Interaction Between Emotion and Memory

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

NEURAL PLASTICITY VOLUME 12, NO.

4, 2005

Interaction between Emotion and Memory: Importance of


Mammillary Bodies Damage in a Mouse Model of the
Alcoholic Korsakoff Syndrome
Daniel Bdracochda

Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, UMR CNRS 5106, Universit de Bordeaux


1, Avenue des facult6s, 33405 Talence, France

SUMMARY KEYWORDS

Chronic alcohol consumption (CAC) can alcohol, ethanol, diencephalons, thalamus,-anxiety,


lead to the Korsakoff syndrome (KS), a memory thiamine deficiency
deficiency attributed to diencephalie damage
and/or to medial temporal or cortical related
dysfunction. The etiology of KS remains unclear. INTRODUCTION
Most animal models of KS involve thiamine-
deficient diets associated with pyrithiamine The Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome is one of
treatment. Here we present a mouse model of the most serious consequences of long-term alcohol
CAC-induced KS. We demonstrate that CAC- abuse. The specific etiology of this syndrome
generated retrieval memory deficits in working/ remains under debate, even though heavy and long-
episodic memory tasks, together with a reduction term alcohol use is the most common association
of fear reactivity, result from damage to the with Wemicke-Korsakoff syndrome. Nevertheless,
mammillary bodies (MB). Experimental lesions according to several authors, the Korsakoff syn-
of MB in non-alcoholic mice produced the same drome (KS) is caused by a lack of thiamine
memory and emotional impairments. Drugs (vitamin B 1), which affects the brain and nervous
having anxiogenic-like properties counteract system. Indeed, the excessive use of alcohol is
such impairments produced by CAC or by MB often the cause of thiamine deficiency, insofar as
lesions. We suggest (a) that MB are the essential many heavy drinkers have poor eating habits.
components of a brain network underlying Thiamine is converted to thiamine pyrophosphate,
emotional processes, which would be critically which serves as a cofactor for several enzymes
important in the retrieval processes involved in involved in glucose utilization. Alcohol interferes
working/ episodic memory tasks, and (b) that with active gastrointestinal transport, and chronic
failure to maintain emotional arousal due to liver disease leads to a decreased activation of
MB damage can be a main factor of CAC- thiamine pyrophosphate from thiamine, as well as
induced memory deficits. Overall, our animal reducing the capacity of the liver to store thiamine.
model fits well with general neuropsychological The particular sensitivity of thiamine metabolism
and anatomic impairments observed in KS. to alcohol intoxication has been documented. As a
case in point, Molina et al. (1994) showed that
chronic alcoholics exhibit a low serum level of
e-mail: d.beracochea@lnc.u-bordeaux .fr thiamine but not of other vitamins; yet, the serum

(C) 2005 Freund & Pettman, U.K. 275


276 DANIEL BIRACOCHIA

level of thiamine was weakly correlated with learning new skills, a marked sensitivity to
cognitive performance, as opposed to the duration interference, an alteration of the temporal order
of alcohol intake and education. The thiamine- judgment, and a deficit of spatial organization
deficiency hypothesis has also been strengthened (Butters, 1985). Confabulation and a lack of insight
by the observations of the Wemicke-Korsakoff into the condition are also observed (for example,
syndrome in non alcoholic patients (Parkin et al., some patients with great gaps in their memory can
1991). believe that their memory is functioning normally);
Korsakoff syndrome is part of the Wemicke- in addition to these cognitive deficits, disturbance
Korsakof syndrome, which comprises two separate of affective judgments, apathy in some cases, are
but related stagesuWemicke’s encephalopathy also frequently observed (Johnson et al., 1985;
and Korsakoff psychosis. It is noteworthy that not Cabanyes, 2004).
all cases of Korsakoff are preceded by an episode Given the heterogeneity of the neuropsycho-
of Wemicke. Wernicke encephalopathy symptoms logical data, three hypotheses have been formulated
involve involuntary eye movements, poor balance, to localize the neural dysfunction responsible for
staggering gait or inability to walk, drowsiness,, and the memory failure of Korsakoff patients.
confusion. Such symptoms can be most often 1. The first hypothesis is that the relevant
reversed by high doses of thiamine injected into a functional damage is restricted to the di-
vein or muscle. In contrast, the memory loss in encephalons.
Korsakoff patients is insensitive to thiamine 2. A second hypothesis is that additional brain
injection (Fellgiebel et al., 2003) and is thought to areas in the medial temporal region are affected;
be an outcome of the structural brain damage and thus, Butters and Stuss (1989) suggested that
thus to be largely irreversible (attempts to explore diencephalic amnesia could arise from disrupted
neuropharmacological therapies have yielded mixed connections between the diencephalon and
results" Martin et al., 1995; Moffoot et al., 1994; medial temporal lobe structures, which would
O’Carroll et al., 1994). account for the high degree of similarity
Anterograde amnesia is characterized by a between the amnesic impairments resulting
difficulty to acquire new information and consti- from diencephalic or temporal damage.
tutes the main KS symptom. The memory loss 3. A third hypothesis is that functional impair-
would mainly result from an impairment of en- ments are widespread, encompassing multiple
coding processes (Butters 1985) and/or a failure of cortical areas (Paller et al., 1997).
retrieval ones (Lhermitte & Signoret, 1972;
Warrington & Weis-Krantz, 1974) as opposed to Lesions of the mammillary bodies (MB) of the
amnesia resulting from medial temporal damage, hypothalamus are large and most frequently
characterized by a more severe accelerated for- observed as opposed to other brain damage (Victor
getting over time (Huppert & Piercy, 1976; et al., 1971; Reed et al., 2003). Mammillary bodies
Thompson, 1981). According to some authors, exhibit distinct anatomical features in mammals.
however, both diencephalic and temporal amnesia In rats, the mammillary region consists of a
share common features so that they would be complex set of nuclei; it is constituted mainly by
almost indistinguishable on neuro-psychological the lateral, the medial, and the supra-mammillary
grounds, insofar as diencephalic amnesia can be nuclei. The MB themselves are divided into two
due to a disconnection between the diencephalon groups, the medial and lateral nuclei. The MB
and the medial temporal lobe (Mayes et al., 1988). receives dense afferent connections from the
Other KS symptoms consisted of a difficulty in hippocampus through the post-commissural fomix,
MAMMILLARY BODIES AND KORSAKOFF SYNDROME 277

which terminate mainly into the medial MB damage in the diencephalon, including the medial
nucleus (Swanson & Cowan, 1977; Meibaach & thalamus and connections with the medial tem-
Siegel, 1977). The MB sends efferent connections poral lobes, rather than MB damage as a key factor
to thalamic nuclei via the mammilllo-thalamic of Korsakoff amnesia. Thus, lesions of thalamic
tract. The thalamic outputs of the medial and nuclei (mainly of the mediodorsal thalamus or of
lateral MB nuclei are topographically distinct: the the anterior thalamic nuclei), in the absence of MB
medial MB nucleus sends outputs to the antero- lesions, were found to be sufficient to produce a
medial and anteroventral thalamic nuclei, whereas severe anterograde amnesia. So far, thalamic
the lateral MB nucleus sends outputs to the damage is thought to be a key factor to produce
anterodorsal thalamic nucleus (Cruce, 1975; Seki amnesia (Victor et al., 1971; Markowitch, 1982;
& Zio, 1984). The medial MB nucleus has Mair, 1994; Harding et al., 2000). In keeping with
reciprocal connections with the ventral tegmental this idea, some studies have also reported no
nucleus of Gudden, whereas the lateral MB memory loss in Wemicke-Korsakoff patients in
nucleus has reciprocal connections with the dorsal whom degeneration into the MB was nevertheless
tegmental nucleus of Gudden; they also project to observed (Victor et al., 1971) even though, con-
different areas of the reticular tegmenta! nucleus versely, existing clinical data show that neuro-
(Cruce, 1977; Hayakawa & Zyo, 1984; 1985; anatomical lesions restricted to the MB can
Allen & Hopkins, 1990). Furthermore, both the induce temporal order judgment deficit and an
lateral and medial MB nuclei are also inner-vated exaggerated vulnerability to interference, in the
by the suprammamillary and the tuberomam- absence of any other brain lesions (Hildebrandt et
millary nuclei, as well as by the frontal cortex, the al., 2001).
septal area, and the medial enthorinal cortex (Allen The issue of whether hippoeampal lesions or
& Hopkins, 1989; Gonzalo-Ruiz et al. 1992; Shibata, indirect hippoeampal dysfunction is a significant
1988). This overall pattern of connections, mainly feature of KS remains open. Indeed, hippocampal
drawn from the study of the rat brain, is also found and diencephalic damage generates some common
in the primate brain. neuropsychological deficits. Further, on the one
As a whole, the topographically distinct ana- hand hippocampal damage was shown to result
tomical connections of the lateral and medial MB from a direct neurotoxicity of alcohol (Freund,
nuclei have suggested that both nuclei belong to 1973; Walker et al., 1980), and post-mortem neuro-
two parallel systems, which have been proposed to pathology of Korsakoff subjects has evidenced
explain the functional involvement of the MB in hippocampal involvement in some cases (see also
memory processes (Vann & Aggleton, 2004). Both Mayes et al., 1988). On the other hand, animal
systems would contribute either separately or studies have also demonstrated that discrete lesions
synergistically (given some of their common sites of the MB or diencephalic damage can disrupt
of convergence) to different processes, such as the hippocampal cholinergic activity (B6racoch6a et
directional firing of head direction cells in the al., 1995b; Savage et al., 2003). Thus, Vann and
anterodorsal thalamus, the transmission of the Aggleton (2004) also pointed out the importance
hippocampal theta rhythm to other limbic sites, or of medial temporal dysfunction resulting from MB
the treatment of allosteric information. damage in KS and diencephalic amnesia.
Interestingly, despite the importance of the The importance of temporal damage as a
anatomical interactions of the MB with the medial critical factor to induce memory loss in Korsakoff
temporal and diencephalic areas, the neuro- patients is challenged, however, by studies using
pathology of KS stresses more the importance of various structural or functional neuro-imaging
278 DANIEL BIRACOCHA

techniques. Such studies showed in KS patients thought to be mainly responsible for the deficits of
normal hippocampal size (Squire et al., 1990) and PTD animals, since experimental mediodorsal
spared hippocampal metabolism (Paller et al., thalamic lesions in monkeys and rodents produced
1997). In contrast, in addition to the hypothalamic memory impairments similar to those resulting from
and diencephalic alterations, some of these studies PTD or from Korsakoff amnesia (Zola-Morgan &
have often evidenced large cortical hypometabol- Squire, 1985). In addition, the PTD model also
ism associated with cognitive cortical dysfunction, allows the study of the relationships between dien-
suggesting that the influence of diencephalic cephalic damage and medial temporal dysfunction.
damage on cortical function areas-would play an Indeed, using tasks involving spontaneous alterna-
essential role in memory loss of KS subjects (Paller tion, Savage et al. (2003) demonstrated that PTD-
et al., 1997; Kopelman, 1995; see also Brokate et treated rats exhibiting diencephalic damage also
al., 2003). show a reduction of the release of acetylcholine
efflux in the hippocampus. There is evidence,
therefore, that the hippocampus is not fully
ANIMAL MODELS OF KS USING activated in memory tasks in rats suffering from
THIAMINE-DEFICIENT DIETS diencephalic damage.
Despite its interest in producing diencephalic
In the field of learning and memory, animal amnesia and in studying the interaction between the
models have been instrumental in shaping our diencephalon and related brain structures on
understanding of how normal and damaged brains memory processes, however, the PTD model does
process information. Thus, animal investigations not provide clear-cut evidence in favor of the pre-
have allowed the description of memory in terms dominance of thiamine deficiency as the main
of multiple systems--competing or interacting or causal factor of Korsakoff amnesia. Indeed, pyri-
functioning in parallelmdepending on the thiamine administration is a drastic way to deplete
cognitive demand or on the psychological nature thiamine metabolism, which largely encompasses
of the task. the common physiological state of thiamine
Given the etiologic data, most attempts to depletion in Korsakoff patients. In fact, most
produce an animal model of the KS have involved studies using a thiamine-deficient diet alone (in the
thiamine deficiency as a tool to induce the neuro- absence of combined pyrithiamine administration)
psychological and cerebral damage observed in evidenced some degree of memory loss in thiamine-
this pathology. The pyrithiamine-induced thiamine deficient animals (Witt & Goldman-Rakic, 1983a-b;
deficiency (PTD) model often consisted of a Mair et al., 1988). Nevertheless, negative findings
combination of a thiamine-deficient diet associated have also been reported (Homewood et al., 1991;
with the application of pyrithiamine, a thiamine- Tako el al, 1991). Moreover, the brain lesions
antagonist. Most of the data using the PTD model induced by thiamine deficiency were less severe
have shown important memory loss and lesions than in PTD and inconsistent from one study to
predominantly located in the diencephalic areas, another. For example, Witt and Goldman-Rakic
but damage was also observed in other brain areas (1983a-b) failed to identify mediodorsal thalamic
(the basal orebrain, several cortical areas, and the damage in monkeys submitted to a thiamine-
MB (Irle & Markowitsch, 1983; Mair et al., 1988; deficient diet, as opposed to findings reported by
Joyce, 1994; Mumby et al., 1995; Langlais & Mair et al. (1988) in rodents; moreover, as
Zhang, 1997). As suggested for Korsakoffpatients, opposed to PTD, a thiamine-deficient diet failed to
damage into the mediodorsal thalamus has been produce the hypothalamic damage often observed
MAMMILLARY BODIES AND KORSAKOFF SYNDROME 279

in Korsakoff amnesia. Indeed, in a comparative observed in Korsakoff patients.


study using independent groups submitted either to In our studies, mice of the Balb/c strain were
chronic alcohol consumption or to a severe thiamine- submitted to a forced consumption of alcohol
deficient diet, we found that only alcohol-treated (12% v/v) for several months (either 6 or 12 mo)
mice exhibited a working memory deficit and large and subsequently were progressively withdrawn
MB damage (Tako et al., 199 !). The sparing of the from alcohol. For that purpose, the water was
MB in thiamine-deficient animals is not specific to progressively substituted for ethanol by steps of
mice; indeed, other studies have also shown that 4% (v/v) a week. The mice drank only water for
the MB are not altered by a thiamine-deficient diet, at least month before behavioral testing began. In
both in monkeys (Witt & Goldman-Rakic, 1983a-b) all studies, alcohol-treated animals were compared
and in rats (Mair et al., 1988). to either pair-fed controls, which drank a dextri-
Interestingly, the studies using chronic alcohol maltose solution isocaloric to the alcohol one over
diets reported more severe memory impairments the same period of treatment, or to mice submitted
than when using a thiamine-deficient diet without to a water and dry food diet ad-libitum (B6ra-
pyrithiamine administration (Homewood et al. coch6a et al., 1985; 1987a, c). At the end of the
1997; Tako et al; 1991; Ciccia & Langlais, 2000). alcohol treatment, alcohol-treated mice exhibited
Other studies also highlighted the finding that the no weight loss as compared with control animals.
interaction between CAC and the age of the Behavioral testing was conducted following
subjects is of critical importance in alcohol- different periods of alcohol consumption. In parallel
induced dysfunction (Krazem et al., 2003). That with memory testing, anatomical experiments were
several studies have demonstrated a direct neuro- conducted to evaluate the effects of alcohol
toxicity of alcohol (Freund, 1973; Walker et al., consumption on the neuronal density of several
1980) suggests that long-term alcohol intake could brain structures or on brain metabolic activity,
be a causal factor of specific neuropathological using the 2-desoxyglucose technique.
features of KS. Indeed, numerous reports suggest Working memory was evaluated using spon-
that a combination of alcohol consumption and taneous alternation, either in sequential or delayed
thiamine deficiency is not only required to produce procedures. Spontaneous alternation (SA) is the
severe, long-lasting memory impairment but also innate tendency of rodents whereby over a series
constitutes the main neurological damage of KS of trials run in a T-maze, mice alternate at each
(Homewood & Bond, 1999). successive trial the choice of the visited goal-arm,
except for the first trial. Moreover, SA does not
require the use of food reinforcement to emerge,
which is of particular interest. Two procedures
A MOUSE MODEL OF KS INDUCED BY CAC: have been used.
IMPORTANCE OF MB DAMAGE. In the sequential procedure, repetitive testing
constitutes a potent source of proactive inter-
An animal model of Korsakoff amnesia using ference. Indeed, from trial to trial, an accurate
CAC as the sole etiologic factor is lacking. performance at a given N trial requires that
Therefore, the scope of our studies was to subjects are able to discriminate the specific target
determine whether CAC in mice could produce trial N-1 from the interfering trial N-2. The target
memory loss and neuroanatomical damage (mainly information required for successful performance
diencephalic in the broader sense, including hypo- varies from trial to trial, so that the subject is
thalamic damage) that could resemble that required not only to keep temporarily in short-term
280 DANIEL BRACOCHA

memory a specific information (for example what impairment in usual testing conditions (B6racoch6a
happened 30 sec earlier) but also to reset it over et al., 1987c; 1989a).
successive runs. The resetting mechanisms and In addition to memory impairment, we found
cognitive flexibility required to alternate over that CAC also produces an abnormal emotional
successive runs are major components of working reactivity in an open-field and in elevated plus
memory processes. maze tasks. More specifically, CAC-treated
In the delayed forced procedure, the acquisi- animals enter more often and spend more time
tion phase involves two successive forced entries in the open arms of the elevated plus maze
into the same goal arm of the maze, entrance into compared with controls. In the open field,
the other arm being blocked by a sliding door. In CAC-treated mice exhibit the shortest latency
the test phase, which occurs after various delay to leave the center of the apparatus, and they
intervals following acquisition, mice can freely exhibit more general activity than do controls.
enter both goal arms, the alternation response So far, CAC-treated mice are less ’anxious’ than
being to enter the arm opposite to the one entered controls (unpublished results).
during the acquisition phase. This procedure In parallel to the behavioral tasks, anatomical
allowed the study of memory over long delay studies have shown that CAC produces a
intervals (up to 48 h in our studies). substantial cell loss (reduction of cell density) into
the MB of the hypothalamus, a moderate cell loss
CAC-induced memory impairment and brain in the anterior and mediodorsal thalamus (B6ra-
damage coch6a et al., 1987a) and in the frontal cortex, and
a weak loss into the hippocampus (B6racoch6a et
We found that CAC produced an increased al., 1985; 1987c; Lescaudron et al., 1984). At the
sensitivity to interference in the sequential pro- end of the CAC period, the hippocampus also
cedure and an accelerated forgetting in the delayed exhibits modifications in number and morphology
one (B6racoch6a et al., 1985; 1987a, c). Interest- of the dendritic spines of the CA1 hippocampal
ingly, these two impairments were not observed in pyramidal neurons, but such alterations disappear
mice submitted to a severe thiamine-deficient diet after month of withdrawal, namely, at the time of
(Tako, 1986; Tako et al., 1991). We demonstrated behavioral testing (Lescaudron et al., 1989).
that both exaggerated sensitivity to interference and Using the 2-desoxyglucose technique in mice,
accelerated forgetting exhibited by CAC-treated we found that MB exhibit a very large reduction of
mice are due to a selective deficit of retrieval desoxy-glucose metabolic activity, which is
processes. Indeed, a change of the context of the dependent on the length of the alcohol treatment;
maze, achieved by adding a large white cardboard at this study also evidenced weak impairments in
the end of the central alley, dramatically improves other diencephalic structures (the anterior and
alternation rates at the cued test trial (B6racoch6a mediodorsal thalamus), but the hippocampal
et al., 1987b; 1989a). Insofar as alternation requires metabolic activity was spared (Bontempi et al.,
memory of the specific goal-arm entered at the 1996).
acquisition phase (delayed procedure) or at the N-1 Overall, the results of neuroanatomical studies
trial (sequential procedure), the improvement of stress that the pathology of the MB is the main
performance induced by the context-change shows lesion resulting from the CAC procedure used in our
that the target information required to alternate is studies. Interestingly, we found that a very severe
not forgotten at the time of testing and that CAC- thiamine-deficient diet in the same strain of mice
treated mice suffer primarily from a retrieval did not produce MB damage (Tako et al., 1991).
MAMMILLARY BODIES AND KORSAKOFF SYNDROME 281

Memory impairment resulting from discrete between memory and ’anxiety’ scores only for
(CAC-treated mice) or experimental lesions of the interfering trials of the sequential alternation task,
MB: reversal by pharmacological compounds namely, the trials involving a heavy memory load.
The relationship between the emotional state of
Given our findings in CAC-treated animals, the subject and alternation scores of the interfering
we investigated the effects of lesioning the MB in trials of the sequential alternation task was also
non-alcoholic mice. For that purpose, an ibotenic conversely evidenced in non-alcoholic mice
acid solution (10 mg/mL) was injected in situ receiving diazepam at anxiolytic doses. We found
through a glass pipette. We found that this lesion that diazepam-treated animals exhibit memory
generated the very same memory impairments as deficits similar to those observed in CAC-treated
those resulting from CAC, namely, an exaggerated or in MB-lesioned animals, both in the alternation
vulnerability to interference and an accelerated tasks (Borde et al., 1997) or in an 8-arms radial
forgetting in the sequential and delayed alternation maze involving a working memory component
procedures respectively (B6racoch6a & Jaffard, (Borde et al., 1998). Interestingly, we also observed
1987; B6racoch6a & Jaffard, 1990). The increased similar deficits following diazepam administration,
sensitivity to interference following MB damage CAC or MB lesions in naive animals in a cognitive
was also evidenced in an 8-arm radial maze learning set task (Borde & B6racoch6a, 1999;
(B6racoch6a et al., 1989). Moreover, as in CAC- Krazem et al., 1995).
treated animals, the memory deficits observed in Thus, the overall pharmacological data show
the alternation tasks also stem from retrieval- that (a) benzodiazepine administration produces
memory impairments (Tako et al., 1988). In retrieval-memory deficits that are similar to those
addition, we also found that MB lesions reduce resulting from CAC or experimental MB lesions,
fear reactivity in an elevated plus maze task (b) methylbetacarboline, an inverse agonist of the
(B6racoch6a & Krazem, 1991), a finding also in GAB A/benzodiazepine receptor with anxiogenic
agreement with our previous observations in CAC- properties, alleviates memory impairments resulting
treated animals. from discrete (CAC-treated mice) or from large
The similarity of the emotional and memory experimental MB lesions, and (c) damage to the
impairments resulting from CAC and MB lesions MB might play a key role in CAC-induced amnesia.
in non-alcoholic mice leads us to suggest that MB Given the resemblance between benzodiaze-
damage play a key role in the memory distur- pine-induced memory impairments and those
bances observed in CAC-treated mice. Given that resulting from MB lesions or CAC treatment, we
MB exhibits the highest density of benzodiazepine hypothesized that emotional disturbances can be a
receptors in the brain (Eymin et al., 1992), we causal factor of the memory impairments resulting
administered methylbetacarboline, an inverse from the CAC or MB lesions.
agonist of the benzodiazepines receptor having
anxiogenic properties prior to memory testing. We
observed that methylbetacarboline increases fear THE MAMMILLARY BODIES: A SET OF
reactivity in the two experimental groups, which NUCLEI AT THE INTERFACE OF MEMORY
therefore behave similarly to controls in an AND EMOTIONAL PROCESSESS
elevated plus maze and, concomitantly, alleviate
the retrieval-memory impairments in the sequential Our data emphasize the role of MB damage in
alternation task (B6racoch6a et al., 1995a). In this CAC-induced amnesia and stress the importance
study, we showed a high positive correlation of emotional impairments resulting either from
282 DANIEL BIRACOCHIA

discrete (CAC-treated animals) or from experi- glucose metabolism following the injection of low
mental MB lesions. Indeed, one additional set of doses of diazepam (Ableitner et al., 1985; see also
MB functions that our studies highlight concerns Schroeder et al., 1994). In keeping with these data,
the involvement of the MB in the interaction we showed that the memory improvement observed
between emotional and memory processes. So far, in CAC-treated mice following methylbetacarboline
we raise questions about the putative causal role of administration is specifically due to the enhance-
emotional dysfunction in the memory deficits ment of the desoxyglucose metabolic activity in
resulting from MB damage or CAC-treatment. the MB, which is normally reduced in alcohol-
The involvement of the MB and surrounding treated animals (Bontempi et al., 1996). Interestingly,
nuclei in emotional processes has been documented. MB have also been found to be a site of the anti-
Lesions of the dorsal premammillary nucleus alter anxiety action of benzodiazepines in conflict-
defensive behavior toward a predator, mainly by punishment procedures (Kataoka et al., 1982) or in
reducing the cognitive assessment of the predator’s mediating the anticonflict action of zopiclone (a
threat (Canteras et al., 1997; 2001; Blanchard et al., cyclopyrrolone derivative, acting at the GABA
2003). Anxiolytic effects of lesions of the medial receptor; Yamashita et al., 1989).
MB nucleus (B6racoch6a & Krazem, 1991) or of The observation that the MB are involved in
the tuberomammillary nucleus (Frisch et al., 1998) emotional processes suggests that MB damage
have been reported during the exploration of an could lead to deficits in memory tasks that are
elevated plus maze. We also reported a deficit of likely to involve either an emotional component
contextual fear conditioning following MB directly or to induce an emotional arousal,
damage, similar to the one generated by dorsal depending on the constraints of the tasks. For
hippocampal lesions (Celerier et al., 2004; see also example, the cognitive demand involved in a given
Radyuskin et al, 2005, for a negative report in task (explicit versus implicit encoding or retrieval
mice suffering from a genetic ablation of the MB, procedures) has been found to be differentially
even though significant procedural differences sensitive to benzodiazepines administration (Danion
could account for the discrepancies). In addition, et al, 1989). In keeping with this idea, we showed
we also reported deficits of fear conditioning that MB damage does not produce any deficit in a
following anterior thalamic damage, a brain delayed matching to place task, requiring much
structure tightly connected with the MB (Celerier more explicit demand than non-matching (alter-
et al., 2000). The MB-induced contextual fear nating) ones, which are spontaneously processed
conditioning deficit fits well with the observations in rodents (Bracoch6a & Jaffard, 1995). The
that the medial mammillary nucleus plays a role in memory load of a given task or the task difficulty
the hormonal response to stress. Indeed, it has (Schneider et al., 1996) could also induce
been found that the normal increase of plasma emotional arousal and so far, MB damage should
corticosterone resulting from an acute stress is differentially affect performance. Accordingly, we
reduced in MB-lesioned animals (Suarez & Perassi, found that CAC-treated mice, which exhibit
1988; 1993; see also Feldman et al., 1975), a significant discrete lesions of the MB, behave
finding also observed in rodents suffering from similarly as diazepam-treated mice in an 8-arms
anterodorsal thalamic lesions after chronic stress radial maze. Furthermore, the largest deficits of
(Suarez et al., 1999; 2001). According to Eymin et both CAC or diazepam-treated mice were observed
al. (1992), MB exhibit a very important density of at the beginning of the behavioral sessions, but
GABA/benzodiazepine receptor sites, and MB were no longer observed when the same types of
were found to exhibit a dramatic reduction of problems were preceded by a mixed series of more
MAMMILLARY BODIES AND KORSAKOFF SYNDROME 283

complex ones (Borde et al., 1998; see also Sziklas to thank Frances Ash (ashberac@free.fr) for her
& Petrides, 1998, for the importance of the task help in improving the English text.
difficulty in MB-induced deficits).
Thus, we suggest that the MB contributes to
maintaining an emotional arousal and/or a REFERENCES
moderate degree of ’anxiety’, which are normally
involved in the processing of memory tasks Ableitner A, Wuster M, Herz A. 1985. Specific
(Chapouthier et al., 2004); this emotional changes in local cerebral glucose utilization in the
rat brain induced by acute and chronic diazepam.
component may be of critical impoi’tance in Brain Res 16: 49-56.
spontaneous retrieval processes involved in Allen GV, Hopkins DA. 1989. Mamillary body in the
’working/episodic’ memory tasks, in which in- rat: topography and synaptology of projections
formation is bound up with specific contextual, from the subicular complex, prefrontal cortex and
emotional and temporal cues. Therefore, impair- midbrain tegmentum. J Comp Neurol 286: 311-
ment of emotional arousal could be a causal factor 336.
Allen GV, Hopkins DA. 1990. Topography and
for certain deficits exhibited by CAC-treated or
synaptology of mamillary body projections to the
MB-lesioned mice. Indeed, a weakness of emotional mesencephalon and pons in the rat. J Comp Neurol
arousal can reduce attention and/or impair the 301: 214-231.
processes involved during memory retrieval. In B6racoch6a D J, Jaffard R. 1985. Memory deficits
keeping with this idea, we showed that the subsequent to chronic consumption of ethanol in
alleviation of the retrieval-memory deficits mice: an analysis based on spontaneous alter-
nation behavior. Behav Brain Res 15" 15-25.
exhibited by CAC-treated or MB-lesioned animals
B6racoch6a DJ, Jaffard R. 1987. Impairment of spon-
in the alternation tasks following a pre-test taneous alternation behavior in sequential test pro-
methylbetacarboline injection is specifically due to cedures following mammillary body lesions in mice
the interaction between the anxiolytic effects of evidence for time-dependent interference related
the treatments (CAC or MB lesions) and the anxio- memory deficits. Behav Neurosci 101 187-197.
B6racoch6a D J, Jaffard R. 1990. Effects of mam-
genic effects of the pharmacological compound
millary bodies lesions on spontaneous or rewarded
(B6racoch6a et al., 1995a; Borde et al., 1996; see sequential alternation in mice. J Cognit Neurosci
also Borde et al., 1997). 2:133-140.
Overall, the MB emerges as an important B6racoch6a DJ, Jaffard R. 1995. The effects of mam-
component of the interface between emotion and millary body lesions on delayed matching and
memory. Given its involvement in emotional delayed non matching to place tasks in the mice.
Behav Brain Res 68: 45-52.
processes, it follows that the MB should be
B6racoch6a DJ, Krazem A. 1991. Effects of ibotenic
involved in a wide range of memory processes and acid lesions of the mammillary body and the
cognitive functions and so far, that MB damage mediodorsal thalamus on elevated plus maze
may directly account for some of the complex exploration. Neuroreport 2: 793-796.
neuropsychological features of the Korsakoff’s B6racochda DJ, Lescaudron L, Verna A, Jaffard R.
syndrome. 1987a. Neuroanatomical effects of chronic ethanol
consumption on dorsomedial and anterior
thalamic nuclei and on substantia innominata in
mice. Neurosci Lett 73:81-84.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS B6racoch6a DJ, Lescaudron L, Tako A, Verna A,
Jaffard R. 1987b. Build-up and release from
This study was supported by grants from the proactive interference during chronic ethanol con-
CNRS and GDA (Paris, France). The author wishes sumption in mice: a behavioral and neuroana-
284 DANIEL BIRACOCI-A

tomical study. Behav Brain Res 25: 63-74. Boiler E, Grafman J, eds, Handbook Of Neuro-
B6racoch6a DJ, Tako A, Jaffard R. 1989a. Ageing and psychology, Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier,
chronic ethanol consumption induce two different 3: 107-148.
forms of amnesia. Psychobiology 17: 358-362. Blanchard DC, Li CI, Hubbard D, Markham CM,
B6racoch6a D J, Alaoui-Bouarraqui F, Jaffard R. Yang M, Takahashi LK, et al. 2003. Dorsal pre-
1989b. Impairment of memory in a delayed non mammillary nucleus differentially modulates
matching to place task following mammillary defensive behaviors induced by different threat
bodies lesions in mice. Behav Brain Res 34: 147- stimuli in rats. Neurosci Lett 345" 145-148.
154. Brand M, Fujiwara E, Kalbe E, Steingass Hp, Kessler
B6racoch6a DJ, Krazem A, Jaffard. R. 1995a. 13-ccm J, Markowitsch HJ. 2003. Cognitive estimation
reverses the memory deficits induced by a chronic and affective judgments in alcoholic korsakoff
ethanol administration and mammillary bodies patients. J Clin Exp Nuropsychol 25: 324-334.
lesions in mice. Psychobiology 23" 52-58. Cabanyes J. 2004. Neuropsychology of korsakoff
B6racoch6a DJ, Micheau J, Jaffard R. 1995b. Alter- syndrome. Neurologia 19:183-192.
ation of hippocampal and cortical cholinergic Canteras NS, Chiavegatto S, Valle LE, Swanson LW.
activities following lesions of the mammillary 1997. Severe reduction of rat defensive behavior
bodies in mice. Brain Res 670: 53-58. to a predator by discrete hypothalamic chemical
Bontempi b, Beracochea D J, Jaffard R, Destrade C. lesions. Brain Res Bull 44" 297-305.
1996. A 2-dg study of a long-t+rm alcohol ad- Canteras NS, Ribeiro-Barbosa ER, Comoli E. 2001.
ministration on regional metabolic activity in Tracing from the dorsal igremammillary nucleus
mice. Neuroscience 72:1141-1153. prosencephalic systems involved in the organiza-
Borde N, Krazem A, Jaffard R, B6racoch6a DJ. 1996. tion of innate fear responses. Neurosci Biobehav
Effects of 13ccm on memory impairments induced Rev 25:661-668.
by chronic alcohol consumption in mice. Prog Celerier A, Ognard R, Decorte L, B6racoch6a DJ.
Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 20: 2000. Deficits of spatial and non spatial memory
1377-1387. and of auditory fear conditioning following anterior
Borde N, Krazem A, Jaffard R, B6racoch6a DJ. 1997. thalamic lesions in mice: comparison with chronic
Memory deficits following diazepam administra- alcohol consumption. Eur J Neurosci 12: 2575-
tion in mice: evidence for a time-dependent 2584.
retrieval memory impairments. Psychobiology 25: Celerier A, Pierard C, B6racoch6a DJ. 2004. Effects
202-209. of ibotenic acid lesions of the dorsal hippocampus
Borde N, Jaffard R, Beracochea DJ. 1998. Effects of on contextual fear conditioning in mice: compari-
chronic alcohol consumption or diazepam admini- son. with mammillary bodies lesions. Behav Brain
stration on item recognition and temporal ordering Res 151: 65-72.
in a spatial working memory task in mice. Eur J Chapouthier G, Negroni J, Venault P. 2004. The
Neurosci 10: 2380-2387. pharmacology of anxiety and its consequences:
Borde N, Beracochea DJ. 1999. Effects of diazepam epilepsy, balance control and memory processes.
administration and chronic alcohol consumption Current Topics in Pharmacology 8:219-229.
on a spatial reversal learning in mice. Pharmacol Ciccia RM, Langlais PJ. 2000. An examination of the
Biochem Behav 62: 719-725. .synergistic interaction of ethanol and thiamine
Brokate B, Hildebrandt H, Eling P, Fichtner H, Runge deficiency in the development of neurological
K, Timm C. 2003. Frontal lobe dysfunction in signs and long-term cognitive and memory
Korsakoff’s syndrome and chronic alcoholism: impairments. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 24: 622-634.
continuity or discontinuity? Neuropsychology 17: Cruce JA. 1975. An autoradiographic study of the
420-428. projections of the mammillothalamic tract in the
Butters N. 1985. Alcoholic Korsakoff’s syndrome: rat. Brain Res 85:21 1-219.
some unresolved issues concerning etiology, Cruce JA. 977. An autoradiographic study of
neuropathology, and cognitive deficits. J Clin Exp descending connections of the mammillary nuclei
Neuropsychol 7: 181-210. of the rat. J Comp Neurol 176:631-644.
Butters N, Stuss DT. 1989. Diencephalic amnesia. In: Danion JM, Zimmermann MA, Willard-Schroeder D,
MAMMILLARY BODIES AND KORSAKOFF SYNDROME 285

Grange D, Singer L. 1989. Diazepam induces a effects of single and repeated episodes of thiamin
dissociation between explicit and implicit memory deficiency on memory in alcohol-consuming rats.
psychopharmacology 99: 238-243. Alcohol 14:81-91.
Eymin C, Kopp N, Laurent B. 1992. Central benzo- Huppert FA, Piercy M. 1976. Temporal context and
diazepine-binding sites in human cerebral familiarity of material. Cortex 12: 3-20.
structures associated with memory processes. Irle E, Markowitsch HJ. 1983. Widespread neuroana-
Dementia 3: 232-238. tomical damage and learning deficits following
Feldman S, Confort N, Chowers I. 1975. Subcortical chronic alcohol consumption or vitamin-bl
pathways involved in the mediation of (thiamine) deficiency in rats. Behav Brain Res 9"
adrenocortical responses following sciatic nerve 277-294.
stimulation. Neuroendocrinology 18" 359-365. Johnson MK, Kim JK, Risse G. 1985. Do alcoholic
Fellgiebel A, Scheurich A, Siessmeier T, Schmidt LG, Korsakoff’s syndrome patients acquire affective
Bartenstein P. 2003. Persistence of disturbed reactions? J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 11"
thalamic glucose metabolism in a case of 22-36.
Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. Psychiatry Res Joyce EM. 1994. Aetiology of alcoholic brain damage:
124:105-112. alcoholic neurotoxicity or thiamine malnutrition?
Freund G. 1973. Chronic central nervous system Br Med Bull 50:99-114.
toxicity. Ann Rev Pharmacol 13" 217-227. Kataoka Y, Shibata K, Gomita Y, Ueki S. 1982. The
Frisch C, Hasenohrl RU, Krauth J, Huston JP; 1998. mammillary body is a potential site of anti-anxiety
Anxiolytic-like behavior after lesions of the action ofbenzodiazepines. Brain Res 10: 374-377.
tuberomammillary nucleus e2-region. Exp brain Kopelman MD. 1995. The Korsakoff syndrome. Br J
Res 19: 260-264. Psychiatry 166:154-173.
Gonzalo-Ruiz A, Alonso A, Sanz JM, Llinas RR. Krazem A, B6racoch6a DJ, Jaffard R. 1995. Effects
1992. Afferent projections to the mammillary of mammillary bodies or mediodorsal thalamic
complex of the rat with special reference to those lesions on the acquisition and retention of a
surrounding hypothalamic regions. J Comp Neurol learning set in mice: paradoxical effect of the
321: 277-299. intersession interval. Behav Brain Res 67, 51-58.
Harding A, Halliday G, Caine D, Kril J. 2000. Degen- Krazem A, Marighetto A, Higueret P Jaffard R.
eration of anterior thalamic nuclei differentiates 2003. Age-dependent effects of moderate chronic
alcoholics with amnesia. Brain 123:141-144. ethanol administration on different forms of
Hayakawa T, Zyo K. 1984. Comparative anatomical memory expression in mice. Behav Brain Res 17:
study of the tegmentomammillary projections in 17-29.
some mammals: a horseradish peroxidase study. Langlais PJ, Zhang SX. 1997. Cortical and subcortical
Brain Res 300: 335-349. white matter damage without wernicke’s enceph-
Hayakawa T, Zyo K. 1985. Afferent connections of alopathy after recovery from thiamine deficiency
gudden’s tegmental nuclei in the rabbit. J Comp in the rat. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 21" 434-443.
Neurol 235: 169-181. Lescaudron D, B6racoch6a DJ, Verna A, Jaffard R.
Hildebrandt H, Muller S, Busman-Mork B, Goebel S, 1984. Chronic ethanol consumption induces neuronal
Eilers N. 2001. Are some memory deficits unique loss in mammillary bodies of the mouse: a
to lesions of the mammillary bodies? J Clin Exp quantitative analysis. Neurosci Lett 50" 151-155.
Neuropsychol 23:490-501. Lescaudron L, Verna A, Jaffard R. 1989. Modifica-
Homewood J, Bond NW. 1999. Thiamin deficiency tions in number and morphology of dendritic spines
and Korsakoff’s syndrome: failure to find memory resulting from chronic ethanol consumption and
impairments following nonalcoholic wernicke’s withdrawal: a Golgi study in the mouse anterior
encephalopathy. Alcohol 19: 75-84. and posterior hippocampus. Exp Neurol 106:
Homewood J, Bond NW, McGregor JC. 1991. The 156-163.
effects of chronic alcohol consumption or thiamine Lhermitte F, Signoret JL. 1972. Analyse neuropsycho-
deficiency on radial-arm maze performance in the logique et differentiation des syndromes amn6siques.
rat. Alcohol 8: 265-272. Rev Neurol 126: 161-178.
Homewood J, Bond NW, Mc Kenzie A. 1997. The Mair RG. 1994. On the role of thalamic pathology in
286 DANIEL BIRACOCHIA

diencephalic amnesia. Rev Neurosci 5" 105-140. release during spontaneous alternation testing.
Mair RG, Anderson CD, Langlais PJ, Mc Entee WJ. Learn Mem 10: 242-246.
1988. Behavioral impairments, brain lesions and Schneider F, Gur RE, Alavi A, Seligman ME,
monoaminergic activity in the rat following Mozley LH, Smith RJ, et al. Cerebral blood flow
recovery from a bout of thiamine deficiency. changes in limbic regions induced by unsolvable
Behav Brain Res 27: 223-239. anagram tasks. Am J Psychiatry 1996. 153: 206-212.
Martin PR, Adinoff B, Lane E, Stapleton JM, Bone Seki M Zyo K. 1984. Anterior thalamic afferents
GA, Weingartner H, et al. 1995. Fluvoxamine from the mammillary body and the limbic cortex
treatment of alcoholic amnesic disorder. European in the rat. J Comp Neurol 229: 242-256.
Neuropsychopharmacol 5: 27-33. Shibata H. 1988. A direct projection from the
Moffoot A, O’Caroll RE Murray C, Dougall N, enthorinal cortex to the mammillary body in the
Ebmeier K, Goodwin GM. 1994. Clonidine rat. Neurosci Lett 90: 6-10.
infusion increases uptake of 99Tc-Exametazime in Squire L, Amaral DG, Press GA. 1990. Magnetic
anterior cingulate cortex in Korsakoff’s psychosis. resonance imaging of the hippocampal formation
Psychol Med 24: 53-61. and mammillary nuclei distinguish medial temporal
Molina JA, Bermejo F, del Ser T, Jimenez-Jimenez lobe and diencephalic amnesia. J Neurosci 0:
FJ, Herranz A, Fernandez-Calle P, et al. 1994. 3106-3117.
Alcoholic cognitive deterioration and nutritional Suarez M, Perassi NI. 1988. Plasma corticosterone
deficiencies. Acta Neurol Scand 89: 384-390. responses to lesions and stimulations of the limbic
Mumby DG, Mana MJ, Pinel JP, David E, Banks K. thalami nuclei, medial mammillary nucleus and
1995. Pyrithiamine-induced thiamine deficiency cingulate cortex. Arch Int Physiol Biochim 96:
impairs object recognition in rats. Behav Neurosci 69-74.
109: 1209-1214. Suarez M, Perassi NI. 1993. Adrenal response to acute
Mayes AR, Meudell PR, Mann D, Pickering A. 1988. stress in mammillary medial nuclei lesioned rats.
Location of lesions in the Korsakoff’s syndrome. Arch Int Physiol Biochim Biophys 101:275-277.
Neuropsychological and neuropathological data Suarez M, Paglini P, Fernandez R, Enders J,
on two patients. Cortex 24: 367-388. Maglianesi M, Perassi N, et al. 1999. Influence of
O’Carroll RE, Moffoot AP, Ebmeier KP, Goodwin anterodorsal thalamic nuclei on the hypophyseal-
GM. 1994. Effects of fluvoxamine treatment on adrenal axix and cardiac beta receptors in rats
cognitive functioning in the alcoholic Korsakoff submitted to variable chronic stress. Acta Physiol
syndrome. Psychopharmacology 116: 85-88. Pharmacol Ther Latinoam 49: 71-78.
Paller KA, Acharya A, Richardson AP, Plaisant O, Suarez MM, Rivarola MA, Molina SM, Perassi NI,
Shimamura AP, Reed BR, et al. 1997. Functional Levin GM, Cabrera R. 2001. Periodic maternal
neuroimaging of cortical dysfunction in alcoholic deprivation and lesion of anterodorsal thalamic
Korsakoff’s syndrome. J Cognitive Neurosci 9: nuclei induce alteration on hypophyso adrenal
277-293. system activity in adult rats. Life Sci 69: 803-
Parkin AJ, B lunden J, Rees JE, Hunkin NM. 1991. 813.
Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome of nonalcoholic Swanson LW, Cowan WM. 1975. An autoradiographic
origin. Brain Cogn 15: 69-82. study of the organization of the efferent connec-
Radyushkin K, Anokhin K, Meyer BI, Jiang Q, tions of the hippocampal formation in the rat. J
Alvarez-Bolado G, Gruss P. 2005. Genetic ablation Comp Neurol 172: 49-84.
of the mammillary bodies in the Foxbl mutant Sziklas V, Petrides M. 1998. Memory and the region of
mouse leads to selective deficit of spatial working the mammillary bodies. Prog Neurobiol 54: 55-70.
memory. Eur J Neurosci 21:219-229. Tako A. 1986. Etude neuropsychologique des amn6sies
Reed L J, Lasserson D, Marsden P, Stanhope N, dienc6phaliques d’origine alcoolique et carentielle
Stevens T, Bello F, et al. 2003. FDG-PET chez la souris. Doctoral dissertation, Universit6
findings in the W ernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. de Bordeauxl, 1-130.
Cortex 39: 1027-1045. Tako A, Beracochea D, Jaffard, R. 1988. Accelerated
Savage LM, Chang Q, Gold PE. 2003. Diencephalic rate of forgetting for spatial information following
damage decreases hippocampal acetylcholine mammillary bodies lesion in mice: a retrieval
MAMMILLARY BODIES AND KORSAKOFF SYNDROME 287

deficit alleviated by a context change occurring rats. Science 209:711-713.


on the retention test. Psychobiology 16: 45-53. Warrington EK, Weiskrantz L. 1974. The effect of
Tako A, Beracochea D, Lescaudron L, Jaffard, R. prior learning on subsequent retention in amnesic
1991. Effects of a chronic ethanol consumption patients. Neuropsychologia 12:419-428.
and thiamine deficiency on spatial working memory Wiit ED, Goldman-Rakic PS. 1983a. Intermittent
in mice: a behavioral and neuroanatomical study. thiamine deficiency in the rhesus monkey, i.
Neurosci Lett 123: 3 7-40 Progression of neurological signs and
Thompson R. 1981. Rapid forgetting of spatial habit neuroanatomical lesions. Ann Neurol 13: 376-
in rats with hippocampal lesions. Science 212: 395.
959-960. Wiit ED, Goldman-Rakic PS. 1983b. Intermittent
Vann SD, Aggleton JP. 2004. The mammillary bodies: thiamine deficiency in the rhesus monkey, ii.
two memory systems in one? Nat Rev Neurosci 5: Evidence for memory loss. Ann Neurol 13" 396-
35-44. 401.
Victor M, Adams RD, Collins GH. 1989. The Yamashita K, Kataoka A, Shibata K, Tominaga K,
Wernicke-Korsakoff’s Syndrome and Related Ueki S. 1989. A key role of the mammillary body
Neurological Disorders due to Alcoholism and in mediation of the antianxiety action of zopiclone,
Malnutrition. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, a cyclopyrrolone derivative. Jpn J Pharmacol 51"
USA: FA Davis. 438-442.
Walker DW, Barnes DE, Zornetzer SF, Hunter BE, Zola-Morgan S, Squire L. 1985. Amnesia in monkeys
Kubanis P. 1980. Neuronal loss in hippocampus after lesions of the mediodorsal nucleus of the
induced by prolonged ethanol consumption in thalamus. Ann Neurol 17: 558-564.

You might also like