Review: Ecology and Evolution of Communication in Social Insects

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Leading Edge

Review

Ecology and Evolution


of Communication in Social Insects
Sara Diana Leonhardt,1,4 Florian Menzel,2,4 Volker Nehring,3,4 and Thomas Schmitt1,*
1Department of Animal Ecology and Tropical Biology, University of Würzburg, 97074 Würzburg, Germany
2Evolutionary Biology, Institute of Zoology, University of Mainz, 55128 Mainz, Germany
3Department of Evolutionary Biology and Animal Ecology, University of Freiburg, 79104 Freiburg, Germany
4Co-first authors

*Correspondence: thomas.schmitt@uni-wuerzburg.de
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2016.01.035

Insect life strategies comprise all levels of sociality from solitary to eusocial, in which individuals
form persistent groups and divide labor. With increasing social complexity, the need to communi-
cate a greater diversity of messages arose to coordinate division of labor, group cohesion, and
concerted actions. Here we summarize the knowledge on prominent messages in social insects
that inform about reproduction, group membership, resource locations, and threats and discuss
potential evolutionary trajectories of each message in the context of social complexity.

Introduction uals start to detect substances emitted by other individuals. At


Communication in social insects has fascinated scientists for that stage the chemicals effectively become ’’cues.’’ Cues are
centuries. One of the most famous examples is the dance lan- informative traits of individuals that are unintentionally presented
guage of honeybees. Karl von Frisch showed that honeybees by an emitter and used by a receiver to infer information about
dance in order to inform their nestmates about the location of the emitter (Figure 1). When receivers respond and emitters
resources (von Frisch, 1967), a breakthrough for which he was benefit from the receivers’ responses, emitters evolve more
granted the Nobel Prize in 1973. Since then, our understanding sophisticated and reliable ‘‘signals,’’ i.e., co-evolved traits in-
of the topics and mechanisms of communication in social in- tended to transfer information; this is defined as ‘‘true com-
sects has significantly advanced for a wide variety of species. munication’’ (Figure 1) (Dusenbery, 1992). However, information
Insects show all levels of social organization, from solitary spe- provided by a sender can also be aimed at manipulating the
cies where conspecific individuals rarely meet to eusocial spe- receiver to its disadvantage (e.g., predators attracting prey by
cies with large, persistent colonies, where groups of individuals mimicking prey signals) or be exploited by a second receiver to
(‘‘castes’’) perform different tasks (division of labor). The evolu- the sender’s disadvantage (e.g., by parasites to locate hosts)
tion of sociality in insects can be compared by analogy to other (Steiger et al., 2011), rendering a precise definition of communi-
major evolutionary transitions, such as the evolution of multicel- cation elusive.
lularity. For example, the division of labor in eusocial insects re- Subsequently, in the animal kingdom signaling modes beyond
sembles the diversification of cells that take over specific tasks in chemicals evolved, including acoustic/vibrational, visual, and
a multicellular organism. Just as somatic cells are disposable tactile communication. In insects, chemical cues and signals
and reproduction is restricted to germline cells, there is division (pheromones) still dominate, although all other modes of
of labor between social insect workers and royals (Boomsma, communication occur (Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 2011; Wyatt,
2013). And just like separate organs communicate using hor- 2014) (Figure 2).
mones, individuals in insect societies use pheromones as mes- Both solitary and social insects communicate. In strictly
sengers. The diversity of information, i.e., the number of different solitary insects, communication is largely restricted to sexual
messages that become encoded by signals, increases with the context and involves signals that attract and inform mating part-
complexity of social organization. ners, frequently with a multitude of signals transferred between
In this Review, we give an overview of the current knowledge mating partners during courtship. The most prominent and wide-
on intraspecific communication in social insects, defined as the spread examples for such signals are sex pheromones (single
transfer between individuals of information that is either mutually substances or blends of chemicals) (Wyatt, 2014), which are
beneficial or beneficial for one and neutral for the other partici- largely species and sex specific to ensure mating with an appro-
pant. We provide insights into the evolution of the messages priate partner. Sexual signals can also be visual, acoustic/vibra-
used by social insects and discuss how their diversity increased tional, or tactile or involve multiple communication modes, e.g.,
with the evolution of social complexity. chemical and tactile/visual signals such as those used for mating
in Drosophila melanogaster (Bontonou and Wicker-Thomas,
Evolution of Communication with Sociality 2014). Mating signals can also bear information on kinship so in-
The oldest mode of communication is the transfer of chemicals dividuals can recognize each other’s relatedness and avoid
(Wyatt, 2014). Chemical communication evolves when individ- inbreeding (Lihoreau et al., 2007).

Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc. 1277


but accept all brood that is of the right age (calculated from time
of egg laying) (Steiger, 2015).
Signals such as aggregation pheromones that help individuals
to find each other, dispersion pheromones that disassemble
groups (Heifetz et al., 1998), alarm signals, and recruitment
signals that guide group members to resources are important
tools for supporting insect societies. Group gathering can be
accomplished by species-specific long-range aggregation pher-
omones that attract both sexes of a species (Figure 4B). Here,
the most prominent and best studied examples are insect pest
species, e.g., gregarious bark beetles, which use species-spe-
cific aggregation pheromones to overcome a tree’s defense in
a concerted action (Byers, 1989).
In the most derived insect societies, reproduction is monopo-
lized by one (or few) individual(s) (‘‘queens’’/’’kings’’), whereas
the others (‘‘workers’’) perform foraging, nest maintenance,
and nursing duties (eusociality; Figure 4D).
Below we highlight four types of messages typically used in
eusocial insect colonies: queen signals (which organize repro-
ductive division of labor), nestmate recognition (which ensures
group cohesion), recruitment, and alarm signals (both of which
organize concerted group actions). We discuss evolutionary
trajectories of each message in the context of social complexity
and finally suggest future directions to better understand the
evolution of communication in insect societies.

Queen Signaling
Figure 1. Proposed Stages in the Evolution of Chemical Communi-
cation in Insects
Signals produced by queens in eusocial colonies indicate the
Ancestrally, chemicals are unintentionally emitted by an individual (emitter) queen’s presence and/or fertility to workers who then abandon
and not detected by any other individual. If another individual (receiver) evolves their own reproduction and help with rearing siblings (Keller
the capability to perceive the unintentionally emitted chemical and benefits and Nonacs, 1993) (Figure 4D). Workers can further use this in-
from the transferred information, the chemical becomes a cue. If there is
additional advantage to the emitter from receiving reciprocal information, the formation to control each other’s reproduction by for instance
emitter becomes a sender and the cue a signal, and this can be considered destroying eggs laid by other workers (egg policing: Ratnieks,
‘‘true communication.’’ Adapted from Wyatt (2014), with image by Ivan Hino- 1988; Ratnieks and Reeve, 1992). When a colony loses its queen
josa (www.flickr.com/photos/ivan_hinojosa).
or the queen loses fertility, the queen signal diminishes and non-
sterile workers can start laying eggs themselves (Keller and Non-
As insects evolved higher levels of sociality, the information acs, 1993). Consequently, communicating the presence and
they needed to exchange between group members diversified fertility of a queen reinforces reproductive division of labor and
to include division of labor, collaborative resource utilization, benefits the social organization within colonies.
and collective defensive actions (Figures 3 and 4). This trend is Signals Used
reflected in the diversity of chemical signals required to maintain In species with small colonies (<100 individuals), queen signals
eusocial insect colonies (Box 1). However, it is important to note can comprise aggressive queen-worker interactions and even vi-
that increased message diversity does not necessarily correlate sual signals in addition to chemical signals (Tannure-Nascimento
with increased complexity of the signals themselves (Kather and et al., 2008). In contrast, large colonies (several 100 individuals)
Martin 2015). Indeed, signals used in solitary insects are in some typically rely entirely on chemical signals, the so-called queen
cases more complex in terms of composition than some signals pheromones (Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990; Keller and Nonacs,
used in social insects, and pheromone signals in social insects 1993). In very large colonies (>1,000 individuals), information
most likely evolved from systems regulating more basal behav- about the queen’s presence and fertility is further communicated
iors in solitary insects (Blum, 1969) (Figure 5). For example, indirectly, e.g., through messenger workers who spread the
although kin recognition is important for solitary insects to queen pheromone in the colony (e.g., in the honeybee: Naumann
avoid inbreeding, in insects with brood-care, kin recognition is et al., 1991) or through a specific chemical signature on queen-
expected to play an even more important role to prevent parents laid eggs (Endler et al., 2004).
from feeding or protecting someone else’s offspring. However, Queen pheromones are likely present in most eusocial insect
kin recognition has rarely been shown (Linsenmair, 1987). species and are thought to be located on the queen’s cuticle
Instead, most species seem to employ rules of thumb whenever (Monnin, 2006; Oi et al., 2015). Different species appear to use
simpler cues suffice. Illustrating this point, parents of the subso- different (albeit partly structurally related) compounds as queen
cial burying beetle Nicrophorus vespilloides (Figure 4A) cannot pheromones (Kocher and Grozinger, 2011; Oi et al., 2015).
discriminate between their own and artificially introduced larvae However, only a few studies experimentally demonstrated that

1278 Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc.


Figure 2. Types of Social Organization and Communication Modes Employed by Major Clades in the Insecta
Triangle = solitary insects: no evidence of social behavior other than inter- and intrasexual; circle = communal insects: gregarious, aggregate/form groups;
diamond = subsocial insects: perform brood care; star = (primitively) eusocial. Communication typically takes place between sexes (e.g., courtship, mating),
among the same sex (e.g., dominance, territorial marking), among groups (e.g., aggregation, nestmate recognition, trail following, alarm), or among family
members (e.g., begging, kin recognition) and can comprise the following modes: ear = acoustic or vibrational, nose = chemical, eye = visual, hand = tactile.
Phylogeny based on Misof et al. (2014). Communication modes used by each taxon were assessed by a key word search with Google Scholar. We
first entered the Latin taxon name or common taxon name and ‘‘communication’’ or (more specifically) ‘‘visual,’’ ‘‘acoustic,’’ ‘‘vibrational,’’ ‘‘tactile,’’ or
‘‘chemical’’ communication.’’ For taxa with many hits, the search was further refined by adding ‘‘intersexual’’ or ‘‘intrasexual’’ (or ‘‘among or between
individuals/sexes/males/females/males and females’’) or ‘‘among or between groups.’’ In doing so, we most likely biased the graph toward more thoroughly
studied taxa (such as Diptera or Hymenoptera), whereas communication modes present in less well-studied taxa (e.g., Zygentoma) may have been
underestimated.

putative queen pheromones actually inhibited reproductive ac- rivatives, two alcohols, and two phenolics) (Keeling et al., 2003)
tivity in workers (Kocher and Grozinger, 2011). For example, that regulate worker ovarian activity, inhibit rearing of new
queens of the black garden ant (Lasius niger) produce large queens and juvenile hormone synthesis in workers, delay the
amounts of a methyl-branched hydrocarbon in both their own transition from nursing to foraging, attract workers and males,
and the chemical profile of their eggs, which inhibits worker and coordinate swarming (Le Conte and Hefetz, 2008; Kocher
aggression and ovarian activity (Holman et al., 2010). Queens and Grozinger, 2011). Thus, QMP can act as both a sex phero-
of the red imported fire ant (Solenopsis invicta) produce several mone and a social coordinator.
compounds (two pyrones and a terpene), which inhibit reproduc- In other insects, various hydrocarbons are positively corre-
tive activity in virgin queens and reinforce helping behavior in lated with ovarian activity and therefore candidates for queen
workers (Kocher and Grozinger, 2011). Similarly, an ester and pheromones, e.g., in paper wasps (Polistes dominulus, Sledge
an alcohol from secondary queens and queen-laid eggs of the et al., 2004), termites (Cryptotermes secundus, Weil et al.,
termite Reticulitermes speratus attract workers and inhibit differ- 2009), and ants (Pachycondyla inversa, D’Ettorre et al., 2004;
entiation of new queens (Matsuura et al., 2010). several Lasius species, Holman et al., 2013).
Beyond doubt, queen pheromones have been most inten- Evolutionary Trajectories
sively studied in the European honeybee Apis mellifera, wherein In many eusocial insect species, queen pheromones seem to be
the queen mandibular pheromone (QMP) represents the most represented by specific cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs, Box 2)
complex queen pheromone or pheromone cocktail examined that increase or decrease in parallel with fertility and ovarian
so far. It comprises at least nine components (five fatty acid de- activity (Monnin, 2006) or with the hormonal changes underlying

Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc. 1279


Figure 4. Social Interactions in Insects
(A) A female of the burying beetle Nicrophorus vespilloides feeds its larvae.
(B) Firebugs (Pyrrhocoris apterus) aggregate.
(C) A forager of the stingless bee Trigona recursa lays a scent mark.
(D) A royal chamber of the West African termite Macrotermes bellicosus. The
queen (white) is surrounded by the substantially smaller king (black, below the
queen) and workers.
(E) Three Crematogaster modiglianii workers from the same colony fight a
non-nestmate (center).
(F) Two Wasmannia auropunctata workers antennate to sense each other‘s
recognition cues.
(G) Two nestmate workers of Polyrhachis ypsilon ants perform trophallaxis to
exchange food and cuticular hydrocarbons.
Photo credit: H. Bellmann (A), S. Leonhardt (B), S. Jarau (C), J. Korb (D),
F. Menzel (E and G), and A. Wild (F).

Figure 3. Levels of Sociality and Evolution of Increasing Social


Complexity in Insects Recent studies suggest that wasps, ants, and some bees all
In solitary insects, individuals hardly interact except for random encounters
and for mating (e.g., many dipteran species); in subsocial insects, either males use structurally related hydrocarbons as queen pheromones,
or females or both remain with their brood (e.g., burying beetles of the genus which likely evolved from conserved signals of solitary ancestors
Nicrophorus); in communal or gregarious insects, individuals of both sexes (Oi et al., 2015; Van Oystaeyen et al., 2014). CHCs associated
aggregate either periodically or permanently for, e.g., better predator defense
with fertility are further similar among several lower termites,
(e.g., tent caterpillars Hyphantria cunea) or to overcome plant defenses
and more efficiently exploit resources (e.g., tent caterpillars of the genus suggesting that the use of CHCs as queen pheromones either
Malacosoma and the moth family Lasiocampidae, bark beetles, Scolytinae); is common to most (if not all) insects or has convergently evolved
in eusocial insects, individuals perform periodic (in the case of primitive in many insect species (Hoffmann et al., 2014; Weil et al., 2009).
eusociality) or permanent (in the case of the most derived eusociality) family
groups with at least two overlapping generations, division of labor, and However, this hypothesis was derived primarily from potential
cooperative brood care (e.g., several bee and wasp species, all ant and termite queen pheromones that were found to be correlated with repro-
species). ductive activity but were not necessarily tested for their physio-
logical or behavioral activity. Given the structural diversity of
the few queen pheromones that have hitherto been function-
reproductive activity (Oi et al., 2015). Evidence from multiple ally confirmed (see above), queen pheromones may not be as
species further indicates that changes in cuticular profiles are conserved as suggested by Oi et al. (2015) and Van Oystaeyen
associated not only with fertility but also with a female’s mating et al. (2014).
status, and that males prefer chemical profiles of unmated over Manipulation or Honest Signal?
mated females (Thomas, 2011). Thus, queen pheromones may Most recent reviews on queen pheromones agree that they
have evolved from compounds that are produced at the onset represent an honest signal that indicates the presence and/
of ovarian activity in solitary insects and, e.g., signal successful or fertility of a dominant, reproductively active female, rather
mating to males (Caliari Oliveira et al., 2015; Oi et al., 2015; than a manipulative cue of mothers to coerce daughters into
Figure 5). However, there are no comprehensive comparative involuntary helping (e.g., Kocher and Grozinger, 2011;
studies of compounds that signal fertility in solitary and social Oi et al., 2015; Ratnieks and Wenseleers, 2008, Keller and
species, which would be needed to test this hypothesis (Kocher Nonacs 1993). However, Kocher and Grozinger (2011) further
and Grozinger, 2011). suggest that the honeybee QMP might represent a control

1280 Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc.


Box 1. Communication Contexts in Insects and Modes and Sig-
Nestmate Recognition
nals Involved: A Non-exhaustive List If an insect colony is to survive, its members must discriminate
nestmates from non-nestmates, only allowing the former to enter
d Mating (in solitary and all social insects): Individuals attract the
opposite sex over a distance, recognize their own species, and
the nest. This discrimination is crucial to prevent non-members
choose between mating partners (e.g., sex pheromones, acoustic of the colony from exploiting a nest’s resources, which would
signals such as chirping). result in the breakdown of the insect colony, as known for hon-
d Brood care (in brood-caring and eusocial insects): Offspring eybee colonies invaded by cape honeybees (Wenseleers et al.,
signals hunger or other needs (e.g., pheromones, tapping). 2011). To discriminate nestmates from non-nestmates, social in-
d Concerted action (in gregarious and eusocial insects): Group sects need colony-specific recognition cues and mechanisms to
members warn each other of potential danger (e.g., alarm perceive and evaluate these cues, followed by either acceptance
pheromones or drumming) and inform others about where to find or rejection (d’Ettorre and Lenoir, 2010).
resources (e.g., trail pheromones, tandem running, waggle dance).
Recognition Cues
d Division of labor (in eusocial insects): Queens specialize in
In species with small colonies, nestmates may recognize each
reproduction and signal their presence and fertility to others
(e.g., queen pheromones); workers take over specific duties
other individually. This is known in ants using chemical cues
(e.g., nursing, entrance guarding, or foraging) and can signal their (van Zweden and d’Ettorre, 2010) and especially common in pa-
task to other workers (e.g., task-specific cuticular profiles). per wasps (Vespidae: Polistes), where nestmates recognize
d Group cohesion (in eusocial insects): To keep resources safe from each other based on visual cues of their faces (Sheehan and Tib-
outsiders, group members must be able to discriminate group betts, 2008). However, virtually all other social insects do not
members from non-members (nestmate recognition). recognize their nestmates individually but rely on colony-specific
chemical cues. These cues typically consist of CHCs (Martin
et al., 2008; van Zweden and d’Ettorre, 2010).
system because one of its components (HVA) acts Perception of the Profile and Behavioral Response
as dopamine antagonist in workers and inhibits their ovarian When two individuals meet, they touch each other with their
activity. Moreover, not all workers equally respond to QMP, antennae and sense each other’s recognition cues (Figure 4F).
which might indicate a counter-strategy by workers to escape If the perceived cues are unfamiliar and/or differ from nestmate
the queen’s influence under a queen-worker arms race cues, the individuals will respond aggressively (Figure 4E) (van
scenario. In turn, Van Oystaeyen et al. (2014) argue that, under Zweden and d’Ettorre, 2010). The decision to attack or accept
such a scenario, queen pheromones should evolve quickly always embodies two sources of error: erroneously rejecting
and be composed of rather complex blends. They found no nestmates and erroneously accepting non-nestmates. Colonies
evidence for such a rapid evolution of queen pheromones trade off the costs of the two errors against each other
but strong evidence in several bee species for workers and adjust the trade-off according to the frequency of intruders
regulating production according to their own interests, (D’Ettorre et al., 2004; Couvillon et al., 2008; Fürst et al., 2012).
and therefore they argue against queen control in social Generally, false acceptance of non-nestmates occurs in various
insects in general and honeybees in particular (Oi et al., species, whereas false rejection of nestmates is very rare
2015). Hence, without more studies on actual functional (Johnson et al., 2011). Honeybee guards, for instance, adjust
properties of putative queen pheromones across a wide(r) their acceptance threshold depending on nectar availability:
range of species, the debate on signal honesty remains unre- when nectar is scarce, the costs of accepting non-nestmates
solved. that steal nectar from the colony are higher. Hence, the guards
Chemical Complexity and Diversification reduce their acceptance threshold at the cost of erroneously
Because the precise components of queen pheromones are rejecting nestmates (Couvillon et al., 2008; Downs and Rat-
unknown for most insect species, we can only speculate on nieks, 2000).
whether they became more complex (i.e., consisting of Evolutionary Trajectories
numerous compounds) and/or specific (i.e., consisting of only The nestmate recognition system probably evolved from kin
one or few compounds) over evolutionary time. If queen phero- recognition. Ancestral insect colonies only consisted of close
mones are evolutionarily conserved, as suggested by Van relatives (Hughes et al., 2008), where individuals with similar
Oystaeyen et al. (2014), there should be little or no increase in CHC cues were likely to be kin. However, many derived euso-
pheromone complexity. Alternatively, signal complexity could cial insect species have multiple nests and have more complex
increase if queen pheromones serve more than one function colony structures with multiple queens or multiple fathers (if the
(Van Oystaeyen et al., 2014). For example, in the honeybee queen mated several times), reducing worker relatedness. Low
genus Apis, major components of queen mandibular pheromone genetic relatedness could in theory result in the evolution of
(the carboxylic acids) appear to be conserved across workers nepotism, wherein multiple family lines within a colony compete
and queens of different species, whereas the less abundant phe- for resources. Nepotism seems however to be rare or absent in
nolics have only been found in derived cavity-nesting species eusocial insects (DeHeer and Ross, 1997; but see Hannonen
(Abrol, 2013). Signal complexity could further increase as a and Sundström, 2003), which is surprising given that, in several
consequence of an arms race between queen manipulation species, CHCs bear honest information on kinship, thus
and workers evolving counter-strategies to escape the manipu- enabling nepotism (Helanterä and d’Ettorre, 2015; Nehring
lation (Kocher and Grozinger, 2011) or to avoid social parasitism et al., 2011). However, in other species, CHCs lack kinship in-
(Martin et al., 2011). formation, which may have evolved to counteract nepotism

Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc. 1281


Figure 5. Evolution of Complex Signal
(Queen Signal) in Eusocial Insect Species
from Signal/Cue that Changes with Physio-
logical State (Ovarian Activity) in Solitary
Ancestor
The CHC profiles of females of several solitary
species change with the onset of ovarian activity
and thus following mating (above left and center)
(e.g., in the Dawson’s burrowing bee Amegilla
dawsoni). This chemical change can be used to
inform a male about a female’s mating status and
thus spares males unsuccessful mating attempts
(above right). In communal or facultative eusocial
species where several females compete for
reproductive dominance, the relationship between
CHC profile and ovarian activity informs both
competitors and potential helpers about the
reproductive potential of each female and can be
used to establish dominance and induce helping
behavior (center) (e.g., in some wasps, bees). In
eusocial species, the signal can then be used to
inform workers on the presence and reproductive
potential of the queen, thereby facilitating division
of labor and cooperative brood care (e.g., in ants,
honeybees).

members of the same colony still


differ chemically: queens and workers
possess different odors, and many spe-
cies possess chemically different worker
castes such as foragers and nurses
(Box 2). To our knowledge, it is still un-
(Wong et al., 2014). Similarly, the CHC profiles of facultative known how these differences are maintained despite contin-
polygynous ant species seem to bear nestmate information uous exchange of CHCs.
but no kinship signal (Helanterä et al., 2011; Martin et al.,
2009). The transition from kin to nestmate recognition was Recruitment to Food Sources
further facilitated by environmentally induced variation in CHC Recruitment is the process of directing foraging workers to a
composition, e.g., through food, gut microbes, or nest-site ma- resource, in most cases food. Insects have invented a wide va-
terial. Thus, even related individuals carry different recognition riety of recruitment strategies and use tactile or chemical sig-
cues if their environments differ (d’Ettorre and Lenoir, 2010). nals, or both. The most famous example of recruitment is the
In ants, individuals of the same colony therefore continuously waggle dance in honeybees (von Frisch, 1967). The dance,
exchange their CHCs through allogrooming (i.e., acquiring pheromones, and flower odors motivate nestmates to forage,
CHCs from a nestmate by licking) and trophallaxis (i.e., and the complex behavioral sequence of tactile and vibrational
mouth-to-mouth transfer of CHCs; Figure 4G). This presumably signals communicates the location of a food resource (Thom
results in a shared colony signature incorporating heritable and et al., 2007).
environment-derived compounds. In bees and wasps, CHC ho- Recruitment Strategies
mogenization seems to be achieved via contact to nest paper In general, a recruiting insect has two tasks: (1) raise attention
or comb wax (van Zweden and d’Ettorre 2010). This colony and motivate others to forage and (2) direct them to the food
odor is likely learned as ants show reduced aggression to source, which can be achieved through either the same or sepa-
non-nestmate cues encountered before. This indicates that rate signals. For example, bumblebees only inform nestmates
they familiarize with, and subsequently tolerate, profiles of about a food resource and its scent through ‘‘excited runs’’
other colonies (Leonhardt et al., 2007) or even other species and the release of a recruitment pheromone but do not provide
(Errard et al., 2006; Menzel et al., 2008). However, the exact information on its location (Dornhaus and Chittka 1999, 2001;
neural processes involved in what is typically described as Dornhaus et al., 2003). Stingless bees similarly excite their nest-
learning of nestmate cues are not well understood and mates but additionally guide them by chemically marking the
currently under debate. The hypotheses put forward range food patch and/or leaving scent trails (Jarau, 2009), which
from receptor adaptation to colony-odor templates stored in consist of pheromone markings left in a ‘‘line’’ between the
the long-term memory (e.g., Ozaki et al., 2005; van Zweden nest and a flower (Figure 4C). Although such scent trails are un-
and d’Ettorre, 2010; Ozaki and Hefetz, 2014; Esponda and known in other bees or wasps, honeybees and bumblebees
Gordon, 2015). Although odor homogenization explains how leave scent marks directly at the food resource. Depending on
nestmate recognition can function based on a shared cue, their foraging success at marked flowers, the other workers learn

1282 Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc.


Box 2. CHCs—Encoding Multiple Messages

CHCs cover the body surface of virtually all insects and protect them against desiccation and pathogens (Blomquist and Bagnères, 2010). The
CHC layer can consist of more than 100 different compounds, and their quantitative and qualitative composition is highly variable across and
within species. Typical compounds include n-alkanes, alkenes, and methyl-branched alkanes (Blomquist and Bagnères, 2010). Their compo-
sition is mainly genetically driven, but also influenced by the insects’ food, nest material (d’Ettorre and Lenoir, 2010), or climatic conditions
(Wagner et al., 2001). CHCs are structurally simple molecules, and many compounds (n-alkanes, monomethyl alkanes) are shared by many
different species. Thus, signal complexity arises not through complex molecules but rather through complex mixtures of structurally simple
compounds.
In solitary insects, CHC profiles contain information about an individual’s species membership and its sex (Thomas and Simmons, 2008). In eusocial
insects, however, CHC profiles encode a multitude of other messages, including fertility or reproductive status (see Queen Signaling) and colony
membership (see Nestmate Recognition). Besides, they can carry information on the bearer’s behavioral caste (scouts, foragers, nurses) (Pam-
minger et al., 2014; Wagner et al., 2001). This facilitates self-organization of division of labor (Greene and Gordon, 2003).
Interestingly, multiple signals encoded in the CHC can override each other. For example, non-nestmate queens are usually accepted in Camponotus
ant nests, likely because the ‘‘queen’’ signal overrides the nestmate recognition signal. As a consequence, the queen is accepted and tended even if
she is a non-nestmate (Helanterä and Ratnieks, 2009; Moore and Liebig, 2010).
How such different information is encoded in the CHC profile has been intensely investigated. Although some information (e.g., queen signals)
might be represented by single substances (Van Oystaeyen et al., 2014), most messages seem to be encoded by quantitative composition of
several compounds, rather than the presence or absence of certain substances (van Zweden and d’Ettorre, 2010). Eusocial insects and especially
ants can effectively discriminate structurally similar hydrocarbons (Sharma et al., 2015). This capacity is reflected on a neuronal and a genetic level:
The numbers of glomeruli and olfactory and gustatory receptor genes increase drastically from non-social (i.e., Nasonia) to eusocial insects
(i.e., bees and ancestors of modern ants) (Tsutsui, 2013; Zhou et al., 2012). At the same time, there is a vast diversification of CHC-producing
gene families (desaturases; Helmkampf et al., 2015), which may be related to the evolution of complex communication systems.

to either avoid or approach them (Saleh and Chittka 2006; Stout pygidial gland provoke trail-following at low concentrations but
and Goulson 2001). alarm at high concentrations (Morgan, 2009). Thus, a complex
In ants and termite recruiters, recruitment strategies seem to interplay of different glands enables finely tuned recruitment,
depend on colony size (Hölldobler and Wilson, 2009). Species which likely evolved from the combination of originally separate
with small colonies often have solitary foragers or perform tan- messages.
dem running, where the recruiters touch recruits to get their
attention and guide them to the food source. Tandem running Alarm Signaling
can also involve additional chemical signals (Hölldobler and Wil- Giant hornets prey on honeybees directly at their hives.
son, 2009). Group recruitment occurs in larger colonies (several When hornet scouts find a suitable prey hive, they call in their
100 workers) and involves trail pheromones to guide a group of nestmates for a mass raid. Some honeybee species (e.g., Apis
foragers to the source. Trail pheromones both motivate and cerana japonica, Apis mellifera cypria) evolved a specific and
direct foragers. effective defense behavior. They can prevent raids by quickly
Evolutionary Trajectories killing the much bigger hornet scouts through concerted action
Dornhaus and Chittka (1999, 2001) suggest that the exciting with thousands of workers forming a ball around the attacking
movements of stingless bees and bumblebees derive from wasp and producing enough heat to roast it to death (Abrol,
ancestral excitatory motor displays of successful foragers that 2013; Ono et al., 1995). To quickly recruit so many defenders,
became ritualized. The honeybee dance, however, is much honeybees evolved a multitude of alarm signals of chemical,
more precise and probably evolved independently from ritual- acoustic, and potentially visual nature (Abrol, 2013; Ono et al.,
ized intention movements (aborted flights toward the target) 1995).
(von Frisch, 1967). Signals Used
In ants, trail pheromones originate from several different Most insect alarm signals are chemical. The honeybee alarm
glands. The gland of origin is usually specific to the ant subfamily pheromone is composed of several substances (Verheggen
(Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990; Morgan, 2009), suggesting that et al., 2010). Its main component is isopentyl acetate (IPA), which
the trail-laying behavior evolved several times independently. is released from the sting apparatus and dispersed by wing
Termites, in contrast, invariably use trail pheromones from the fanning. IPA is emitted during the stinging process but also
sternal gland, suggesting a single evolutionary origin (Jaffe when the sting apparatus remains stuck in a victim, thereby trig-
et al., 2012). gering more honeybees to sting (Bortolotti and Costa, 2014). The
In the ant Paratrechina, compounds from the Dufour’s gland response to the alarm pheromone differs with individual, age,
excite nestmates, whereas compounds from the hind gut trigger and context. For example, honeybees only react to IPA when
trail-following. Dufour’s gland products are typically volatile and they are in groups, and they are most sensitive when they are
function as alarm pheromones in many ant species, which may close to the colony entrance (Bortolotti and Costa, 2014).
explain their exciting effect. The less volatile, longer-lived hind- Alarm pheromones vary among species but are common in all
gut excretions used to demarcate the trail (Witte et al., 2007) eusocial insect lineages and occur in every ant species studied
may have originated from ritualized feces deposition (Hölldobler (Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990). They are emitted from a number of
and Wilson, 1990). In the ant Tapinoma, excretions from the different glands and comprise a surprising diversity of substance

Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc. 1283


classes, from hydrocarbons to spiroacetals, but are typically of from head glands, which contain alarm-eliciting volatile chemi-
low molecular weight so that they are volatile and can transmit cals (Pasteels and Bordereau, 1998).
information over a distance (Bruschini et al., 2008; Hölldobler Chemical Complexity and Diversification
and Wilson, 1990; Verheggen et al., 2010). The phylogenetically most widespread alarm pheromones in
There are two typical responses to alarm pheromones. Spe- ants and wasps are ketones, which may indicate that their
cies with small colonies and ephemeral nests seem to quickly biophysical properties are particularly suited for this function.
retrieve brood and disperse upon alarm (‘‘panic alarms,’’ Wil- Common additions are alcohols, esters, aldehydes, and terpe-
son and Regnier, 1971), whereas species with large compact noids. Wasp venom alarm volatiles can contain similar sub-
colonies tend to attack the cause of the alarm and defend stances but typically contain spiroacetals and pyrazines
the nest. (Bruschini et al., 2008; Moritz and Bürgin, 1987; Sledge et al.,
When it comes to communication over a distance, acoustic 1999). Termites mostly rely on terpenes, e.g., farnesene, which
signals travel much faster and further than chemicals and should is also used for alarm signaling in aphids (Verheggen et al.,
thus be superior when messages, such as threats, are urgent 2010). Other than that, there is very little taxonomic coherence
(Hölldobler, 1999; Nehring et al., 2013). Consequently, acoustic concerning the substances used by different species, which
alarm signals also evolved in some social insects, such as suggests that alarm pheromone blends underlie strong ecolog-
termite species that produce substrate-borne vibrations ical selection (Wilson and Regnier, 1971) or evolved indepen-
(Inta et al., 2009; Virant-Doberlet and Cockl, 2004) or ants of dently multiple times. Across species, the same substances
the genus Camponotus, which drum their abdomen on the sub- can be used as alarm and trail pheromones, so that alarm pher-
strate. This drumming increases awareness of nestmates but omones may have been co-opted as recruitment pheromones,
does not directly elicit an aggressive response. However, when and potentially vice versa, which would further explain variability
ants are alarmed by drumming, they respond faster and more in alarm pheromones (Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990; Witte et al.,
aggressively to subsequently emitted alarm pheromones than 2007).
without prior drumming (Hölldobler, 1999).
Evolutionary Trajectories Perspective
Chemical alarm cues are supposedly old and evolutionarily Although our current knowledge on the evolution of communica-
widespread. In their simplest form, they are chemical sub- tion in eusocial insects has drastically advanced in the last de-
stances leaking from injured bodies and can be used by other cades, many questions still await further research. Most studies
individuals as cues to flee, shoal, seek shelter, or ramp up de- on communication in eusocial insects have largely focused on a
fenses. Receivers subsequently improve perception to reduce few model organisms such as the honeybee (Apis mellifera) and
the predation risk. Emitters, on the other hand, may often not some ant species (e.g., Camponotus, Lasius). Most of these
benefit directly from warning others, unless the receivers’ model systems are highly derived eusocial species compared
behavior (e.g., agitated movements) disturbs and confuses to often simpler structured societies in other species. Thus, it
predators. As soon as emitters benefit, dynamic co-evolution is unclear to what extent generalizations can be made. For a bet-
between senders and receivers can occur, resulting in powerful ter understanding of general evolutionary trajectories of specific
alarm signals. messages in eusocial insects, we need more information from a
A second evolutionary trajectory of alarm pheromones are variety of phylogenetically independent eusocial species.
defensive chemicals excreted by insects when attacked. Similar Studies of solitary and subsocial insects will help to identify po-
to leaking substances, these secretions can be used by tential precursors of signals and the origin of messages used in
bystanders to sense danger and have evolved into more sophis- eusocial insects. Future research should focus on comparing
ticated alarm signals suitable for communication over long chemical signals and message types between multiple, phyloge-
distances. The subsequent use of defensive substances as alarm netically independent lineages of both solitary and eusocial
pheromones is still verifiable in several eusocial species. For species. Although there is little debate that increasing social
example, honeybee stinging releases alarm pheromones (Borto- complexity requires a higher diversity of signals being sent and
lotti and Costa, 2014), and ants spraying toxic formic acid like- received, we have not even begun to understand how the diver-
wise induce alarm behavior (Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990). sity of messages are communicated without interfering with
The ancestral chemical alarm signals in eusocial Hymenoptera each other. This is all the more surprising as the chemical
appear to be components of the sting venom, as is still found in complexity of the involved cues and signals (e.g., CHC) does
vespine and polistine wasps (Bruschini et al., 2008; Moritz and not seem to increase with social complexity. Thus, it remains
Bürgin, 1987; Sledge et al., 1999). In honeybees, pheromone unclear how the same cue or signal can encode multiple
production is spatially removed from the venom glands to Ko- messages that do not seem to interfere. Several contexts of
schevnikov and sting sheath glands (Bortolotti and Costa, communication, such as queen signaling and nestmate recogni-
2014) and to the mandibular glands, which are also used across tion, have been subject to intense research but still lack an inte-
all clades of ants (Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990). Various ant gration of concepts and data. In both cases, several partly
clades additionally use compounds from Dufour’s and pygidial opposing hypotheses have been put forward, none of which
glands. Formicine ants further revert to a defensive secretion, can explain all the data. This includes the question of to what
formic acid, to alarm nestmates (Hölldobler and Wilson, 2009). extent communication between queens and workers is honest
Termites, however, evolved a different system of chemical de- signaling versus coercion. Thus, future research should aim
fense. Species with soldiers often secrete defensive chemicals at formulating testable hypotheses that help to distinguish

1284 Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc.


between alternative explanations and their rigorous testing Downs, S.G., and Ratnieks, F. (2000). Adaptive shifts in honey bee (Apis
among multiple eusocial species in a robust phylogenetic mellifera L.) guarding behavior support predictions of the acceptance
threshold model. Behav. Ecol. 11, 326–333.
framework. First steps in this direction are being taken, but
there is still a long way to go until we gain enough evidence Dusenbery, D.B. (1992). Sensory Ecology (New York: W.H. Freeman and Com-
pany).
for solid conclusions on the evolution of communication in the
context of social complexity. Endler, A., Liebig, J., Schmitt, T., Parker, J.E., Jones, G.R., Schreier, P., and
Hölldobler, B. (2004). Surface hydrocarbons of queen eggs regulate worker
reproduction in a social insect. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 2945–2950.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Errard, C., Hefetz, A., and Jaisson, P. (2006). Social discrimination tuning in
We apologize to all the colleagues whose work we were not able to include due ants: template formation and chemical similarity. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59,
to space limitations. We are very grateful for the helpful comments provided by 353–363.
our working groups and three anonymous reviewers. We further thank Stefan Esponda, F., and Gordon, D.M. (2015). Distributed nestmate recognition in
Jarau, Judith Korb, Alexander Wild, and Heiko Bellmann for allowing us to use ants. Proc. R. Soc. B. 282, 20142838.
their photographs.
Fürst, M.A., Durey, M., and Nash, D.R. (2012). Testing the adjustable threshold
model for intruder recognition on Myrmica ants in the context of a social para-
REFERENCES site. Proc. Biol. Sci. 279, 516–522.
Greene, M.J., and Gordon, D.M. (2003). Social insects: Cuticular hydrocar-
Abrol, D.P. (2013). Asiatic Honeybee Apis cerana: Biodiversity, Conservation,
bons inform task decisions. Nature 423, 32.
and Agricultural Production (Berlin: Springer).
Blomquist, G.J., and Bagnères, A.-G. (2010). Introduction: history and over- Hannonen, M., and Sundström, L. (2003). Sociobiology: Worker nepotism
view of insect hydrocarbons. In Insect Hydrocarbons. Biology, Biochemistry, among polygynous ants. Nature 421, 910.
and Chemical Ecology, G.J. Blomquist and A.-G. Bagnères, eds. (Cambridge: Heifetz, Y., Miloslavski, I., Aizenshtat, Z., and Applebaum, S.W. (1998). Cutic-
Cambridge University Press), pp. 3–18. ular surface hydrocarbons of desert locust nymphs, Schistocerca gregaria,
Blum, M.G. (1969). Alarm pheromones. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 14, 57–80. and their effect on phase behavior. J. Chem. Ecol. 24, 1033–1046.

Bontonou, G., and Wicker-Thomas, C. (2014). Sexual communication in the Helanterä, H., and Ratnieks, F.L.W. (2009). Two independent mechanisms of
Drosophila genus. Insects 5, 439–458. egg recognition in worker Formica fusca ants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63,
573–580.
Boomsma, J.J. (2013). Beyond promiscuity: mate-choice commitments in so-
cial breeding. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20120050. Helanterä, H., and d’Ettorre, P. (2015). A comparative study of egg recognition
Bortolotti, L., and Costa, C. (2014). Chemical communication in the honey bee signature mixtures in Formica ants. Evolution 69, 520–529.
society. In Neurobiology of Chemical Communication, C. Mucignat-Caretta, Helanterä, H., Lee, Y.R., Drijfhout, F.P., and Martin, S.J. (2011). Genetic diver-
ed. (Boca Raton: CRC Press). sity, colony chemical phenotype, and nest mate recognition in the ant Formica
Bradbury, J.W., and Vehrencamp, J.W. (2011). Principles of Animal Communi- fusca. Behav. Ecol. 22, 710–716.
cation (Sunderland: Sinauer Associates). Helmkampf, M., Cash, E., and Gadau, J. (2015). Evolution of the insect desa-
Bruschini, C., Cervo, R., Protti, I., and Turillazzi, S. (2008). Caste differences in turase gene family with an emphasis on social Hymenoptera. Mol. Biol. Evol.
venom volatiles and their effect on alarm behaviour in the paper wasp Polistes 32, 456–471.
dominulus (Christ). J. Exp. Biol. 211, 2442–2449. Hoffmann, K., Gowin, J., Hartfelder, K., and Korb, J. (2014). The scent of roy-
Byers, J.A. (1989). Chemical ecology of bark beetles. Experientia 45, 271–283. alty: a p450 gene signals reproductive status in a social insect. Mol. Biol. Evol.
Caliari Oliveira, R., Oi, C.A., do Nascimento, M.M.C., Vollet-Neto, A., Alves, 31, 2689–2696.
D.A., Campos, M.C., Nascimento, F., and Wenseleers, T. (2015). The origin Hölldobler, B. (1999). Multimodal signals in ant communication. J. Comp.
and evolution of queen and fertility signals in Corbiculate bees. BMC Evol. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 184, 129–141.
Biol. 15, 254.
Hölldobler, B., and Wilson, E.O. (1990). The Ants (Berlin: Springer).
Couvillon, M.J., Robinson, E.J.H., Atkinson, B., Child, L., Dent, K.R., and
Hölldobler, B., and Wilson, E.O. (2009). The Superorganism: The Beauty,
Ratnieks, F.L.W. (2008). En garde: rapid shifts in honeybee, Apis mellifera,
Elegance, and Strangeness of Insect Societies (New York: WW Norton & Com-
guarding behaviour are triggered by onslaught of conspecific intruders.
pany).
Anim. Behav. 76, 1653–1658.
Holman, L., Jørgensen, C.G., Nielsen, J., and d’Ettorre, P. (2010). Identification
d’Ettorre, P., and Lenoir, A. (2010). Nestmate recognition. In Ant Ecology, L.
of an ant queen pheromone regulating worker sterility. Proc. Biol. Sci. 277,
Lach, C.L. Parr, and K.L. Abbott, eds. (Oxford: Oxford University Press),
3793–3800.
pp. 109–194.
D’Ettorre, P., Heinze, J., Schulz, C., Francke, W., and Ayasse, M. (2004). Does Holman, L., Lanfear, R., and d’Ettorre, P. (2013). The evolution of queen pher-
she smell like a queen? Chemoreception of a cuticular hydrocarbon signal in omones in the ant genus Lasius. J. Evol. Biol. 26, 1549–1558.
the ant Pachycondyla inversa. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 1085–1091. Hughes, W.O.H., Oldroyd, B.P., Beekman, M., and Ratnieks, F.L.W. (2008).
DeHeer, C.J., and Ross, K.G. (1997). Lack of detectable nepotism in multiple- Ancestral monogamy shows kin selection is key to the evolution of eusociality.
queen colonies of the fire ant Solenopsis invicta (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Science 320, 1213–1216.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 40, 27–33. Inta, R., Evans, T.A., and Lai, J.C.S. (2009). Effect of vibratory soldier alarm sig-
Dornhaus, A., and Chittka, L. (1999). Evolutionary origins of bee dances. nals on the foraging behavior of subterranean termites (Isoptera: Rhinotermiti-
Nature 401, 1999–1999. dae). J. Econ. Entomol. 102, 121–126.
Dornhaus, A., and Chittka, L. (2001). Food alert in bumblebees (Bombus Jaffe, K., Issa, S., and Sainz-Borgo, C. (2012). Chemical recruitment for
terrestris): Possible mechanisms and evolutionary implications. Behav. Ecol. foraging in ants (Formicidae) and Termites (Isoptera): A revealing comparison.
Sociobiol. 50, 570–576. Psyche (Stuttg.) 2012, 694910.
Dornhaus, A., Brockmann, A., and Chittka, L. (2003). Bumble bees alert to food Jarau, S. (2009). Chemical communication during food exploitation in stingless
with pheromone from tergal gland. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neu- bees. In Food Exploitation by Social Insects, S. Jarau and M. Hrncir, eds.
ral Behav. Physiol. 189, 47–51. (Boca Raton: CRC Press), pp. 223–250.

Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc. 1285


Johnson, B.R., van Wilgenburg, E., and Tsutsui, N.D. (2011). Nestmate recog- Ono, M., Igarashi, T., Ohno, E., and Sasaki, M. (1995). Unusual thermal
nition in social insects: overcoming physiological constraints with collective defence by a honeybee against mass attack by hornets. Nature 377, 334–336.
decision making. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. (Print) 65, 935–944. Ozaki, M., and Hefetz, A. (2014). Neural mechanisms and information process-
Kather, R., and Martin, S.J. (2015). Evolution of cuticular hydrocarbons in the ing in recognition systems. Insects 5, 722–741.
Hymenoptera: a meta-analysis. J. Chem. Ecol. 41, 871–883. Ozaki, M., Wada-Katsumata, A., Fujikawa, K., Iwasaki, M., Yokohari, F., Satoji,
Keeling, C.I., Slessor, K.N., Higo, H.A., and Winston, M.L. (2003). New compo- Y., Nisimura, T., and Yamaoka, R. (2005). Ant nestmate and non-nestmate
nents of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) queen retinue pheromone. Proc. Natl. discrimination by a chemosensory sensillum. Science 309, 311–314.
Acad. Sci. USA 100, 4486–4491. Pamminger, T., Foitzik, S., Kaufmann, K.C., Schützler, N., and Menzel, F.
Keller, L., and Nonacs, P. (1993). The role of queen pheromones in social (2014). Worker personality and its association with spatially structured division
insects: queen control or queen signal? Anim. Behav. 45, 787–794. of labor. PLoS ONE 9, e79616.
Kocher, S.D., and Grozinger, C.M. (2011). Cooperation, conflict, and the evo- Pasteels, J.M., and Bordereau, C. (1998). Releaser pheromones in termites. In
lution of queen pheromones. J. Chem. Ecol. 37, 1263–1275. Pheromone Communication in Social Insects, Ants, Wasps, Bees, and Ter-
mites, R.K. VanderMeer, M.D. Breed, M.L. Winston, and K.E. Espelie, eds.
Le Conte, Y., and Hefetz, A. (2008). Primer pheromones in social hymenoptera.
(Boulder, CO: Westview Press), pp. 193–215.
Annu. Rev. Entomol. 53, 523–542.
Ratnieks, F.L.W. (1988). Reproductive harmony via mutual policing by workers
Leonhardt, S.D., Brandstaetter, A.S., and Kleineidam, C.J. (2007). Reformation
in eusocial Hymenoptera. Am. Nat. 132, 217–236.
process of the neuronal template for nestmate-recognition cues in the carpen-
ter ant Camponotus floridanus. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Ratnieks, F.L.W., and Reeve, H.K. (1992). Conflict in single queen hymenopter-
Behav. Physiol. 193, 993–1000. an societies: the structure of conflict and processes that reduce conflict in
advanced eusocial species. J. Theor. Biol. 158, 33–65.
Lihoreau, M., Zimmer, C., and Rivault, C. (2007). Kin recognition and incest
avoidance in a group-living insect. Behav. Ecol. 18, 880–887. Ratnieks, F.L.W., and Wenseleers, T. (2008). Altruism in insect societies and
beyond: voluntary or enforced? Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 45–52.
Linsenmair, K.E. (1987). Kin recognition in subsocial arthropods in particular in
the desert isopod Hemilepistus reaumuri. In Kin Recognition in Animals, D.J.C. Saleh, N., and Chittka, L. (2006). The importance of experience in the interpre-
Fletcher and C.D. Michener, eds. (Chichester: Wiley), pp. 121–208. tation of conspecific chemical signals. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 215–220.

Martin, S.J., Vitikainen, E., Helanterä, H., and Drijfhout, F.P. (2008). Chemical Sharma, K.R., Enzmann, B.L., Schmidt, Y., Moore, D., Jones, G.R., Parker, J.,
basis of nest-mate discrimination in the ant Formica exsecta. Proc. Biol. Sci. Berger, S.L., Reinberg, D., Zwiebel, L.J., Breit, B., et al. (2015). Cuticular hydro-
275, 1271–1278. carbon pheromones for social behavior and their coding in the ant antenna.
Cell Rep. 12, 1261–1271.
Martin, S.J., Helanterä, H., Kiss, K., Lee, Y.R., and Drijfhout, F.P. (2009).
Polygyny reduces rather than increases nestmate discrimination cue diversity Sheehan, M.J., and Tibbetts, E.A. (2008). Robust long-term social memories in
in Formica exsecta ants. Insect. Soc. 56, 375–383. a paper wasp. Curr. Biol. 18, R851–R852.
Sledge, M.F., Dani, F.R., Fortunato, A., Maschwitz, U., Clarke, S.R., Frances-
Martin, S.J., Helanterä, H., and Drijfhout, F.P. (2011). Is parasite pressure a
cato, E., Hashim, R., Morgan, D., Jones, G.R., and Turillazzi, S. (1999). Venom
driver of chemical cue diversity in ants? Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 496–503.
induces alarm behaviour in the social wasp Polybioides raphigastra (Hyme-
Matsuura, K., Himuro, C., Yokoi, T., Yamamoto, Y., Vargo, E.L., and Keller, L. noptera : Vespidae): an investigation of alarm behaviour, venom volatiles and
(2010). Identification of a pheromone regulating caste differentiation in ter- sting autotomy. Physiol. Entomol. 24, 234–239.
mites. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 12963–12968.
Sledge, M.F., Trinca, I., Massolo, A., Boscaro, F., and Turillazzi, S. (2004).
Menzel, F., Blüthgen, N., and Schmitt, T. (2008). Tropical parabiotic ants: High- Variation in cuticular hydrocarbon signatures, hormonal correlates and estab-
ly unusual cuticular substances and low interspecific discrimination. Front. lishment of reproductive dominance in a polistine wasp. J. Insect Physiol. 50,
Zool. 5, 16. 73–83.
Misof, B., Liu, S., Meusemann, K., Peters, R.S., Donath, A., Mayer, C., Frand- Steiger, S. (2015). Recognition and family life: recognition mechanisms in the
sen, P.B., Ware, J., Flouri, T., Beutel, R.G., et al. (2014). Phylogenomics biparental burying beetle. In Social Recognition in Invertebrates, L. Aquiloni
resolves the timing and pattern of insect evolution. Science 346, 763–767. and E. Tricarico, eds. (Heidelberg: Springer), pp. 249–266.
Monnin, T. (2006). Chemical recognition of reproductive status in social in- Steiger, S., Schmitt, T., and Schaefer, H.M. (2011). The origin and dynamic
sects. Ann. Zool. Fenn. 43, 515–530. evolution of chemical information transfer. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 970–979.
Moore, D., and Liebig, J. (2010). Mixed messages: Fertility signaling interferes Stout, J.C., and Goulson, D. (2001). The use of conspecific and interspecific
with nestmate recognition in the monogynous ant Camponotus floridanus. scent marks by foraging bumblebees and honeybees. Anim. Behav. 62,
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 1011–1018. 183–189.
Morgan, E.D. (2009). Trail pheromones of ants. Physiol. Entomol. 34, 1–17. Tannure-Nascimento, I.C., Nascimento, F.S., and Zucchi, R. (2008). The look
Moritz, R.F.A., and Bürgin, H. (1987). Group response to alarm pheromones in of royalty: visual and odour signals of reproductive status in a paper wasp.
social wasps and the honeybee. Ethology 76, 15–26. Proc. Biol. Sci. 275, 2555–2561.
Naumann, K., Winston, M.L., Slessor, K.N., Prestwich, G.D., and Webster, F.X. Thom, C., Gilley, D.C., Hooper, J., and Esch, H.E. (2007). The scent of the
(1991). Production and transmission of honey bee queen (Apis mellifera L.) waggle dance. PLoS Biol. 5, e228.
mandibular gland pheromone. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29, 321–332. Thomas, M.L. (2011). Detection of female mating status using chemical signals
Nehring, V., Evison, S.E.F., Santorelli, L.A., d’Ettorre, P., and Hughes, W.O. and cues. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 86, 1–13.
(2011). Kin-informative recognition cues in ants. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 1942– Thomas, M.L., and Simmons, L.W. (2008). Sexual dimorphism in cuticular hy-
1948. drocarbons of the Australian field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus (Orthoptera:
Nehring, V., Wyatt, T.D., and D’Ettorre, P. (2013). Noise in chemical communi- Gryllidae). J. Insect Physiol. 54, 1081–1089.
cation. In Animal Signals and Communication, H. Brumm, ed. (New York: Tsutsui, N.D. (2013). Dissecting ant recognition systems in the age of geno-
Springer), pp. 373–405. mics. Biol. Lett. 9, 20130416.
Oi, C.A., van Zweden, J.S., Oliveira, R.C., Van Oystaeyen, A., Nascimento, Van Oystaeyen, A., Oliveira, R.C., Holman, L., van Zweden, J.S., Romero, C.,
F.S., and Wenseleers, T. (2015). The origin and evolution of social insect queen Oi, C.A., d’Ettorre, P., Khalesi, M., Billen, J., Wäckers, F., et al. (2014).
pheromones: Novel hypotheses and outstanding problems. BioEssays 37, Conserved class of queen pheromones stops social insect workers from re-
808–821. producing. Science 343, 287–290.

1286 Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc.


van Zweden, J.S., and d’Ettorre, P. (2010). Nestmate recognition in social in- Wenseleers, T., Alves, D.A., Francoy, T.M., Billen, J., and Imperatriz-Fonseca,
sects and the role of hydrocarbons. In Insect Hydrocarbons. Biology, V.L. (2011). Intraspecific queen parasitism in a highly eusocial bee. Biol. Lett. 7,
Biochemistry, and Chemical Ecology, G.J. Blomquist and A.-G. Bagnères, 173–176.
eds. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), pp. 222–243. Wilson, E.O., and Regnier, F.E. (1971). The evolution of the alarm-defense
Verheggen, F.J., Haubruge, E., and Mescher, M.C. (2010). Alarm pheromones- system in the Formicine ants. Am. Nat. 105, 279–289.
chemical signaling in response to danger. Vitam. Horm. 83, 215–239. Witte, V., Attygalle, A.B., and Meinwald, J. (2007). Complex chemical commu-
nication in the crazy ant Paratrechina longicornis Latreille (Hymenoptera:
Virant-Doberlet, M., and Cockl, A. (2004). Vibrational communication in in-
Formicidae). Chemoecology 17, 57–62.
sects. Neotrop. Entomol. 33, 121–134.
Wong, J.W.Y., Meunier, J., Lucas, C., and Kölliker, M. (2014). Paternal signa-
von Frisch, K. (1967). The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees (Cam-
ture in kin recognition cues of a social insect: concealed in juveniles, revealed
bridge: Harvard University Press).
in adults. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281, 20141236.
Wagner, D., Tissot, M., and Gordon, D. (2001). Task-related environment alters Wyatt, T.D. (2014). Pheromones and Animal Behavior: Chemical Signals and
the cuticular hydrocarbon composition of harvester ants. J. Chem. Ecol. 27, Signatures (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
1805–1819.
Zhou, X., Slone, J.D., Rokas, A., Berger, S.L., Liebig, J., Ray, A., Reinberg, D.,
Weil, T., Hoffmann, K., Kroiss, J., Strohm, E., and Korb, J. (2009). Scent of a and Zwiebel, L.J. (2012). Phylogenetic and transcriptomic analysis of chemo-
queen-cuticular hydrocarbons specific for female reproductives in lower ter- sensory receptors in a pair of divergent ant species reveals sex-specific signa-
mites. Naturwissenschaften 96, 315–319. tures of odor coding. PLoS Genet. 8, e1002930.

Cell 164, March 10, 2016 ª2016 Elsevier Inc. 1287

You might also like