Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/258194224

PTSD Extinction, Reconsolidation, and the Visual-Kinesthetic Dissociation


Protocol

Article  in  Traumatology · June 2012


DOI: 10.1177/1534765611431835

CITATIONS READS
25 1,296

2 authors, including:

Richard Gray
The Research and Recognition Project
25 PUBLICATIONS   84 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The Clinical Efficacy of NLP: A critical appraisal View project

PTSD Extinction, Reconsolidation, and the Visual-Kinesthetic Dissociation Protocol View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Gray on 22 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


431835
11431835Gray and LiottaTraumatology
TMT18210.1177/15347656

Traumatology

PTSD: Extinction, Reconsolidation, and the 18(2) 3­–16


© The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
Visual-Kinesthetic Dissociation Protocol sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1534765611431835
http://tmt.sagepub.com

Richard M. Gray1 and Richard F. Liotta1

Abstract
Every year thousands of returning military, state, and local police officers and civilians of every description suffer from the
intrusive symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Current treatments rooted largely in extinction protocols
require extensive commitments of time and money and are often ineffective. This study reviews several theories of PTSD
and two important mechanisms that explain when treatment does and doesn’t work: extinction and reconsolidation. It
then reviews the research about and suggests an explanatory mechanism for the visual-kinesthetic dissociation protocol (V/
KD), also known as the rewind technique. The technique is notable for its lack of discomfort to the client, the possibility of
being executed as a content-free intervention, its speed of operation, and its long-term, if largely anecdotal, efficacy. A case
study, specific diagnostics for extinction, and reconsolidative mechanisms and suggestions for future research are provided.

Keywords
PTSD, PTSD treatment, PTSD models, visual-kinesthetic dissociation, rewind technique, extinction, reconsolidation

Although recognized in various forms from the time of Diagnosis is made when symptoms (at least one from the
Homer’s Iliad to the present, the symptoms of posttraumatic reexperiencing cluster, three from the avoidance cluster, and
stress disorder (PTSD) were first designated as a distinct two from the hyperarousal cluster) cause clinically signifi-
psychiatric diagnosis with the publication of Diagnostic and cant distress or discomfort and have persisted for a minimum
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder, third edition (DSM- of 1 month (APA, 1994).
III; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1980; Bodkin, The prevalence of PTSD among veterans of the Gulf War,
Detke, Pope, & Hudson, 2007; Davidson & Foa, 1991; Foa Operation Iraqi Freedom, and Operation Enduring Freedom
& Meadows, 1997; Friedhelm & Sack, 2002; McHugh & has been estimated at nearly twice the rate of the noncombat
Treisman, 2007; Spitzer, Wakefield, & First, 2007). population (6%). For Gulf War veterans the rate of preva-
PTSD is defined by the DSM-IV (APA, 1994) in terms of lence ranges between 10% and 11% and between 13% and
five criteria. The first criterion is the traumatizing event. To 17% for veterans of the Iraqi and Afghanistani theaters
qualify as a traumatizing event both of the following must (Gradus, 2010; Hogue et al., 2008). These returning service-
appear: (a) The person experienced, witnessed, or was other- men present a serious problem for treatment professionals on
wise confronted with one or more events that actually two fronts. First, they represent a significant influx of prob-
involved or threatened death, serious injury, or some other lem clients, and second, the currently approved treatments
threat to the physical integrity of that individual or others, provide inconsistent results with varying levels of effective-
and (b) The individual’s response involved intense feelings ness and subsequent relapse. Another problem is that
of horror, fear, or helplessness. extinction-based treatments, which have enjoyed a favorable
The diagnostic criteria are further divided into three position in the treatment literature, often require long-term
symptom clusters: the reexperiencing, the avoidance/ commitments in spite of their inconsistent results (Foa, Keane,
numbing, and the arousal. According to Foa and Meadows & Friedman, 2000; Foa & Meadows, 1997; Massad &
(1997) the intrusion or reexperiencing symptoms include
the hallmark signs of PTSD, including nightmares, intrusive 1
School of Criminal Justice and Legal Studies, Fairleigh Dickinson
thoughts, and flashbacks. The avoidance group includes University, Teaneck, NJ, USA.
efforts to avoid memories of the traumatic experience and
Corresponding Author:
symptoms of emotional numbing. The third symptom cluster
Richard M. Gray, PhD, Assistant Professor, School of Criminal Justice and
includes symptoms of hyperarousal, including sleeplessness, Legal Studies, Fairleigh Dickinson University, 1000 River Road, T-RA2-01,
irritability, and hypervigilance. Other criteria include the Teaneck, NJ 07666, USA
impact and duration of the symptoms (APA, 1994). Email: rmgray@fdu.edu
4 Traumatology 18(2)

Hulsey, 2006; McNally, 2007; Rothbaum, Davis, King, sees emotions as information networks that are presumed to be
Ferris, & Lederhendler, 2003; Rothbaum, Meadows, Resick, stored in long-term memory as both a set of propositions
& Foy, 2000; Schiller et al., 2010; Shalev, Bonne, & Eth, about the circumstance and a set of motor programs in
1996; Ursano et al., 2004; Wessa & Flor, 2007). This article response to the circumstance. When a person experiences an
introduces and suggests a mechanism for a relatively emotion he or she evokes three levels of information: infor-
unknown, short-term treatment for PTSD— the visual- mation about the stimulus and its meaning in a given con-
kinesthetic dissociation protocol (V/KD). text, the appropriate response to the stimulus (emotional
The V/KD protocol (as discussed in the following) is an language, appropriate motor responses, and the autonomic
intervention originally designed in the early 1980s for use responses that support them), and the relationships between
with phobias. The protocol is one of many anecdotally sup- the stimulus and possible responses. For fearful responses,
ported interventions that emerges from the field of neurolin- the networks tend to hold together as consistent patterns of
guistic programming (NLP). NLP is a controversial approach response; they are easily activated—even by partial stimulus
to modeling, replicating, and transforming behavior that representations—and whether or not they result in overt
includes a number of specific therapeutic techniques; among action, there is always a characteristic autonomic response.
these is the V/KD protocol. NLP finds its roots in models of According to this model, treatment of PTSD, or other
the therapeutic techniques of Milton Erickson, Virginia fear-based pathologies, is dependent on two elements:
Satir, and Fritz Perls. NLP has generated a great deal of (1) elicitation of the fearful experience as fully as possible,
anecdotal evidence of effectiveness but has undergone little and (2) modification of the memory structure to include
empirical testing. There have been recent calls from within information (experiences) that transform the meaning of the
the NLP Community for more research into its efficacy memory structure. The PTSD response is generally linked to
claims (Bandler & Grinder, 1975b, 1979; Bolstad, 2002; multiple stimuli and is more easily evoked than other fearful
Bostic St. Clair & Grinder, 2002; Dilts, Grinder, Bandler, & responses. To insure effective treatment, care must be taken
DeLozier, 1980; Wake, 2008). to insure that the fear response is evoked as fully as possible.
In transforming the memory structure at the root of PTSD
symptoms, new information that is incompatible with some
Theoretical Models of PTSD or all of its pathological structure must be incorporated into
Classical Learning Theory the memory schema itself (Foa et al., 2000; Foa & Kozak,
1986; Foa & Meadows, 1997).
There are multiple theories of the mechanisms of PTSD. The
earlier theories were rooted in basic behavioral psychology.
Consistent with classical conditioning paradigms, the trau- Dual Representational Theory
matic memory is associated with implicit and explicit cues More recently, behavioral explanations have given way to
that evoke the memory outside of the conscious control of explanations rooted in cognitive theory that involve levels of
the sufferer. Implicit memory effects related to amygdalar conscious control and an hypothesized need to integrate the
function and conditioned evocation explain anxiety effects memory of the trauma into a coherent life narrative. Among
and attentional effects. Implicit and explicit conditioned these is the dual representation theory of Brewin and
associations to flashbulb memories explain certain aspects Dalgleish (Brewin, Dalgleish, & Joseph, 1996).
of the reexperiencing facets. Secondary effects are attributed Brewin and Dalgleish hold that traumatic memories are
to operant conditioning of efforts to avoid continuing stored in two separate forms: explicit, verbally accessible
trauma. According to Wessa and Flor (2007), aversive learn- memories, and implicit, situationally accessible memories.
ing in PTSD is extremely resistant to extinction because it is Each type of memory is stored in a different manner and in a
supported by a network of second-order conditionings that different brain system. PTSD occurs when traumatic events
maintain behavioral strength in spite of the absence of the produce incomplete or inconsistent records in the two kinds
original unconditioned trauma. There is also evidence of of memory store and when those memories prove to be
impaired responsivity of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex incompatible with the existing schemas and beliefs that
(VMPFC), a crucial mediator of extinction-based learning define the client’s world. Healing proceeds through two
and modulation of amygdalar responses (Diamond, stages as the client moves to (a) integrate the explicit narra-
Campbell, Park, Halonen, & Zoladz, 2007; Foa et al., 2000; tive of the traumatic event with his or her beliefs and expec-
Foa & Meadows, 1997; Gharakhani, Mathew, & Charney, tations (as recommended by social-cognitive theories), and
2006; Layton & Krikorian, 2002; Liberzon, Sripada, & De (b) make the implicit experience explicit by reexperiencing
Kloet, 2007; Rescorla, 1988). and coming to grips with the nonverbal traumatic experi-
ence (as recommended by information-processing theories).
Accordingly, healing is never complete until both kinds of
The Bioinformational Theory processing are accomplished.
A bioinformational theory set forth by Foa and her colleagues Many of these theories are unsatisfactory insofar as they
(Foa et al., 2000; Foa & Kozak, 1986; Foa & Meadows, 1997) tend to rely on top-down processes and ideas of integration
Gray and Liotta 5

rather than a direct assault on the emotional core of the prob- This activation of the hippocampus is short lived and does
lem. In a 2006 review of progress in the neurobiology of not produce a coherent narrative of the event but only the
PTSD, van der Kolk notes that emotions are seldom under sharply focused memory of the trauma or its precursors.
conscious control. He concludes that they are much more These are the flashbulb memories that accompany trauma
often in control of us than we are of them. and are often characterized by what Loftus has called gun
It is instructive that, in line with root behavioral princi- focus and others have described as tunnel vision. It is impor-
ples, Foa et al. (1989), Brewin et al. (1996), and others have tant to note, however, that in the present formulation the hip-
pointed to the need to direct our attention to the core facet of pocampus enters a phase of lessened efficiency rather than
the problem, the traumatic memory itself. total inactivation. The continued lowered sensitivity of the
hippocampus explains the disorientation, amnesia, and the
perception that the event is not connected in time and not
The Temporal Dynamics Model susceptible to integration into the continuing narrative of the
of Emotional Memory Processing client’s life.
A model that appears to specify all of the mechanisms involved At the same time that the hippocampus is recording what
in the basic phenomenology of PTSD at the level of physiol- will become the fragmented flashbulb memories (vivid,
ogy is set forth by Diamond et al. (2007) in their exhaustive eidetic representations of the memory formed under periods of
review, “The Temporal Dynamics Model of Emotional great emotional impact) of the incident (driven by amygdalar
Memory Processing.” According to them, the phenomenol- afferents), the amygdala is also in a phase of enhanced LTP. In
ogy of PTSD is explained by temporally sequenced patterns this case, the amygdala, the center of emotion, creates
of long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression long-lasting and extinction-resistant behavioral connections
(LTD) in the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex between the emotional response and the cues surrounding the
(PFC) as part of a generalized response to stress. event. These connections become the root of the flashbacks,
LTP is the (generally assumed) process whereby short- nightmares, and phobic aspects of PTSD. The event is encoded
term memory is converted into long-term memory. It as a verbally inaccessible set of conditioned responses. Like
includes protein synthesis and physical changes in the struc- the hippocampus, the amygdala enters a refractory period after
ture of neurons at the synapse. LTD refers to processes that the initial period of enhanced LTP. The length of activation
inhibit the creation of memories more generally and sub- for this implicit conditioning phase appears to last somewhat
sumes a more diffuse set of phenomena. longer for the amygdala. In this case, the amygdalar deactiva-
At the center of the theory is the observation that intense tion would explain numbness, fugue states, and dissociations
fear or stress sets in motion a period of enhanced LTP in both in the immediate aftermath of the event.
the amygdala and the hippocampus that is followed shortly Unlike the amygdala and the hippocampus, the PFC—
by a refractory period in which these structures only con- which normally modulates amygdalar function—is not
tinue processing with some difficulty. Like the dual-factor subject to enhanced LTP under the effects of extreme stress
model proposed in Brewin and colleagues’ (1996) and and almost immediately falls into a state of LTD. Insofar as
Layton and Krikorian’s (2002) appeal to amygdalar and hip- it is no longer capable of modulating amygdalar function,
pocampal mechanisms, Diamond et al. (2007) also appeal to the amygdala becomes the driving force in the current state
activation of the amygdala to explain the implicit aspects of of the organism. Moreover, because the PFC is the center
the fear memory and the sequential activation and partial of executive function and is responsible for most higher-
suppression of hippocampal function to explain the flash- level functions, including evaluations, decision making,
bulb aspects of traumatic memory. Crucially, their theory and divided attention tasks, when it enters a state of less-
depends on LTP as the signal that the structure is currently ened responsivity, the capacity for creating a coherent nar-
responding to stressors and focuses on temporal sequencing rative of the stressful circumstance is lost.
as key to the observed effects. Furthermore, because the LTP in the amygdala may occur
Diamond et al. (2007) provide extensive support for the during the refractory phase of hippocampal function, both
idea that under conditions of extreme stress, which create the PFC and the hippocampal editing and organizing func-
high levels of amygdalar activation, the hippocampus tions will be impaired, resulting in emotionally driven but
switches from its higher-order function as integrator of time, often inaccurate memories of the circumstances. These dis-
place, and sequence to a simpler focus on the immediate con- ordered recollections of the event—as a result of stress-
text. Stress initiates a brief period of facilitated LTP, which related inhibition of the integrative functions of the PFC and
is followed by a longer refractory period in which the struc- the hippocampus—may later give rise to fabricated memories,
ture may be thought of as having limited response potentiali- rationalizations, and various psychological and psychiatric
ties. This refractory period isolates the memory and creates a responses that contribute to the inability to make sense of the
near indelible trace of the dangerous circumstance that is traumatic event.
not subject to interference from subsequent stimuli (in the With the caveat that the model provided by Diamond et al.
immediate context) and may be useful for protecting the (2007) is based on animal studies, and some of the mechanisms
organism from similar threats in the future. have not been subjected to extensive confirmation, this
6 Traumatology 18(2)

theory may serve as a foundation for understanding the root somewhat more fragile than the original memories. Extinction,
mechanisms of PTSD and to make useful differentiations therefore, in the classical learning paradigm, refers to the
between the multiple dimensions of the problem. At the heart learning of new information about the changed learning
of the problem are the intrusion or reexperiencing symp- context as it is now provided by the CS. It does not refer to
toms, including flashbacks, intrusive thoughts, and night- the elimination, forgetting, or modification of the memory.
mares. These appear to be rooted in the emotional memories Extinction models in the treatment of PTSD are character-
as both implicit and explicit elements that are fully explained ized by four specific effects through which the behavior may
by the mechanism just described. These may be understood be reestablished or through which relapse occurs. As they
as the core of the disorder and, for the most part, they are the appear in the posttreatment or relapse behavior of PTSD
aversive stimuli that drive the next group: the avoidance clients, they may be viewed as diagnostic of the fact that
cluster. extinction is the specific mechanism underlying the treat-
Within the avoidance cluster are found efforts to avoid ment. These effects are spontaneous recovery, contextual
memories of the traumatic experience and symptoms of renewal, reinstatement, and rapid reacquisition (Bouton,
emotional numbing. Note that numbing may not be fully 2004; Bouton & Moody, 2004; Dillon & Pizzagalli, 2007;
explained by operant processes, for it may arise as the result Hartley & Phelps, 2009; Massad & Hulsey, 2006; Quirk &
of habituation to the more intense stimulation of the trau- Mueller, 2007; Rescorla, 1988; Vervliet, 2008).
matic eruptions. Spontaneous recovery refers to the reoccurrence of the
The third symptom cluster arises as a direct expression of extinguished or unreinforced fear response after the passage
implicit memory effects mediated by the amygdala in the of time. It was first observed by Pavlov and is one of the first
absence of PFC and hippocampal regulation. They include evidences that extinction does not remove the memory. As
sleeplessness, irritability, and hypervigilance (APA, 1994; noted, extinction involves the creation of a new contextual
Davidson & Foa, 1991; Diamond et al., 2007; Foa & Meadows, association to the effect that, in this context, the CS does not
1997; Keane, Weathers, & Foa, 2000; Lamprecht & Sack, predict the feared stimulus (the UCS) and, therefore, the fear-
2002; McHugh & Treisman, 2007; Spitzer et al., 2007). ful response is irrelevant. That new memory of the new con-
A second layer of emotional responses often character- tingencies, if unreinforced, is subject to a time-based decay. It
izes PTSD clients. These include sadness, anger, fear for the is forgotten over time and the fear reemerges (Bouton, 2004;
future, and an inability to make plans. These are character- Bouton & Moody, 2004; Dillon & Pizzagalli, 2007; Massad
ized by Brewer, Dalgleish, and Joseph (1996) as secondary & Hulsey, 2006; Rescorla, 1988; Vervliet, 2008).
emotions. Depending on the level to which they have Contextual renewal refers to the reemergence of the con-
become integrated into the life schemas and personal defini- ditioned response in a new circumstance where the extinc-
tions of the individual, they may or may not be resolved with tion memory was not created. If the client is subjected to
the elimination of the core symptoms. On a final level are unreinforced (extinction) trials in one context, so that the CS
depression, family dysfunction, and substance abuse issues. fails to evoke the feared response in that context, a subse-
These and other comorbidities will generally require treat- quent test of that same CS in another context may show little
ment independent of the central issue, the primary symp- or no reduction in expression. Even though the original fear
toms of PTSD. response may generalize to multiple contexts, extinction
phenomena are much more context dependent. Contextual
renewal is contextually bound; the response is only renewed
Extinction and Reconsolidation in the contexts where the UCS has again appeared (Bouton,
Effects in the Treatment of PTSD 2004; Bouton & Moody, 2004; Dillon & Pizzagalli, 2007;
If PTSD is understood as being, at heart, a group of behaviors Massad & Hulsey, 2006; Rescorla, 1988; Vervliet, 2008).
associated with a classically conditioned fear response, then Reinstatement occurs when the fearful stimulus, the UCS,
neural models of learning and forgetting become crucial is presented without the CS. In that context where the origi-
components of our understanding. Two specific mechanisms nal UCS is presented, despite the fact that the fearful response
appear to be most important theoretical models for the treat- had been fully extinguished, the CS will be restored. It will
ment of PTSD. These are extinction and reconsolidation. not, however, reappear in other contexts where the UCS has
not been presented (Bouton, 2004; Bouton & Moody, 2004;
Dillon & Pizzagalli, 2007; Massad & Hulsey, 2006; Rescorla,
Extinction 1988; Vervliet, 2008).
When the memory linkage between a conditioned stimulus Rapid reacquisition, as the name suggests, describes the
(CS) and a fear-evoking event (unconditioned stimulus reacquisition of the fear memory after it has been success-
[UCS]) is extinguished, a new memory is created that com- fully extinguished. In this case, there is a net savings in the
municates the absence of the feared object and blocks access number of trials needed to reacquire the memory. If, for
to the original memory that signaled the onset of the feared example, the original fear association took 10 trials to install,
event. These new memories tend to be context sensitive and during postextinction training it may take only 3 trials
Gray and Liotta 7

(Bouton, 2004; Bouton & Moody, 2004; Dillon & Pizzagalli, best clinical and research track records. A meta-analysis by
2007; Massad & Hulsey, 2006; Rescorla, 1988; Vervliet, 2008). Bisson and colleagues (2007) evaluated comparative
Extinction has traditionally been held to be the tool of treatments but provided little useful information for our
choice for the treatment of PTSD. Foa and her colleagues purposes.
have indicated that extinction, in its various forms—from Pitman et al. (1996) reviewed several cases of Vietnam
desensitization through imaginal and in vivo exposure—inter Veterans who were subjected to multiple sessions of imagi-
alia, is the most well-researched and most highly regarded of nal flooding. An average improvement between pre- and
treatments and, in combination with cognitive behavioral post-measures of intrusion and avoidance at 26% reduced
interventions or supplements, represents the scientific treat- to 14% in posttreatment follow-up. These findings were con-
ment of choice (Foa et al., 2000; Foa & Meadows, 1997; sistent with other comparisons between combat and non-
Rothbaum et al., 2003; Wessa & Flor, 2007). combat treated trauma victims, including clients from the
Extinction-based exposure treatment is the most common Israeli “Koach” program where similar problems were
form of intervention, and one of the only treatments sup- encountered. In their final analysis, the authors determined
ported and funded by the federal government. Nevertheless, that the extent of emotional processing was unrelated to the
one would expect that relapse data from extinction-based efficacy of treatment measured in terms of the decrease of
studies will provide the following predictable kinds of intrusive symptoms.
relapse behavior. Because PTSD and trauma-related memo-
ries are resistant to extinction, it is to be expected that extinc-
tion effects would be variable at best. Because the extinction Reconsolidation
memory is subject to decay over time, extinction-based treat- During the early 21st century, reconsolidation came to the
ments may be expected to be characterized by a certain level forefront of memory research when it was illustrated that
of temporal instability. In light of the crucial role played by propranolol, a noradrenalin inhibitor, when injected directly
the VMPFC in the inhibition of amygdalar function in into the amygdalar tissue of rats, was capable of modifying
extinction training, the decreased function of those circuits or erasing the trace of traumatic memories. Other studies
under conditions of extreme stress mitigates the efficacy of found that the introduction of anisomysin into the basolat-
exposure models. For these reasons, without further treat- eral amygdala had similar results. Further chemical studies
ment, extinction measures alone may be only partially effec- revealed that the crucial element, further upstream, was the
tive (Diamond et al., 2007; Gharakhani et al., 2006; Liberzon inhibition of glutamate production and release, which pre-
et al., 2007; Wessa & Flor, 2007). vents the reconsolidation of the activated memory (Cao et al.,
An examination of the exhaustive work of Foa and her 2008; Jacek, Ecedil, & LeDoux, 2006; Riccio, Millin, &
colleagues (Foa et al., 2000; Foa & Meadows, 1997) along Bogart, 2006; Tronson & Taylor, 2007).
with data from other researchers (Massad & Hulsey, 2006; This research revived a long-standing debate into the
McNally, 2007; Wessa & Flor, 2007) reflects that extinction- nature of long-term memory. Whereas classical extinction,
based results are inconsistent in their long-term effects. as noted previously, held that new memories blocked access
A review by Shalev and colleagues (1996) finds mixed to permanent associations between conditioned stimuli and
results with cognitive behavioral interventions based largely responses in long-term memory stores, reconsolidation the-
on extinction practices. Rothbaum and colleagues (2000) ory held that the process of long-term memory consolidation
reviewed 19 studies of extinction-based treatments for PTSD took place over time and that each access to the memory
at various levels of sophistication. The studies included rendered the association labile, subject to change. The
exposure treatments, systematic desensitization, and combi- research has also differentiated reconsolidation from extinc-
nations of treatments including exposure elements. The tion; reconsolidation, it was emphasized, is not facilitated
results were uneven and ranged from little or no effect on PTSD extinction (Akirav & Maroun, 2006; Alberini, 2005; Cao
symptoms to 85% reductions in symptoms. Unfortunately, et al., 2008; Debiec, Doyre, Nader, & LeDoux, 2006;
they do not report follow-up studies in a consistent manner Duvarci & Nader, 2004; Forcato, Pedreira, & Maldonado,
that would allow us to assess whether the treatments contin- 2009; Gharakhani et al., 2006; Lee, Milton, & Everitt, 2006;
ued to produce positive effects, lost strength, or possibly Milekic & Alberini, 2002; Nader, Schafe, & LeDoux, 2000;
increased in effect over time. Measurements were often Riccio et al., 2006; Tronel, Milekic, & Alberini, 2005).
reported in terms of percent of change in treatment score, Insofar as the reactivated memory associations were
and when effect sizes were reported they were most often in always reinforced with the same or similar data, in the same
comparison to waiting-list controls and relaxation condi- or a similar context (including subjective contexts) over
tions. This suggests only that the treatments were better than time, the memory grew less and less susceptible to change.
nothing. Practice guidelines for the treatment of PTSD pro- If, however, when the memory was reactivated, the organism
vided by the APA (Ursano et al., 2004) find similar uneven encountered new information, the actual content of the mem-
results and report that exposure-based treatments and EMDR ory may be modified or even erased. Here is a clear mecha-
(eye movement desensitization and reprocessing) have the nism for understanding the variability of long-term memory
8 Traumatology 18(2)

as examined by Elizabeth Loftus and others (Loftus & Procedurally, reconsolidation and extinction are initiated
Yuille, 1984). Lee (2009) has characterized reconsolidation using distinct protocols. The crucial difference appears to be
as a mechanism for maintaining the relevance of learned that a single, short presentation of the CS reactivates and
associations (see also Alberini, 2005; Hupbach, Hardt, labilizes the memory and longer presentations and multiple
Gomez, & Nadel, 2008; Labar, 2007; Tronel et al., 2005). presentations activate the extinction memory. When protein
Just as extinction procedures have distinctive characteris- synthesis inhibitors or other chemical agents are used, they
tics, so, reconsolidation mechanisms can also have their signa- must be administered before the reactivation trial to affect
ture aspects. These include diminution or complete elimination either process. Where experiential effects are used as amnes-
of the target memory, resistance to spontaneous recovery, lack tic agents—essentially modifying, overwriting, or eliminat-
of net gain in reacquisition learning, and lack of contextual ing the target memory—their efficacy is in part determined
renewal. by their temporal proximity to the eliciting stimulus (Lee,
The root material regarding reconsolidation is a body of 2009; Pedreira, Perez-Cuesta, & Maldonado, 2004).
studies of the blockade or enhancement of cellular processes In human studies of the phenomenon, it has been found
related to glutamate activity in various areas of the brain. In that reconsolidation can be used to eliminate or modify the
general, in the activation of a presumably permanent, non– emotional component of a traumatic memory while leaving
hippocampus-dependent memory (Eichenbaum, 2006), infu- the declarative elements intact. It has also been shown that
sions of protein inhibitors administered before memory reconsolidation processes have a direct effect on first-order
activation tended to erase or decrease the intensity of the memory but not on associative chains based on that original
memory, whereas infusions of chemicals that support protein experience (Kindt et al., 2009).
synthesis, enhanced or stabilized the memory. Crucially, the Shiller et al. (2010) have illustrated the efficacy of recon-
memory must be activated by a brief exposure to a first-order solidation in the modification of conditioned fear memories
cue. The effects are time dependent and only occur within a in humans. In this study, massed extinction trials during a
specific window of opportunity after the memory is acti- reconsolidation window created reductions in autonomic
vated. These changes typically do not appear in short-term responses that were not subject to spontaneous recovery or
memory but in measures of long-term memory. The effects reinstatement. Those effects persisted after 1 full year.
may thus be seen to increase over time (Akirav & Maroun,
2006; Alberini, 2005; Debiec, LeDoux, & Nader, 2002;
Duvarci & Nader, 2004; Forcato et al., 2007; Kaang, Lee, & Reconsolidation and the Visual-
Kim, 2009; Kindt, Soeter, & Vervliet, 2009; Nader et al., Kinesthetic Dissociation Protocol
2000; Riccio et al., 2006). Reconsolidation effects have been suggested as a powerful
Memories so treated tend to be permanently removed or method for the treatment of PTSD. Although historically the
permanently modified. If they are subject to spontaneous mechanism has been associated with the nonpharmacologi-
recovery, they reappear in the modified form (Loftus & cal creation of retroactive amnesia, there has been little work
Yuille, 1984). If they have been erased or replaced they can- that explicitly links this material to treatments for PTSD.
not be revived without retraining de novo (Cao et al., 2008; Despite other explanations, the authors believe that a little
Duvarci & Nader, 2004; Forcato et al., 2007, 2009; Kindt known intervention originally described by Richard Bandler
et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2006). There is also some evidence to (1985) makes specific use of reconsolidation mechanisms
suggest that, unlike extinction, reconsolidation may not and deserves serious reconsideration (Forcato et al., 2007;
apply to instrumental conditioning and pure place condition- Labar, 2007; Riccio et al., 2006).
ing. The same evidence may be understood as pointing to The technique, visual-kinesthetic dissociation (V/KD), is
differences in reconsolidation windows for different kinds of supported by anecdotal reports by practitioners that cover
memories or sensitivity to different levels of pretraining for nearly a quarter century. Consistent with the pattern of mem-
different kinds of memories (Lee, 2009). ory reconsolidation, the intervention includes minimal evo-
Reacquisition in extinction typically shows enhanced cation of the problem response along with multiple overlays
efficiency of relearning; that is, it takes fewer trials to rees- of new associations that, in essence, rewrite the memory. As
tablish the subject behavior. In memories that have been in previously reported reconsolidation studies, the traumatic
modified using reconsolidation-based procedures, there is no event either becomes inaccessible, significantly modified, or
net gain in relearning; when relearned, they are learned as if subject to nontraumatic declarative access (Alberini, 2005;
they were totally new behaviors (Cao et al., 2008; Duvarci & Hupbach et al., 2008; Labar, 2007; Tronel et al., 2005).
Nader, 2004; Kindt et al., 2009). Among the anecdotal reports are those provided by
Contextual renewal does not occur. This means that even Richard Bandler, Steve and Connierae Andreas, Robert
when the unconditioned fear stimulus is presented in a novel Dilts, and William McDowell who severally relate that each
context, the learned fear response does not reappear. This is of them has treated thousands of persons suffering from
not enhanced extinction (Duvarci & Nader, 2004). PTSD and phobic conditions with immediate, lasting results
Gray and Liotta 9

from this short-term intervention. In many cases, they report after the trauma, with each of the clients merging her own
complete symptom alleviation after long-term follow-up dissociated identities with her imagined selves on the screen
(Andreas & Andreas, 1989; Bandler, 1985; Dilts & Delozier, and sharing the learnings from her experience. Unspecified
2000; McDowell & McDowell, n.d.). measures of 28 dependent variables showed significant
The procedure was originated by Richard Bandler and changes in pre-post comparisons with near-total abatement
first appeared in his work, Using Your Brain for a Change of symptoms in one client.
(1985). An expanded version of the procedure appeared in Muss (1991, 2002) reports having used the technique first
Andreas’ description, in their work, Heart of the Mind with 19 police officers who met the DSM-III diagnostic cri-
(Andreas & Andreas, 1989). Dilts and Delozier (2000) pro- teria for PTSD and later with all types of traumatized per-
vided a slightly different version of the protocol in their The sons (Muss, 2002). In nearly all of the 19 police cases, he
Encyclopedia of Systemic Neuro-linguistic Programming. reports remission of symptoms. He provides no control con-
The technique has since been popularized in the United ditions and few details of the study; however, in long-term
Kingdom as the rewind technique (Guy & Guy, 2003; Muss, follow-ups (3 months to 3 years), for 15 of the 19 cases, he
1991, 2002). A fully manualized and standardized version of reports a complete absence of intrusive imagery. Crucially,
the protocol, renamed Reconsolidation of Traumatic Memory as noted by Andreas and Andreas, he indicates that the tech-
(RTM) is now the subject of pilot studies at the Brain nique is appropriate to clients whose primary symptoms are
Resource Center in New York (F. Bourke, personal commu- experienced as intense, suddenly arising experiences of the
nication, November 25, 2011). trauma symptoms usually experienced as flashbacks or panic
In early studies, the roots of the technique were traced to reactions (Andreas, 2008, Personal communication).
the work of Erica Fromm and her use of hypnosis for the A third study, by Hossack and Bental (1996), included five
control of pain and anxiety. In light, however, of Bandler and clients who were treated with a combination of guided visual-
Grinder’s extensive studies of the work of Milton Erickson izations, Jacobsen’s deep muscle relaxation, and two sessions
and their participation in his classes and with his students, it of the V/KD protocol. Although one of the five clients was
would seem more likely that the technique has its roots in unable to complete the visualizations associated with the
Erickson’s well-documented use of dissociative techniques V/KD procedure, the other four who completed such visual-
(Bandler, 1985; Bandler & Grinder, 1975b, 1979; Dietrich, izations all reported significant reduction of intrusive images
2000; Dietrich et al., 2000; Erickson, 1964/1980a, 1964/1980b; and were able to return to normal life activities.
Fromm, 1965; Koziey & McLeod, 1987). Guy and Guy report that the technique, renamed by Muss
Psychological investigations of the technique are limited (1991, 2002) as the rewind technique, was applied to 30 peo-
to three scientific studies, two reviews, and several mentions ple between 2000 and 2002. All were diagnosed with PTSD
in the literature. Figley reviews the technique and provides a or partial PTSD. Participants were interviewed 10 days post-
case study. Each of the referenced studies recommends the treatment. Guy and Guy report that 40% of the clients treated
technique as a valuable tool for treating PTSD and makes by them adjudged their improvement as extremely success-
suggestions for further research (Carbonell & Figley, 1999; ful, 53% adjudged their treatment as successful, and 7% as
Dietrich et al., 2000; Figley, 2002; Hossack & Bentall, 1996; acceptable. None rated the treatment either as poor or as a
Koziey & McLeod, 1987; Muss, 1991, 2002). There is also failure (Guy & Guy, 2003).
one non–peer-reviewed study that is reported online (Guy & The following description of the basic protocol depends
Guy, 2003). on extensive personal communications with Andeas, Dilts,
Koziey and McCleod, writing in 1987, reported their and Hallbom and continued reference to their descriptions of
experiences in treating two rape victims with a mixed tech- the protocol in several written sources (Andreas & Andreas,
nique employing Bandler’s three-place dissociation in com- 1989; Bandler, 1985; Dilts & Delozier, 2000). A detailed
bination with hypnotic trance. An initial pretreatment session protocol is available from the author.
was used to review the technique and to complete an assess-
ment package. In a second session, the authors used hypnotic
trance to provide a resource state to ensure that the traumatic The Visual-Kinesthetic Dissociation
memories would not become overwhelming. One week later, It is important to understand that Andreas emphasizes that this
in a second treatment session, the clients completed another technique is only effective when the client’s difficulties are
set of evaluations, were hypnotized, and then led through the essentially a phobic, instantaneous, conditioned response to
three-part dissociation. In particular, they were led through stimuli related to a traumatic event. In general, the symptoms
an imagined, dissociated review of the trauma in which they will focus on flashbacks and other immediate panic responses
watched themselves watching themselves viewing a movie to stimuli associated with the traumatic event. In terms of clas-
of the trauma. The movie began with a still image of the cli- sical PTSD symptomatology, this is associated with simple
ent in a safe time before the traumatic event, projected on an PTSD. Although it may be effective in complex cases, its effi-
imaginary screen. The experience ended with a safe place cacy will be limited to the intrusive and avoidant symptoms.
10 Traumatology 18(2)

The technique begins by establishing rapport and framing the previous step until the viewing of black and white, dis-
the intervention as a short visualization process. The process sociated movie can be completed comfortably.
is usually comfortable for clients, but it does sometimes Beginning with the safe, black and white representation
induce a very short period of moderate discomfort. This of him or herself that is on the screen at the end of the dis-
stage may include practice behaviors, including the running sociated rehearsal, the client is now instructed to imagine
of an innocuous, unrelated experience backwards in subjec- stepping into the movie and experiencing the entire sequence,
tive experience. fully associated, in color, in reverse, and at very high speed
Insofar as the technique may be pursued content free, the (in 2 seconds or less). This step may take several repetitions
clinician should decide whether to proceed with or without unless it has been practiced during the framing. If the client
content. After framing, the problem state is briefly accessed reports that the reversed rehearsal has gone well, he or she
by questioning and probing until the client responds physio- may be debriefed and a determination made whether further
logically. Access to the problem state is typically marked by iterations are necessary.
physiological and paralinguistic elements that reflect height- To determine whether the procedure has had the desired
ened arousal and fast onset of the physiological and paralin- effect, every effort is now made to evoke the problem state
guistic symptoms of fear or trauma. These may include using the same questions and probes that evoked the problem
changes in breathing, heart rate, skin tone and color, vocal state at the beginning of the procedure. Special attention
pitch, and speech rate. should be given to those questions that were associated with
As soon as the state is identified, the client’s attention a clear physiological reaction.
should be moved from the problem state and reoriented to Consistent with practices suggested by Foa and Kozak
the present context. (1986), each sensory system should be probed for possible
The actual three-part dissociation begins with asking the triggers for the problem behaviors. If there is no reaction, the
client to imagine that he or she is seated in a movie theater. intervention is presumed to have worked. When the practi-
On the screen is a still image of the client performing some tioner is satisfied that he or she cannot evoke the PTSD
neutral activity in a safe context, at a time before the trauma response, the basic intervention is complete.
occurred. The picture on the screen represents one level of Some practitioners suggest the addition of a third level of
dissociation, whereas watching the picture constitutes a sec- novel experience that includes a reliving of the traumatic
ond level of dissociation. Having established these initial event in which they are either protected from harm, only act-
dissociative experiences, the client is asked to dissociate ing as if they were a stunt double in a movie of the event, or
from the image of him or herself sitting in the theater by they missed the crucial trauma altogether. This level is espe-
imagining him or herself floating away from their body in cially recommended in the case where physical injury was
the theater to a projection booth behind a Plexiglas barrier. part of the traumatic event.
From this vantage point, they are instructed to watch them- Mechanism. Each of the authors noted above attempts to
selves in the theater, watching themselves on the screen. explain the mechanism of the intervention by various means,
At this point, an anchor or conditioned association is including changes in the perceptual structure of memory
made between a specific touch stimulus and the current dis- (McDowell & McDowell, n.d.), changes in the integration
sociated experience. This is intended to reinforce the experi- of memory mediated by modulation of arousal (Dietrich,
ence of dissociation from the image on the screen. This may 2000), and dissociation from any traumatic sequelae (Dietrich
also be used to evoke or enhance dissociation during later et al., 2000).
parts of the protocol. Having reviewed the literature on PTSD, extinction, and
After taking care to create a dissociated context and a CS reconsolidation, the authors believe that the mechanism of
that can evoke and reinforce that context, the client is the V/KD intervention is most parsimoniously explained in
instructed to observe a black and white picture of him or terms of memory restructuring through the mechanism of
herself on the screen of the movie theater, at a time before reconsolidation.
anything ever happened. As the client focuses on the imag- In accordance with previously outlined studies of the phe-
ined picture, he or she is directed to watch him or herself in nomenon, the technique begins with a short-term activation
the theater as he or she watches a black and white movie of of the traumatic experience. Insofar as the experience is
the triggering event or the root trauma. The client is to con- marked by significant changes in physiology, it meets Foa
tinue to watch the observer in the theater, seeing him or her- and Kozak’s suggestion that an appropriate intervention be
self going through it, all the way to a point past the end of it, rooted in a full activation of the event. Here, full activation
where he or she can see that he or she survived and is safe does not imply flooding, only sufficient activation of the
once again. The client is further instructed that upon reach- core memory to render it labile. Nevertheless, to prevent the
ing the end of the movie, he or she should stop the movie as shutdown of the cognitive capacities detailed by Diamond
a still, black and white image of a safe time after the trauma et al. (2007) the client is distracted from the trauma and his
has passed. After the signal to the clinician that all is well, consciousness returned to the present context, as quickly as
the protocol is to either proceed to the next step or to repeat possible. This not only prevents retraumatization but also
Gray and Liotta 11

limits the activation of the memory to a temporal window significant restructuring of the memory. The associated
that is appropriate to reconsolidation but too brief to support reversal of the experience leaves the client with a subjective
extinction (Akirav & Maroun, 2006; Alberini, 2005; Foa & memory of the problem, “undoing itself.” This is performed
Kozak, 1986; Hupbach et al., 2008; LeBar & Phelps, 1998; quickly. The speed takes advantage of the narrow window of
Riccio et al., 2006). memory lability and the heightened salience accorded to
This foreshortening of exposure may be crucial to the ini- fast-moving, multisensory stimuli. Bandler suggested that
tiation of memory labilization that allows for reconsolidative the reversal of memory sequence was a valuable tool for
memory modification. According to Pedreira et al. (2004), undoing decisions, preconceived notions, and other artifacts
neither reconsolidation nor extinction is possible without the of temporal experience (Bandler, 1985; Simons, Detenber,
termination of the CS—the fear-evoking stimulus context. Reiss, & Shults, 2000).
According to their research, a stimulus offset is required to As with many of the elements of this intervention, novelty
effect significant change. Both processes require the termi- may be a significant element in its efficacy. It has already
nation of the fear context before new learning can occur. been noted how the unexpected features of the intervention
Having awakened the traumatic memory, it now becomes may support the reconsolidation mechanism. The complex-
subject to reinforcement or modification depending on the ity of this part of the intervention may also support further
immediate stimulus context. Remembering that the memory modification of the experience through simple cognitive
is still active in the background, through its intentional reviv- overload. Given the limited capacity of short-term memory,
ification and its presence in the semantic context, the V/KD the simple act of learning and executing the reverse rehearsal
model now provides several experiences of dissociation: the may not leave sufficient capacity in short-term memory to
dissociative anchor, the dissociated safe representation on access the fear response (Miller, 1956; Pedreira et al., 2004).
the imagined movie screen, and the client’s floating out of After completing the multisensory, high-speed, reversed,
the body to view the viewer who is sitting in the theater imaginal exposure, the client is again debriefed and every
watching the screen. Insofar as the memory is dissociated effort is made to reaccess the trauma response. If the inter-
and is not actively reinforcing the fear response, its novel vention has been successful, the client may retain declarative
stimulus properties may preferentially support memory updat- access to the event, but without the strong negative affect
ing through reconsolidation rather than extinction (Pedreira that characterizes the symptoms of PTSD (Andreas &
et al., 2004). Andreas, 1989; Bandler, 1993; Dilts & Delozier, 2000; Kindt
At this point, the first of several layers of active interven- et al., 2009; Muss, 1991).
tion in the structure of the traumatic memory begins. The In those cases where an imagined restructuring of the
dissociated black and white movie provides a multileveled original event is performed, the mechanism may be as sim-
opportunity for reshaping the memory context. First, it is tri- ple as layering in another set of experiences that are incom-
ply dissociated (Dietrich, 2000; Hossack & Bentall, 1996; patible with the trauma. Moreover, the addition of modified
Koziey & McLeod, 1987; Muss, 2002). Second, insofar as it memories reflects one of the standard reappraisal methods of
is a voluntary reexperience of the trauma, the context is cognitive regulation strategies. Such interventions are known
restructured as voluntary rather than involuntary. This is to increase activity in the VMPFC, which exercises a modula-
what various authors have described as prescribing the tory influence on the amygdala. Insofar as the emotional impact
symptom. It is also a direct remedy for the loss of control of the traumatizing memory has already been significantly
described by Foa as a significant contributor to PTSD symp- modified, the new version of the traumatic event may serve
tomatology (Bandler & Grinder, 1979; Erickson & Rossi, to provide a coherent narrative for the now-nontraumatizing
1980; Foa & Meadows, 1997; Haley, 1973). In this context, memory (Diamond et al., 2007; Hartley & Phelps, 2009;
the novelty of symptom prescription may enhance the recon- Williams et al., 2006).
solidation response as noted by Pedreira and colleagues and
also by Lee. These authors indicate that unexpected stimulus
properties support reconsolidative updating of memory con- Case Study
tent (Lee, 2009; Pedreira et al., 2004). Third, because the A recent case study provided by William A. McDowell,
movie is viewed in black and white, its emotional impact is PhD, professor emeritus and chair of counseling, Marshall
further vitiated (Bandler, 1985; Bandler & MacDonald, University, Huntington, West Virginia (William McDowall,
1987; Kringelbach, 2005). In theory, all of these elements personal communication, July 9, 2010) involved a 30-year-
are incorporated into the structure of the original memory. old veteran of the Iraq war. The Veterans Administration
Once the client has successfully completed the dissoci- (VA) had diagnosed him with PTSD, and he reported to the
ated review, another layer of new meanings is added to the NLP therapist after 1.5 years of standard treatment from the
memory through the reverse, associated rewind of the mem- VA, including individual and group psychotherapy, with no
ory. At this point, attempts have been made to recall the abatement of symptoms. At the time that he came to the
affect associated with the traumatic event and, by now, it NLP-trained therapist, he was reporting the following symp-
should already be difficult. This rewind phase constitutes a toms: flashbacks, nightmares, high anxiety while driving
12 Traumatology 18(2)

(e.g., unable to let others drive him; getting panicked by The V/KD model is supported by 25 years or more of
large trucks moving near his vehicle, often forcing him to anecdotal reports covering thousands of clients. The interven-
pull off the road). He also reported a fear of crowds and tion does not retraumatize the client and can be completed in
enclosed spaces (restaurants, classrooms) where he had to be a short duration—about 45 minutes. Although the V/KD
near an exit with a clear view of the entire space. He also model was assessed only in 3 peer-reviewed evaluations in
reported some anger problems at home and an inability to the past 25 years, each of these 3 evaluations deemed it wor-
have the doors unlocked. Although the client felt his family thy of further investigation. One scholar-practitioner, Muss,
problems were minimal, his wife reported that the family has continued using the V/KD technique, and through his
was experiencing severe difficulties. efforts it is now a recognized treatment for PTSD in the
The client was seen for three 1-hour videotaped sessions, United Kingdom (Carbonell & Figley, 1999; Dietrich, 2000;
with a 3-day break between the sessions. Pre- and post-tests Koziey & McLeod, 1987; McDowell & McDowell, n.d.;
using the PTSD Checklist—Civilian Version (PCL; Muss, 1991, 2002).
Weathers, Litz, Herman, Huska, & Keane, 1993, 1994) were Until recently, the possible mechanism of action for this
completed before treatment, at the beginning of the second highly innovative treatment was difficult to specify and was
treatment session, after the third session, and 30 days post- often described in terms of brain lateralization, changes in
treatment. Pretreatment scores on the PCL were about 90%. perspective, and other nonoperationalizable constructs.
After the first treatment session, the scores were reduced to Here, an argument for a mechanism based on the emerging
30%. All symptoms disappeared after the third treatment. At evidence for memory reconsolidation has been presented
30 days posttreatment, the symptom scores were still zero that is congruent with the structural elements of the interven-
and no symptoms were reported by the client. tion and predicts similar results (Dietrich, 2000; Hossack &
After the second session, the client reported no more Bentall, 1996; Koziey & McLeod, 1987; McDowell &
nightmares and abatement of all symptoms by the end of McDowell, n.d.).
treatment. To behaviorally evaluate the client’s posttreat- As noted previously, reconsolidation protocols depend on
ment improvement, after the last session he was driven to a a brief reactivation of the traumatic memory followed, after
crowded, noisy McDonalds restaurant and ate with his back stimulus cessation, by an intervening amnestic or confound-
to the door. He reported having no anxiety with someone ing event. In the V/KD protocol, the memory is briefly acti-
else driving him and was able to eat without being concerned vated and several layers of dissociative experience and
for the exits. A video record of the excursion validates his confounding imaginal memories are introduced during the
visible physiological responses and demeanor. (presumed) labile period. For experiences subjected to
He and his wife were interviewed together at the last ses- amnestic reconsolidation, insofar as a sufficiently intense
sion and in a 1-month follow-up. At the 1-month follow-up, memory is introduced during the labile phase of reconsolida-
he reported being able to allow his wife to drive and experi- tion, the memory may be disrupted, erased, or modified.
enced no anxiety while driving near large trucks. He also After the V/KD process, the original memory becomes either
reported being anxiety free while in crowded places. inaccessible, innocuous, or is transformed into a similar but
For independent verification, the client’s family was nonthreatening memory. Human studies of the reconsolida-
asked to substantiate his recovery. His wife reported in a tion phenomenon have found that although the affective
3-month posttreatment clinical interview that her husband no dimensions of previously negative memories are gone, the
longer checked the locks in the house more than once, was events remain accessible on a declarative level. Similarly,
able to let her drive, and no longer demonstrated the high clients who have undergone treatment with the V/KD proto-
anxiety and anger at home that he had before treatment. In col retain declarative and episodic access to the stimulus
her words, “I feel like I have my husband like he was before event but without the traumatic affect (Andreas & Andreas,
he left for Iraq.” 1989; Bandler, 1985; Dilts & Delozier, 2000; Kindt et al.,
2009; Lee, 2009; Lee et al., 2006; Riccio et al., 2006).
This analysis leads to several falsifiable predictions and
Discussion diagnostic indicators of the underlying mechanism in PTSD
PTSD is a problem that is currently affecting as many as 13% treatments. Because the mechanism outlined here depends
to 17% of returning Iraqi veterans and 10% to 11% of those on the known process of reconsolidation, interventions for
returning from Afghanistan. Reports indicate that up to 40% PTSD may be behaviorally evaluated in terms of their results
of these returning veterans affected with PTSD remain to determine whether extinction or reconsolidation is oper-
untreated. Aside from cultural issues that discourage mental ative. Where extinction mechanisms have been invoked,
health treatment, the long-term commitments required by spontaneous recovery, contextual renewal, reinstatement,
standard treatment modalities and inconsistent results do lit- and rapid reacquisition will characterize the posttreatment
tle to change motivation to obtain treatment (Gradus, 2010; period and further treatment will be necessary to deal with
Hoge et al., 2004). the intrusive elements of the disorder (Bouton, 2004; Bouton
Gray and Liotta 13

& Moody, 2004; Dillon & Pizzagalli, 2007; Massad & of extinction (Bouton, 2004; Bouton & Moody, 2004; Dillon
Hulsey, 2006; Rescorla, 1988; Vervliet, 2008). Where recon- & Pizzagalli, 2007; Massad & Hulsey, 2006; Rescorla,
solidative mechanisms have been appropriately marshaled, 1988; Vervliet, 2008). To the contrary, in clients treated
the memories will be transformed, inaccessible, and, even if with the V/KD protocol the previously traumatizing and
accessible, to declarative and episodic recall, they will have intrusive memories will be transformed, often inaccessible,
been rendered nontraumatizing. They will not be subject to and, where accessible, to declarative and episodic recall,
spontaneous recovery, contextual renewal, reinstatement, they will have been rendered nontraumatizing. The clients
and rapid reacquisition (Cao et al., 2008; Duvarci & Nader, will not be subject to spontaneous recovery, contextual
2004; Forcato et al., 2007, 2009; Kindt et al., 2009; Lee renewal, reinstatement, and rapid reacquisition (Cao et al.,
et al., 2006). These results may also be used as diagnostics 2008; Duvarci & Nader, 2004; Forcato et al., 2007, 2009;
for the evaluation of a mechanism where technique and results Kindt et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2006).
are variable and may further lead to the refinement of These results should be measurable using repeated appli-
results based on the length and intensity of CS presentation cations of a standard PTSD symptom inventory such as the
and the timing of introduction of confounding or amnestic PCL-M (PTSD Checklist—Military). The authors predict
stimulus (Lee, 2009; Pedreira et al., 2004). Finally, these consistently reduced or continuously reducing scores for the
observations may lead to new interventions based on recon- intrusion and avoidance measures of the scales for clients
solidative mechanisms. treated with the V/KD protocol and variable or increasing
Future research into this technique should look toward scores for the extinction-based treatments (Weathers et al.,
large-scale trials of the protocol in the treatment of PTSD. 1993, 1994). Increasing scores would indicate the presence
The international troops returning from service in various of the various kinds of mechanisms that are associated with
theaters of war could provide a significant test population for extinction: spontaneous recovery, contextual renewal, rein-
this already established and relatively unknown treatment. statement, and rapid reacquisition. Continued reduction of
There remain hundreds of thousands of war victims, refu- symptoms and/or symptom intensity would indicate the
gees from earthquakes, and tsunami victims who would pro- action of reconsolidation.
vide a large pool of clients. To test this prediction a pilot study should be conducted
Follow-up studies and surveys to take advantage of the using a large sample of recent veterans of the Iraqi or
now-anecdotal evidence compiled by NLP practitioners Afghanistani wars who have been diagnosed with PTSD that
would also be instructive. Such follow-up studies could pro- is primarily characterized by intrusive and avoidant symp-
vide crucial long-term reports of the incidence of posttreat- toms. A standard diagnostic instrument with well-established
ment relapse that would be capable of falsifying the proposal validity may be used on all clients, in order to exclude those
that the technique is rooted in reconsolidation rather than with significant comorbidities and for whom the intrusive
extinction. and avoidant symptoms are not a primary issue. Exclusion
Further research might also investigate the construction criteria would eliminate veterans who suffer from severe
of even shorter interventions that depend on reconsolidation. cases of comorbidity (severe, long-standing alcohol and drug
For instance, just as the current technique relies in part on a addictions, preexisting personality disorders, psychoses, or
conditioned resource to amplify dissociation, it may be pos- any of these secondary to PTSD symptoms). It should be
sible to create an even more efficient restructuring of mem- remembered that the V/KD protocol is specifically targeted
ory or an event using a powerful, positive resource state as a at the reliving and phobic elements of PTSD and is not indic-
conditioned response whose introduction during the labile ative of those characterized by Brewin and colleagues (1996)
period would create a memory transformation or erasure. as secondary emotions and comorbidities such as alcohol-
The current explanation invites further exploration of the ism, drug addictions, and family dysfunctions.
mechanism of reconsolidation to other interventions that After prescreening, clients would be randomly assigned
have been, up to now, poorly understood. to treatment groups using either the V/KD or an extinction-
based protocol. In general, the evaluation should follow the
gold standards for evaluation research as reported by Foa
Directions for Future Research and Meadows (1997).
Thus far, the article has discussed a theoretical mechanism
for the V/KD protocol to be used in the treatment of PTSD Declaration of Conflicting Interests
and has made specific predictions with regard to its efficacy The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
and how the proposed mechanism, that is, reconsolidation, to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
may be differentiated from protocols that invoke extinction.
It was predicted that clients treated with the V/KD protocol Funding
would not be subject to spontaneous recovery, contextual The authors received no financial support for the research, author-
renewal, reinstatement, and rapid reacquisition—all hallmarks ship, and/or publication of this article.
14 Traumatology 18(2)

References Debiec, J., Doyre, V., Nader, K., & LeDoux, J. (2006). Directly
reactivated, but not indirectly reactivated, memories undergo
Akirav, I., & Maroun, M. (2006). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is reconsolidation in the amygdala. Proceedings of the National
obligatory for consolidation and reconsolidation of object rec- Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(9),
ognition memory. Cerebral Cortex, 16(12), 1759-1765. 3428-3433.
Alberini, C. M. (2005). Mechanisms of memory stabilization: Are Debiec, J., LeDoux, J., & Nader, K. (2002 ). Cellular and systems
consolidation and reconsolidation similar or distinct processes? reconsolidation in the hippocampus. Neuron, 36, 527-538.
Trends in Neurosciences, 28(1), 51-56. Diamond, D., Campbell, A., Park, C., Halonen, J., & Zoladz, P. (2007).
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statis- The temporal dynamics model of emotional memory processing:
tical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: A synthesis on the neurobiological basis of stress-induced
Author. amnesia, flashbulb and traumatic memories, and the Yerkes-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statis- Dodson Law. Neural Plasticity, 1-33. doi:10.1155/2007/60803
tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Dietrich, A. (2000). A review of visual/kinesthetic disassociation in
Author. the treatment of posttraumatic disorders: Theory, efficacy and
Andreas, C., & Andreas, S. (1989). Heart of the mind. Moab, UT: practice recommendations. Traumatology, 6(2), 85-107.
Real People Press. Dietrich, A., Baranowsky, A., Devich-Navarro, M., Gentry, J.,
Bandler, R. (1985). Using your brain for a change. Moab, UT: Real Harris, C., & Figley, C. (2000). A review of alternative
People Press. approaches to the treatment of post traumatic sequelae. Trauma-
Bandler, R., & Grinder, J. (1975a). Patterns in the hypnotic tech- tology, 6(4), 251-271.
niques of Milton H. Erickson, MD (Vol. 1). Cupertino, CA: Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2007). Inhibition of action,
Meta Publications. thought, and emotion: A selective neurobiological review.
Bandler, R., & Grinder, J. (1975b). The structure of magic (Vol. 1). Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 99-114.
Cupertino, CA: Science and Behavior Books. Dilts, R., & Delozier, J. (2000). The encyclopedia of systemic
Bandler, R., & Grinder, J. (1979). Frogs into princes. Moab, UT: neuro-linguistic programming and NLP new coding. Retrieved
Real People Press. from http://www.nlpu.com
Bandler, R., & MacDonald, W. (1987). An insider’s guide to sub- Dilts, R., Grinder, J., Bandler, R., & DeLozier, J. (1980). Neuro-
modalities. Moab, UT: Real People Press. linguistic programming. Vol. 1: The structure of subjective
Bisson, J., Ehlers A., Matthews, R., Pilling, S., Richards, D., & experience. Cupertino, CA: Meta Publications.
Turner, S. (2007). Psychological treatments for chronic post- Duvarci, S., & Nader, K. (2004). Characterization of fear memory
traumatic stress disorder: Systematic review and meta-analysis. reconsolidation. Neuroscience, 24(42), 9269-9275.
The British Journal of Psychiatry, 190(2), 97-104. Eichenbaum, H. (2006). The secret life of memories. Neuron,
Bodkin, J., Detke, M. J., Pope, H., & Hudson, J. (2007). Is PTSD caused 50(3), 350-352.
by traumatic stress? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 211, 76-18. Erickson, M. H. (1980a). The confusion technique in hypnosis. In
Bolstad, R. (2002). Resolve: A new model of therapy. Williston, E. L. Rossi (Ed.), The collected papers of Milton H. Erickson
VT: Crown House Publishing. on hypnosis. Vol. 1: The nature of hypnosis and suggestion
Bostic St. Clair, C., & Grinder, J. (2002). Whispering in the wind. (pp. 258-291). New York, NY: Irvington. (Original work pub-
Scotts Valley, CA: J & C Enterprises. lished 1964)
Bouton, M. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Erickson, M. H. (1980b). An hypnotic technique for resistant
Learning and Memory, 11(5), 485-494. patients: The patient, the technique, and its rationale and field
Bouton, M., & Moody, E. (2004). Memory processes in classical experiments. In E. L. Rossi (Ed.), The collected papers of
conditioning. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(7), Milton H. Erickson on hypnosis. Vol. 1: The nature of hypno-
663-674. sis and suggestion (pp. 299-330). New York, NY: Irvington.
Brewin, C. R., Dalgleish, T., & Joseph, S. (1996). A dual repre- (Original work published 1964)
sentation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Erickson, M. H., & Rossi, E. L. (1980). Indirect forms of sugges-
Review, 103(4), 670-686. tion. In E. L. Rossi (Ed.), The collected papers of Milton H.
Cao, X., Wang, H., Mei, B., An, S., Yin, L., Wang, L. P., & Tsien, J. Z. Erickson on hypnosis. Vol. 1: The nature of hypnosis and sug-
(2008). Inducible and selective erasure of memories in the mouse gestion (pp. 478-490). New York, NY: Irvington.
brain via chemical-genetic manipulation. Neuron, 60, 353-366. Figley, C. (Ed.). (2002). Brief treatments for the traumatized.
Carbonell, J., & Figley, C. (1999). Promising PTSD treatment Westport, CT: Greenwood.
approaches: A systematic clinical demonstration of promising Foa, E., Keane, T., & Friedman, M. (2000). Effective treatments for
PTSD treatment approaches. Traumatology, 5(1), 32-48. PTSD. New York, NY: Guilford.
Davidson, J., & Foa, E. (1991). Diagnostic issues in posttraumatic Foa, E., & Kozak, M. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Expo-
stress disorder: Considerations for the DSM-IV. Journal of sure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1),
Abnormal Psychology, 100(3), 346-355. 20-35.
Gray and Liotta 15

Foa, E., & Meadows, E. (1997). Psychosocial treatments for post- Labar, K. (2007). Beyond fear: Emotional memory mechanisms in
traumatic stress disorder: A critical review. Annual Review of the human brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
Psychology, 48, 449-480. 16(4), 173-177.
Foa, E., & Tolin, D. (2000). Comparison of the PTSD Symptom Lamprecht, F., & Sack, M. (2002). Posttraumatic stress disorder
Scale—Interview Version and the Clinician-Administered PTSD revisited. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 222-237.
Scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, 181-191. Layton, B., & Krikorian, R. (2002). Memory mechanisms in post-
Forcato, C., Burgos, V., Argibay, P., Molina, V., Pedreira, M., & traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clin-
Maldonado, H. (2007). Reconsolidation of declarative memory ical Neurosciences, 14(3), 254-261.
in humans. Learning & Memory, 14(4), 295-303. LeBar, K., & Phelps, E. (1998). Arousal-mediated memory consoli-
Forcato, C., Pedreira, M., & Maldonado, H. (2009). Human recon- dation: The role of the medial temporal lobe in humans. Psy-
solidation does not always occur when a memory is retrieved: chological Science, 9(6), 490-493.
The relevance of the reminder structure. Neurobiology of Lee, J. (2009). Reconsolidation: Maintaining memory relevance.
Learning and Memory, 91(1), 50-57. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(8), 413-420.
Fromm, E. (1965). Hypnoanalysis: Theory and two case excerpts. Lee, J., Milton, A., & Everitt, B. (2006). Reconsolidation and
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 2(3), 127-133. extinction of conditioned fear: Inhibition and potentiation. Neu-
Gharakhani, A., Mathew, S., & Charney, D. (2006). Neurobiology roscience, 26(39), 10051-10056.
of anxiety disorders and implications for treatment. The Mount Liberzon, I., & Sripada, C. (2007). The functional neuroanatomy
Sinai Journal of Medicine, 73(7), 941-949. of PTSD: A critical review. Progress in Brain Research, 167,
Gradus, J. (2010). Epidemiology of PTSD. Washington, DC: 151-169.
National Center for PTSD. Retrieved from http://www.ptsd Loftus, E., & Yuille, J. (1984). Departures from reality in human
.va.gov/professional/pages/epidemiological-facts-ptsd.asp perception and memory. In H. Weingartner & E. S. Parker
Guy, K., & Guy, N. (2003). The fast cure for phobia and trauma: (Eds.), Memory consolidation: Psychobiology of cognition
Evidence that it works. Retrieved from http://www.hgi.org.uk/ (pp. 163-184). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
archive/rewindevidence.htm Massad, P., & Hulsey, T. (2006). Exposure therapy renewed. Jour-
Haley, J. (1973). Uncommon therapy. New York, NY: Norton. nal of Psychotherapy Integration, 16(4), 417-428.
Hartley, C., & Phelps, E. (2009). Changing fear: The neurocircuitry of McDowell, W., & McDowell, J. (n.d.). Neuro-linguistic programming
emotion regulation. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 136-146. applied: The use of visual-kinesthetic dissociation to cure anxiety
Hoge, C., Castro, C., Messer, S., McGurk, D., Cotting, D., & disorders. Brief Treatments for the Traumatized. Retrieved from
Koffman, R. (2004). Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, http://mailer.fsu.edu/~cfigley/Book/BTT/VKDx.htm
mental health problems, and barriers to care. New England Jour- McHugh, P., & Treisman, G. (2007). PTSD: A problematic diag-
nal of Medicine, 351(1), 13-22. nostic category. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 211-222.
Hossack, A., & Bentall, R. (1996). Elimination of posttraumatic McNally, R. (2007). Mechanisms of exposure therapy: How neuro-
symptomatology by relaxation and visual-kinesthetic dissocia- science can improve psychological treatments for anxiety dis-
tion. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9(1), 99-110. orders. Clinical Psychology Review, 27(6), 750-759.
Hupbach, A., Hardt, O., Gomez, R., & Nadel, L. (2008). The Milekic, M., & Alberini, C. (2002). Temporally graded requirement
dynamics of memory: Context-dependent updating. Learning for protein synthesis following memory reactivation. Neuron,
& Memory, 15(8), 574-579. 36, 521-525.
Jacek, D., Ecedil, B., & LeDoux, J. (2006). Noradrenergic signaling Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two.
in the amygdala contributes to the reconsolidation of fear mem- The Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.
ory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 521-524. Muss, D. (1991). A new technique for treating post-traumatic stress
Kaang, B., Lee, S., & Kim, H. (2009). Synaptic protein degradation disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30(1), 91-92.
as a mechanism in memory reorganization. The Neuroscientist, Muss, D. (2002). The rewind technique in the treatment of post-
15(5), 430-435. doi:1073858408331374 traumatic stress disorder: Methods and application. In C. R. Figley
Keane, T., Weathers, F., & Foa, E. (2000). Diagnosis and assess- (Ed.), Brief treatments for the traumatized (pp. 306-314). Westport,
ment. In E. B. Foa, T. M. Keane, & M. Friedman (Eds.), Effec- CT: Greenwood.
tive treatment for PTSD (pp.18-36). New York, NY: Guilford. Nader, K., Schafe, G., & LeDoux, J. (2000). The labile nature of con-
Kindt, M., Soeter, M., & Vervliet, B. (2009). Beyond extinction: solidation theory. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 1(3), 216-219.
Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Pedreira, M., Perez-Cuesta, L., & Maldonado, H. (2004). Mismatch
Nature Neuroscience, 12(3), 256-258. between what is expected and what actually occurs triggers memory
Koziey, P., & McLeod, G. (1987). Visual-kinesthetic dissocia- reconsolidation or extinction. Learning & Memory, 11(5), 579-585.
tion in treatment of victims of rape. Professional Psychology: Pitman, R. K., Orr, S. P., Altman, B., Longpre, R. E., Poiré, R. E.,
Research and Practice, 18(3), 276-282. Macklin, M. L., et al. (1996). Emotional processing and out-
Kringelbach, M. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking come of imaginal flooding therapy in vietnam veterans with
reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry,
6(9), 691-702. 37(6), 409-418.
16 Traumatology 18(2)

Quirk, G., & Mueller, D. (2007). Neural mechanisms of extinction Ursano, R., Bell, C., Eth, S., Friedman, M., Norwood, A., Pfef-
learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(1), 56-72. ferbaum, B., Pynoos, R. S., Zatzick, D. F., & Benedek, D. M.
Rescorla, R. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you (2004). Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with
think it is. American Psychologist, 43(3), 151-160. acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder.
Riccio, D., Millin, P., & Bogart, A. (2006). Reconsolidation: A Washington, DC: APA Practice Guidelines.
brief history, a retrieval view, and some recent issues. Learning van der Kolk, B. (2006). Clinical implications of neuroscience
& Memory, 13(5), 536-544. research in PTSD. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-
Rothbaum, B., Davis, M., King, J., Ferris, C., & Lederhendler, I. ences, 1071, 277-293.
(2003). Applying learning principles to the treatment of post- Vervliet, B. (2008). Learning and memory in conditioned fear
trauma reactions. In J. A. King, C. F. Ferris, & I. I. Lederhendler extinction: Effects of d-cycloserine. Acta Psychologica, 127(3),
(Eds.), The roots of mental illness in children (pp. 112-121). 601-613.
New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences. Wake, L. (2008). Neurolinguistic psychotherapy: A postmodern
Schiller, D., Monfils, M., Raio, C., Johnson, D., LeDoux, J., & perspective. London: Routledge.
Phelps, E. (2010). Preventing the return of fear in humans using Weathers, F., Litz, B., Herman, D., Huska, J., & Keane, T. (1993,
reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature, 463(7277), 49-53. October). The PTSD checklist (PCL): Reliability, validity, and
Shalev, A., Bonne, O., & Eth, S. (1996). Treatment of posttrau- diagnostic utility. The Annual Meeting of International Society
matic stress disorder: A review. Psychosomatic Medicine, for Traumatic Stress Studies. San Antonio, TX. Retrieved from
58(2), 165-182. http://www.pdhealth.mil/library/downloads/PCL_sychometrics .doc
Simons, R., Detenber, B., Reiss, J., & Shults, C. (2000). Image Weathers, F., Litz, B., Herman, D., Huska, J., & Keane, T. (1994).
motion and context: A between- and within-subjects compari- PCL-M for DSM-IV. Washington, DC: National Center for
son. Psychophysiology, 37(5), 706-710. PTSD. Retrieved from http://www.hospicefed.org/hospice_
Spitzer, R., Wakefield, J., & First, M. (2007). Saving PTSD from pages/PCL-M.htm
itself in DSM-V. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 233-241. Wessa, M., & Flor, H. (2007). Failure of extinction of fear responses
Tronel, S., Milekic, M., & Alberini, C. ( 2005). Linking new infor- in posttraumatic stress disorder: Evidence from second-order con-
mation to a reactivated memory requires consolidation and ditioning. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(11), 1684-1692.
not reconsolidation mechanisms. PLoS Biology, 3(9), 293. Williams, L., Kemp, A., Felmingham, K., Barton, M., Olivieri, G.,
Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/ Peduto, A., Kemp, A. H., Felmingham, K., Barton, M., Olivieri,
PMC1188238/?tool=pubmed G., Peduto, A., Gordon, E., & Bryant, R. A. (2006). Trauma mod-
Tronson, N., & Taylor, J. (2007). Molecular mechanisms of memory ulates amygdala and medial prefrontal responses to consciously
reconsolidation. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 8(4), 262-275. attended fear. NeuroImage, 29(2), 347-357.

View publication stats

You might also like