Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2017

VOL. 31, NO. 5, 1023–1030


http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1179174

BRIEF ARTICLE

Understanding the role of mind wandering in stress-related working


memory impairments
Jonathan B. Banksa and Adriel Boalsb
a
College of Psychology, Nova Southeastern University, Fort Lauderdale, FL, USA; bDepartment of Psychology, University of North
Texas, Denton, TX, USAD

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Mind wandering has been identified as a possible cause for stress-related working Received 15 July 2015
memory (WM) task impairments following laboratory stressors. The current study Revised 2 March 2016
attempted to induce mind wandering regarding negative, positive, or neutral Accepted 10 April 2016
events using an expressive writing task and examined the impact on WM task
KEYWORDS
performance. We examined the role of mind wandering in understanding the Working memory; mind
impact of life stress on WM. Additionally, we explored the role of thought wandering; stress; thought
suppression on the relationship between mind wandering and WM. One hundred suppression
and fifty participants completed WM measures before (Time 1) and after (Time 2)
the writing manipulation. The writing manipulation did not alter mind wandering or
WM task performance. Time 1 WM predicted mind wandering during the Time 2
WM task, which subsequently predicted poorer Time 2 WM task performance. The
impact of daily life stress on WM was mediated by mind wandering. Trait levels of
thought suppression moderated the impact of mind wandering on WM. Specifically,
higher levels of suppression resulted in stronger negative impact of mind
wandering on WM task performance. Findings are discussed in terms of the impact
of mind wandering on WM task performance.

Understanding factors that influence state fluctuations control failures × personal concerns model (McVay &
in working memory (WM) performance is critical given Kane, 2010) suggests mind wandering reflects an inter-
the stable nature of WM and its role in higher order action between personal concerns triggered by the
cognitive functions (Ilkowska & Engle, 2010). Psycho- environment and failure of WM to control attention.
logical stress from laboratory manipulations Mind wandering can be triggered by priming partici-
(Schoofs, Preuss, & Wolf, 2008) and life event stress pants with sufficient personal concerns such that WM
(Klein & Boals, 2001; Sliwinski, Smyth, Hofer, & fails to prevent mind wandering from occurring
Stawski, 2006) result in decrements in WM perform- (McVay & Kane, 2013). Importantly, this model suggests
ance. These decrements may be a result of mind wan- the extent to which mind wandering occurs is con-
dering (Curci, Lanciano, Soleti, & Rimé, 2013; Klein & trolled by WM. Smallwood and Schooler’s (2006) execu-
Boals, 2001). Mind wandering refers to thoughts tive resource model suggests WM explains how mind
about personal concerns or goals unrelated to the wandering is supported after it occurs, such that WM
current task (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). These resources enable mind wandering. In this view higher
task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs) have been shown to WM individuals may engage in a task and mind
impair performance on lab-based cognitive tasks wander concurrently given sufficient available
(McVay & Kane, 2009) and self-rated performance on resources. However, lower WM capacity individuals
everyday tasks (McVay, Kane, & Kwapil, 2009). lack sufficient resources to simultaneously support
WM has been proposed to be a critical factor for mind wandering and successful task performance.
explaining how and why mind wandering occurs. The Mind wandering may be facilitated by WM in a

CONTACT Jonathan B. Banks jonathan.banks@nova.edu


© 2016 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
1024 J. B. BANKS AND A. BOALS

context-dependent manner such that higher WM positive or neutral events, will experience increased
capacity individuals are more likely to engage in TUTs and subsequent decreases in WM task perform-
mind wandering than lower WM individuals on tasks ance. We hypothesised that the positive writing task
that place a low demand on WM resources, such as would not result in these changes due to the greater
low-perceptual load visual search tasks, due to excess ability of negative information to capture attention
WM resources (Levinson, Smallwood, & Davidson, (Pratto & John, 1991).
2012). Lower WM capacity individuals may lack suffi- The second aim of the study was to examine the
cient resources, even on low-demand tasks, to simul- role of WM capacity in controlling mind wandering.
taneously mind wandering and complete the task. Consistent with the control failures × personal concerns
Although models differ in predictions of when model (McVay & Kane, 2010), our second hypothesis
mind wandering will impair task performance, all was that TUT rates would be negatively related to
agree impairments are likely to occur on tasks WM task performance and Time 1 WM would predict
placing large demands on attentional resources, rates of TUTs at Time 2. Although Time 1 WM perform-
including WM tasks. Randall, Oswald, and Beier ance will be influenced by mind wandering (Unsworth
(2014) demonstrated that higher WM capacity was & Robison, 2016), this initial measurement, prior to the
associated with fewer TUTs, but occurrences of TUTs writing task, can be used as a predictor for subsequent
impair ongoing task performance, with greater impair- TUT percentage and WM performance. Our third
ments on more complex tasks. Multiple studies have hypothesis was that trait levels of suppression
demonstrated impairments in WM due to mind wan- attempts would moderate the impact of TUTs on
dering induced by negative stressful life events WM. Specifically, higher levels of suppression attempts
(Klein & Boals, 2001), state anxiety (Banks, Tartar, & would result in poorer WM performance.
Tamayo, 2015), and negative affect (Curci et al., A third aim of the current study was to examine the
2013). On resource demanding tasks, including the relationship between self-report measures of stress
WM tasks in these studies, the occurrence of mind and WM. Participants completed self-report measures
wandering was sufficient to reduce performance. of experienced daily stressors, life stress, and intrusive
When someone experiences mind wandering, they and avoidant thinking. Our fourth hypothesis was that
may attempt to suppress such thoughts to remain TUTs would mediate the negative relationship
focused on the relevant task. However, attempts to between self-report measures of stress and intrusive
suppress unwanted thoughts consume WM resources, and avoidant thoughts, during the seven days prior
leading to further impairments in relevant task per- to Time 2, and WM.
formance (Klein & Bratton, 2007; Wegner, 1994).
Therefore, suppression or attempts to suppress TUTs
may be partially responsible for mind wandering- Methods
related impairments, rather than mind wandering
Participants
itself. Hence examining the impact of thought sup-
pression may further our understanding of the conse- One hundred and fifty undergraduates (84 females; M
quences of mind wandering. If performance age = 21.28 years) from the University of North Texas
impairments are due to suppression, rather than participated for partial course credit. Participants
mind wandering, it would suggest that mind wander- were randomly assigned to the negative event (n =
ing is not supported by WM resources, as Smallwood 50), positive event (n = 50), or control condition (n =
and Schooler (2006) propose. 50). Twenty-one participants did not return to com-
The first aim of the current study was to manipulate plete Time 2 (85% retention rate). No differences
rates of TUTs and WM task performance via a writing were found on Time 1 measures between participants
task requiring participants to write about either a who did and did not return for Time 2. Three partici-
future negative, future positive, or a neutral life pants failed to fully complete the questionnaire
event. The writing task was designed to induce TUTs measures during Time 2, resulting in a final Time 2
by priming personal goals. WM performance and sample size of N = 126 (negative event condition n =
TUTs were measured during an initial assessment 45, positive event condition n = 41, and control con-
(Time 1) and one week later, following the writing dition n = 40). Sample size was determined using
task (Time 2). Our first hypothesis is that participants power analyses estimating sample size for the
who write about a future negative, but not future writing manipulation and effect of stress on WM,
COGNITION AND EMOTION 1025

with significance level set at p = .05 and desired on-task (option a), task-related interference (option
power = .95. Smallest reported effects for the writing b), or off-task (options c–f). Percentage of probe-
manipulation on WM and TUTs and stress on WM caught TUTs was calculated by summing the
(Curci et al., 2013; partial η 2 = .04; Klein & Boals, number of off-task response options selected and
2001; r’s= −.36 to −.46, respectively) were used to esti- dividing by the number of probes.
mate required sample size. Required sample size Following thought probe response, participants
ranged from N = 94, for the effect of stress, to N = 96, indicated what percentage of their thoughts was off-
for the writing manipulation. Sample size was task during the previous set (between 0% and
chosen to exceed estimated requirements. A sample 100%). Percentage of self-report TUTs was calculated
size of n = 40, per group, provides a power = .94 for by averaging the self-reported off-task thought per-
effects as small as η 2 = .03. centages. Fifteen probes and self-report off-task
thought percentage questions were inserted into
each WM task, resulting in a total of 30 probes and
Measures
self-report TUT percentages during the 2 tasks.
Working memory. Participants completed the Auto- Daily Inventory of Stressful Events (Almeida,
mated Operation Span Task (AOSPAN) and the Auto- Wethington, & Kessler, 2002). The Daily Inventory of
mated Reading Span Task (RSPAN; Unsworth, Heitz, Stressful Events (DISE) is a seven-item scale measuring
Schrock, & Engle, 2005). Participants maintained a the number of daily stressors experienced. Partici-
series of letters in memory while completing a second- pants indicated if an event had occurred in the last
ary task. The AOSPAN task requires participants to 24 hours. The DISE is scored by summing the
verify the correctness of the solution to a simple math- number of items endorsed, creating a possible range
ematical equation ((3 × 2) + 1 = 6). The RSPAN task from 0 to 7.
requires participants to verify the meaningfulness of Life Experiences Scale (Sarason, Johnson, & Siegel,
sentences (“The ship sailed across the dishwasher”). 1978). The Life Experiences Scale (LES) is a 47-item
In both tasks, a capital letter (out of 12 possible checklist measuring the amount of life stress partici-
letters) appears for 250 ms, 200 ms after the reading pants have experienced. Participants indicate if the
or operation verification response. After a set of 3–7 event occurred in the last six months. If it occurred,
verification-letter pairs, a grid containing all 12 poss- they rated the direction (negative or positive) and
ible letters was presented. Participants recalled all degree of the impact on a scale from 0 (no impact)
letters from the set in the order presented. Each set to 3 (extremely positive/negative). Scores were com-
length (3–7 items) was presented 3 times for a total puted by summing impact ratings of negatively
of 15 sets per task. WM tasks were scored by rated events.
summing the total number of items recalled in the Impact of Events Scale (Horowitz, Wilner, & Alvarez,
correct serial position (Conway et al., 2005). 1979). The Impact of Events Scale (IES) is a 15-item
Mind wandering. Mind wandering was measured questionnaire measuring the frequency of avoidant
during the WM tasks, using two measures, probe- and intrusive thoughts experienced during the last
caught TUTs and self-reported percentage of TUTs. seven days about a nominated event. Participants
Thought probes (McVay & Kane, 2009) measured respond to statements (e.g. “I thought about it when
what participants were thinking prior to the probe I didn’t mean to”) on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5
(probe-caught TUTs). Participants were asked, “What (extremely). Items were summed to produce an
were you just thinking about” and selected one of overall intrusion and avoidance score.
five options: (a) Task-related thought, exclusively, (b) White Bear Suppression Inventory (Wegner &
Task performance/evaluative thoughts, (c) Task-unre- Zanakos, 1994). The White Bear Suppression Inventory
lated, neutral content, (d) Task-unrelated, positive (WBSI) is a 15-item questionnaire measuring the
content, (e) Task-unrelated, negative content. During degree to which individuals are likely to engage
Time 2, a sixth response option was added to allow in thought suppression or experience intrusive
individuals to select an option about the writing thoughts. Participants respond to items (e.g. “I
manipulation: (f) Task-unrelated, about the event I always try to put problems out of my mind”) on a
wrote about. Thought probes were inserted into WM scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
tasks following recall of the letters at the end of Items are summed so that higher scores indicate
each set. Thought probes were classified as being greater suppression.
1026 J. B. BANKS AND A. BOALS

Procedure all p’s > .05. A significant effect of time was found on
the TUT composite, F (1, 123) = 24.66, p < .0001,
During Time 1 participants completed the AOSPAN
partial η 2 = 0.16, such that TUTs increased from Time
and RSPAN in a counterbalanced order, with thought
1 (M = 0.00, SD = 0.84) to Time 2 (M = 0.37, SD = 0.98),
probes and self-reported off-task thought percentage
but no effect of condition or time by condition inter-
inserted into each task. Participants then completed
action, p’s > .05. Based on the lack of significant differ-
the DISE and WBSI. Time 2 occurred 2–10 days follow-
ences between conditions on TUTs or WM, all three
ing Time 1. Control condition participants wrote for 2
conditions were collapsed for subsequent analyses.
min about the prior days activities in a non-emotional
manner.
Negative event condition participants nominated WM, mind wandering, and suppression
their worst fear, such as a fear of spiders, snakes, or
To test the second hypothesis that TUTs would be
heights. Positive event condition participants nomi-
negatively related to WM performance and WM per-
nated a possible future event with the best possible
formance would predict TUTs on a subsequent task,
outcome. Participants in the negative and positive
we conducted a correlational analysis. Consistent
event conditions then imagined an event in which
with our hypothesis, WM was negatively related to
they would have to deal with their nominated event.
TUTs at Time 1, r (146) = −.55, p < .0001, and Time 2,
Participants engaged in an expressive writing task
r (125) = −.42, p < .0001. Not surprisingly, WM at
for 2 min in which they imagined how they might
Time 1 was strongly related to WM at Time 2, r (125)
feel experiencing the event and described the experi-
= .77, p < .0001. To determine if the relationship
ence in detail.
between WM at Time 1 and Time 2 was mediated by
Participants then completed the AOSPAN and
TUTs during Time 2, a mediation analysis was con-
RSPAN in the same order as Time 1. Thought probes
ducted using the process macro for SAS (Hayes,
and self-reported off-task thought percentages were
2013). The method provides a bootstrapped 95% con-
inserted into both WM tasks. Finally, participants com-
fidence interval (CI) around the indirect effect. The
pleted the LES, IES, DISE, and a demographics
indirect effect is interpreted as significant when the
questionnaire.
CI does not contain zero, indicating the mediation
effect is different from zero (Preacher & Hayes,
Results 2008). As given in Table 1, a significant indirect
effect was observed, such that Time 2 TUTs mediated
All measures were examined for outliers. Any values the relationship between WM at Time 1 and Time
beyond three standard deviations were replaced 2. Due to the possibility that the relationship
with a three standard deviation value. This resulted between WM at Time 1 and Time 2 was due to the
in replacement of six values on the AOSPAN at Time influence of TUTs on performance at both time
1, six values on the AOSPAN at Time 2, two values points, TUTs at Time 1 were included as a covariate
on the RSPAN at Time 1, and three values on the in the model. The mediation model remained signifi-
RSPAN at Time 2. A composite WM score was cant, indirect effect = 0.04, bootstrapped SE = 0.02,
created by averaging z-scores of the AOSPAN and bootstrapped 95% CI [0.002, 0.11].
RSPAN scores following outlier transformations. A To test the third hypothesis that the tendency to
composite mind wandering score, TUT composite, suppress thoughts should moderate the impact of
was created by averaging z-scores of probe-caught TUTs on WM, a regression analysis was conducted.
TUTs and self-report TUT percentage. Time 1 TUTs, WBSI score, and the interaction
between WBSI and TUTs were included as predictors
of Time 1 WM. The overall model was significant,
Effects of writing task
R 2 = .34, F(3, 141) = 24.45, p < .0001, with the inter-
To test the first hypothesis that writing about a action between WBSI and TUTs serving as the only sig-
future negative event, but not positive or neutral nificant predictor, B = −0.014, t = −2.03, p < .05. Simple
events, will increase TUTs and decrease WM perform- slopes analyses examined the relationship between
ance, two mixed-model ANOVAs were conducted. Time 1 TUTs and Time 1 WM at three levels of the
No effects of condition, time, or time by condition WBSI – the mean and ±1 standard deviation from
interactions were found for the WM composite score, the mean. Analyses revealed a significant negative
COGNITION AND EMOTION 1027

Table 1. Mediation analysis results.


Independent Mediator Dependent Effect of IV Effect of M to Total effect Direct effect Indirect 95% CI for Kappa-
variable (IV) (M) variable (DV) on M (a) DV (b) (c) (c′ ) effect indirect effect squared
WM T1 TUTs T2 WM T2 −0.43**** −0.14* 0.83**** 0.77**** 0.06 0.2, 0.14 0.09
DISE T1 TUTs T1 WM T1 0.08* −0.64**** −0.01 0.04 −0.05 −0.11, −0.01 0.11
DISE T2 TUTs T2 WM T2 0.03 −0.43**** 0.06 0.08+
−0.02 −0.06, 0.03 0.03
IES TUTs T2 WM T2 0.02** −0.44**** −0.004 0.005 −0.01 −0.02, −0.002 0.12
LES TUT T2 WM T2 0.01 −0.41**** −0.01 −0.01 −0.005 −0.02, 0.002 0.05
Note: Values represent unstandardised path coefficients; +p < .07, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, ****p < .0001, Time 1 analysis N = 145, Time 2
analysis N= 126, bootstrap value = 5000 samples for all analyses. Bold values represent the models with significant indirect effects.

relationship between TUTs and WM at lower levels of stress on WM. Additionally, we examined the role of
WBSI, B = −0.49, t = −7.74, p < .0001, mean levels of thought suppression in altering the impact of mind
WBSI, B = −0.62, t = −8.27, p < .0001, and higher wandering on WM performance. The first aim of the
levels of WBSI, B = −0.76, t = −7.59, p < .0001, such study was to manipulate psychological stress using a
that the relationship between TUTs and WM increased writing task. The writing task did not alter WM task
as WBSI increased. performance or mind wandering. TUTs increased
across time points in all conditions, possibly due to a
Self-report stress, mind wandering, and WM decrease in task novelty (Randall et al., 2014).

To test the fourth hypothesis that self-report measures


of stress and intrusive and avoidant thoughts will have WM, mind wandering, and suppression
a negative impact on WM task performance, and this The second aim of the current study was to examine
relationship is mediated by TUTs, a series of mediation the relationship between mind wandering and WM.
analyses were conducted examining the self-report Although the writing manipulation did not alter WM
stress measures and WM and TUT measures at the cor- task performance or TUT percentage, the results
responding time. As seen in Table 1, a significant provide support for the control failures × personal con-
mediation was found such that higher scores on the cerns model (McVay & Kane, 2010). Time 1 WM pre-
DISE during Time 1 lead to higher rates of TUTs, which dicted TUTs at Time 2 and TUTs at Time 2 were
lead to lower Time 1 WM scores. No significant relation- negatively related to WM at Time 2, even when con-
ship was found with the Time 2 measures of DISE, WM, trolling for TUTs at Time 1. This supports the notion
and TUTs. Likewise, TUTs did not mediate the relation- that WM is responsible for preventing the occurrence
ship between LES and WM at Time 2. However, TUTs of mind wandering but when mind wandering does
mediated the relationship between IES and WM, such occur, WM performance is impaired. However, we
that scores on the IES predicted TUTs and TUTs sub- must be cautious in our interpretation since our
sequently predicted poorer WM task performance. Time 1 WM measure cannot be seen as a true WM
Despite the lack of a significant mediation effect for capacity measure due to concerns about TUTs impact-
the LES on WM performance through TUTs, we hypoth- ing WM task performance, thus altering WM capacity
esised LES scores would predict IES scores, which in estimates (Unsworth & Robison, 2016), and the
turn would predict TUTs, resulting in poorer WM per- inclusion of thought probes in the Time 1 WM
formance. We predicted two mediators operating in measure.
succession, IES and TUTs. As seen in Figure 1, a signifi- Further, control over mind wandering may be
cant indirect effect was found, indirect effect= −0.004, context dependent such that it varies based on the
bootstrapped SE = 0.002, bootstrapped 95% CI [−0.01, resources required for the ongoing task. Higher WM
−0.001]. Higher LES scores predicted higher IES scores, capacity individuals report more frequent TUTs
which predicted higher rates of TUTs, which predicted during a low-perceptual load visual search task than
lower WM performance. lower WM capacity individuals, but WM did not
predict TUTs during a high-perceptual load visual
search task (Levinson et al., 2012). Consistent with
Discussion
alternative views (McVay & Kane, 2009), our results
The current study examined the role of mind wander- suggest that during tasks requiring greater attentional
ing as a mediator for the impact of psychological demands, higher WM individuals reported fewer TUTs
1028 J. B. BANKS AND A. BOALS

Figure 1. Mediated mediation model.


Note: Values represent unstandardised path coefficients. The unstandardised regression coefficient for the relationship between LES and WM at
Time 2, prior to controlling for the indirect paths (c) is presented in parentheses. c′ represents the direct path after controlling for the indirect
path. T2 = Time 2. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, ****p < .0001, N= 126.

that lower WM individuals. However, it is important to decrements. The impact of daily stressors on WM
point out that the visual task used by Levinson differs was mediated by TUTs during Time 1, but not during
in nature and difficulty than the task used in the Time 2. This partially supported our hypothesis that
current study. Thus comparisons between the two life stressors increase mind wandering, thereby
studies have to be considered in light of these meth- decreasing WM performance. It is possible our Time
odological differences. 2 findings differed because increased TUTs between
Following the work of Wegner (1994), it was sessions altered the relationship between these con-
expected that suppression of TUTs will negatively structs. Klein and Boals (2001) suggested that the
impact task performance. Although directed suppres- impact of life stress on WM was due to intrusive
sion of thoughts has been linked with a rebound of to- thoughts, as measured in their study with the IES.
be-suppressed thoughts (Wegner, 1994) and Our results did not show a direct relationship
decreases in cognitive task performance (Klein & between the IES or LES and WM, or that IES mediated
Bratton, 2007), our findings suggest that trait levels the relationship between LES and WM performance.
of thought suppression moderate the impact of However, as mentioned by Klein and Boals, IES
TUTs on WM performance. WM performance impair- scores may not be indicative of thoughts occurring
ments are greatest when individuals exert cognitive during the WM tasks. Our results demonstrated the
control over TUTs, to return to the current task. The impact of the IES on WM was mediated by TUTs
suppression of mind wandering is a resource demand- during the task. These results must be interpreted cau-
ing process (Wegner, 1994), requiring WM resources to tiously due the correlational nature of the analyses
suppress unwanted thoughts. Although trait level of and the order in which the tasks were completed.
thought suppression moderated the impact of TUTs The DISE, IES, and LES were completed after the WM
on WM task performance, TUTs remained predictive tasks at Time 2. Mediation analyses require the
of poorer WM performance at low levels of suppres- outcome variable (WM performance) to occur after
sion, suggesting that suppression alone is not respon- all other variables. However, the DISE, IES, and LES
sible for impaired performance. However, we are retrospective in nature, asking participants to
examined suppression at a trait level rather than in report events that occurred prior to the Time 2
response to TUTs during the task. Future work manip- session. Despite being measured after the WM tasks,
ulating suppression of TUTs may provide a better participants reported events that occurred prior to
understanding of the role of suppression on the con- the WM tasks. Thus examining the impact of these
sequences of mind wandering. measures on WM performance is reasonable.
These results appear to add support for the role of
mind wandering in cognitive impairments observed
Stress, WM, and mind wandering
as a result of psychological stress. Given the mixed
The third aim of the study was to examine mind wan- results with the DISE the results need to be interpreted
dering as a factor in stress-related WM performance cautiously. Further, the lack of a direct effect of the LES
COGNITION AND EMOTION 1029

on WM performance suggests experiencing negative Banks, J. B., Tartar, J. L., & Tamayo, B. A. (2015). Examining factors
life events does not automatically result in mind wan- involved in stress related working memory impairments:
Independent or conditional effects? Emotion, 15(6), 827–836.
dering about the event during ongoing tasks.
Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm,
However, when negative life events lead to thoughts O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A meth-
about the event, increases in TUTs occur and impair odological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin and
task performance. Review, 12, 769–786. doi:10.3758/BF03196772
Curci, A., Lanciano, T., Soleti, E., & Rimé, B. (2013). Negative emotional
experiences arouse rumination and affect working memory
capacity. Emotion, 13(5), 867–880. doi:10.1037/a0032492
Limitations
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and
One limitation in the current study was the lack of an conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
effect of the writing manipulation. This may be due to
Horowitz, M. J., Wilner, N. R., & Alvarez, W. (1979). Impact of event
the short duration of the writing task, such that partici- scale. A measure of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine,
pants may not have thought about the event enough 41, 209–218.
to induce sufficient personal concerns to trigger TUTs Ilkowska, M., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Trait and state differences in
(McVay & Kane, 2010). Alternatively, thinking about a working memory capacity. In A. Gruszka, G. Matthews, & B.
Szymura (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in cognition.
future event may be more difficult than thinking
Attention, memory, and executive control (pp. 295–320).
about a prior event. Thus, it may not trigger sufficient New York, NY: Springer.
personal concerns to induce TUTs. A second limitation Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001). The relationship of life event stress
in the present study is the use of only two measures of and working memory capacity. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
WM. Many studies examining mind wandering and 15, 565–579. doi:10.1002/acp.727
Klein, K., & Bratton, K. (2007). The cost of suppressing stressful
WM use three tasks to provide a clear estimate of
memories. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1496–1512.
WM capacity (see McVay & Kane, 2009). Future Levinson, D. B., Smallwood, J., & Davidson, R. J. (2012). The per-
research should replicate the current findings with a sistence of thought: Evidence for a role of working memory
larger number of WM measures. in the maintenance of task-unrelated thinking. Psychological
Science, 23(4), 375–380.
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought:
Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering
Conclusion in an executive-control. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35,
The current findings present evidence for the role of mind 196–204. doi:10.1037/a0014104
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect
wandering in determining the impact of stress on WM executive function or executive failure? comment on
task performance. The impact of multiple indices of life Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008).
stress, including daily stressors and larger life stressors, Psychological Bulletin, 136, 188–197. doi:10.1037/a0018298
appears to decrease WM task performance due to McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2013). Dispatching the wandering
mind? Toward a laboratory method for cuing “spontaneous”
increases in mind wandering during the task. New to
off-task thought. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 570. doi:10.3389/
the current study, suppression moderates the impact of fpsyg.2013.00570
mind wandering on WM such that higher levels of sup- McVay, J. C., Kane, M. J., & Kwapil, T.R. (2009). Tracking the train of
pression attempts are related with a stronger impact of thought from the laboratory into everyday life: An experience-
mind wandering on WM. Our hope is that the results of sampling study of mind wandering across controlled and eco-
logical contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 857–863.
the current study increase our understanding of the
doi:10.3758/PBR.16.5.857
impact of thought suppression, which is critical to the cre- Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-
ation of a unified model of mind wandering. grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling
Disclosure statement strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in mul-
tiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. 891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
Randall, J. G., Oswald, F. L., & Beier, M. E. (2014). Mind-wandering,
cognition, and performance: A theory-driven meta-analysis of
attention regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1411–1431.
References doi:10.1037/a0037428
Almeida, D., Wethington, E., & Kessler, R. (2002). The Daily Sarason, I., Johnson, J., & Siegel, J. (1978). Assessing the impact of
Inventory of Stressful Events: An interview-based approach life changes: Development of the life experiences survey.
for measuring daily stressors. Assessment, 9, 41–55. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 932–946.
1030 J. B. BANKS AND A. BOALS

Schoofs, D., Preuss, D., & Wolf, O.T. (2008). Psychosocial Unsworth, N., Heitz, R., Schrock, J., & Engle, R. (2005). An auto-
stress induces working memory impairments in mated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research
an n-back paradigm. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(5), Methods, 37, 498–505. doi:10.3758/BF03192720
643–653. Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2016). The influence of lapses of
Sliwinski, M., Smyth, J., Hofer, S., & Stawski, R. (2006). attention on working memory capacity. Memory and
Intraindividual coupling of daily stress and cognition. Cognition, 44, 188–196.
Psychology and Aging, 21, 545–557. doi:10.1037/0882-7974. Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control.
21.3.545 Psychological Review, 101(1), 34–52.
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppres-
Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958. sion. Journal of Personality, 62, 615–640.
Copyright of Cognition & Emotion is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

You might also like