Reappraisal As A Mediator in The Link Between 5-HTTLPR and Social Anxiety Symptoms, Emotion 2013

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Emotion © 2013 American Psychological Association

2013, Vol. 13, No. 6, 1012–1022 1528-3542/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0033383

Reappraisal as a Mediator in the Link Between 5-HTTLPR and Social


Anxiety Symptoms

Andrei C. Miu Romana Vulturar, Adina Chiş, and


Babeş-Bolyai University Loredana Ungureanu
Iuliu Hat᝺ieganu University of Medicine and Pharmacy

James J. Gross
Stanford University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Social anxiety symptoms have been related to (a) polymorphisms in the serotonin-transporter gene-
promoter region (also, serotonin-transporter-linked polymorphic region; 5-HTTLPR) and (b) reduced use
of adaptive forms of emotion regulation such as reappraisal. It is not known, however, whether
reappraisal functions as a mediator in the link between 5-HTTLPR and social anxiety. To address this
issue, 182 unselected community volunteers were tested for 5-HTTLPR status, and self-report measures
of social anxiety symptoms and reappraisal use were obtained. Relative to other participants, those with
two low-expressing alleles displayed increased social anxiety and decreased reappraisal. As predicted,
the influence of 5-HTTLPR on social anxiety symptoms was transmitted via reappraisal, and this effect
of 5-HTTLPR was observed using two different measures of reappraisal. These findings suggest that
cognitive reappraisal may be an intermediate phenotype of the social anxiety spectrum, and that
individuals with low-expressing 5-HTTLPR genotypes may benefit the most from cognitive– behavioral
psychotherapy because they do not appear to engage as frequently as others in reappraisal.

Keywords: 5-HTTLPR, emotion regulation, social anxiety, cognitive reappraisal, serotonin

Social anxiety is defined as a spectrum that ranges from ordi- Serotonin Transporter Polymorphisms and Social
nary shyness and mild social anxiety symptoms at one end of the Anxiety Symptoms
continuum to generalized social anxiety disorder (SAD) at the
other end (McNeil, 2001). Social anxiety symptoms are known to In light of the therapeutic efficacy of selective serotonin
emerge early in life (Stein & Stein, 2008), and often remain stable reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in SAD (van der Linden, Stein, &
into adulthood (Wittchen, Stein, & Kessler, 1999), increasing the van Balkom, 2000), and the impact of psychopharmacological
risk for depression (Kessler, Stang, Wittchen, Stein, & Walters, manipulations of serotonin availability (e.g., tryptophan deple-
1999; Stein et al., 2001), suicide attempts (Sareen et al., 2005), tion) on social anxiety symptoms (Argyropoulos et al., 2004),
alcohol abuse (Grant et al., 2004; Schneier, Martin, Liebowitz, many genetic association studies have focused on the serotonin-
Gorman, & Fyer, 1989), incomplete educational attainment (Kes- transporter gene (5-HTT). Several functional polymorphisms
sler, Foster, Saunders, & Stang, 1995), and severe social restric- have been described in the promoter region of this gene (5-
tions (Pine, Cohen, Gurley, Brook, & Ma, 1998). HTTLPR), including an insertion/deletion (indel) polymor-
phism (Lesch et al., 1996) and a single-nucleotide polymor-
phism (SNP rs25531; Hu et al., 2006; Wendland, Martin, Kruse,
Lesch, & Murphy, 2006).
This article was published Online First June 24, 2013. The 5-HTTLPR indel polymorphism consists of 14 (the S al-
Andrei C. Miu, Department of Psychology, Babeş-Bolyai University,
lele), 16 (the L allele) or more copies (the XL alleles) of a 20 –23,
Cluj-Napoca, Romania; Romana Vulturar and Adina Chiş, Department of
Cell and Molecular Biology, Iuliu Hat᝺eganu University of Medicine and base-pair, imperfect repeat sequence (Ehli, Hu, Lengyel-Nelson,
Pharmacy, Cluj-Napoca, Romania; Loredana Ungureanu, Department of Hudziak, & Davies, 2012; Lesch et al., 1996). The SNP rs25531
Dermatology, Iuliu Hat᝺eganu University of Medicine and Pharmacy; James involves the substitution of an adenine (A) to a guanine (G) in the
J. Gross, Department of Psychology, Stanford University. 6th nucleotide within the first of two extra 20 –23 base-pair repeats
This work was supported by a grant from the Romanian National in the L allele of the indel polymorphism (Hu et al., 2006; Kraft,
Authority for Scientific Research, Executive Agency for Higher Education, Slager, McGrath, & Hamilton, 2005; Nakamura, Ueno, Sano, &
Research, Development and Innovation Funding (CNCS–UEFISCDI), Tanabe, 2000; Wendland et al., 2006). Therefore, the two resulting
project number PNII-ID-PCCE-2011–2-0045. The authors thank Ihno Lee
L alleles, LA and LG, together with the S allele, comprise a
for statistical advice.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Andrei C. triallelic locus (Hu et al., 2006; Nakamura et al., 2000). The A¡G
Miu, Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychology, substitution creates a binding site for the transcription factor AP2
Babeş-Bolyai University, 37 Republicii, 400015 Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Ro- (from Activating Protein 2), which suppresses the 5-HTT gene
mania. E-mail: andreimiu@psychology.ro expression in the presence of the LG and S alleles in dorsal raphe
1012
5-HTTLPR, REAPPRAISAL, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 1013

neurons (Hu et al., 2006; Mortensen, Thomassen, Larsen, Whitte- lovey, Stroud, Woolery, & Epel, 2002; Turk, Heimberg,
more, & Wiborg, 1999). Consequently, the LG and S alleles are Luterek, Mennin, & Fresco, 2005), in contrast to general symp-
usually grouped together (i.e., carriers of one [S’L’] or two low- toms, such as increased negative affect and reduced positive
expressing alleles [S’S’]) because they reduce the 5-HTT expres- affect, which are shared by most anxiety disorders (Bradley et
sion with equal magnitude, and then compared with the LA allele al., 2011; Kashdan, 2007; Watson, Clark, & Carey, 1988).
(i.e., LA homozygotes [L’L’]). Recent studies have shown that the Therefore, disease-specific interventions have started to target
triallelic approach to 5-HTTLPR (i.e., genotyping both polymor- emotion regulation as a key mechanism of change in SAD
phisms, not only the indel) is essential for correctly categorizing (Kley, Heinrichs, Bender, & Tuschen-Caffier, 2012; Mosco-
genotypes and identifying more reliable associations with pheno- vitch et al., 2012).
types such as obsessive– compulsive disorder (Hu et al., 2006), Recent studies have identified atypical patterns of emotion
therapeutic response to SSRI (Kraft et al., 2005), anterior cingulate regulation in SAD. More specifically, these studies have identified
volume (Selvaraj et al., 2011), and trait-behavioral inhibition a link between difficulties with reappraisal and social anxiety
(Whisman, Richardson, & Smolen, 2011). symptoms (Goldin, Manber-Ball, Werner, Heimberg, & Gross,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The S allele has been consistently associated with anxiety- 2009; Goldin, Manber, Hakimi, Canli, & Gross, 2009). Both
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

related personality traits (Sen, Burmeister, & Ghosh, 2004), in- children and adults with SAD use reappraisal less frequently in
creased attention to negative emotional stimuli (Pergamin-Hight, daily life; they are less able to generate reappraisals of potentially
Bakermans-Kranenburg, van Ijzendoorn, & Bar-Haim, 2012), in- threatening situations; and their self-efficacy concerning reap-
creased amygdala reactivity to stress (Drabant et al., 2012; Mu- praisal is reduced, compared with healthy controls (Carthy,
nafò, Brown, & Hariri, 2008), and emotional disorders such as Horesh, Apter, Edge, & Gross, 2010; Goldin, Manber-Ball, et al.,
obsessive– compulsive disorder (Lin, 2007) and depression (Caspi, 2009; Werner, Goldin, Ball, Heimberg, & Gross, 2011). In addi-
Hariri, Holmes, Uher, & Moffitt, 2010; Uher & McGuffin, 2010). tion, the lesser down-regulation of negative affect through reap-
Recent genetic association studies have also explored the influence praisal in SAD is related to the severity of social anxiety symp-
of 5-HTTLPR on social anxiety. The low-expressing alleles of toms, which suggests that the intensity of SAD contributes to
5-HTTLPR polymorphisms (e.g., S or LG) have been significantly emotion dysregulation (Carthy et al., 2010; Goldin, Manber-Ball,
associated with symptoms such as state-anxiety escalation and et al., 2009). Two functional neuroimaging studies found that SAD
amygdala hyperactivity during public speaking (Furmark et al., patients displayed decreased early recruitment of the brain systems
2004), increased shyness and reduced discrimination of angry and (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) implicated in reappraisal of
neutral faces (Battaglia et al., 2005), and blushing propensity harsh faces or negative self-beliefs (Goldin, Manber-Ball, et al.,
(Domschke et al., 2009) in SAD patients. In addition, these low- 2009; Goldin, Manber, et al., 2009). These studies support the
expressing alleles also predicted reduced responses to SSRI (Stein, view that SAD patients may need more time or more external cues
Seedat, & Gelernter, 2006) and placebo treatments that reduce to access and implement reappraisal during social threat. Reap-
amygdala hyperactivity during public speaking in SAD (Furmark praisal thus appears to be an important predictor of social anxiety
et al., 2008). These converging results seem to support the influ- symptoms.
ence of 5-HTTLPR on key dimensions of SAD, although another
recent study found no association between 5-HTT promoter poly- 5-HTTLPR, Emotion Regulation, and Social Anxiety
morphisms and the SAD diagnosis (Strug et al., 2010). This
Symptoms
underscores the possibility that intermediate phenotypes, rather
than the global diagnosis of SAD, are associated with 5-HTT One crucial question that has not yet been empirically addressed
promoter polymorphisms. is whether emotion regulation is a mediator in the link between the
5-HTTLPR polymorphisms and social anxiety symptoms. This
idea is consistent with the widely discussed hypothesis that emo-
Emotion Regulation and Social Anxiety Symptoms
tion regulation may be a candidate intermediate phenotype of
Psychological theories have drawn attention to the important emotional disorders (Canli, Ferri, & Duman, 2009; Canli & Lesch,
role that emotion regulation plays in the onset and maintenance 2007; Hariri & Holmes, 2006; Meyer-Lindenberg & Weinberger,
of social anxiety symptoms (Clark & Wells, 1995; Hofmann, 2006). The main support for this idea came from a functional
2007). Individuals with social anxiety engage in maladaptive neuroimaging study that reported reduced functioning of an
regulatory strategies, such as avoidance and safety behaviors amygdala-cingulate neural circuit, which is critical for the extinc-
and rumination after the social situation has passed, which leads tion of negative emotions, in carriers of the S allele of 5-HTTLPR
to maintenance and further exacerbation of social anxiety symp- (Pezawas et al., 2005).
toms (Hofmann, 2007). In addition to making greater use of Although these findings were replicated and extended by sub-
maladaptive regulation strategies, individuals with social anx- sequent neuroimaging studies in which emotion regulation was
iety make lesser use of adaptive forms of emotion regulation, manipulated (Gillihan et al., 2010; Lemogne et al., 2011; but see
such as reappraisal, an emotion-regulation strategy that in- Firk, Siep, & Markus, 2013; Heinz et al., 2005; Schardt et al.,
volves changing the meaning of a situation to change the 2010), there are surprisingly few direct behavioral findings of an
emotional response to that situation (Giuliani & Gross, 2009; association between 5-HTTLPR and individual differences in
Sheppes & Gross, 2011; Szasz, Szentagotai, & Hofmann, emotion regulation. Carriers of the low-functioning alleles of
2011). Several studies have recently suggested that emotion- 5-HTTLPR have been shown to display reduced emotional resil-
regulation difficulties are more specific to SAD than general ience (Stein, Campbell-Sills, & Gelernter, 2009) and self-efficacy
anxiety disorder (Mennin, McLaughlin, & Flanagan, 2009; Sa- related to coping with negative emotions (Szily, Bowen, Unoka,
1014 MIU, VULTURAR, CHIŞ, UNGUREANU, AND GROSS

Simon, & Keri, 2008). Three other studies reported higher levels of (severe/usually). The internal consistency of LSAS-SR in our
rumination (Canli et al., 2006; Clasen, Wells, Knopik, McGeary, sample was excellent (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .9). LSAS-SR shows good
& Beevers, 2011) and lower levels of reappraisal (Schardt et al., sensitivity and specificity to clinical American Psychiatric Asso-
2010) in S carriers, but a systematic investigation of the associa- ciation’s DSM–IV criteria for SAD; a cut-off score of 30 on
tion between 5-HTT promoter polymorphisms and specific LSAS-SR correctly identifies over 93% of SAD patients (Mennin
emotion-regulation strategies is still lacking. In particular, an in- et al., 2002), but studies on student samples emphasize that scores
vestigation of the links among 5-HTTLPR, reappraisal, and emo- above 55 more reliably indicate moderate and severe social anxiety
tional symptoms is clearly needed. (Russell & Shaw, 2009). Although these clinical cut-offs were
informative regarding possible SAD diagnoses in our participants,
The Present Study a clinical LSAS-SR score was not an inclusion criterion.
One of the measures of individual differences in emotion reg-
The goal of this study was to investigate whether the effect of ulation was the cognitive reappraisal score from the Emotion
5-HTTLPR polymorphisms on social anxiety symptoms is trans- Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003). Using a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mitted via reappraisal. The sample included a randomly selected scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), the partic-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

set of community-dwelling participants who were screened for ipants rated their habitual use of reappraisal (e.g., when I want to
social anxiety symptoms using a clinical scale. This allowed us to feel less negative emotion, I change what I’m thinking about) and
generalize our findings to a broad segment of the social anxiety suppression (e.g., I keep my emotions to myself). The internal
spectrum. We predicted that carriers of the low-expressing alleles consistency of ERQ reappraisal in our sample was very good
(i.e., S’S’ and S’L’) of the 5-HTTLPR indel and rs25531 poly- (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .84). Because individual differences in cognitive
morphisms would be associated with increased social anxiety reappraisal were the focus of this study, we used a second, con-
symptoms and reduced use of cognitive reappraisal. Moreover, we vergent measure of reappraisal from the Cognitive Emotion Reg-
hypothesized that cognitive reappraisal would function as a medi- ulation Questionnaire (CERQ; Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spinhoven,
ator in the link between triallelic 5-HTTLPR genotypes and social 2001). In this questionnaire, the participants rated on a scale from
anxiety symptoms. 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always) the frequency of using
reappraisal when confronted with negative events (e.g., I look for
Method the positive sides to the matter). The internal consistency of CERQ
reappraisal in our sample was good (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .72). The
significant correlation between ERQ reappraisal and CERQ reap-
Participants
praisal scores (Pearson r ⫽ .33, p ⫽ .005) in the present sample
Participants for this study were 182 (139 women) volunteers. confirmed that we could use these scales as parallel measures of
They were all students at Babeş-Bolyai University in Cluj-Napoca, the frequency with which cognitive reappraisal is used. ERQ
Romania. An a priori sample size analysis had indicated that a expressive suppression scores (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .76) were used in
minimum of 164 participants were needed (small effect size; ␣ ⫽ additional analyses designed to explore the specificity of cognitive
.05; power ⱖ 0.8). Prior to study participation, written informed reappraisal as a mediator.
consent was obtained from all the volunteers. They were all
Caucasians of Romanian descent, and came from the same well- Genotyping
circumscribed geographical area. Age ranged from 16 to 42 (M ⫽
21.3 years). None of the participants reported cardiovascular or DNA was extracted from leukocytes (ethylenediaminetet-
neurological conditions, and they were not on medication (e.g., raacetic acid; EDTA-anticoagulated blood) using the Genomic
anxiolytics, beta-adrenergic antagonists, psychotropics). Since our DNA Extraction Kit (Fermentas, Vilnius, Lithuania) and kept at
sample was not balanced for sex, all the statistical analyses in- ⫺20 °C. Both 5-HTTLPR indel and rs25531 genotyping were
cluded sex as covariate in order to control for the effect of this performed using published protocols (Miu et al., 2012; Vultu-
potential confound. All the participants were compensated for their rar, Chis, Ungureanu, & Miu, 2012). Briefly, the polymerase
time. The study followed the recommendations of the American chain reaction (PCR) assay conditions were optimized as fol-
Medical Association’s Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical lows: Each reaction was carried out in a 25-␮l volume: 50 ng of
Association, 1964/2008), and it was approved by the Babeş-Bolyai genomic template, 12.5-␮l PCR mastermix (2x); the forward
University Research Council. No experimental manipulations were primer (5=-GGCGTTGCCGCTCTGAATGC-3=) and reverse
made and all the measures that were administered are described in primer (5=-GAGGGACTGAGCTGGACAACCAC-3=) from
this section. Generi-Biotech (Hradec Kralove, Czech Republic) were used to
amplify a region encompassing 5-HTTLPR. These primers
yield amplicons of 529 (for the L allele) or 486 base pairs (for
Assessment
the S allele). Thermal cycling consisted of 3 min of initial
Social anxiety symptoms were assessed by the total score of the denaturation at 94 °C followed by 31 cycles of 94 °C (40 s), 57
self-report version of the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS- °C (40 s) and 72 °C (40 s), each with a final extension step of
SR; Fresco et al., 2001; Liebowitz, 1987), which quantifies fear 4 min at 72 °C. The LG and LA alleles of rs25531 were
and avoidance in social and performance situations. The partici- subsequently studied by enzymatic digestion of 10 ␮l of PCR
pants had to rate the degree to which she or he feared or avoided products that were digested by HpaII. This restriction enzyme
social situations (e.g., speaking in front of an audience, talking to recognizes and cuts a 5=-C/CGG-3= sequence resulting in the
someone in authority), using a scale from 0 (none/never) to 4 following fragments: 340 base pairs, 127 base pairs and 62 base
5-HTTLPR, REAPPRAISAL, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 1015

pairs for the LA allele; 174, 166, 127 and 62 base pairs for the Results
LG allele; 297 base pairs, 127 base pairs and 62 base pair for the
SA allele; and 166, 131, 127 and 62 base pairs for the SG allele. 5-HTTLPR and Social Anxiety Symptoms
Finally, 10 ␮l of remaining PCR product and 15 ␮l of
restriction-enzyme assay solution were loaded onto a 2.5% Table 1 presents the scores of participants from each genotype
agarose gel and visualized by ethidium bromide for size esti- group on anxiety symptoms (LSAS-SR). Participants with
mation. The 5-HTTLPR allele frequencies were 0.44 for th SA LSAS-SR scores over 55 represented 49% (N ⫽ 89), categorized
allele, 0.48 for the LA allele and 0.07 for the LG allele, similar using Russell and Shaw’s (2009) cut-off scores as follows: Mod-
to the ones that were reported for oCaucasians by Hu et al. erate social anxiety (Scores 55– 64: 20%); marked social anxiety
(2006). One of the participants carried an XL (⬎ 16 repeats) (Scores 65–79: 14%); severe social anxiety (Scores 80 –95: 10%);
allele (Ehli et al., 2012). The genotypes were categorized into and very severe social anxiety (Scores 96⫹: 4%). An ANCOVA,
S’S’ (i.e., carriers of two low-expressing alleles, SS, LGLG, with 5-HTTLPR genotype group as between-subjects factor and
SLG: N ⫽ 50); S’L’ (i.e., carriers of one low-expressing allele, sex as covariate, indicated a significant effect of 5-HTTLPR on
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

SLA, LGLA: N ⫽ 81); and L’L’ (i.e., carriers of two high- LSAS-SR scores, F(2, 179) ⫽ 20.32, p ⬍ .01, ␩P2 ⫽ 0.281. The
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

expressing alleles, LALA, LAXL: N ⫽ 51). These genotypes S’S’ group showed significantly higher LSAS-SR scores than the
S’L’ and L’L’ groups.
were in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (␹2 ⫽ 2.2, n.s.; Rodri-
guez, Gaunt, & Day, 2009). There were no age differences
between the genotypes. 5-HTTLPR and Emotion Regulation
Table 1 also shows reappraisal scores from ERQ and CERQ by
Data Analysis genotype group. An ANCOVA, with the 5-HTTLPR genotype
group as between-subjects factor and sex as covariate, indicated
There were no missing data. Analyses of covariance (AN- that genotype had a significant effect on the ERQ-reappraisal
COVA), with sex as covariate, were used to test for differences scores, F(2, 179) ⫽ 15.8, p ⬍ .01, ␩p2 ⫽ 0.32. The S’S’ group
between genotypes in reappraisal, suppression, and in social displayed significantly lower ERQ-reappraisal scores than both the
anxiety symptoms. There were no sex differences in ERQ S’L’ (p ⬍ .01) and the L’L’ groups (p ⬍ .01). The difference
reappraisal, CERQ reappraisal, ERQ suppression, or LSAS-SR between the S’L’ and L’L’ genotypes on ERQ-reappraisal scores
scores (all p ⬎ .05). Mediation was tested using multiple was not significant (p ⫽ .99). The significant effect of the
regression (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 5-HTTLPR genotype was also evident on reappraisal scores from
2004; Hoyt, Imel, & Chan, 2008), following the steps suggested CERQ, F(2, 179) ⫽ 8, p ⬍ .01, ␩p2 ⫽ 0.192. The S’S’ group had
by Baron and Kenny (1986); see Figure 1: (1) the initial significantly lower CERQ-reappraisal scores than the L’L’ group
variable (genotype group) was significantly related to the out- (p ⬍ .01), and the S’L’ group had significantly lower scores than
come variable (LSAS-SR score; Path c); (2) the initial variable the the L’L’ group (p ⬍ .01). The difference between the S’S’ and
was related to the mediator (emotion-regulation score; Path a); S’L’ groups was not significant (p ⬍ .5). ERQ-suppression scores
and (3) the mediator was significantly associated with the were not significantly different between genotype groups (see
outcome variable when regressed on both the mediator and the Table 1).
initial variable (Path b), and the effect of the initial variable
(Path c’) was reduced compared with that (Path c) in the first 5-HTTLPR, Reappraisal, and Social Anxiety
regression. Because there is no general consensus on the addi- Symptoms
tive or dominant status of the low-expressing alleles of
Table 2 presents the regression coefficients from the analyses of
5-HTTLPR, the genotype was coded according to the number of the mediation model, in which the 5-HTTLPR genotype was the
S’ alleles present (instead of dummy coding comparing low- initial variable, LSAS-SR was the outcome variable, and the
expressing alleles carriers with L’ homozygotes: 0 ⫽ L’L’; 1 ⫽ ERQ-reappraisal was the potential mediator (see also Figure 1A).
S’L’; and 2 ⫽ S’S.’ (For similar approaches, see Conway et al., To test the direct effect (Path c in Figure 1A), LSAS-SR scores
2012; Heinz et al., 2005). We tested the significance of the were first regressed on the genotype. As hypothesized, the geno-
indirect effect of 5-HTTLPR on social anxiety through emotion type was significantly associated with social anxiety. By regress-
regulation using the bootstrapping method (bias corrected, with ing ERQ-reappraisal scores on 5-HTTLPR, we also confirmed that
1,000 iterations; Preacher & Hayes, 2004; Shrout & Bolger, this genotype was significantly related to individual differences in
2002). The indirect effect was significant if the CI from boot- cognitive reappraisal (Path a in Figure 1A). In the third step, social
strapping did not include zero (Frazier et al., 2004; Shrout &
Bolger, 2002). Two sets of analyses were conducted, one for 1
each of the two reappraisal measures (i.e., ERQ, CERQ). In The effect of genotype remained significant and the effect size was
comparable even after excluding the covariate (i.e., sex) from the analysis,
addition, we tested an alternative model in which ERQ- F(2, 179) ⫽ 13.18, p ⬍ .01, ␩2p ⫽ 0.28. The effects of age and Age ⫻
suppression was specified as a mediator in the relation between 5-HTTLPR on LSAS-SR scores were not significant.
2
5-HTTLPR and social anxiety symptoms. This allowed us to Excluding the covariate from the analyses did not change the effects of
genotype on ERQ reappraisal, F(2, 179) ⫽ 15.73, p ⬍ .01, ␩2p ⫽ 0.31 and
explore the specificity of cognitive reappraisal as a mediator.
CERQ reappraisal, F(2, 179) ⫽ 7.46, p ⬍ .01, ␩2p ⫽ 0.18. Similarly,
All the analyses, including the bootstrapping tests, were run in excluding the covariate from the analyses on ERQ-suppression scores did
SPSS. not change the null result, F(2, 179) ⫽ 0.41, p ⬎ .05.
1016 MIU, VULTURAR, CHIŞ, UNGUREANU, AND GROSS

We confirmed that the genotype was significantly related to


individual differences in CERQ reappraisal (Path a in Figure 1B),
and CERQ reappraisal and social anxiety were significantly related
while controlling for the genotype (Path b in Figure 1B). The CI
[5.63, 19.8] from bootstrapping did not include zero, which con-
firmed that CERQ reappraisal was a significant mediator between
the genotype and social anxiety.4 The indirect effect was also
significant when we restricted the analyses to the participants who
scored above 55 on LSAS: the CI from bias-corrected bootstrap-
ping was [2.26; 14.41]. The reverse causality analysis that tested
the indirect effect of the genotype on CERQ reappraisal through
social anxiety revealed no significant mediation, CI [⫺1.19, 0.04]
from bias-corrected bootstrapping.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

To examine the specificity of reappraisal as a mediator, we


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tested whether ERQ-suppression mediated the relation between


5-HTTLPR and social anxiety symptoms (see Table 4). The
5-HTTLPR was not related to ERQ suppression (Path a in Figure
1C), and suppression and social anxiety were not significantly
related while controlling for the genotype (Path b in Figure 1C).
The indirect effect was not significant, as indicated by the CI
[⫺3.35; 4.93] from bootstrapping.

Discussion
Low-expressing 5-HTTLPR genotypes were associated with
decreased reappraisal and increased social anxiety. Using two
measures of cognitive reappraisal, we found converging evidence
that individual differences in this emotion-regulation strategy carry
the influence of 5-HTTLPR genotype on anxiety symptoms. By
contrast, expressive suppression was not linked to 5-HTTLPR and
social anxiety. These findings support the hypothesis that
5-HTTLPR contributes to social anxiety symptoms via decreased
reappraisal.
Figure 1. Standardized regression coefficients for the relationship be-
tween the 5-HTTLPR genotype and social anxiety symptoms as carried by Cognitive Reappraisal Is an Intermediate Phenotype
ERQ reappraisal (A), CERQ reappraisal (B) and ERQ suppression (C).
Individual differences in reappraisal are known to influence
levels of positive and negative affect, as well as social support and
anxiety was regressed on both the genotype and the reappraisal well-being in the general population (Gross & John, 2003; Sheppes
scores. Reappraisal and social anxiety were significantly related & Gross, 2011). In contrast, decreased use of such adaptive
when controlling for the genotype (Path b in Figure 1). The emotion-regulation strategies in daily life significantly contributes
coefficient associated with the relation between the genotype and to the development of psychopathology (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema,
social anxiety decreased, but remained significant when control- & Schweizer, 2010; Bradley et al., 2011; Szasz et al., 2011),
ling for reappraisal in the equation (Path c’ in Figure 1). Using including social anxiety symptoms (Rusch, Westermann, & Lin-
bias-corrected bootstrapping (with 1,000 iterations), we tested the coln, 2011). In a diathesis-stress framework, is has been hypoth-
significance of the indirect effect of the genotype on social anxiety esized that the risk of social anxiety problems, associated with the
through ERQ reappraisal. The 95% CI [0.01, 14.28] for the indi- decreased use of cognitive reappraisal, is related to a certain
rect effect did not include zero, indicating that ERQ reappraisal genetic background. However, until now, there were no studies
was a significant mediator.3 When we restricted the analyses to the that traced the pathway from genetic susceptibilities to social
participants who scored above the clinical cut-off (i.e., ⬎ 55) on anxiety, through emotion regulation. The present study found that
LSAS-SR (n ⫽ 89), the indirect effect remained significant (CI for the habitual use of cognitive reappraisal significantly mediated the
the indirect effect: [0.74, 13.66]). A reverse-causality analysis that link between the 5-HTTLPR genotype and social anxiety symp-
tested a model in which the effect of the genotype on ERQ toms.
reappraisal was transmitted via social anxiety found a nonsignifi-
cant indirect effect (CI from bias-corrected bootstrapping: [⫺2.5,
3
0.23]). Based on Preacher and Kelley (2011), we estimated the effect size as
the ratio of the obtained indirect effect to the maximum possible indirect
We tested a similar model in which CERQ reappraisal was the effect: k2 ⫽ 0.27.
mediator between the genotype and social anxiety symptoms (Ta- 4
The effect-size index (Preacher & Kelley, 2011) for this indirect effect
ble 3 and Figure 1B). is k2 ⫽ 0.09.
5-HTTLPR, REAPPRAISAL, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 1017

Table 1
Anxiety Symptoms and Emotion-Regulation Scores by 5-HTTLPR Genotype

Cognitive reappraisal Positive reappraisal Expressive suppression


Liebowitz social (Emotion regulation (Cognitive emotion (Emotion regulation
Genotype anxiety scale questionnaire) regulation questionnaire) questionnaire)

L’L’ 44.84 ⫾ 4.07 5.41 ⫾ 0.06 16.47 ⫾ 0.66 2.77 ⫾ 0.31


S’L’ 49.78 ⫾ 3.86 5.39 ⫾ 0.07 13.6 ⫾ 0.61ⴱⴱ 3.09 ⫾ 0.21
S’S’ 76.57 ⫾ 5.02ⴱⴱ 4.9 ⫾ 0.06ⴱⴱ 12.47 ⫾ 0.76ⴱⴱ 3.05 ⫾ 0.26
Note. Values in cells are M ⫾ 1 SEM.
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

This finding complements previous neuroimaging reports of an imaging results, the behavioral results reported in these studies
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

association between 5-HTTLPR and activity in an amygdala- were more convergent. In line with the present results, S-allele
cingulate circuit related to emotion regulation (Firk et al., 2013; carriers displayed reduced ERQ reappraisal, but similar ERQ-
Heinz et al., 2005; Pezawas et al., 2005; Schardt et al., 2010). In suppression scores (Schardt et al., 2010). Moreover, S carriers
the largest of these studies (N ⫽ 94), S-allele carriers displayed were less succesful than L homozygotes in reducing negative
reduced functional connectivity between the rostral subgenual affect by cognitive reappraisal (Firk et al., 2013). This is also
portion of the anterior cingulate cortex and the amygdala during supported by studies that found reduced coping potential in
perception of fearful stimuli (Pezawas et al., 2005). The reduced S-allele carriers (Szily et al., 2008). Our present results confirmed
correlation of activity in this cingulate-amygdala circuit predicted the association between 5-HTTLPR and habitual cognitive reap-
almost 30% of the variance in dispositional anxiety (Pezawas et praisal in a larger sample, and showed for the first time that this
al., 2005). However, in a similar study, Heinz et al. (2005) reported association is one of the mechanisms by which 5-HTTLPR influ-
an increased functional connectivity between the ventromedial ences social anxiety.
prefrontal cortex and the amygdala during the perception of aver- A large theoretical and empirical literature in the diathesis-stress
sive images in S-allele carriers. By manipulating emotion regula- framework (for review see Caspi et al., 2010; Canli & Lesch,
tion, two recent studies also found that emotional detachment 2007) suggests that genetic differences might moderate the rela-
(Schardt et al., 2010) or cognitive reappraisal (Firk et al., 2013) tionship between stress and emotional disorders. These studies
with aversive images triggered increased activity in brain areas highlight the possibility that the magnitude of the effects of
associated with the top-down control of emotions (e.g., ventrolat- 5-HTTLPR on emotion regulation might differ as a function of
eral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; left medial prefrontal cor- exposure to stressors. More specifically, it recently has been hy-
tex; rostral anterior cingulate cortex) in S carriers. pothesized that people who are genotypically more reactive to
As pointed out by Pezawas et al. (2005), the differences between stress might develop emotional problems, particularly when their
these neuroimaging results may be explained by the focus on emotion regulation, which developed to allow adaptation to the
nonoverlapping prefrontal and cingulate regions, based on differ- type of stressor that was encountered early in life, is challenged
ent algorithms for the selection of the regions of interest. In during adulthood by a different type of stressor (Homberg, 2012).
addition, differences in emotion tasks (i.e., perception vs. regula- This stress-coping mismatch hypothesis is based on animal
tion), sample size and genotyping procedures (i.e., biallelic vs. studies, and it has started to be supported in 5-HTT knockout
triallelic 5-HTTLPR) may have also contributed to the heteroge- rodents exposed to escapable or inescapable stress (van der
neity of neuroimaging observations. In constrast with the neuro- Doelen, Kozicz, & Homberg, 2013). Based on the recent findings

Table 2
Results of Multiple Regression Analyses on the Mediator Role of ERQ Cognitive Reappraisal in the Relationships Between 5-HTTLPR
Genotype and Social Anxiety Symptoms

Testing steps in mediation model B SE B 95% CI ␤

Testing Step 1 (Path c)


Outcome: LSAS social anxiety
Predictor: Genotype (L’L’ vs. S’L’ vs. S’S’)a 15.86 3.5 9.39, 22.52 0.48ⴱⴱ
Testing Step 2 (Path a)
Outcome: ERQ-reappraisal
Predictor: Genotype ⫺1.52 0.34 ⫺2.09, ⫺0.93 ⫺0.47ⴱⴱ
Testing step 3 (Paths b and c’)
Outcome: LSAS social anxiety
Mediator: ERQ-reappraisal (Path b) ⫺5.89 1.01 ⫺7.58, ⫺4.09 ⫺0.57ⴱⴱ
Predictor: Genotype (Path c“) 6.88 3.27 0.02, 13.84 0.21ⴱ
Note. Abbreviations: B ⫽ unstandardized regression coefficient; ␤ ⫽ standardized regression coefficient; CI ⫽ confidence interval; SE ⫽ standard error.
a
0 ⫽ L’L’ (i.e., LA/LA and LA/XL genotypes); 1 ⫽ S’L’ (i.e., S/LA and LG/LA genotypes); 2 ⫽ S’S’ (S/S, LG/LG and S/LG genotypes).

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
1018 MIU, VULTURAR, CHIŞ, UNGUREANU, AND GROSS

Table 3
Results of Multiple Regression Analyses on the Mediator Role of CERQ Positive Reappraisal in the Relationships Between 5-HTTLPR
Genotype and Social Anxiety Symptoms

Testing steps in mediation model B SE B 95% CI ␤

Testing step 1 (Path c)


Outcome: LSAS social anxiety
Predictor: Genotype (L’L’ vs. S’L’ vs. S’S’)a 15.86 3.5 9.39, 22.52 0.48ⴱⴱ
Testing step 2 (Path a)
Outcome: CERQ-reappraisal
Predictor: Genotype ⫺2 0.54 ⫺2.94, ⫺1.01 ⫺0.41ⴱⴱ
Testing step 3 (Paths b and c’)
Outcome: LSAS social anxiety
Mediator: CERQ-reappraisal (Path b) ⫺1.51 0.76 ⫺2.78, ⫺0.13 ⫺0.22ⴱ
Predictor: Genotype (Path c’) 12.85 3.27 5.9, 19.93 0.39ⴱⴱ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. Abbreviations: B ⫽ unstandardized regression coefficient; ␤ ⫽ standardized regression coefficient; CI ⫽ confidence interval; SE ⫽ standard error.
a
0 ⫽ L’L’ (i.e., LA/LA and LA/XL genotypes); 1 ⫽ S’L’ (i.e., S/LA and LG/LA genotypes); 2 ⫽ S’S’ (S/S, LG/LG and S/LG genotypes).

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.

that people choose certain emotion-regulation strategies depending other emotion-regulation strategies (e.g., distraction) and the sim-
on the intensity of stress (i.e., reappraisal for low-intensity stress ilarity between the early and adult levels of stress.
vs. distraction for high-intensity stress; Sheppes, Scheibe, Suri, &
Gross, 2011; Sheppes et al., 2012), and that like 5-HTT knockout
Implications for Psychotherapy
rodents, people of the SS genotype of 5-HTTLPR show increased
sensitivity to stress (Drabant et al., 2012), the following predic- The present study has important implications for psychotherapy.
tions could be made in humans, in line with the stress-coping Cognitive– behavioral therapy (CBT) for anxiety and depression
mismatch hypothesis: (a) Exposure to lower or higher levels of has focused on teaching reappraisal skills (Clark & Beck, 2010;
stress during childhood would be specifically associated with Mennin, Ellard, Fresco, & Gross, 2013). Recent studies have
higher habitual reappraisal and distraction, respectively; (b) emo- demonstrated that the successful acquisition of these skills differ-
tional problems in adults would be increased in habitual reapprais- entiated between responders and nonresponders to CBT, and cor-
ers who were exposed to low levels of stress during childhood, but related with decreases of symptoms in SAD (Heinz et al., 2005;
currently experience high levels of stress, as well as in habitual Kley et al., 2012; Moscovitch et al., 2012). Moreover, 5-HTTLPR
distractors who were exposed to high levels of stress during was also found to influence the response to CBT in a large sample
childhood, but currently experience low levels of stress; and (c) the of children with anxiety disorders (Eley et al., 2012). The SS
latter effect would be facilitated in S homozygotes for 5-HTTLPR. genotype was associated with increased response to therapy, in
These interesting predictions remain to be tested in future studies comparison with SL and LL genotypes (Eley et al., 2012).
(ideally, longitudinal studies); they also offer an explanation for Based on the present finding that low-expressing 5-HTTLPR
the present finding that habitual reappraisal is a partial mediator of genotypes are also associated with decreased use of cognitive
the relation between 5-HTTLPR and social anxiety. In light of our reappraisal, and in light of the observation that cognitive reap-
perspective on the stress-coping mismatch hypothesis, it is possi- praisal is a key mechanism of change in CBT, we suggest that the
ble that the magnitude of this mediation is influenced by the use of patients with the low-expressing 5-HTTLPR genotypes may ben-

Table 4
Results of Multiple Regression Analyses on the Mediator Role of ERQ Expressive Suppression in the Relationships Between 5-
HTTLPR Genotype and Social Anxiety Symptoms

Testing steps in mediation model B SE B 95% CI ␤

Testing step 1 (Path c)


Outcome: LSAS social anxiety
Predictor: Genotype (L’L’ vs. S’L’ vs. S’S’)a 15.86 3.5 9.39, 22.52 0.48ⴱⴱ
Testing step 2 (Path a)
Outcome: ERQ-suppression
Predictor: Genotype 0.13 0.2 ⫺0.26, 0.53 0.08
Testing step 3 (Paths b and c”)
Outcome: LSAS social anxiety
Mediator: ERQ-suppression (Path b) 0.96 2.11 ⫺3.25, 5.19 0.04
Predictor: Genotype (Path c“) 15.73 3.53 8.67, 22.79 0.47ⴱⴱ
Note. Abbreviations: B ⫽ unstandardized regression coefficient; ␤ ⫽ standardized regression coefficient; CI ⫽ confidence interval; SE ⫽ standard error.
a
0 ⫽ L’L’ (i.e., LA/LA and LA/XL genotypes); 1 ⫽ S’L’ (i.e., S/LA and LG/LA genotypes); 2 ⫽ S’S’ (S/S, LG/LG and S/LG genotypes).
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01.
5-HTTLPR, REAPPRAISAL, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 1019

efit the most from CBT because their cognitive reappraisal skills regulation—reappraisal— using two different measures allowed us
are markedly deficient. Therefore, our findings may have timely to show that our core findings were robust in that they were
implications for the new field of “therapygenetics” (Beevers & evidenced using two different measures of reappraisal. However,
McGeary, 2012; Eley et al., 2012), by highlighting the role of we explored the specificity of reappraisal by showing that sup-
cognitive reappraisal in the link between 5-HTTLPR and response pression was not a mediator. Our decision to focus on just one
to CBT. genetic susceptibility factor was motivated by the fact that prior
research strongly pointed to this risk factor as a promising candi-
date susceptibility factor. One important direction for future re-
Limitations and Future Directions
search might be to investigate the additive effects of 5-HTTLPR
Although promising, the present study has several limitations. and Tryptophan Hydroxylase 2 gene polymorphisms (i.e.,
One is the relatively modest sample size. Recent meta-analyses rs4570625), which were both related to emotion appraisal (this
have failed to replicate Gene ⫻ Environment interaction effects study; Szily et al., 2008; Szily & Keri, 2012) and are known to
(e.g., 5-HTTLPR ⫻ Child Abuse on depression), which have been interactively affect other emotion phenotypes (Herrmann et al.,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

reported in samples too underpowered to detect small genetic 2007). In the present study, the frequency of using reappraisal or
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

effects (Duncan & Keller, 2011; Risch et al., 2009). These results suppression was assessed using self-report instruments (i.e., ERQ
highlight the importance of the sample size in reducing the prob- and CERQ), which, however reliable, are subject to demand char-
ability of Type I errors, which would require over a thousand acteristics and limited to emotion regulation processes to which
participants to be recruited for detection of a genetic influence with participants have introspective access. Therefore, future studies
small effect size (Duncan & Keller, 2011). However, similar, more could also measure other aspects (e.g., ability rather than fre-
inclusive meta-analyses (Karg, Burmeister, Shedden, & Sen, 2011; quency) and processes of emotion regulation (e.g., automatic
Uher & McGuffin, 2010), i.e., studies with alternative measures or rather than voluntary), using performance-based and neural assess-
statistical models were considered, replicated the original effects. ments (Firk et al., 2013; Schardt et al., 2010). Moreover, other
Although there is general agreement that results from genetic clinically relevant forms of emotion regulation (e.g., reappraisal
association studies with underpowered samples should be taken vs. distraction) that may be adaptive or maladaptive depending on
with caution until they are replicated in large-scale studies or the level of stress during development and adulthood (Clasen et al.,
meta-analyses (Caspi et al., 2010), it has also been acknowledged 2011; Homberg, 2012), could also be investigated in the future.
that the rate of false positive findings may not be that high in these The present study supported the hypothesis that reappraisal is a
studies (Lohmueller, Pearce, Pike, Lander, & Hirschhorn, 2003). potential intermediate phenotype on the pathway from 5-HTTLPR
Large-scale genetic association studies may offer a sample-size to social anxiety symptoms. These results have important impli-
standard that has seldom been achieved in psychological research, cations for diathesis-stress theories of SAD, by identifying
and the solution of organizing multicentric studies may be prob- 5-HTTLPR as a genetic candidate that influences social anxiety
lematic because of ethnic stratification and increased variability of symptoms through cognitive reappraisal, and for clinical interven-
the 5-HTTLPR allele distribution in different populations, for tions that include an emphasis on cognitive reappraisal training,
instance (Murdoch, Speed, Pakstis, Heffelfinger, & Kidd, 2013). such as CBT.
Even though they do not offer definitive results, hypothesis-driven
genetic association studies such as this one, i.e., those with explic-
itly defined phenotypes, precise localization of polymorphisms, References
low genotyping error rate, availability of genotype counts and Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-
analyses, that avoid overlap with previous studies (Buckland, regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review.
2001; Conneally & Sparkes, 1998; Lohmueller et al., 2003), may Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11
guide the discovery of true genetic influences on psychological .004
phenotypes. Notwithstanding these favoring perspectives, we ac- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Man-
knowledge that our promising results should be independently ual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.; DSM–IV–TR). Washington,
DC: Author.
replicated before they begin to influence mainstream research on
Argyropoulos, S. V., Hood, S. D., Adrover, M., Bell, C. J., Rich, A. S.,
social anxiety. Nash, J. R., . . . Nutt, D. J. (2004). Tryptophan depletion reverses the
A second limitation of the present study is the lack of clinical therapeutic effect of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in social
diagnosis of the participants. Previous genetic studies failed to find anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 503–509. doi:10.1016/j
an association between 5-HTTLPR and the SAD diagnosis (Strug .biopsych.2004.07.006
et al., 2010), but identified significant influences of this genotype Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable
on particular phenotypes that characterize the social anxiety spec- distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
trum. This motivated our decision to focus on social anxiety statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
symptoms in nonpatient volunteers, which allows us to generalize 51, 1173–1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
our findings to a broad segment of the social anxiety spectrum. Battaglia, M., Ogliari, A., Zanoni, A., Citterio, A., Pozzoli, U., Giorda, R.,
. . . Marino, C. (2005). Influence of the serotonin transporter promoter
However, it must be acknowledged that the generalization of the
gene and shyness on children’s cerebral responses to facial expressions.
present findings might be limited by the underrepresentation of Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 85–94. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62
men in this sample. .1.85
A third important limitation of this study is that we focused on Beevers, C. G., & McGeary, J. E. (2012). Therapygenetics: Moving to-
just one form of emotion regulation and just one genetic suscep- wards personalized psychotherapy treatment. Trends in Cognitive Sci-
tibility factor. Our decision to focus on one form of emotion ences, 16, 11–12. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.004
1020 MIU, VULTURAR, CHIŞ, UNGUREANU, AND GROSS

Bradley, B., DeFife, J. A., Guarnaccia, C., Phifer, J., Fani, N., Ressler, and response to psychological therapy. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 236 –
K. J., & Westen, D. (2011). Emotion dysregulation and negative affect: 237. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.132
Association with psychiatric symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, Firk, C., Siep, N., & Markus, C. R. (2013). Serotonin transporter genotype
72, 685– 691. doi:10.4088/JCP.10m06409blu modulates cognitive reappraisal of negative emotions: A functional
Buckland, P. R. (2001). Genetic association studies of alcoholism– magnetic resonance imaging study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neu-
problems with the candidate gene approach. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 36, roscience, 8, 247–258. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr091
99 –103. doi:10.1093/alcalc/36.2.99 Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator and
Canli, T., Ferri, J., & Duman, E. A. (2009). Genetics of emotion regulation. mediator effects in counseling psychology research. Journal of Coun-
Neuroscience, 164, 43–54. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.06.049 seling Psychology, 51, 115–134. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.51.1.115
Canli, T., & Lesch, K. P. (2007). Long story short: The serotonin trans- Fresco, D. M., Coles, M. E., Heimberg, R. G., Liebowitz, M. R., Hami, S.,
porter in emotion regulation and social cognition. Nature Neuroscience, Stein, M. B., & Goetz, D. (2001). The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale:
10, 1103–1109. doi:10.1038/nn1964 A comparison of the psychometric properties of self-report and
Canli, T., Qiu, M., Omura, K., Congdon, E., Haas, B. W., Amin, Z., . . . clinician-administered formats. Psychological Medicine, 31, 1025–1035.
Lesch, K. P. (2006). Neural correlates of epigenesis. PNAS: Proceedings doi:10.1017/S0033291701004056
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Furmark, T., Appel, L., Henningsson, S., Ahs, F., Faria, V., Linnman, C.,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

103, 16033–16038. doi:10.1073/pnas.0601674103 . . . Fredrikson, M. (2008). A link between serotonin-related gene


Carthy, T., Horesh, N., Apter, A., Edge, M. D., & Gross, J. J. (2010). polymorphisms, amygdala activity, and placebo-induced relief from
Emotional reactivity and cognitive regulation in anxious children. Be- social anxiety. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 13066 –13074. doi:
haviour Research and Therapy, 48, 384 –393. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2534-08.2008
.12.013 Furmark, T., Tillfors, M., Garpenstrand, H., Marteinsdottir, I., Langstrom,
Caspi, A., Hariri, A. R., Holmes, A., Uher, R., & Moffitt, T. E. (2010). B., Oreland, L., & Fredrikson, M. (2004). Serotonin transporter poly-
Genetic sensitivity to the environment: The case of the serotonin trans- morphism related to amygdala excitability and symptom severity in
porter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases and traits. patients with social phobia. Neuroscience Letters, 362, 189 –192. doi:
The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 509 –527. doi:10.1176/appi 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.02.070
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events,
.ajp.2010.09101452
cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personality and
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive theory and therapy of
Individual Differences, 30, 1311–1327. doi:10.1016/S0191-
anxiety and depression: Convergence with neurobiological findings.
8869(00)00113-6
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 418 – 424. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.06
Gillihan, S. J., Rao, H., Wang, J., Detre, J. A., Breland, J., Sankoorikal,
.007
G. M., . . . Farah, M. J. (2010). Serotonin transporter genotype modulates
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In
amygdala activity during mood regulation. Social Cognitive and Affec-
R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope & F. R. Schneier (Eds.),
tive Neuroscience, 5, 1–10. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp035
Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment and treatment (pp. 69 –93). New
Giuliani, N., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Reappraisal. In D. Sander & K. Scherer
York, NY: Guilford Press.
(Eds.), Oxford companion to the affective sciences (pp. 329 –330). New
Clasen, P. C., Wells, T. T., Knopik, V. S., McGeary, J. E., & Beevers, C. G.
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(2011). 5-HTTLPR and BDNF Val66Met polymorphisms moderate
Goldin, P. R., Manber, T., Hakimi, S., Canli, T., & Gross, J. J. (2009).
effects of stress on rumination. Genes, Brain & Behavior, 1, 740 –746.
Neural bases of social anxiety disorder: Emotional reactivity and cog-
doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00715.x
nitive regulation during social and physical threat. Archives of General
Conneally, P. M., & Sparkes, R. S. (1998). Molecular genetics of alcohol-
Psychiatry, 66, 170 –180. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.525
ism and other addiction/compulsive disorders. General discussion. Al- Goldin, P. R., Manber-Ball, T., Werner, K., Heimberg, R., & Gross, J. J.
cohol, 16, 85–91. (2009). Neural mechanisms of cognitive reappraisal of negative self-
Conway, C. C., Hammen, C., Espejo, E. P., Wray, N. R., Najman, J. M., beliefs in social anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 1091–1099.
& Brennan, P. A. (2012). Appraisals of stressful life events as a doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.014
genetically-linked mechanism in the stress-depression relationship. Cog- Grant, B. F., Stinson, F. S., Dawson, D. A., Chou, S. P., Dufour, M. C.,
nitive Therapy and Research, 36, 338 –347. doi:10.1007/s10608-011- Compton, W., . . . Kaplan, K. (2004). Prevalence and co-occurrence of
9368-9 substance use disorders and independent mood and anxiety disorders:
Domschke, K., Stevens, S., Beck, B., Baffa, A., Hohoff, C., Deckert, J., & Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related
Gerlach, A. L. (2009). Blushing propensity in social anxiety disorder: Conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 807– 816. doi:10.1001/
Influence of serotonin transporter gene variation. Journal of Neural archpsyc.61.8.807
Transmission, 116, 663– 666. doi:10.1007/s00702-008-0090-z Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion
Drabant, E. M., Ramel, W., Edge, M. D., Hyde, L. W., Kuo, J. R., Goldin, regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
P. R., . . . Gross, J. J. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348 –362.
5-HTTLPR-related sensitivity to acute stress. American Journal of Psy- doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
chiatry, 169, 397– 405. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111699 Hariri, A. R., & Holmes, A. (2006). Genetics of emotional regulation: The
Duncan, L. E., & Keller, M. C. (2011). A critical review of the first 10 role of the serotonin transporter in neural function. Trends in Cognitive
years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychi- Sciences, 10, 182–191. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.011
atry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 1041–1049. doi:10.1176/appi Heinz, A., Braus, D. F., Smolka, M. N., Wrase, J., Puls, I., Hermann,
.ajp.2011.11020191 D., . . . Buchel, C. (2005). Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a
Ehli, E. A., Hu, Y., Lengyel-Nelson, T., Hudziak, J. J., & Davies, G. E. genetic variation of the serotonin transporter. Nature Neuroscience, 8,
(2012). Identification and functional characterization of three novel 20 –21. doi:10.1038/nn1366
alleles for the serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region. Molec- Herrmann, M. J., Huter, T., Muller, F., Muhlberger, A., Pauli, P., Reif, A.,
ular Psychiatry, 17, 185–192. doi:10.1038/mp.2010.130 . . . Lesch, K. P. (2007). Additive effects of serotonin transporter and
Eley, T. C., Hudson, J. L., Creswell, C., Tropeano, M., Lester, K. J., tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variation on emotional processing. Ce-
Cooper, P. . . . Collier, D. A. (2012). Therapygenetics: The 5HTTLPR rebral Cortex, 17, 1160 –1163. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl026
5-HTTLPR, REAPPRAISAL, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY 1021

Hofmann, S. G. (2007). Cognitive factors that maintain social anxiety Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 661– 673. doi:10.1016/S0887-
disorder: A comprehensive model and its treatment implications. 6185(02)00134-2
Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 36, 193–209. doi:10.1080/ Mennin, D. S., McLaughlin, K. A., & Flanagan, T. J. (2009). Emotion
16506070701421313 regulation deficits in generalized anxiety disorder, social anxiety disor-
Homberg, J. R. (2012). The stress-coping (mis)match hypothesis for nature der, and their co-occurrence. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 866 –
x nurture interactions. Brain Research, 1432, 114 –121. doi:10.1016/j 871. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.04.006
.brainres.2011.11.037 Meyer-Lindenberg, A., & Weinberger, D. R. (2006). Intermediate pheno-
Hoyt, W. T., Imel, Z. E., & Chan, F. (2008). Multiple regression and types and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders. Nature Reviews
correlation techniques: Recent controversies and best practices. Reha- Neuroscience, 7, 818 – 827. doi:10.1038/nrn1993
bilitation Psychology, 53, 321–339. doi:10.1037/a0013021 Miu, A. C., Crisan, L. G., Chis, A., Ungureanu, L., Druga, B., & Vulturar,
Hu, X. Z., Lipsky, R. H., Zhu, G., Akhtar, L. A., Taubman, J., Greenberg, R. (2012). Somatic markers mediate the effect of serotonin transporter
B. D., . . . Goldman, D. (2006). Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of- gene polymorphisms on Iowa Gambling Task. Genes, Brain and Behav-
function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Amer- ior, 11, 398 – 403. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00774.x
ican Journal of Human Genetics, 78, 815– 826. doi:10.1086/503850 Mortensen, O. V., Thomassen, M., Larsen, M. B., Whittemore, S. R., &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Karg, K., Burmeister, M., Shedden, K., & Sen, S. (2011). The serotonin Wiborg, O. (1999). Functional analysis of a novel human serotonin
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

transporter promoter variant (5-HTTLPR), stress, and depression meta- transporter gene promoter in immortalized raphe cells. Molecular Brain
analysis revisited: Evidence of genetic moderation. Archives of General Research, 68, 141–148. doi:10.1016/S0169-328X(99)00083-2
Psychiatry, 68, 444 – 454. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.189 Moscovitch, D. A., Gavric, D. L., Senn, J. M., Santesso, D. L., Miskovic,
Kashdan, T. B. (2007). Social anxiety spectrum and diminished positive V., & Schmidt, L. A. (2012). Changes in judgment biases and use of
experiences: Theoretical synthesis and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychol- emotion-regulation strategies during cognitive behavioral therapy for
ogy Review, 27, 348 –365. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.12.003 social anxiety disorder: Distinguishing treatment responders from non-
Kessler, R. C., Foster, C. L., Saunders, W. B., & Stang, P. E. (1995). Social responders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36, 261–271. doi:10.1007/
consequences of psychiatric disorders, I: Educational attainment. Amer- s10608-011-9371-1
ican Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1026 –1032. Munafò, M. R., Brown, S. M., & Hariri, A. R. (2008). Serotonin transporter
Kessler, R. C., Stang, P., Wittchen, H. U., Stein, M., & Walters, E. E. (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: A meta-analysis. Bio-
(1999). Lifetime co-morbidities between social phobia and mood disor-
logical Psychiatry, 63, 852– 857. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.08.016
ders in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine,
Murdoch, J. D., Speed, W. C., Pakstis, A. J., Heffelfinger, C. E., & Kidd,
29, 555–567. doi:10.1017/S0033291799008375
K. K. (2013). Worldwide population variation and haplotype analysis at
Kley, H., Heinrichs, N., Bender, C., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2012). Pre-
the serotonin transporter gene SLC6A4 and implications for association
dictors of outcome in a cognitive-behavioral group program for children
studies. Biological Psychiatry. Advance online publication. doi:
and adolescents with social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disor-
10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.006
ders, 26, 79 – 87. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.09.002
Nakamura, M., Ueno, S., Sano, A., & Tanabe, H. (2000). The human
Kraft, J. B., Slager, S. L., McGrath, P. J., & Hamilton, S. P. (2005).
serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) shows ten
Sequence analysis of the serotonin transporter and associations with
novel allelic variants. Molecular Psychiatry, 5, 32–38. doi:10.1038/sj
antidepressant response. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 374 –381. doi:
.mp.4000698
10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.048
Pergamin-Hight, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H.,
Lemogne, C., Gorwood, P., Boni, C., Pessiglione, M., Lehericy, S., &
& Bar-Haim, Y. (2012). Variations in the promoter region of the
Fossati, P. (2011). Cognitive appraisal and life stress moderate the
serotonin transporter gene and biased attention for emotional informa-
effects of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism on amygdala reactivity. Human
Brain Mapping, 32, 1856 –1867. doi:10.1002/hbm.21150 tion: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 71, 373–379. doi:10.1016/
Lesch, K. P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Petri, j.biopsych.2011.10.030
S., . . . Murphy, D. L. (1996). Association of anxiety-related traits with Pezawas, L., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Drabant, E. M., Verchinski, B. A.,
a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Munoz, K. E., Kolachana, B. S., . . . Weinberger, D. R. (2005).
Science, 274, 1527–1531. doi:10.1126/science.274.5292.1527 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interac-
Liebowitz, M. R. (1987). Social phobia. Modern Problems of Pharmaco- tions: A genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nature Neu-
psychiatry, 22, 141–173. roscience, 8, 828 – 834. doi:10.1038/nn1463
Lin, P. Y. (2007). Meta-analysis of the association of serotonin transporter Pine, D. S., Cohen, P., Gurley, D., Brook, J., & Ma, Y. (1998). The risk for
gene polymorphism with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Progress in early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with
Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 31, 683– 689. doi: anxiety and depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55,
10.1016/j.pnpbase pair.2006.12.024 56 – 64. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.56
Lohmueller, K. E., Pearce, C. L., Pike, M., Lander, E. S., & Hirschhorn, Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for
J. N. (2003). Meta-analysis of genetic association studies supports a estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Re-
contribution of common variants to susceptibility to common disease. search Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36, 717–731. doi:10.3758/
Nature Genetics, 33, 177–182. doi:10.1038/ng1071 BF03206553
McNeil, D. W. (2001). Terminology and evolution of constructs related to Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation
social phobia. In S. G. Hoffmann & P. M. DiBartolo (Eds.), Social models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psy-
phobia and social anxiety: An integration (pp. 8 –19). Needham Heights, chological Methods, 16, 93–115. doi:10.1037/a0022658
MA: Allyn & Bacon. Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., Liang, K. Y., Eaves, L., Hoh, J., . . .
Mennin, D. S., Ellard, K. K., Fresco, D. M., & Gross, J. J. (2013). United Merikangas, K. R. (2009). Interaction between the serotonin transporter
we stand: Emphasizing commonalities across cognitive-behavioral ther- gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A
apies. Behavior Therapy, in press. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2013.02.004 meta-analysis. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association,
Mennin, D. S., Fresco, D. M., Heimberg, R. G., Schneier, F. R., Davies, 301, 2462–2471. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.878
S. O., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2002). Screening for social anxiety disorder Rodriguez, S., Gaunt, T. R., & Day, I. N. (2009). Hardy-Weinberg equi-
in the clinical setting: Using the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. librium testing of biological ascertainment for Mendelian randomization
1022 MIU, VULTURAR, CHIŞ, UNGUREANU, AND GROSS

studies. American Journal of Epidemiology, 169, 505–514. doi:10.1093/ Strug, L. J., Suresh, R., Fyer, A. J., Talati, A., Adams, P. B., Li, W., . . .
aje/kwn359 Weissman, M. M. (2010). Panic disorder is associated with the serotonin
Rusch, S., Westermann, S., & Lincoln, T. M. (2011). Specificity of transporter gene (SLC6A4) but not the promoter region (5-HTTLPR).
emotion regulation deficits in social anxiety: An internet study. Psychol- Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 166 –176. doi:10.1038/mp.2008.79
ogy and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 84, 273–287. Szasz, P. L., Szentagotai, A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2011). The effect of
Russell, G., & Shaw, S. (2009). A study to investigate the prevalence of emotion-regulation strategies on anger. Behaviour Research and Ther-
social anxiety in a sample of higher education students in the United apy, 49, 114 –119. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.011
Kingdom. Journal of Mental Health, 18, 198 –206. doi:10.1080/ Szily, E., Bowen, J., Unoka, Z., Simon, L., & Keri, S. (2008). Emotion
09638230802522494 appraisal is modulated by the genetic polymorphism of the serotonin
Salovey, P., Stroud, L. R., Woolery, A., & Epel, E. S. (2002). Perceived transporter. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 819 – 822. doi:
emotional intelligence, stress reactivity, and symptom reports: Further 10.1007/s00702-008-0029-4
explorations using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Psychology and Health, Szily, E., & Keri, S. (2012). Emotion appraisal and the tryptophan hydrox-
17, 611– 627. doi:10.1080/08870440290025812 ylase 2 (TPH2) gene. Journal of Neural Transmission, 119, 1261–1265.
Sareen, J., Cox, B. J., Afifi, T. O., de Graaf, R., Asmundson, G. J., ten doi:10.1007/s00702-012-0769-z
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Have, M., & Stein, M. B. (2005). Anxiety disorders and risk for suicidal Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., Luterek, J. A., Mennin, D. S., & Fresco,
D. M. (2005). Delineating emotion regulation deficits in generalized
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ideation and suicide attempts: A population-based longitudinal study of


adults. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 1249 –1257. doi:10.1001/ anxiety disorder: A comparison with social anxiety disorder. Cognitive
archpsyc.62.11.1249 Therapy and Research, 29, 89 –106. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1651-1
Schardt, D. M., Erk, S., Nusser, C., Nothen, M. M., Cichon, S., Rietschel, Uher, R., & McGuffin, P. (2010). The moderation by the serotonin trans-
M., . . . Walter, H. (2010). Volition diminishes genetically mediated porter gene of environmental adversity in the etiology of depression:
amygdala hyperreactivity. NeuroImage, 53, 943–951. doi:10.1016/j 2009 update. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 18 –22. doi:10.1038/mp.2009
.neuroimage.2009.11.078 .123
Schneier, F. R., Martin, L. Y., Liebowitz, M. R., Gorman, J. M., & Fyer, van der Doelen, R. H., Kozicz, T., & Homberg, J. R. (2013). Adaptive
A. J. (1989). Alcohol abuse in social phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disor- fitness; early life adversity improves adult stress coping in heterozygous
ders, 3, 15–23. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(89)90025-X serotonin transporter knockout rats. Molecular Psychiatry. Advance
Selvaraj, S., Godlewska, B. R., Norbury, R., Bose, S., Turkheimer, F., online publication. doi:10.1038/mp.2012.186
Stokes, P., . . . Cowen, P. J. (2011). Decreased regional gray matter van der Linden, G. J., Stein, D. J., & van Balkom, A. J. (2000). The
volume in S’ allele carriers of the 5-HTTLPR triallelic polymorphism. efficacy of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors for social anxiety
Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 471– 473. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.40 disorder (social phobia): a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials.
Sen, S., Burmeister, M., & Ghosh, D. (2004). Meta-analysis of the asso- International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 15, S15–S23.
ciation between a serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5- Vulturar, R., Chis, A., Ungureanu, L., & Miu, A. C. (2012). Respiratory
HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits. American Journal of sinus arrhythmia and serotonin transporter promoter gene polymor-
Medicine Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet, 127B, 85– 89. doi:10.1002/ phisms: Taking a triallelic approach makes a difference. Psychophysi-
ajmg.b.20158 ology, 49, 1412–1416. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01445.x
Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Is timing everything? Temporal con- Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative
siderations in emotion regulation. Personality and Social Psychology affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders. Journal
Review, 15, 319 –331. doi:10.1177/1088868310395778 of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 346 –353. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.97.3
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion- .346
regulation choice. Psychological Science, 22, 1391–1396. doi:10.1177/ Wendland, J. R., Martin, B. J., Kruse, M. R., Lesch, K. P., & Murphy, D. L.
0956797611418350 (2006). Simultaneous genotyping of four functional loci of human
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., Radu, P., Blechert, J., & Gross, J. J. SLC6A4, with a reappraisal of 5-HTTLPR and rs25531. Molecular
(2012). Emotion Regulation Choice: A Conceptual Framework and Psychiatry, 11, 224 –226. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001789
Supporting Evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Werner, K., Goldin, P. R., Ball, T. M., Heimberg, R. G., & Gross, J. J.
Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/a0030831 (2011). Assessing emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder: The
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non- Emotion Regulation Interview. Journal of Psychopathology and Behav-
experimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psycho- ioral Assessment, 33, 346 –354. doi:10.1007/s10862-011-9225-x
logical Methods, 7, 422– 445. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422 Whisman, M. A., Richardson, E. D., & Smolen, A. (2011). Behavioral
Stein, M. B., Campbell-Sills, L., & Gelernter, J. (2009). Genetic variation inhibition and triallelic genotyping of the serotonin transporter promoter
in 5HTTLPR is associated with emotional resilience. American Journal (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. Journal of Research in Personality, 45,
of Medicine Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet, 150B, 900 –906. doi: 706 –709. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.08.009
10.1002/ajmg.b.30916 Wittchen, H. U., Stein, M. B., & Kessler, R. C. (1999). Social fears and
Stein, M. B., Fuetsch, M., Muller, N., Hofler, M., Lieb, R., & Wittchen, social phobia in a community sample of adolescents and young adults:
H. U. (2001). Social anxiety disorder and the risk of depression: A Prevalence, risk factors and co-morbidity. Psychological Medicine, 29,
prospective community study of adolescents and young adults. Archives 309 –323. doi:10.1017/S0033291798008174
of General Psychiatry, 58, 251–256. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.251 World Medical Association. (1964/2008). WMA Declaration of Helsinki -
Stein, M. B., Seedat, S., & Gelernter, J. (2006). Serotonin transporter gene Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects.
promoter polymorphism predicts SSRI response in generalized social Retrieved from http://www.wma.net/en/30publications/10policies/b3/
anxiety disorder. Psychopharmacology, 187, 68 –72. doi:10.1007/
s00213-006-0349-8 Received November 29, 2012
Stein, M. B., & Stein, D. J. (2008). Social anxiety disorder. The Lancet, Revision received May 2, 2013
371, 1115–1125. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60488-2 Accepted May 10, 2013 䡲

You might also like