Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Audio-Video Correspondence and

Its Role in Altenfion and Memory


[] Tom Grimes

Tom Grimes is Assistant Professor and Research [] A good deal of the research in mass com-
Associate, School of Journalism and Mass munication (e.g., Graber, 1988; Gunter, 1987;
Communication, University of Wisconsin- R o b i n s o n & Levy, 1986) is d e v o t e d to an-
Madison, Madison, WI 53706. swering the question, W h y do people seem
to learn so little from television news? In-
deed, field surveys suggest that people who
watch TV news remember very little of what
This study measured the effect of audio- they see and hear (Katz, Adoni, & Parness,
video channel correspondence on attention 1977; Neuman, 1976; Robinson & Levy, 1986,
and memory. Three versions of four TV news pp. 107-132; Stauffer, Frost, & Rybolt, 1983).
stories were the stimuli. The high- The question extends beyond TV news; it
correspondence version employed a specific e x t e n d s to all m u l t i m e d i a presentations.
audio-video semantic match, the medium- Reeves (1989) sees n e w s as a " m e r e con-
correspondence version a less specific match, tainer" for the psychological phenomena that
and the no-correspondence version no mass media researchers examine, no differ-
match. High channel correspondence ent from TV advertisements, sitcoms, or, for
promoted the most efficient division of that matter, educational presentations. The
attention and the best memory scores on reason is that the processes being evaluated
visual and factual recognition measures. are so basic to h u m a n cognition that a mes-
The medium-correspondence version yielded sage's program title should have no effect on
significantly worse visual memory and the cognitive processes under study (Reeves,
attention scores, but factual attention and 1989, p. 194).
memory scores equal to those for the high- If Reeves is correct, then TV news may
correspondence version. The no- serve as the paradigm for a range of similar
correspondence version yielded high visual audiovisual presentations. Understanding
recognition scores, but the worst visual which variables influence learning from TV
attention scores, suggesting that attentional news s h o u l d help in u n d e r s t a n d i n g h o w
capacity boundaries were overwhelmed by learning is accomplished across several types
the stimulus. The results across all of audiovisual presentations.
conditions suggest that when attentional In considering those "learning variables"
capacity is exceeded, memory of the overall that are inherent in all audiovisual presen-
story is degraded. tations, one deserves special attention. It is
channel correspondenceithe semantic
match or mismatch between the audio and
video messages in the presentation. Channel
correspondence should be singled out be-
cause it is a troublesome variable for all au-

ETR&D.Vol, 38, No. 3, pp. 15-25 ISSN1042-t629 t5


'~ ETR&D,Vol. 38, No. 3

diovisual presentations. In TV news, the pic- tention--two of the most salient processes in
tures and words seldom match exactly. TV audiovisual message reception--are lumped
news, by its nature, is a hurry-up profession together into simple measures of recognition.
in which loose ends mar many newscasts. What is called for, then, is a test that uses
One of those loose ends is audio-video cor- a valid medium-correspondence Condition
respondence. and separates memory from attentional pro-
In most audiovisual presentations, tight cesses. Consequently, a study was designed
coordination between what is seen and what that made several assumptions about the way
is heard is inconstant. In an effort to better people learn from TV news and other audio-
understand how channel correspondence visual presentations, as discussed below.
mediates message understanding, Drew and Channel correspondence probably affects
Grimes (1987) created three correspondence the way in which attention is divided be-
conditions. They used the same seven-story tween the auditory and visual channels. Be-
series in each condition, but varied the de- cause attention is a limited c o m m o d i t y
gree to which the video matched the audio. (Kahneman, 1973), more attentional capacity
The high-correspondence condition featured is probably required to encode and integrate
matching audio and video messages. In the in m e m o r y the two information channels
no-correspondence condition, there was no when those two channels convey slightly to
semantic relationship between the audio and moderately different messages. With enough
video information channels. The third con- difference in messages (e.g., a medium-cor-
dition was one of medium correspondence, respondence condition), attention's capacity
with half high-correspondence and half no- threshold will probably be a p p r o a c h e d ,
correspondence messages. Drew and Grimes maybe even exceeded, and viewers may try
found that in the optimum viewing condi- to concentrate on the auditory channel--the
t i o n - h i g h channel correspondefice--audio who, what, when, where, why channel--at
recall and story understanding were the the expense of the less specific, and less use-
highest, and video recall the lowest. In the ful, visual channel.
least optimum viewing condition--no chan- On the other hand, when the message
nel correspondence--video recall was the channels correspond more than they differ,
highest, audio recall was the lowest, and attention should be more efficiently distrib-
story understanding was the worst. uted between channels, which would require
Unfortunately, Drew and Grimes (1987) less capacity. (This represents the high-cor-
created a medium-correspondence condition respondence condition.) This is because high
that was less a condition than a combination channel correspondence should allow the
of conditions. What is called for is a true me- viewer to monitor the auditory channel for
dium match between the audio and the the who, what, when, and where of the story
video, with the two channels neither unre- and to shadow the visual channel for infor-
lated nor tightly bound together semantically. mation that supplements the narration and
It is, in fact, this medium-correspondence can be integrated into the information sup-
condition that should be the most interesting, plied by it. Thus, attention to, and memory
because it best resembles the condition of au- for, the visual channel should be greater in
dio-video correspondence in most television this condition than in the medium-corre-
news reports. spondence condition.
Additionally, Drew and Grimes never However, if the conflict between the two
clearly defined which cognitive processes information channels is great enough (e.g.,
should be affected by channel correspon- the no-correspondence condition), attention
dence. Simple measures of recognition do not to the visual channel will override attention
tell us enough about the cognitive processes to the auditory channel. This idea is sup-
that ultimately blend the two information ported not only by the no-correspondence
channels into what we call "the story." With- data from the Drew and Grimes study, but
out additional information, memory and at- also by the work of Colavita (1974) and Pos-
AUDIO-VIDEO CORRESPONDENCE 17

net, Nissen, and Klein (1976). They report should be slower if the subject is devoting a
that visual stimuli dominate auditory stimuli lot of attention to the visual channel. On the
when pictures and words have no relation- other hand, if the subject is not devoting
ship to one another. This may be due to the much attention to visual messages, then re-
fact that pictures access semantic codes more s p o n s e to the p r e s e n c e of a light p r o b e
quickly than do words (Dhawan & Pelle- should be fast, because visual messages are
grino, 1977; Pellegrino, Rosinski, Chiesi, & not competing with the probe for entry into,
Siegel, 1977; Smith & Magee, 1980). That is, and analysis by, the human information-pro-
visual signals appear to go through fewer ex- cessing system. The same logic holds for tone
trapolations than auditory signals before probes.
they result in memory codes (Cohen, 1973). The point is that attending and respond-
When people are presented pictures and ing to a probe requires additional attentional
words that have no semantic relationship, the capacity not devoted to encoding and assim-
pictures require less effort to encode and ilating the messages within which the probe
store. Thus, there should be less attention is embedded. That is why reaction times to a
applied to the visual channel because less is probe should be slow when the subject is
required; the visual channel should be very attending to the messages containing the
easy to encode, and visual memory should probe: The subject must "disengage" from
be high because encoding is so easy. the messages in order to respond to the pres-
To test subject response to news stories in ence of the probe. On the other hand, re-
the three correspondence conditions, two de- sponding to the presence of the probe should
pendent measures were created. One meas- take much less time when the subject is not
ured allocation of attention between chan- trying to simultaneously encode complex
nels, and the other measured visual and messages conveyed within the sam e channel
factual recognition. as the probe appears. The amount of time
The dependent measure that tracked at- these processes take is measured in millisec-
tention employed secondary task methodol- onds.
ogy, which has been used in earlier commu- In summary, this study tracked the locus
nication research (e.g., Thorson, Reeves, & of attention as it was split between the audi-
Schleuder, 1985, 1986) and in cognitive psy- tory and visual information channels. The
chology (e.g., Britton, Muth, & Glynn, 1986). purpose was to discover which channel, if
Secondary task methodology makes three either, received less attention due to dimin-
assumptions. First, attention is limited. This ishing channel correspondence. If attentional
limited resource cannot be distributed evenly measures shifted among the three correspon-
among competing demands when those de- dence conditions, visual and factual recog-
mands require more resource than is avail- nition measures were taken to track the ef-
able (Kahneman, 1973; Norman & Bobrow, fects on memory of these shifts.
1975). Second, if competing demands require
more resource than is available, some de-
mands will use more of the resource than
other demands, creating an unequal distri- METHOD
bution of the resource. Third, this inequality
can be measured. Supers
Thorson, Reeves, and Schleuder (1985) il-
lustrated this phenomenon by using the anal- The subjects were 202 undergraduate com-
ogy of driving a car (p. 430). It is more diffi- munication majors ranging from sophomore
cult to answer questions (the secondary task) to senior classification who were recruited
when traffic is heavy and the driver must from two different classes. The subjects were
devote most of his or her attention to the act split among six cells in a 3 x 2 factorial de-
of driving (the primary task). Likewise, sub- sign ( A u d i o - V i d e o C o r r e s p o n d e n c e x
ject response to the presence of a light probe Probe).
,~ ETR&D,k,~ol.38, No. 3

Stimulus Materials crete, nonabstract ideas were conveyed, there


was audio-video correspondence.
Four fictitious but realistic television news MEDIuM-CORRESPONDENCE. In the me-
stories, each lasting 1 minute 30 seconds, dium-correspondence version, the two infor-
were used. The video for each was obtained mation channels were thematically related
from one of three sources: the CBS Evening but the individual audio and video messages
News with Dan Rather, ABC World News were not correspondent. This was achieved
Tonight with Peter Jennings, or the local news by accompanying a specific reference in the
of the CBS affiliate. The narrations were cre- narration, for example, "A needle the size of
ated by the author. Each story dealt with con- a screwdriver is inserted into the herniated
crete, nonabstract ideas. disk [in the spine]," with highly general
Each of the correspondent and partially video that was not semantically correspond-
c o r r e s p o n d e n t stories was evaluated by a ent with the reference, for example, "needle"
panel of professional broadcast journalists and "the size of a screwdriver." In this in-
(two local n e w s directors, one n e w s pro- stance, a patient was shown being wheeled
ducer, a n d one a n c h o r m a n ) . T h e y were into surgery as the reporter referred to the
asked to determine whether each of the sto- surgical tool.
ries was suitable for airing on their news
No-CORRESPONDENCE. In the no-corre-
broadcasts. All four correspondent stories
spondence version, the video did not match
were judged suitable for airing, as were all
the audio. Unlike the low-correspondence
four partially c o r r e s p o n d e n t stories. This
condition in Drew and Grimes' (1987) study,
evaluation was conducted in order to pro-
the n o - c o r r e s p o n d e n c e condition in this
duce an ecologically valid stimulus, one that
study did not employ coherent video. The
would persuade practitioners of the validity
visual track for each of the four stories em-
of the result.
ployed shots of events that were mixed to-
gether in a kind of visual potpourri. For in-
s t a n c e , as the n a r r a t o r d e s c r i b e d h o w
Story Versions herniated disks are repaired, the viewer saw
a barge loaded with g a r b a g e a n c h o r e d in
Three versions of each story were created:
New York Harbor. W h e n the shot changed,
high correspondence, m e d i u m correspon-
it s h o w e d celebrants of the m o s t recent
dence, and no correspondence.
Chinese New Year, while the narration con-
tinued to discuss surgery. The purpose of
HIGH-CORRESPONDENCE. In the high-cor-
this condition was to maximize channel con-
respondence version, there was a "tight" pic-
flict between the audio and video as a basis
t u r e - w o r d match. For instance, w h e n the
of comparison with the other extreme con-
narration in one of the stories stated, " A
dition, tight audio-video correspondence.
needle the size of a screwdriver is inserted
into the herniated disk [in the spine]," the Story narrations were not varied; they were
accompanying video showed a needle, the the same across all conditions.
size and shape of a screwdriver, about to be
inserted into the spine of a patient. When the Visual and Auditory Probes
script referred to more abstract concepts,
such as "[Patients] have to remain in the hos- Subjects were e x p o s e d to visual or tone
pital u p to t h r e e - a n d - a - h a l f w e e k s , " the probes embedded within each story for the
video was more general but related (in this p u r p o s e of measuring subject attention to
instance, people in wheelchairs were shown either the visual or auditory channel. Half
in a hospital corridor). In other words, there the subjects were exposed to seven visual
were some places in each script where there probes nested w i t h i n each story, and the
was no specific visual correlation between other half were exposed to analogous tone
the audio and video. However, where con- probes.
AUDIO-VIDEOCORRESPONDENCE t9

Visual probes consisted of National Tele- story was being prepared for use in this
vision Standards Committee Split Field Color study. A total of 36 frames were shown to
Bars lasting 33 milliseconds. These color bars subjects, with subjects making yes/no deci-
filled the entire screen when they appeared. sions as to whether each freeze frame ap-
In the high-correspondence condition, the peared in one of the four stories. Freeze
visual probes were placed at points where frames were clustered by story.
word and picture messages were presented
simultaneously. In the other two conditions,
the probes were placed at the same points Procedure
where they appeared in the high-correspon-
dence condition. Subjects were run individually by 1 of 12
The tone probes were analogous to the trained experimenters. A diary was kept by
visual probes in number and placement. each experimenter. After data were collected,
Where color bars appeared in the visual each subject was asked whether it was easy
probe condition, tones appeared in the au- or difficult to attend to the narration and
ditory probe condition. Tones were generated video. Subjects were also asked whether the
by a Hewlett-Packard Wide Range Oscillator probes to which they were exposed were dis-
and were set at 10,000 Hz. tracting. Any other observations subjects
In both probe conditions, subjects were wanted to offer were recorded as well.
told to push a telegraph key as soon as they Prior to viewing, subjects were read in-
heard a tone or saw a color bar. The amount structions that told them they were partici-
of time it took for them to press the key from pating in a memory test designed to find out
the onset of the probe--measured in milli- what sort of television news stories are best
seconds-provided the estimate used to infer remembered. They were told to remember as
attentional locus. The longer it took, the more much as possible of what they were about to
attention was assumed to be devoted to the see and hear. Subjects were intentionally
channel within which the probe appeared. misled into believing that if they did not score
The faster the response to a probe's pres- well enough on either the auditory or visual
ence, the less attention was assumed to be test, they would have to return to take the
devoted to the channel within which the tests again. This was done to emphasize the
probe appeared. importance of remembering the contents of
the auditory and visual channels (the pri-
mary task).
Recognition Tests To control for an order effect, four orders
of the four stories were employed. The topics
Subjects were given factual memory and vis- of the stories were maple syrup production,
ual memory tests after each story. The factual a new technique for repairing herniated disks
memory test was a 53-question, multiple- in the spine, a new method for scoring tennis
choice examination that probed for memory games, and tagging of hibernating bears in
of facts presented in the narration--facts that northern Wisconsin.
could not be inferred from information pre-
sented in the visual channel.
In the visual memory test, subjects were Data Analysis
shown freeze frames that included frames
taken from the visual channel of each story All reaction-time outliers for tone and color
and frames taken from distractor material de- bar reaction times were removed by calculat-
signed to look very much like material that ing each subject's mean time and then elim-
appeared in the story. Distractors, in fact, inating times that fell more than three stan-
came from "outtakes," that is, video that d a r d d e v i a t i o n s either side of t h a t
originally appeared when each story aired individual's mean. Outliers are always pres-
but was edited out by the author when each ent in reaction-time data because of differ-
20 ETR&D, Vol. 38, No. 3

460 -

449
440,

O3

v 420,
~J VISUAL PROBE

Z 400. 406

~) AUDITORY PROBE
<
~J 380

360

353
340
HIGH
' MEDIUM
' N'O

CORRESPONDENCE
FIGURE 1 [] ReactionTimes for Visual and Auditory Probes

ences among subjects' motor skills. Recent and no-correspondence conditions. Reaction
use of alcohol or drugs or simple fatigue can time was significantly slower in the high-cor-
greatly affect reaction time scores for any respondence condition (M = 449 millisec-
given subject. onds [msecs]) than in the m e d i u m - c o r r e -
Analysis of variance was performed on spondence condition (M = 406 msecs), p <
the data using Newman-Kuels post hoc tests .005. Reaction time was significantly slower
where appropriate. in the high-correspondence condition (M =
449 msecs) than in the no-correspondence
condition (M = 416 msecs), p < .02. No dif-
RESULTS ference was found between the medium-cor-
respondence and no-correspondence times,
Results f r o m the r e a c t i o n - t i m e data are p < .25.
shown in Figure 1. There was no statistically There was no interaction effect between
significant difference a m o n g the auditory probe type and level of correspondence, F(2,
probe conditions, F(2, 104) = 1.010, p < .368. 200) = 0.836, p < .435.
The reaction-time results for the visual
probe conditions (Figure 1) show a main ef-
fect, F(2, 96) = 3.833, p < .025. The slowest Visual and Factual Memory Scores
mean reaction time was in the high-corre-
spondence condition, and the fastest reaction Figure 2 plots visual and factual m e m o r y
times were in the medium-correspondence scores not as mean number correct, but as
AUDIO-VIDEOCORRESPONDENCE 2t
7O
66%
64%
VISUAL F~COG

U
~ 60

0
U 54%
50,
Z

40

---- FACTUALREGOG

30 ! !

MEDIUM NO

CORRESPONDENCE
FIGURE 2 [ ] Visual and Factual Memory Recognition Scores

percentage of answers correct. This was done the mean number correct was 19.712 (55%
so that visual and factual m e m o r y scores correct). The two conditions differed from
could be compared. each other at the .001 level of significance.
There was a main effect a m o n g factual The mean number correct for the medium-
memory scores, F(2, 185) = 87.467, p < .001. correspondence condition and the no-corre-
Mean recognition scores were 29.76 (out of 53 spondence condition (23.104, or 64% correct)
possible), or 56% correct, for the high-corre- differed at the .001 significance level. There
spondence condition, 28.44 (54% correct) for was no statistically significant difference be-
the medium-correspondence condition, and tween scores in the high- and low-correspon-
18.35 (35% correct) for the n o - c o r r e s p o n - dence conditions.
dence condition. The scores of subjects in the Pearson's p r o d u c t - m o m e n t correlations
high-correspondence and medium-corre- were performed on all reaction-time and rec-
spondence conditions did not differ signifi- ognition data. All significant relationships
cantly, p < .10, but the scores of those in both were o b t a i n e d u s i n g B o n f e r r o n i - a d j u s t e d
the high-correspondence and medium-cor- probabilities that guarantee that family-wise
respondence conditions differed from the comparisons were not larger than an alpha
scores in the no-correspondence condition at set at 0.05. All probabilities were based on
the .001 level of significance. one-tailed tests.
With r e g a r d to the visual recognition In the high-correspondence condition, a
scores, there was a main effect, F(2, 198) = positive correlation between audio reaction
49.436, p < .001. The mean number correct times and visual recognition scores obtained
for the high-correspondence condition was (r = .387, r a = .150, p < .025). That is, longer
23.851 (out of 36 possible), or 66% correct. audio reaction times were associated with
For the medium-correspondence condition, higher visual recognition scores. There were
22 ETR&D,Vol. 38, No. 3

no significant correlations in the medium- However, when the two channels exhibit
correspondence conditions. some audio-video dissonance, as repre-
There were two significant correlations in sented by the medium-correspondence con-
the no-correspondence condition. Subjects dition, attentional capacity limitations be-
exposed to tone probes exhibited a negative come manifest. Capacity limitation, in this
correlation between audio reaction times and study, is expressed as a decrement in visual
visual recognition scores (r = -.310, ra = memory. The decrement in visual memory,
.096, p < .045). There was a similar negative associated with a decline in visual attention,
correlation between reaction times and visual suggests that attention and memory are pos-
recognition scores for subjects in the visual itively correlated. In TV news stories, the
probe version of the no-correspondence con- narration usually contains the who, what,
dition (r = -.308, r 2 = .095, p < .047). when, where, and why of a story. When ca-
pacity limits were reached, the attentional
shift away from the video of a news story may
Post-Experiment Interviews have helped maintain coherency in message
reception. This may be an artifact of the
As described earlier in the Procedure section, "graceful degradation" phenomenon de-
each of the 12 experimenters who collected scribed by Norman and Bobrow (1975), the
data kept a diary and asked questions of the attempt by the human information-process-
subjects. The questions revolved around ing system to continue functioning, even if
channel correspondence, attention, and performance is degraded by a limitation in
memory: Were the visual and auditory chan- capacity.
nels easy to attend to? Did the stories make The following four dependent measures
sense? Were the probes bothersome? Were provide support for these conclusions.
the stories easy to remember? Some subjects
were more articulate than others, and some
had difficulty understanding such concepts Reaction Times
as attention and channel correspondence. To
complicate matters, self-reports on attention If years of watching television have had an
are always suspect. Thus, there is no formal effect on these subjects, perhaps it explains
analysis of the data; however, some refer- the statistically flat auditory probe reaction
ences to the data appear in the Discussion times. There was no statistically significant
section where appropriate. difference among the three conditions on this
measure. This suggests that the subjects
were monitoring the narration with consis-
DISCUSSION tency across conditions. Perhaps it was be-
cause the narration contains the who, what,
The data patterns generally support the as- when, where, and why of the story. Even in
sumptions stated earlier about the way peo- the no-correspondence condition, in which
ple watch TV news. TV news viewers seem auditory factual recognition scores were very
able to split attention between the visual and low, auditory probe reaction times were not
auditory channels more efficiently when au- statistically different from those in the other
dio-video correspondence is high than when two conditions. Indeed, all subjects in the no-
correspondence is not as high, or when there correspondence condition said, in postexper-
is no correspondence. Efficiently split atten- iment interviews, they had tried to listen to
tion, in this study, means that the highest the narration but found it either difficult or
visual and factual recognition scores were re- impossible.
corded in the high-correspondence condi- The times of reaction to visual probes sug-
tion. In other words, attention and memory gest that subjects invested more attentional
seem to be positively correlated when corre- capacity in the visual channel when it corre-
spondence is high. sponded to the narration than when it did
AUDIO-VIDEOCORRESPONDENCE '~3

not. The section below, entitled Reaction tween visual recognition scores for those two
Time/Test Score Relationships, should make conditions.
it more clear why the visual probe reaction R e m e m b e r that professional broadcast
times yielded this pattern. journalists had judged stories in the me-
d i u m - c o r r e s p o n d e n c e condition to be of
broadcast quality. Professionally at least--
and even aesthetically--these medium-cor-
Factual Memory
respondence stories were judged acceptable.
Yet they uniformly produced significantly
There was no statistically significant differ-
lower visual memory scores. This result sug-
ence between factual memory scores in the
gests that professional judgments about what
high- and medium-correspondence condi-
is acceptable audio-video correspondence
tions. This, along with the auditory-reaction
may not necessarily lead to optimal message
times for these two correspondence condi-
memory. These data suggest that there is a
tions, suggests that subjects were closely
measurable difference between what profes-
monitoring the auditory channel.
sionals believe is acceptable audio-video cor-
However, in the no-correspondence con-
respondence and what the human informa-
dition, factual recognition scores declined to
tion-processing system will tolerate.
their lowest level, but auditory probe reaction
As for the high visual recognition scores
times remained unchanged. This suggests
in the no-correspondence condition, it is nec-
that subjects were paying attention to the au-
essary to interpret them via the attention
ditory channel, but that they did not remem-
scores. In addition to the extreme audio-
ber much of the factual information con-
video conflict, which severs all semantic links
tained within.
between the two information channels, the
The recognition data for the no-corre-
shots in the no-correspondence condition
spondence condition are consistent with the
had no relationship to one another. This pro-
observations of Drew and Grimes (1987), and
moted viewing that was not systematic or co-
with similar extreme auditory-visual con-
herent, but rather viewing that was very
flicts observed by Cohen (1973), Posner et al.
much like watching a kaleidoscope of uncon-
(1976), and Colavita (1974). Apparently, if un-
nected visual stimuli. The relatively fast re-
related auditory and visual stimuli are com-
action times, then, imply that very little at-
peting for simultaneous processing, then the
tention was being applied to the visual task;
visual encoding primacy Cohen described
indeed, little was needed. Cohen (1973) has
may be particularly noticeable: Factual infor-
argued that accurate visual recognition can
mation may never get enough attentional ef-
be accomplished with very little cognitive ef-
fort to instantiate most factual messages into
fort, in other words, with an expenditure of
memory when visual and factual messages
very little attention. Thus, high recognition
are competing for entry into the human in-
scores and fast reaction times to visual
formation-processing system. Visual mes-
probes suggest that the visuals in this con-
sages do not need as much attention to be
dition were processed only to the point of
efficiently encoded, and therefore get priority
recognition. Had there been coherent visual
entry, of sorts, into the human information-
messages in this condition--as in the other
processing system.
two c o r r e s p o n d e n c e c o n d i t i o n s - - t h e n
greater attention would probably have been
required to piece the shots together in order
Visual Memory to infer a unitary visual message(s). But rec-
ognition scores, like those in the high-corre-
Even though the correspondence manipula- spondence condition, would have remained
tion between the high- and medium-corre- high. In other words, the attention data are
spondence conditions was very subtle, there useful in interpreting the visual recognition
was a statistically significant difference be- scores across conditions.
24 ETR&D,Vol.38, No. 3

Reaction Time/'l'est Score Relationships appealing. These cognitive processes, he has


argued, are inexorable and will proceed re-
High message-channel correspondence ap- gardless of message type. If he is correct,
pears associated with a more thorough pro- then the data and conclusions from this ex-
cessing of the visual channel. The high visual periment should apply to similar audiovisual
recognition scores and slow visual probe re- presentations that are formally defined as
action times in the high-correspondence con- "'educational."
dition suggest that subjects were investing It would seem unwise to apply conclu-
more attention in the visual channel com- sions inferred from this study to media pre-
pared with the other two conditions. This sentations that differ from the audiovisual
conclusion is supported by the lowest corre- paradigm tested here. For instance, presen-
lation between visual reaction times and vis- tations employing overhead or slide projec-
ual recognition scores (r = .038, r 2 = .001, p tion, and scripted or ad libbed commentary
< .425) among the various correspondence presented live, vary in significant ways. It is
and probe conditions. A scatter plot analysis impossible to calculate the effect a live pre-
shows reaction times concentrated in a nar- senter might have in further confusing, or
row band between 350 and 500 msecs. This clarifying, an audiovisual presentation.
explains why there is a weak correlation and Different formats notwithstanding, per-
a low probability level: Subjects in this con- haps the most unforgiving mandate for tight
dition were uniform in their heightened at- audio-video correspondence occurs when a
tention to the visual channel. Little variation narration excerpts a document, which is, it-
in reaction times produced a very low prod- sell excerpted visually. For instance, Reese
uct-moment correlation. (1982) has demonstrated that when docu-
Reaction times and test scores of subjects ments are summarized verbally and simulta-
in the high-correspondence, auditory probe neously shown verbatim, viewers inevitably
condition provide corroboration. In this con- read the document and ignore the verbal
dition, there was enough variation between summary. This appears to be an involuntary
reaction times and visual recognition scores response. Subjects report they "can't help"
to produce a correlation: As times of reaction but read the accompanying document. Only
to tone probes got slower, visual recognition exact duplicates on screen of what is being
scores got better (r = .387, r ~ = .150, p < said in the narration appear to lead to un-
.025). This suggests that as subjects paid impeded processing of the intended mes-
more attention to the narration, they were sage.
able to process the video better, and so they This might be extended to include any oral
produced better visual recognition scores. In presentation in which an accompanying doc-
other words, as subjects paid more attention ument departs from what is being said. Peo-
to what was said, they used their remaining ple do not look at the written word without
attentional capacity to integrate what they reading it. Reading is automatic. Using legi-
saw with what they heard. This result sug- ble documents as visual support for points
gests that when the two information chan- being made in a narrative when the narrative,
nels are c o r r e s p o n d e n t , the n a r r a t i o n not the document used as visual support, is
"drives" the processing of the visuals. the principal message, invites channel con-
flict.
This study made ample use of psycholog-
ical models, models built from highly puri-
IMPLICATIONS FOR fied stimuli. These stimuli were designed to
EDUCATIONAL MEDIA give psychologists maximum experimental
control. Although we in educational psychol-
Reeves' (1989) observations about the gener- ogy and mass communication research envy
alizability of fundamental cognitive re- that e x p e r i m e n t a l control, we are bur-
sponses to all audiovisual presentations is dened--or blessed--with much more noisy
AUDIO-VIDEOCORRESPONDENCE ~

stimuli that, at times, seem to defy control. Colavita, E B. (1974). Human sensory dominance.
They invite myriad alternative, and irksome, Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 409-412.
hypotheses. Dhawan, M., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1977). Acoustic
and semantic inference effects in words and pic-
Yet these stimuli yield information that tures. Memory & Cognition, 5, 340-346.
more simple stimulus sets might not yield. Drew, D. G., & Grimes, T. (1987). Audio-visual
For instance, note how, in the no-correspon- redundancy and TV news recall. Communication
dence condition, the visuals a p p e a r to con- Research, •4(4), 452-461.
tact the mental lexicon much more efficiently Graber, D. (1988). Processing the news: How people
tame the information tide. New York: Longman.
than words do, just as psychological models Gunter, B. (1987). Poor reception: Misunderstanding
have suggested would be the case (Cohen, and forgetting broadcast news. Hillsdale, NJ: Eft-
1973; Dhawan & Pellegrino, 1977; Pellegrino baum.
et al., 1977; Smith & Magee, 1980). But then Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. New
note how, w h e n visuals are paired with se- York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Katz, E., Adoni, H., & Parness, P. (1977). Remem-
mantically relevant words, visual facts are no bering the news: What pictures add to recall.
better r e m e m b e r e d than narrative facts, a re- Journalism Quarterly, 54, 231-239.
sult that seems to impose a caveat on visual- Neuman, R. (1976). Patterns of recall among tele-
processing models. vision news viewers. Public Opinion Quarterly, 40,
As both we and cognitive psychologists 115-121.
Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-
are discovering, the h u m a n information-pro- limited and resource-limited processes. Cogni-
cessing system apparently reshapes itself to tive Psychology, 7, 44-64.
conform to the goals i m p o s e d u p o n it, some- Pellegrino, J. W., Rosinski, R. B., Chiesi, H. L., &
thing that makes model building more diffi- Siegel, A. (1977). Picture-word differences in de-
cult. O u r noisy, hard-to-control stimuli may cision latency: An analysis of single and dual
memory models. Memory & Cognition, 5(4), 383-
actually place in high relief the versatility of 396.
the h u m a n information-processing system. Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Klein, R. M. (1976).
Further research, then, might focus not on Visual dominance: An information-processing
conforming to models of attention and mem- account of its origins and significance. Psycholog-
ory, but rather on teasing out permutations. ical Review, 83(2), 157-171.
Reese, S. D. (1982). Multi-channel redundancy effects
In that way, a mosaic of attentional and mem- on television news learning. Unpublished doctoral
ory patterns may emerge that better explain dissertation, University of Wisconsin.
how we think and learn. [] Reeves, B. (1989). Theories about news and theo-
ries about cognition: Arguments for a more rad-
ical separation. American Behavioral Scientist,
33(2), 191-198.
The author gratefully acknowledges the Robinson, J. E, & Levy, M. R. (1986). The main
theoretical and methodological contributions of source: Learning from television news. Beverly Hills,
Professors Arthur Glenberg and John Theios, CA: Sage Publications.
Department of Psychology, University of Smith, M. C., & Magee, L. E. (1980). Tracing the
Wisconsin-Madison. time course of picture-word processing. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 373-392.
Stauffer, J., Frost, R., & Rybolt, W. (1983). The
attention factor in recalling network television
REFERENCES news. Journal of Communication, 33, 29-37.
Thorson, E., Reeves, B., & Schleuder, J. (1985).
Britton, B. K., Muth, K. D., & Glynn, S. (1986). Message complexity and attention to television.
Effects of text organization on memory: Test of Communication Research, 12(4), 427-454.
a cognitive effort hypothesis with limited expo- Thorson, E., Reeves, B., & Schleuder, J. (1986).
sure time. Discourse Processes, 9, 475-487. Attention to local and global complexity in tele-
Cohen, G. (1973). How are pictures registered in vision messages. In M. L. McLaughlin (Ed.),
memory? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy- Communication Yearbook 10 (pp. 366-383). New-
chology, 25, 557-564. bury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

You might also like