Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 21

Development and Psychopathology 21 (2009), 1211–1231

Copyright # Cambridge University Press, 2009


doi:10.1017/S0954579409990125

Does change in temperament predict


change in schizoid personality disorder?
A methodological framework and illustration
from the Longitudinal Study of Personality
Disorders

MARK F. LENZENWEGERa AND JOHN B. WILLETT b


a
State University of New York at Binghamton; and b Harvard University

Abstract
Personality disorders (PDs) have been thought historically to be enduring, inflexible, and set in psychological stone
relatively firmly; however, empirical findings from recent prospective multiwave longitudinal studies establish otherwise.
Nearly all modern longitudinal studies of personality disorder have documented considerable change in PDs over time,
suggesting considerable flexibility and plasticity in this realm of psychopathology. The factors and mechanisms of change
in the PDs remain essentially opaque, and this area of PD research is just beginning to be probed using candidate predictors
of change, such as personality systems. In this report, we investigate whether change in temperament dimensions
(emotionality, activity, and sociability) predicts change in schizoid personality disorder. We present a latent growth
framework for addressing this question and provide an illustration of the approach using data from the Longitudinal Study
of Personality Disorders. Schizoid personality disorder was assessed using two different methodologies (structured
psychiatric interview and self-report) and temperament was assessed using a well-known psychometric measure of
temperament. All constructs were measured at three time points over a 4-year time period. To analyze these panel data, we
fitted a covariance structure model that hypothesized simultaneous relationships between initial levels and rates of change
in temperament and initial levels and rates of change in schizoid personality disorder. We found that rates of change in the
core temperament dimensions studied do not predict rates of change in schizoid personality over time. We discuss the
methodological advantages of the latent growth approach and the substantive meaning of the findings for change in
schizoid personality disorder.

It has long been assumed that personality disor- within the individual. For years the prevailing
der (PDs) features remain stable over time, diagnostic nomenclature (Diagnostic and Sta-
tistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM];
American Psychiatric Association [APA],
This research was supported in part by Grant MH-45448 1980, 1987, 1994) has embraced this assump-
from the National Institute of Mental Health (to M.F.L.). tion in asserting that PDs, which are relatively
We thank Armand W. Loranger for providing training
prevalent disorders affecting about 10% of the
and consultation on the use of the International Personal-
ity Disorder Examination and Theodore Millon for pro-
population (Lenzenweger, Loranger, Korfine,
viding consultation on the use of the Millon Clinical & Neff, 1997; Lenzenweger, Lane, Loranger,
Multiaxial Inventory—II. We are grateful to Lauren & Kessler, 2007), are “enduring patterns” of
Korfine for project coordination in the early phase of individual behavior that are “inflexible” and
the study. “stable over time” (APA, 1994, pp. 629–639).
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Mark
F. Lenzenweger, Department of Psychology, State Univer-
However, an emerging corpus of data from
sity of New York at Binghamton, Science IV, Binghamton, modern prospective longitudinal studies reveals
NY 13902-6000; E-mail: mlenzen@binghamton.edu. that there is considerable individual change in

1211
1212 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

personality pathology over time and, in addi- described as “loners” or “recluses” by the lay
tion, that these changes differ across indi- observer. The lack of desire for contact of any
viduals, whether PDs are conceptualized as di- sort (including sexual contact) with other hu-
mensions or diagnostic entities (Johnson, man beings in SZDPD is profound. The nearly
Cohen, Kasen, Skodol, Hamagami, & Brooks, complete absence of any expression of emotion
2000; Lenzenweger, 1999; Lenzenweger, creates the impression of coldness, detachment,
Johnson, & Willett, 2004; Shea et al., 2002; Za- and almost complete affective flattening (for an
narini, Frankenburg, Hennen, & Silk, 2003; Za- excellent review, see Bernstein et al., 2009).
narini, Frankenburg, Hennen, Reich, & Silk, Affecting roughly 1–2% of the general pop-
2005). In short, PDs do not appear to be set ulation (Lenzenweger, 2008),1 SZDPD resides
like plaster or carved in granite. Rather, the con- in the “odd/eccentric” Cluster A section of the
temporary picture of the PDs is one character- DSM system Axis II taxonomy with both
ized by plasticity, individual change, and inter- schizotypal personality disorder and paranoid
individual heterogeneity in change over time. personality disorder. However, whereas schizo-
The reasons for this plasticity, change, and het- typal and paranoid personality disorder have
erogeneity over time, nonetheless, remain fun- come to be considered alternative expressions
damentally opaque. of schizophrenia liability (Lenzenweger, 1998,
We have been pursuing a program of re- 2009), SZDPD appears to be considerably less
search designed to illuminate and uncover vari- related to schizophrenia liability. SZDPD af-
ables that may be responsible for both interindi- fected individuals rarely come to the attention of
vidual and intraindividual growth and change treating clinicians, as most do not appear in
in the PDs (Lenzenweger & Castro, 2005; Len- either inpatient or outpatient settings; rather,
zenweger, Clarkin, Fertuck, & Kernberg, 2004; they live and function in the community. Those
Lenzenweger & Willett, 2007). Our program SZDPD individuals that present to clinical
has been facilitated by the availability of pro- services are more likely than not to present in
spective, multiwave PD and personality data some form of crisis (e.g., drug abuse, suicidal
from the Longitudinal Study of Personality Dis- behavior, depressive episode). A recent study
orders (LSPD; for an overview, see Lenzenwe- of the prevalence of PDs in a large series
ger, 2006) as well as contemporary develop- of outpatients found relatively few (1.4%) to
ments in quantitative methods suited for the have SZDPD (Zimmerman, Rothschild, &
study of growth and change (Singer & Willett, Chelminski, 2005). Moreover, in contrast to
2003), especially those inspired by the individ- schizotypal and paranoid PDs (as well as most
ual growth curve framework for the study of other PDs), SZDPD represents more of a mono-
change over time (Rogosa & Willett, 1985). thetic disorder, phenotypically manifesting it-
In this report, we seek to provide a window self as a profound disruption in sociability.
on this methodological and quantitative ap- This disruption may perhaps be reflective of a
proach by adopting a longitudinal focus on massive impairment in the underlying affilia-
schizoid personality disorder (SZDPD). tion system (see Depue & Lenzenweger,
SZDPD has long been described in the clinical 2001, 2005). Finally, the disorder seems to
literature as a diagnostic entity of considerable
interest (Bernstein, Arntz, & Travaglini,
2009), yet still very little is known about its
long-term longitudinal course within the indi- 1. In the National Comorbidity Survey—Replication
(NCS-R) study of Axis II psychopathology in the United
vidual over time and the determinants of differ- States population, a prevalence of 4.9% was found for
ences in change across individuals (if any). schizoid personality disorder in the clinical reappraisal
According to the DSM-IV (APA, 1994), “the sample of the study (Lenzenweger, Lane, Loranger, &
essential feature of schizoid personality disor- Kessler, 2007). This figure is most likely biased upward
der is a pervasive pattern of detachment from compared with the estimates generated for the full NCS-R
study sample, which served to generate the population prev-
social relationships and a restricted range alence estimates for Axis II PDs. In the full NCS-R sample a
of expression of emotion in interpersonal population prevalence estimate was generated only for “any
settings (p. 638).” SZDPD individuals may be Cluster A” disorder, not for individual Cluster A disorders.
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1213

appear with comparable frequency in men and statistically significant decrease in SZDPD fea-
women (Bernstein et al., 2009). tures from baseline assessment to a 10-year fol-
Not unlike the general picture of plasticity low-up in outpatient psychiatric patients who
and change over time emerging for nearly all had concurrent mood disorders. However, it is
of the other PDs in the DSM taxonomy, SZDPD somewhat difficult to discern whether some of
is also characterized by change over time rather the residual impairment and affective phenom-
than immutable stability. For example, in the enology present in individuals who are affec-
LSPD, the study from which we drew the data tively ill could impact the presentation and,
for the current exercise, although individuals perhaps, assessment of SZDPD features.
maintained their position in the overall rank or- Fortunately, Durbin and Klein used a well-con-
dering of SZDPD features across assessment structed semistructured psychiatric interview
waves, we observed statistically significant (the International Personality Disorder Exami-
changes in the mean levels (declines) of nation [IPDE]; Loranger, 1999), which has
SZDPD features in both interview and self-re- been shown to be robust for Axis II assessments
port assessments (Lenzenweger, 1999). Len- even in the face of mood and mental state dis-
zenweger, Johnson, et al. (2004) used the turbances (Loranger et al., 1991). Finally, the
more sensitive and fine-grained analytic ap- results of a study in the United Kingdom found
proach of individual growth curve analysis evidence for change in SZDPD features over
and found that SZDPD features showed a statis- time, on average, but in contrast to all other
tically significant (rate of change ¼ 20.07, p , studies, these investigators reported an increase
.004) rate of change (toward decreasing symp- in SZDPD features with the passage of time
tomatology) over time, consistent with a small (Seivewright, Tyrer, & Johnson, 2002). Thus,
to medium effect size. Clearly, SZDPD was it seems reasonable to claim, informed by em-
found to show evidence of growth/change pirical data (as opposed to clinical impression
over a 4-year study period in the LSPD. The re- or traditional psychiatric lore), that SZDPD dis-
sults of the Children in the Community Study plays a degree of plasticity over time.3 The de-
(CIC), under the direction of Patricia Cohen, velopmental psychopathology task at hand,
also reported that levels of SZDPD features de- therefore, is to discern those variables responsi-
clined over time, showing a 47% drop in mag- ble for this plasticity (i.e., the individual
nitude from the 9- to 12-year-old period to the changes) in the SZDPD as a psychopathologi-
25- to 28-year-old period (Johnson et al., cal entity. Moreover, we should seek to illumi-
2000). Of interest, unlike the LSPD results, rel- nate the mechanism by which these changes,
atively low levels of stability in individual dif- and the interindividual differences among
ferences (rank order stability) were found for them, in SZDPD occur over time.
SZDPD, and therefore, SZDPD features in ado- What could a set of candidate variables that
lescence did not predict SZDPD in adulthood were responsible for change in SZDPD over
successfully. Thus, contemporary prospective time look like? One could consider any number
multiwave studies suggest not only a lessening of potential predictors of change in SZDPD
of SZDPD features over time, on average, but symptomatology over time. However, one im-
also the presence of interindividual heterogene- mediate research vector presents itself, namely,
ity in the emergence of the disorder over time temperament and those processes subserving
(in the CIC).2 temperament. Thus, we hypothesize that one
In contrast to the findings of the LSPD and possible mechanism that might account for
CIC, Durbin and Klein (2006) did not find a the change in SZDPD over time could be
those underlying temperament processes that
show themselves some plasticity over time.
2. There are two other prospective, multiwave studies un-
derway in the personality disorder area, namely, the Col-
laborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study 3. We note that the short-term (e.g., 2 months), test–retest
(Shea et al., 2002) and the McLean Study of Adult De- reliability of SZDPD features as assessed by the IPDE
velopment (Zanarini et al., 2005). However, neither of is relatively high for both level and rank order analyses
these studies includes SZDPD as a research focus. (Loranger et al., 1991).
1214 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

Temperament denotes a collection of con- A necessary and logical precursor to ad-


structs, and the overarching concept of tempera- dressing the question of whether change in tem-
ment core to many developmental approaches perament over time predicts change in SZDPD
to psychopathology. However, why would tem- over time is to specify and fit statistical models
perament be of relevance to the development of that represent simultaneously, in a thoughtful
personality disorders in general, and SZDPD in and fine-grained manner, individual growth
particular? In many models, temperament is over time in measures of both temperament
conceptualized as being the foundation that un- (emotionality, sociability, and activity) and
derlies personality and therefore, presumably, SZDPD features. In this paper, we use longitu-
personality disorders. Temperament is often dinal data on both SZDPD symptomatology
conjectured as being grounded in genetically and the three major temperament dimensions
influenced neurobehavioral systems and, more- to test our hypotheses about how individual
over, it is thought to maintain its influence on change in emotionality, activity, and sociability
the development of interindividual differences impact individual change in SZDPD features
over time through childhood (e.g., Putnam, over time. As our hypotheses concern potential
Rothbart, & Gartstein, 2008) on in to adoles- relationships among individual change over
cence and adulthood (e.g., Schwartz, Wright, time in both our outcome and our predictors,
Shin, Kagan, & Rauch, 2003). Several major in our analyses we must simultaneously model
theoretical positions/frameworks regarding the change over time in personality disorders and
nature of temperament have been advanced in the interactive temperament systems (emo-
over the years (e.g., Buss & Plomin, 1984; Der- tionality, activity, and sociability) that we be-
ryberry & Rothbart, 1988; Kagan, Reznick, & lieve could impact that change in SZDPD. We
Snidman, 1988; Thomas & Chess, 1977; see do this by applying latent growth modeling, a
also Shiner, 1998)4 and, in many respects, the recently developed analytic approach that has
similarities among them are greater than their mapped the methods of individual growth mod-
differences. For the purposes of the LSPD, a eling onto the general covariance structure
measure of temperament was needed that could model (Willett & Bub, 2005; Willett & Keiley,
be completed by individuals who were young 2000; e.g., Lenzenweger & Willett, 2007).
adults at the time of assessment, yet as a mea- Our application of latent growth modeling is
sure it would presumably be sensitive to the relatively new in developmental psychopathol-
central constructs of temperament known to ogy research, and builds directly on more tradi-
be prominent in childhood and, by implication, tional methods of analyzing longitudinal data.
their phenotypic expression in adulthood. Thus, It is an extension of our earlier work on the
Buss and Plomin’s (1984) Emotionality–Activ- analysis of individual growth, in which we
ity–Sociability (EAS) Adult Temperament used multilevel modeling to investigate sys-
Scale was administered in the LSPD to assess tematic interindividual differences in growth
the primary temperamental constructs of emo- by examining the relationships between time-
tionality, activity, and sociability in the study invariant predictors, such as a subject’s gender,
participants. and an individual’s change in an outcome, like
personality disorders, over time (Lenzenweger,
Johnson, et al., 2004).
Under the standard individual growth mod-
4. We note the Cloninger model (Cloninger et al., 1993) of eling approach, individual change in the per-
temperament and character in this context; however, we
view the primary “temperament” components of the
sonality disorder outcome is described as a
Cloninger model as representing heterogenous blends function of time by specifying a “Level 1” indi-
of underlying personality dimensions (see Waller, Li- vidual growth model (Singer & Willett, 2003).
lienfeld, Tellegen, & Lykken, 1991). Moreover, Clonin- The strength of the growth modeling approach
ger et al. (1993) view the character dimensions of their is that it is possible to specify a variety of linear
taxonomy (e.g., transcendence) as having primary
causal influence on the development of personality pa-
and nonlinear functional forms for representing
thology. As such, this model mixes both personality con- individual change over time, seeking the most
structs and more ethereal constructs in a hybrid system. meaningful, well-fitting, and substantively ap-
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1215

pealing representation. However, when there can be measured, and the more complex the in-
limited numbers of waves of longitudinal data dividual trajectories that can be fit. This often
available for analysis, the functional form of opens up a richer arena of more interesting re-
the latter Level 1 growth model must necessar- search questions that can be addressed, and
ily be simple. With three waves of data on each with increasing precision. For instance, with
individual, for instance, as in the current exam- more waves of data designed into the data col-
ple, one can only adopt an individual growth lection, the investigator is no longer limited to
model that has, at most, two parameters. This a simple assessment of what predicts heteroge-
typically means that individual change is de- neity in the initial status and rate of change of
scribed as a linear function of time. Such a the individual trajectories, but it may also be
model contains two individual growth parame- able to examine more complex curvilinear
ters: (a) an intercept parameter that represents growth. For instance, one could track individual
the individual’s “true initial status” (providing logistic and negative exponential trajectories
that the first occasion of measurement is chosen over time, say, testing for interindividual differ-
as the origin of time: a process known as “cen- ences in their asymptotes, half-lives, and instan-
tering”), and (b) a slope parameter that repre- taneous slopes at entry. Furthermore, in addi-
sents the individual’s “true rate of change tion to suffering no limits on the number of
over time.” waves of data that can be incorporated into
Once the Level 1 model has been defined, any analysis, the individual growth modeling
the two individual growth parameters can then approach allows different individuals to con-
stand in place of the individual change trajec- tribute different numbers of waves of data,
tory in subsequent analyses of interindividual each obtained on a unique schedule, to the
differences in change. These latter analyses same analysis: something that more traditional
are achieved by specifying a “Level 2” statisti- approaches to analyzing panel data, such as re-
cal model in which the interindividual hetero- peated-measures analysis of variance, cannot
geneity in the Level 1 parameters; that is, the entertain. Finally, where appropriate, it is also
differences across people in individual true in- straightforward to hypothesize individual
itial status and rate of change are hypothesized growth trajectories that are discontinuous, and
to depend upon selected time-invariant predic- that exhibit their discontinuities at important
tors of change, such as subject gender. transitions in an individual’s life. This permits
The Level 1 and Level 2 models can be fitted the investigator to extend the research to assess-
simultaneously to longitudinal data, usually by ing the impact of experimental treatments and
maximum-likelihood methods. By testing and disrupive life events, such as divorce, on indi-
interpreting the obtained Level 2 parameter es- vidual growth over time.
timates, we can detect and assess any systematic Our current work is a direct extension of the
interindividual differences in change that are individual growth modeling framework to a
present. As a consequence, we may conclude multivariate context, with growth in several
that the true initial status and rate of change in constructs being modeled simultaneously. To
personality disorder differ for men and women, achieve this, we use the methods of covariance
and consequently, that the entire personality structure analysis to model growth over time
growth trajectory differs by gender, on average, simultaneously in multiple domains, such as
in the population (Singer & Willett, 2003, pro- SZDPD and temperament. This “latent growth
vide an extensive description of the approach). modeling” approach differs only in that it per-
The individual growth model approach has mits the concurrent specification of several Level
many important advantages for the serious 1 individual growth models to describe change
analysis of longitudinal data. It is not limited in both the outcome and the predictors simul-
by the number of waves of data that can be in- taneously. Consequently, at Level 2, we can ex-
corporated into the analysis. In fact, the more amine potential relationships among the multiple
waves of data that are collected and analyzed, trajectories of change, rather than simply seeking
the more statistical power accrues to the analy- time-invariant predictors of those changes (al-
sis, the greater the reliability with which change though the latter can be incorporated also).
1216 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

Our approach is concerned fundamentally the purposes of our work, we defined our out-
with understanding of both stability and change come as the total number of SZDPD features
(growth) in psychopathology over time. A num- evidenced by each subject at each wave. Fortu-
ber of other statistical approaches have been nately, we had two independent measures of
brought to bear on questions of stability and SZDPD available for all of our participants:
change in psychopathology over time, of one based on a structured interview and the
course, in longitudinal data like ours. One ap- other based on self-report (see below). Both
proach, for example, treats the dimensions of of these clinical measures are well-validated
temperament as time-varying predictors of from the standpoint of psychopathology assess-
change in SZDPD symptomatology and incor- ment. The use of two measures that are based on
porates measures of temperament directly into two different methodologies (interview vs. self-
the Level 1 individual growth model itself, report) provides useful alternative vantage
along with the time predictor. Although this is points from which to view change in SZDPD.
a feasible analytic approach for handing time- Therefore, to address our research questions,
varying covariates, it addresses an entirely dif- we modeled individual change simultaneously
ferent research question from ours, asking in- in emotionality, activity, and sociability and
stead whether there is a concurrent relationship SZDPD features using latent growth modeling,
between time-varying levels of symptomatol- and examined the prediction of individual
ogy and temperament while both are changing change in the latter outcome variable by the in-
over occasions (thereby asking, “Is SZDPD dividual changes in emotionality, activity, and
symptomatology related to measures of tem- sociability. We present and describe our statis-
perament, controlling for time?”), rather than tical models in greater detail, below.
the relationship that we have examined here be- Finally, in this context we note that our focus
tween concurrent changes in both outcome and on the participants in the LSPD data set neces-
predictor. A second popular approach has been sarily begins with the longitudinal study of per-
to use path analysis and structural equation sons that entered the study at age 18 as they
modeling to investigate simultaneous set of were selected initially from a university sample.
lagged pairwise relationships, between levels Moreover, the DSM approach to the diagnosis
of the time-varying predictors on one occasion of personality disorders demarcates age 18 as
and the level of the outcome on a subsequent the minimum age for the diagnosis of personal-
occasion, over multiple waves of data (this is ity disorders. Although the LSPD began with
a path-analytic extension of the now mostly dis- subjects at age 18, there is no reason to believe
used and largely discredited cross-lagged corre- that the development of personality pathology
lation approach). Again, although this kind of began at age 18. In fact, although the DSM ap-
analysis can certainly succeed in detecting the proach to personality disorders has selected age
presence of multiple simultaneous pairwise re- 18 as a formal cutoff, in the LSPD we actually
lationships, and examine and compare the mag- had to evaluate the history of the participants as
nitudes of their effects over time, it cannot ad- it related to personality disorders back to the
dress a research question about change over age 13. This was so because our primary diag-
time nor one about hypothesized links between nostic interview necessitated knowledge of the
simultaneous changes in both outcome and pre- PD-relevant history for each participant for
dictor, as does the approach we have imple- the current year as well as for the prior 5 years.
mented here. Insofar as schizoid PD is concerned, or any PD
In the present study, therefore, we sought to for that matter, it is our view that the develop-
investigate, using prospective multiwave data ment and emergence of personality pathology
from the LSPD (Lenzenweger, 1999, 2006; essentially begins at conception. By this we
Lenzenweger et al., 1997; Lenzenweger, John- mean to emphasize that all PDs are emergent
son, et al., 2004; Lenzenweger & Willett, phenomena that develop from the complex in-
2007), whether individual change in emotion- teraction of genetically influenced brain-based
ality, activity, and sociability predicted individ- neurobehavioral systems (Depue & Lenzenwe-
ual change in SZDPD features over time. For ger, 2001, 2005, 2006) with environmental
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1217

inputs (e.g., parenting, peer relations, specific Table 1. Descriptive statistics and selected
contextual enrichments, trauma, and so on). demographic characteristics of participants
To this end, we present our analyses and results in the Longitudinal Study of Personality
as a dynamic snapshot of the developmental pe- Disorders sample (N ¼ 250)
riod from roughly 18 to 21 in the LSPD sub-
jects; however, we emphasize that we see the Father Mother
development and emergence of SZDPD as hav-
Parental education
ing roots well before this period. Furthermore,
1–8 years 1.6% 0.8%
we note that additional study of the subjects 9–11 years 2.4% 2.8%
in the LSPD will provide important long-term 12 years 8.4% 15.2%
data on the life span development of SZDPD 13–15 years 16.0% 20.4%
on into the later life course. 16þ years 68.8% 58.8%
Not available 2.8% 2.0%
Parental occupation
Method Laborer/service 2.0% 2.4%
Operatives (machine) 2.8% 1.2%
Craftsman/foreman 3.2% 1.6%
Data set: LSPD Clerical/sales 4.0% 16.8%
Management/official 26.8% 12.8%
The LSPD (Lenzenweger, 1999, 2006; Lenzen-
Professional/technical 52.4% 42.0%
weger, Johnson, et al., 2004), which was begun Not available or
in 1991 as the first National Institutes of Mental homemaker 8.8% 23.2%
Health-sponsored longitudinal study of PDs, has Race
a prospective multiwave longitudinal design African American 3.6%
Latin Hispanic 4.8%
with participants evaluated at three points in
Caucasian Anglo 72.0%
time, corresponding to their first, second, and Asian Pacific Islander 17.2%
fourth years in college. Interview assessments Native American 0.8%
for PD (i.e., Axis II) and Axis I disorders were Other 1.6%
conducted by PhD or advanced Master Social
Worker clinicians with extensive Axis II diagnos-
tic experience, at each of the three assessment helped to ensure diversity across sociodemo-
waves (Lenzenweger et al., 1997). All participants graphic background variables. All participants
in the LSPD also completed a self-report inven- gave voluntary written informed consent and
tory for personality pathology at all three assess- received an honorarium of $50.00 at each
ment waves. Finally, as the LSPD is a naturalistic wave of data collection. Extensive detail con-
prospective study, participants were free to seek cerning the initial subject selection procedure
psychological treatment of their own accord. Ex- and sampling is given elsewhere (Lenzenwe-
tensive detail regarding the LSPD, including reli- ger, 1999; Lenzenweger et al., 1997). Of
ability assessments and other technical matters, is the initial 258 participants, 250 completed
given in prior publications and the interested all three assessment waves and are included
reader is referred to those papers (Lenzenweger, in our current analysis. We summarize a se-
1999, 2006; Lenzenweger et al., 1997). lection of their individual characteristics in
Table 1.
Participants
Initial screening measure
The 258 participants in the LSPD were drawn
initially from a population consisting of 2,000 IPDE DSM-III-R Screen (IPDE-S). The IPDE-
first-year undergraduate students (Lenzenwe- S is a 250-item self-administered true–false
ger, 1999; Lenzenweger et al., 1997; Lenzen- PD screening inventory developed by A. W.
weger, Johnson, et al., 2004). The participants Loranger. The diagnostic efficiency and psycho-
were recruited from both the private and public metric properties of the IPDE-S, in a two-stage
(State University of New York) colleges at Cor- screen application, were generally excellent and
nell University in Ithaca, New York, which have been described previously (Lenzenweger
1218 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

et al., 1997). The grouping variable for the study 175-item, true–false, self-administered inven-
participants described below was based on this tory of personality pathology designed to coor-
measure and is, thus, retained as a control vari- dinate with the DSM-III-R Axis II PDs. The
able in the analyses we report; specific scores MCMI-II possesses excellent psychometric
from the IPDE-S are not used in the analyses. properties and yields separate dimensional
scores for each of the 11 Axis II PDs (Millon,
1987). We defined our other SZDPD outcome
Outcome measures
variable as the scale score for the disorder de-
Personality disorder assessments. rived from the MCMI-II, specifically the
SZDPD base rate corrected score (BR; for tech-
IPDE. The IPDE is the well-known semi-
nical detail on BR scores, see Millon, 1987).
structured interview procedure that assesses
both DSM and ICD-10 PD features (Loranger,
1999; Loranger et al., 1994; Loranger, Sartor- Axis I diagnostic measure
ious, & Janca, 1996), and was used in the World
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R
Health Organization/Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and
Disorders: Nonpatient Version (SCID-NP).
Mental Health Administration sponsored Inter-
The SCID-NP (Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, &
national Pilot Study of Personality Disorders
First, 1990) is a semistructured DSM-III-R
(Loranger et al., 1994, 1996). The IPDE has ex-
Axis I clinical interview for use with nonpa-
cellent psychometric properties, and it has been
tients. The clinical interviewers were trained
shown to be robust as a diagnostic assessment
on the use of the SCID-NP using the video-
tool for PDs even in the face of mental state
tape-based training system provided with the
(anxiety, depression) changes. The DSM-III-R
SCID-NP. All interviewers achieved high reli-
criteria were assessed in this study because
ability with criterial cases (intraclass correlation
these were the criteria in effect at the time the
coefficients . .80 for symptom assessments).
LSPD was undertaken. We note the DSM-III-
The SCID interview was done first followed
R and the later DSM-IV criteria bear consider-
by the IPDE as is customary practice for thor-
able resemblance to one another and the funda-
ough Axis II assessments. The Axis I diagnostic
mental PD constructs are the same in both
information generated by the SCID is not a pri-
nomenclatures. Clinically experienced inter-
mary focus of this report, although we do note
viewers received intensive training in IPDE
the results of some sensitivity analyses that in-
administration and scoring by Armand W.
corporate data from the SCID later in this report.
Loranger, and were supervised throughout the
project by the first author, who was blind to
the participants’ identity, putative PD status, Predictors
and all prior assessment information. The inter-
Because our questions focus on change, time is
rater reliability for IPDE assessments (based on
the principal “Level 1” predictor in our analy-
intraclass correlation coefficients) was excel-
ses. It was measured in years from a subject’s
lent at all three waves, ranging between .84
entry into the study. Although all participants
and .92 for all PD dimensions. The interviewers
contributed three waves of data, each being as-
were (a) blind to the putative PD group status of
sessed repeatedly on the same schedule, the
the participants, (b) blind to all prior LSPD PD
time between assessments varied marginally
assessment data, and (c) the same interviewer
from subject to subject because of the practical
never assessed the same subject more than
constraints of data collection. For instance, the
once. We defined one of our SZDPD outcome
average age of participants at each was therefore
variables as the dimensional score total for the
18.78 years (SD ¼ 0.51) at the first wave, 19.83
disorder derived from the IPDE.
years (SD ¼ 0.54) at the second wave, and
21.70 years (SD ¼ 0.56) at the third wave. To
Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—II implement the latent growth analysis, which
(MCMI-II). The MCMI-II (Choca & Van Den- buys some of its analytic sophistication at the
berg, 1997; Millon, 1987) is the well-known expense of the flexibility of the individual
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1219

growth modeling approach, we needed to as- ticipants’ initial assignment (established prior
sume a common measurement schedule across to IPDE and MCMI-II data collection) to either
all participants, and so we set the times of mea- a “possible personality disorder” (PPD) or “no
surement for each person equal to their be- personality disorder” (NPD) group according to
tween-person averages, at each wave. Declaring the IPDE-Screen (IPDE-S; Lenzenweger,
the origin of time as entry into the study, then, 1999; Lenzenweger et al., 1997). As detailed
in our analyses time takes on the values of 0, in Lenzenweger at al. (1997), PPD participants
0.95 and 2.82 years at the first, second, and met the diagnostic threshold on the IPDE-S for
third waves, respectively. In our analyses, we at least one specific DSM-III-R PD, whereas
then took the natural logarithm of its values NPD participants: (a) did not meet the DSM-
(i.e., time value þ 1) before incorporating III-R defined threshold for diagnosis, and (b)
time into out Level 1 models to linearize the in- had fewer than 10 PD features across all disor-
dividual growth trajectories and promote effi- ders on the screener (see Lenzenweger, 2006,
ciency in the estimation in the latent growth for greater detail). Five PPD and three NPD
models (Singer & Willett, 2003). Thus, the participants did not complete all three waves
transformed time values (see Appendix A) of data collection; two of these noncompleting
were 0.00, 0.67, and 1.34. participants died during the course of the study,
one in each subject group. Control predictor
Temperament. The EAS Adult Temperament male identified the subject as either male or fe-
Scale (Buss & Plomin, 1984), is a 20-item male: the sample contained 121 males (47%)
self-report measure, for use with adults, of the and 137 females (53%). We also included a
three major temperament constructs, emotion- two-way interaction of the group and sex pre-
ality (E) (which breaks down further to “fear,” dictors as a covariate in our analyses.
“distress,” and “anger” subdimensions), activ-
ity (A), and sociability (S), which are thought
Statistical analysis
to underlie personality processes in children
and adults. The EAS requires a subject to read As noted above, to evaluate the simultaneous
20 statements and decide how characteristic or impact of individual change in emotionality, ac-
typical each statement is of him/herself on a tivity, and sociability on individual change in
5-point scale. Factor analysis of the childhood the SZDPD features on the IPDE and MCMI-
and adult versions of the EAS has revealed a II over time, we adopted a latent growth model-
consistent structure that supports the basic three ing approach (Willett & Bub, 2005; Willett &
temperaments model of Buss and Plomin Keiley, 2000). Under this approach, we used
(1984; see also Mathieson & Tambs, 1999; the Y and X measurement submodels present
Nærde, Røysamb, & Tambs, 2004) and test– within the general covariance structure model
retest reliability of the Adult EAS is acceptable. to specify individual growth models for our
time-varying outcome and for all three (emo-
tionality, activity, and sociability) of our time-
Covariates
varying predictors simultaneously. With only
Although none of their effects are a principal three waves of data on both outcome and pre-
focus of the findings we report here, we also in- dictors for each subject, we were limited in
cluded three important covariates to control for the complexity of our growth specification.
intrinsic features of the study’s sampling and re- Consequently, after preliminary graphical ex-
search design, in all of our analyses. These were ploration, we specified linear trajectories to
(a) the subject’s age at entry into the study, (b) represent change over time in each of the mea-
the subject’s assignment to putative PD risk sures. Thus, individual trajectories of change
group prior to study onset, and (c) the subject’s in the outcome and each of the predictors are
sex. Age-at-entry was measured in years and implicitly represented, in each case, by a “true
was included to account for marginal heteroge- initial status” and a “true rate of change”
neity in the ages of participants at the beginning parameter for each participant. Then, in the
of the study. The group variable described par- standard structural component of the general
1220 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

Figure 1. The hypothesized path model depicting the relationships among true initial status (elevation) and true
rate of change in four personality indicators of neurobehavioral systems and true initial status (elevation) and true
rate of change in personality disorder (PD) symptomatology over time (control predictors omitted for clarity, see
text for additional details). [A color version of this figure can be viewed online at journals.cambridge.org/dpp]

covariance structure model, we specified Level specify the hypothesized Level 1 change trajec-
2 relationships among the respective individual tories in the outcome and the predictors, and the
growth parameters to articulate our hypotheses structural model that occupies the center of the
about the prediction of change in SZDPD fea- path diagram defines the hypothesized Level 2
tures over time by changes in emotionality, ac- relationships among the individual growth pa-
tivity, and sociability. rameters. Although we included additional con-
In Figure 1, we present a simplified path dia- trol variables (as described above) in all of our
gram that displays, for a generic SZDPD fea- statistical models as predictors of change in
tures outcome (for either IPDE or MCMI-II SZPD features, we have omitted them from
data), the several hypothesized measurement the path diagram in Figure 1 to avoid clutter.
models and structural model that underpin our It is easy to see how this kind of path model
covariance structure analyses. As noted above, could easily be extended to incorporate any
the Y and X measurement models on the right number of additional outcomes and predictors,
and left sides of the path model, respectively, each changing over time, and each measured on
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1221

its own schedule of data collection. It would inferential statistics for both the “full” and the
even be possible to generalize the latent growth “reduced” models later in the paper. A full tax-
model to accommodate differing numbers of onomy of fitted models is available from the
waves of data for each outcome and predictor. first author, on request. All hypothesized latent
We fitted the complete path model to our growth models were fitted assuming that the
longitudinal data using the LISREL software Level 1 measurement errors were homoscedas-
(Version 8.7; Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2004), sepa- tic over time, within each growth process, be-
rately for each of the two SZPD outcome mea- cause supplementary analyses in which the er-
sures (IPDE—interview; MCMI-II—self-re- rors were permitted to be heteroscedastic
port). Appendix A presents the LISREL VIII confirmed that this was reasonable. Finally,
program for fitting the basic latent growth we also conducted sensitivity analysis in which
model discussed here. we fit models in which we included two addi-
As can be seen in Figure 1, having three tional predictors of true initial level and true
waves of longitudinal data available on each rate of change in the SZDPD dimensions,
outcome and predictor permitted us to specify namely, the presence or absence of any form
individual growth models in which predictors of Axis I disorder before or during the study pe-
describing participants’ emotionality, activity, riod as well as the presence or absence of any
and sociability were hypothesized simultane- form of psychological/psychiatric treatment be-
ously to be linear functions of time. This im- fore or during the study period, as predictors.
plied the presence, in the path model, of three We detected no important differences in our
latent constructs representing the true initial findings on inclusion of these covariates.
levels (or elevations, p EMOT
1 , p ACT
1 , p SOC
1 ) and
true slopes (or rates of change, p EMOT 2 , p ACT
2 ,
Results
p SOC
2 ) of the growth trajectories for these neuro-
behavioral indicators. Growth in the total
Clinical characteristics of the sample
SZDPD features (IPDE or MCMI-II), was sim-
ilarly specified in the latent growth model, The lifetime DSM-III-R Axis I diagnoses for
yielding another pair of latent constructs that study participants have been reported pre-
represented the true initial level ( p SZDPD1 ) and viously (Lenzenweger, 1999; Lenzenweger,
true rate of change ( p SZDPD
2 ) of the PD features Johnson, et al., 2004). In overview, 81
growth trajectory in each of the clusters. Then, (62.8%) of the PPD participants received a def-
the individual latent growth parameters repre- inite or probable Axis I diagnosis compared to
senting change over time in emotionality, activ- 32 (26.4%) NPD participants (x 2 ¼ 33.30, df ¼
ity, and sociability were hypothesized to predict 1, p , .00001). Forty-one (31.8%) PPD partic-
the latent growth parameters that represented ipants versus 21 (17.4%) NPD participants re-
change in SZDPD features in the structural ported a prior history of treatment by Wave III
part of the path model, providing 12 structural (x 2 ¼ 6.97, df ¼ 1, p , .008). Finally, at
regression parameters (gammas) that were the Wave I, 11% of the participants qualified for
focus of our analytic attention and addressed an Axis II diagnosis of some sort. The raw rates
our research questions. of diagnosed PDs in the LSPD sample as of
In an initial round of statistical analysis, we Wave I were as follows: 1.2% paranoid, 1.2%
fitted the “full” path model specified in Figure 1 schizoid, 1.6% schizotypal, 0.8% antisocial,
(including all covariates) for each of our two 1.6% borderline, 3.5% histrionic, 3.1% narcis-
outcomes (IPDE SZDPD and MCMI-II sistic, 1.6% obsessive–compulsive, 0.8% pas-
SZDPD features). Then, we tested sequential sive–aggressive, 1.2% avoidant, 0.8% depen-
nested hypotheses to ascertain a parsimonious dent, and 4.3% PD—not otherwise specified.
final fitted model in which we omitted several Moreover, as of the Wave III assessment (i.e.,
of the originally hypothesized paths because 4 years, 16% of the sample received a probable
they were not required (for a description of or definite diagnosis for at least one Axis II PD
this approach, see Singer & Willett, 2003). (or PD—not otherwise specified). Thus, by
We provide parameter estimates and associated Wave III, approximately one in every six
1222 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

Table 2. Bivariate correlations among temperament in this approach is evident in Fig-


temperament and schizoid personality ure 2. In exploratory analyses, Figure 2 presents
disorder (N ¼ 250) at baseline (Wave 1) individual ordinary least squares fitted growth
curves for each of the 250 participants in the
Temperament Dimensions LSPD across the study period. The most striking
aspect of this collection of empirical individual
Emotionality Activity Distress
growth trajectories for both the IPDE and
Schizoid PD MCMI-II SZDPD features scores is the large
IPDE .01 2.14* 2.38** heterogeneity that is evident in the observed
MCMI-II .08 2.16* 2.54** growth. Clearly, the participants did not all fol-
low the same trajectory, nor did their outcome
Note: Values are Pearson product-moment correlation co-
efficients with two-tailed p values. PD, personality disor-
values remain completely consistent over time,
der; IPDE, International Personality Disorder Examination as would have been predicted by the DSM defi-
(Loranger et al., 1991); MCMI-II, Millon Clinical Multiax- nition of PD. Indeed, although some partici-
ial Inventory—II (Millon, 1987). Temperament dimensions
of emotionality, activity, and distress were assessed using
pants’ SZDPD features scores did, in fact, re-
the Buss and Plomin (1984) EAS Adult Temperament main largely unchanged over time, others lost
Scale. SZDPD features over time and yet others devel-
*p , .05. **p , .01.
oped more SZDPD features over time. It was this
variability in SZDPD growth that we sought to
participants in the sample met formal DSM-III- model as a function of change in temperament,
R thresholds for PD diagnoses based on the in our latent growth analyses.
highly conservative IPDE (Loranger, 1999). Table 3 reports our full and reduced fitted la-
When converted to weighted population preva- tent growth models for each of the two SZDPD
lence rates (see Lenzenweger, 2006), the rates outcomes (IPDE and MCMI-II). We present pa-
of diagnosed PD accord well with estimates rameter estimates, approximate p values and se-
for the general population of the United States lected goodness of fit statistics (goodness of fit
(Lenzenweger, Lane, et al., 2007). chi square, Akaike information criterion, root
mean square residual, standardized root mean
square residual) for all fitted models. We also re-
Baseline (Wave I) associations among
port a pseudo-R 2 statistic describing the success
temperament and SZDPD measures
of the prediction of variability in each SZDPD
Although the principal focus of this report is the growth parameter in each model. This latter sta-
latent growth analysis that we report next, we tistic is akin to the adjusted R 2 statistic, more fa-
anticipate that some psychopathologists would miliar in regular regression analyses, and esti-
have an interest in the baseline (Wave I) asso- mates the proportion of variance in the relevant
ciations that were observed between the two SZDPD growth parameter that has been ex-
measures of SZDPD and the temperament vari- plained simultaneously by all predictors in the
ables. We present these statistics in Table 2. In- particular model. The overall average trend for
spection of the table reveals that SZDPD has a SZDPD growth over the study period was to-
robust inverse relationship with sociability ward a lessening of SZDPD features with the
(large effects) across both clinical measures passage of time; however, there were indeed a
and activity has a modest, but statistically sig- nontrivial number of cases where SZDPD symp-
nificant, inverse relationship (small effects) tomatology worsened (i.e., increased) with time.
with activity across both clinical measures. At We now discuss the results for the IPDE and
baseline, SZDPD has very weak associations MCMI-II measures of SZDPD separately.
with the emotionality temperament dimension. With respect to the IPDE SZDPD results, we
found that in the full model, which included the
full array of temperament dimensions as well as
Latent growth analyses
all control variables (sex, group, Group  Sex
The variability in SZDPD growth across partic- interaction, and age at study entry), the true in-
ipants that we sought to model as a function of itial status of sociability was inversely related to
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1223

Figure 2. Individual ordinary least-squares fitted growth trajectories in schizoid personality disorder fea-
tures for 250 participants over the study period. Time is reported in years since the beginning of the study,
and it is centered for each subject using the subject’s age at entry into the study. (a) The International Per-
sonality Disorder Examination (IPDE; Loranger, 1999) assessments and (b) the Millon Clinical Multiaxial
Inventory—II (MCMI-II; Millon, 1987) assessments.
Table 3. Predicting individual growth in schizoid personality disorder features (true initial status and true rate of change) by individual growth
in temperament dimensions of emotionality, activity, and sociability (N ¼ 250)

Growth in Schizoid Personality Disorder Features

IPDE MCMI-II

Full Model Reduced Model Full Model Reduced Model

p1 p2 p1 p2 p1 p2 p1 p2

Parameter Estimates and Approximate p Values

Intercept 7.232 23.916 5.035† 2.071 155.137† 224.754 163.868*** 64.710


EMOT
p1 0.012 0.079 — — 0.644 20.432 — —
p2 0.781 20.403 — — 2.564 26.529 — —
ACT
p1 20.185 0.089 — — 24.125 20.436 — —
p2 20.347 0.122 — — 26.875 5.137 — —
1224

SOC
p1 20.926*** 0.109 2.758*** — 222.975*** 21.599 223.92*** —
p2 20.266 21.670 — — 24.499 232.030 — —
Sex (male) 20.075 0.301 0.001 0.092 10.221* 21.259 9.68* 22.093
Group (PPD) 20.025 20.039 20.163 0.081 1.475 25.515 0.301 26.594*
Group by Sex 0.693 20.797 0.494 20.381† 26.654 1.394 25.994 9.992*
Age at entry 20.147 0.135 20.095 20.118 21.523 1.849 22.262 23.448

Goodness of Fit Statistics

x 2 statistic (df ) 81.84 (58) 108.79 (69) 88.09 (58) 118.39 (69)
Pseudo R2 33.2% 32.5% 24.6% 7.1% 53.3% 82.4% 52.4% 25.1%
AIC 324.25 339.84 255.53 336.18
RMSR 0.071 0.089 5.253 8.99
SRMSR 0.059 0.076 0.035 0.046

Note: The table contains parameter estimates and approximate p values from the structural portion of selected full and reduced (final) fitted latent growth models for the interview (IPDE) and
self-report (MCMI-II) data. IPDE, International Personality Disorder Examination (Loranger et al., 1991); MCMI-II, Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—II (Millon, 1987); p1 , true initial
status; p2 , true rate of change; EMOT, emotionality temperament dimension; ACT, activity temperament dimension; SOC, sociability temperament dimension; AIC, Akaike information
criterion; RMSR, root mean square residual; SRMSR, standardized root mean square residual. Sex is coded 1 ¼ male, 0 ¼ female; Group (PPD) is coded 1 ¼ probable personality disorder,
0 ¼ normal control.
†p , .10. *p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001 (two tailed).
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1225

true initial SZDPD status. No other tempera- change in SZPD such that male/PPD participants
ment nor control variable predicted the true in- exhibited a positive rate of change in SZDPD
itial status of SZDPD. In terms of rate of change (i.e., greater amount of symptomatology) over
in the SZDPD features from the IPDE, no pre- time. There were no other predictors that were re-
dictor among the temperament or control vari- lated to true rate of change in SZDPD for the
able set was statistically significantly related MCMI-II data in the reduced model.
to true rate of change. We fitted a reduced
model that included the true initial level of
Sensitivity analyses: Impact of treatment
sociability in the prediction of true initial status
and/or Axis I
of SZDPD as well as the control variables as
predictors of both true initial status and true In addition to our primary analyses of SZDPD
rate of change in SZDPD. Not unlike the results described above, we also conducted sensitivity
for the full model, true initial status of sociabil- analyses to evaluate the impact of the presence
ity was inversely related to true initial status or absence of any Axis I disorder and/or the pre-
levels for SZDPD. No other predictors had a sence or absence of any form of psychiatric/
statistically significant relationship with true psychological treatment in the participants ei-
rate of change in SZDPD for the IPDE data, ther before, or during, the study period on the
in the reduced model. growth observed for the SZDPD feature dimen-
With respect to the MCMI-II SZDPD results, sions on the IPDE and MCMI-II. We believed
we found that in the full model, which also in- that this was a reasonable set of analyses to
cluded the full array of temperament dimensions undertake given the possibility that comorbid
as well as the control variables (sex, group, Group Axis I pathology might alter the growth trajec-
 Sex interaction, and age at study entry), again tories, as might the impact of any form of treat-
true initial status of sociability was inversely re- ment. We found that the presence of any form
lated to the true initial SZDPD status of the partic- of Axis I disorder either before or during the
ipants. In this analysis, subject sex was also asso- study period was unrelated to true rate of
ciated with true initial status of SZDPD, with change for SZDPD features over time, and
males exhibiting higher levels. No other tempera- that the presence of any form of psychiatric/
ment or control variables were related to true in- psychological treatment was also unrelated to
itial status of SZDPD. In terms of rate of change true change for SZDPD features over time.
in the SZDPD features from the MCMI-II, no pre-
dictor in the temperament or control variable set
Discussion
was statistically significantly related to true rate
of change in SZDPD features. The emerging corpus of empirical results from
We again fitted a reduced model that included the longitudinal study of personality pathology
the true initial level of sociability as a predictor of suggests that the disorders are considerably
true initial status of SZDPD as well as the control more flexible and plastic than previously
variables as predictors of both true initial status thought. Indeed, the prevailing doctrine in psy-
and true rate of change in SZDPD. Not unlike chiatry and clinical psychology over the past
the results for the full model, true initial status 100 years held that PDs are stable, enduring,
of sociability was inversely related to true initial and traitlike. Like the other PDs for which
status levels for SZDPD. In the reduced model, data on long-term growth are now available,
as before, males exhibited higher levels of true SZDPD also shows evidence of flexibility,
initial status of SZDPD features. With respect change, and malleability. The data from the
to the rate of change in SZDPD features on the LSPD for SZDPD underscore this flexibility
MCMI-II, there were no statistically significant for SZDPD on both structured diagnostic inter-
predictors of the true rate of change in SZDPD view assessments as well as self-report scores
features in the full model. However, of interest, on a psychometric inventory. In this study, we
in the reduced model, we found a statistically sig- sought to determine if changes in temperament
nificant relationship between the “Subject Group over time were in some manner related to
[PPD vs. NC]Sex interaction” and true rate of growth in SZDPD over time. The overall thrust
1226 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

of our findings was that true rate of change in ogy might be more disclosing on a psychomet-
any of three core temperament dimensions ric inventory than in an interview? We hope to
(emotionality, activity, sociability) was essen- pursue this issue in subsequent analyses.
tially unrelated to true rate of change in SZDPD One of the interesting developmental ques-
over time. The only statistically significant pre- tions that arose for us in this set of analyses con-
dictive relationship that we found for a tempera- cerned the ultimate fate of participants with ele-
ment construct was with sociability. That find- vated SZDPD features. Could it be the case that
ing was relatively straightforward in that we for some participants with noteworthy SZDPD
found that participants with lower initial levels features at study entry cease to show SZDPD
of sociability exhibited higher initial levels of features over time because their psychopathol-
SZDPD, for both the IPDE and MCMI-II ogy has shifted somewhat, perhaps toward
data. This consistency in finding across the more severe Cluster A or (even) psychotic phe-
self-report and interview assessment instrument nomenology? There are few data bearing upon
modalities was encouraging and suggested to this developmental question; however, a hint of
us that sociability, perhaps reflective of the un- this possibility comes from the interesting work
derlying affiliation system (see Depue & Len- of Sula Wolff. She and her colleagues (Wolff,
zenweger, 2001, 2005), was an important deter- Townshend, McGuire, & Weeks, 1991) as-
minant of schizoid symtomatology. It also sessed adult longitudinal outcomes in children
accord well with a prior analysis of growth in initially diagnosed as schizoid. A considerable
the Cluster A symptomatology in the LSPD proportion (i.e., 75%) of those children initially
study participants in relation to growth in com- diagnosed as schizoid went on to develop more
munal positive emotion (affiliation; Lenzenwe- clearly identifiable schizotypal (as opposed to
ger & Willett, 2007), such that initial levels of schizoid) personality disorder features in adult-
communal positive emotion were inversely re- hood. In fact, of those studied (n ¼ 32), two
lated to initial levels of Cluster A features (on went on to develop clinically diagnosed schizo-
the IPDE). Somewhat surprising to us was phrenia. Thus, the import of the Wolff et al.
that changes in emotionality, which consists (1991) findings for the present study is that
of anxiety, fear, and anger, were not associated longer term longitudinal followup may shed
either with true initial level or rate of change in light on this critical question: Do many of the
SZDPD. This was surprising, because a wealth more severely affected schizoid LSPD partici-
of clinical observation and conjecture (see pants go on to develop schizotypal disorders
Bernstein et al., 2009) suggests that consider- (or schizotypal features) as they move into the
able fear and anxiety attend schizoid pathology. later decades of life? Would such conversion
It is possible, however, that with only three to schizotypal PD be associated with identifi-
waves of data available on the outcome and pre- able changes in temperament or would this re-
dictors, that we did not have sufficient statistical flect, perhaps, the developmental unfolding of
power to detect these relationships. The statisti- a schizophrenia-related liability (as opposed to
cal power of any longitudinal research design, a temperament based alteration)? We hope to
and the reliability with which we can estimate examine this possibility as we develop a meth-
individual true rate of change (Singer & Willett, odological extension of the present latent
2003), are strongly dependent on the number of growth curve approach for that purpose.
waves of data that have been collected, provid- On a broader methodological note of inter-
ing a considerable incentive for including addi- est, the latent growth modeling approach that
tional waves of data in any panel. Finally, one we utilized here offers powerful advantages in
aspect of our results raises an interesting meth- the study of the predictors of change or growth
odological question for us, namely, why was it in developmental psychopathology research.
that male/PPD participants showed a higher This is particularly true with respect to systems
(positive) rate of change (worsening of that involve multiple component parts or pro-
SZDPD) over time on the MCMI-II, but not cesses that could be affecting (or be correlated
on the IPDE? Could it be that in some instances, with) a given outcome simultaneously. For ex-
male participants with some evidence of pathol- ample, as it is likely that SZDPD (and all PDs
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1227

for that matter) features reflect the phenotypic able latent growth mixture components (Mu-
manifestations of multiple dynamic interactive thén, 2004, 2006; Nagin, 1999, 2005; Nagin &
neurobehavioral systems at work and each of Tremblay, 2001; see also Connell & Frye, 2006).
these interactive systems themselves likely Several caveats must also be considered with
show change and growth across the life course, these data. First, the LSPD sample is clearly
then an understanding of this process requires more homogenous in age, educational achieve-
an appropriate analytic strategy that must be ment, and social class than the US population at
able to handle simultaneous growth in several large as well as consists of young adults, fea-
systems, as well as take various background tures that may have differentially affected the
variables into account (e.g., sex, age). Our la- study results. Perhaps the most effective way
tent growth modeling approach achieves this to assess the generalizability of findings from
goal as it unifies the powerful growth curve the LSPD is to evaluate whether prior core
methodology (Rogosa, Brandt, & Zimoski, LSPD findings have been replicated, and they
1982; Rogosa & Willett, 1985) that has proven have. For example, the LSPD-based estimate
so useful in the investigation of change in psy- for PD prevalence in the community (11%;
chological and behavioral phenomena over Lenzenweger et al., 1997) has now been
time with the flexibility of covariance structure broadly replicated several times in US nonclini-
analysis. In this way, we have been able to move cal community samples (Crawford et al., 2005;
freely through an exploration of the impact of Samuels et al., 2002) and the US general popu-
multiple important variables, some changing lation (Lenzenweger, Lane, et al., 2007), as
(emotionality, activity, sociability) and others well as in Norwegian (Torgersen, Kringlen, &
not (sex, group status, age at entry), in predict- Cramer, 2001) and United Kingdom (Coid,
ing growth in SZDPD. We also note that our Yang, Tyrer, Roberts, & Ullrich, 2006) samples
present approach for investigating “growth on (for review, see Lenzenweger, 2008). Further-
growth” represents a compelling and superior more, the basic patterns of change in mean levels
statistical alternative to the analysis of cross- of general PD features over time initially re-
lagged correlations, which has been a common, ported for the LSPD sample (Lenzenweger,
but problematic (Rogosa, 1980), way of exam- 1999) were subsequently replicated in both clin-
ining relationships between two variables that ical (Shea et al., 2002; Zanarini et al., 2003) and
are changing simultaneously over time. nonclinical community (Johnson et al., 2000)
In exploring SZDPD data from the LSPD, it samples. Thus, although the present sample is
has become evident to us that it is likely that our somewhat more compressed in terms of demo-
study sample, like many in all areas of psycho- graphic background characteristics, this has not
pathology research, probably contains an ad- lead to findings at odds with those obtained in
mixture of participants. For example, our sam- other epidemiological or longitudinal PD re-
ple contains a proportion of participants that search. As noted above, the prevalence of
were unimpaired at the baseline entry point of SZDPD in the population tends to run in the
the study and remained so throughout the study range of 1.0%, which was precisely the rate of
period. Such participants reside within the data SZDPD in the population studied here.
set and, in a manner of speaking, they have their Second, we also note that adjustment to uni-
own (somewhat) unique growth pattern: they versity life across the college years may have
show no growth and inflate the data set with played some role in the changes we observed.
zero values (i.e., known statistically as “zero in- However, we do note that the IPDE specifically
flation”). This suggests to us that the potential requires one to respond after reflecting on 5
utility of using mixture modeling approaches, years of behavior (not just the current time or re-
which have proven useful in other psychopathol- cent past) (Loranger, 1999), and it is relatively
ogy efforts (e.g., Lenzenweger, Clarkin, Yeo- robust in the face of mental state alterations
mans, Kernberg, & Levy, 2008; Lenzenweger, (Loranger et al., 1991). Third, given that the
McLachlan, & Rubin, 2007; Lenzenweger & LSPD participants were selected from a popula-
Moldin, 1990), to parse the observed growth tra- tion of first-year university students, the sample
jectories of the LSPD participants into identifi- may have been somewhat censored for a portion
1228 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

of the most severely PD affected individuals. design is observational not experimental and
However, one must be cautious in ascribing un- so, although we have specified the hypothe-
due levels of mental health to participants who sized direction of relations in the growth model,
happen to be selected for academic achieve- we cannot address questions of causation. The
ment as such selection does not confer immu- temperament systems studied here are thought
nity to psychopathology. In fact, several forms to reflect underlying neurobehavioral, brain-
of PD might actually be enriched in such a sam- based systems and they may be, in part, respon-
ple (e.g., obsessive–compulsive PD, narcissis- sible ultimately for personality and personality
tic PD, schizoid PD). To this end, we note disorder; however, our data analyses cannot test
that 16% (or one in every six) of the LSPD sam- these conjectures.
ple participants was diagnosed with a formal It has long been assumed that SZDPD and the
Axis II disorder by the end of the study period other PDs are in place relatively early in the life
(i.e., by Wave III) using the highly conservative course (i.e., late adolescence, early adulthood;
IPDE. Many other participants met intermedi- see DSM; APA, 1980, 1987, 1994) and that
ate levels of PD criteria (e.g., two or three cri- they stay in place for the most part across the
teria) that fell short of DSM diagnostic thresh- life span, as if carved in granite. In our prior anal-
old counts, but indicated the presence of some yses of the LSPD (Lenzenweger, 1999; Lenzen-
degree PD disturbance of clinical intensity weger, Johnson, et al., 2004) data, we have re-
nonetheless according to the IPDE. Such inter- ported evidence of flexibility in most PD
mediate levels of PD pathology are frequently features over time, a fact that challenged this
seen even in clinical samples (e.g., Loranger long-held assumption regarding the traitlike qual-
et al., 1991). We note also that 45.2% of the ity of PDs over time. In short, those prior reports
LSPD participants had a lifetime (or current) documented that an appreciable amount of
Axis I disorder by the end of college and these change was occurring in the PDs on average
data are broadly consistent with the ubiquitous over time. However, those reports did not specify
nature of Axis I disorders in the US population what was driving, or was correlated with, such
(see Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, 2005; change. The present paper extends those initial
Kessler et al., 1994). observations by using an approach that seeks to
Fourth, we are mindful that there may be identify the temperament variables (constructs)
other predictors of change in personality disor- that do (or do not) predict individual change in
der. Some may be a consequence of important PD features over time, thus moving from a
time-varying processes (e.g., other personality merely descriptive to a more explanatory,
factors), whereas others may not (e.g., presence model-testing research posture. Although Wil-
of severe childhood sexual abuse would be a liam James (1890, p. 121) claimed “by the age
time invariant predictor). Thus, there may thirty, the character has set like plaster, and will
well be other factors that would be predictive never soften again,” we now know this is true
of true rate of change in the PDs over time. only for some some aspects of normal personality
We hope to establish such hypotheses, and (Srivastava, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2003; Ro-
probe the LSPD database more deeply in terms berts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006). Of impor-
of SZDPD as well as the other PDs, in the com- tance, we now know, insofar as one might want
ing years to explore such possibilities as well as to suggest SZDPD is a form of character or per-
collect additional data relevant to this possibil- sonality, SZDPD is not set like “plaster” at an
ity upon upcoming Wave IV and Wave V as- early age with little prospects for change as as-
sessments. On a related note, it may also be sumed by the official psychiatric nomenclature
the case that some of the DSM PDs may not re- and common clinical lore (Johnson et al., 2000;
flect solely the functioning of personality sys- Lenzenweger, 1999; Lenzenweger, Johnson,
tems, specifically the Cluster A PDs. It may et al., 2004; Shea et al., 2002; Zanarini et al.,
be the case that paranoid and schizotypal PDs 2003). Our research findings provide some initial
represent variants of schizophrenia and thus coordinates for future explorations of those as-
are less related to personality (cf. Lenzenweger, pects of both temperament and personality disor-
1998). Finally, we emphasize that our research der where the plaster seems not to harden.
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1229

References Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., Skodol, A. E., Hama-
gan, F., & Brook, J. S. (2000). Age-related change in
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and personality disorder trait levels between early adolescence
statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed). Wash- and adulthood: A community-based longitudinal inves-
ington, DC: Author. tigation. Acta Psychiatra Scandinavica, 102, 265–275.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2004). LISREL 8.7 [Com-
statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed. rev.). puter Software]. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software
Washington, DC: Author. International.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Wash- bases of childhood shyness. Science, 240, 167–171.
ington, DC: Author. Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., & Walters, E. E.
Bernstein, D. P., Arntz, A., & Travaglini, L. (2009). Schiz- (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of the
oid and avoidant personality disorders. In P. H. Blaney 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbid-
& T. Millon (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psychopathology ity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry,
(2nd ed., pp. 586–601). New York: Oxford University 62, 617–627.
Press. Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B.,
Blonigen, D. M., Carslon, M. D., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime and
F., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). Stability and change in per- 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders
sonality traits from late adolescence to early adulthood: in the United States: Results from the National Comorbid-
A longitudinal-twin study. Journal of Personality, 76, ity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8–19.
229–266. Lenzenweger, M. F. (1998). Schizotypy and schizotypic
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early psychopathology: Mapping an alternative expression
developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. of schizophrenia liability. In M. F. Lenzenweger & R.
Choca, J. P., & Van Denburg, E. (1997). Interpretive guide H. Dworkin (Eds.), Origins and development of schizo-
to the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (2nd ed.). phrenia: Advances in experimental psychopathology
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. (pp. 93–121). Washington, DC: American Psychologi-
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D., & Przybeck, T. (1993). A cal Association.
psychobiological model of temperament and character. Lenzenweger, M. F. (1999). Stability and change in person-
Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990. ality disorder features: The Longitudinal Study of Per-
Coid, J., Yang, M., Tyrer, P., Roberts, A., & Ullrich, S. sonality Disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry,
(2006). Prevalence and correlates of personality disor- 56, 1009–1015.
der among adults aged 16 to 74 in Great Britain. British Lenzenweger, M. F. (2006). The Longitudinal Study of Per-
Journal of Psychiatry, 188, 423–431. sonality Disorders: History, design considerations, and
Connell, A. M., & Frye, A. A. (2006). Growth mixture mod- initial findings. Journal of Personality Disorders, 20,
eling in developmental psychology: overview and dem- 645–670.
onstration of heterogeneity in developmental trajecto- Lenzenweger, M. F. (2008). Epidemiology of personality
ries of adolescent antisocial behavior. Infant and Child disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 31,
Development, 15, 609–621. 395–403.
Crawford, T. N., Cohen, P., Johnson, J. G., Kasen, S., First, Lenzenweger, M. F. (2009). Schizotypic psychopathology:
M. B., Gordon, K., et al. (2005). Self-reported person- Theory, evidence, and future directions. In P. H. Blaney
ality disorder in the Children in the Community Sample: & T. Millon (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psychopathology
Convergent and prospective validity in late adolescence (2nd ed., pp. 692–722). New York: Oxford University
and adulthood. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, Press.
30–52. Lenzenweger, M. F., & Castro, D. D. (2005). Predicting
Depue, R. A., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (2001). A neuro- change in borderline personality: Using neurobehav-
behavioral dimensional model of personality disorders ioral systems indicators within an individual growth
In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), The handbook of personality curve framework. Development and Psychopathology,
disorders (pp. 136–176). New York: Guilford Press. 17, 1207–1237.
Depue, R. A., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (2005). A neurobe- Lenzenweger, M. F., Clarkin, J. F., Fertuck, E. A., & Kern-
havioral model of personality disturbance. In M. F. berg, O. F. (2004). Executive neurocognitive function-
Lenzenweger & J. F. Clarkin (Eds.), Major theories of ing and neurobehavioral systems indicators in border-
personality disorder (2nd ed., pp. 391–453). New York: line personality disorder: A preliminary study. Journal
Guilford Press. of Personality Disorders, 18, 421–438.
Depue, R. A., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (2006). A multidi- Lenzenweger, M. F., Clarkin, J. F., Yeomans, F. E., Kern-
mensional neurobehavioral model of personality distur- berg, O. F., & Levy, K. N. (2008). Refining the border-
bance. In R. F. Krueger & J. L. Tackett (Eds.), Person- line personality disorder phenotype through finite mix-
ality and psychopathology (pp. 210–261). New York: ture modeling: Implications for classification. Journal
Guilford Press. of Personality Disorders, 22, 313–331.
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Arousal, affect, Lenzenweger, M. F., Johnson, M. D., & Willett, J. B.
and attention as components of temperament. Journal (2004). Individual growth curve analysis illuminates
of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 958–966. stability and change in personality disorder features:
Durbin, E. C., & Klein, D. N. (2006). Ten-year stability of The Longitudinal Study of Personality Disorders.
personality disorders among outpatients with mood dis- Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 1015–1024.
orders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 75–84. Lenzenweger, M. F., Lane, M., Loranger, A. W., & Kessler,
James, W. (1950). Principles of psychology (Vol. 1). R. C. (2007). DSM-IV personality disorders in the
New York: Dover Publications. (Original work pub- National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R).
lished 1890) Biological Psychiatry, 62, 553–564.
1230 M. F. Lenzenweger and J. B. Willett

Lenzenweger, M. F., Loranger, A. W., Korfine, L., & Neff, Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006).
C. (1997). Detecting personality disorders in a non- Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits
clinical population: Application of a 2-stage procedure across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal
for case identification. Archives of General Psychiatry, studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 1–25.
54, 345–351. Rogosa, D. (1980). A critique of cross-lagged correlations.
Lenzenweger, M. F., McLachlan, G., & Rubin, D. B. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 245–258.
(2007). Resolving the latent structure of schizophre- Rogosa, D., Brandt, D., & Zimowski, M. (1982). A growth
nia endophenotypes using expectation–maximization- curve approach to the measurement of change. Psycho-
based finite mixture modeling. Journal of Abnormal logical Bulletin, 92, 726–748.
Psychology, 116, 16–29. Rogosa, D. R., & Willett, J. B. (1985). Understanding cor-
Lenzenweger, M. F., & Moldin, S. O. (1990). Discerning relates of change by modeling individual differences in
the latent structure of hypothetical psychosis proneness growth. Psychometrika, 50, 203–228.
through admixture analysis. Psychiatry Research, 33, Samuels, J. E., Eaton, W. W., Bienvenu, O. J., Brown, C.,
243–257. Costa, P. T., & Nestadt, G. (2002). Prevalence and cor-
Lenzenweger, M. F., & Willett, J. B. (2007). Modeling relates of personality disorders in a community sample.
individual change in personality disorder features as a British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 536–542.
function of simultaneous individual change in personal- Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., &
ity dimensions linked to neurobehavioral systems: The Rauch, S. L. (2003). Inhibited and uninhibited infants
Longitudinal Study of Personality Disorders. Journal “grown up”: Adult amygdalar response to novelty.
of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 684–700. Science, 300, 1952–1953.
Loranger, A. W. (1999). International Personality Disorder Seivewright, H., Tyrer, P., & Johnson, T. (2002). Change in
Examination: DSM-IV and ICD-10 interviews. Odessa, personality status in neurotic disorders. Lancet, 359,
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. 2253–2254.
Loranger, A. W., Lenzenweger, M. F., Gartner, A., Susman, Shea, M., Stout, R., Gunderson, J., Morey, L., Grilo, C.,
V., Herzig, J., Zammit, G. K., et al. (1991). Trait–state McGlashan, T., et al. (2002). Short-term diagnostic sta-
artifacts and the diagnosis of personality disorders. bility of schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obses-
Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 720–728. sive–compulsive personality disorders. American Jour-
Loranger, A. W., Sartorius, N., Andreoli, A., Berger, P., nal of Psychiatry, 159, 2036–2041.
Buchheim, P., Channabasavanna, S. M., et al. (1994). Shiner, R. L. (1998). How shall we speak of children’s per-
The International Personality Disorder Examination sonalities in middle childhood? A preliminary taxon-
(IPDE). The World Health Organization/Alcohol, omy. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 308–332.
Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration Inter- Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal
national Pilot Study of Personality Disorders. Archives data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence.
of General Psychiatry, 51, 215–224. New York: Oxford University Press.
Loranger, A. W., Sartorius, N., & Janca, A. (Eds). (1996). Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., Gibbon, M., & First, M.
Assessment and diagnosis of personality disorders: (1990). Users guide for the structured clinical interview
The International Personality Disorder Examination for DSM-III-R. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
(IPDE). New York: Cambridge University Press. Press.
Mathieson, K. S., & Tambs, K. (1999). The EAS Tempera- Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J.
ment Questionnaire—Factor structure, age trends, reli- (2003). Development of personality in early and middle
ability, and stability in a Norwegian sample. Journal adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change? Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 431–439. of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1041–1053.
Millon, T. (1987). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—II Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and devel-
manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. opment. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Muthén, B. (2004). Latent variable analysis: Growth mixture Torgersen, S., Kringlen, E., & Cramer, V. (2001). The prev-
modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. In alence of personality disorders in a community sample.
D. Kaplan (Ed.), Handbook of quantitative methodology Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 590–596.
for the social sciences (pp. 345–368). Newbury Park, Waller, N. G., Lilienfeld, S. O., Tellegen, A., & Lykken, D.
CA: Sage. T. (1991). The Tridimensional Personality Question-
Muthén, B. (2006). The potential of growth mixture modeling. naire: Structural validity and comparison with the Mul-
Infant and Child Development, 15, 623–625. tidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Multivariate
Nærde, A., Røysamb, E., & Tambs, K. (2004). Temperament Behavioral Research, 26, 1–23.
in adults—Reliability, stability, and factor structure of the Willett, J. B., & Bub, K. L. (2005). Structural equation
EAS Temperament Survey. Journal of Personality Assess- modeling: Latent growth curve analysis. In B. S. Everitt
ment, 82, 71–79. & D. C. Howell (Eds.), Encyclopedia of statistics in
Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: behavioral science (Vol. 2, pp. 772–779). Chichester:
A semiparametric group-based approach. Psychological Wiley.
Methods, 4, 139–157. Willett, J. B., & Keiley, M. K. (2000). Using covariance
Nagin, D. A. (2005). Group-based modeling of develop- structure analysis to model change over time. In
ment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. H. E. A. Tinsley & S. D. Brown (Eds.), Handbook of
Nagin, D. S. & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Analyzing develop- applied multivariate statistics and mathematical model-
mental trajectories of distinct but related behaviors: A ing (pp. 665–694). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
group-based method. Psychological Methods, 6, 18–34. Wolff, S., Townshend, R., McGuire, R. J., & Weeks, D. J.
Putnam, S. P., Rothbart, M. K., & Gartstein, M. A. (2008). (1991). “Schizoid” personality in childhood and adult
Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of fine-grained life II: Adult adjustment and the continuity with schizo-
temperament during infancy, toddlerhood, and early typal personality disorder. British Journal of Psychia-
childhood. Infant and Child Development, 17, 387–405. try, 159, 620–629.
Change in schizoid personality disorder and temperament 1231

Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., Reich, B., psychopathology: 6-year prospective follow-up of the
& Silk, K. R. (2005). The McLean Study of Adult phenomenology of borderline personality disorder.
Development (MSAD): Overview and implications of American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 274–283.
the first six years of prospective follow-up. Journal of Zimmerman, M., Rothschild, L., & Chleminski, I. (2005).
Personality Disorders, 19, 505–523. The prevalence of DSM-IV personality disorders in psy-
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., & Silk, K. chiatric outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry,
R. (2003). The longitudinal course of borderline 162, 1911–1918.

Appendix A

LISREL VIII program for fitting the hypothesized latent growth model in Figure 1

Job title
TI Temperament growth predicts schizoid personality disorder growth
Raw data input
DA NI¼16 NO¼250
RA FI¼C:\IPDESZD.dat
Variable labeling and selection
LA
szd1 szd2 szd3 emot1 emot2 emot3 act1 act2 act3 soc1 soc2 soc3 Male PPD GS Entry
SE
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16/
Model specification
MO NY¼3 NE¼2 TY¼ZE LY¼FU,FI TE¼DI,FR C
NX¼13 NK¼10 TX¼ZE LX¼FU,FI TD¼DI,FR PH¼SY,FR C
AL¼FR GA¼FU,FR KA¼FR BE¼ZE PS¼SY,FR
LE
SzdPi1 SzdPi2
LK
Emotpi1 Emotpi2 Actpi1 Actpi2 Socpi1 Socpi2 Male PPD GS Entry
VA 1 LY(1,1) LY(2,1) LY(3,1)
VA 0.00001 LY(1,2)
VA 0.67 LY(2,2)
VA 1.34 LY(3,2)
EQ TE(1,1) TE(2,2) TE(3,3)
VA 1 LX(1,1) LX(2,1) LX(3,1) LX(4,3) LX(5,3) LX(6,3) LX(7,5) LX(8,5) LX(9,5)
VA 0.00001 LX(1,2) LX(4,4) LX(7,6)
VA 0.67 LX(2,2) LX(5,4) LX(8,6)
VA 1.34 LX(3,2) LX(6,4) LX(9,6)
VA 1 LX(10,7) LX(11,8) LX(12,9) LX(13,10)
EQ TD(1,1) TD(2,2) TD(3,3)
EQ TD(4,4) TD(5,5) TD(6,6)
EQ TD(7,7) TD(8,8) TD(9,9)
FI TD(10,10) TD(11,11) TD(12,12) TD(13,13)
VA .00001 TD(10,10) TD(11,11) TD(12,12) TD(13,13)
Creation of path diagram
PD
Creation of output
OU TV RS AD¼800 SS IT¼20000 ND¼4

Note: See Willett and Keiley (2000) and Singer and Willett (2003) for extended discussion of other examples of using
covariance structure analysis to model multivariate change over time.

You might also like