much more rapidly to centrally lo-
rect omissions, correct responses or com- cated than to peripherally located light sig-
mission errors during sleep loss. This find- nals. Later on in the task, sleep-deprived
ing provides support for the notion that the subjects showed a decreasing difference in
increased omission errors in the sleepless response times to signals from the two
state reflect a greater frequency of sleep-like sources, whereas non-deprived subjects
lapses. maintained the initial difference. This pro-
It is probably true, as the lapse hypothesis vides at least indirect support for the notion
assumes, that part ofthe performance decre- that sleep deprivation reduces attentional
ment in the sleepless state is due to transient selectivity. However, it is not clear from this
periods of unusually low arousal. However, study whether sleep loss affects attentional
there is also some more pervasive impair- selectivity or response selection.
ment which occurs with sleep deprivation, Less equivocal data were obtained by
and this may involve the generally sub-opti- Hockey (1973). There were three light
mal arousal characteristic of sleep loss. At sources and three buttons, and pressing one
the theoretical level, there is a danger of de- of the buttons enabled the state of the corre-
fining a 'lapse' solely in terms of perform- sponding light source to be checked. The
ance; in other words, using it as a purely de- subject's task was to attempt to detect the
scriptive term and ignoring its nature and occurrence of 'faults' at any of the light
causes. More specifically, since lapses are sources; faults were distributed over the
used to explain performance decrements, three sources in the ratio 6:3: I. Sleep loss re-
and lapses are in effect defined as perform- sulted in a reduction of sampling of the
ance decrements, we have a rather nasty source associated with the highest fault
case of circularity. What is needed is some probability, especially towards the end of
independent measure of the occurrence of a the experimental session. In other words,
lapse, and psychophysiological recordings sleep-deprived subjects appeared to show
may help to provide such a measure. Fi- less evidence of attentional selectivity. An
nally, it seems important to decide whether interpretation of these data in terms of low-
lapses represent an attentional deficit or a ered arousal was strengthened by a further
lack of responsiveness. finding reported by Hockey (1973): noise
had the opposite effect to sleep loss, lead-
ing to increased sampling of the high prob-
7.3.4 Attentional Mechanisms ability source, i.e. greater attentional selec-
tivity.
We have seen in previous chapters of this Norton (1970) used a task in which packs
book that as predicted by Easterbrook of cards had to be sorted according to a sim-
(1959), high arousal typically leads to great- ple principle. The packs used in the experi-
er attentional selectivity. Although the em- ment varied in the number of irrelevant sti-
Sleep Deprivation 153
muli per card, which ranged between zero sideration of those tasks that are especially
and eight. The magnitude of the adverse ef- susceptible to the effects of sleeplessness.
fect of sleep loss on sorting speed increased Such tasks (e.g. vigilance and reaction time)
with the number of irrelevant cues on the tend to be relatively monotonous and unin-
cards, presumably because sleeplessness teresting, i.e. they are not intrinsically moti-
leads to greater distractibility. The findings vating. The longer the period of time over
can also be construed as indicating reduced which such tasks need to be performed, the
attentional selectivity following sleep depri- greater is the difficulty of maintaining an
vation. adequate level of motivation. In this con-
A major effect of sleep loss is to reduce an nection, it is worth remembering that sleep
individual's capacity to maintain attention- loss often interacts with time on-task, with
al focus on the critical aspects of stimula- the adverse effects of sleeplessness increas-
tion. This in turn increases the distracting ef- ing over time.
fect of irrelevant stimuli. It is tempting to In contrast, sleep loss has been found to
apply this theoretical account to several of have negligible effects on the performance
the findings hitherto subsumed under the of many complex but intrinsically interest-
lapse hypothesis. In particular, it may be the ing tasks. For example, performance ofvar-
case that the transient periods of non-re- ious intelligence tests is essentially unaffect-
sponse often regarded as attributable to ed by sleep deprivation. Wilkinson (1964)
lapses (i.e. non-responsiveness of the sys- found that performance of a rather compli-
tem) may actually be due to lack of focussed cated and challenging task called the Battle
attention. Game was, if anything, improved by a 30-h
loss of sleep; this was true throughout the
l-h period of work. Even at 0500 on the 3rd
7.3.5 Motivation day of sleeplessness, performance was com-
parable to that obtained under non-de-
It has frequently (and plausibly) been as- prived conditions.
sumed that one of the main problems expe- Part of the reason for the lack of impair-
rienced by the sleep-deprived person is mo- ment on the Battle Game may have been
tivational in nature. This point of view was that there was almost immediate knowledge
favoured by Wilkinson (1969), who claimed of results: successful and abortive attempts
that it is "difficult for us to assess the 'real' at interception were signalled as hits or mis-
effect oflost sleep upon subjects' capacity as ses by means of flashing lights. When Wil-
opposed to their willingness to perform" kinson (1964) used a similar task (Coded
(p.39). The finding (e.g. Wilkinson 1961) Decision Taking) but in a highly coded form
that motivational factors such as knowledge with delayed knowledge of success and fail-
of results can largely eliminate the adverse ure, sleep loss caused subjects to neglect
effects of sleep loss is obviously consistent their primary instructions to make intercep-
with a motivational theory of sleep loss. tions at all costs.
However, as we saw earlier in the chapter, Why exactly is knowledge of results so
several other arousing manipulations (e.g. successful in reducing the negative effects
noise, electric shocks and d-amphetamine) of sleeplessness on performance? While in
have also been found to attentuate the detri- general terms it seems probable that knowl-
mental effects of sleeplessness. According- edge of results increases the motivational
ly, it is not clear that the beneficial effects of level of sleep-deprived subjects, Kjellberg
motivational variables on sleep-deprived (1975) put forward a more specific reason.
subjects should be interpreted in motiva- He suggested that knowledge of results
tional rather than arousal terms. helps to prevent sleep-deprived subjects
A motivational interpretation of sleep- from lowering their standard of perform-
loss phenomena is also suggested by a con- ance, because it makes failure more obvious
154 Endogenous Determinants of Arousal

and less acceptable. Subjects were given The above findings suggest that sleep de-
number series of the form '24681', with privation may affect the level of arousal.
some of the problems being insoluble. Fail- Physiological evidence reveals that sleep de-
ures to solve were made less obvious and privation is usually associated with re-
more acceptable for some of the subjects by duced arousal level, but the precise findings
informing them that some of the problems are somewhat situation specific. At the be-
were insoluble. Among these informed sub- havioural level, the notion that sleep loss
jects, sleep deprivation led to less time being tends to produce sub-optimal arousal is
spent on the task and more items being left strongly supported by the fact that a galli-
unsolved. There were no effects of sleep de- maufry of arousers has been found to elimi-
privation for those subjects who were not in- nate, or greatly reduce, the negative effects
formed that some of the problems had no of sleep deprivation.
solution. Thus the significance attaching to While it is true that a simple arousal ex-
knowledge of results clearly depends upon planation ofsleep-loss phenomena is surpris-
the context in which the feedback is pro- ingly succesful in predicting the behaviour-
vided. al consequences of sleeplessness, there are
A rather different approach was taken by still grounds for claiming that sleep loss af-
Wilkinson (1962). He discovered that sleep fects two separate arousal systems. The in-
loss reduced the speed of performance on a itial effect of sleep loss is to reduce the activ-
test of mental arithmetic. However, concur- ity in a relatively non-specific, passive
rent recording of the electromyograph arousal system; the 'natural' result of this
(EMG) revealed an interesting qualifica- de-arousal is to impair performance on most
tion: sleep loss primarily led to performance tasks. However, sleep loss can also indirect-
impairment for those subjects whose EMG ly affect a second, more active arousal sys-
measure of muscle tension showed only a tem, especially when motivational factors
small increase over the normal level. Wilkin- (e.g. knowledge of results or shock for poor
son suggested that the high level of perform- performance) are introduced into the exper-
ance attained by those subjects whose mus- imental situation. In other words, if sleep-
cle tension increased the most was attribut- deprived subjects are induced to monitor
able to compensatory effort or motivation. their task performance fairly closely, then
Muscle tension was seen as reflecting effort the naturally detrimental effects of sleep
rather than arousal per se. loss can often be reduced by an active com-
pensatory process. An important implication
of this viewpoint is that when sleep-de-
7.3.6 Summary prived subjects perform a task as well as
non-deprived subjects, they are achieving
It has been found, with relatively few excep- this performance comparability at greater
tions, that sleep loss either impairs perform- 'cost' in terms of the involvement of process-
ance or has no effect. Performance decre- ing resources. This was shown by Wilkinson
ments are especially likely to occur under (1962), who found that sleep-deprived sub-
the following circumstances: (I) prolonged jects who managed to perform a mental
sleep loss, (2) use of a monotonous and un- arithmetic task successfully did so at the ex-
stimulating task, (3) long task duration and pense of very high levels of muscle tension.
(4) minimal feedback. In contrast, sleep loss The de-aroused sleepless state produces a
often has no effect on performance when mode of behaviour in which responding is
short, interesting tasks are used and/or fair- rather slow and deliberate, but in which ac-
I ly complete knowledge of results is avail- curacy is maintained at a high level. There
able. In general terms, the more the task is are grounds for believing that there is a posi-
conducive to de-arousal, the more sleepless- tive relationship between arousal level and
ness is detrimental for performance. the internal speed of processing, although
Sleep Deprivation 155
this relationship can undoubtedly be strated: (1) reduced rate of mental process-
severed by varying the instructional empha- ing, (2) decreased capacity to focus atten-
sis on speed or accuracy. When the experi- tion on crucial stimuli, (3) increased distrac-
mental task is experimenter paced, and the tibility and (4) momentary lapses of atten-
subject is thus not free to perform at his tion. However, perhaps the most important
chosen rate, then the characteristic effect of and surprising finding of all is the large
sleeplessness is to increase the number of number of tasks and conditions under
failures to respond, i.e. errors of omission. which sleep deprivation has no discernible
The precise effects of sleep loss on the effect on the quality of performance. This
processing system have not as yet been esta- suggests that any adequate theory of sleep
blished. However, it is reasonable to claim deprivation must incorporate some kind of
that the following effects have been demon- compensatory mechanism.
8 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise

A relatively straightforward method of ed. In terms of this analysis, unpredictable


studying the effects of arousal on perform- bursts of noise are more 'surprising' than
ance is to manipulate the level of arousal by predictable bursts, and thus attract more at-
means of exogenous or external stimulation. tention.
Ideally, such stimulation should be intense An additional factor of some importance
but non-informative, so that it produces a is whether or not people feel that they have
state of elevated arousal without at the same control over the duration of the noise.
time attracting attention. In practice, white Blechman and Dannemiller (1976) exposed
noise (i.e. a noise consisting of all sound fre- two groups of subjects to the same pattern
quencies and resembling hissing) has been of noise while proofreading and solving an-
far and away the most used exogenous de- agrams, but they led one group to believe
terminant of arousal. There is fairly general that rapid and continuous button pressing
agreement that intense white noise produces would turn off the noise. The group with
heightened physiological arousal, and di- perceived control over noise did better than
rect evidence in support of this contention the group with no perceived control on the
was obtained by Berlyne and Lewis (1963), relatively simple proofreading task, but not
who discovered that moderate intensity on the anagram task.
white noise produced a significant drop in In this chapter, we initially consider the
skin resistance. effects of noise on a number of standard
It is worth noting at the outset that it performance tasks. Thereafter, there is more
would be erroneous to assume that all kinds emphasis on the theoretical interpretation
of noise have equivalent effects on behav- of such effects, with special stress being laid
iour. Of especial importance is the distinc- on attentional mechanisms.
tion between predictable and unpredictable
noise, with impaired performance being
more likely to occur with unpredictable
noise. Other characteristics of noise, such 8.1 Vigilance Task Performance
as its intensity, meaningfulness and appar-
ent controllability, also need to be consid-
ered. Vigilance task performance is undoubtedly
One of the more crucial aspects of noise affected by many factors, including the rate
in determining its effects on performance of signal presentation, the predictability of
may well be the extent to which it is de- signals and the discriminability of signals.
manding of attention. Berlyne (1960) found However, the prevailing state of arousal of
that stimuli that were novel, surprising or in- subjects performing a vigilance task is cer-
congruous were attention demanding, and tainly one of the more important determi-
he explained this by suggesting that the nants of performance: as we saw in Chap. 7,
common characteristic of these stimuli is it seems to be broadly true that poor vigil-
that they produce a discrepancy between ance performance and a pronounced vigil-
what is expected and what is in fact present- ance decrement are most likely to occur in
Continuous Tasks: Serial Reaction and Tracking 157

states of low arousal and lack of alertness. towards the end of the session. While the
Perhaps the first demonstration of the im- reasons for this are not altogether clear, it is
portance of arousal in affecting vigilance likely that noise produces progressively less
performance was that of Mackworth (1950). arousal, stress and distraction over time as
He found that the characteristic decline or habituation occurs.
decrement in vigilance performance that oc- Early work in this area focussed on the ef-
curs during the course of a long session fects of noise on the number of signals de-
could be reduced, or even eliminated, by the tected. However, it is obvious that valuable
administration of the arousing drug am- information is lost if the number of occa-
phetamine or by a telephone call half-way sions on which a signal is reported in its ab-
through the task. sence (i.e. the false alarm rate) is ignored.
Since several reviews of the literature are An appropriate method of combining infor-
available (e.g. Broadbent 1978, Hockey mation about signal detections and false
1979), no detailed examination of the find- alarms was proposed several years ago by
ings seems necessary here. In general terms, signal detection theorists, who distin-
noise tends to impair vigilance performance guished between sensitivity to external stim-
when the task is relatively complex due to a uli and the response criterion. Broadbent
high signal rate or to the presence of more and Gregory (1965) found that noise re-
than one signal source. On the other hand, duced the number of detections associated
detection performance on simple vigilance with a high false alarm rate (i.e. a risky crite-
tasks is often improved under noise condi- rion of report), the state of affairs usually
tions. The importance of task complexity found in vigilance tasks having high signal
was demonstrated by Jerison (1957, 1959), rates. In contrast, noise did not reduce (or
using the Mackworth clock task in which oc- even increased) the detection rate with low
casional double jumps of the second hand signal rates, which are associated with low
must be detected. Noise had no effect on levels of false alarms (i.e. a cautious criteri-
performance when one clock was used, but on). Broadbent and Gregory attributed
did impair performance when three clocks these effects to a change in the decision
were used. mechanism rather than the sensitivity to sig-
The question of what effects noise has on nals; basically, in a noisy environment, sub-
performance depends to quite an extent on jects make less use of intermediate catego-
the level of arousal of the subjects in the ries of confidence in reporting whether or
control group who receive either no noise or not a signal is present.
low intensity noise. Mullin and Corcoran
(1977) compared the effects of noise on vi-
gilance performance of subjects doing the
task at 0800 and 2000. They discovered that 8.2 Continuous Tasks: Serial Reaction
intense noise produced a substantial im- and Tracking
provement in performance in the morning,
but had no discernible effect in the evening.
In view of the time of-day research dis- One of the prevalent assumptions about
cussed in Chap. 7, it is reasonable to assume noise is that it increases the propensity to
that the subjects were less aroused in the momentary attentional lapses. As a result,
morning than in the evening; their under- there has been considerable interest in ob-
aroused morning state enabled noise to fa- serving the effects of noise on tasks placing
cilitate performance. continuous processing demands on the sub-
The evidence suggests that the length of ject. Probably the best known of such tasks
the vigilance session is a relevant factor. By is the five-choice serial reaction task devel-
and large, there are usually more pro- oped by Leonard (1959) in an explicit at-
nounced effects of noise on performance tempt to provide a sensitive method of ex-
158 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise

amining stress effects. A new stimulus ap- each other out when they are applied togeth-
pears as soon as the correct response has er. Corcoran (I 962a) found that white noise
been made to the previous stimulus, and the reduced the number of gaps in sleep-de-
task lasts for 30 min or so. When the task is prived subjects, whereas Wilkinson (1963)
used under normal conditions there are discovered that sleep-deprived subjects
three major effects over time: (I) the num- were less prone to errors when exposed to
ber of errors increases progressively, (2) the noise. A further finding that is consistent
number of very long response times (usually with an arousal-based explanation of noise
known as 'gaps') goes up and (3) the average effects was also obtained by Wilkinson
rate of work remains unchanged. Since gaps (1963), who observed that incentives im-
and errors can both be regarded as reflect- proved performance to a greater extent
ing a transient inefficiency in processing, we in conditions of quiet than in noisy con-
may simply conclude that the task is per- ditions.
formed less efficiently over time. Such findings led Hockey (1979) to the
There is a characteristic pattern of the ef- following conclusion: "Sleep loss impairs
fects of intense noise on performance of the efficiency by reducing the level of arousal
five-choice serial reaction task. Noise typi- below that which is required by the task; in-
cally has no effect on the rate of work but centive increases arousal level from a point
does produce increased errors, especially below the optimal level to the optimum;
towards the end of the experimental session. while noise decreases efficiency by produc-
These results have been obtained several ing too high an arousal level in the subject"
times (e.g. Broadbent 1953, 1957; Hartley (p. 151). There is some merit in this view-
and Carpenter 1974, Wilkinson 1963). In ad- point, but it has difficulty in explaining the
dition, it has sometimes been found that fact that while the effects of noise and sleep
white noise increases the number of gaps loss on arousal are opposite, both stressors
towards the end of the session. are similar in that their adverse effects on
Hartley (1973) argued that there were two performance typically occur towards the
possible reasons why noise has its greatest end of long sessions. As Broadbent (1971)
detrimental effect at the end of the experi- pointed out, such findings suggest the exist-
ment. The first is that prolonged perform- ence of two arousal systems. Sub-or supra-
ance of the task makes subjects more sus- optimal arousal in the first arousal system
ceptible to the effects of noise, and the sec- can be compensated for by efficient func-
ond is that it is simply the exposure to noise tioning of the second system, but this sec-
that produces adverse effects on subjects' ond system becomes progressively less effi-
internal states. These factors can be disen- cient as time on task increases.
tangled by initially exposing subjects either Whereas Broadbent's formulation clearly
to the serial reaction task alone or to noise explains why noise impairs performance
alone. When this was done, Hartley found late on in a task, Hockey's ideas seem to
that prior task performance and previous lead to the prediction that noise-related per-
exposure to noise both contributed inde- formance decrements should be most likely
pendently to the subsequent impairment of when noise has the greatest impact on
performance in noise. arousal level. This is at the beginning of the
In general terms, it has been established task session, before adaptation to the noise
fairly conclusively that many of the effects stimulus has occurred.
of noise on performance of the serial reac- Research with the continuous serial reac-
tion task are mediated by the arousal sys- tion task has apparently demonstrated that
tem. Important confirmation of this posi- noise can increase the number of instants of
tion comes from studies showing that the ef- inefficiency. However, Poulton (1977) has
fects of noise and of sleep deprivation (basi- claimed that the detrimental effects of noise
cally a state of low arousal) typically cancel on this task are due to the fact that noise
Learning and Memory 159

masks auditory cues and thus provides rela- fer to the condition in which bursts of white
tively impoverished feedback to subjects in noise were presented concurrently with the
the noise condition. This is a meretricious paired associates; Berlyne et al. (1966) also
proposal, which has been convincingly re- found that there was no effect of white noise
futed by Broadbent (1978). In any case, on long-term recall when the noise bursts
there are findings using different paradigms were presented slightly after each paired as-
that also point to the existence of increased sociate. This suggests that noise affects
transient inefficiency under intense noise long-term retention by virtue of its effects
conditions. Broadbent (1978) observed that on attentional processes at the time of input.
visual tracking tasks place essentially con- In other work, McLean (1969) found that
tinuous demands on the subject and dis- noise impaired immediate recall, but im-
cussed four studies in which noise produced proved long-term recall; the significant in-
inferior levels of performance. teraction between noise conditions and
length of the retention interval was due
largely to the superior performance under
low-noise conditions at immediate recall.
8.3 Learning and Memory Berlyne and Carey (1968) did not measure
immediate retention, but white noise at in-
put resulted in superior retention a day
The most celebrated study on the relation- later. In a free recall study, Haveman and
ship between arousal and memory was car- Farley (1969) obtained a significant interac-
ried out by Kleinsmith and Kaplan (1963). tion between noise conditions and retention
They asked their subjects to concentrate interval, with better recall of noise than of
carefully on a list of paired associates, and control items after one day but not immedi-
they recorded skin resistance during list ately.
presentation so that each subject's items In view of the inconsistent effects of noise
could be assigned to a high- or low-arousal on short-term retention, it is instructive to
category. There was a dramatic interaction consider the data of Hamilton et al. (1972).
between arousal and retention interval, with They pointed out that the order in which
low-arousal items being much better re- paired associates are tested usually differs
called than high-arousal items at short re- randomly from the order in which they are
tention intervals (up to several minutes), presented for learning. When they arranged
with the reverse occurring at the long reten- matters so that the pairs were tested in the
tion intervals. same order as they had been presented, the
Research on the effects of noise on mem- usual adverse effect of noise on immediate
ory has typically produced a broadly similar recall was replaced by a substantial im-
interaction between arousal and length of provement (see Fig. 8.1). This implies that
the retention interval. It has definitely the typically poor immediate recall of
been established that intense noise at input paired associates learned in noise is due to
improves long-term retention but rather less competing responses based on order infor-
consistent findings have emerged from a mation.
consideration of short-term retention. Ber- Despite various inconsistencies in the da-
lyne et al. (1965) found inferior immediate ta, a significant interaction between noise
recall of paired associates presented in conditions and length of the retention inter-
white noise, but recall after 24 h was better val has generally been obtained. The most
for those items. Somewhat different results celebrated account of this interaction was
were obtained by Berlyne et al. (1966); they offered by Walker (1958), whose action de-
found no effect of white noise at immediate crement theory was discussed at some
recall, but there was a facilitatory effect of length earlier in this book. For present pur-
noise on recall 1 day later. These results re- poses, his key assumption was that high
160 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise

12 As I have indicated, effects on immediate re-


....I
....I 11 ,D FIXED call are ambiguous" (p. 156). While an at-
<{ ,," ORDER tentional account is reasonable, Hockey
U
w 10 ,"" went on to argue that noise leads to en-
a:
/ ""
I- 9 hanced physical processing and reduced se-
u ,'"
w 8 mantic processing. Since long-term reten-
a: r:I'
a: tion is typically much better after semantic
0
U
7
~RANDOM processing than non-semantic processing
z 6 ORDER
<{ (Craik and Lockhart 1972), we are left with a
w 5
~ rather paradoxical situation.
l' I I
An alternative theoretical approach
would be to emphasize the fact that the en-
QUIET NOISE
codings formed under noise conditions
Fig. 8.1. Recall of paired associates in quiet and seem to contain less contextual information
in noise as a function of the order of testing.
(Hamilton et al. 1972) than those formed in quiet conditions
(Hockey and Hamilton 1970, Davies and
Jones, 1975). In other words, encodings pro-
arousal leads to a longer lasting active mem- duced in noise conditions may be simple
ory trace; this results in greater long-term and based primarily on the to-be-learned
memory via enhanced consolidation, but re- stimulus material, whereas encodings pro-
trieval is inhibited while the trace remains duced in quiet may be more complex and
active. may include much more contextual infor-
Walker's theory can certainly account for mation that is only tangentially related to
the typical interaction between arousal con- the to-be-learned material. According to the
ditions and retention interval, but there are encoding specificity principle (Tulving
many findings it cannot explain. Eysenck 1979), the probability of successful retrieval
(l976a) pointed out that there were at least of a stored item "is a monotonically increas-
six free recall and three recognition studies ing function of informational overlap be-
in which high arousal facilitated immediate tween the information present at retrieval
retention, a result which is inconsistent with and the information stored in memory"
Walker's hypothesis. Furthermore, action (p. 408). Especially at long retention inter-
decrement theory argues that the effects of vals, it is plausible to assume that the infor-
arousal on the consolidation process are mation present at retrieval will relate direct-
non-specific, so that the introduction of ly to the stimulus material presented for
bursts of loud noise after the presentation of learning rather than to purely contextual in-
the to-be-learned material should produce formation. Thus the overlap between stored
effects comparable to those obtained when and retrieval information will be greater for
the bursts accompany stimulus presenta- noise encodings, producing enhanced long-
tion. We have already seen that the evidence term retention.
does not support this prediction (e.g. Ber- Memory theorists who have drawn a dis-
lyne et al. 1966). tinction between short-term and long-term
The failure of Walker's theory means that processes or stores have usually envisaged
we must consider alternative hypotheses. short-term storage as lasting for a matter of
Hockey (1979) has argued for an attentional a few seconds and involving attentional and
interpretation: "An arousing stimulus, or an rehearsal processes (e.g. Aktkinson and
increase in the tonic level of arousal, is asso- Shiffrin 1968). A re-conceptualization of the
ciated with greater attention at the time of construct of the 'short-term store' was of-
presentation. This would certainly result in fered by Baddeley and Hitch (1974). Ac-
stronger registration and make items more cording to their model (discussed in
resistant to decay or interference over time. Chap. 3), the concept of the 'short-term
Learning and Memory 161
store' should be replaced by that of 'work- has a certain degree of plausibility, Broad-
ing memory'; the working memory system bent (1978) quite properly pointed out that
comprises a central work space of limited rehearsal seems to involve an articulatory
capacity together with a peripheral articula- rather than an acoustic code, and so there is
tory loop. no reason why noise should disrupt rehear-
Most of the available evidence suggests sal in a direct manner. However, it remains
that intense white noise reduces the capacity an attractive possibility that people exposed
of the short-term store or working memory. to white noise literally 'cannot hear them-
In unpublished work, Wilkinson compared selves think'. As a consequence, they may
immediate serial recall of digit strings pre- sub-articulate to an increased extent in or-
sented under 70-db or 90-db noise condi- der to compensate for the acoustic impair-
tions. He found that recall was inversely re- ment produced by the noise. In other words,
lated to noise intensity. Hamilton et al. the acoustic nature of noise exerts an indi-
(1977) tested the running memory span. The rect influence on the process of rehearsal by
technique they used involved presenting a increasing its importance as a storage mech-
set of items of indeterminate length; when anism.
the sequence is interrupted, the subject at- Some interesting findings were obtained
tempts to recall as many of the most recently by Dornic (1975), who postulated a lower
presented items as possible in their correct storage mechanism relying mainly on physi-
order. Noise was found to reduce running cal features and a higher storage mechanism
memory span. involving identification of stimuli according
Hamilton et al. (1977) obtained addition- to their names or meaning. He discovered
al relevant information in a further experi- that white noise was associated with reason-
ment. They used a letter transformation task able retention of order information but with
in which two factors were manipulated: the poor item retention. He interpreted these
amount of transformation and the number findings by suggesting that noise led to in-
of letters (between one and four) requiring creased use of the lower, more rudimentary
transformation. It is plausible to assume storing mechanism; in other words, noise
that the demands on working memory inten- led to 'parrotting back'.
sify progressively as the number ofletters re- A further experiment by Dornic (1975)
quiring transformation increases. In line may provide some insight into what is hap-
with the research already discussed, intense pening here. Short strings of items were pre-
noise improved performance when the de- sented for immediate recall either on their
mands on working memory were minimal own or while subjects were concurrently
but impaired performance considerably performing a visual task making demands
when several letters had to be transformed. on the capacity of the central processor. Re-
We have seen that noise characteristically call of item information, but not of order in-
reduces the capacity of working memory. It formation, was reduced in the presence of
is still necessary to answer the question as to the visual task, indicating that retention of
whether the adverse effect of noise is pri- order information makes relatively small
marily on the central processor or the articu- demands on central processing capacity.
latory loop. A related question is whether or With respect to the noise data, the implica-
not the effects of noise on working memory tion is that noise reduces the capacity of the
are typical of all arousers. Poulton (1977, central processor that is available for task
1978, 1979) has argued that noise has rather performance, and this leads to increased re-
specific effects due to the fact that it pro- liance on the relatively undemanding articu-
vides an auditory input. He claimed that the latory loop.
short-term retention data can be accounted If retention of information in the correct
for in terms of acoustic masking slowing order depends mainly on use of the articula-
down the rate of rehearsal. While this notion tory loop, then there are other studies sup-
162 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise

porting the notion that there is greater utili- of the central processor. If so, it might be ex-
zation of the articulatory loop in the noise pected that noise would reduce the amount
state. Several investigators, including Hock- of complex processing of, for example, a se-
ey and Hamilton (1970), Hamilton et al. mantic nature.
(1972), Daee and Wilding (1977) and Millar In one well-known study, Hormann and
(1979), have found that noise facilitates or- Osterkamp (1966) found that white noise in-
dered recall. terfered with spontaneous word categoriza-
The hypothesis that noise increases use of tion in memory and thus could be said to
the articulatory loop can be tested by em- have reduced semantic processing. How-
ploying articulatory suppression (e.g. rapid ever, since the noise bursts only occurred
repetition of a monosyllabic word such as when the subjects were responding vocally,
'the') in order to prevent the articulatory it is possible that the reason why noise im-
loop being used to process to-be-Iearned in- paired semantic organization was simply
formation. Wilding and Mohindra (unpub- because it masked auditory feedback.
lished) compared ordered recall of a fixed Other studies have also indicated that
set of letters presented in several different noise can affect semantic analysis. Daee
orders under two levels of white noise. and Wilding (1977) looked at free recall of
When phonemically confusable letters were categorized word lists, and found less cate-
used, the more intense level of white noise gorical clustering with moderate intensity
improved recall under normal non-suppres- noise than with very intense noise or with
sion conditions. Under suppression condi- quiet. Hockey (unpublished) considered re-
tions, however, there was no effect of noise tention of information from a visually pre-
intensity, presumably because greater use of sented factual passage which was accom-
the articulatory loop with intense white panied by white noise. While immediate re-
noise was no longer possible. The picture is call of names from the passage was better
complicated by the results of Millar (1979), following the more intense white noise, the
who found that the advantage of intense opposite was the case with a more semanti-
white noise on ordered recall was actually cally oriented comprehension test.
greater under suppression than non-sup- In sum, a number of studies indicate that
pression conditions. noise biases the system towards relatively
While there is plenty of evidence suggest- low-level processing (especially order inf()r-
ing that the articulatory loop is more exten- mation and articulatory features) and away
sively used under noise conditions, closer from more complex and demanding forms
examination indicates that some qualifica- of processing (e.g. semantic). It is unfortu-
tions are in order. It seems to be the case that nate that the natural prediction based on
the articulatory loop is used in conditions of this conclusion is that noise should impair
intense noise irrespective of whether or not long-term retention, whereas it actually
the initial learning instructions emphasize tends to improve it, but we have already dis-
serial recall; in contrast, the articulatory cussed possible ways of resolving the para-
loop is mainly used in quiet conditions dox.
when the task conditions indicate that order Of course, it would be rather simplistic to
information must be retained. assume that noise inevitably leads to more
In general terms, noise usually leads to in- primitive and less semantic forms of pro-
creased reliance on the articulatory loop. cessing. This may tend to occur when there
What about the effects of noise on the cen- are relatively few constraints on processing,
tral work space component of working but there is little evidence to support this as-
memory? Noise may sometimes alter the sumption when subjects are given specific
balance with respect to the two components processing activities to perform. Smith and
of working memory, increasing the use of Broadbent (1981) found that noise in-
the articulatory loop and decreasing the use creased free recall, but there was no interac-
Learning and Memory 163
tion between noise and the type of process- There are a few studies which have con-
ing (case, rhyme or semantic) which had to sidered whether noise affects the retrieval
be performed on the list words. In other ex- process. Both Uehling and Sprinkle (1968)
periments they used the Posner task in and Baumeister and Kistler (1975) dis-
which subject had to decide whether two let- covered that noise bursts presented shortly
ters had the same name; on positive trials before a retention test improved perform-
there was sometimes a physical and name ance. While this presumably reflects an
match (e.g. 'AA') and sometimes only a alerting function of noise, it is somewhat
name match (e.g. 'aA'). If noise led to in- perplexing that incentives have proved so
creased physical processing, it should have much less successful in enhancing retention
facilitated performance on physical match (see Chap. 5).
trials, but not on name match trials. In fact, Eysenck (1975c) argued that the effects of
there was no interaction between noise and noise in focussing attention upon conceptu-
type of trial. ally dominant or high-priority features of
What ought one to conclude about the ef- the external environment (Broadbent 1971)
fects of noise on processing? Smith and might also apply to the internal environ-
Broadbent (1981) drew the following con- ment or memory. In broad agreement with
clusion from the various apparently incon- the notion that noise increases attentional
sistent findings: "The preferred strategy selectivity, he found that noise did not affect
may depend on the task requirements; and recall speed for highly dominant or accessi-
if noise were to accentuate preference, this ble information, but did reduce speed of re-
would sometimes favour semantic and call for relatively inaccessible information,
sometimes physical encoding" (p. 140). especially among subjects who were highly
Such a formulation is clearly preferable to aroused prior to noise administration (see
the notion that there is a passive noise-in- Fig. 8.2).
duced shift in the level of processing or use Related findings were reported by Millar
of the articulatory loop. However, it is still (1979), who found that intense noise
possible that the earlier generalizations of speeded up high-dominance word recogni-
Eysenck (1977), Broadbent (1978) and tion, but had no effect on low-dominance
others may have some merit when subjects word recognition. He concluded as follows:
are free to select their own processing strate- "Facilitated retrieval of high-dominance
gy. Smith and broadbent's emphasis on the material permits a loose analogy between
effects of noise on strategy clearly allows for noise influence upon stimulus selection
fairly variable effects of noise on perform- from the external environment where simi-
ance but suffers from the disadvantage that lar, highly dominant or high-priority stimu-
we often have no way of predicting what lus features have enhanced probability of
processing strategies will be selected in selection in the stressor's presence. The data
noise. also support Eysenck's point that the retrie-

III 060
w
..- .... HIGH ACTIVATION:
u~
NO NOISE
~~
f-Il: 0-50
«0 - HIGH ACTIVATION
-ilJ.. NOISE
III
wz 0-0 LOW ACTIVATION:
~ ~ 040 NO NOISE
lrf-
Ill\!) 0-·-0 LOW ACTIVATION:
l\! g 030 NOISE
z~

f~__,,________~,___
Fig. 8.2. Speed of recall as a func-
tion of activation level white noise ~
and response dominance. (Eysenck HIGH LOW
1975) DOMINANCE
164 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise
val process may be vulnerable to changes in serial learning task under no noise, 75-db
arousal and that the probability of such in- 85-db or 100-db noise conditions, and inci-
fluence should be accounted for in other dental learning involved additional words
aspects of memory research" (p. 236). in small, light print presented in the peri-
Smith and Broadbent (unpublished) have phery of vision. There was no effect of noise
recently discovered that the precise effects on serial learning (main task), but incidental
of noise on retrieval depend on such factors recall was less than half as high under 85-db
as the immediately previous experiences of or 100-db compared to no noise or 75-db.
the subjects and the procedural details. In more recent work, Hockey et al.
Their data suggested that noise affected the (unpublished) asked subjects to read and
selection of a retrieval strategy in various memorize one passage of prose that was in-
complex ways. terleaved with a second, irrelevant passage.
When the passage was read in intense noise,
recognition memory for the attended mes-
sage (i.e. main task) improved, but there was
8.4 Attentional Selectivity no effect of noise on recognition memory
for the rejected passage (i.e. subsidiary
task). Cohen and Lezak (1977) presented
Easterbrook's (1959) hypothesis, which was nonsense syllables and slides of people en-
discussed in Chap. 4, can be construed as gaged in various activities at the same time
claiming that high arousal leads to in- in quiet or with intermittent indistinguish-
creased attentional selectivity. This hypo- able sounds presented at 95-db. Subjects
thesis can most readily be evaluated by us- were simply told to learn the nonsense syl-
ing a dual-task paradigm in which subjects lables for free recall. There was no effect of
perform a main or high-priority task and a noise on the primary free recall task, but
secondary or low-priority task during the noise reduced recognition memory for the
same period of time. Two main variants of slides (subsidiary task).
this basic paradigm have been used in noise There is an impressive consistency about
studies: (I) the main task involves intention- the results from these five studies. The mo-
allearning, whereas the subsidiary task in- dal pattern (which was obtained in four of
volves incidental learning and (2) the main the studies) is of noise having no effect on
task is visual tracking and the subsidiary the main task but impairing performance on
task is a simple visual or auditory task. the subsidiary task. This pattern of results
There is a fundamental difference between can be interpreted by arguing that noise in-
these two experimental approaches: in the creases the cost in terms of attentional re-
first, the subjects are not aware that there is sources associated with adequate perform-
a subsidiary task on which their perform- ance of the primary task. However, since the
ance will be assessed, whereas they are in- task instructions did not refer to the inciden-
formed in the second approach. tal learning task, poor incidental learning
Work on the effects of noise on concur- does not necessarily reflect reduced spare
rent intentional and incidental learning was processing capacity.
initiated by Hockey and Hamilton (1970). Research in which a visual tracking main
They found that intense noise did not affect task was used has produced somewhat in-
ordered recall of words (main task) but did consistent findings. In one such study
have a detrimental effect on incidental re- (Hockey 1970a), subjects performed a pri-
tention of the spatial locations at which the mary pursuit-tracking task while concur-
words had appeared (subsidiary task). rently monitoring an array of six lights for
These findings were subsequently replicat- flashes. Intense white noise improved per-
ed by Davies and Jones (1975). O'MaUey formance of the main task but had no effect
and Poplawsky (1971) gave their subjects a on the subsidiary task.
Attentional Selectivity 165
In further work, Hockey (1970b) used the ciated with the occurrence of unpredictable
same pursuit-tracking and light-monitoring noise in contrast to predictable noise ...
tasks but manipulated the distribution of The effort involved in noise habituation
signals across light sources. When the lights contributes to the loading of the organism's
were unbiased (i.e. there was an equal prob- channel capacity, so that he is less able to
ability of each light being illuminated), cope with other environmental demands
noise led to improved tracking but to a de- and expectations, for example, a subsidiary
crement in monitoring. When the lights task" (p. 213).
were biased, noise improved tracking but These tracking studies indicate that in-
had no effect on monitoring. Unfortunately tense noise produces a re-allocation of at-
Forster and Grierson (1978) were unable to tentional resources, so that performance on
replicate these results in a series of four ex- the main task is more likely than perform-
periments, but they used less intense noise ance on the subsidiary task to be maintained
than Hockey had done. or improved. However, some inconsisten-
Hartley (1981) attempted to replicate the cies emerge when a more detailed analysis
findings of Hockey (1970b) using a biased of the findings is carried out. Tracking per-
distribution of lights. Hartley found that formance was sometimes unaffected by
noise reduced tracking performance, al- noise and sometimes improved; a task of
though the adverse effect of noise did di- relatively long duration is usually required
minish over time. The findings with respect in order to demonstrate an enhancement ef-
to the light-monitoring task were very simi- fect of noise.
lar to those of Hockey: there was no overall An unusually thorough dual-task study
effect of noise, but the advantage in terms of was conducted by Boggs and Simon (1968).
speed of responding to central rather than Their primary task was a reaction-time task
peripheral lights was enhanced under noise that was either simple or complex, and the
conditions. subsidiary task was listening for odd-even-
Bell (1978) also used a pursuit-tracking odd sequences of digits. Unpredictable
task as the main task, but the secondary task bursts of white noise had no discernible ef-
required the subject to indicate whether fect on speed of responding on the main
each in a series of two-digit numbers was task but produced a very substantial in-
less than or greater than the preceding num- crease in errors on the subsidiary task. Of
ber.White noise at 95-db did not alter the most interest, there was an interaction be-
amount of time on target in the tracking task tween noise and complexity of the primary
but produced more errors on the number task (see Fig. 8.3) in which the detrimental
task. effect of noise on the secondary task was
A similar task was conducted by Finkel- greater when the primary task was complex
man and Glass (1970), in which the main and thus relatively attention demanding.
task was tracking a visual input and the sub- The crucial finding from these studies
sidiary task involved short-term memory. that requires explanation is that intense
Unpredictable noise bursts at 80-db had no white noise is far more likely to impair per-
effect on the main task but impaired per- formance of the less important of two tasks
formance on the subsidiary task. In con- than of the more important. The simplest in-
trast, predictable bursts did not affect either terpretation is based on attentional selectivi-
task. Perhaps unpredictable bursts make ty in noise, and an account along these lines
greater demands on the processing system was put forward by Hockey (1970b): "The
than predictable bursts, as Finkelman and increase in selectivity with noise... thus
Glass concluded : "The use of a subsidiary seems best described as an enhancement of
task measure of reserve mental capacity pro- .attention paid to sources already being giv-
vided support for the hypothesized reduc- en priority with a resulting withdrawal of at-
tion in information processing load asso- tention from low priority sources" (p. 41).
166 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise

20 COMPLEX
TASK

:.::
If) If) 15
o::<!
01- SIMPLE
0::>- TASK
~~ 10
z-
<!e
WIf)

2~ 5
If)
Fig. 8.3. Errors on the subsidiary task
in quiet and in noise as a function of
the complexity of the primary task.
QUIET NOISE (Boggs and Simon 1968)

Reasonable evidence that noise induces a of performance are produced by the same
shift in the allocation of attention was ob- process of attentional selectivity? In parti-
tained by Hockey (1973) using a three- cular, if noise merely leads to an increased
source monitoring task. The task involved selectivity of attention, it is by no means
detecting faults in any of the sources, with clear why noise sometimes impairs subsidi-
faults being distributed over the three ary task performance without any compen-
sources in the ratio 6:3: 1. The state of any satory improvement in main task perform-
source could be checked by pressing the ap- ance.
propriate button. Under paced conditions, A further difficulty is that it is not clear
there was an increase in the selectivity of why noise produces increased attentional se-
sampling in intense noise, i.e. the tendency lectivity. Easterbrook (1959) assumed that
to sample the information source most like- high arousal led to a narrowing of attention
ly to be faulty was increased by noise. in a rather automatic fashion. However, this
The notion that noise produces increased explanation leads quite directly to what one
attentional selectivity but has no effect on might refer to as 'Easterbrook's paradox':
attentional capacity leads naturally to the since subjects attend more and more exclu-
prediction that performance of the main sively to task cues as arousal increases, it fol-
task should improve under intense noise, lows necessarily that the more intense white
whereas subsidiary task performance noise becomes, the less it is attended to! This
should be impaired. In fact, noise has only does not seem very plausible.
been found to improve performance on the An alternative and preferable way of ac-
more important of two tasks in 5 out of a to- counting for the data is to argue that intense
tal of 17 experimental comparisons, and noise reduces attentional capacity in addi-
even then the superiority took 30 min or tion to increasing attentional selectivity.
more to manifest itself. The basic assumption is that the reduced at-
Hockey (1978) returned to the fray with a tentional capacity under intense noise leads
more general version of an attentional selec- to a voluntary shift in the allocation of the
tivity hypothesis, according to which at least remaining attentional resources in an at-
three different patterns of results are all in- tempt to maintain a reasonable level of per-
dicative of increased selectivity: improved formance on the main task. More specifical-
main task performance combined with eith- ly, some of the spare processing capacity
er no effect on, or impairment of, subsidiary normally used to monitor the enviroment is
task performance, or no effect on main task re-allocated to the main task.
performance combined with impairment of This theoretical account has at least two
subsidiary task performance. However, is it important advantages over the simpler at-
reasonable to claim that all of these patterns tentional selectivity hypothesis: (1) it pre-
Theoretical Formulations 167

dicts that noise will typically have no effect emphasis here will be on the detailed theory
on main task performance but will impair which he suggested in 1979. He began by ar-
secondary task performance, which is, be- guing that any satisfactory theory must be
yond peradventure, the modal pattern and able to explain the fact that continuous
(2) it suggests a reason for the increased at- noise can either improve or degrade per-
tentional selectivity found in the noise state. formance. Quintessentially, Poulton (1979)
According to the theoretical position ad- claimed that there are two major effects of
vocated here, people exposed to intense noise: (1) there is masking of acoustic feed-
noise are only able to maintain an adequate back and inner speech and (2) there is an in-
level of performance on most primary tasks crease in the level of arousal. The masking
by utilizing more of their available process- effect of intense noise will produce a per-
ing resources than would be needed in quiet formance decrement on any task in which
conditions. Some intriguing evidence that acoustic information is necessary for suc-
any given level of performance may be cessful performance, whereas the arousing
achieved at greater 'cost' to the noise- effect of intense noise always enhances per-
aroused system was obtained by Dornic formance. However, as a result of adapta-
(1977). He found that there was no effect of tion, the arousal produced by noise de-
noise on the performance of a moderately creases gradually, and there is a concomit-
complex closed-system-thinking task; how- ant reduction in the beneficial effect of
ever, self-reported effort was greater when noise on task performance.
the task was performed in noise rather than The main thrust of the theory was pithily
quiet. A more complex version of the same expressed by Poulton (1979) in the follow-
task was performed less well in noise than in ing words: "An experimenter who wishes to
quiet, but self-reported effort was the same find a reliable deterioration in continuous
in both conditions. It may well be that such noise should select a task that is susceptible
results will only be obtained when attention- to masking ... An experimenter who wishes
ally-demanding tasks are used. to find a reliable improvement in continu-
ous noise should select a task that responds
well to arousal but is not susceptible to
masking" (p. 363).
8.5 Theoretical Formulations Before considering in more detail the way
in which noise allegedly affects perform-
ance on specific tasks, it is worth noting that
In recent years, there have been a number of Poulton attached s,ome importance to the
attempts to provide some theoretical inte- notion of asymmetrical negative transfer,
gration for the numerous effects of noise on which he claimed explains why noise does
performance which have been reported in not always have a detrimental effect on per-
the literature. In this connection, the views formance when it is expected to do so. The
of Poulton (1977, 1978, 1979), Broadbent idea is that subjects may be forced to use a
(1978), and Hockey (1979) are of particular relatively inefficient task strategy in noise
interest and importance. These three theo- conditions and may then inappropriately
retical positions will be considered in the re- transfer that strategy when performing the
mainder of this chapter. task in quiet.
Poulton suggested that the detrimental ef-
fects of noise on tasks such as the five-
8.5.1 Poulton (1977,1978,1979) choice continuous serial reaction task, visu-
al tracking and vigilance could all be ex-
While Poulton has put forward his ideas plained quite straightforwardly. The appar-
concerning the effects of noise on perform- atus commonly used with all of these tasks is
ance in several different publications, the of such a type that responding by the subject
168 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise

produces an audible click or other sound. extremely doubtful whether all of the detri-
This acoustic feedback provides useful in- mental effects of noise on performance can
formation to the subject but is unavailable be explained in such simple terms. As
under conditions of intense noise because of Broadbent (1978) has pointed out, practical-
masking. The adverse effects of masking on ly all of the major studies incorporate var-
performance are counteracted initially by ious controls and checks against artefacts
the state of high arousal created by the such as masking effects. For example, the
noise. After the subject has spent more time usual practice has been to use a low level of
on the task, however, the level of arousal masking noise in the quiet or control condi-
falls back towards the resting level, and then tion, so that acoustic cues from the appara-
the negative effects of masking become ap- tus would not be available even to subjects
parent. in the control condition.
Poulton explained the detrimental effects In many cases, it is not clear how acoustic
of noise on the storage of words in working feedback could prove useful to subjects run
memory along similar lines. He drew a dis- in quiet. Consider as an example the consist-
tinction between a relatively long-lasting ent finding that noise increases the inci-
sensory or echoic form of storage and a dence of errors on the five-choice continu-
shorter lasting articulatory form of storage ous serial reaction task. It is undeniable that
requiring more frequent rehearsal. He ar- each response makes a sound and thus pro-
gued that noise interferes with sensory stor- vides acoustic feedback; however, since er-
age and thus forces the subject to resort to ror responses make the same sound as cor-
articulatory storage. This necessary shift in rect responses, it is difficult to see how such
processing strategy under noise conditions feedback could be used to improve perform-
means that less capacity is available for ad- ance.
ditional processing. Perhaps the strongest evidence against
The assumption that high arousal always the notion that virtually all of the adverse ef-
tends to enhance performance is supported fects of noise on performance can be
by some of the work of Frankenhaeuser and ascribed to masking comes from several
her associates (Frankenhaeuser 1975), ac- studies in which deterioration of perform-
cording to Poulton (1979). Frankenhaeuser ance on one part of a task is accompanied by
observed a consistent relationship between improvement on another part, in spite of
good performance and excretion of adrenal- the fact that acoustic cues are the same in
in (an arouser) at above the median rate. both cases. For example, Boggs and Simon
This relationship was obtained with such di- (1968) found that noise worsened perform-
verse tasks as learning nonsense syllables, ance of an auditory task when it was per-
the Stroop test, mental arithmetic and visual formed in conjunction with a difficult visual
vigilance. However, elevated levels of choice reaction task, but not when it was
adrenalin excretion in these circumstances performed with an easy choice reaction
presumably reflect high motivation and ef- task. Since any acoustic masking in noise
fort expenditure, and arousal produced in should have applied equally to both ver-
this way may well differ importantly from sions of the choice reaction task, the ob-
arousal produced by noise administration. tained pattern of results is difficult for Poul-
The theoretical position of Poulton (1979) ton to explain. Broadbent (1978) considers
has now been described in some detail. How several more studies in which an interpreta-
adequately does he account for the major ef- tion of the basic findings in terms of acous-
fects of noise on performance? That ques- tic masking is untenable.
tion can best be answered by considering One of Poulton's (1979) other major the-
Poulton's main theoretical assumptions one oretical assumptions was expressed by him
at a time. The assumption that noise has a in the following way: "While performing a
masking effect is undoubtedly true, but it is task, increased physiological arousal is as-
Theoretical Formulations 169

sociated with improvements in perform- efficiency; the evidence for this comes from
ance" (p. 367). In view of the overwhelming studies using the five-choice continuous
evidence that arousers such as heat, incen- serial reaction task, in which noise increases
tives and stimulant drugs all impair per- the number of errors and gaps but has no ef-
formance under some circumstances, this is fect on the speed of working. The third ad-
a very dubious assumption. Consider as an verse effect of noise is to produce concentra-
example the effects of arousal on the five- tion on some parts of a complex display at
choice continuous serial reaction task. Ac- the expense of others; perhaps the most di-
cording to Poulton (1979), noise degrades rect support for this contention comes from
performance on this task by means of acous- studies using the dual-task methodology, in
tic masking. However, heat has also been which performance on a low-priority or sub-
found to increase errors on this task (e.g. sidiary task is much more likely to be wors-
Poulton and Edwards 1974), and similar re- ened in noise than performance on a high-
sults have been obtained under conditions priority task.
of very high incentive (Willett 1964b). Since At a more speculative level, Broadbent
heat and incentives obviously do not pro- (1978) wondered whether these three sepa-
duce acoustic masking, common sense indi- rate effects of noise might in fact be dynami-
cates that the equivalent effects of noise, cally interrelated. For example, the prepo-
heat and incentives on this task are mediat- tent effect of the state of high arousal in-
ed by supra-optimal levels of arousal. duced by noise may be that information
Another unequivocal refutation of the as- from a smaller area of the environment is se-
sumption that high arousal always enhances lected and attended to. As a consequence of
performance is available in studies using the this funnel vision effect, environmental in-
dual-task paradigm. The less important of formation may be selected or rejected in a
the two concurrent tasks is nearly always more emphatic manner in the noise state,
detrimentally affected by high arousal (Eas- and this might reduce the number of quali-
terbrook 1959). fied or doubtful decisions made by the sub-
All in all, Poulton's theory must be reject- ject. If the amount of information processed
ed. Noise does produce acoustic masking, at any moment is very small in noise condi-
but very few of the available findings can be tions, then an attentional shift away from
explained properly in terms of masking. task stimuli would produce unequivocal evi-
Noise does produce elevated arousal, but dence of inefficiency of any task requiring
the further assumption that all of the effects continuous performance (e.g. visual track-
of high arousal on performance are benefi- ing or continuous serial reaction).
cial is erroneous. Since the two major theo- Broadbent also considered the detrimen-
retical assumptions are erroneous, the the- tal effects of noise on verbal memory. He re-
ory as a whole cannot be adequate. jected the idea that these effects were due to
masking of internal speech (Poulton 1977),
pointing out that there is good evidence in-
8.5.2 Broadbent (1978) dicating that visually presented verbal mate-
rial is not recoded into a form in which it
Broadbent (1978) argued that three import- would be susceptible to acoustic masking.
ant harmful effects of noise on skilled per- In Broadbent's (1978) own words, "Internal
formance have been clearly identified. The speech is not masked by noise; it is a motor
first of these is a reduction in the detection pattern, not a senSJry one" (p. 1062).
of visual signals that are reported with risky As an alternative, Broadbent speculated
cirteria of judgement; this finding has that the articulatory loop component of
emerged from studies of vigilance. The sec- working memory might be extensively used
ond detrimental effect of noise is an in- when subjects were in the noise state. This
crease in the number of brief moments of in- increased use of rehearsal can be seen as a
170 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise

matter of "talking to oneself at all costs" effects of arousers on performance can be


(p. 1062). We saw earlier in the chapter that incorporated within this conceptualization.
for whatever reason, noise often produces Largely on the basis of noise research, he
greater reliance on the articulatory loop in suggested the following hypothesis: "Low
verbal memory tasks. arousal is a suitable state for using storage
Broadbent has undoubtedly identified and a poor one for speed; high arousal is
many of the major effects of intense noise poor for storage and good for speed. The
on performance. However, it seems likely Yerkes-Dodson Law is but a gross over-sim-
that a further effect of noise that was not plification of this shift in the balance of ef-
stressed by him is to produce a reduction in fectiveness of the different processing re-
the total processing resources available. As sources to the person" (p. 168). In this con-
we argued previously, if there is simply a text, 'storage' means working or operational
funnel vision effect (i.e. increased attention- memory.
al selectivity), then noise should typically The ideal method of testing this hypothe-
improve performance on the main task at sis is obviously to construct a series of relat-
the expense of the subsidiary task. Since the ed tasks that differ in terms of the demands
modal result is that noise impairs subsidiary they make on processing speed and storage
task performance but has no effect on pri- capacity. This was done by Hamilton et al.
mary task performance, the obvious inter- (1977) who devised a total of 16 versions of a
pretation is that equivalent primary task letter transformation task. In the simplest
performance in noise and in quiet has been version of the task, subjects only had to indi-
achieved at greater 'cost' in the noise state. cate the letter immediately following a given
In other words, it may be preferable to ac- letter (e.g. 'F + 1 = G'). The task can be
count for the dual-task findings in terms of changed by varying the number of letters re-
an active re-allocation of reduced resources quiring transformation between one and
rather than in terms of a relatively automatic four (e.g. 'MWER + 1 = NXFS'), and ~ot
increase in attentional selectivity. allowing the subject to respond until all of the
letters have been transformed. The task can
also be changed by varying the size of the
8.5.3 Hockey (1979) required transformation (1, 2, 3 or 4 letters).
The results conform to the Yerkes-Dod-
Hamilton et al. (1977) and Hockey (1979) son Law, in that high arousal in the form of
have recently proposed an interesting theo- noise tended to improve performance on the
retical framework for noise research. While simpler version of the task but to worsen
they make several important points, their performance on the more difficult versions.
most crucial theoretical assumption con- However, it is much more revealing to note
cerns the balance between speed of process- that noise enhanced performance by ap-
ing and the capacity of a transient short- proximately 10% on those tasks where the
term store or working memory. More speci- primary emphasis was on processing speed
fically, they argued that speeded operations (i.e. one-letter tasks), but it slowed down
and rapid action are most readily achieved performance speed by almost 20% on tasks
when the short-term store is not overloaded making substantial demands on working
with information, and conversely short-term memory (i.e. three- and four-letter tasks).
storage capacity is maximal when there is no Hockey (personal communication) has
great impulsion to action. In other words, recently used the same letter transformation
there may be a trade-off or inverse relation- task but this time under conditions requir-
ship between the ability to process at high ing the subject to transform each letter out
speed and the capacity to store information loud. In addition, the letters are presented
transiently. sequentially, and the subject presses a but-
According to Hockey (1979), many of the ton after he or she has processed a letter in
Theoretical Formulations 171

order to see the next letter. The total pro- An alternative method of ascertaining the
cessing time per letter can be divided into characteristic speed of processing in the
three separate successive stages: (1) be- noise state would be to require subjects to
tween presentation of the letter and the on- perform a simple task (e.g. reciting the al-
set of transformation (2) transformation phabet) at their own preferred speed. The
time and (3) storage time between the end of tempo of such processing might be much
the transformation and the presentation of faster in noise than in quiet.
the next letter. Noise speeds up the rate of Hockey (1979) argued that noise had ef-
transformation but slows down the subse- fects over and above increasing the speed of
quent storage time. In some unspecified processing and reducing short-term storage
way, noise appears to disrupt some of the or- capacity. These include increased attention-
ganizational skills involved in task perform- al selectivity, increased response selectivity
ance. and a reduced use of intermediate catego-
The general notion that noise reduces ries of confidence. Most of these effects are
short-term holding capacity receives sup- reasonably well established and have al-
port from studies on digit span (Wilkinson ready been discussed in detail earlier in the
unpublished) and running memory span chapter.
(Hamilton et al. 1977). However, while the In conclusion, Hockey emphasizes the
additional assumption that noise increases fact that the state induced by noise should
the speed of processing is intuitively reason- be regarded as producing only a predisposi-
able, the evidence in its favour is less than tion towards certain form of mental activity
compelling. The five-choice continuous and away from others. In practical terms,
serial reaction task places the emphasis very the noise-induced state will typically only
much on performance speed, and yet noise impose modest limitations on what an indi-
typically has no discernible effect on the vidual can achive in terms of cognitive pro-
speed of working on that task. It may be that cessing. As we have stressed a number of
subjects run in quiet conditions allocate ex- times in this book, an individual who dis-
tra resources in order to comply with the ex- covers that noise, fatigue or anxiety is im-
plicit or implicit requirement to perform as pairing his performance will often attempt
rapidly as possible. to rectify matters by utilizing compensatory
The typical failure of noise to alter pro- mechanisms. The implication for noise re-
cessing rate can also be interpreted in terms search is that the 'natural' effects of noise on
of the distinction between resource-limited performance will often be masked by these
and data-limited processes (Norman and compensatory mechanisms. However, the
Bobrow 1975). A task is resource limited if extent to which such compensation is
an increase in allocated processing re- needed depends upon the compatibility be-
sources enhances performance, and it is da- tween the state of arousal created by noise
ta limited when performance is independent and the demands of task performance: the
of changes in allocated resources. Since sub- greater the degree of compatibility, the less
jects usually seem to operate at the boun- effort is required of the compensatory sys-
dary between these two conditions, per- tem.
formance speed in noise may be restricted In sum, the most interesting contribution
by data limitations. One way of attempting of Hamilton et al. (1977) and Hockey (1979)
to make a task resource-limied is to prolong is the idea that noise makes some kinds of
the work period. When Hamilton et al. resources more available (e.g. those con-
(1977) did this by requiring subjects to per- cerned with processing speed) but makes
form the one-letter transformation task with other kinds of resources less available (e.g.
randomly varying transformation size for those concerned with temporary storage of
40 min, speed of performance in noise be- information). While their specific sugges-
came progressively faster than in quiet. tions do not have overwhelming support,
172 Exogenous Determinants of Arousal: Noise

the basic notion that noise has differential fects on attentional selectivity could result
effects on the various component processes simply from a prudent re-allocation of the
involved in cognitive functioning is un- remaining resources. This interpretation al-
doubtedly fruitful. so explains why noise usually degrades per-
formance of a subsidiary task without any
compensatory improvement in main task
performance.
8.6 Summary It is interesting to compare the kinds of re-
search carried out using different arousers.
There is reasonable evidence that some of
It is probably true to say that we have a the adverse effects of introversion and of
clearer picture of the effects of noise on per- anxiety are attributable to increased distrac-
formance than of any other arouser. There tibility, and yet nothing appears to be
are good grounds for rejecting Poulton's in- known about distractibility in the noise
terpretation of noise-induced changes in state. It may well be that some of the in-
performance, but both Broadbent (1978) creased error making in noise is due to high
and Hockey (1979) have drawn reasonable distractibility, and the issue could easily be
conclusions from the available evidence. investigated.
One major issue does merit further con- As with most other arousers, noise affects
sideration, however. The notion that noise performance largely via attentional me-
increases attentional selectivity is often chanisms. Both the reduced attentional ca-
treated as a deus ex machina, that is to say as pacity in noise and the increased selectivity
an unexplained and possibly inexplicable of attention seem to play fundamental roles
phenomenon. However, if noise reduces the in determining the behavioural conse-
available processing resources, then its ef- quences of noise-induced arousal.
9 Conclusions and Speculations

9.1 The Yerkes-Dodson Law (1967) reviewed the evidence available at


that time and concluded that it was neces-
In this book we have considered some of the sary to distinguish among behavioural, au-
major effects which such variegated arous- tonomic and cortical arousal. The main rea-
ers such as anxiety, introversion-extraver- son for this was that measures of each of
sion, incen.tives, time of day, white noise these three varieties of arousal are frequent-
and sleep deprivation have on performance. ly poorly related to each other.
To what extent are there any general themes Lacey (1967) also discussed the well-
running through this gallimaufry of re- known fact that even different measures of
search? One such theme is formed by the the same kind of arousal often fail to agree
Yerkes-Dodson law, which seems to have with each other. This seems to be especially
served as the basis for much of the research true of autonomic arousal or activation.
endeavour in this area. While we have been More specifically, Lacey claimed that dif-
at pains to point out that one of the law's ferent patterns of autonomic activity are
disadvantages is its ability to encompass found in different task situations. All of the
many different patterns of results with no major measures of autonomic activation
apparent difficulty, it is nonetheless of inter- tend to show increased levels of activity
est that it does describe reasonably ade- when the task requires active manipUlation
quately findings involving extremely heter- of information as occurs in reasoning, prob-
ogenous arousers. lem solving or mental arithmetic. In con-
How can we transform the Yerkes-Dod- trast, when the task requires attention to an
son law with its purely descriptive formula- external source of information (e.g. the fore-
tion of the interrelationships among several period of a reaction-time experiment), the
different factors into a more explanatory characteristic pattern is one of directional
theory? One obvious strategy (and the one fractionation in which heart rate decelerates
followed here) is to consider in turn each of while other measures reflect enhanced auto-
the key constructs enshrined in the Yerkes- nomic activity.
Dodson law and to re-evaluate them in the It seems plausible to assume that heart
light of recent theoretical and empirical ad- rate deceleration during the foreperiod of
vances. reaction-time tasks is related to the marked
According to the Yerkes-Dodson law, reduction in motor activity and increased
'arousal' can be regarded as a uni-dimen- steadiness of gaze that occur at the same
sional construct. The notion that different time. However, Elliott (1969) found that
levels of arousal merely represent quantita- heart rate decelerated on the Stroop task, in
tive variation along some internal continu- spite of the fact that subjects were verbaliz-
um has been popular for many years but is ing throughout the task, and there was also
undoubtedly erroneous. One way of assess- considerable evidence of motor activity.
ing this notion is to consider whether or not A related conceptualization was favoured
physiological work indicates the existence by Pribram and McGuinness (1975). They
of a single arousal based system. Lacey argued for the existence of three separate
174 Conclusions and Speculations

neural systems. The first of these systems sion B. It would clearly be unwise to assume
controls arousal; it is based in the amygdala that all arousers produce broadly compar-
and is associated with phasic physiological able effects on arousal, whether arousal is
responses to environmental stimuli. The sec- indexed in terms of self-reported arousal or
ond system controls activation and is de- in terms of physiological measures.
fined in terms of tonic physiological readi- We must therefore conclude that the sim-
ness to respond; its locus is the basal ganglia plistic notion of a single arousal system
of the forebrain. The third and final system must be rejected and replaced by a more
is centred on the hippocampus and reflects complex conceptualization. Perhaps the
effort in the sense of activity co-ordinating most obvious division between different
arousal and activation processes. varieties of arousal is that between arousal
A clear implication of these physiological produced as a result of the active investment
formulations is that two or more qualitative- of processing resources in a performance
ly different activation or arousal states must task and arousal produced in a more passive
be distinguished. In that case, a simple uni- way by the manipulation of non-task arous-
dimensional arousal construct would seem ers (e.g. white noise or stimulant drugs).
to be doomed. However, it is worth noting This distinction will be developed later in
that most of the different specific arousal or the chapter.
activation states which have been identified The first major assumption of the Yerkes-
seem to reflect fairly closely the processing Dodson law is that there is an inverted-U re-
requirements of particular kinds of task. lationship between arousal and the efficien-
Thus it may be reasonable to assume that cy of performance, with increasing levels of
the arousal (or effort) produced by the de- arousal first improving and then impairing
mands of some tasks differs qualitatively performance. A moment's reflection will in-
from the arousal produced by the demands dicate that the notion of a single, global as-
of other tasks. On the other hand, arousal sessment of performance efficiency is inade-
produced in other ways (e.g. by intense quate. Perhaps the clearest illustration is
noise, threat of shock or stimulant drugs) contained in findings obtained by Hockey
may prove to be of a general and undifferen- (personal communication). He found that
tiated nature. white noise produced a substantial slowing
As we saw in Chap. 4, recent factor ana- down of performance in that condition of
lytic studies of self-reported arousal have al- the letter transformation task in which four
so suggested that there are two or more sep- letters have to be added to each of four let-
arate arousal dimensions. At the very least, ters. In conventional terms, this simply
it is clear that a purely intensive arousal di- means that high arousal in the form of in-
mension must be supplemented by a dimen- tense noise reduced performance efficiency.
sion reflecting hedonic tone or pleasant- However, a fine-grain analysis of perform-
ness-unpleasantness. Thayer (1978a) argued ance revealed a more complicated state of
that there were two major independent di- affairs: noise reduced the speed with which
mensions within this two-dimensional each letter was stored after transformation
space; he referred to them as Activation Di- but actually increased the speed with which
mension A (ranging from energetic and vi- each letter was transformed.
gorous to sleepy and tired) and Activation Such findings indicate the dangers of re-
Dimension B (ranging from tense to placid lying on an overall assessment of processing
and still). Of particular importance, Thayer efficiency. When only a global measure is
discovered that time of day and physical ex- used, the presumption must be that arousal
ercise both affected Activation Dimen- has comparable effects on all of the compo-
sion A more than Activation Dimension B, nent processes involved in performing the
whereas anxiety stress and white noise pre- task. Since it is probable that differential ef-
dominantly affected Activation Dimen- fects of arousal on each task process are the
The Yerkes-Dodson Law 175

norm rather than the exception, it is mani- standing of the notion of task difficulty, we
fest that performance must be analyzed in may distinguish between objective task dif-
sufficient detail to permit measurement of ficulty and subjective task difficulty. The
each of the separate stages or processes of difference between these two kinds of task
information processing. difficulty can be seen quite clearly if we con-
There is a further, largely unrecognized, sider the effects of practice on performance
problem that can result from paying too of a cognitive task: while the objective diffi-
much attention to a global measure of per- culty of any particular task remains constant
formance efficiency. It is fatally easy to as- over time, the subjective difficulty of the
sume that, if an arouser does not produce a task will often be markedly reduced as sub-
significant effect on overall task perform- jects become more familiar with the skills re-
ance, this means that arousal has had no ef- quired for successful task performance.
fect on internal processing of task informa- Task difficulty has typically been construed
tion. In fact, of course, any particular level as an objective characteristic of tasks, but it
of task performance can be achieved in a seems more sensible to regard it as involving
number of different ways, depending on fac- an interaction between an individual and a
tors such as the amount of on-task effort task. In other words, subjective task diffi-
and the efficiency of the various component culty is more psychologically relevant than
task processes. At the empirical level, the objective task difficulty.
clearest examples of the inadequacy of rely- While there are undoubtedly many rea-
ing on a single measure of performance oc- sons why some tasks are regarded as more
cur in dual-task studies (Easterbrook 1959). difficult than others, it seems probable that
The modal finding is that arousal has no one major factor is the amount of attention-
significant effect on the main or high-priori- al capacity required by any given task. As a
ty task, which suggests that arousal has min- first approximation, we could argue that a
imal effects on processing. However, exami- task which severely disrupts almost any
nation of subsidiary or low-priority task per- other concurrent activity is more attention-
formance typically reveals a substantial ad- ally demanding and thus more difficult than
verse effect of high arousal. Whatever the another task which produces little or no dis-
most appropriate explanation of this pat- ruption of concurrent processing. Of course,
tern of results, it is crystal clear that the one task may interfere with a second task be-
usual procedure of evaluating the overall cause both tasks use the same specific pro-
impact of high arousal on a single task has cessing mechanisms (e.g. if two visual tasks
very little to recommend it. are used, the subject cannot look in two dif-
We have seen that two of the constructs ferent directions at once), so that assessment
involved in the Yerkes-Dodson law (i.e. of the attentional demands of any given task
'arousal' and 'performance efficiency') requires a number of dual-task experiments
must be re-conceptualized in order to ac- in which that task is paired with each of a
count for more recent findings. The third disparate collection of concurrent tasks.
major construct used by Yerkes and Dod- Perhaps the single most important de-
son (1908) was 'task difficulty'; they terminant of the attentional demands of a
claimed that the optimal level of arousal task is the extent to which the task requires
was inversely related to task difficulty. How temporary storage of information. Thus
should we define 'task difficulty'? In their within the field of mental arithmetic, it is
original study, Yerkes and Dodson defined easier to calculate the answer to a multipli-
task difficulty solely in terms of ease of dis- cation problem such as '8 x 4 = l' than to
crimination along a brightness dimension, do the same for a problem such as
but probably no one today would subscribe '81 x 42 = 1'. Of course, the latter problem
to such a narrow definition. involves more stages of processing in arriv-
As a first step towards a fuller under- ing at a solution, but the major reason that it
176 Conclusions and Speculations

is a difficult problem is that it requires sto- The detrimental effects of supra-optimal


rage of the products of initial processing at levels of arousal on performance have
the same time as active processing of the la- usually been accounted for in a rather sim-
ter stages of the problem. As Kahneman plistic way. Most theorists have emphasized
(1973) pointed out, it is not strictly accurate a single mechanism, ranging from an exces-
to assume that the attentional demands of a sive narrowing of attention (Easterbrook
task such as mental arithmetic remain con- 1959) to attention to the arousal state rather
stant throughout task processing. While than to the task (Mandler 1975). Probably
multiplying two two-digit numbers together the main reason for performance decre-
is demanding, it is probable that the de- ments under high arousal is a reduced abili-
mands only become substantial relatively ty to engage in parallel or shared processing.
late in the course of problem solution. As a consequence, adverse effects of high
In the light of our discussion so far, we arousal on performance are most pro-
can now return to the task of attempting to nounced on tasks requiring concurrent oper-
provide a theoretical underpinning for the ations of various kinds (e.g. accessing long-
two assumptions incorporated into the term memory, temporarily holding informa-
Yerkes-Dodson law. Why does task per- tion and actively processing task stimuli).
formance characteristically improve as the Why does this reduced ability to process
level of arousal begins to increase but wors- in parallel occur? There appear to be two
en as the arousal level continues to in- main possible answers: (1) there is a reduc-
crease? The initial improvement is most tion in total attentional capacity in high
readily explained by assuming that the pro- arousal, or (2) total attentional capacity is
portion of the available processing re- not reduced, but a smaller proportion of at-
sources invested in the task increases as tentional capacity is available for task pro~
arousal goes up from its basal level. This is cessing. The experimental evidence is less
in essence the classic Easterbrook (1959) ex- clear than one would like, but it does tend to
planation based on the notion of greater at- favour the latter answer. This is certainly
tentional selectivity under high arousal. true when anxiety manipulations are used
However, there is an important difference but may not apply to all arousers. The fact
between the viewpoint espoused here and that distractibility tends to increase in states
that of Easterbrook. It is intuitively appeal- of high arousal is, of course, consistent with
ing to assume that this increased focussing the idea that there is more of-task process-
of attention on the task at moderate levels of ing under high than moderate levels of
arousal is more probable when arousal re- arousal.
sults from incentive conditions than when it Off-task processing can involve either en-
results from stress. vironmental or internal sources of informa-
In addition to increased on-task atten- tion. As Broadbent (1971) has demonstrat-
tion, moderate levels of arousal may also ed, some non-task environmental process-
speed up the rate with which simple mental ing under high arousal is attributable to re-
and response operations occur, especially if duced ability to discriminate perceptually
people are asked to respond at their 'natu- between task and non-task stimuli. Indeed,
ral' rate or tempo rather than in conformity it is possible that the greater distractibility
with speed instructions. Kahneman (1973) observed in states of high arousal only oc-
has claimed a third advantage for a moder- curs when the distracting stimuli resemble
ate level of arousal over the normal, resting task stimuli fairly closely.
arousal level, namely that attentional capac- Off-task processing of internal informa-
ity is greater. While this is an intuitively ap- tion has been assumed by many theorists to
pealing notion, it must be said that no com- account for most, or even all, of the adverse
pelling evidence in favour of fluctuating at- effects of anxiety on performance. In spite
tentional capacity is as yet available. of its popularity, this assumption has never
The Yerkes-Dodson Law 177

been properly tested, and thus remains the individual subject's skills and knowl-
somewhat speculative. What needs to be edge. However, it is rather complicated to
done is to find some environmental manipu- interpret the interaction between arousal
lation that will control the incidence of ir- and task difficulty. The basic reason for this
relevant internal processing (e.g. worry, self- is that the task itself provides a source of
concern, self-consciousness). One possibili- arousal. The work of Kahneman and others
ty would be to provide feedback indicative has shown clearly that the level of arousal is
offailure at certain pre-specified points dur- usually higher when subjects are perform-
ing the performance of a task. The expecta- ing a difficult task than when they are carry-
tion is that anxious subjects would contem- ing out a relatively simple task. This appears
plate the implications of their poor perform- to be mainly due to the application of in-
ance over a longer period of time after the creased effort on harder tasks, presumably
feedback was provided than would non- because of the greater attentional demands
anxious subjects, and this would be reflect- which they impose.
ed in a longer performance decrement after A second complicating factor was dis-
each feedback presentation. Furthermore, if cussed in Chap. 6. The amount of anxiety
anxious subjects did, in fact, process the fai- experienced during the performance of a
lure feedback more thoroughly than non- task tends to be greater for difficult rather
anxious subjects, then they would presu- than easy tasks. The reason for this is prob-
mably subsequently remember the details of ably that subjects are more likely to receive
the feedback provided better than non-anx- feedback indicative of failure (e.g. error
ious subjects. making, slow responding) when engaging in
In sum, it is most unlikely that the curvili- a difficult task. Since it is known that failure
near relationship between arousal and per- feedback produces anxiety (especially the
formance can be accounted for in terms of a worry component) in a fairly direct fashion,
single mechanism or process. The beneficial it is easy to see why there is a direct relation-
effects of moderate arousal are primarily at- ship between task difficulty and experi-
tributable to increased attentional selectivi- enced anxiety.
ty, which leads to a greater proportion of the The upshot of the discussion so far is that
available processing resources being allo- we cannot take interactions between arousal
cated to task performance. In addition, the and task difficulty at face value. Subjects
natural speed of processing is enhanced as performing difficult tasks are more aroused
arousal increases above its resting level, and and anxious than subjects performing easy
there may be an increase in total attentional tasks, even in the absence of any arouser
capacity. The detrimental effects of high such as white noise or a stimulant drug. The
arousal largely revolve around a reduced conventional assumption is that subjects
ability to engage in shared or parallel pro- performing easy and hard tasks who are ex-
cessing. High arousal also leads to increased posed to, for example, identical levels of
distractibility to both external and internal white noise are equivalently aroused. This is
informational sources and an impaired abil- probably erroneous, since task-induced
ity to make perceptual discriminations. Fi- arousal increases in line with task difficulty.
nally, but more speculatively, high arousal Indeed, the simplest explanation of the in-
may reduce attentional capacity. teraction between arousal and task diffi-
It is now time to turn to the second as- culty is to argue that the optimal level of
sumption of the Yerkes-Dodson law, name- arousal is actually not lower for difficult
ly that the optimal level of arousal is inverse- than for easy tasks: it merely appears so be-
ly related to task difficulty. We have already cause the actual arousal level is underesti-
argued that task difficulty should be as- mated for difficult tasks.
sessed in terms of the demands on attention, In fact, while there is undoubtedly some
which in turn depend jointly on the task and merit in the argument adumbrated in the
178 Conclusions and Speculations

last paragraph, it is still likely that there are is obviously of limited interest to focus sole-
genuine differential effects of arousal on lyon the issue of whether arousal facilitates
tasks of varying complexity. If, as we have or impairs performance. A more fruitful ap-
assumed, arousal level is inversely related to proach is to attempt to 'decompose' a task
the capacity to engage in parallel process- into its constituent processing operations,
ing, then it follows that the detrimental ef- followed by an assessment of the effects of
fects of arousal should be especially severe arousal on each of the identifiable process-
on tasks requiring parallel or shared pro- ing operations. While few researchers work-
cessing. In general terms, it is obvious that ing on stress and arousal have adopted this
tasks necessitating multiple concurrent pro- strategy, there are various methods avail-
cessing operations would usually be re- able for separating out the various compo-
garded as more difficult or complex than nent processes within a task.
tasks not requiring parallel processing.
Why are simple tasks so well performed
under high levels of arousal? A major rea- 9.2.1 Donders' Subtraction Method
son is that the natural processing rate in-
creases in line with arousal, and the empha- Historically, the first major attack on this
sis in most simple tasks is on speed of per- problem was the subtraction method of
formance (Hockey 1979). On the other Donders (1868), which can be used when
hand, it could very well be argued, high the performance of a task requires a number
arousal leads to increased distractibility, of sequential processing operations. In e's-
and so high arousal ought to disrupt the per- sence, the method is based on the assump-
formance even of simple tasks. A potentially tion that the duration of any single process-
important finding in this connection was ob- ing operation can be assessed by comparing
tained very recently by one of my students the reaction time on a version of the task re-
(Jan Graydon). She used the letter transfor- quiring that processing operation with a sec-
mation task (Hamilton et al. 1977) and pro- ond version of the task differing from the
duced distraction by presenting random let- first only by the deletion of the processing
ters over headphones. The simple versions operation of interest. The claim is that the
of the task (Le. only one or two letters requir- difference in total processing time in the two
ing transformation) were virtually unaffect- conditions is equal to the duration of the
ed by distraction, whereas the more difficult isolated processing operation. It follows
versions (i.e. three- and four-letter prob- that the use of several different versions of
lems) showed a 25 % performance impair- the task should permit the processing time
ment with distraction. While this finding for the complete task to be sub-divided
needs to be replicated, it is certainly intui- among its constituent processes.
tively reasonable that complex tasks involv- While the subtraction method is poten-
ing multiple component processes operat- tially powerful, it cannot always be applied
ing in parallel should be more readily dis- successfully. With many tasks it is simply
rupted by distraction than simple tasks in- not possible to reduce the processing re-
volving a very small number of well-learned quirements in such a way that only one pro-
speeded operations. cessing operation is deleted. In other cases
one processing operation cannot be deleted
from an experimental task without changing
the nature of the other constituent mental
9.2 Stages of Processing operations. Indeed, the most prevalent criti-
cism of the subtraction method is that it of-
ten produces a qualitative change in the na-
If one is interested in the effects of arousal ture of the task, rather than the intended
on the performance of various tasks, then it quantitative change. However, this criticism
Stages of Processing 179
can sometimes be rebutted, especially if a small number of items to be memorized (the
mathematical model of task performance is positive set) was followed by a probe; the
available. If such a model assumes that sub- subject's task was to decide as rapidly as
traction of an operation does not change the possible whether or not the probe corre-
remaining processing operations, then it sponded to any of the items in the positive
will presumably provide a poor fit to the da- set. Four experimental factors were manipu-
ta if the assumption is erroneous. A final dif- lated: stimulus quality, size of the positive
ficulty is that the subtraction method as- set, response type (positive or negative), and
sumes that processing occurs by means of a relative frequency of the two response types.
series of sequential processing operations. Sternberg discovered that the effects of
This assumption is often made even when these various factors on response time ap-
there is no adequate reason for rejecting the peared to be additive, thus suggesting that
notion that some of the processes overlap in all four factors were affecting different pro-
time. cessing stages. More specifically, he con-
cluded that stimulus quality affected the sti-
mulus-encoding stage, size of the positive
9.2.2 S. Sternberg's Additive-Factor set affected the serial comparison stage, re-
Method sponse type affected the binary decision
stage and relative frequency affected the
A more recent strategy for uncovering the stage of translation and response organiza-
processing stages involved in a task is the tion.
additive-factor method proposed by S. While the additive-factor method has
Sternberg (1969), which resembles the sub- been used extensively, it suffers from a num-
traction method more than is usually recog- ber of inherent limitations. It does not indi-
nized. The additive-factor method assumes cate the duration of any stage, it provides no
that task processing comprises a sequence direct indication of the order in which the
of independent processing stages, that each processing stages occur and it also fails to
stage receives an input from the preceding offer appropriate information for the sub-
stage, transforms it and passes it on to the stantive interpretation of any stage. A fur-
next stage and, of crucial importance, that ther problem is that it is entirely possible for
the transformation which occurs at each two experimental factors to affect the same
stage is independent of the duration of any stage and have additive effects on total pro-
previous stage of processing. It is further as- cessing time, or for two factors to affect dif-
sumed that if an experimental manipulation ferent processing stages and to interact;
affects the speed with which a task can be both of these possibilities are inconsistent
performed, then it does so only by altering with the basic logic of the additive factor
the temporal duration of one or more of the method. The assumption that two factors af-
processing stages. Finally, if two different fecting separate stages of processing have
experimental manipulations affect two sep- additive effects is especially dubious if a
arate stages, then they will produce inde- factor affects not only the duration of a
pendent effects on total processing time; in stage but also its output; under those cir-
other words, their effects will be additive. In cumstances, an experimental factor would
contrast, if two experimental factors exert exert indirect effects on one or more stages
an interactive effect on the speed of per- of processing occurring after the stage di-
forming a task, then the implication is that rectly affected by that factor.
they are both affecting some common stage The subtraction method and the additive
of processing. factor method are both used in the develop-
An excellent concrete example of addi- ment of linear sequential stage models; such
tive-factor logic was provided by S. Stern- models refer to what are known in contem-
berg (1969), who used a situation in which a porary jargon as 'bottom-up' or 'data-driv-
180 Conclusions and Speculations

en' processing systems. Models of that type 'D'. The general strategy is to present the
may be contrasted with those which empha- subjects with progressively greater amounts
size the individual's active control of his mo- of information in the first part of the trial, so
ment-by-moment allocation of resources that progressively less processing is re-
('top-down' or 'resource-driven' models). quired in the second part of the trial. It is as-
Since subjects typically exert a flexible and sumed that this method of assessing the time
continuous control over their processing ac- to perform parts of the problem (Le. the col-
tivities, any methods or theories that do not lection of interval scores) does not alter the
allow for such control must be inadequate. nature of the task. This assumption can be
It is probable that the processing system can tested and does appear to hold for the ana-
be either largely 'data-driven' or 'resource- logical reasoning task.
driven', depending on the precise nature of The second stage in a componential anal-
task demands. Some theorists (e.g. Neisser ysis is the formulation and testing of a com-
1976) have assumed quite plausibly that ponent model expressed in mathematical
processing typically represents an amalgam terms. The fundamental unit in such a mod-
of bottom-up and top-down components. el is the component, which is "an elemen-
tary information process that operates upon
internal representations of objects or sym-
9.2.3 R. Sternberg's Componential bols" (p. 65). It should be noted that the in-
Method terval scores obtained from the breakdown
of the task into sub-tasks provide valuable
Fairly recently a new approach to the prob- information when testing the model, but do
lem of teasing out the stages of processing not necessarily correspond to distinct stages
within a task was proposed by R. Sternberg of processing. However, the model specifies
(1977). While his method of componential the interval or intervals within which each
analysis shares some of the inadequacies of componential process occurs. Componen-
the subtraction and additive factor methods tial models are expressed in information-
(e.g. a bottom-up processing system is as- processing terms and can usually be repre-
sumed), it is more sophisticated than either sented by flow charts which describe in de-
of those methods. In essence, the compon- tail the processes involved in task solution.
ential method involves three distinct stages of The third stage of a componential analy-
analysis, each of which is described below. sis is used if the model appears to be suc-
The first stage in a componential analysis cessful in accounting for the data. Parame-
is to form what are called 'interval scores' ters are estimated for the individual compo-
from the breakdown of a task into a number nent processes postulated by the model, and
of sub-tasks. Consider as an example a true- these are then correlated with a factor-ana-
false analogical reasoning task of the form lysed battery of reference ability tests (e.g.,
A:B::C :D. Under normal circumstances, verbal or spatial ability measures). The ar-
subjects are given all parts of the problem gument is that this procedure is of value in
together, and time to solve the problem is distinguishing between component pro-
measured. However, it is also possible to cesses that are of general importance and
give the subject the 'A' term initially and to those that are highly task specific. Broadly
see how long he takes to process that term speaking, only components of the former
before asking to see the rest of the problem. type should correlate highly and meaning-
In that way, we would be able to sub-divide fully with the appropriate reference abili-
the total time into two stages: time to pro- ties. As Sternberg so pertinently pointed
cess 'A', and time to process 'B::C:D' and out, information-processing theorists have
work out the answer. The method can be ex- usually failed to provide any evidence that
tended so that we present 'A:B' initially, fol- the processes which they postulate have any
lowed by 'C:D', or 'A:B::C', followed by general significance.
Stages of Processing 181

One of the advantages of the componen- memory of the alphabet sequence, (2) the
tial analysis method over the subtraction transformation time and (3) the speed of
and additive-factor methods is that its ap- storage-updating operations.
proach to identification of a task's compo- Much valuable information was gained
nent stages seems less likely to produce un- by this technique of obtaining time mea-
wanted alterations in either the nature or the sures of each of the 12 constituent sub-tasks
sequence of the underlying processes. How- involved in solving a four-letter problem. In
ever, there may be many tasks that cannot particular, it transpired that in spite of the
readily be broken down into sub-tasks in the overall slowing effect of noise, each letter
way required for a componential analysis. was actually transformed fast in noise. This
is exactly the kind of finding that would not
be expected on the basis of the limited infor-
9.2.4 Arousal and Processing Stages mation provided by solution time. The pri-
mary locus of the slowing down observed in
It is unfortunate that very few attempts have noise was the task component concerned
been made to investigate the effects of with storage and updating operations.
arousal on performance in terms of any of One of the earliest attempts to use multi-
the three methods just described. One ex- ple behavioural measures to elucidate the
ception is some recent work by Hockey (un- effects of arousal on different stages of pro-
published) using an approach resembling in cessing was made by Broadbent (1971). He
some ways the method of componential assumed that there were at least three suc-
analysis. The task used was the letter trans- cessive, independent information-process-
formation task, in which the subject is re- ing stages: perceptual encoding, a transla-
quired to transform each letter in a sequence tion process and response selection and ex-
by counting through the alphabet according ecution. Some indication of the effects of
to a fixed rule such as 'add 4' (e.g. arousal on these various stages was avail-
'HeOU + 4', for which the answer is able from work carried out on the five-
'LGSY'). The number of letters requiring choice serial reaction task, from which three
transformation varies between one and measures have typically been obtained: av-
four, and response is not permitted until all erage response rate, number of errors and
of the letters have been transformed. number of unusually slow responses
One of the findings with this task (Hamil- ('gaps'). The first point of interest is that if
ton et ai., 1977) is that intense noise slows only the most obvious performance measure
down performance on the four-letter ver- were available (i.e. average response rate), it
sion. Although this is a potentially interest- would be concluded that noise has no effect
ing finding, it would be desirable to ascer- at all on performance. In fact, noise typical-
tain more precisely the locus of the noise ef- ly increases the number of errors, and this
fect. This was done (Hockey unpublished) differential pattern of effects on work rate
by having the subject control the length of and error rate constitutes important evi-
presentation of each letter by pressing a but- dence.
ton; the processing time for each letter was In his interpretation of these noise data,
inferred from the time between successive Broadbent (1971) argued that, since errors
button presses. In addition, the subject was occurred independently of average re-
required to perform the transformation out sponse rate and gaps, the factors producing
loud, and the length of the resulting speech errors were independent of the factors con-
phase was timed by computer. As a conse- trolling speed of response selection and eX-
quence of this wealth of data collection, it ecution. He concluded that errors were due
was possible to measure the duration of to interference with perceptual analysis, and
three separate processing components for thus the performance impairment under
each letter: (1) speed of access to long-term noise was due to momentary diversions of
182 Conclusions and Speculations

attention. In contrast, sleep loss has more a satisfactory level. In addition, extra effort
widespread effects on performance of the and resources may be allocated to the task
five-choice serial reaction task, reducing the simply because subjects who are, for exam-
rate of work and increasing the number of ple, sleep-deprived are well aware that their
gaps and errors. This led Broadbent to su- ability to process information is reduced by
spect that sleep loss affected the stage of re- tiredness.
sponse selection and execution as well as The distinction between a relatively pas-
perceptual selection and processing. sive arousal system and an active, cognitive
It is clear that enhanced understanding of control system that monitors the operations
the effects of arousal on performance has of the first arousal system is similar to that
been achieved by the strategy of using var- proposed by Broadbent (1971) between a
ious methods to examine the stage-by-stage lower mechanism and an upper mechanism.
processing involved in several complex Broadbent suggested that activity in the low-
tasks. In general terms, an increase in the er mechanism was increased by noise and
number of measures of performance can be amphetamine and decreased by sleepless-
expected to produce a commensurate in- ness and chlorpromazine. In contrast, in-
crease in our understanding of what arousal volvement of the upper mechanism is en-
is doing to the processing system. However, hanced by short tasks and introversion and
as we have already pointed out, this entire is greater in the afternoon; it is decreased by
approach is based on the rather unlikely as- long tasks, extraversion, alcohol and morn-
sumption that all tasks are processed in a se- ing performance. While Broadbent has not
quence of non-overlapping processing done so, it seems reasonable to include in-
stages. centive as a factor primarily affecting the
upper mechanism.
What evidence did Broadbent (1971) pro-
vide for his proposed distinction? Perhaps
9.3 Two Arousal Systems the single most important piece of evidence
is the fact that interactive effects on per-
formance are more frequently obtained
While detailed analysis of the effects of when two arousers primarily affect the same
arousal on the component stages of task per- arousal mechanism than when one affects
formance is certainly of value, it may well the lower mechanism and the other the up-
prove necessary to recognize that arousal per mechanism. Some of the other argu-
can have two very different kinds of conse- ments adduced by Broadbent are discussed
quences for information processing. In the in Chap. 4.
first place, there is a relatively passive effect Of course, there are other possible expla-
on performance which is due to the appro- nations. One tempting hypothesis is that
priateness or otherwise of the induced each arouser has two kinds of effects on per-
arousal state for the task that needs to be formance: (1) a general arousing effect and
performed. In the second place, there is a (2) specific or idiosyncratic effects. For ex-
more active and effortful response to the ample, while intense white noise increases
naturally occurring effects of arousal on arousal, some of its effects may be due to the
performance; as a first approximation, the fact that it is an auditory stimulus or that it is
more detrimental are the apparent effects of a source of distraction. This hypothesis is
raised or lowered arousal on performance, superficially parsimonious, in that only one
the greater will be the compensatory active, arousal mechanism is proposed; however,
effortful response. This compensatory sys- an excessive number of idiosyncratic effects
tem is assumed to operate when the highly may need to be postulated to account for the
aroused or de-aroused subject perceives data. Nevertheless, it is undoubtedly true
that his level of performance is falling below that at least some arousers do have idiosyn-
Two Arousal Systems 183
cratic effects, and any satisfactory theory dence for the two arousal systems proposed
must take account of this fact. here has been obtained from work on sleep
We have argued that there are two arousal deprivation. As we saw in Chap. 7, one of
systems. One of the most striking findings the main effects of sleep deprivation is to
supporting such a theoretical position is produce an overall decrease in the level of
that manipulations designed to produce arousal. However, this reduction in arousal
substantial increases or decreases in the le- is typically found only when the sleep-de-
vel of arousal typically have rather modest prived person is sitting passively or engaged
(or even non-existent) effects on perform- in performing a relatively unstimulating
ance, especially if tasks of relatively short task. If sleep-deprived subjects are giveri a
duration are used. For example, until com- stimulating or stressful task, the sleep depri-
paratively recently it was believed that the vation is often associated with enhanced
loss of one or two nights' sleep did not im- arousal. For example, Malmo and Surwillo
pair performance on most tasks. In similar (1960) found raised autonomic activity in
fashion, the typical effects of intense noise sleep-deprived subjects when they were re-
on performance are much less marked than quired to remain upright and alert in order
intuition would suggest. The apparently to avoid a painful forehead bum. Such find-
great flexibility with which the human pro- ings clearly suggest that the 'natural' conse-
cessing system copes with very low or very quence of sleep deprivation is lowered
high levels of arousal is rather perplexing arousal, but this arousal reduction can be
for advocates of a uni-dimensional arousal eliminated or even reversed by compensato-
theory; from our present perspective, these ry effort if motivation is sufficiently high.
results simply attest to the involvement of One of the most striking pieces of evi-
the active compensatory system. In other dence in this connection was obtained by
words, arousal usually does not exert a di- Wilkinson (1962a), in a study in which
rect effect on behaviour; rather, its effects sleep-deprived and non-deprived subjects
are indirect and are mediated by a central had to perform a test of addition for 20 min.
control system. Those sleep-deprived subjects who did the
It is often rather difficult to distinguish task fastest and most accurately were the
satisfactorily between the passive arousal ones who showed the greatest rise in the
state and the compensatory effort response. electromyogram (EMG) or muscle tension
One of the reasons for this is that passively during the performance of the task. The sim-
induced arousal and active compensatory plest interpretation of these data is that mus-
processes both increase the level of arousal cle tension in this case provides a measure
as indexed by conventional physiological of compensatory effort. As Wilkinson
measures. However, it seems reasonable to (1962a) concluded, "Sleep deprivation may
assume that the arousal state induced by, for cause inefficiency even in subjects who
example, white noise or caffeine will be rela- maintain performance if their raised EMG
tively general and undifferentiated, whereas reflects greater effort or energy expendi-
the arousal state produced as a by-product ture; this may be the cost of maintaining
of active processing of the task will usually normal levels of arousal and performance in
be fairly specific. An example of the kinds the face of the depressing influence of sleep
of specific arousal patterns that can occur in deprivation per se" (p. 570).
response to varying task demands was men- In similar fashion, it may be reasonable to
tioned earlier in the chapter: heart rate ac- assume that individual differences in arous-
celerates when a task involves active manip- al level while performing a non-stressful
ulation of information, but decelerates task reflect at least approximately variations
when a task calls for attention to an external in expended effort. Frankenhaeuser (1975)
source of information. has found a fairly consistent positive rela-
Some of the clearest physiological evi- tionship between good performance at a
184 Conclusions and Speculations

task and high levels of excretion of adrena- ion, the typical 10% variation in perform-
line and noradrenaline. She obtained this ance occurring during the course of the wak-
relationship across tasks as variegated as vi- ing day is often completely eleminated by
gilance, arithmetic, choice reaction time and knowledge of results (see Chap. 7). At the
verbal learning, and summarized her find- very least, such findings indicate that people
ings in the following way: "Among normal, exposed to arousing or de-arousing condi-
healthy individuals those who have relative- tions can cope with the resultant stress in a
ly higher catecholamine excretion levels more active and flexible manner than has
tend to perform better in terms of speed, ac- usually been assumed.
curacy, and endurance than those who have One of the more straightforward predic-
lower levels. This relationship is particular- tions from the theoretical framework pro-
ly marked in the case of adrenaline excre- posed here is that a reasonable level of per-
tion, but seems to hold also for noradrena- formance can be achieved in at least two dif-
line" (pp. 83-84). At a more theoretical le- ferent ways: (1) near-optimal arousal with
vel, adrenaline output in these studies may little need for compensatory effort or
reflect active processing effort rather than a (2) sub- or supra-optimal arousal combined
more global arousal state. with much compensatory effort (i.e. at high
In sum, there is at least some physiologi- 'cost' to the system). A relatively ignored im-
cal support for the two proposed arousal plication of the original Yerkes-Dodson law
systems. What about the behavioural evi- is that any given level of performance (other
dence? One of the major predictions follow- than the very best) can be achieved at two
ing from the theoretical position espoused different levels of arousal. Thus it is wrong
here is that the behavioural consequences of (if tempting) to conclude from the existence
manipulations designed to increase or de- of a non-significant effect of arousal on task
crease the level of arousal will frequently be performance that arousal has no effect at all
much smaller than would be expected on on internal processing. On the contrary,
the basis of a uni-dimensional arousal the- comparable performance efficiency on the
ory. The most striking results have been ob- part of control and highly aroused subjects
tained in work on into version-extraversion. is often achieved at greater 'subjective cost'
Several rigorous manipulations of arousal to the more aroused subjects (i.e. reduced
level have been found to alter the perform- processing effectiveness). This is especially
ance of extraverts to a great extent but to true when cognitively demanding tasks are
have minimal effects on introverts' perform- used.
ance. This lack of correspondence between How do we know that high-arousal sub-
arousal and performance levels in introverts jects achieve performance levels compar-
is embarrassing to a simple arousal theory, able to less aroused subjects at greater cost
but is easily explained in terms of a greater to themselves? If we assume that high-
willingness by introverts than by extraverts arousal subjects invest more of their avail-
to utilize the compensatory effort system. able resources in task performance, then
The basic notion that the natural effects they have less spare processing capacity
of arousal manipulations on performance than less aroused subjects. As a conse-
can be greatly reduced by compensatory ef- quence, subjects in a state of high arousal
fort is also supported by much of the work are less able to handle any additional pro-
using knowledge of results as an incentive. cessing requirements (e.g. rejecting informa-
In an early study, Wilkinson (l96Ia) dis- tion contained in extra-task stimulation or
covered that performance on the five-choice processing a second task at the same time as
serial reaction task among sleep-deprived the first). There is overwhelming evidence
subjects could be dramatically improved up that high arousal reduces the ability to cope
to non-sleep-deprived levels simply by pro- with a concurrent secondary task (cf. Easter-
viding knowledge of results. In similar fash- brook 1959), in spite of the fact that primary
Two Arousal Systems 185
task performance rarely suffers. There is al- nipulations that affect only one of the two
so accumulating evidence (e.g. Dornic 1977, arousal systems. The most obvious candi-
Howarth 1969, Morgenstern et al. 1974, date for a manipulation that affects the
Pallak et al. 1975) that the ability to resist at compensatory cognitive control system but
least some kinds of environmental distrac- not the passive arousal system is incentive
tion is noticeably reduced in states of high of relatively small magnitude. Such a ma-
arousal. nipulation changes the effective task de-
While we have identified two separate mands and leads the cognitive control sys-
arousal systems, it is important to note that tem to re-allocate resources without affect-
they are often interdependent in their func- ing activity in the passive arousal system. A
tioning. Each arousal system has definite ef- manipulation that affects the passive arous-
fects on the other arousal system. When al system but has minimal impact on the
arousal in the passive arousal system is non- cognitive control system is non-contingent
optimal for the task in hand, there will typi- electric shock, i.e. shock administered irre-
cally be enhanced activity of the compensa- spective of performance level.
tory system. The compensatory system re- Our preferred solution to the complexi-
sponds to task demands and attempts to ties of the research literature is to move from
maintain task performance at a satisfactory a unitary concept of arousal to a theoretical
level. If the attempt to do so is unsuccessful position that postulates two arousal sys-
(as, for example, when high incentives lead tems. An alternative way of accounting for
to the setting of unrealistic goals), then this the complexity and apparent inconsisten-
produces arousal in the passive arousal sys- cies in the data would be to abandon the no-
tem. tion of arousal altogether and to move to-
Broadbent (1971) emphasized the notion wards separate theories of anxiety, incen-
that different arousers affect one or the tive, noise, time of day, sleeplessness and so
other arousal system. It has been one of the on.
major themes of this book that it is prefer- Such fractionation certainly has some ap-
able to claim only that any arouser affects peal when one looks at the data at a relative-
one arousal system relatively more than the ly fine-grain level. Consider for example
other. Perhaps the clearest example con- one of the most robust findings in the entire
cerns incentive. The primary effect of incen- literature on arousal and performance,
tive is on the cognitive control system which namely that high arousal enhances long-
is responsive to task demands; however, it term retention. While that finding seems to
seems clear that high levels of incentive typi- support a unitary arousal concept, detailed
cally also increase activity in the passive examination of the evidence points in the
arousal system. opposite direction. The available evidence
A second example concerns the effects of suggests that long-term retention is better in
sleeplessness on arousal. The primary effect the afternoon than in the morning because
of sleeplessness is to reduce activity in the high arousal leads to deeper or more seman-
passive arousal system; however, the fact tic processing. On the other hand, intense
that it is rather difficult to demonstrate ad- white noise enhances long-term retention in
verse effects of one night's loss of sleep on spite of the fact that the arousal produced
most performance tasks indicates that sleep- by noise leads to shallower or less semantic
lessness indirectly involves the compensato- processing. Incentive seems to improve
ry cognitive control system. long-term retention by producing increased
One of the dangers associated with the elaboration of processing without any dis-
postulation of two interdependent arousal cernible effect on the depth of processing.
systems is that it is difficult to specify their In contrast, anxiety reduces the elaboration
separate contributions to performance. of processing rather than the depth of pro-
Ideally one should be able to identify ma- cessing; furthermore, the arousal produced
186 Conclusions and Speculations
by anxiety reduces long-term retention rath- other words, there is sufficient similarity in
er than enhances it. the behavioural effects of different arousers
In sum, there is a complete schism be- to warrant continued adherence to an arous-
tween the effects of various arousers at the al model. In addition, the ways in which
molar and molecular levels in studies of different arousers interact can frequent-
long-term retention. Unfortunately it is un~ ly be predicted in a fairly precise way on
clear whether these performance differ- the basis of an extremely simple arousal
ences are due to each arouser affecting dif- model.
ferent internal mechanisms or whether they Why is it that two arousers will usually in-
are more simply explicable in terms of vary- teract in a predictable fashion in determin-
ing task demands. ing performance of a particular task, where-
There are other examples of arousers af- as the same two arousers applied separately
fecting memorial functioning in rather dif- to two similar tasks will often produce very
ferent ways. While there are some excep- different patterns of behaviour? The sim-
tions, it has usually been found that intro- plest answer is that there are, in fact, one or
version and anxiety both impair the efficien- two arousal systems, but these systems are
cy of retrieval. In contrast, noise seems to greatly affected by apparently trivial task
improve retrieval efficiency rather than to variations. This conclusion is supported by
impair it, and incentive nearly always has no the fact that the effects of any single arouser
effect on retrieval. On the other hand, at- on task performance can be greatly affected
tempts to reduce arousal by means ofrelax- by small alterations in task parameters. The
ation instructions have usually facilitated implication is that our understanding of the
retrieval. On the basis of such evidence it effects of any single arouser on task per-
would be extremely difficult to claim that formance will be increased only when we
there is any consistent effect of arousal on can identify more precisely the ways in
retrieval. which tasks affect processing activities. It is
Since it is the rule rather than the excep- the interactions between qualitatively dif-
tion to discover that arousers have different ferent arousal states and task demands that
influences on behaviour at a fine-grain level determine specific processing strategies; as
of analysis, the entire notion that the effects yet, the nature of those interactions is poorly
of arousers are mediated by one or two in- understood.
ternal arousal systems loses some of its ap- What does the future hold? Prophecy is
peal. However, it is of crucial importance always a hazardous business, but it is prob-
that any two arousers used together typical- able that there will be an increasing aware-
ly interact with each other in determining ness among cognitive psychologists that
performance. It is rather difficult to explain there are important bi-directional influ-
the presence of interaction rather than addi- ences between arousal and information-pro-
tivity if each arouser were affecting a sepa- cessing operations and that the attempt to
rate system or mechanism. Furthermore, decouple cognition from other systems (e.g.
most (if not all) arousers appear to affect at- motivational, emotional) is fundamentally
tentional mechanisms in similar ways; more ill-judged. If, as the author naturally hopes,
specifically, high arousal produced in sever- these predictions are proved correct, then
al different ways leads to greater attentional this book has dealt with an embryo rather
selectivity and increased distractibility. In than an empty shell.
References

Adam EE (1972) An analysis of changes in per- memory. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 14:575-589
formance quality with operant conditioning Bahrick HP (1954) Incidental learning under two
procedures. J Appl PsychoI56:480-486 incentive conditions. J Exp Psychol47: 170--172
Agnew N, Agnew M (1963) Drive level effects on Bahrick HP, Fitts PM, Rankin RE (1952) Effects
tasks of narrow and broad attention. Q J Exp of incentives upon reactions to peripheral stim-
Psychol 15 :58--62 uli. J Exp Psychol44:400-406
Akerstedt T (1977) Inversion of the sleep wakeful- Bakan P (1959) Extraversion-introversion and im-
ness pattern: Effects on circadian variations in provement in an auditory vigilance task. Br J
psychophysiological activation. Ergonomics PsychoI50:325-332
20:459-474 Bakan P, Belton JA, Toth JC (1963) Extraversion-
Allen GJ (1970) Effect of three conditions of ad- introversion and decrement in an auditory vig-
ministration on "trait" and "state" measures of ilance task. In: Buckner DN, McGrath JJ (eds)
anxiety. J Consult Clin Psychol 34:355-359 Vigilance: A symposium. McGraw-Hill, Lon-
Allport DA, Antonis B, Reynolds P (1972) On the don
division of attention: A disproof of the single Barton K, Cattell RB (1974) Changes in psycho-
channel hypothesis. Q J Exp Psychol logical state measures and time of day. Psycho I
24:225-235 Rep 35:219-222
Amelang M, Wendt W, Frondt H (1977) Zum Ein- Bass B, Ninias P (1974) The effects of verbal rein-
fluB von Extraversion/Introversion auf Kon- forcement upon the WAIS performance of nor-
sidierungsprozesse beim Behalten verbalen mal adults. J Clin PsychoI30:170--172
Materials. Z Exp Angew Psychol23 :525-545 Baumeister AA, Kistler D (1975) Facilitation of
Antos SJ (1979) Processing facilitation in a lexical retention by white noise. J Exp Child Psychol
decision task. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept) 20:13-21
5:527-545 Bavelas J, Lee ES (1978) Effects of goal level on
Atkinson RC, Holmgren JE, Juola JF (1969) Pro- performance: A trade-off of quantity and qual-
cessing time as influenced by the number of ity. Can J PsychoI32:219-240
elements in a visual display. Percept Psycho- Beatty J, Wagoner BL (1978) Pupillometric signs
phys 6:321-336 of brain activation vary with level of cognitive
Atkinson RC, Shiffrin RM (1968) Human memo- processing. Science 199:1216-1218
ry: A proposed system and its control pro- Bell PA (1978) Effects of noise and heat stress on
cesses. In: Spence KW, Spence JT (eds) The primary and subsidiary task performance.
psychology of learning and motivation: Ad- Hum Factors 20:749-752
vances in research and theory, vol 2. Academic Benton AL (1936) Influence of incentives upon
Press, London intelligence test scores of school children. J
Atkinson RC, Wickens TD (1971) Human memo- Genet PsychoI49:494-497
ry and the concept of reinforcement. In: Glaser Bergstrom B, Gillberg M, Amberg P (1973) Ef-
R (ed) The nature of reinforcement. Academic fects of sleep loss and stress upon radar watch-
Press, London ing. J Appl PsychoI58:158-162
Bacon SJ (1974) Arousal and the range of cue uti- Berlyne DE (1960) Conflict, arousal, and curiosi-
lization. J Exp PsycholI02:81-87 ty. McGraw-Hill, London
Baddeley AD, Hatter JE, Scott D, Snashall A Berlyne DE, Borsa DM, Craw MA, Gelman RS,
(1970) Memory and time of day. Q J Exp Mandell EE (1965) Effects of stimulus com-
Psycho I 22:605-609 plexity and induced arousal on paired-asso-
Baddeley AD, Hitch G (1974) Working memory. ciate learning. J Verb Learn Verb Behav
In: Bower GH (ed) The psychology oflearning 4:291-299
and motivation, vol 8. Academic Press, Lon- Berlyne DE, Borsa DM, Hamacher JH, Koenig
don IDV (1966) Paired-associate learning and the
Baddeley AD, Thomson N, Buchanan M (1975) timing of arousal. J Exp Psychol 72:1--6
Word length and the structure of short-term Berlyne DE, Carey ST (1968) Incidental learning
188 References

and the timing of arousal. Psychon Sci Broadbent DE (1953) Noise, paced performance,
13:103-104 and vigilance tasks. Br J PsychoI44:295-303
Berlyne DE, Lewis JL (1963) Effects of height- Broadbent DE (1957) Effects of noises of high
ened arousal on human exploratory behaviour. and low frequency on behaviour. Ergonomics
Can J Psycho I 17 :398-41 0 1:21-29
Bindra D (1969) The interrelated mechanisms of Broadbent DE (1958) Perception and communi-
reinforcement and motivation, and the nature cation. Pergamon, London
of their influence on response. In: Arnold WJ, Broadbent DE (1971) Decision and stress. Aca-
Levine D (eds) Nebraska symposium on moti- demic Press, London
vation, vol 17. University of Nebraska Press, Broadbent DE (1978) The current state of noise
Lincoln research: Reply to Poulton. Psychol Bull
Bindra D (1974) A motivational view of learning, 85:1052-1067
performance, and behaviour modification. Psy- Broadbent DE, Gregory M (1965) Effects of noise
chol Rev 81 :199-213 and of signal rate upon vigilance analysed by
Bindra D, Palfai T (1967) Nature of positive and means of decision theory. Hum Factors
negative incentive: Motivational effects on 7:155-162
general activity. J Comp Physiol Psychol Broadhurst PL (1957) Emotionality and the
63:288-297 Yerkes-Dodson law. J Exp PsychoI54:345-352
Bjerner B (1949) Alpha depression and lowered Broadhurst PL (1959) The interaction of task dif-
pulse rate during delayed actions in a serial ficulty and motivation: The Yerkes-Dodson
reaction test: A study in sleep deprivation. Acta law revived. Acta Psychol (Amst) 16:321-338
Physiol Scand 19:1-93 Brower PE, Mueller JH (1978) Serial position ef-
Blake MJF (1967a) Time of day effects on per- fects in immediate and final free recall as a
formance in a range of tasks. Psychon Sci function of test anxiety and sex. Bull Psychon
9:349-350 Soc 12:61-63
Blake MJF (1967b) Relationship between circadi- Bruning JL, Cap age JE, Kozuh GF, Young PF,
an rhythm of body tempe:ature and introver- Young WE (1968) Socially induced drive and
sion-extraversion. Nature 215 :896-897 range of cue utilization. J Pers Soc Psychol
Blake MJF (1971) Temperament and time of day. 9:242-244
In: Colquhoun WP (ed) Biological rhythms Calder BJ, Staw BM (1975) Self-perception of in-
and human behaviour. Academic Press, Lon- trinsic and extrinsic motivation. J Pers Soc Psy-
don chol 31 :599-605
Blechman EA, Dannemiller EA (1976) Effects on Callaway E, Stone G (1960) Re-evaluating the fo-
performance of perceived control over noxious cus of attention. In: Uhr L, Miller JG (eds)
noise. J Consult Clin PsychoI44:601-607 Drugs and behaviour. Wiley, London
Blowers GH, Ongley GC (1975) The effect of Capretta, PJ, Berkun M (1962) Validity and relia-
knowledge of results upon contingent negative bility of certain measures of psychological
variation in a reaction time situation with a var- stress. Psychol Rep 10:875-878
iable foreperiod. Physiol Psychol 3 :257-260 Carr G (1971) Introversion-extraversion and vig-
Boggs DH, Simon JR (1968) Differential effect of ilance performance. Proc Annu Meet AmPsy-
noise on tasks of varying complexity. J Appl chopathol Assoc 79:379-380
PsychoI52:148-153 Cattell RB, Child D (1975) Motivation and dy-
Bohlin G (1971) Monotonous stimulation, sleep namic structure. Holt, Rinehart & Winston,
onset and habituation of the orienting reaction. London
Electroenceph alogr Clin Neurophysiol Cherry EC (1953) Some experiments on the re-
31 :593-601 cognition of speech with one and two ears. J
Bohlin G (1973) The relationship between arous- Acoust Soc Am 25 :975-979
al level and habituation of the orienting reac- Clements PR, Hafer MD, Vermillion ME (1976)
tion. Physiol Psycholl :308-312 Psychometric, diurnal, and electrophysiologi-
Boor M, Harmon J (1971) Comment on Weiner's cal correlates of activation. J Pers Soc Psychol
(1966) study: role of success and failure in the 33:387-394
learning of easy and complex tasks. J Pers Soc Cohen JS, Telegdy GA, Laroche JP, Getz Y
Psychol 18:256-257 (1973) Cue utilization as a function of mone-
Brebner J, Cooper C (1974) The effect of a low tary incentive and learning efficiency. Bull Psy-
rate of regular signals upon the reaction times chon Soc I :452-454
of introverts and extraverts. J Res Pers Cohen S, Lezak A (1977) Noise and inattentive-
8:263-276 ness to social cues. Environ Behav 9 :559-572
Brebner J, Flavel R (1978) The effects of catch-tri- Collins WE (1977) Some effects of sleep depriva-
als on speed and accuracy among introverts tion on tracking performance in static and dy-
and extraverts in a simple RT task. Br J Psychol namic environments. J Appl Psychol
69:9-16 62:567-573
References 189
Colquhoun WP (1960) Temperament, inspection and incentives upon attention in short-term
efficiency, and time of day. Ergonomics 3 :377- memory. Br J PsychoI66:61-68
378 Deci EL (1971) Effects of externally mediated re-
Colquhoun WP, Blake MJF, Edwards RS (1968) wards on intrinsic motivation. J Pers Soc Psy-
Experimental studies of shift-work I: A com- choI18:105-115
parison of 'rotating' and 'stabilized' 4-hour Deci EL (1975) Intrinsic motivation. Plenum,
shift systems. Ergonomics 11 :437-453 London
Condry J (1977) Enemies of exploration: Self-in- Deffenbacher JL (1978) Worry, emotionality, and
itiated versus other initiated learning. J Pers task-generated interference in test anxiety: An
Soc Psychol 35 :459-477 empirical test of attentional theory. J Educ Psy-
Condry J, Chambers J (1978) Intrinsic motivation choI70:248-254
and the process of learning. In: Lepper MR, Demetrescu M, Demetrescu M, Iosif G (1965)
Greene D (eds) The hidden costs of reward: The tonic control of cortical responsiveness by
New perspectives on the psychology of human inhibitory and facilitatory diffuse influences.
motivation. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, IN Electroenceph alogr Clin Neurophysiol
Corballis MC (1969) Patterns of rehearsal in im- 18:1-24
mediate memory. Br J PsychoI60:41-49 Deutsch JA, Deutsch D (1963) Attention: Some
Corcoran DWJ (1962a) Noise and loss of sleep. theoretical considerations. Psychol Rev
Q J Exp PsychoI14:178-182 70:80-90
Corcoran DWJ (1962b) Individual differences in Deutsch JA, Deutsch D (1967) Comments on "se-
performance after loss of sleep. PhD thesis, lective attention: perception or response?" Q J
University of Cambridge Exp Psychol 19 :362-363
Corcoran DWJ (1965) Personality and the invert- Dey MK, Kaur G (1965) Facilitation of perform-
ed-U relation. Br J Psychol 56:267-273 ance by experimentally induced ego motiva-
Corcoran DWJ (1972) Studies of individual dif- tion. J Gen Psychol 73 :237-247
ferences at the Applied Psychology Unit. In: Dickstein LS, Ayers J (1973) Effect of an incen-
Nebylitsyn VD, Gray JA (eds) Biological bases tive upon intelligence test performance. Psy-
of individual behaviour. Academic Press, Lon- chol Rep 33:127-130
don Dixon PN, Cameron AE (1976) Personality and
Craig MJ, Humphreys MS, Rocklin T, Revelle W motivational factors on an intentional-inciden-
(1979) ImpUlsivity, neuroticism, and caffeine: tal learning task. Psychol Rep 39:1315-1320
Do they have additive effects on arousal? J Res Doctor RM, Altman F (1969) Worry and emotion-
Pers 13 :404-419 ality as components of test anxiety, with repli-
Craik FIM, Lockhart RS (1972) Levels of process- cation and further data. Psychol Rep
ing: A framework for memory research. J Verb 24:563-568
Learn Verb Behav II :671-684 Donders FC (1868) Die Schnelligkeit psychischer
Cummings LL, Schwab DP, Rosen M (1971) Per- Processe. Arch Anat Physio1657-681
formance and knowledge of results as determi- Dornbush RL (1965) Motivation and positional
nants of goal setting. J Appl Psychol cues in incidental learning. Percept Mot Skills
55:526-530 20:709-714
Cutting JE (1975) Orienting tasks affect recall Dornic S (1975) Some studies on the retention of
performance more than subjective impressions order information. In: Rabbitt PMA, Dornic S
of ability to recall. Psychol Rep 36:155-158 (eds) Attention and performance, vol V. Aca-
Cuvo AJ (1974) Incentive level influence on overt demic Press, London
rehearsal and free recall as a function of age. J Dornic S (1977) Mental load, effort, and individu-
Exp Child PsychoI18:167-181 al differences. Rep Dept Psychol Univ Stock-
Daee S, Wilding JM (1977) Effects of high inten- holm, no 509
sity white noise on short-term memory for posi- Duffy E (1962) Activation and behaviour. Wiley,
tion in a list and sequence. Br J Psychol68 :335- London
349 Duncan J (1979) Divided attention: The whole is
Davenport WG (1968) Auditory vigilance: The ef- more than the sum of its parts. J Exp Psychol
fects of costs and values on signals. Aust J Psy- [Hum Perceptj5:216-228
choI20:213-218 Duncan J (1980) The demonstration of capacity
Davenport WG (1969) Vibrotactile vigilance: The limitation. Cogn PsychoI12:75-96
effects of costs and values on signals. Percept Easterbrook JA (1959) The effect of emotion on
Psychophys 5 :25-28 cue utilization and the organization of behav-
Davies DR, Hockey GRJ (1966) The effects of iour. Psychol Rev 66:183-201
noise and doubling the signal frequency on in- Elliott R (1969) Tonic heart rate: Experiments on
dividual differences in visual vigilance per- the effects of collative variables lead to a hypo-
formance. Br J PsychoI57:381-389 thesis about about its motivational signifi-
Davies DR, Jones DM (1975) The effects of noise cance. J Pers Soc PsychoI12:211-228
190 References

Elliott R (1970) Simple reaction time: Effects as- sonality. In: Eysenck HI (ed) A model for per-
sociated with age, preparatory interval, incen- sonality. Springer, London
tive-shift, and mode of presentation. 1 Exp Eysenck MW, Eysenck HI (1980) Mischel and the
Child Psycho I 9:86-107 concept of personality. Br 1 Psychol
Elliott R. Bankart CP, Flaherty B (1976) Relation 71 :191-294
of heart-rate deceleration and simple reaction Eysenck MW, Eysenck MC (l979a) Processing
time. Percept Mot Skills 42:1075-1084 depth, elaboration of encoding, memory stores,
Endler NS, Magnusson D, Ekehammar B, Okada and expended processing capacity. 1 Exp Psy-
M (1976) The multidimensionality of state and chol [Hum Learn] 5:472-484
trait anxiety. Scand 1 Psycho I 17:81-96 Eysenck MW, Eysenck MC (l979b) Memory
Eysenck HI (1944) The effect of incentives on scanning, introversion-extraversion, and levels
neurotics, and the variability of neurotics as of processing. 1 Res Pers 13 :305-315
compared with normals. Br 1 Med Psychol Eysenck MW, Eysenck MC (1980) Effects of
20:100-103 monetary incentives on rehearsal and on cued
Eysenck HI (1967) The biological basis of person- recall. Bull Psychon Soc 15:245-247
ality. Thomas, Springfield Eysenck MW, Folkard S (1980) Personality, time
Eysenck HI, Eysenck SBG (1969) The structure of day, and caffeine: Some theoretical and con-
and measurement of personality. Routledge & ceptual problems in Revelle et al. 1 Exp Psy-
Kegan Paul, London chol [Gen] I 09 :32-41
Eysenck HI, Levey A (1972) Conditioning, intro- Eysenck SBG, Eysenck HI (1977) The place of
version-extraversion and the strength of the impulsiveness in a dimensional system of per-
nervous system. In: Neblitsyn VD, Gray lA sonality description. Br 1 Soc Clin Psychol
(eds) Biological bases of individual behaviour. 16:57-68
Academic Press, London Farley FH (1966) Individual differences in solu-
Eysenck HI, Warwick KM (1964) The effects of tion time in error-free problem solving. Br 1 Soc
drive level on a multiple-choice reaction task. Clin Psychol 5 :306-309
In: Eysenck HI (ed) Experiments in motiva- Farr lL (1976) Task characteristics, reward con-
tion. Pergamon, Oxford tingency, and intrinsic motivation. Organ Be-
Eysenck MW (I 974a) Extraversion, arousal, and hav Hum Perform 16:294-307
retrieval from semantic memory. 1 Pers 42 :319- Feldman MP (1964) Response reversal perform-
331 ance as a function of drive level. In: Eysenck
Eysenck MW (l974b) Individual differences in HI (ed) Experiments in motivation. Pergamon,
speed of retrieval from semantic memory. 1 Res Oxford, pp 108-117
Pers 8 :307-323 Ferguson HH (1937) Incentives and an intelli-
Eysenck MW (I 975a) Arousal and speed of recall. gence test. Aust 1 Psychol Philos 15:39-53
Br 1 Soc Clin PsychoI14:269-277 Finkelman 1M, Glass DC (1970) Reappraisal of
Eysenck MW (I 975b) Extraversion, arousal, and the relationship between noise and human per-
speed of retrieval from semantic memory. 1 formance by means of a subsidiary task mea-
Pers 43 :390-40 I sure. J Appl PsychoI54:211-213
Eysenck MW (l975c) Effects of noise, activation Fischler I, Goodman GO (1978) Latency of asso-
level, and response dominance on retrieval ciation activation in memory. 1 Exp Psychol
from semantic memory. 1 Exp Psychol [Hum [Hum Percept] 4:455-470
Learn] I :143-148 Folkard S (I 979a) Changes in immediate memory
Eysenck MW (1976a) Arousal, learning, and strategy under induced muscle tension and
memory. Psychol Bull 83 :389-404 with time of day. Q 1 Exp Psychol31 :621-633
Eysenck MW (1976b) Extraversion, verbal learn- Folkard S (l979b) Time of day and level of pro-
ing, and memory. Psychol Bull 83 :75-90 cessing. Mem Cognit 7:247-252
Eysenck MW (1977) Human memory: Theory, re- Folkard S (1980) A note on 'Time of day effects in
search and individual differences. Pergamon, school children's immediate and delayed recall
Oxford of meaningful material' - the influence of the
Eysenck MW (1978) Verbal remembering. In: importance of the information tested. Br 1 Psy·
Foss BM (ed) Psychology survey, vol I. Allen & chol 71 :95-97
Unwin, London Folkard S, Greeman AL (1974) Salience, induced
Eysenck MW (l979a) Anxiety, learning, and muscle tension, and the ability to ignore irrele-
memory: A reconceptualization. 1 Res Pers vant information. Q 1 Exp Psycho126:360-367
13 :363-385 Folkard S, Monk TH (1978) Time of day in imme-
Eysenck MW (1979b) Depth, elaboration, and diate and delayed memory. In: Gruneberg
distinctiveness. In: Cermak L, Craik FIM (eds) MM, Morris PE, Sykes RN (eds) Practical
Levels of processing in human memory. Erl- aspects of memory. Academic Press, London
baum, Hillsdale Folkard S, Monk TH (1980) Circadian rhythms in
Eysenck MW (1981) Learning, memory and per- human memory. Br 1 Psychol 71 :295-307
References 191
Folkard S, Monk TH, Bradbury R, Rosenthall J cognitive test performance. J Psychol 96:111-
(1977) Time of day effects in school children's 122
immediate and delayed recall of meaningful Gordon VM, Berlyne DE (1954) Drive level and
material. Br J Psychol 68 :45-50 flexibility in paired associate nonsense syllable
Forster PM, Grierson AT (1978) Noise and atten- learning. Q J Exp PsychoI6:181-185
tional selectivity: A reproducible phenomen- Gottsdanker R (1975) The attaining and main-
on? Br J Psychol 69 :489-498 taining of preparation. In: Rabbitt PM A, Dor-
Fowler CJH, Wilding J (1979) Differential effects nic S (eds) Attention and performance, vol V.
of noise and incentives on learning. Br J Psy- Academic Press, London
choI70:149-153 Gray JA (1970) The psychophysiological basis of
Frankenhaeuser M (1975) Sympathetic-adren- introversion-extraversion. Behav Res Ther
omedullary activity, behaviour and the psycho- 8:249-266
social environment. In: Venables PH, Christie Gray JA (1972) The psychophysiological nature
MJ (eds) Research in psychophysiology. Wiley, of introversion-extraversion: A modification of
London Eysenck's theory. In: Nebylitsyn VD, Gray JA
Fuller AR (1978) Personality and paired-asso- (eds) Biological basis of individual behaviour.
ciate learning. Int J Psychol 13: 123-128 Academic Press, London
Furneaux WD (1961) Intellectual abilities and Gray JA (1973) Causal theories and how to test
problem solving behaviour. In: Eysenck HJ them. In: Royce JR (ed) Multivariate analysis of
(ed) Handbook of abnormal psychology. Basic psychological theory. Academic Press, London
Books, London Gray JA, Wedderburn AA (1960) Grouping
Gale A (1973) The psychobiology of individual strategies with simultaneous stimuli. Q J Exp
differences: Studies of extraversion and the PsychoI12:180-184
EEG. In: Kline P (ed) New approaches to psy- Green DM, Swets JA (1966) Signal detection the-
chological measurement. Wiley, London ory and psychophysics. Wiley, London
Ganzer VJ (1968) Effects of audience presence Gupta BS (1976) Extraversion and reinforcement
and test anxiety on learning and retention in a in verbal operant conditioning. Br J Psychol
serial learning situation. J Pers Soc Psychol 67:47-52
8 :194-199 Gupta BS, Nagpal M (1978) Impulsivity/socia-
Gaudry E (1977) Studies of the effects of experi- bility and reinforcement in verbal operant con-
mentally induced experiences of success or fai- ditioning. Br J Psychol 69 :203-206
lure. In: Spielberger CD, Sarason IG (eds) Hamilton P, Hockey GR, Quinn JG (1972) Infor-
Stress and anxiety, vol 4. Halsted, London mation selection, arousal, and memory. Br J
Gieseking CF, Williams HL, Lubin A (1957) The PsychoI63:181-189
effect of sleep deprivation upon information Hamilton P, Hockey GRJ, Rejman M (1977) The
learning. Am Psycho112:406 place of the concept of activation in human in-
Gillespie CR, Eysenck MW (1980) Effects of in- formation processing theory: An integrative
troversion-extraversion on continuous recogni- approach. In: Dornic S (ed) Attention and per-
tion memory. Bull Psychon Soc 15 :233-235 formance, vol VI. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
Glanzer M, Cunitz AR (1966) Two storage me- Hamilton P, Wilkinson RT, Edwards RS (1972) A
chanisms in free recall. J Verb Learn Verb Be- study offour days partial sleep deprivation. In:
hav 5:351-360 Colquhoun WP (ed) Aspects of human effi-
Glanzmann P, Laux L (1978) The effects of trait ciency: Diurnal rhythm and loss of sleep. Eng-
anxiety and two kinds of stressors on state anx- lish Universities Press, London
iety and performance. In: Spielberger CD, Sar- Hamilton V (1975) Socialization anxiety and in-
ason IG (eds) Stress and anxiety, vol 5. Wiley, formation processing: A capacity model of
London anxiety-induced performance deficits. In: Sar-
Glaubman H. Orbach I, Aviram 0, Frieder I, ason IG, Spielberger CD (eds) Stress and anx-
Frieman M, Pelled 0, Glaubman R (1978) iety, vol 2. Academic Press, London
REM deprivation and divergent thinking. Psy- Hamilton V (1978) The cognitive analysis of per-
chophysiology 15 :75-79 sonality related to information-processing defi-
Glover CB, Cravens RW (1974) Trait anxiety, cits with stress and anxiety. Paper presented at
stress, and learning: A test of Saltz's hypothe- the British Psychological Society meeting, Lon-
sis. J Res Pers 8 :243-253 don
Glucksberg S (1962) The influence of strength of Harkins S, Geen RG (1975) Discriminability and
drive on functional fixedness and perceptual criterion differences between extraverts and in-
recognition. J Exp Psychol 63 :36-41 troverts during vigilance. J Res Pers 9:335-340
Glucksberg S (1964) Problem solving: Response Harley WF (l965a) The effect of monetary incen-
competition and the influence of drive. Psychol tive in paired associate learning using the abso-
Rep 15 :939-942 lute method. Psychon Sci 3 :141-142
Goh DS, Farley FH (1977) Personality effects on Harley WF (I 965b) The effect of monetary incen-
192 References

tive in paired associate learning using a differ- Wiley, Chichester


ential method. Psychon Sci 2:377-378 Hockey GRJ, Colquhoun WP (1972) Diurnal var-
Harley WF (1968) Delay of incentive cues in iation in human performance. A review. In:
paired-associate learning and its effect on or- Colquhoun WP (ed) Aspects of human effi-
ganizing responses. J Verb Learn Verb Behav ciency - Diurnal rhythm and loss of sleep. Eng-
7:924-929 lish Universities Press, London
Hartley LR (1973) Effect of prior noise and prior Hockey GRJ, Hamilton P (1970) Arousal and in-
performance on serial reaction. J Exp Psychol formation selection in short-term memory. Na-
101 :255-261 ture 226 :866-867
Hartley LR (1981) Noise, attentional selectivity, Hodges WF (1968) Effects of ego threat and
serial reactions and the need for experimental threat of pain on state anxiety. J Pers Soc Psy-
power. Br J Psychol 72:101-107 choI8:364-372
Hartley LR, Carpenter A (1974) Comparison of Hodges WF, Durham RL (1972) Anxiety, ability,
performance with headphone and free-field and digit span performance. J Pers Soc Psycho I
noise. J Exp PsychoI103:377-380 24:401-406
Hasher L, Zacks RT (1979) Automatic and effort- Hormann H, Osterkamp U (1966) Ober den Ein-
ful processes in memory. J Exp Psychol [GenJ fluB von kontinuierlichem Liirm auf die Orga-
108 :356-388 nisation von Gediichtnisinhalten. Z Exp An-
Haveman JE, Farley FH (1969) Arousal and re- gew Psychol13:31-38
tention in paired associate, serial and free Home JA, Brass CG, Pettitt AN (1980) Circadian
learning. Wisconsin Univ Center for Cogn performance differences between morning and
Learn, Tech Rep 91 evening types. Ergonomics 23 :29-36
Hebb DO (1961) Distinctive features of learning Houston BK (1969) Noise, task difficulty, and
in the higher animal. In: Delafresnaye JF (ed) Stroop colour-word performance. J Exp Psy-
Brain mechanism and learning. Oxford Uni- chol 82 :403-404
versity Press, London Houston BK (1977) Dispositional anxiety and the
Heider F (1958) The psychology of interpersonal effectiveness of cognitive coping strategies in
relations. Wiley, London stressful laboratory and classroom situations.
Hirst W, Spelke ES, Reaves CC, Caharack G, In: Spielberger CD, Sarason IG (eds) Stress
Neisser U (1980) Dividing attention without al- and anxiety, vol 4. Halsted, London
ternation or automaticity. J Exp Psychol [GenJ Houston BK, Jones TM (1967) Distraction and
109:98-117 Stroop colour-word performance. J Exp Psy-
Hitch GJ (1978) The role of short-term working choI74:54-56
memory in mental arithmetic. Cogn Psychol Howarth E (1969) Personality differences in seri-
10:302-323 al learning under distraction. Percept Mot
Hitch GJ (1980) Developing the concept of work- Skills 28 :379-382
ing memory. In: Claxton G (ed) Cognitive psy- Howarth E, Eysenck HJ (1968) Extraversion,
chology: New directions. Routledge & Kegan arousal, and paired-associate recall. J Exp Res
Paul, London Pers 3:114-116
Hitch GJ, Baddeley AD (1976) Verbal reasoning Humphreys MS, Revelle W, Simon L, Gilliland K
and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol28 :603- (1980) Individual differences in diurnal
621 rhythms and mUltiple activation states: A reply
Hockey GRJ (l970a) Effect of loud noise on at- to M. W. Eysenck and Folkard. J Exp Psychol
tentional selectivity. Q J Exp Psychol22 :28-36 [GenJ 109:42-48
Hockey GRJ (1970b) Signal probability and spa- Imam A (1974) Extraversion and incidental learn-
tiallocation as possible bases for increased se- ing. Pak J Psychol 7 :41-54
lectivity in noise. Q J Exp PsychoI22:37-42 Jacoby LL (1974) The role of mental contiguity in
Hockey GRJ (1970c) Changes in attention alloca- memory: Registration and retrieval effects. J
tion in a multi-component task under loss of Verb Learn Verb Behav 13:483-496
sleep. Br J Psychol61 :473-480 James W (1890) Principles of psychology, vol I.
Hockey R (1973) Changes in information-selec- Holt, New York
tion patterns in multi-source monitoring as a Jensen A (1964) Individual differences in learn-
function of induced arousal shifts. J Exp Psy- ing: Interference factor. Cooperative Research
chollOl :35-42 Project No1867, Office of Education, US De-
Hockey R (1978) Attentional selectivity and the partment of Health, Education, and Welfare,
problems of replication: A reply to Forster & Washington
Grierson. Br J Psychol 69 :499-503 Jensen AR (1966) The measurement of reactive
Hockey R (1979) Stress and the cognitive compo- inhibition in humans. J Gen PsychoI75:85-94
nents of skilled performance. In: Hamilton V, Jerison HJ (1957) Performance on a simple vigil-
Warburton DM (eds) Human stress and cogni- ance task in noise and quiet. J Acoust Soc Am
tion: An information processing approach. 29:1163-1165
References 193
Jerison HJ (1959) Effects of noise on human per- Kjellberg A (1977b) Sleep deprivation and some
formance. J Appl.PsychoI43:96-101 aspects of performance: 2. Lapses and other at-
Jinks JL, Fulker DW (1970) A comparison of the tentional effects. Waking Sleeping 1:145-148
biometrical genetical, MAVA, and classical ap- Kjellberg A (1977c) Sleep deprivation and some
proaches to the analysis of human behaviour. aspects of performance: 3. Motivation, com-
Psychol Bull 73 :311-349 ment and conclusions. Waking Sleeping 1:149-
Johnson R, Thomson C (1962) Incidental and in- 153
tentional learning under three conditions of Klein KE, Herrmann R, Kuklinski P, Wegmann
motivation. Am J Psychol 75 :284-288 H-M (1977) Circadian performance rhythms:
Johnston WA (1978) The intrusiveness offamiliar Experimental studies in air operations. In:
nontarget information. Mem Cognit 6:38-42 Mackie RR(ed) Vigilance: Theory, operational
Johnston WA, Heinz SP (1978) Flexibility and ca- performance, and physiological correlates. Ple-
pacity demands of attention. J Exp Psychol num, London
[GenlI07:420-435 Kleinsmith U, Kaplan S (1963) Paired associate
Johnston WA, Heinz SP (1979) Depth of nontar- learning as a function of arousal and interpo-
get processing in an attention task. J Exp Psy- lated interval. J Exp Psycho165:190-193
chol 5 :168-175 Kleitman N (1963) Sleep and wakefulness. Chica-
Kahneman D (1970) Remarks on attention con- go University Press, Chicago
trol. Acta Psycho I 33:118-131 Knox WJ, Grippaldi R (1970) High levels of state
Kahneman D (1973) Attention and effort. Pren- or trait anxiety and performance on selected
tice Hall, Englewood Cliffs verbal WAIS subtests. Psychol Rep 27 :375-379
Kahneman D, Beatty J, Pollack I (1967) Perceptu- Kohn H (1954) Effects of variations of intensity
al deficit during a mental task. Science of experimentally induced stress situations up-
157:218-219 on certain aspects of perception and perform-
Kahneman D, Peavler WS (1969) Incentive ef- ance. J Genet Psychol 85 :289-304
fects and pupillary changes in association Kruglanski AW, Alon S, Lewis T (1972) Retro-
learning. J Exp Psychol 79:312-318 spective misattribution and task enjoyment. J
Kahnemann D, Tursky B, Shapiro D, Crider A Exp Soc Psychol 8 :493-50 I
(1969) Pupillary, heart rate and skin resistance Krugman AD (1958) A comparative study of the
changes during a mental task. J Exp Psychol effect of induced failure, induced success and a
79:164-167 neutral task upon the retentive processes of
Katz L (1969) Motivation and retrieval in short- anxiety and normal subjects. Diss Abstr 18 :662
term free recall. J Exp Psycho179:580-581 Krupski A, Raskin D, Bakan P(1971) Physiologi-
Kausler DH, Laughlin PR, Trapp EP (1963) Ef- cal and personality correlates of commission
fects of incentive-set on relevant and irrelevant errors in an auditory vigilance task. Psychophy-
(incidental) learning in children. Child Dev siology 8 :304-311
34:195-199 LaBerge D (1973) Attention and the measurement
Kausler DH, Trapp EP (1962) Effects of incen- of perceptual learning. Mem Cognit 1:268-276
tive-set and task variables on relevant and ir- Lacey JI (1967) Somatic response patterning and
relevant learning in serial verbal learning. Psy- stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In:
chol Rep 10:451-457 Appley MH, Trumbull R (eds) Psychological
Kausler DH, Trapp EP, Brewer CL (1959) Inten- stress. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York
tional and incidental learning under high and Lansing RW, Schwartz E, Lindsley D (1959)
low emotional drive levels. J Exp Psychol Reaction time and EEG activation under alert-
58:452-455 ed and non-alerted conditions. J Exp Psychol
Kazdin AE, Bootzin RR (1972) The token econ- 58:1-7
omy: An evaluative review. J Appl Behav Anal Laverty SG (1958) Sodium amytal and extraver-
1:5 sion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 21 :50-54
Keister ME, McLaughlin RJ (1972) Vigilance per- Lawson EA (1966) Decisions concerning the re-
formance related to extraversion-introversion jected channel. Q J Exp Psychol 18 :260-265
and caffeine. J Exp Res Pers 6:5-11 Leonard JA (1959) 5-choice serial reaction appar-
Kernoff P, Weiner B, Morrison M (1966) Affect atus. Applied Psychology Research Unit Re-
and short-term retention. Psychon Sci 4:75-76 port No 326/59, Medical Research Council,
Kerr B (1973) Processing demands during mental Cambridge
operations. Mem Cognit 1:401-412 Lepper MR, Greene D, Nisbett RE (1973) Under-
Kjellberg A (1975) Effects of sleep deprivation on mining children's intrinsic interest with extrin-
performance of a problem-solving task. Psy- sic rewards: A test of the "overjustification" hy-
chol Rep 37:479-485 pothesis. J Pers Soc PsychoI28:129-137
Kjellberg A (l977a) Sleep deprivation and some Levine JM (1966) The effects of values and costs
aspects of performance: I. Problems of arousal in the detection and identification of signals in
changes. Waking Sleeping 1:139-143 auditory vigilance. Hum Factors 8 :525-537
194 References

Lewis J (1970) Semantic processing of unat- lessness: Effects of escape and avoidance be-
tended messages using dichotic listening. J Exp haviour of (a) the CS-UCS contingency and
PsychoI85:225-228 (b) the independence of voluntary responding.
Liebert RM, Morris LW (1967) Cognitive and In: Campbell BA, Church RM (eds) Punish-
emotional components of test anxiety: A dis- ment and aversive behaviour. Appleton-Centu-
tinction and some initial data. Psychol Rep ry-Crofts, London
20:975-978 Maller JB, Zubin J (1932) The effect of motiva-
Lisper HO, Kjellberg A )1972) Effects of 24-hour tion upon intelligence test scores. J Genet Psy-
sleep deprivation on rate of decrement in a 10- chol41 :136-151
minute auditory reaction time task. J Exp Psy- Malmo RB, Surwillo WW (1960) Sleep depriva-
choI96:287-290 tion: Changes in performance and physiologi-
Locke EA (1968) Torward a theory of task moti- cal indicants of activation. Psychol Monogr
vation and incentives. Organiz Behav Hum Per- 47:1-24
form 3:157-189 Mandler G (1975) Mind and emotion. Wiley,
Locke EA, Bryan JF, Kendall LM (1968) Goals London
and intentions as mediators of the effects of Mandler G, Sarason SB (1952) A study of anxiety
monetary incentives on behaviour. J Appl Psy- and learning. J Abnorm Soc Psychol
choI52:104-121 47:166-173
Locke EA, Bryan JF (1968) Goal-setting as a de- Markowitz A (1969) Influence of the repression-
terminant of the effect of knowledge of score sensitization dimension, affect value, and ego
on performance. Am J Psychol 81 :398-406 threat on incidental learning. J Pers Soc Psy-
Lockhart RS, Craik FIM, Jacoby LL (1976) chol II :374-380
Depth of processing, recognition and recall. Martin M (1980) Attention to words in different
In: Brown J (ed) Recall and recognition. Wiley, modalities: Four-channel presentation with
London physical and semantic selection. Acta Psychol
Loeb M, Schmidt EA (1963) A comparison of the (Amst) 44:99-115
effects of different kinds of information in Martuza VR, Kallstrom DW (1974) Validity of the
maintaining efficiency on an auditory monitor- State-Trait Anxiety Inventory in an academic
ing task. Ergonomics 6:75-81 setting. Psychol Rep 35:363-366
Loftus GR (1972) Eye fixations and recognition McDougall W (1923) Outline of psychology.
memory for pictures. Cognit Psycho I Scribner, New York
3:525-551 McGraw KO (1978) The detrimental effects ofre-
Loftus GR, Wickens TD (1970) Effect of incen- ward on performance: A literature review and a
tive on storage and retrieval processes. J Exp prediction model. In: Lepper MR, Greene D
PsychoI85:141-147 (eds) The hidden costs of reward: New per-
Logan GD (1979) On the use of a concurrent spectives on the psychology of human motiva-
memory load to measure attention and auto- tion. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
maticity. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept) McGraw KO, McCullers JC (1979) Evidence of a
5:189-207 detrimental effect of extrinsic incentives on
Lopes LL (1971) Preference for stimuli as a func- breaking a mental set. J Exp Soc Psychol
tion of impact and activation level. master's 15:285-294
thesis, California State University, Long Beach McLaughlin RJ (1968) Retention in paried-asso-
Lucaccini LF, Freedy A, Lyman J (1968) Motiva- ciate learning related to extraversion and neu-
tional factors in vigilance: The effects of in- roticism. Psychon Sci 13 :333-334
structions on performance in a complex vigil- McLaughlin RJ, Kary SK (1972) Amnesic effects
ance task. Percept Mot Skills 26:783-786 in free recall with introverts and extraverts.
Lucas JD (1952) The interactive effects of anxie- Psychon Sci 29 :250-252
ty, failure, and intraserial duplication. Am J McLean PD (1969) Induced arousal and time of
Psychol 65 :59-66 recall as determinants of paired-associate re-
Macfarlane DA (1930) The role of kinaesthesis in call. Br J PsychoI60:57-62
maze learning. Univ Calif Publ Psychol McLeod P (1977) A dual task response modality
4:277-305 effect: Support for multiprocessor models of
Mackay C, Cox T, Burrows G, Lazzerini T (1978) attention. Q J Exp PsychoI29:651-667
An inventory for the measurement of self-re- McLeod P (1978) Does probe RT measure central
ported stress and arousal. Br J Soc Clin Psy- processing demand? Q J Exp Psychol30 :83-89
chol 17 :283-284 McNamara HJ, Fisch RI (1964) Effect of high
Mackworth NH (1950) Researches in the mea- and low motivation on two aspects of attention.
surement of human performance. Med Res Percept Mot Skills 19:571-578
Counc Spec Rep Ser 268 Meyer WU (1970) Selbstverantwortlichkeit und
Maier SF, Seligman MEP, Solomon RL (1969) Leistungsmotivation. PhD thesis, Ruhr Uni-
Pavlovian fear conditioning and learned help- versity, Bochum
References 195
Millar K (1979) Word recognition in loud noise. cesses in memory. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 5 :288-
Acta Psychol (Amst) 43:225-237 294
Miller DJ, Mueller JH, Goldstein AG, Potter TL Mueller JH, Bailis KL, Goldstein AG (1978)
(1978) Depth of processing and test anxiety in Depth of processing and anxiety in facial re-
landscape recognition. Bull Psychon Soc cognition. Paper given at the Midwestern Psy-
II :341-343 chological Association, Chicago
Miller ME, Dost JA (1964) Stimulus vividness Mueller JH, Carlomusto M, Marler M (1977) Re-
and anxiety level in intentional-incidental call as a function of method of presentation
learning. Psychol Rep 14:819-825 and individual differences in test anxiety. Bull
Miller NE (1948) Studies of fear as an acquirable Psychon Soc 10:447-450
drive: 1. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction Mueller JH, Overcast TD (1976) Free recall as a
as reinforcement in learning of new responses. function of test anxiety, concreteness, and in-
J Exp Psycho I 38:89-106 structions. Bull Psychon Soc 8:194-196
Mirsky AF, Rosvold HE (1960) The use of psy- Mullin J, Corcoran DWJ (1977) Interaction of
choactive drugs as a neuropsychological tool in task amplitude with circadian variation in audi-
studies of attention in man. In: Uhr L, Miller tory vigilance performance. Ergonomics
JG (eds) Drugs and behaviour. Wiley, New 20:193-200
York Nachreiner F (1977) Experiments on the validity
Moray N (1959) Attention in dichotic listening: of vigilance experiments. In: Mackie RR (ed)
Affective cues and the influence of instruc- Vigilance: Theory, operational performance,
tions. Q J Exp Psychol 11 :56-60 and physiological correlates. Plenum, London
Moray N, Fitter M, Orstry D, Favreau D, Nagy V Niiiitiinen R (1973) The inverted-U relationship
(1976) Attention to pure tones. Q J Exp Psychol between activation and performance: A critical
28:271-283 review. In: Kornblum S (ed) Attention and per-
Morgenstern FS, Hodgson RJ, Law J (1974) Work formance, vol IV. Academic Press, London
efficiency and personality: A comparison of in- Navon D, Gopher D (1979) On the economy of
troverted and extraverted subjects exposed to the human-processing system. Psychol Rev
conditions of distraction and distortion of sti- 86:214-255
mulus in a learning task. Ergonomics Neely JH (1977) Semantic priming and retrieval
17:211-220 from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless
Morris LW, Brown NR, Halbert BL (1977) Ef- spreading activation and limited-capacity at-
fects of symbolic modeling on the arousal of tention. J Exp Psychol [Gen] 106:226-254
cognitive and affective components of anxiety Neill WT (1979) Switching attention within and
in preschool children. In: Spielberger CD, Sar- between categories: Evidence for intracategory
as on IG (eds) Stress and anxiety, vol 4. Halst- inhibition. Mem Cognit 7 :283-290
ed,London Neisser U (1976) Cognition and reality. Freeman,
Morris LW, Liebert RM (1970) Relationship of San Francisco
cognitive and emotional components of test Nelson TO (1976) Reinforcement and human
anxiety to physiological arousal and academic memory. In: Estes WK (ed) Handbook of
performance. J Consult Clin Psychol learning and cognitive processes, vol 3. Wiley,
35:332-337 London
Morris LW, Liebert RM (1973) Effects of nega- Nichols R (1959) The effect of ego involvement
tive feedback, threat of shock, and level of trait and success experience on intelligence test re-
anxiety on the arousal of two components of sults. J Consult Psychol 23 :92
anxiety. J Counseling PsychoI20:321-326 Nicholson IN, Gray JA (1972) Peak shift, behavi-
Mowbray GH, Rhoades MV (1959) On the reduc- oural contrast and stimulus generalization as
tion of choice-reaction times with practice. Q J related to personality and development in
Exp Psycholll :16-23 children. Br J PsychoI63:47-68
Mueller JH (1976) Anxiety and cue utilization in Nicholson WM (1958) The influence of anxiety
human learning and memory. In: Zuckerman upon learning: Interference or drive incre-
M, Spielberger CD (eds) Emotions and anxie- ment? J Pers 26:303-319
ty: New concepts, methods and applications. Nideffer RM (1976) Test of attentional and inter-
Erlbaum, Potomac personal style. J Pers Soc PsychoI34:394-404
Mueller JH (1977) Test anxiety, input modality, Norman DA (1968) Toward a theory of memory
and levels of organization in free recall. Bull and attention. Psychol Rev 75 :522-536
Psychon Soc 9:67-69 Norman DA, Bobrow DG (1975) On data-limited
Mueller JH (1978) The effects of individual dif- and resource-limited processes. Cognit Psy-
ferences in test anxiety and type of orienting chol 7 :44-64
task on levels of organization in free recall. J Norman DA, Bobrow DG (1976) On the analysis
Res Pers 12:100-116 of performance. operating characteristics. Psy-
Mueller JH (1979) Anxiety and encoding pro- chol Rev 83:508-510
196 References

Norton R (1970) The effects of acute sleep depri- Psychol Rev 82:116-149
vation on selective attention. Br J Psychol Pritchard RD, Curts MI (1973) The influence of
61:157-162 goal setting and financial incentives on task
Nottelman ED, Hill KT (1977) Test anxiety and performance. Organiz Behav Hum Perform
off-task behaviour in evaluative situations. 10:175-183
Child Dev 48 :225-231 Ray WJ, Katahn M, Snyder CR (1971) Effects of
Notz WW (1975) Work motivation and the nega- test anxiety on acquisition, retention, and gen-
tive effects of extrinsic rewards: A review with eralization of a complex verbal task in a class-
implications for theory and practice. Am Psy- room situation. J Pers Soc PsychoI20:147-154
choI30:884-891 Rebert CS (1972) The effect of reaction time feed-
O'Malley JJ, Poplawsky A (1971) Noise induced back on reaction time and contingent negative
arousal and breadth of attention. Percept Mot variation. Psychophysiology 9 :334-339
Skills 33:887-890 Reeves FB, Bergum BO (1972) Perceptual nar-
Osgood GE (1953) Method and theory in experi- rowing as a function of peripheral cue relev-
mental psychology. Oxford University Press, ance. Percept Mot Skills 35:719-724
New York Reitman JS (1974) Without surreptitious rehears-
Otto DA (1971) The effect of modifying response al, information in short-term memory decays.
and performance feedback parameters on the J Verb Learn Verb Behav 13 :365-377
CNV in humans. Paper presented at the Sec- Revelle W, Humphreys MS, Simon L, Gilliland K
ond International CNV Congress, Vancouver (1980) The interactive effect of personality,
Pagano DF, Katahn M (1967) Effects of test anx- time of day and caffeine: A test of the arousal
iety on acquisition and retention of psycho log i- model. J Exp Psychol [Gen] 109:1-31
cal terms and single letter stimulus-response Revelle W, Michaels EJ (1976) The theory of
pairs. J Exp Res Pers 2 :260-267 achievement motivation revisited: The implica-
Pallak MS, Pittman TS, Heller JF, Munson P tions of inertial tendencies. Psychol Rev
(1975) The effect of arousal on Stroop colour- 83:394-404
word task performance. Bull Psychon Soc Richardson FC, O'Neil HF, Whitmore S, Judd
6:248-250 WA (1977) Factor analysis of the Test Anxiety
Patterson J (1974) Stimulus-seeking behaviour as Scale and evidence concerning the components
a function of white noise induced activation le- of test anxiety. J Consult Clin Psychol45 :704-
vel. master's thesis, California State University, 705
Long Beach Rogers JL, Battig WF (1972) Effect of amount of
Polzella DJ (1975) Effects of sleep deprivation on prior free recall learning on paired-associate
short-term recognition memory. J Exp Psychol transfer. J Exp PsychoI92:373-377
[Hum Learn] 104:194-200 Ross M (1975) Salience of reward and intrinsic
Posner MI (1978) Chronometric explorations of motivation. J Pers Soc Psychol 33 :245-254
mind. Erlbaum, Hillsdale Rubin BM, Shantz DW, Smock CD (1962) Per-
Posner MI, Klein R, Summers J, Buggie S (1973) ceptual constriction as a function of incentive
On the selection of signals. Mem Cognit I :2-12 motivation. Percept Mot Skills 15 :90
Posner MI, Snyder CRR (l975a) Facilitation and Russell JA (1979) Affective space is bipolar.
inhibition in the processing of signals. In: Rab- J Pers Soc Psychol37 :345-356
bitt PMA, Dornic S (eds) Attention and per- Russell JA, Mehrabian A (1977) Evidence for a
formance, vol V. Academic Press, London three-factor theory of emotion. J Res Pers
Posner MI, Snyder CRR (1975b) Attention and II :273-294
cognitive control. In: Solso RL (ed) Informa- Rutenfranz J, Aschoff J, Mann H (1972) The ef-
tion processing and cognition: The Loyola fects of a cumulative sleep deficit, duration of
symposium. Erlbaum, Hillsdale preceding sleep period and body-temperature
Poulton EC (1977) Continuous intense noise on multiple choice reaction time. In: Colqu-
masks auditory feedback and inner speech. houn WP (ed) Aspects of human efficiency.
Psychol Bull 84:977-1001 English Universities Press, London
Poulton EC (1978) A new look at the effects of Saltz E (1970) Manifest anxiety: Have we misread
noise: A rejoinder. Psychol Bull 85:1068-1079 the data? Psychol Rev 77 :568-573
Poulton EC (1979) Composite model for human Saltz E, Hoehn AJ (1957) A test of the Taylor-
performance in continuous noise. Psychol Rev Spence theory of anxiety. J Abnorm Soc Psy-
86:361-375 choI54:114-117
Poulton EC, Edwards RS (1974) Interactions and Sarason IG (1956) Effect of anxiety, motivational
range effects in experiments on pairs of instructions, and failure on serial learning.
stresses: Mild heat and low-frequency noise. J Exp Psychol51 :253-260
J Exp PsycholI02:621-628 Sarason IG (1957a) The effect of associative val-
Pribram KH, McGuinness D (1975) Arousal, acti- ue and differential motivating instructions on
vation, and effort in the control of attention. serial learning. Am J Psychol 70 :620-623
References 197
Sarason IG (1957b) Effect of anxiety and two Spelke ES, Hirst WC, Neisser U (1976) Skills of
kinds of failure on serial learning. J Pers divided attention. Cognition 4 :215-230
25:383-392 Spence JT, Spence KW (1966) The motivational
Sarason IG (1972) Experimental approaches to components of manifest anxiety: Drive and
test anxiety: Attention and the uses of inform a- drive stimuli. In: Spielberger CD (ed) Anxiety
tion. In: Spielberger CD (ed) Anxiety: Current and behaviour. Academic Press, London
trends in theory and research, vol 2. Academic Spence K W (1958) A theory of emotionally based
Press, London drive (D) and its relation to performance in
Sarason IG (1975) Anxiety and self-preoccupa- simple learning situations. Am Psychol13: 131-
tion. In: Sarason IG, Spielberger CD (eds) 141
Stress and anxiety, vol 2. Wiley, London Spence KW (1964) Anxiety (drive) level and per-
Sarason I, Minard J (1962) Test anxiety, experi- formance in eyelid conditioning. Psychol Bull
mental instructions, and the Wechsler Adult 61 :129-139
Intelligence Scale. J Educ Psychol 53 :299-302 Spielberger CD (1966) The effects of anxiety on
Schachter S, Singer JE (1962) Cognitive, social, complex learning and academic achievement.
and physiological determinants of emotional In: Spielberger CD (ed) Anxiety and behav-
state. Psychol Rev 69 :379-399 iour. Academic Press, London
Schneider W, Shiffrin RM (1977) Controlled and Spielberger CD (1972) Anxiety as an emotional
automatic human information processing: I. state. In: Spielberger CD (ed) Anxiety: Current
Detection, search and attention. Psychol Rev trends in theory and research, vol I. Academic
84:1-66 Press, London
Schneller WF, Garske JP (1976) Effects of Spielberger CD, Gonzalez HP, Taylor CJ, Algaze
extraversion and neuroticism on learning B, Anton WD (1978) Examination stress and
and memory: A test of Eysenck's theory of in- test anxiety. In: Spielberger CD, Sarason IG
dividual differences in arousal. J Psychol (eds) Stress and anxiety, vol 5. Halsted, Lon-
94:27-32 don
and memory. J Res Pers 9:217-225 Spielberger CD, Gorsuch R, Lushene R (1970)
Schwartz S (1979) Differential effects of personal- The State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) test
ity on access to various long-term memory manual. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo
codes. J Res Pers 13 :396-403 Alto
Segal SJ, Fusella V (1970) Influence of imaged Spielberger CD, O'Neil HF, Hansen DN (1972)
pictures and sounds on detection of visual and Anxiety, drive theory, and computer-assisted
auditory signals. J Exp Psychol 83 :458-464 learning. In: Maher BA (ed) Progress in experi-
Shanmugan TE, Santhanam MC (1964) Personal- mental personality research, vol 6. Academic
ity differences in serial learning when interfer- Press, London
ence is presented at the marginal visual level. J Stein BS (1978) Depth of processing re-ex-
Indian Acad Appl Psychol I :25-28 amined: The effects of the precision of encod-
Shedletsky R, Endler NS (1974) Anxiety: The ing and test appropriateness. J Verb Learn
state-trait model and the interaction model. .T Verb Behav 17:165-174
Pers 42 :511-527 Steriade M (1970) Ascending control of thalamic
Shields .T (1962) Monozygotic twins brought up and cortical responsiveness. Int Rev Neurobiol
apart and brought up together. Oxford Uni- 12:24-47
versity Press, London Sternberg RJ (1977) Intelligence, information
Shiffrin RM, Schneider W (1977) Controlled and processing, and analogical reasoning. Erl-
automatic human information processing: II. baum, Hillsdale
Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and Sternberg S (1966) High speed scanning in human
a general theory. Psychol Rev 84:127-190 memory. Science 153 :652-654
Silverman RE (1954) Anxiety and the mode of re- Sternberg S (1969) The discovery of processing
sponse. J Abnorm Soc PsychoI49:538-542 stages: Extensions of Donders' method. Acta
Silverman RE, Blitz B (1956) Learning and two Psychol (Amst) 30:276-315
kinds of anxiety. J Abnorm Soc Psychol Sternberg S (1975) Memory scanning. New find-
52:301-303 ings and current controversies. Q J Exp Psy-
Simon HA (1967) Motivational and emotional choI27:1-32
controls of cognition. Psychol Rev 74:29-39 Straughan JH, Dufort WH (1969) Task difficulty,
Smith AP, Broadbent DE (1981) Noise and levels relaxation, and anxiety level during verbal
of processing. Acta Psychol (Amst) 47: 129-142 learning and recall. J Abnorm Psychol
Sostek AJ (1978) Effects of electrodermal lability 74:621-624
and payoff instructions on vigilance perform- Sullivan L (1976) Selective attention and second-
ance. Psychophysiology 15 :561-568 ary message analysis: A reconsideration of
Spearman CE (1927) The abilities of man: Their Broadbent's filter model of selective attention.
nature and measurement. Macmillan, London Q J Exp Psycho I 28:167-178
198 References

Swets JA (1977) Signal detection theory applied Treisman A (1977) Focused attention in the per-
to vigilance. In: Mackie RR (ed) Vigilance: ception and retrieval of multidimensional stirn
Theory, operational performance, and physio- uli. Percept Psychophys 22:1-11
logical correlates. Plenum, London Treisman AM, DaviesA( 1973) Dividedattention to
Tarpy RM, Glucksberg S (1966) Effects of incen- ear and eye. In: Kornblum S (ed) Attention and
tive and incentive-cue position on short-term performance, vol IV. Academic Press, London
retention. Psychon Sci 5:313-314 Treisman AM, Geffen G (1967) Selective atten-
Tecce JJ, Tarnell M (1965) Focal and incidental tion: Perception or response? Q J Exp Psychol
movement time as a function of shock arousal 19:1-17
in humans. J PsychoI59:155-158 Treisman AM, Gelade G (1980) A feature-inte-
Tennyson RD, Wooley FR (1971) Interaction of gration theory of attention. Cognit Psychol
anxiety with performance on two levels of task 12:97-136
difficulty. J Educ PsychoI62:463-467 Treisman AM, Riley JGA (1969) Is selective at-
Terborg JR (1976) The motivational components tention selective perception or selective re-
of goal setting. J Appl Psychol 61 :613-621 sponse? A further test. J Exp PsychoI79:27-34
Terborg JR, Miller HE (1978) Motivation, behav- Treisman AM, Squire R, Green J (1974) Semantic
iour, and performance: A closer examination processing in dichotic listening: A replication.
of goal setting and monetary incentives. J Appl Mem Cognit 2:641-649
Psychol 63 :29-39 Tulving E (1979) Relation between encoding spe-
Thackray RI, Jones KN, Touchstone RM (1974) cificity and levels of processing. In: Cermak
Personality and physiological correlates of per- LS, Craik FIM (eds) Levels of processing in hu-
formance decrement on a monotonous task re- man memory. Erlbaum, Hillsdale
quiring sustained attention. Br J Psychol Tune GS (1966) Errors of commission as a func-
65:351-358 tion of age and temperament in a type of vigi-
Tharp VK (1978) Sleep loss and stages of infor- lance task. Q J Exp PsychoI18:358-361
mation processing. Waking Sleeping 2:29-33 Uehling BS, Sprinkle R (1968) Recall of a serial
Thayer RE (1967) Measurement of activation list as a function of arousal and retention inter-
through self-report. Psychol Rep 20:663-678 val. J Exp Psychol 78:103-106
Thayer RE (1970) Activation states as assessed by Underwood G (1974) Moray vs. the rest: The ef-
verbal report and four psychophysiological fect of extended shadowing practice. Q J Exp
variables. Psychophysiology 7 :86-94 PsychoI26:368-372
Thayer RE (l978a) Toward a psychological the- Voici C, Vranceanu M (1975) Influence of the dy-
ory of multidimensional activation (arousal). namism of nervous processes on certain, indi-
Motiv Emot 2:1-34 vidual memory differences. Rev Roum Sci Soc
Thayer RE (1978b) Factor analytic and reliability Ser PsychoI19:13-20
studies on the Activation-Deactivation Adjec- Von Wright JM, Anderson K, Stenman U (1975)
tive Check List. Psychol Rep 42:747-756 Generalization of conditioned GSRs in dichot-
Thayer RE, Anderson J, Spadone A, White V ic listening. In: Rabbitt PMA, Dornic S (eds)
(1970) Stimulus-seeking behaviour and activa- Attention and performance, vol V. Academic
tion level as a function of white noise. Paper Press, London
presented at the meeting of the Western Psy- Wachtel PL (1968) Anxiety, attention, and coping
chological Association, Los Angeles with threat. J Abnorm Psychol 73:137-143
Thayer RE, Carey D (1974) Spatial stimulus gen- Walker EL (1958) Action decrement and its rela-
eralization as a function of white noise and ac- tion to learning. Psychol Rev 65:129-142
tivation level. J Exp PsycholI02:539-542 Walker RE (1961) The interaction between fai-
Thayer RE, Moore LE (1972) Reported activation lure, manifest anxiety, and task-irrelevant re-
and verbal learning as a function of group size sponses in paired-associate learning. PhD the-
(social facilitation) and anxiety-inducing in- sis, Northwestern University, Evanston
structions. J Soc Psychol 88 :277-287 Walley RE, Weiden TD (1973) Lateral inhibition
Thorndike EL (1913) The psychology oflearning. and cognitive masking: A neuropsychological
Columbia University Press, New York (Educa- theory of attention. Psycho1 Rev 80:284-302
tional psychology, vol 2) Walley RE, Weiden TD (1974) Giving flesh to a
Thornton JW, Powell GD (1974) The effect of "straw man": A reply to Feeney, Pittman, and
contingent shock (punishment) on the perform- Wagner. Psychol Rev 81 :540-542
ance of a paired-associates learning task. J Psy- Wanner E, Shiner S (1976) Measuring transient
choI86:111-114 memory load. J Verb Learn Verb Behav
Treisman AM (1964) Verbal cues, language, and 15:159-167
meaning in selective attention. Am J Psychol Wardlaw K, Kroll NEA (1976) Automatic re-
77:206-219 sponses to shock-associated words in a nonat-
Treisman A (1969) Strategies and models of selec- tended message: A failure to replicate. J Exp
tive attention. Psychol Rev 76:282-299 Psychol [Hum Percept] 2 :357-360
References 199
Warm JS, Epps BD, Ferguson RP (1974) Effects 12:36-40
of knowledge ofresults and signal regularity on Wilkinson RT (1961) Interaction of lack of sleep
vigilance performance. Bull Psychon Soc with knowledge of results, repeated testing and
4:272-274 individual differences. J Exp Psychol
Warm JS, Kanfer FH, Kuwada S, Clark JL (1972) 62:263-271
Motivation in vigilance: Effects of self-evalua- Wilkinson RT (1962) Muscle tension during men-
tion and experimenter controlled feedback. J tal work under sleep deprivation. J Exp Psy-
Exp PsychoI92:123-127 choI64:565-571
Wasserman EA, Weiner B, Houston JP (1968) An- Wilkinson RT (1963) Interaction of noise with
other failure for motivation to enhance trace re- knowledge of results and sleep deprivation. J
trieval. Psychol Rep 22:1007-1008 Exp Psychol 66:332-337
Watkins MJ, Watkins OC, Craik FIM, Mazuryk Wilkinson RT (1964) Effects of up to 60 hours'
G (1973) Effect of nonverbal distraction on sleep deprivation on different types of work.
short-term memory. J Exp Psychol Ergonomics 7:175-186
101 :296-300 Wilkinson RT (1969) Sleep deprivation: Perform-
Waugh NC, Norman DA (1965) Primary memo- ance tests for partial and selective sleep depri-
ry. Psychol Rev 72:89-104 vation. In: Abt LA, Reiss JR (eds) Progress in
Weick KE (1964) Reduction of cognitive disso- clinical psychology. Grune & Stratton, New
nance through task enhancement and effort ex- York
penditure. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 68 :533-539 Wilkinson RT, Colquhoun WP (1968) Interaction
Weiner B (l966a) Role of success and failure in of alcohol with incentive and with sleep depri-
the learning of easy and complex tasks. J Pers vation. J Exp Psychol 76:623-629
Soc Psychol 3 :339-344 Wilkinson RT, EI-Beheri S, Gieseking CC (1972)
Weiner B (l966b) Motivation and memory. Psy- Performance and arousal as a function of in-
chol Monogr 80 centive, information load, and task novelty.
Weiner B (1967) Motivational factors in short- Psychophysiology 9 :589-599
term retention: II. Rehearsal or arousal? Psy- Willett RA (1964) Experimentally induced drive
chol Rep 20:1203-1208 and performance on a five-choice serial reac-
Weiner B (1972) Theories of motivation: From tion task. In: Eysenck HJ (ed) Experiments in
mechanism to cognition. Markham, London motivation. Pergamon, Oxford
Weiner B, Potekan PA (1970) Personality charac- Williams HL, Gieseking CF, Lubin A (1966)
teristics and affective reactions towards exams Some effects of sleep loss on memory. Percept
of superior and failing college students. J Educ Mot Skills 23:1287-1293
Psychol61 :144-151 Williams HL, Kearney OF, Lubin A (1965) Signal
Weiner B, Schneider K (1971) Drive versus cogni- uncertainty and sleep loss. J Exp Psychol
tive theory: A reply to Boor and Harmon. J Pers 69:401-407
Soc Psychol 18 :258-262 Williams HL, Lubin A (1967) Speeded addition
Weiner B, Walker EL (1966) Motivational factors and sleep loss. J Exp PsychoI73:313-317
in short-term retention. J Exp Psychol Williams HL, Lubin A, Goodnow JL (1959) Im-
71 :190-193 paired performance with acute sleep loss. Psy-
Weist RM (1972) The role of rehearsal: Recopy or chol Monogr 73: 1-26
reconstruct. J Verb Learn Verb Behav Willis J, Ehrlick S (1975) Tangible reinforcement
II :440-445 and feedback effects on the I. Q. scores of col-
Welch J (1898) On the measurement of mental ac- lege students. J Clin Psychol 31 :463-464
tivity through muscular activity and the deter- Wilson GD, Tunstall OA, Eysenck HJ (1971) In-
mination of a constant of attention. Am J Psy- dividual differences in tapping performance as
chol I :288-306 a function of time on the task. Percept Mot
Weltman G, Egstrom GH (1966) Perceptual nar- Skills 33 :375-378
rowing in novice divers. Hum Factors Winch WH (1912a) Mental fatigue in day school
8:499-506 children as measured by immediate memory,
Weltman G, Smith JE, Egstrom GH (1971) Per- part I. J. Educ PsychoI3:18-28
ceptual narrowing during simulated pressure- Winch WH (1912b) Mental fatigue in day school
chamber exposure. Hum Factors 13 :99-\07 children as measured by immediate memory,
White K (1968) Anxiety, extraversion-introver- part II. J Educ Psycho I 3 :75-82
sion, and divergent thinking ability. J Creat Be- Wine J (1971) Test anxiety and direction of atten-
hav 2:119-127 tion. Psychol Bull 76:92-104
Wickens DD, Simpson CK (1968) Trace cue posi- Wolk S, DuCette J (1974) Intentional perform-
tion, motivation, and short-term memory. J ance and incidental learning as a function of
Exp Psychol 76:282-285 personality and task dimensions. J Pers Soc
Wilkinson RT (1960) The effect of lack of sleep PsychoI29:90-101
on visual watch keeping. Q J Exp Psycho I Yerkes RM, Dodson JD (1908) The relation of
200 References

strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-forma- Zajonc RB, Brickman P (1969) Expectancy and
tion. J Comp Neurol PsychoI18:459-482 feedback as independent factors in task per-
Zaffy DJ, Bruning JL (1966) Drive and the range formance. J Pers Soc Psycholll :148-156
of cue utilization. J Exp Psychol 71 :382-384
Author Index

AdamEE 83 80, 83, 89, 92, 124, 130, 138, Deffenbacher JL 54,98, 120
AgnewM 65 157-159, 161-163, 167-170, Demetrescu M 50
Agnew N 65 172,176,181,182,185 Dutsch D8, 11,22,27,35-37,
Akerstedt T 139 Broadhurst PL 4, 5 59
Allen GJ 96 Brower PE 100 Deutsch JA 8, 11,22,27,
Allport DA 10,42 Bruning JL 120 35-37,59
Altman F 98 Bryan IF 89 DeyMK 79
Ame1ang M 128 Dickstein LS 76
Antos SI 18 Dixon PN 52, 83
Atkinson RC 19, 22, 28, 31, Calder BJ 86 Doctor RM 98
68-;-72,74, 160 CaJlaway E 65 Dodson JD 4,5,47-49,56,59,
Ayers 1 76 Cameron AE 52, 83 79,90,93, 133, 137, 139, 150,
Capretta PI 109 175
Carey D 62 Donders FC 178
Bacon SI 50, 51 Carey ST 159 Dornbush RL 52, 83
Baddeley AD 22, 28-33, 39, Carpenter A 158 Dornic S 54, 59,60, 110, 120,
44,64,99,101,142,160 Carr G 130, 136 161,167,185
Bahrick HP 52, 83 CatteJl RB 5, 139 DostJA 51
Bakan P 130-132 Chambers J 76 DuCette 1 52, 83
Barton K 139 Cherry EC 9,10 Duffy E 3
Bass B 76 Child D 5 Dufort WH 104, 105
Battig WF 105 Clements PR 139 Duncan 1 40, 42
Baumeister AA 163 Cohen IS 52, 83 Durham RL 100
Bave1as J 83, 88, 89 Cohen S 52,164
Beatty J 58 Collins WE 148
Bell PA 52,165 Colquhoun WP 54,79, Easterbrook lA 1, 3, 49-55,
Benton Al 76 138-140, 144 65,84,120,121,128,129,142,
Bergstrom B 148 Condry J 7fJ,76 152, 164, 166, 169, 175, 176,
Bergum BO 51 Cooper C 136 184
Berkum M 109 Corballis MC 32 Edwards RS 169
Ber1yne DE 95, 112, 142, 156, Corcoran DWJ 79, 91, 127, Egstrom GH 52
159, 160 136,144,148,157,158 Ehrlick S 76
Bindra D 68 Craig MJ 118 Elliott R 78, 173
Bjerner B 151 Craik FIM 71, 116, 143, 160 Endler NS 96
Blake MJF 124, 125, 137, 138, Cravens RW 113 Eysenck HJ 55, 76, 79, 88, 96,
140-145 Cummings LL 89 124-127, 130, 132, 133, 136,
Blechman EA 156 Cunitz AR 31 137
Blitz B 51 Curts MI 89 Eysenck MC 37-39, 71, 128,
Blowers GH 77,78 Cutting JE 72 134, 135
Bobrow DG 33-35, 39, 40, Cuvo AJ 70, 71 Eysenck MW 4,37-39,64,66,
171 71,73,91,95,96,99-101,112,
Boggs DH 52, 165, 168 116, 119, 121, 122, 128, 130,
Bohlin G 147 Daee S 162 132-137, 141 145, 150, 160,
Boor M 107 Dannemiller EA 156 163
Bootzin RR 85 Davenport WG 80 Eys~nck SBG 126,127
Brebner J 136 Davies A 41,42,44
Brickman P 115 Davies DR 52, 83,92,
Broadbent DE 3, 8, 10,27, 35, 130-132, 160, 164 Farley FH 136,159
36, 54-56, 59, 62, 66, 70, 79, Deci EL 84-87 Farr JL 88
202 Author Index

Feldman MP 79, 83 Hirst W 13 Lee ES 83,88, 89


Ferguson HH 76 Hitch GJ 22,28-33,39,44,64, Leonhard JA 157
Finkelman JM 52,165 99,101,160 Lepper MR 86
Fisch RI 51,52,83 Hockey GRJ 53,130-132, Levey A 125,127
Fischler I IS 138, 139, 152, 157, 158, 160, Levine JM 80
Flavel R 136 162,164-167,170-172,178 Lewis J 17,26
Folkard S 65, 137, 142, 143, Hodges WF 96,100,112 Lewis JL 156
145 Hoehn AJ 104 Lezak A 52,164
Forster PM 53, 165 Hormann H 64,162 Liebert RM 96, 98, 113
Fowler CJH 92 Horne JA 140, 141 Lisper HO 151
Frankenhaueser M 168, 183 Houston BK 65, 98 Locke EA 83, 88-90
FulkerDW 96 Howarth E 129, 132, 133, 185 LockhartRS 71,116,143,160
Fuller AR 132 Humphreys MS 5, 145, 146 Loeb M 81
Furneaux WD 136 Loftus GR 70-72
Fusella V 41,42,44 Logan GO 23,24,34
ImamA 128 Lopes LL 62
Lubin A 149
Gale A 124 Jacoby LL 116 Lucaccini LF 80
Ganzer VJ 98 James W 8, 14,28 Lucas JD 112
Garske JP 132 Jensen AR 133,136
Gaudry E 107 Jerison HJ 157
Geen RG 130, 136 Jinks JL 96 Macfarlane DA 68
Geffen G 11,38 Johnson R 52, 83 Mackay C 61
Gelade G 23-27 Johnston WA 35-39,43,44 Mackworth NH 80,81,157
Gieseking CF ISO Jones OM 52,83,92, 160, 164 Maier SF 109
Gillespie CR 135, 136 Jones TM 65 Maller JB 76,83
Glanzer M 31 Malmo RB 147, 183
Glanzmann P 96, 112 Mandler G 56,97,176
Glass DC 52, 165 Kahneman 0 4, 6, 35, 39, 40, Markowitz A 51
Glaubmann H 147 46, 48, 57-60, 63, 108, 111, Martin M 44
Glover CB 113 122,176 Martuza VR 96
Glucksberg S 74,75,82 Kallstrom OW 96 McCullers JC 75,82
Goh OS 136 Kaplan S 141,142,159 McDougall W 67
Goodman GO IS Kary SK 132, 135 McGraw KO 75,81,82
Gopher 0 28, 39-44 Katahn M 119 McGuinness 0 173
Gordon VM 112 Katz L 72 McLaughlin RJ 130-132,135
Gootsdanker R 77 KaurG 79 McLean PO 159
Gray JA 11,91, 117, 125-127, Kausler DH 51,52,83 McLeod P 59
130 Kazdin AE 85 McNamara HJ 51,52,83
Greeman AL 65 Keister ME 130-132 Mehrabian A 61,95
Green OM 130 KernoffP 73 MeyerWU 114,115
Gregory M 56,157 Kerr B 13,14 Michaels EJ 87,109
Grierson AT 53,165 Kistler 0 163 Millar K 162, 163
Grippaldi R 100 KjellbergA 147,150,151,153 Miller OJ 118
Gupta BS 127,128 Klein KE 139 Miller HE 83
Kleinsmith LJ 141,142,159 Miller ME 51
Kleitman N 141 Miller NE 108
Hamilton P 49, 53, 64, 65, 93, Knox WJ 100 Minard J 76,77
150, 159-162, 164, 170, 171, Kohn H 51 Mirsky AF 55
178,181 Kroll NEA 10 Monk TH 143,145
HamiltonV 111,121 Kruglanski AW 86 Moore LE 62, 63
Harkins S 130, 136 Krugman AD 112 Moray N 9,12
Harley WF 69,70 Krupski A 130, 136 Morgenstern FS 129,185
Harman J 107 Morris LW 96-98, 113
Hartley LR 53,158,165 Mowbray GH 23
Hasher L 63-66 LaBerge 0 33 Mueller JH 100,105,118-120
Haveman JE 159 Lacey 11 4, 173 Mullin J 144, 157
Hepp DO 32 Lansing RW 77
Heider F 113 Laux L 96, 112
Heinz SP 35-39,43,44 Laverty SG 125 Niiiitlinen R 56, 57
Hill KT 54,98,99, 120 Lawson EA 38 Nachreiner F 80
Author Index 203
Nagpal M 128 Sarason IG 76, 77, 97, 107, Tulving E 160
Navon D 28,39-44 108,111,112 Tune GS 130,136
Neely JH 16,18 Sarason SB 97
Neill WT 18 Schachter S 3
Neisser U 180 Schmidt EA 81 Uehling BS 163
Nelson TO 69,70 Schneider K 103, 107, 108 Underwood G 9
Nichols R 76 Schneider W 18-20,23,24,26,
Nicholson IN 127 27,36,37,44,45,63
Nicholson WM III Schneller WF 132
Nideffer RM 128 Schwartz S 64,117-119, Voici C 64
Ninias P 76 134 Von WrightJM 10,17
Norman DA 28,31,33-35,39, SegalSJ 41,42,44 Vranceanu M 64
40,171 Shanmugan TE 129
Norton R 152 Shedletsky R 96
Nottelman ED 54,98,99, 120 Shields J 96 Wachtel PL 51,52
Notz WW 86 Shiffrin RM 18-20, 22-24, Wagoner BL 58
26-28, 31, 36, 37, 44, 45, 63, Walker EL 73, 119, 132, 159
68,69,160 Walker RE 112
O'Malley JJ 53, 164 Shiner S 30 Walley RE 49,90
Ongley GC 77, 78 Silverman RE 51 Wanner E 30
Osgood GE 8 Simon HA 1 Wardlow K 10
Osterkamp U 64,162 Simon JR 52,165,168 WarmJS 81
Otto DA 78 Simpson CK 72,74 Warwick KM 79, 88
Overcast TD 10 Singer JE 3 Wasserman EA 72
Smith AP 162,163 Watkins MJ 29,43
Snyder CRR 13-19,24,27,45, Waugh NC 28,31
PaganoDF 119 63 Wedderburn AA 11
Palfai T 68 Sostek AJ 81 Weick KE 87
Pallak MS 54, 120, 185 Spearman CE 76 Weiden TD 49,90
Patterson J 62 Spelke ES 12 Weiner B 72,73,103,
Peavler WS 58 Spence JT 101-108,112 106-108,113,114
Polzella DJ 150 Spence KW 101-108, 112 WeistRM 71
Poplawsky A 53,164 Spielberger CD 95,98, 106 WelchJ 43
Posner MI 13-19, 24, 44, 45, Sprinkle R 163 Weltman G 52
58,63,77 Staw BM 86 White K 134
Potekan PA 114 Stein BS 116, 117 Wickens DD 72,74
Poulton EC 158,161,167-169 Steriade M 50 Wickens TD 68-72, 74
Powell GD 51 Sternberg RJ 180,181 Wilding J 92,162
Pribram KH 173 Sternberg S 19,179,180 Wilkinson RT 48, 54, 79, 83,
Pritchard RD 89 Stone G 65 94, 148, 149, 153, 154, 158,
Straughan JH 104,105 183, 184
Sullivan L 39 Willett RA 48,78,82,88, 169
Ray WJ 119 Surwillo WW 147,183 Williams HL 149-152
Rebert CS 78 Swets JA 80, 130 Willis J 76
Reeves FB 51 Wilson GD 136
Reitman JS 29 Winch WH 142
Revelle W 87, 109, 124, 125, WineJ 97,99,108
127, 137, 144, 145 Tarpy RM 74 Wolk S 52,83
Rhoades MV 23 Tarnell M 51 Wooley FR 106
Richardson FC 97 Tecce 51
Riley JGA 11,38,39 Tennyson RD 106
Rogers JL 105 Terborg JR 83,89
Ross M 55,86 Thackray RI 136 Yerkes RM 4,5,47-49,56,59,
Rosvold HE 55 Tharp VK 151 79,90,93, 133, 137, 139, 150,
Rubin BM 52, 83 Thayer RE 60-63,139,174 175
Russell JA 61,95 Thomson C 52, 83
Rutenfranz J 141 Thorndike EL 67
Thornton JW 51 Zacks RT 63-66
Trapp EP 52, 83 Taffy DJ 120
Saltz E 104, 112 TreismanAM 8,11,17,23-27, Zajonc RB 115
Santhanam MC 129 35,38,39,41,42,44,46 Zubin J 76,83
Subject Index

Academic achievement and worry 98 - and cognitive masking 50


Action-decrement theory 73 -, definition of 3,4
- and anxiety 119 - and distraction 56, 176
- and introversion-extraversion 132, 133 - and incentive 90, 92-94
-, problems with 73,74 -,measurementof 4,6,173
- and white noise 159,160 - and narrowing of attention 49-54,84
Activation Deactivation Adjective Check -, optimal level of 4,5,47-49,56,57
List 60-62 -, patterning of 57
- and introversion-extraversion 134 - and performance 168,169
- and time of day 139 -, physiology of 174
Additive-factor method 179, 180 - and processing capacity 35,40,138,154
Alcohol 55,182 -, self-reported dimensions of 60-63,95, 134,
- and serial reaction 54 174
Alertness and reaction time 77 - and short-term memory 146
Amphetamine and arousal 182 - and sleep deprivation 147,154
Anagram task 98 - and time of day 139
- and anxiety 99,101,102 -, two-mechanism theory of 55, 66, 158,
- and white noise 156 182-186
Analogical reasoning 180 - and white noise 156,167
Anxiety 95-123 Attention 8-27
- and attentional selectivity 120 - and arousal 7, 66, 146, 160
- and attribution theory 113-115 -, bottleneck theories of 8-13, 59
- and concentration 54, 176, 177 - and collative variables 156
- and conditioning 102 -, divided vs. focussed 22
-, definition of 95,97 - and expectancy 14,16
- and distractibility 54,98 - and feature detection 25,26
- anddual-taskperformancce 51,52 - and incentive 69,70
- and ego-involving instructions 110, III - and introversion-extraversion 128,129
- and failure 106-108, 112, 113 -, narrowing of 2,49-53
- and free recall 105 - and parallel processing 8-10,12,13
- and heredity 96 -, problems with 39-43
- and incentive 77,91 - and selective interference 44
- and length of retention interval 119 - and short-term memory 28
- and level of processing 115-119 - and spare capacity I
- and motivation 108-112 - vs. automatic processing 13-27,63-66
- and paired-associate learning 101-108 Attentional selectivity 49-53
- and performance 5 - and anxiety 120
- and punishment susceptibility 91,92, 126 - and arousal 55,56,63--66,176,186
- and response competition 101-108 - and incentive 69,70,84,92
- and retrieval 104, 105 - and introversion-extraversion 128, 129
- and self-reported arousal 62, 174 - and neuropsychology 49,50
- and shock 112,113 - and sleep deprivation 152, 153
-, state vs. trait 95,96 - and time of day 142
-, and task difficulty 48, 103, 106, 107, 110, - and white noise 92,164-167,169,172
120,177 Attribution theory 113-115
-, theoretical positions on 120-123 Automatic processing 13-27
- and working memory 99-101 - and arousal 63--66
Arousal 145,146 -, definition of 13
- and categorical clustering 64 -, inflexibility of 21
206 Subject Index

- of meaning 10,15,16 - and anxiety 51,52,101,111,122


- and parallel processing 9,10,20,21,23,26 - and arousal 50-53,169,175,184
- and practice 26 - and attention 17, 18,23
-, sped of 15 - and capacity 39,40,43
Awareness and sematic processing 10 - and data-limited processes 33-35
- and effort 57,58
- and expended processing capacity 36-38
Behavioural contrast and anxiety 127 - and incentive 52,83
Behaviourism and attention concept 8 - and introversion-extraversion 128,129
Body temperature 125 - and narrowing of attention 50-53
- and reaction time 141 - and resource-limited processes 33-35
- andtimeofday 139,144 - and selective interference 41,42,44,59
Caffeine 118 - and serial recall 161
- and arousal 183 - and white noise 164-168
- and impulsivity 129,144,145 - and working memory 29
- and introversion-extraversion 127
- and paired-associate learning 118
- and vigilance 132 EEG 77
- and introversion-extraversion 124, 125
- and reaction time 77
Cancellation task 79 - and sleep deprivation 147,152
- and incentive 79, 127 Effort 57-60
- and introversion-extraversion 79,91,127 - and anxiety 108,110,111,122,123
- and knowledge of results 143,114 - and arousal and attention 57-60
- and time of day 139,141,143,144 - and extrinsic motivation 87
Card sorting 89 - and goal setting 90
- and goal setting 89 - and incentive 57,58
- and incentive 89 -, on-task 5,6,145,146
- and sleep deprivation 149,152,153 - and performance 182
- andtimeofday 138,139,141 - and pupil dilation 58
Catecholamines 139 -, self-reported 58,59,167
- and performance 168,183,184 - and sleep deprivation 183
- and sleep deprivation 148 - and task difficulty 48,49, 177
- and time of day 139 Ego-involving instructions 110
Chlorpromazine and arousal 182 - and anxiety 110, III
Cocktail party problem 9 - and intelligence-test performance 76
Cognition and self-reported arousal 63 Elaboration of encoding 71
Componential method 180, 181 - and anxiety 118,119
Conditioning and anxiety 102 - and incentive 71,72
Controlled processing 18-24 Electric shock 50, 51
Cued recall 71, 72 - and anxiety 96, 112, 113
- and anxiety 118,119 - and arousal 185
- and incentive 71,72 - and dual-task performance 50,51
- and learning 108
- and short-term memory 73,74
Data-limited processes vs. resource-limited - and vigilance 148
processes 33-35,171 EMGandsleepdeprivation 154,183
Depression and self-reported arousal 63 Emotion 61
Dichotic listening task 10, II - and anxiety 95
Discrimination learning and arousal 4,5,47 - and arousal 61
Distinctiveness of encoding 116, 117 - and interrupt system 2
Distractibility 185, 186 -, three-dimensional theory of 61
- and anxiety 54,98,110,120 Emotionality 97
- and arousal 176, 185, 186 - and failure 113
- and incentive 84 - and performance 97-99,122
- and introversion-extraversion 129 Encoding specificity principle 160
- and sleep deprivation 152, 153 Examination performance and worry 98
Divergent thinking 134 Eye fixations and incentive 70
- and introversion-extraversion 134
- and REM sleep 147
Dual-task performance 50-53 Failure 96,97
Subject Index 207
- and anxiety 97,106-108,112,113,177 Intrinsic motivation 84-87
- and attribution theory 113-115 Introversion-extraversion 124-138
Feature perception 25 - and arousal 124,125,137,138,182,184
Filter theory of attention 8-13 - and attention 128,129
Free recall 31 - and cancellation task 91
- and anxiety 100, lOS, 117, 118 - and conditinablility 125,127
- and arousal 133,160 - and cortical arousal 55
-, categorical clustering in 64 - and imagery 64
- and incentive 69,70,72 - and incentive 91
-, primacy and recency in 31 - and level of processing 117, 118
- and white noise 159,162,163 - and memory 132-136
- and working memory 29,32 - and other arousers 55
Frustration 91 - and performance speed 136,137
- and anxiety 127 - and reward and punishment 127, 128
- and incentive 91 - and serial reaction 91
Functional fixedness and incentive 75,76 -, theories of 137,138
- and thinking task 110
Inverted-U relation between arousal and
Goal level 88-90 performance 4,5,47-49,56,57,93
- and incentive 83 - and anxiety 103
-, explanation of 59, 173-178
- and incentive 79,80
Habituation and sleep deprivation 147 - and introversion-extraversion 133
Heat and serial reaction 169 - and narrowing of attention 49
- and sleep deprivation ISO
- and white noise 170
Imagery 41
- and arousal 64
Impulsivity 126,127 Knowledge of results 77,78
- and caffeine 129,144,145 - and reaction time 77,78
- and reward susceptibility 91,128 - and serial reaction task 79, 184
Incentive 67-94 - and sleep deprivation 153, 154
- and arousal 70,92-94,182,185 - and vigilance 81
- and cancellation task 79
- and dual-task performance 52,83
- and goal setting 88-90 Law of Effect 67
- and incidental learning 82 Learned helplessness 109
- and intelligence 76,77 Letter-transformation task 93
- and memory 68-74 - and distraction 178
- and motivation 87 - and incentive 93
- and performance efficiency 82-84 - and processing stages 181
- and problem solving 75,76 - and white noise 49,65,93,170,171,181
- and reaction time 77, 78 Levels of processing 116
- and reinforcement 68 - and anxiety 115-119
- and self-reported arousal dimensions 62 - and incentive 71, 72
- and serial reaction 54, 78, 79, 82, 169 - and introversion-extraversion 134, 135
- and task characteristics 48,74-83 -, problems with 116
- and vigilance 80, 81 - and processing demands 37-39
- and white noise 92 - and time of day 143
Incidentallearning 51-53 - and white noise 160-163
- and arousal 51-53,92,93 Lexical decision 16
- and introversion-extraversion 128 - and automatic processing IS
- and white noise 164 Locus of control 113
Instinct 67 Long-term memory 69
Intelligence 4 - and anxiety 119
- and incentive 76,77 - and arousal 66,185
- and introversion-extraversion 136 - and free recall 31
Intentionallearning 51-53 - and introversion-extraversion 132,133
- and arousal 51-53,92,93 -, retrieval from 37, 38, 145
- and introversion-extraversion 128 - and sleep deprivation 150
- and white noise 164 -, theoretical view of 22
208 Subject Index

- and time of day 141-143 - and type of processing 36-38


- and white noise 142,159,160,162 - and white noise 92,93,166
Psychological refractory period 40
Pupil dilation and processing demands 6
Memory span 29,30
- and anxiety 99,100,109
- and incentive 90 Reaction time 77
- and time of day 139,142,143 - and arousal 56,57, 173
- and white noise 161 - and body temperature 141
- and working memory 29,30,32 - and failure liS
Mental set and incentive 75,76 - and incentive 77,79
Morning types and evening types 140 - and introversion-extraversion 128, 136
Motivation 5 - and practice 23,34
- and anxiety 108-112 - and sleep deprivation 151, 152
- and incentive 67,84-87 - and stimulus-response mapping 40
- and multiple goals 1,2 -, subsidiary task 36-38, 51,52
- and probablility of success 109 - and white noise 165
Multiple-resource theory 39-43 Recognition memory 70
Multi-store memory model 22,160 - and arousal 133, 160
- and incentive 68,69 - and incentive 70
-, problems with 28,29 - and introversion-extraversion 135
- vs. working memory 31,161 - and sleep deprivation ISO
Muscle tension and the Stroop effect 65 Rehearsal 69
- and arousal 64
- and incentive 69-72,74
Neuroticism 126 -, maintenance vs. elaboration 71,72
- and heredity 96 - and verbal reasoning 22
- and level of processing 117, 118 - and white noise 161,169,170
- and thinking task 11 0 Reinforcement 67
- and anxiety reduction 108
- and incentive 68
Paired-associate learning 101-103 - and intrinsic motivation 85-87
- and anxiety 101-108,117 - and performance 74
- and arousal 133,141,142,159 Relaxation and anxiety 104
- and caffeine 118 Resultant achievement motivation 114, liS
- and incentive 72,90 Retrieval 134,135
- and white noise 159,160 - and anxiety 104, 105
Peak shift and anxiety 127 - and arousal 186
Physical exercise and self-reported arousal 62 - and incentive 69,72,74
Physilogy 4 - and introversion-extraversion 134, 135
- and arousal 60, 174 - and level of processing 116
- and evoked potential 18
- and introversion-extraversion 125,126
- and pupillometry 6,58 Serial learning 133
Posner task 58 - and anxiety 107
- and pupil dilation 58 - and incentive 90,92
- and white noise 163 - and introversion-extraversion 133
Practice 45 - and sleep deprivation 149
- and anxiety 103 - andtimeofday 143
- and automatic processing 9, 10,20,21,23, - and white noise 92,161
26 Serial reaction task 157,158
- andprarallelprocessing 9,10,12,13 - and alcohol 54
- and resource allocation 33 - and heat 169
Principle of complementarity 34,35,40,41 - and incentive 78,79,90,127,148
Problem solving and incentive 75,76,82 - and introversion-extraversion 91, 127, 136,
Processing capacity 63-66 137
- and anxiety 121 - and knowledge of results 184
- and arousal 35,40,43,59,63-66, 176 - and sleep deprivation 148,149,158,184
-, hierarchical model of 46 - and time of day 138,139,141
- and incentive 92,93 - and white noise 148,157-159,167-169,181
- and task difficulty 175 Shadowing 9
Subject Index 209
- and discriminability 37 - and memory 141-143
- and target detection ll, 12,38,39 - and self-reported arousal 62,139,174
Short-term memory 69 - and shift work 140, 143
- and anxiety 99, 100 - and short-term memory 139
- and arousal 6, 146 - and vigilance 144, 157
- and attention 19 Token economy 85
- and incentive 72-74 Tsai-Partington Number Tracing Test and
-, retrieval from 19, 20, 29, 37, 38, 128, 134, incentive 90
179
- and sleep deprivation 150 Verbal fluency and introversion-extraversion
-, theoretical view of 22 134
- andtimeofday 142 Verbal reasoning and anxiety 101
- and white noise 52, 165, 170 Vigilance 80
- and working memory 28 - and adrenalin 168
Signal detection measures 80,81 - and incentive 80, 81
- and dual-task performance 50 - and introversion-extraversion 129-132
- and expectancy 14,15 - and signal-detection theory 80,81
- and recognition memory 135 - and sleep deprivation 148,149,152
- and vigilance 80,81,130,157 - and time of day 138,139,141, 144
Sleep deprivation 146-155 - and white noise 131, 144, 148, 156, 157,
- and arousal 55,147,154,155,183,185 167,168
- and attention 152, 153 Visual search 19
- and incentive 92 -, effect oftarget definition on 25
- andlapses 151,152 - and illusory conjunctions 26
- and memory 150, 151 -, parallel vs. serial 25,26
- and motivation 153,154 Visual tracking and white noise 164,165
-, REM 146,147
- and self-reported arousal 62 White noise 156-172
- and serial reaction 91, 148, 149, 158, 182, - and arousal 55,156,182,183
184 - andattentionalselectivity 164-167,172
- and time on task 54,55 - and dual-task performance 52
- and vigilance 148,152 - and incentive 92
- and visual tracking 148 - and long-term memory 142,159,160,162
Sodium amytal and introversion-extraversion - and masking 159,161,167-169
125 - and paired-associate learning 64, 142
Speed-accuracy trade-off and incentive 76,83 - and perceptual selection 56
Spoonerisms 32 - and resources 171
Stress 95,96 - and retrieval 128,163,164
- and distractibility 98 - and self-reported arousal 62, 174
- andfailureandshock 1I2, 1I3 - and serial reaction 79, 157-159, 171, 181,
- and memory span 100 182
- and self-reported arousal 174 - and serial recall 64,161,162
Stroop effect 16,17,40 - and short-term memory 159-162,171
- and adrenalin 168 - and the letter-transformation task 49, 65,
- and anxiety 120 161,170,171
- and heart rate 173 - and time on task 54,55,157,158
- and muscle tension 65 - and vigilance 13 I, 144, 156, 157
- and white noise 65 - and working memory 161,162,169,170
Subtraction method 178, 179 Working memory 28-33
Success 113 - and anxiety 99-101,121-123
- and anxiety 107 - and arousal 50
- and attribution theory 113 -, articulatory component of 28-30, 32, 44, 45
-, capacity of 32
-, definition of 28
Timeofday 138-146 - and free recall 29,32
- and arousal 145,146 - and mental arithmetic 30, 31
- and body temperature 125,139-141,144 - and rehearsal 64, 65
- and caffeine 144, 145 - and white noise 16 I, 162
- and impulsivity 144, 145 Worry 96,97
- and introversion-extraversion 144,145 - and failure 1I3
- and levels of processing 143 - and performance 97-99, 122
C. S. Carver, M. F. Scheier

Attention and
Self-Regulation
A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behavior

1981. XVI, 403 pages


(Springer Series in Social Psychology)
ISBN 3-540-90553-7

Contents: Background. - Information and the Use of Recog-


nitory Schemas. - Attention and Motivation. - Interruption,
Expectancy and the Reassertion-Withdrawal Decision. -
Implications for Specific Problems in Social and Personality
Psychology. - Conclusion. - References. - Author Index. -
Subject Index.

In a stimulating approach to the study of human behavior,


this new volume in the Springer Series in Social Psycho-
logy applies the concepts of control theory - cybernetics -
to behavioral self-regulation.
After defining cybernetic concepts, which have had a signifi-
cant impact on many scientific disciplines, the authors clearly
demonstrate how control theory can be used to bridge the
gap between information-processing models ofcognition and
the motivated actions that are generally ignored by the infor-
mation-processing approach. Carver and Scheier construct
a highly integrated model of human behavior that departs
considerably from traditional paradigms. The integrative
potential of the Carver-Scheier model is reflected in the wide
range of topics discussed in this volume, including schema
formation and use, achievement motivation, egotism, social
facilitation, helplessness, anxiety-based behavior, and
impression management.
Springer-Verlag Intertwining recent research and theory in social, personality,
Berlin and cognitive psychology, Attention and Self-Regulation
has a comprehensive perspective that cuts across existing
Heidelberg boundaries in the field. As a theoretical break-through,
Attention and Self-Regulation promises to be a milestone
NewYork in a burgeoning area of psychological inquiry.
Children's Logical and V. A Kovalevsky
Mathematical Cognition Image Pattern Recognition
Progress in Cognitive Development Research Translated from the Russian by A Brown
Editor: C. 1. Brainerd 1980.54 figures, 9 tables. Xl, 241 pages
1982. Approx. 16 figures. Approx. 200 pages ISBN 3-540-90440-9
(Springer Series in Cognitive Development)
ISBN 3-540-90635-5
A Model for Personality
Editor: H. 1. Eysenck
H.1. Eysenck With contributions by numerous experts
1981. 75 figures. XII, 287 pages
The Structure and Measurement ISBN 3-540-10318-X
of Intelligence
With contributions by D. W Fulker
1979.69 figures, 38 tables. V, 253 pages D.WPfafT
ISBN 3-540-09028-2
Estrogens and Brain Function
Neural Analysis of a Hormone-Controlled
Mammalian Reproductive Behavior
1980. 109 figures, 1 in color, 20 tables.
WH.Gaddes X, 281 pages
Learning Disabilities and ISBN 3-540-90487-5
Brain Function
A Neuropsychological Approach
Foreword: W Cruickshank E.Szekely
1980.45 figures, 4 tables .. XVI, 403 pages
ISBN 3-540-90486-7 Functional Laws of Psycho-
dynamics
1979.63 figures, 10 tables. VIII, 353 pages
ISBN 3-540-90371-2
W W Henton, 1. H. Iversen
Classical Conditioning and
Operant Conditioning Verbal Processes in Children
A Response Pattern Analysis Progress in Cognitive Development Research
1978. 106 figures, 16 tables. XVII, 355 pages Editors: C. 1. Brainerd, M. Pressley
ISBN 3-540-90326-7 1982. Approx. 10 figures. Approx. 320 pages
(Springer Series in Cognitive Development)
ISBN 3-540-90648-7

Improving the Human Condition:


Quality and Stability
in Social Systems
Proceedings of the Silver Anniversary Inter-
national Meeting, London, England, Springer-Verlag
August 20-24,1979
Editor: R F. Ericson Berlin
1979.253 figures, 61 tables.
XXVIII, 1051 pages
Heidelberg
ISBN 3-540-90442-5 NewYork

You might also like