Schizophrenia, Smoking Status, and Performance On The Matrics Cognitive Consensus Battery PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Psychiatry Research 246 (2016) 1–8

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Schizophrenia, smoking status, and performance on the matrics


Cognitive Consensus Battery
Alexandra C. Reed a, Josette G. Harris a, Ann Olincy a,b,n
a
Center for Schizophrenia Research, Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado, 13001 East 17th Place, Aurora CO 80045, USA
b
Health Sciences Center, Veterans Affairs Medical Research Service, Denver, CO, USA

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Cognitive deficits and high rates of nicotine dependence are consistently documented in the schizo-
Received 19 December 2015 phrenia literature. However, there is currently no consensus about how regular smoking influences
Received in revised form cognition in schizophrenia or which cognitive domains are most affected by chronic smoking. Previous
21 August 2016
studies have also failed to disambiguate the effects of chronic nicotine from those of acute exposure. The
Accepted 27 August 2016
current study uses a novel approach to testing nicotine addicted patients at a time-point between acute
Available online 9 September 2016
enhancement and withdrawal and implements the MATRICS Cognitive Consensus Battery (MCCB) to
Keywords: compare the overall cognitive performance of regular smokers (n¼ 40) and nonsmokers (n¼36) with
Schizophrenia schizophrenia. Controlling for age, gender, and education, smokers with schizophrenia were significantly
Nicotine
more impaired on a visual learning task, the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R), than
Smoking
their nonsmoking peers. Among smokers, smoking behavior (i.e., exhaled carbon monoxide levels of
MATRICS
Cognition smokers) predicted BVMT-R T score; greater smoking was associated with more impaired visual learning.
Self-Medication Negative symptom severity was not predictive of greater visual learning deficits in smokers or non-
smokers. Future longitudinal research will be required to determine if there is a dose-response re-
lationship between chronic nicotine and visual learning impairment in patients at various stages of
psychotic illness.
& 2016 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction impacting sustained attention (Sacco et al., 2005) and visuospatial


working memory (George et al., 2002).
Individuals with serious mental illness (SMI) abuse nicotine- However, the cognitive effects of chronic nicotine use in this
containing tobacco products at high rates. Experts estimate that population are less clear. Existing cross-sectional studies have
65–90% of patients with schizophrenia are regular smokers (Aubin crucial methodological and sampling differences. The definitions
et al., 2012). The dramatic association between SMI and nicotine of “smoker,” “nonsmoker,” and “former smoker,” change with each
use has spurred research and debate over nicotine's effects on investigation. Studies have employed a wide range of cognitive
attention and cognition. Empirical investigations of both the short- batteries representing diverse cognitive domains, varying in
term and long-term effects of nicotine on cognitive functions have duration, order of testing, and comparability. Differences between
yielded mixed results. One theory suggests that nicotine self-ad- smoking and non-smoking groups on clinical measures of symp-
ministration is a form of self-medication for the negative symp- tomology and severity of illness are inconsistent across studies.
toms and cognitive deficits associated with psychotic disorders Additionally, when to allow the use of nicotine is critical and
(Kumari and Postma, 2005; Evans and Drobes, 2009). varies.
Acute nicotine can improve the performance of patients with One of the first studies to compare the cognitive performance
schizophrenia on cognitive tasks including measures of novelty of smokers with schizophrenia (n ¼ 23, Mage¼ 43.9) to non-
detection memory (Jubelt et al., 2008), spatial organization and smoking outpatients (n ¼ 8, Mage ¼41.5) was George et al. (2002).
verbal memory (Smith et al., 2002), and sustained attention (Barr Smokers were determined to have a Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine
et al., 2008). Withdrawal can induce cognitive impairments, Dependence (FTND) score greater than 5, carbon monoxide (CO)
level greater than 10 ppm, cotinine levels of more than 150 ng/ml
n
(plasma) and 600 ng/ml (urine), and reported smoking at least 20
Correspondence to: Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Denver,
13001 East 17th Place, Aurora, CO 80045, Mail Stop F546, USA.
cigarettes per day. Nonsmokers and smokers were evaluated on
E-mail addresses: alexandraclairereed@gmail.com (A.C. Reed), the computerized Stroop Color Word Test (SCWT) and the Vi-
josette.harris@ucdenver.edu (J.G. Harris), ann.olincy@ucdenver.edu (A. Olincy). suospatial Working Memory (VSWM) task. At baseline evaluations,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.062
0165-1781/& 2016 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
2 A.C. Reed et al. / Psychiatry Research 246 (2016) 1–8

it was unclear when the smokers had last smoked. Nonsmokers nonsmokers with schizophrenia to smokers with schizophrenia on
had significantly higher total scores on the Positive and Negative the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological
Syndrome Scale (PANSS) than smokers and significantly more Status (RBANS). Current smokers (n ¼456) were defined as in-
negative symptoms. In contrast, smokers reported significantly dividuals who smoked at least one cigarette per day for at least a
more depression on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) than year. Nonsmokers (n ¼124) were made up of former smokers (n
their nonsmoking peers. George et al. (2002) observed that smo- ¼20) (individuals who had previously smoked at least one cigar-
kers had better VSWM performance than nonsmokers. However, ette per day and successfully quit for at least a year) and never
this difference failed to reach statistical significance (p¼ 0.20). smokers (n ¼104) (participants who reported smoking less than
These results were negative, perhaps due to the small sample size 100 lifetime cigarettes). Smokers (Mage ¼48.6) were significantly
of the non-smoking group. older than nonsmokers (Mage ¼46.3). Current smokers were al-
To further clarify how chronic smoking affects cognitive func- lowed to smoke before testing and were offered smoking breaks
tioning in adult outpatients with schizophrenia, Wing et al. (2011) during the test session. As in George et al. (2002), Zhang et al.
compared the performance of current smokers (n ¼38, (2012) found that their nonsmokers had significantly more nega-
Mage ¼41.9), former smokers (n ¼16, Mage ¼42.5), and never tive symptoms than their smoker group. However, the study's
smokers (n ¼26, Mage ¼42.5) on a neuropsychological battery. primary finding was that smokers scored significantly lower than
Current smokers had FTND scores of at least 5, CO level of greater nonsmokers on the RBANS total score and two subscales: Vi-
than 10 ppm, and were smoking at least 10 cigarettes per day. suospatial /Constructional and Immediate Memory. The perfor-
Former smokers had quit smoking at least 6 months before study mance differences on the RBANS total score and on the Visuos-
enrollment, and never smokers had smoked fewer than 100 life- patial/Constructional index remained significant even when the
time cigarettes. Both former smokers and never smokers had CO authors controlled for differences in age, number of hospitaliza-
levels that were less than 10 ppm. Importantly, current smokers tions, PANSS total score, negative symptoms, antipsychotic drugs,
were not deprived of nicotine more than 30 min throughout and anticholinergic drugs. These findings are incompatible with
cognitive testing. The cognitive battery was composed of the George et al. (2002), in which smokers outperformed nonsmokers
Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT), the SCWT, and the on visuospatial memory tasks.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) to examine sustained atten- Further investigations comparing smokers with schizophrenia
tion, response inhibition and processing speed, and cognitive to nonsmokers with schizophrenia include a study by Iasevoli
flexibility respectively. Although the schizophrenia groups were et al. (2013) which examined cognitive differences in outpatients
similar in negative and positive symptoms, current smokers had with treatment resistant schizophrenia (TRS). TRS patients were
significantly lower IQ than former smokers. Controlling for IQ, defined as individuals who had not responded to, “at least two or
never smokers demonstrated lower CPT hit rate and slower reac- three antipsychotic agents” (Iasevoli et al., 2013, p 1114). Their
tion time on SCWT (across congruent, neutral, and incongruent nonsmoking group (n ¼28) smoked less than 100 lifetime cigar-
trials) than former or current smokers. The authors came to the ettes and less than one cigarette per day at the time of enrollment
same conclusion as George et al. (2002), that never smokers with and their smoker group (n ¼31) smoked more than one cigarette
schizophrenia have more severe cognitive deficits than current or each day for more than one year. Smokers (Mage ¼37.61) and
former smokers with this disorder (Wing et al., 2011). However, nonsmokers (Mage ¼ 36.67) enrolled were slightly younger than
they did not replicate the neurocognitive findings as they selected previously tested groups. Smoker TRS patients had significantly
different cognitive tests. Furthermore, there were disparate find- higher scores on the total PANSS than nonsmokers. This difference
ings on the one task that was shared between studies (SCWT), was driven by high negative symptom ratings in the smoker TRS
possibly due to differences in sample size and clinical character- patients. Although the authors state that all smoking participants
istics (differences between groups on depression, PANSS and ne- had their last cigarette, “within 1 h from assessments,” (Iasevoli
gative symptoms). et al., 2013, p1115) the PANSS and the Personal and Social Perfor-
A third study, Morisano et al. (2013), used the same criteria for mance Scale (PSP) were completed before the cognitive testing,
their smoking and nonsmoking groups as Wing et al. (2011) except which was assessed with the Brief Assessment of Cognition in
that smokers had at least 15 cigarettes per day. As in Wing et al. Schizophrenia (BACS). Cognitive domains assessed by the BACS
(2011), smokers (n ¼ 32, Mage ¼41.3) and nonsmokers (n ¼15, were the following: verbal memory by the List Learning task,
Mage ¼39.9) were similar for positive and negative symptoms, working memory by the Digit Sequencing task, verbal fluency by
and smoking participants did not go longer than 30 min without the Category Instances task, processing speed by the Symbol
smoking during the cognitive testing. The neurocognitive assess- Coding task, problem solving by the Tower of London task, and
ment included CPT, Trail Making Test, WCST, VSWM, Digit Span, motor speed by the Token Motor task. Smokers and nonsmokers
and the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II). Smoking patients with TRS performed similarly on the BACS with an important ex-
outperformed nonsmoking patients on specific domains of the ception; smokers performed significantly worse than nonsmokers
CVLT-II; smokers correctly recalled more items after short and long in the problem solving domain. Across both the nonsmoker and
delays when semantic cues were provided and when a forced smoker groups, poorer cognitive performance was associated with
choice between words was offered (during recognition). Although more severe negative symptoms. The authors state their results
Morisano et al. (2013) had similar design and sample to Wing et al. may indicate that smokers with TRS schizophrenia are more cog-
(2013), and demonstrated worse cognition in non-smokers with nitively impaired than TRS nonsmokers. Although this is a similar
schizophrenia, they did not replicate the specific finding of lower finding to Zhang (2012), of more severe deficits in smokers, the
CPT hit rate. The authors cite “methodological, statistical and po- samples differed in the distribution of negative symptoms be-
pulation differences between this and other studies” to explain the tween groups and highlighted different domains where cognitive
discrepancies (Morisano et al., 2013, p. 51). deficits were most pronounced.
Cumulatively, the results of Morisano et al. (2013) Wing et al. Based on the current literature, it is unclear how chronic
(2011) and George et al. (2002) suggest chronic nicotine has po- smoking influences cognition in schizophrenia. There is also little
sitive effects on cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. However, consensus over which cognitive domains are most affected by
some suggest that chronic nicotine may further impair cognitive chronic nicotine abuse and how chronic smoking relates to clinical
deficits. In a large sample of male Han Chinese inpatients with symptomology. Most prior studies targeting these questions have
schizophrenia, Zhang et al., (2002) compared the performance of allowed patients to smoke before or during testing to avoid testing
A.C. Reed et al. / Psychiatry Research 246 (2016) 1–8 3

patients when they are in withdrawal; nicotine withdrawal wor- Table 2


sens cognitive performance (AhnAllen et al., 2008; Sacco et al., Antipsychotic Medications of All Participants.
2005; George et al., 2002). However, this methodology introduces
Antipsychotic Medication Smokers Nonsmokers (n¼ 36)
a confound. It obscures the unknown effects of chronic smoking (n¼ 40)
with the known enhancing effects of acute nicotine.
The present study takes a novel approach to finding the “sweet No Antipsychotics 1 2
Risperidone Only 6 10
spot” for cognitive testing with patients who are using nicotine. To
Risperidone, Aripiprazole 2 0
avoid testing participants who were in acute enhancement or Risperidone, Asenapine 0 1
withdrawal, we chose to administer cognitive measures at least Risperidone, Quetiapine 1 0
30 min after our participants smoked their last cigarette. Our aim Risperidone, Olanzapine 1 0
was to test our participants when nicotinic receptors were at a Olanzapine Only 5 8
Olanzapine, Aripiprazole 1 0
stage between acute activation and desensitization. The current
Olanzapine, Quetiapine 1 0
study also employs the Measurement and Treatment Research to Quetiapine Only 3 1
Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia Consensus Cognitive Battery Quetiapine Fumarate Only 0 1
(MATRICS CCB, Nuechterlein et al., 2008) a test battery, which was Quetiapine, Asenapine 1 0
Aripiprazole Only 2 3
specifically designed to aid the development of pharmacological
Ziprasidone Only 1 0
treatments for the neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia (Green Ziprasidone, Quetiapine Fumarate 1 0
et al., 2004). The MCCB is a new gold standard for schizophrenia Paliperidone Palmitate 2 1
research as it affords comparisons of overall cognitive functioning Asenapine 0 2
(composite score) and performance across seven distinctive cog- Lurasidone 0 1
Haloperidol Only 2 3
nitive domains (e.g., speed of processing, attention/vigilance, Fluphenazine Only 2 1
working memory, verbal learning, visual learning, reasoning and Perphenazine Only 3 0
problem solving, and social cognition). The purpose of this study Loxapine 1 0
was to determine whether chronic smoking was associated with Haloperidol, Perphenazine 1 0
Ziprasidone, Thiothixene 1 0
significantly diminished or enhanced cognitive functioning in a
Haloperidol, Risperidone 1 0
sample with schizophrenia. Olanzapine, Lurasidone, Fluphenazine 1 0
Decanoate
Perphenazine, Asenapine, Quetiapine 0 1
Perphenazine, Olanzapine 0 1
2. Methods

2.1. Participants
into groups based on smoking status. Current smokers were de-
Seventy-Six current smokers, former smokers, and never fined as individuals who had smoked at least one cigarette per day
smoking participants with schizophrenia or schizoaffective dis- and had been smoking at least one month (n ¼40). Detailed
order, between ages 20 and 62 inclusive, Mage ¼ 45 years smoking histories were obtained from 92.5% of current smokers
(SDage ¼12.92 years), were recruited from the community, the (Table 1). Former smokers were participants who reported no
Denver VA, and the University of Colorado School of Medicine's smoking within the last month, but previous lifetime smoking of
outpatient psychiatric clinic (Table 1). Diagnoses of schizophrenia at least one cigarette per day for at least a month (n ¼ 8). Never
and schizoaffective were confirmed by SCID-I administration at smokers reported no lifetime nicotine use or quit attempts (n
the screening visit (First et al., 1997). Participants were divided ¼28). Former smoking and never smoking statuses were con-
firmed by exhaled carbon monoxide levels of less than 10 ppm.
Table 1 Due to a small sample size of former smokers, former smokers and
Demographics of Smokers and Nonsmokers with Schizophrenia. never smokers were treated as one group, current nonsmokers (n
¼36), for all major analyses.
Characteristic Smokers (n ¼ 40) Nonsmokers Test for At the time of study enrollment, 3 participants were not on
(n ¼ 36) significance
antipsychotic medications (1 smoker, 2 nonsmokers), 13 partici-
Gender 8 Female 11 Female X2(1, n ¼ 76) ¼ pants were on typical antipsychotics (9 smokers, 4 nonsmokers),
32 Male 25 Male 1.13, p o 0.29 55 patients were on atypical antipsychotics (27 smokers, 28 non-
Diagnosis 29 Schizophrenia 27 Schizophrenia X2(1, n ¼ 76) ¼ smokers), and 5 participants were on a combination of typical and
11 Schizoaffective 9 Schizoaffective 0.06, p o 0.81
atypical antipsychotic medications (3 smokers, 2 nonsmokers)
Age (years) 44.20 7 13.30 45.14 7 12.66 t(74) ¼ 0.31, p ¼
Mean 7 SD 0.75 (Table 2). Only decisionally capable individuals were eligible to
Education 12.65 7 2.26 14.32 7 1.98 t(74) ¼ 3.41 p ¼ participate. Exclusion criteria were current substance abuse or
(years) 0.001n dependence, positive urine drug screen, current use of smokeless
Mean 7 SD tobacco or nicotine replacement therapy (e.g., e-cigarettes, chew,
Cigarettes per 20.08 7 11.23 – –
day
nicotine gum or patch), pregnancy, neurological disorder, history
Mean 7 SD of severe head trauma, and current suicidal ideation. The study
Range 4.00  60.00 – – was approved by the Colorado Multiple Institutional Review
Pack years Board. Prospective participants received a complete description of
Mean 7 SD 22.64 7 20.47 – –
the study, were asked questions to ensure their understanding of
Range 0.06  90.00 – –
Pre-MCCB CO the procedures, and provided written informed consent at the
level screening visit. Patients received compensation for participation
(ppm) 24.83 7 17.47 2.06 7 1.22 – and travel to and from the study was provided if required.
Mean 7 SD 3.00  89.00 0.00 – 6.00 – After consent procedures, clinical assessments were performed
Range
by a research psychiatrist (A.O.). All participants completed the
n
Indicates a significant difference po 0.05, smokers were significantly less Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (Overall and Gorham, 1962),
educated than nonsmokers. the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS)
4 A.C. Reed et al. / Psychiatry Research 246 (2016) 1–8

Table 3
Clinical Symptoms and MCCB Scores of Smokers and Nonsmokers (mean 7 SD).

Measure Nonsmokers N ¼ 36 Mean 7 SD Smokers N¼ 40 Mean 7 SD t df p

Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms


Total SANS Score 4.25 7 2.79 5.28 7 2.99 1.54 74 0.127
Affective flattening 0.67 7 0.99 0.58 7 0.96 0.41 74 0.682
Alogia 0.19 7 0.58 0.35 7 0.74 1.02 74 0.312
Anhedonia 1.94 7 1.37 2.25 7 1.30 1.00 74 0.321
Avolition/Apathy 1.44 7 1.03 2.10 7 1.24 2.50 74 0.015nn
Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale
BPRS Total Score 35.50 7 7.65 36.63 7 9.83 0.55 74 0.577
Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia
CDSS Total Score 3.53 7 3.71 4.00 7 4.15 0.52 72.5 0.604
MCCB Domain Score (age, gender and education corrected)
Speed of Processing (TMT, BAC SC, and Fluency) 38.36 7 11.41 36.28 7 14.15 0.70 74 0.485
Attention/Vigilance (CPT-IP) 36.19 7 13.36 35.30 7 14.16 0.28 74 0.778
Working Memory (WMS-III and LNS) 42.75 7 12.28 40.00 7 13.96 0.91 74 0.367
Verbal Learning (HVLT-R) 41.56 7 10.56 40.60 7 9.66 0.41 74 0.681
Visual Learning (BVMT-R) 46.00 7 10.69 37.43 7 13.36 3.07 74 0.003n
Reasoning and Problem Solving (NAB Mazes) 56.67 7 16.37 51.00 7 13.53 1.65 74 0.103
Social Cognition (MSCEIT) 42.42 7 12.39 38.88 7 12.00 1.27 74 0.210
MCCB Overall Composite (All tests) 36.97 7 12.49 31.35 7 14.66 1.79 74 0.078

nn
SANS subscale Avolition/Apathy did not remain statistically significant after Bonferroni alpha-level correction p o0.01.
n
MCCB Visual Learning remained statistically significant after Bonferroni alpha-level correction p o 0.00625.

(Andreasen,1982) and the Calgary Depression Scale for Schizo- program to produce t-scores, percentiles, and composite scores
phrenia (CDSS) (Addington, Addington, and Schissel, 1990) (Ta- corrected for age, gender, and education level.
ble 3). On the first study visit, participants also completed health
history questionnaires, a current medications list, and provided 2.3. Statistical analyses
urine samples that were screened for recreational drugs and
pregnancy. Statistical analyses were completed using SPSS version 22.0.
Cognitive measures were administered at a second study visit. Significance was set at p o0.05. A one-way ANOVA with a post
Smoking participants were not instructed to abstain or change hoc Tukey HSD test was used to evaluate group differences be-
their normal smoking routine before this study visit. A trained tween current smokers, former smokers, and never smokers on
research assistant recorded vital signs (blood pressure and pulse) our MCCB outcomes. No difference was found between former and
and CO level (Vitalograph Breath CO carbon monoxide monitor, never smokers in the post hoc analysis; therefore we collapsed the
Lenexa, KS) before cognitive testing began. groups and performed other tests on two groups, current smokers
(n ¼40) and nonsmokers (n ¼36). Categorical data were analyzed
2.2. Cognitive measures: MCCB using X2 test and two-tailed independent samples t-tests were
used to evaluate for group differences on continuous demographic
The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, a brief cognitive test variables. Independent two-tailed t-tested were also used to de-
battery specifically designed for use in schizophrenia clinical trials termine group differences on the MCCB total and 7 individual
and cognitive remediation research (MCCB Forms A and B: domains. Although there are 10 subtests on the MCCB, which yield
Nuechterlein et al., 2008), was administered individually to all raw scores individually, several related subtests measuring the
participants. To reduce the potential for participant discomfort and same functional cognitive domains were grouped for analysis. To
nicotine withdrawal effects on cognitive testing, the MCCB was control for multiple comparisons, Bonferroni corrections were
administered within two hours of the participant's arrival. Smok- applied to the resulting seven MCCB cognitive domain tests. The
ing participants who requested a smoke break before the cognitive alpha level was divided by the number of comparisons (i.e., the
testing session were allowed to smoke; however, participants who Bonferroni alpha level was 0.05/8 measures or 0.00625).
smoked were required to wait at least 30 min before beginning the T-tests were also used to determine relationships between
cognitive testing. group (smoking status), gender, clinical symptoms, and cognitive
The MCCB takes approximately 65 min to complete and is test scores. Tests of clinical ratings with multiple domain scores
composed of ten subtests, which map on to seven cognitive do- were also Bonferroni corrected. Relationships between significant
mains. These domains are 1. Speed of processing (comprised of domain scores and carbon monoxide levels were assessed using
Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: Symbol Coding, Pearson's product moment correlation coefficients. The relation-
Category Fluency: Animal Naming, and the Trail Making Test: Part ships between significant cognitive domains and negative symp-
A), 2. Attention/Vigilance (comprised of the Continuous Perfor- toms were also assessed with correlations.
mance Task-Identical Pairs), 3. Working memory (comprised of the
Wechsler Memory Scale Spatial Span and the Letter-Number
Span), 4. Verbal learning (comprised of the Hopkins Verbal 3. Results
Learning Test), 5. Visual learning (comprised of the Brief Visuos-
patial Memory Test-Revised), 6. Reasoning and problem solving 3.1. Demographics
(composed of the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery: Mazes),
and 7. Social cognition (comprised of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Smokers and nonsmokers did not differ in age or gender dis-
Emotional Intelligence Test: Managing Emotions). Raw scores were tribution (Table 1). However, nonsmokers completed significantly
double-checked and corrected by a second rater. Scores from each more years of education than smokers with schizophrenia, t(74) ¼
test were double entered into the MCCB computer scoring 3.41 p ¼0.001. This is consistent with the fact that in the general
A.C. Reed et al. / Psychiatry Research 246 (2016) 1–8 5

population, smoking is associated with lower educational attain- 90


ment (Latvala et al., 2014; Wagenknecht et al., 1990). Although the 80
authors of the MCCB suggest controlling only for age and gender 70
when deriving normative scores for clinical trials for schizophrenia

CO Level (ppm)
60
(Nuechterlein and Green, 2006), all MCCB scores used in this study
50
were corrected for age, gender, and education level. This choice
was made because nonsmokers and smokers had statistically 40
significantly different levels of educational attainment. As there 30
were a number of older people in the sample, we additionally 20
conducted an ANCOVA to assess the effect of the covariate age on 10
the dependent variable BVMT-R and the independent variable
0
smoking status. There was no significant effect of age as a cov- 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
ariate F(1,73) ¼3.024, p ¼0.086, N.S. Thus we felt it was appro- BVMT-R T-Score
priate to include the entire sample regardless of age.
Nonsmoking and smoking participants had similar levels of Fig. 2. Correlation between carbon monoxide levels in schizophrenia smokers and
visual learning as measured by the BVMT-R T score.
negative symptoms (total SANS), psychiatric symptoms (total
BPRS), and depression (CDSS) (Table 3). Smokers were more
symptomatic then nonsmokers on the avolition/apathy subscale of statistically significant differences between former smokers and
the SANS, t(74) ¼2.50, p ¼0.015. However, this finding did not either current smokers or never smokers on BVMT-R T scores
survive Bonferroni correction (α ¼0.01). (p ¼0.081).
Gender distribution did not differ between smoking and non-
smoking groups and BVMT-R score was not significantly different
3.2. Hypothesis Testing
by gender t(74) ¼0.375, p ¼ 0.71. Non-smoking females out-
performed smoking females on the BVMT-R t(17) ¼ 2.23, p ¼0.040
Table 3 compares cognitive domain scores on the MCCB by
and non-smoking males outperformed smoking males t(55) ¼2.33,
smoking status. While smokers achieved lower average overall
p¼ 0.023. There were no significant differences on visual learning
composite scores than nonsmokers on the MCCB, this difference
by gender within groups; nonsmoking male and females per-
was not statistically significant (p¼ 0.078). Average scores across formed similarly on the BVMT-R, t(34) ¼0.10, p¼ 0.921, as did
all of the distinct MCCB domains were lower for smokers than smoking males and females, t(38) ¼ 0.958, p ¼0.34. Nor did females
nonsmokers; however, differences in speed of processing, atten- smoke significantly less than males based on pretest CO levels,
tion/vigilance, reasoning/problem solving, working memory, ver- t(38) ¼1.244, p ¼0.22.
bal learning, and social cognition failed to reach significance. For the current smoker only group, MCCB BVMT-R T-scores
Smokers (M ¼37.43, SD ¼13.36) were significantly more im- were negatively correlated with pretest carbon monoxide levels,
paired than nonsmokers (M ¼46.00, SD ¼10.69) on the visual r(40) ¼  0.32, p ¼0.045 (Fig. 2).
learning domain (BVMT-R task), t(74) ¼3.07, p ¼0.003 (Fig. 1). This Despite the trend-level finding that smokers had more avoli-
finding remained statistically significant when participants who tion and apathy on the SANS than the nonsmokers, higher levels of
were not on antipsychotic medication (n ¼ 3) were excluded, and avolition/apathy in smokers were not associated with greater vi-
it survived Bonferroni correction. sual learning impairment r(40) ¼  0.16, p ¼0.31 or higher CO le-
A one-way ANOVA was used to determine if former smokers vels r(40) ¼  0.16, p ¼0.32. Similarly, higher levels of avolition/
performed differently from never smokers or current smokers on apathy were not predictive of lower BVMT-R scores in nonsmoking
the BVMT-R. Although there was a statistically significant differ- participants, r(36) ¼ 0.06, p ¼0.72.
ence between groups, (F(2, 73) ¼4.94, p ¼ 0.010)), this indicated
the significantly lower scores of smokers compared to never
smokers on the BVMT-R. A Tukey HSD post-hoc test revealed no 4. Discussion

60 The present study examined cognitive differences in non-


smokers and chronic smokers with schizophrenia. Although non-
smoking participants outperformed their smoking peers on the
overall composite and all seven of the unique cognitive domains
* tested by the MCCB, the only statistically significant difference by
smoking status was in the visual learning domain. Smokers with
BVMT-R Scores

40
schizophrenia had more severe visual learning deficits than non-
smokers. Additionally, there was a significant negative correlation
between smoking behavior and visual learning; among regular
smokers, more smoking (indicated by higher CO levels) predicted
greater impairment on the visual learning task. Finally, the present
20 study also found that smokers with schizophrenia had higher
ratings of avolition and apathy on the SANS than nonsmokers,
although this finding did not survive conservative Bonferroni
correction.
Generally, our findings are a departure from some of the pre-
vious literature (Morisano et al., 2013; Wing et al., 2011; George
0 et al., 2002), which suggests that smokers with schizophrenia are
Smokers Nonsmokers more cognitively intact than nonsmokers across a variety of do-
Fig. 1. Comparison of schizophrenia smokers to nonsmokers on the BVMT-R scores mains. However, it is appropriate to be skeptical of studies that
(Mean 7 SD). claim chronic smokers have enduring, “relative enhancements”
6 A.C. Reed et al. / Psychiatry Research 246 (2016) 1–8

compared to nonsmokers as many of these studies fail to dis- cortical DA hypofunction (George et al., 2002).
ambiguate the effects of “chronic” nicotine from the enhancing DA hypofunction may extend to other brain regions including
effects of acute nicotine. Our findings do not resemble those of the prefrontal cortex, which has been implicated in negative
Morisano et al. (2013), Wing et al. (2011), or George et al. (2002), symptoms. The global hypodominergia may explain possible re-
because of a crucial methodological difference; smokers in the lationships between negative symptoms and cognition. Findings of
present study did not smoke within 30 min of cognitive testing negative symptoms in smokers and nonsmokers are mixed. Iase-
and were abstinent throughout the MCCB, while smokers in prior voli et al., 2013 and the present investigation found smokers had
studies received acute nicotine before and during cognitive test more severe negative symptoms than nonsmokers. Other studies
batteries. have found the opposite relationship, that nonsmokers had more
Though it is challenging to directly compare prior studies, as negative symptoms than smokers (George et al., 2002; Zhang
they did not rely on the same cognitive measures or recruit et al., 2012), or no differences between smokers and nonsmokers
identical schizophrenia samples, it is noteworthy that the cogni- (Wing et al., 2011; Morisano et al., 2013).
tive tasks chosen by Morisano, Wing, and George largely measured The studies that found differences between smokers and non-
domains that have been shown to improve with acute nicotine smokers in negatives symptoms chose a variety of methods to
administration (i.e., working memory, attention, and inhibition) analyze the relationships with cognition. George et al., 2002 did
(Barr et al., 2008; Jubelt et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2002). It seems not analyze the relationship between negative symptoms and
likely that allowing participant smoking before and during testing cognition. Zhang et al., 2012 used multivariate regression analysis
influenced smoker's scores on these measures. In addition to our and backward stepwise procedures to determine significant as-
“sweet spot” approach to avoid testing patients during acute ni- sociations with cognitive findings, including negative symptoms
cotine enhancement or withdrawal, the present study also relied and found none. Iasevoli et al., 2013 found that there were weak
on a more comprehensive test battery, the MCCB, to evaluate negative correlations between several cognitive domains (e.g.,
performance across these previously tested domains and untried verbal memory, verbal fluency and working memory) and negative
others. symptoms for their smoking participants. The same weak corre-
The only study we reviewed that did not administer acute ni- lation between verbal fluency and negative symptoms, a more
cotine to its smoking subjects was the study of chronic tobacco use robust correlation between negative symptoms and verbal mem-
in TRS by Iasevoli et al. (2013). Iasevoli and colleagues (2013) did ory, and a significant association between negative symptoms and
not find the difference between smokers and nonsmokers in visual processing speed immerged for nonsmokers in the sample. There
learning, indicated in the present study, because they used a was no significant association between working memory and ne-
cognitive test battery, the BACS, which does not have a test that gative symptoms for nonsmokers. The present study found no
corresponds to this cognitive domain. They did, however, find that correlations between avolition/apathy on the SANS and visual
TRS nonsmokers outperformed chronic smokers on a problem learning on the BVMT-R in smokers or non-smokers. Further in-
solving/executive function task (i.e. tower of London). The current vestigations of the associations between chronic smoking, cogni-
study did not replicate a significant problem-solving deficit in tion, and clinical symptoms are needed to clarify these relation-
smokers on the MCCB, perhaps because our sample was healthier ships. Tobacco smoking has been suggested to increase dopamine
(i.e., not a TRS sample). While the significant domain between release by mesocortical projections as a consequence of choliner-
studies is different, both studies suggest that chronic smokers are gic receptor stimulation by nicotine. However, with chronic ex-
more cognitively impaired than nonsmoking patients with posure, it is possible that nicotine could worsen hypodominergia
schizophrenia. by either presynaptic (i.e., reduced dopamine synthesis) or post-
The present study is also a partial replication of Zhang et al. synaptic (i.e., increase COMT activity and breakdown of dopamine)
(2012), which found that smokers performed significantly worse mechanisms. Whether the fairly chronic sensitization of the ni-
than nonsmokers on the RBANS Visuospatial/ Constructional in- cotinic receptors resulting in down-regulation of the receptor with
dex. The Visuospatial/Constructional index of the RBANS is made regular cigarette smoking makes them function less well, or con-
up of figure drawing and line orientation tasks. The BVMT-R on the tributes to poorer cognitive performance, is uncertain. Comparing
MCCB consists of an array of six figures, which are shown to the smokers and nonsmokers on other physiologic measures influ-
participant for 10 s at a time over three learning trials (Nuech- enced by nicotinic receptors, such as P50 gating or PPI could help
terlein and Green, 2006). After the stimulus is shown, the parti- resolve this issue.
cipant is asked to draw the figures from memory as accurately as The present study had several limitations. Our “sweet spot”
possible, in their correct locations on a blank page. Both the RBANS approach to cognitive testing with smoking participants cannot
Visuospatial/Constructional index tasks and the BVMT-R require fully rule out the possibility of withdrawal effects. Because the
overlapping skills (i.e., psychomotor skills for drawing, spatial MCCB takes about 65 min to administer, most participants were
approximation/estimation, and visual memory). Thus, our finding abstinent at least 95 min by the end of the cognitive testing ses-
of visual learning deficits in smokers is a natural extension of sion. Previous research has suggested that nicotine withdrawal
Zhang et al. (2012). may induce problems with VSWM in particular (George et al.,
Current theories about why VSWM may be more sensitive to 2002; Sacco et al., 2005). However, it is unlikely that nicotine
chronic nicotine than other cognitive domains are highly spec- withdrawal during the MCCB testing in this study caused the
ulative. Chronic nicotine alters dopamine (DA) regulation (George significantly reduced visual learning performance of our smoker
et al., 2002) and VSWM is thought to be, at least partially de- group because similar deficits have been found in chronic nicotine
pendent on prefrontal cortical DA systems (Tan et al., 2014). In- studies where withdrawal was impossible as acute administration
dividuals with schizophrenia typically have deficits in the number was ongoing. For example, Zhang et al. (2012) found Visuospatial/
of nicotine receptors and their functioning. This includes both high Constructional deficits in fully satiated smokers. This suggests that
affinity and low affinity nicotinic cholinergic receptors. Chronic chronic nicotine use, regardless of whether acute nicotine is also
smoking decreases expression of high affinity nicotinic acet- present, is independently associated with impairments on a vari-
ylcholine receptors, which are down-regulated by chronic smok- ety of tasks that involve visual and spatial navigation, recognition,
ing, which then further reduces DA activity (Leonard et al., 2007; construction, and learning.
Park et al., 2000). The current study's observation of greater vi- The present study used CO to assess the intensity of smoking
suospatial deficits in schizophrenic smokers may be, in part due to because it a sensitive indicator, often called the “stethoscope of
A.C. Reed et al. / Psychiatry Research 246 (2016) 1–8 7

smoking cessation” (Bittoun, 2008, p 69). CO has a half-life of ap- Conflict of Interest
proximately 5 h and is often measured to test for smoking ab-
stinence in cessation trials since a regular smoker's CO level will All of the authors declare they have no sources of conflict of
return to a normal nonsmoking level within 24–48 h of abstinence interest.
(Bittoun, 2008). However, CO is an indirect measure of nicotine
and we had to exclude participants using smokeless nicotine, (e.g.
e-cigarette, chew, patch, gum) as CO monitors are unable to detect Acknowledgment
these sources. Although we found a significant correlation be-
tween CO and visual learning outcomes in the current study, fu- Thank you to Dr. Robert Freedman for encouraging this in-
ture investigations should also include a direct measure of nicotine vestigation and editing the manuscript. Special thanks to James
or cotinine to clarify this result. Saunders, Jason Smucny, Emma Lyons, Lindsay Eichman, and Holly
Additionally, the present study did not systematically collect Gindes who assisted with data collection.
data on participant's history of institutional stays. We also did not
assess IQ, although our analysis controlled for educational attain-
ment differences between non-smoking and smoking participants. References
Though the sample included older adults, smoking and non-
smoking groups did not differ in age and neurocognitive measures Addington, D., Addington, J., Schissel, B., 1990. A depression rating scale for schi-
were age corrected. Exclusion criteria included current drug abuse, zophrenics. Schizophr. Res. 3 (4), 247–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964
(90)90005-R.
dependence, and positive toxicology. However, past substance AhnAllen, C.G., Nestor, P.G., Shenton, M.E., McCarley, R.W., Niznikiewicz, M.A., 2008.
abuse was unknown and could have differed between smoking Early nicotine withdrawal and transdermal nicotine effects on neurocognitive
and nonsmoking groups, possibly accounting for cognitive differ- performance in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res 100 (1–3), 261–269. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.07.030.
ences. Although the smokers and nonsmokers were equally dis- Andreasen, N.C., 1982. Negative Symptoms in schizophrenia: definition and relia-
tributed on atypical and typical antipsychotic medications, we did bility. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 39, 784–788.
not specifically compare chlorpromazine equivalents in the two Aubin, H.J., Rollema, H., Svensson, T.H., Winterer, G., 2012. Smoking, quitting, and
psychiatric disease: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36 (1), 271–284. http:
samples as some of the subjects were unable to supply accurate //dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.06.007.
dose information. Thus, there could be dose effects on cognition Barr, R.S., Culhane, M.A., Jubelt, L.E., Mufti, R.S., Dyer, M.A., Weiss, A.P., et al., 2008.
that were not detected. Lastly, our study treated former smokers The effects of transdermal nicotine on cognition in nonsmokers with schizo-
phrenia and nonpsychiatric controls. Neuropsychopharmacol 33 (3), 480–490.
(n ¼8) as nonsmokers; as a result, it is possible our results un- http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301423.
derestimate the relationship between chronic smoking and VSWM Bittoun, R., 2008. Carbon monoxide meter: The essential clinical tool-the “stetho-
deficits. Future studies should strive to include a distinct group of scope”-of smoking cessation. J. Smok. Cessat. 3 (2), 69–70. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1375/jsc.3.3.69.
former smokers to determine whether their cognitive perfor- Evans, D.E., Drobes, D.J., 2009. Nicotine self-medication of cognitive-attentional
mance is similar to never smokers or current smokers and to processing. Addict. Biol. 14 (1), 32–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
continue to clarify a potential dose-response relationship to cog- j.1369-1600.2008.00130.x.
First, M.B., Spitzer, R.L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J.B.W., 1997. Structured Clinical In-
nition and negative symptoms. terview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Patient Edition. (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0,
The present study highlights the association between chronic 4/97 revision). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New
smoking in schizophrenia and a specific deficit in visual learning. York.
George, T.P., Vessicchio, J.C., Termine, A., Sahady, D.M., Head, C.A., Pepper, W.T.,
However, this is a cross-sectional study design that is not meant to et al., 2002. Effects of Smoking Abstinence on Visuospatial Working Memory
infer causality of smoking in schizophrenia. Further longitudinal Function in Schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacol 26 (1), 75–85. http://dx.doi.
studies of nicotine use in schizophrenia are warranted to de- org/10.1038/S0893-133X(01)00296-2.
Green, M.F., Nuechterlein, K.H., Gold, J.M., Barch, D.M., Cohen, J., Essock, S., et al.,
termine whether smoking causes cognitive deficits, exacerbates 2004. Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizo-
cognitive issues, or partially remediates some pre-existing im- phrenia: the NIMH-MATRICS conference to select cognitive domains and test
pairments. By continuing to investigate the mechanisms and time- criteria. Biol. Psychiatry 56 (5), 301–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
biopsych.2004.06.023.
course of nicotine's neurocognitive effects, these longitudinal Iasevoli, F., Balletta, R., Gilardi, V., Giordano, S., de Bartolomeis, A., 2013. Tobacco
studies may yield new targets for cognitive intervention. smoking in treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients is associated with im-
paired cognitive functioning, more severe negative symptoms, and poorer so-
cial adjustment. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2013 (9), 1113–1120. http://dx.doi.
org/10.2147/NDT.S47571.
Funding Jubelt, L.E., Barr, R.S., Goff, D.C., Logvinenko, T., Weiss, A.P., Evins, A.E., 2008. Effects
of transdermal nicotine on episodic memory in non-smokers with and without
schizophrenia. Psychopharmacol. (Berl.) 199 (1), 89–98. http://dx.doi.org/
This work was supported by the National Institute of Health 10.1007/s00213-008-1133-8.
and National Institute of Mental Health (P50MH086383) “Basic to Kumari, V., Postma, P., 2005. Nicotine use in schizophrenia: the self-medication
Clinical Neurobiology of Nicotinic Receptors in Schizophrenia” and hypotheses. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29 (6), 1021–1034. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.02.006.
by Veteran's Affairs Merit Award (5101Cx000145) “Nicotinic Re- Latvala, A., Rose, R.J., Pulkkinen, L., Dick, D.M., Korhonen, T., Kaprio, J., 2014.
ceptors and Schizophrenia.” Funders had no further role in the Drinking, smoking, and educational achievement: cross-lagged associations
collection, analysis or interpretation of data, or the decision to from adolescence to adulthood. Drug Alcohol Depend. 137, 106–113. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.01.016.
submit this study for publication. Leonard, S., Mexal, S., Freedman, R., 2007. Smoking, Genetics and Schizophrenia:
Evidence for Self Medication. J. Dual Diagn. 3 (3–4), 43–59. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1300/J374v03n03_05.
Morisano, D., Wing, V.C., Sacco, K.A., Arenovich, T., George, T.P., 2013. Effects of
Contributors tobacco smoking on neuropsychological function in schizophrenia in compar-
ison to other psychiatric disorders and non-psychiatric controls. Am. J. Addict.
Alexandra Reed and Ann Olincy were responsible for the study 22 (1), 46–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1521-0391.2013.00313.x.
Nuechterlein, K.H., Green, M.F., 2006. MCCB MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery
design, protocol, clinical assessments, data collection, statistical Manual. MATRICS Assessment Inc., Los Angeles.
analysis, and manuscript preparation. Josette Harris was re- Nuechterlein, K.H., Green, M.F., Kern, R.S., Baade, L.E., Barch, D.M., Cohen, J.D., et al.,
sponsible for selecting the MCCB and supervised all neurocogni- 2008. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, relia-
bility, and validity. Am. J. Psychiatry 165 (2), 203–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/
tive assessments. All authors have contributed to and approved of appi.ajp.2007.07010042.
the final manuscript. Overall, J.E., Gorham, D.R., 1962. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psycholo. Rep.
8 A.C. Reed et al. / Psychiatry Research 246 (2016) 1–8

10, 799–812. MATRICS consensus cognitive battery and P50 sensory gating in a Han Chinese
Park, S., Knopnick, C., McGurk, S., Meltzer, H.Y., 2000. Nicotine impairs spatial population. Drug Alcohol Depend. 143, 51–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
working memory while leaving spatial attention intact. Neuropsychopharmacol drugalcdep.2014.06.045.
22 (2), 200–209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00098-6. Wagenknecht, L.E., Perkins, L.L., Cutter, G.R., Sidney, S., Burke, G.L., Manolio, T.A.,
Sacco, K.A., Termine, A., Seyal, A., Dudas, M.M., Vessicchio, J.C., Krishnan-Sarin, S., et al., 1990. Cigarette smoking behavior is strongly related to educational sta-
et al., 2005. Effects of cigarette smoking on spatial working memory and at- tus: the CARDIA study. Prev. Med. 19 (2), 158–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
tentional deficits in schizophrenia: involvement of nicotinic receptor me- 0091-7435(90)90017-E.
chanism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62 (6), 649–659. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ Wing, V.C., Bacher, I., Sacco, K.A., George, T.P., 2011. Neuropsychological perfor-
archpsyc.62.6.649. mance in patients with schizophrenia and controls as a function of cigarette
Smith, R.C., Singh, A., Infante, M., Khandat, A., Kloos, A., 2002. Effects of cigarette smoking status. Psychiatry Res 188 (3), 320–326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
smoking and nicotine nasal spray on psychiatric symptoms and cognition in psychres.2011.05.037.
schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacol 27 (3), 479–497. http://dx.doi.org/ Zhang, X.Y., Chen, da C., Xiu, M.H., Haile, C.N., Sun, H., Lu, L., et al., 2012. Cigarette
10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00324-X. smoking and cognitive function in Chinese male schizophrenia: a case-control
Tan, S.P., Jie-Feng, Cui, Fan, F.M., Zhao, Y.L., Chen, N., Fan, H.Z., et al., 2014. Smoking, study. PLoS One 7 (5), e36563. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036563.

You might also like