Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

This article was downloaded by: [Florida Institute of Technology]

On: 22 August 2014, At: 10:32


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcap20

A Dyadic Longitudinal Model of Adolescent Dating


Aggression
K. Daniel O'Leary & Amy M. Smith Slep
Published online: 07 Jun 2010.

To cite this article: K. Daniel O'Leary & Amy M. Smith Slep (2003) A Dyadic Longitudinal Model of Adolescent Dating
Aggression, Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 32:3, 314-327, DOI: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3203_01

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3203_01

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology Copyright © 2003 by
2003, Vol. 32, No. 3, 314–327 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

A Dyadic Longitudinal Model of Adolescent Dating Aggression


K. Daniel O’Leary and Amy M. Smith Slep
Psychology Department, Stony Brook University

The stability of and dyadic influences on physical aggression in adolescents’ dating


relationships have implications for understanding the etiology of intimate partner vi-
olence and, in turn, prevention efforts. We studied the stability of aggression and
tested a longitudinal dyadic model of psychological and physical aggression in sam-
ples of adolescent males and females who remained in relationships for 3 months.
Physical aggression against dating partners was remarkably stable. Verbal aggres-
sion, jealous behavior, and controlling behavior formed a latent construct psycholog-
ical aggression. Psychological aggression predicted physical aggression both con-
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

currently and longitudinally. Dyadic relations were evident for both psychological
and physical aggression, and these dyadic relations highlight the need for prevention
and intervention incorporating dyadic issues with young dating couples.

Dating aggression among high school students has edly. Based on aggregate data from very large samples,
been examined sporadically since the early 1980s, prevalence appears to peak between 20 to 25 years of age
when Henton, Cate, Koval, Lloyd, and Christopher and then decreases across the latter part of the life span
(1983) first assessed the scope of this problem. The ex- (O’Leary, 1999). Of course, some individuals continue
act percentage of adolescents reporting physical ag- to engage in aggression and even escalate in severity
gression against an intimate partner varies consider- across the age span, making it all the more important that
ably, but such aggression appears to characterize 35% we better understand predictors of aggression over time.
to 50% of teens’ relationships (Cascardi, Avery-Leaf, Prevention programs targeting dating violence have
O’Leary, & Slep, 1999; Foshee, 1996; Henton et al., typically been psychoeducational in nature. They have
1983; Jones, 1987; Kaufman & Jasinski, 1995; Malik, shown changes in knowledge and attitudes, but they
Sorenson, & Aneshensel, 1997; O’Keeffe, Brockopp, have had limited success in changing physically ag-
& Chew, 1986; Roscoe & Kelsey, 1986; Schwartz, gressive behavior and rarely have follow-up (O’Leary,
O’Leary, & Kendziora, 1996). The modal forms of ag- Woodin, & Fritz, 2002; Wekerle & Wolfe, 1999). This
gression include pushing, slapping, and shoving, or lack may be due in part to the extremely limited
what is now called mild aggression. amount of longitudinal research on dating aggression
Partner aggression is an important cause of injury (Wekerle & Wolfe, 1999). One notable exception is the
and mental health sequelae in adult samples (e.g., work of Capaldi (2002), who has evaluated dyadic and
Cantos, Neidig, & O’Leary, 1994; Cascardi, individual influences on partner aggression in a young
Langhrinrichsen, & Vivian, 1992; Straus & Gelles, adult high-risk sample. Research that informs us about
1990). Partner aggression is also a risk factor for some the naturally occurring course of dating violence may
of the most troubling and endemic adolescent health allow us to refine our prevention efforts and direct
problems. Silverman, Raj, Mucci, and Hathaway them to where they might have the greatest impact.
(2001) found that about 20% of the approximately We had several goals for this article. First, we
4,000 female high school students studied reported be- sought to assess the stability of dating violence across a
ing physically or sexually hurt by their dating partners. several-month period in a sample of steadily dating
Further, physical and sexual dating violence was a sig- couples. Second, we sought to better understand how
nificant risk factor for substance abuse, unhealthy psychological aggression, a key risk factor for partner
weight control behaviors, pregnancy, and suicidality. violence in adult relationships, contributed to violence
The adolescent years may be the developmental in teens’ relationships. Specifically, we were interested
phase when individuals are at highest risk for intimate in whether a higher order psychological aggression
partner violence. Between the ages of 15 and 25, the construct would emerge from separate indicators that,
prevalence of physical partner violence increases mark- based on theories of psychological aggression in adult
relationships, might be less interrelated in a teen sam-
This research was supported in part by National Institute of Men- ple. Third, we were interested in how important psy-
tal Health Grants MH 47801 and MH 57985.
Requests for reprints should be sent to K. Daniel O’Leary, Psy-
chological aggression might be in predicting physical
chology Department, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, aggression among teens. Finally, we wanted to deter-
NY 11794–2500. E-mail: K.D.OLeary@sunysb.edu mine the importance of dyadic influences in changes in

314
DYADIC MODEL OF DATING AGGRESSION

physical aggression over time. The introduction to this Coherence Among Measures of
article is organized around these four issues. Psychological Aggression

There have been a number of scholars and investi-


Stability of Partner Aggression gators who have developed classifications of different
types of psychological aggression in adult relation-
The majority of high school students have multiple, ships (see Maiuro, 2001). For example, classification
short-term dating relationships (Slep, Cascardi, schemes have been proffered by individuals working
Avery-Leaf, & O’Leary, 2001). Prior to understanding with battered women (Marshall, 1996), physically abu-
how moving in and out of relationships with multiple sive men (Tolman, 1999), and college students
partners might influence the stability of dating violence, (Murphy & Hoover, 2001). All of these frameworks for
it is necessary to first understand the stability of physical defining psychological aggression are strongly influ-
aggression in steady relationships. The rules of relation- enced by the notion of coercive male dominance within
ships in the late teenage years are still in formative a patriarchical marriage (Dobash & Dobash, 1979;
stages, and, based on the frequency of re-partnering Schechter, 1982). This model emphasizing power and
alone, one would expect that most teens would not control tactics of men posits that physical aggression is
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

choose to remain in relationships they felt were “unsat- just one of many possible ways through which abusive
isfactory.” If physical aggression is stable in dating part- men impose their will and control over their partners.
ners, such aggression may presage more serious forms Based on this framework, a variety of controlling tac-
of partner aggression such as partner abuse as defined in tics (e.g., monitoring partner’s whereabouts, limiting
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor- contact with her friends, belittling her) are thought to
ders (4th ed. [DSM–IV], American Psychiatric Associa- constitute psychological aggression. Because the
tion, 1994; O’Leary & Jacobson, 1997). classes of behavioral indicators of psychological ag-
Stability of physical aggression against married gression have been based on theory first developed
partners has been investigated in both community and with battered women, it is not clear whether these same
clinical samples. Taken together, these studies suggest classes of behaviors (a) occur in teen dating relation-
that (a) partner aggression is quite stable, and (b) more ships and (b) interrelate as meaningfully in teen dating
severe or frequent partner aggression is more stable. relationships as they do in marriages.
Physical aggression in young married couples is al- Research suggests that at least some distinct classes
ready quite stable (O’Leary et al., 1989; Quigley & of behaviors typically considered to indicate psycho-
Leonard, 1996). More specifically, O’Leary et al. logical aggression do occur in college students’ dating
(1989) found that if a man engaged in physical aggres- relationships. Murphy and Hoover (2001) found evi-
sion against a partner in the year before marriage, there dence of four types of psychological aggression that
was an approximately 50% chance that he would ag- can be identified in a college dating sample: hostile
gress during the 1st year of marriage. Quigley and withdrawal (e.g., refused to discuss problems), domi-
Leonard (1996) found that 76% of young married men nation or intimidation (e.g., threatened to harm part-
who were physically aggressive in the 1st year of mar- ner’s friends), denigration (e.g., calling partner ugly),
riage were physically aggressive at some point in the and restrictive engulfment (e.g., asking where partner
next 2 years of marriage. The stability of partner vio- has been). Verbal aggression, such as yelling, although
lence seems to be even higher among married men se- not identified as a type of psychological aggression by
lected for frequent or severe partner aggression. Murphy and Hoover, is perhaps the most commonly
Among these men, 93% remained aggressive across a assessed potential type of psychological aggression in
2-year period (Jacobson, Gottman, Gortner, Berns, & college dating relationships (e.g., Riggs & O’Leary,
Shortt, 1996). These findings may suggest that partner 1996). However, existing research does not speak to
violence might be less stable in teen dating relation- the higher order concordance of these behavioral
ships, where exiting the relationship may be easier. classes for adolescents’ behavior or whether these be-
However, our own research indicated remarkable con- haviors relate to physical aggression in dating relation-
sistency in the extent of physical aggression across ships as they do in marriages.
partners during the high school years (Cano, To address these questions, we assessed three
Avery-Leaf, Cascardi, & O’Leary, 1998; Chase, classes of behavior that are indicators of psychological
Treboux, O’Leary, & Strasberg, 1998). We do not have abuse in the battering literature (O’Leary, 1999). These
much data to indicate how stable physical aggression is included controlling behaviors (e.g., tried to keep my
with the same dating partner. If the stability of dating partner from talking with his or her friends), jealous
aggression is much lower in dating than marital rela- behaviors (e.g., checked up on my partner’s where-
tionships, this may indicate that etiological influences abouts), and verbal aggression (e.g., yelling). Overall,
may be more “in flux” and potentially more malleable we hypothesized a model in which these three con-
in teens than in adults. structs could be subsumed under one overall latent

315
O’LEARY AND SLEP

construct for both boys and girls. In turn, we predicted tors of physical partner aggression. With 1,307 male
that, as is the case in marital relationships (e.g., and 1,477 female Navy recruits, White, Merrill, and
O’Leary, Malone, & Tyree, 1994), one’s psychological Koss (2001) found the background-situational model
aggression would predict one’s physical aggression accounted for 67% of the variance in the men’s physi-
both concurrently and longitudinally. cal aggression and 55% of the variance in women’s
physical aggression. Partners’ verbal aggression was
the single best predictor of physical aggression, and
Psychological Aggression as a partners’ physical aggression was the second best pre-
Predictor of Physical Aggression in dictor. Psychological or verbal aggression is also
Teen Dating Relationships among the most robust predictors of cross-sectional
physical partner aggression in high school students
Past multivariate research has shown that many fac- (e.g., Cano et al., 1998). Thus, we were particularly in-
tors are correlated cross-sectionally with the use of terested in the role of psychological aggression be-
physical aggression against a dating partner. Most of tween partners as a longitudinal predictor of physical
this research has been with college students and young aggression in high school relationships.
adults, but the research has relevance for the prediction
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

of physical aggression in teens’ dating relationships.


Further, the multivariate studies allow one to evaluate Dyadic Patterns in Partner Aggression
the relative value of different variables to predict partner
aggression. Variables found to be predictive include the Although there is strong evidence that partners’ ver-
following: violence in the family of origin (Barnett & bal and physical aggression are important predictors of
Fagan, 1993; Reitzel-Jaffe & Wolfe, 2001; Widom, concurrent aggression, understanding potential dyadic
1989), family conflict (Andrews, Foster, Capaldi, & influences on dating has not been a priority. The dating
Hops, 2000), antisocial behavior in childhood and ado- violence research has been heavily influenced by the
lescence (Andrews et al., 2000; Capaldi, Dishion, battering literature, which understandably emphasizes
Stoolmiller, & Yoerger, 2001), relationship dissatisfac- the power and control tactics of men within a patriar-
tion (Makepeace, 1989; Riggs & O’Leary, 1996), atti- chal society as the key issue in partner violence
tudes supporting the use of physical aggression (Cano et (Dobash & Dobash, 1979). Some propose that physical
al., 1998; Malik et al., 1997; Reitzel-Jaffee & Wolfe, aggression in marriages should not be addressed in a
2001; Slep et al., 2001), hostile talk about women with dyadic context, because the aggression or abuse is the
friends or negative peer associations (Capaldi et al., man’s problem, “not the relationship’s or his partner’s”
2001; Reitzel-Jaffee & Wolfe, 2001), and verbal con- (for debate see McMahon & Pence, 1996, vs. O’Leary,
flicts with the partner (O’Keefe, 1997; Riggs & 1996). This view, though especially warranted in part-
O’Leary, 1996). ner violence cases consistent with the construct of pa-
Our own theoretical framework is the back- triarchal terrorism (Johnson, 1995), may be less appro-
ground-situational model of dating aggression (Riggs & priate in cases of what has been termed by Johnson
O’Leary, 1989, 1996), initially developed for college “common couple violence.”
dating relationships. Background factors such as expo- In fact, there is considerable evidence for dyadic in-
sure to interpartner aggression, parent to child aggres- fluence on partner aggression among young married
sion, and prior aggressive behavior place an individual couples. Relationship discord and communication
at risk for aggressive behavior against a partner. Later, problems have been repeatedly documented in part-
situational factors that increase conflict in a relation- ner-aggressive couples (Pan, Neidig, & O’Leary,
ship, such as verbal aggression and jealousy, increase 1994). Further, partner aggression most frequently oc-
the probability of physical aggression. Because risk curs in the context of an argument (Cascardi & Vivian,
from background factors is established early on, these 1995; Dobash & Dobash, 1984; O’Leary, 1993). It may
influences are less amenable to change through preven- be that for younger couples in fluid relationships,
tion efforts administered during high school. In contrast, dyadic influences are relatively more important than
situational factors within the context of early dating re- they are for couples in long-established marriages in
lationships may be especially powerful leverage points which power dynamics are more established. If dyadic
for intervention. In the early stages of a relationship and influences on teen dating aggression are substantial, it
early in one’s dating history, it is likely that individuals’ may suggest that prevention programs focus on rela-
responses to situational triggers are less practiced and, tionship factors as well as individual factors. Among
therefore, less automatic (Bargh & Chartrand, 2000). adolescent couples, we predicted that there would be
The background-situational model (Riggs & strong dyadic associations between partners’ psycho-
O’Leary, 1989) has been evaluated in several samples logical and physical aggression.
of young adults, and psychological partner aggression In summary, we hypothesized that relations be-
has consistently been one of the most powerful predic- tween Time 1 and Time 2 behavior would be signifi-

316
DYADIC MODEL OF DATING AGGRESSION

cant and sizable, reflecting the overall stability of ag- health education in the Spring 1995 or Fall 1996 se-
gression. Further, we hypothesized a model (see Figure mesters and were participants in a longitudinal study to
1) wherein each partner’s verbal aggression, control evaluate the efficacy of a dating violence prevention
tactics, and jealous tactics form a latent construct of program (an intervention vs. control comparison).
psychological aggression that directly and specifically Seven Suffolk County, New York, high schools were
predicts each partner’s concurrent physical aggression. selected on the basis of racial and ethnic diversity and
Further, each partner’s Time 1 psychological aggres- interest in the project. The average per-pupil expendi-
sion was hypothesized to predict their physical aggres- ture rate across the seven schools was $8,889 (range:
sion 3½ months later through its relation with concur- $7,107 to $10,776); the average dropout rate was 4.5%
rent physical aggression. We predicted significant (range: 3.1 to 5.8). Participation in the study was vol-
stability in physical aggression from Time 1 to Time 2. untary and passive parental consent was obtained; no
Based on the White et al.’s (2001) research, we pre- parents refused their children’s participation and no
dicted that the physical aggression of the partner at adolescents refused to participate. More specifically,
Time 1 would be strongly related to one’s own Time 2 letters were sent home to parents indicating that a team
physical aggression. With some exceptions, such as ex- from the university was conducting an evaluation of a
posure to weapons and violent injury for boys (Malik curriculum to assess the impact of the program on pre-
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

et al., 1997), the variables that predict perpetration of venting dating violence. The curriculum was part of
physical aggression have been very similar for both the regular health education program, and because the
sexes (e.g., Riggs & O’Leary, 1986; White et al., research was evaluating a curriculum and survey data
2001). Thus, we hypothesized the same overall model were not identifiable, passive consent was possible.
for boys and girls. However, we modeled physical ag- During our teacher training, teachers and guidance
gression by boys and girls separately because male and counselors were alerted that there might be students
female aggression may have different dynamics. who would seek them out following discussions of dat-
ing aggression, and, in turn, referral sources outside the
school were given to guidance counselors. Assessment
Method
procedures and protections are detailed later. In addi-
tion, as part of the research, the responses of students in
Participants
physically aggressive relations to dating violence were
Participants were 206 high school students, 86 boys assessed (Watson, Cascardi, Avery-Leaf, & O’Leary,
and 120 girls. All students were enrolled in mandatory 2001). In brief, many students reported that they talked

Figure 1. Hypothesized model. Only paths indicated with a solid line were tested in the Stage 1 (isolated stability) model. All paths were in-
cluded in the Stage 2 model.

317
O’LEARY AND SLEP

with peers about dating aggression after the program tered surveys. These monitors were available to ex-
but few talked with guidance counselors or reported plain survey instructions, answer student questions in a
aggression to authorities. standardized manner, and appropriately respond to any
Our model was evaluated for both boys and girls. To students who were distressed or wanted to seek help as
be included in these analyses, participants had to report a result of the assessments. No students ever became
that they were in the same relationships at the initial as- noticeably upset by the assessments.
sessment and at the post-assessment (approximately
14 weeks later), and they had to be from control
Measures
schools. These restrictions ensured that all participants
completed the behavioral measures about the same Modified Conflict Tactics Scale (mCTS; Pan et
partners at both assessments and that the longitudinal al., 1994). The mCTS, a modified version of the
relations were not influenced by participation in a dat- CTS (Straus, 1979), is a 19-item instrument measuring
ing violence prevention program. The 14-week fol- an individual’s means of resolving conflict during the
low-up period resulted from practical and theoretical course of a disagreement with his or her partner.
concerns. First, in most schools, health is a one-semes- Examples of verbal aggression items are “Have you
ter course, and a 14-week assessment lag was the lon- done or said something to spite your boyfriend/girl-
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

gest lag we could build in that would still enable us to friend?” and “Have you insulted or sworn at your boy-
survey students while in class. Second, to evaluate the friend or girlfriend?” Physical aggression items in-
impact of the prevention program on aggressive behav- clude “Have you thrown something at your
ior, we determined that a 3-month postprogram lag was boyfriend/girlfriend?” and “Have you kicked, bit, or
minimally sufficient to detect short-term program ef- hit your boyfriend/girlfriend?” Respondents indicate
fects. These participants are a subset of 2,320 students how often each item occurred on a 5-point scale rang-
included in a psychometric study of the Conflict Tac- ing from 1 (never) to 5 (very often).
tics Scale (CTS; Cascardi et al., 1999) based on the ini- The mCTS differs from the CTS in that the mCTS
tial assessment data from the overall project. It should contained two items that were not in the CTS, namely,
be emphasized that the number of participants herein “Have you physically restrained your partner?” and
includes only those students who continued to date the “Have you raped your partner?” This later item and the
same partner 14 weeks after the initial assessment. item “Have you used a knife or gun against your part-
This subset includes 27.6% of the boys and 38.0% of ner?” were excluded in our current use of the mCTS
the girls who attended both assessments at control because school administrators requested their removal.
schools, reflecting the brevity and instability of rela- Given the instability and varying duration of dating re-
tionships among high school students. lationships, modifications to the most typical adminis-
Participant characteristics appear in Table 1. tration of the CTS were made. At the initial assess-
Briefly, the average age was approximately 16.5 years ment, students were asked to rate each CTS behavior
for both boys and girls. Almost all of the students were for themselves and their dating partners for the entire
in 11th or 12th grade, with the majority in the 11th duration of their current dating relationship. At the
grade. On average, these participants had started dating time of the follow-up assessment, students were asked
at the age of 12 and had had four to six dating partners. to rate each CTS behavior since the last time they com-
Most participants characterized their current relation- pleted the questionnaires (with the date of the prior as-
ships as “steady” or “serious.” Approximately 17% of sessment given). Also, to allow comparability of re-
male participants and 31% of female participants re- ports given the differing time periods students were
ported physically aggressing against their current dat- reporting on, response options on the CTS were modi-
ing partners, whereas 24% of male participants and fied from the more typical frequency count to a more
22% of female participants reported that their current global 5-point rating scale (from 1 [never] to 5 [very
dating partners had physically aggressed against them. often]). The self-reported verbal aggression (Items 4 to
Finally, there was greater ethnic diversity among our 8, excluding “cried”) and physical aggression (Items 9
female than our male participants; slightly less than to 18) indexes were used in this study. The
70% of male participants and 60% of female partici- psychometrics of this format of the mCTS, using data
pants described themselves as Caucasian. from the same study as reported herein, are described
in detail by Cascardi et al. (1999). Briefly, there is sup-
port roughly for comparable factor structures of the
Procedure
scale for both boys’ and girls’ perpetration of and vic-
Students completed all questionnaires during the timization of dating aggression, although factors were
pre- and postprogram assessments as part of a larger not invariant across all conditions. Taken as a whole,
survey associated with the program evaluation. Sur- the Cascardi et al.’s (1999) results suggest that this
veys were completed within a 40-min period in health modified version of the CTS has psychometric proper-
classes. Trained graduate research assistants adminis- ties consistent with those of the original CTS. Verbal

318
DYADIC MODEL OF DATING AGGRESSION

Table 1. Sample Characteristics


Boys Girls

Mean age in years (SD) 16.51 (0.70) 16.40 (0.73)


Current grade in school
10th grade 1.2% 0.8%
11th grade 67.4% 77.7%
12th grade 31.4% 21.5%
Mean age when first dated in years (SD) 12.38 (2.17) 12.69 (2.16)
Total number of dating relationships (SD) 4.71 (3.31) 5.97 (6.41)
Seriousness of current relationship (at pre)
New 13.7% 9.5%
Casual 0% 6.3%
Steady 47.1% 42.1%
Serious 37.3% 40.0%
Engaged 2.0% 2.1%
Prevalence of physical aggression in
current relationship (at pre)
Aggressed against partner 17.4% 30.6%
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

Victimized by partner 24.4% 22.3%


Race/ethnicity
Caucasian 69.8% 59.2%
African American 12.8% 17.5%
Hispanic/Latino 7.0% 9.2%
Asian American 4.7% 3.3%
Mixed race 5.8% 8.3%
Other 0% 2.5%

aggression and physical aggression factor scores were examples of jealous tactics are as follows: “I was jeal-
quite strongly related for boys and strongly related for ous and suspicious of my boyfriend’s/girlfriend’s
girls; aggression scores are significantly related with friends,” and “I accused my boyfriend/girlfriend of
other measures of psychological aggression; and phys- seeing another girl/boy.” The measure yields a jealous
ical aggression scores are significantly, though mod- tactics score and a control tactics score for the respon-
estly, related to several measures of attitudes accepting dent and his or her partner. Due to time restrictions for
of dating aggression (Slep et al., 2001). the administration of the assessment, only conceptu-
ally distinct items with factor loadings greater than .38
Dominating and Jealous Tactics Scale. An ab- in the Kaisan and Painter (1992) factor analysis were
breviated version of the Psychological Maltreatment of retained for this study.
Women Inventory (Tolman, 1989) was used to assess Internal consistency of the dominance scale was .72
both dominance and jealous behaviors. Although the (alpha) and the internal consistency of the jealousy
Maltreatment Inventory has been used primarily with scale was .76 (alpha) in a sample of 280 high school
men and women in moderate to severely abusive rela- students who had dating experience (Cano et al.,
tionships, Kasian and Painter (1992) had 1,600 college 1998).
students complete the measure, and they identified six
factors in the Maltreatment Inventory. Two of those
factors, Control and Jealousy, were used in this study. Results
The Control and Jealous scales were among the stron-
gest correlates of self-reported physical aggression, as- Prior to testing the proposed models with structural
sessed with a modified version of the CTS, in an urban equation modeling, all data were examined for
sample of adolescents ages 10 to 14 years (O’Brien, univariate and multivariate normality. As expected,
Cascardi, & Avery-Leaf, 1999). several variables of interest were significantly skewed,
The 22-item Dominance and Jealous Tactics Scale leptokurtic, or both, and mulitvariate normality indica-
assesses the frequency with which both the respondent tors (i.e., Mardia’s coefficients) were highly signifi-
and his or her dating partner engaged in seven control cant. Because structural equation modeling is paramet-
and four jealous tactics in their current relationship ric and has been shown to be sensitive to extreme
(Time 1) or the last assessment (Time 2). Two exam- violations of normality (e.g., West, Finch, & Curran,
ples of control tactics are as follows: “I tried to keep 1995), it was necessary to transform variables.
my boyfriend/girlfriend from seeing or talking to Square-root transformations normalized verbal ag-
his/her family” and “I monitored my partner’s time and gression and jealous tactics variables. Controlling tac-
made him/her account for his/her whereabouts.” Two tics variables and all physical aggression variables re-

319
O’LEARY AND SLEP

quired inverse transformations. Transformation nor- .01) and girls’ (r = .62, p < .01) reports of their partners’
malized controlling tactics variables for all partici- physical aggression.
pants. For girls, two physical aggression variables (i.e., Next, stability was examined by looking at the con-
physical aggression against partner at Time 1 and ditional probabilities of patterns of stable and unstable
physical aggression by partner at Time 2) were normal- physical aggression status across the two time points.
ized by transformation. The other two physical aggres- Conditional probabilities were calculated by dividing
sion variables, although substantially improved, re- the number of people who met the target condition
mained nonnormal. For boys, all four physical (e.g., physical aggression at Time 2) by the number of
aggression variables were improved substantially but people who met the group definition (e.g., physical ag-
remained significantly nonnormal. For ease of inter- gression at Time 1). For boys, conditional probability
pretation, inverse transformed variables were reflected of Time 2 physical aggression given Time 1 physical
to preserve the original direction of the variables. Final aggression was .47; for girls, it was .78. In contrast, the
Mardia’s coefficients remained significant for both conditional probability of physical aggression at Time
boys and girls. Two approaches to modeling 2, given no physical aggression at Time 1, was .06 for
nonnormal data appear to result in satisfactory results: boys and .15 for girls. These data show that, for both
robust maximum likelihood estimation (including the boys and girls, there was a much greater likelihood of
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

Satorra–Bentler chi-square and robust standard errors engaging in physical aggression at the second assess-
for evaluating the significance of parameter estimates) ment if they had engaged in physical aggression at the
and asymptotic distribution free estimation (West et initial assessment. Conversely, if boys and girls did not
al., 1995). Because asymptotic distribution free meth- engage in physical aggression at the initial assessment,
ods require very large (i.e., N > 1,000) to work well, we they were not very likely to do so at the second assess-
selected robust maximum likelihood estimation, avail- ment.
able in EQS modeling software (Bentler, 1995). Ro- Stability was also assessed by looking at the overall
bust estimation methods are not available for level of self-reported physical aggression at Time 1 and
multigroup analyses in the most recently released ver- at Time 2 using repeated measures t tests. There was no
sion of EQS (i.e., EQS 5.7b). Thus, because we were significant difference in the levels of physical aggres-
using robust maximum likelihood estimation methods sion across time for either boys (Time 1 mean = 1.42,
to model our nonnormal data from a moderately sized Time 2 mean = 1.34; t = .55; df = 14, ns) or girls (Time 1
sample, it was necessary to evaluate the models for mean = 1.44, Time 2 mean = 1.47; t = –.58. df = 36, ns).
males and females separately. If unconstrained by nor-
mality considerations, models for boys and girls could
Model Testing
have been evaluated in tandem in a multigroup analy-
sis. However, the nonnormality of the data led us to pri- Models were tested with the EQS program (Bentler,
oritize robust estimation of effect sizes. 1995). Several indexes are commonly used to deter-
As a final step prior to model testing, all zero-order mine goodness of fit. Because our data were
associations were examined. A full correlation matrix multivariately nonnormal, we used the scaled or
appears in Table 2. Generally, medium-to-strong asso- Satorra–Bentler chi-square statistic to evaluate model
ciations are found among the variables for both boys fit, as it corrects for the tendency of the chi-square sta-
and girls (Cohen, 1977), supporting the evaluation of tistic to inflate in the presence of nonnormal indicators
our proposed models. (Curran, West, & Finch, 1996). The standardized root
mean square residual also appears to perform well un-
der all distributional conditions (Hu & Bentler, 1998).
Stability Analyses
Other standard and robust fit indexes were also used to
Stability was evaluated in three ways in this sample evaluate the models (average off-diagonal standard-
of steadily dating partners: (a) correlations of physical ized residuals, the standardized root mean square re-
aggression at Time 1 with physical aggression at Time sidual, the root mean square error of approximation,
2; (b) conditional probabilities (the probability of the Bentler–Bonnett Normed Fit Index [NFI] and
self-reported physical aggression at Time 2, given Nonnormed Fit Index [NNFI], and the comparative fit
self-reported physical aggression at Time 1); and (c) index [CFI]). In addition to goodness of fit, signifi-
mean levels of aggression at Time 1 compared to mean cance testing of the paths was conducted using z
levels of physical aggression at Time 2. scores. When data are nonnormal, a scaled z score (z
As is evident in Table 2, the 3-month stability coef- score robust) is used. This adjusts the standard error for
ficients (i.e., Pearson correlations computed with in- nonnormality.
versely transformed variables) were quite high for the As a first step in testing the models, we evaluated
self-report of physical aggression by boys (r = .55, p < measurement models for both boys and girls. These
.01) and girls (r = .57, p < .01). Moreover, the stability models specified a self-psychological aggression fac-
coefficients were similarly high for boys’ (r = .69, p < tor and a partner psychological aggression factor. The

320
DYADIC MODEL OF DATING AGGRESSION

Table 2. Correlations Among Psychological and Physical Aggression Measures


Phys Agg Phys Vict Phys Agg Phys Vict
Verb Agg Jeal Agg Cont Agg Verb Vict Jeal Vict Cont Vict (pre) (pre) (f-u) (f-u)

Verb Agg — 0.55** 0.57** 0.81** 0.44** 0.40** 0.46** 0.51** 0.45** 0.44**
Jeal Agg 0.31** — 0.39** 0.41** 0.55** 0.33* 0.23* 0.23* 0.25* 0.21*
Cont Agg 0.54** 0.46** — 0.50** 0.53** 0.79** 0.31* 0.40** 0.41** 0.37**
Verb Vict 0.80** 0.36** 0.58** — 0.43** 0.54** 0.52** 0.69** 0.61** 0.50**
Jeal Vict 0.43** 0.54** 0.50** 0.48** — 0.52** 0.09 0.37** 0.39** 0.13
Cont Vict 0.41** 0.43** 0.81** 0.53** 0.50** — 0.26* 0.41** 0.42** 0.29*
Phys Agg (pre) 0.61** 0.17 0.37** 0.52** 0.25* 0.36** — 0.73** 0.55** 0.69**
Phys Vict (pre) 0.44** 0.21* 0.34** 0.57** 0.26* 0.45** 0.78** — 0.77** 0.62**
Phys Agg (f-u) 0.35** 0.21* 0.22* 0.43** 0.16 0.33** 0.57** 0.67** — 0.81**
Phys Vict (f-u) 0.46** 0.23* 0.25* 0.41** 0.20* 0.28* 0.69** 0.62** 0.85** —

Note: Correlations for boys (n = 86) appear in the upper right and girls (n = 120) in the lower left diagonals. Verb = verbal tactics; Jeal = jealous
tactics; Cont = controlling tactics; Phys = physical tactics; Agg = self-reported aggression; Vict = self-reported victimization; Pre = initial assess-
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

ment; f-u = 14 week follow-up assessment.


*p < .05. **p < .001.

loadings of the verbal aggression indicators on each of low-up remained free to covary. The variances of latent
these latent factors were fixed to 1.0. These factors variables and residuals were also all free to vary.
were free to covary. The CTS and Dominating and To enable us to identify the most parsimonious ver-
Jealous Tactics Scale administer items in couplets, ask- sion of our model consistent with the data, we tested
ing about how often the respondent committed a given the models in a two-stage process. In the first stage, we
act against his or her partner and then how often the evaluated an isolated stability model, as represented by
partner committed the same act against the respondent, the solid lines in the model diagrammed in Figure 1
and because all acts on a particular factor are adminis- (Vuchinich, Bank, & Patterson, 1992). This model did
tered in sequence. We reasoned that these formatting not take cross-dyad relations into account, and it did
conventions might contribute to patterns of associa- not provide an adequate fit to the data for either boys
tions in residuals. Therefore, residuals of like variables and girls. For boys, the maximum likelihood χ2 =
across perpetrator were also permitted to covary (e.g., 174.33, df = 29, p < .001, Satorra–Bentler χ2 = 139.86,
self verbal aggression residual was free to correlate df = 29, p < .001, the NFI, NNFI, CFI, and robust CFI
with partner verbal aggression residual). Finally, all were all < .78, the standardized root mean square resid-
physical aggression variables (measured variables) uals were .20, the root mean square error of approxi-
were free to covary among themselves and with the la- mation was .24. For girls, the maximum likelihood χ2 =
tent variables. Testing these models revealed that this 148.69, df = 28, p < .001, Satorra–Bentler χ2 = 99.61,
was a good measurement model for boys, but the re- df = 28, p < .001, the NFI, NNFI, CFI, and robust CFI
sults of the Lagrange Multiplier Test indicated that the were all < .86, the standardized root mean square resid-
fit of the model would be significantly improved for uals were .17, the root mean square error of approxi-
girls if the self verbal aggression residuals and self mation was .19.
physical aggression at Time 1 were also free to covary Because this isolated stability model (i.e., positing
(this change would not have significantly changed the stability of each person’s aggression, but no dyadic re-
fit of the models for boys). Given that verbal aggres- lations) did not fit the data, in the second stage we pro-
sion and physical aggression scales are both from the ceeded to test our hypothesized model, which included
same measure (the mCTS), and given that this associa- both stability and cross-dyad effects (all paths indi-
tion was within perpetrator but across the two scale cated in Figure 1). For boys, the hypothesized
score residuals, we thought freeing this association cross-dyad model was consistent with the data. All
likely improved our ability to model measure-driven standard indexes suggested an adequate but not excel-
variance and was theoretically acceptable. This modi- lent fit of the model. The maximum likelihood χ2 =
fication was made to the measurement model for girls. 56.39, df = 27, p < .001, Satorra–Bentler χ2 = 54.00, df
We then evaluated the hypothesized models (as dia- = 27, p < .001, NFI = .92, NNFI = .92, CFI = .95, robust
gramed in Figure 1), which were the same for boys and CFI = .90. Moreover, the standardized root mean
girls, with the exception of the difference noted previ- square residuals were .07, the root mean square error of
ously in the measurement models. In the hypothesized approximation was .11, the average off-diagonal stan-
models, the latent variables, the residuals of self and dardized residuals were small, and all but two hypothe-
partner’s physical aggression at Time1, and the residu- sized paths were statistically significant. Self and part-
als of self and partner’s physical aggression at fol- ner’s psychological aggression latent factors were

321
O’LEARY AND SLEP

strongly associated (r = .83, p < .001). Standardized in- relationships. This was true for boys and girls, and
dicator loadings and paths are presented in Figure 2. for both self-reported aggression and victimization.
The model accounted for 24.7% and 51.4% of the vari- These stability coefficients are similar to those ob-
ance in self and partner’s physical aggression at Time 1 tained in samples of newly married couples (O’Leary
and 58.7% and 50.1% of the variance in self and part- et al., 1989). The high stability of aggression in high
ner’s physical aggression at follow-up. The hypothe- school dating relationships is perhaps surprising be-
sized cross-dyad model was also consistent with the cause of the less stable and more heterogeneous na-
data for girls. All standard indexes suggested an excel- ture of teen relationships. Further, the relatively low
lent fit of the model. The maximum likelihood χ2 = frequency of aggressive acts might lead one to be-
38.52, df = 26, p > .05, Satorra–Bentler χ2 = 30.73, df = lieve that a 14-week assessment interval would have
26, p > .05, NFI = .96, NNFI = .98, CFI = .99, and ro- resulted in underestimates of the continuation of ag-
bust CFI = .99. Moreover, the standardized root mean gression.
square residuals were .05, the root mean square error of In a comprehensive review of personality traits
approximation was .06, the average off-diagonal stan- from childhood to old age, Roberts and Del Vecchio
dardized residuals were small, and all but two hypothe- (2000) used longitudinal studies to address the issue
sized paths (the same two as for boys) were statistically of consistency of traits across the life span, using
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

significant. Self and partner’s psychological aggres- rank-ordered consistency as their measure of stability.
sion latent factors were strongly associated (r = .93, p < They concluded that trait consistency is lowest in the
.001). Standardized indicator loadings and paths are first 3 years of life but that it approaches .50 during
presented in Figure 2. The model accounted for 33.0% the years 18 to 21. Further, they reported that stability
and 37.1% of the variance in self and partner’s physical increases somewhat until ages 50 to 59, when the sta-
aggression at Time 1 and 45.8% and 50.3% of the vari- bility reaches a plateau. The stability reported herein
ance in self and partner’s physical aggression at fol- on self-reported physical aggression is as high as the
low-up. stability of personality traits like extraversion, agree-
ableness, and neuroticism noted by Roberts and Del
Vecchio (2000). The conditional probabilities ob-
Discussion tained for boys and girls were also quite similar to
those we found for the stability of partner aggression
Our results indicate substantial stability in physi- in newly married couples (O’Leary et al., 1994).
cal dating aggression over a 3-month period in stable These stability data were obtained across a much

Figure 2. Supported model. Results for males appear first; results for females appear below. As described in the text, residuals of like
variables across perpetrators were also free to vary, but were not included in this diagram for clarity.

322
DYADIC MODEL OF DATING AGGRESSION

shorter interval with the high school students (14 female to date, and they are expected to be the initia-
weeks) than with the newly married individuals (1 tor or aggressor of affectionate and sexual behavior.
year), and the stability across a shorter time interval Perhaps in teen relationships, both partners may have
suggests that physical aggression may be more con- equal opportunities for re-partnering, thereby contrib-
sistent in young couples who are steadily dating, es- uting to more equal power and differing from older
pecially for high school girls. couples, with whom economic dependence is more
The stability and conditional probability results salient. Our findings regarding the coherence among
suggest that prevention programs for dating aggression measures of psychological aggression and rates of
should occur in high school or earlier and the programs psychological aggression for both boys and girls sug-
should address physical aggression of both boys and gest that prevention programs might be well served to
girls. For those boys and girls who did engage in physi- target such behaviors in both boys and girls. By tar-
cal aggression against a partner at the initial assess- geting these perhaps less severe behaviors for both
ment, there was about a 50% chance that it would occur boys and girls, we may be able to capitalize on the
again at the next assessment for boys and approxi- link between psychological aggression and physical
mately a 75% chance that it would occur for girls. aggression and both within-person and cross-dyad ef-
Moreover, the percentage of boys and girls who re- fects to reduce dating violence.
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

ported that they engaged in aggression at the initial as- Finally, the results of the model testing are striking
sessment against their partners was not small (17% and, although consistent with the hypothesized model,
boys; 31% girls). Thus, about 8% of boys and 15% of somewhat surprising in the context of our stability
girls who dated the same partner across a 3½-month findings. Our cross-dyad model hypothesized both a
period engaged in repeated aggression against their significant influence of each person’s earlier aggres-
partners. To prevent the occurrence of stable aggres- sion on later aggression (i.e., stability) and also a sig-
sion in this sizeable proportion of high school students, nificant influence of the other person’s earlier aggres-
both universal interventions aimed at younger teens sion on each person’s later aggression (i.e., cross-dyad
(e.g., Foshee et al., 1996) and targeted interventions di- influence). This cross-dyad model fit the data for both
rected at high-risk teens (e.g., Wekerle & Wolfe, 1999) boys and girls. Notably, however, in the context of the
may be indicated. We will return to these possibilities cross-dyad model, the strength of the stability paths
in the context of the implications from our other major drops to nonsignificance. Although they were part of
findings. the hypothesized model, these stability paths could
Regarding psychological aggression, the different have been fixed to zero and not negatively affected the
forms we assessed appeared moderately related at the fit of the model.
zero-order level, and the structural equation modeling All of the other hypothesized paths in the
supported their aggregation into a latent construct for cross-dyad model were statistically significant for
psychological aggression for self and partner for both both boys and girls. Further, approximately one
boys and girls. All three observed variables had signifi- fourth of the variance in boys’ reports and one third
cant and sizeable indicator loadings on the construct of the variance in girls’ reports of their Time 1 physi-
psychological aggression. Across sex, the indicator cal aggression were accounted for by their own psy-
loadings ranged from .49 to .89 for one’s own psycho- chological aggression. Approximately half the vari-
logical aggression, and they ranged from .56 to .88 for ance in boys’ reports and one third the variance in
partner’s psychological aggression. girls’ reports of their partners’ physical aggression
These findings suggest that a variety of both mild were accounted for by their partners’ psychological
and more serious nonviolent tactics, from yelling to aggression.
trying to make a partner think he or she is crazy, are At the 3-month follow-up, the variance accounted
perhaps as interrelated in normative teen samples as for by the model was even greater. For boys, we were
in older, clinical samples. This is somewhat contrary able to account for 58% of the variance in the partner’s
to conventional wisdom that power and control dy- physical aggression and 50% of their own physical ag-
namics may depend, in part, on economic depend- gression. For girls, we were able to account for 50% of
ence. Also of note is that these psychological tactics the variance in the partner’s physical aggression and
do not appear to be differentially related for boys and 46% of their own physical aggression. Thus, the con-
girls, either according to teens’ reports about their structs of psychological aggression at the initial assess-
own behaviors or their partners’. This may suggest ment and the same partners’ physical aggression at the
that the gender balance of power is more equal in initial assessment account for approximately half the
teens’ relationships. However, although male and fe- variance in the other partner’s physical aggression at
male teens may have more equal power in relation- the 3-month follow-up.
ships than intimate adult partners, boys and girls are The modeling analyses showed that including the
exposed to societal messages that depict differential cross-dyad longitudinal influences was necessary to
roles. For example, males are expected to ask the account for our data. In addition, within the context

323
O’LEARY AND SLEP

of the modeling, these cross-dyad influences were abuse. This is encouraging in that psychologists are of-
more predictive of one’s own physical aggression ten in a better position to implement changes that affect
than stability. This finding held for both boys and individuals as compared to communities, and psycho-
girls. These findings suggest that, even in the context logical aggression is a variable that can be changed
of substantial stability, dyadic or relationship process (O’Leary, Heyman, & Neidig, 1999).
over time may be an important prompt for one to be Another factor of note is the high association of
physically aggressive. To some, these findings may self-reported physical aggression with self-reported
seem inconsistent with the results of our stability victimization for both boys and girls at both assess-
analyses discussed earlier. How can stability both be ments. In fact, the zero-order correlations of self-re-
substantial and near zero in the same data? The key ports of physical aggression and victimization were
lies in the nature of the analyses and the interrelations .73 for boys and .78 for girls. At follow-up, the corre-
among the variables. When only one person’s behav- lations were slightly higher, indicating a great deal
ior is considered, the relation between earlier behav- covariance in partners’ aggression. Such mutuality is
ior and later behavior is substantial, indicating stabil- similar to the reports that have been obtained in re-
ity. However, when both partners’ behaviors are search with college dating couples (Riggs & O’Leary,
considered simultaneously, stability alone is not 1996), newly married couples (O’Leary et al., 1989),
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

enough to account for the interrelations among the a military sample (White et al., 2001), and maritally
variables. When the necessary cross-dyad effects are discordant couples (Cascardi et al., 1992; O’Leary et
incorporated into the model, the best fitting coeffi- al., 1999). Also these data are quite consistent with
cients indicate that stability is not necessary if the the mutuality of physical aggression in intimate rela-
cross-dyad effects are estimated. This is because of tionships reported in representative samples by Stets
the relations between the cross-dyad and stability ef- and Straus (1990), who found that, in about 50% of
fects, which are not independent. Thus, both stability the cases in which one partner engaged in physical
and cross-dyad effects are indicated by our results. aggression, the other partner also was reported to
These cross-dyad effects, which in the case of our have engaged in physical aggression. In this study,
modeling analyses consistently overshadow stability, mutuality was also evident in psychological aggres-
may have important implications for the next genera- sion arena. Self and partner’s psychological aggres-
tion of dating violence research and prevention efforts. sion latent factors were very strongly associated. In
First, dating violence research and prevention have addition, the zero-order correlations for each form of
typically focused on the individual. This is true even in psychological aggression and victimization were
this study. However, given that a partner’s prior physi- moderately to strongly related for both boys and girls.
cal aggression is the relatively best predictor of an indi- Some might suggest that our finding of dyadic in-
vidual’s later aggression, perhaps a more dyadic focus fluences on dating violence indicates that a great deal
is warranted. It could be that the dyadic influence is of girls’ aggression is self-defensive, and it would
even greater as teens move in and out of relationships therefore be inadvisable to consider the dyadic con-
and that our findings (for teens in stable relationships) text in intervention. Data suggests, however, that es-
represent the lower bound of dyadic effects. One possi- pecially at low levels of severity, aggression in dating
bility is that teens are less motivated or able to inhibit relationships (Hettrich, 2002) and in marriage
their physical aggression when their partners have ag- (Cascardi & Vivian, 1995) is not primarily in self-de-
gressed against them. The bottom line is that, if one fense. It is likely that both sexes consider male ag-
wishes to reduce physical aggression in teenage dating gression to be more of a threat than female aggres-
relationships, a dyadic focus may be crucial. We return sion, however (Schumacher, Slep, & O’Leary, 1998).
to the dyadic issue later when we address these rela- Although women’s physical aggression more rarely
tions of psychological and physical aggression. leads to injury, such acts of aggression, especially if
The role of psychological aggression in predicting repeated, increase the probability of aggression by
physical aggression is a major one, apparently even in men (Straus, 1993). Thus, efforts focused on both
the relatively unstable world of teen dating relation- boys and girls decreasing their own psychological ag-
ships. Indeed, in our own research as well as that of oth- gression and physical aggression, and on the interac-
ers like White et al. (2001), psychological aggression tive nature of such aggression, seem warranted.
was one of the two best cross-sectional predictors of The overall fit of the longitudinal model, although
physical aggression. In this research, we observed that excellent for girls, was merely adequate for boys.
psychological aggression was both a cross-sectional Thus, replicating our findings for boys will be impor-
and a longitudinal predictor of partner aggression. tant. However, several factors contribute to our conclu-
Given the predictive value of psychological aggression, sion that the fit is adequate. First, the hypothesized
it seems that prevention and intervention programs model was tested as an a priori model, and thus modifi-
should place considerable emphasis on reducing psy- cation indexes were not used to improve fit. Second,
chological aggression as a means of reducing partner the supported model is identical for boys and girls with

324
DYADIC MODEL OF DATING AGGRESSION

all parameters generally consistent for both samples. Andrews, J. A., Foster, S. L., Capaldi, D., & Hops, H. (2000). Ado-
lescent and female predictors of physical aggression, communi-
Finally, boys’ data were more nonnormal, even after
cation, and satisfaction in young couples: A prospective analy-
transformation and from a slightly smaller sample that sis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68,
may have contributed to the inconsistency across fit in- 195–208.
dexes. Thus, evidence in support of the model is suffi- Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). Studying the mind in the
cient to warrant interpretations, although these inter- middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research.
In H. Reis & C. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in
pretations must be considered more tentative than
social and personality psychology (pp. 253–285). New York:
those offered for girls. Cambridge University Press.
Research on prevention of partner violence with ad- Barnett, O. W., & Fagan, R. W. (1993). Alcohol use in male spouse
olescents and young adults is in its infancy. However, abusers and their female partners. Journal of Family Violence,
controlled research documents that attitudes support- 8, 1–25.
Bentler, P. (1995). EQS: Structural equations program manual.
ing the use of physical aggression can be reduced in
Encino, CA: Multivariate Software.
large-scale prevention programs targeting all partici- Cano, A., Avery-Leaf, S., Cascardi, M., & O’Leary, K. D. (1998).
pants in a school (see O’Leary et al., 2002). There is Dating violence in two high school samples: Discriminating
less evidence that physically aggressive behavior can variables. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 18, 431–446.
be changed, but significant reductions have been Cantos, A. L., Neidig, P. H., & O’Leary, K. D. (1994). Injuries of
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

women and men in a treatment program for domestic violence.


shown in prevention programs that are rather intensive
Journal of Family Violence, 9, 113–124.
(i.e., ten 45-min sessions for all students [Foshee et al., Capaldi, D. (2002, May). Understanding aggression in young cou-
2000]; or eighteen 2-hr sessions for targeted individu- ples: A focus on development, the dyad, and design. Paper pre-
als at high risk for such aggression [Wekerle & Wolfe, sented at Family Psychology Conference, Northwestern Uni-
1999]). As documented in the introduction of this arti- versity, Evanston, IL.
Capaldi, D. M., Dishion, T. J., Stoolmiller, M., & Yoerger, K. (2001).
cle, physical aggression appears to occur in approxi-
Aggression toward female partners by at-risk young men: The
mately one third of high school dating relationships, contribution of male adolescent friendships. Developmental
and this physical aggression appears (a) quite stable Psychology, 37, 61–73.
and (b) dyadically influenced. Further, psychologi- Cascardi, M., Langhinrichsen, J., & Vivian, D. (1992). Marital ag-
cally aggressive tactics, including controlling behav- gression: Impact, injury, and health correlates for husbands and
wives. Archives of Internal Medicine, 152, 1178–1184.
iors previously thought more typical in battering rela- Cascardi, M., & Vivian, D. (1995). Context for specific episodes of
tionships, seem to be part of dating violence among marital violence: Gender and severity of violence differences.
high school students. Our data do not suggest substan- Journal of Family Violence, 10, 265–293.
tially different relations among forms of psychological Cascardi, M., Avery-Leaf, S. A., O’Leary, K. D., & Slep, A. M. S.
aggression between psychological and physical ag- (1999). Factor structure and convergent validity of the Conflict
Tactics Scale in high school students. Psychological Assess-
gression for boys as compared to girls. Thus, it seems ment, 11, 546–555.
most prudent to have prevention programs that are uni- Chase, K. A., Treboux, D., O’Leary, K. D., & Strasberg, Z. (1998).
versal (for all students), potentially at younger ages, as Specificity of dating aggression and its justification among high
well as for individuals targeted because they are at high risk adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26,
risk for partner aggression (O’Leary et al., 2002). The 467–473.
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci-
former may have an impact on students with little or no ences (rev. ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
physical aggression, whereas with the latter, more in- Inc.
tensive programs may reduce physical aggression in Curran, P. J., West, S. G., & Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test
individuals who are already physically aggressive or statistics to normality and specification error in CFA. Psycho-
who are at risk for such aggression because of other logical Methods, 1, 16–29.
Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1979). Violence against wives. New
problems (truancy, school suspensions because of ag- York: Free Press.
gression). Finally, to support the development of in- Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1984). The nature and antecedents
creasingly effective prevention initiatives, we must of violent events. British Journal of Criminology, 24, 269–288.
continue to refine our theories of dating violence, Foshee, V. (1996). Gender differences in adolescent dating abuse
prevalence, types, and injuries. Health Education Research, 11,
which have largely been borrowed from the marital vi-
275–286.
olence area, to reflect the developmental issues that Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Greene, W. F., Koch, G. G., Linder, G.
may distinguish dating violence among teens from bat- F., & MacDougall, J. E. (2000). The safe dates program: 1-year
tering in older, married samples. follow-up results. American Journal of Public Health, 90,
1619–1622.
Foshee, V. A., Linder, G. F., Bauman, K. E., Langwick, S. A.,
Arriaga, X. B., Heath, J. L., et al. (1996). The Safe Dates Pro-
ject: Theoretical basis, evaluation design, and selected base-
References line findings. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 12,
39–47.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statisti- Henton, J. M., Cate, R. M., Koval, J., Lloyd, S., & Christopher, S.
cal manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Au- (1983). Romance and violence in dating relationships. Journal
thor. of Family Issues, 4, 467–482.

325
O’LEARY AND SLEP

Hettrich, E. (2002, May). Attributions by 125 females for their use of O’Leary, K. D., & Jacobson, N. S. (1997). Partner relational prob-
physical aggression in a dating relationship. Paper presented to lems with physical abuse. In Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
the Honors Program, Stony Brook, NY. sourcebook (4th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 673–692). Washington, DC:
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure American Psychiatric Association.
modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model O’Leary, K. D., Heyman, R. E., & Neidig, P. N. (1999). Treatment of
misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 424–453. wife abuse: A comparison of gender-specific and conjoint ap-
Jacobson, N. S., Gottman, J. M., Gortner, E., Berns, S., & Shortt, J. proaches. Behavior Therapy, 30, 475–505.
W. (1996). Psychological factors in the longitudinal course of O’Leary, K. D., Malone, J., & Tyree, A. (1994). Physical aggression
battering: When do the couples split up? When does the abuse in early marriage: Prerelationship and relationship effects.
decrease? Violence and Victims, 11, 371–392. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 594–602.
Johnson, M. P. (1995). Patriarchical terrorism and common couple O’Leary, K. D., Woodin, E., & Fritz, P. (2002, May). Preventing
violence: Two forms of violence against women. Journal of partner aggression during adolescence and young adulthood:
Marriage and the Family, 57, 283–294. Lessons from smoking and alcohol prevention programs. Paper
Jones, L. E. (1987). School curriculum project evaluation report. presented at the Department of Defense Conference, Alexan-
Minneapolis, MN: Minnesota Coalition for Battered Women. dria, VA.
Kasian, M., & Painter, S. L. (1992). Frequency and severity of psy- Pan, H., Neidig, P. H., & O’Leary, K. D. (1994). Male–female and
chological abuse in a dating population. Journal of Interper- aggressor–victim differences in the factor structure of the Mod-
sonal Violence, 7, 350–364. ified Conflict Tactics Scale. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
Kaufman, G. K., & Jasinksi, J. L. (1995, July). Prevention of teen 9, 336–382.
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

dating violence: Evaluation of a multidimensional model. Pa- Quigley, B. M., & Leonard, K. E. (1996). Desistance of husband ag-
per presented at the Fourth International Family Violence Re- gression in the early years of marriage. Violence and Victims,
search Conference, Durham, NH. 11, 355–370.
Maiuro, R. D. (2001). Sticks and stones will break my bones, but Reitzel-Jaffee, D., & Wolfe, D. A. (2001). Predictors of relationship
names will also hurt me: Psychological abuse in domestically violence among young men. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
violent relationships. In K. D. O’Leary & R. D. Maiuro (Eds.), 16, 99–115.
Psychological abuse in violent relationships (pp. ix–xx). New Riggs, D. S., & O’Leary, K. D. (1989). A theoretical model of court-
York: Springer. ship aggression. In M. A. Pirog-Good & J. E. Stets (Eds.), Vio-
Makepeace, J. M. (1986). Gender differences in courtship violence lence in dating relationships: Emerging social issues (pp.
victimization. Family Relations, 35, 383–388. 53–70). New York: Praeger.
Makepeace, J. M. (1989). Dating, living together, and courtship vio- Riggs, D. S., & O’Leary, K. D. (1996). Aggression between hetero-
lence. In M. A. Pirog-Good & J. E. Stets (Eds.), Violence in dat- sexual dating partners: An examination of a causal model of
ing relationships: Emerging social issues (pp. 94–107). courtship aggression. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 11,
Praeger: New York. 519–540.
Malik, S., Sorensen, S., & Aneshensel, C. (1997). Community and Roberts, B. W., & Del Vecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank order consis-
dating violence among adolescents: Perpetration and victimiza- tency of traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review
tion. Journal of Adolescent Health, 21, 291–302. of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 3–25.
Marshall, L. L. (1996). Psychological abuse of women: Six distinct Roscoe, B., & Kelsey, T. (1986). Dating violence among high school
clusters. Journal of Family Violence, 11, 369–399. students. Psychology: A Journal of Human Behavior, 23,
McMahon, M., & Pence, E. (1996). Replying to Dan O’Leary. Jour- 53–59.
nal of Interpersonal Violence, 11, 452–455. Schechter, S. (1982). Women and male violence: The visions and
Murphy, C. M., & Hoover, S. A. (2001). Measuring emotional abuse struggles of the battered women’s movement. Boston: South
in dating relationships as a multifactorial construct. In K. D. End.
O’Leary & R. D. Maiuro (Eds.), Psychological abuse in violent Schumacher, J. A., Slep, A. M. S., & O’Leary, K. D. (1998, Novem-
relationships (pp. 29–46). New York: Springer. ber). The context and consequences of female to male dating
O’Brien, M. K., Cascardi, M., & Avery-Leaf, S. (1999, July). Dis- aggression. Poster presented at the 32nd annual convention of
criminating variables of dating violence in a middle school the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy,
sample. Paper presented in symposium at the Sixth Interna- Washington, DC.
tional Family Violence Conference, Durham, NH. Schwartz, M., O’Leary, S. G., & Kendziora, K. (1996). Dating ag-
O’Keeffe, M. (1997). Predictors of dating violence among high gression among high school students. Violence and Victims, 12,
school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 12, 295–305.
546–568. Silverman, J. G., Raj, A., Mucci, L. A., & Hathway, J. E. (2001).
O’Keeffe, N. K., Brockopp, K., & Chew, E. (1986). Teen dating vio- Dating violence against adolescent girls and associated sub-
lence. Social Work, 31, 465–468. stance use, unhealthy weight control, sexual risk behavior,
O’Leary, K. D. (1993). Through a psychological lens: Personality pregnancy, and suicidality. Journal of the American Medical
traits, personality disorders, and levels of violence. In R. J. Association, 286, 572–579.
Gelles & D. Loseke (Eds.), Current controversies on family vio- Slep, A. M. S., Cascardi, M., Avery-Leaf, S., & O’Leary, K. D.
lence (pp. 7–29). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. (2001). Two new measures of attitudes about the acceptability
O’Leary, K. D. (1996). Physical aggression in intimate relationships of teen dating aggression. Psychological Assessment, 13,
can be treated within a marital context under certain circum- 306–318.
stances. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 11, 450–453. Stets, J. E., & Straus, M. A. (1990). Gender differences in reporting
O’Leary, K. D. (1999). Developmental and affective issues in assess- marital violence and its medical and psychological conse-
ing and treating partner aggression. Clinical Psychology: Sci- quences. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical vio-
ence and Practice, 6, 400–414. lence in American families (pp. 151–165). New Brunswick, NJ:
O’Leary, K. D., Barling, J., Arias, I., Rosenbaum, A., Malone, J., & Transaction Press.
Tyree, A. (1989). Prevalence and stability of physical aggres- Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence:
sion between spouses: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Con- The conflict tactics (CT) scales. Journal of Marriage and the
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 263–268. Family, 41, 75–86.

326
DYADIC MODEL OF DATING AGGRESSION

Straus, M. A. (1993). Physical assaults by wives: A major social Wekerle, C., & Wolfe, D. A. (1999). Dating violence in mid-adoles-
problem. In R. J. Gelles & D. R. Loseke (Eds.), Current contro- cence: Theory, significance, and emerging prevention initia-
versies on family violence (pp. 67–87). Newbury Park, CA: tives. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 435–456.
Sage. West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation
Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (Eds). (1990). Physical violence in models with nonnormal variables. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Struc-
American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in tural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications
8,145 families. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. (pp. 56–75). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Tolman, R. M. (1989). The development of a measure of psychologi- White, J. W., Merrill, L. L., & Koss, M. P. (2001). Prediction of
cal maltreatment of women by their male partners. Violence and premilitary courtship violence in a Navy recruit sample. Vio-
Victims, 4, 159–177. lence and Victims, 16, 910–927.
Tolman, R. M. (1999). The validation of the psychological maltreat- Widom, C. S. (1989). Does violence beget violence? A critical re-
ment of women inventory. Violence and Victims, 14, 25–37. view of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 3–28.
Vuchinich, S., Bank, L., & Patterson, G. R. (1992). Parenting, peers,
and the stability of antisocial behavior in preadolescent boys.
Developmental Psychology, 28, 510–521.
Watson, J., Cascardi, M., Avery-Leaf, S., & O’Leary, K. D. (2001).
High school responses to dating aggression. Violence and Vic- Received June 19, 2002
tims, 16, 339–348. Accepted February 7, 2003
Downloaded by [Florida Institute of Technology] at 10:32 22 August 2014

327

You might also like