Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

REVIEWS

NMDA RECEPTORS, PLACE CELLS AND


HIPPOCAMPAL SPATIAL MEMORY
Kazu Nakazawa* ‡§, Thomas J. McHugh*‡, Matthew A. Wilson‡ and Susumu Tonegawa*‡
N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) in the rodent hippocampus have been shown to be
essential for spatial learning and memory, and for the induction of long-term synaptic plasticity at
various hippocampal synapses. In this review, we examine the evidence concerning the role of
NMDARs in hippocampal memory processes, with an emphasis on the function of NMDARs in
area CA1 of the hippocampus in memory acquisition, and the unique role of NMDARs in area
CA3 in the rapid acquisition and associative retrieval of spatial information. Finally, we discuss the
data that have emerged from in vivo hippocampal recording studies that indicate that the activity
of hippocampal place cells during behaviour is an expression of a memory trace.

Scoville and Milner’s study in the 1950s of patient H.M., NMDA subset of glutamate receptors11. The NMDAR
who had undergone a bilateral resection of the hippo- possesses a voltage-dependent magnesium block,
campus and associated cortical areas, indicated that these high calcium permeability and slow activation and
parts of the brain, collectively called the medial temporal deactivation kinetics12–15. Consequently, the NMDAR
lobe (MTL), are crucial in the formation of declarative can be opened by glutamate only when the postsynaptic
memory (memory of facts and events)1. Subsequent neuron is depolarized, thereby allowing the receptor to
studies on H.M. and other patients, as well as studies on function as a detector and integrator of coincident
animal models, have enriched our knowledge about the activity at the synapse. The finding that the induction
roles of the MTL and its components in declarative of hippocampal LTP depends on the activation of
memory2–5. It has become clear that the hippocampus NMDARs further strengthened the link between LTP
has an essential role in, among other types of memory, and Hebb’s synaptic hypothesis for memory storage, in
*Howard Hughes Medical spatial memory — a type of declarative memory that which modifications of synaptic efficacy by coincident
Institute and

The Picower Center for is concerned with spatial locations6,7. In 1971, O’Keefe input was the central theme. The demonstration that
Learning and Memory, and Dostrovsky discovered ‘place cells’8 (BOX 1), showing infusion of AP5 into the ventricles of rats caused an
RIKEN-MIT Neuroscience that space can be encoded in the firing pattern of the impairment in spatial learning indicated that NMDAR
Research Center, Center for hippocampus. activation might be crucial for this type of memory16.
Cancer Research, and
Departments of Biology and
Concurrent with these studies, Bliss and Lømo Studies on the role of NMDARs in memory inten-
Brain and Cognitive discovered that high-frequency stimulation of the sified when the genes encoding the receptor were
Sciences, Massachusetts hippocampal input fibres can result in long-lasting cloned during the early 1990s17, allowing the NMDAR
Institute of Technology, enhancement of transmission efficacy at downstream subunits to be identified and characterized. Among the
Cambridge, Massachusetts synapses9. This discovery of long-term potentiation seven identified subunits (NR1, NR2A–D, and NR3A
02139, USA.
§
National Institute of (LTP) provided the first experimental support for and B), the NR1 subunit is the only one that is indis-
Mental Health, National Hebb’s theory on the neural representation of memory10. pensable for the formation of a functional receptor; its
Institutes of Health, 9000 Hippocampal LTP has subsequently been subjected elimination would abolish all functional NMDARs in a
Rockville Pike, Bethesda, to extensive study as a candidate mechanism for cell18,19. As AP5 inhibits LTP induction in hippocampal
Maryland 20892, USA.
Correspondence to S.T.
learning and memory. The induction of LTP in area slices and spatial memory in intact animals, it might
e-mail: tonegawa@mit.edu CA1 of the hippocampus is blocked by D(–)-2-amino-5- be expected that elimination of the NR1 gene would
doi:10.1038/nrn1385 phosphonovaleric acid (AP5), an antagonist of the lead to similar physiological and behavioural deficits.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 5 | MAY 2004 | 3 6 1

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

Box 1 | Place cells


In 1971, O’Keefe and Dostrovsky a
Cell 1
found that single hippocampal
neurons increased their firing rate Cell 2
whenever a rat traversed a particular Cell 3
region of a chamber 8. When they Cell 4
recorded extracellular action
potentials from the hippocampus of
freely moving rats, some hippocampal
pyramidal cells demonstrated firing
b
patterns that seemed to depend on the
animal’s location in the environment.
When the rat left the ‘place’ that was
encoded by a given cell, the cell fell
almost silent. In an open field, the
firing rate was also independent of the
direction in which the animal entered
the area and the direction that it was
facing. The firing of each cell seemed to
indicate a specific location in the
environment of the rat; so these cells
are called ‘place cells’.
Panel a represents the place-specific
firing properties of hippocampal
pyramidal cells as a rodent runs down a
linear track. Each cell fires only on a
specific region of the track. A common
method of representing these place
fields (right) is a firing rate map — a
top-down view of the environment
with areas of high firing rate coloured red and yellow and areas with no firing coloured blue. Panel b shows 80 firing rate
maps of cells simultaneously recorded from area CA1 of a rat exploring a square arena. Most cells are silent as the rat
forages, with only about 30% of the pyramidal cells active in this environment. The six cells that fire throughout the
environment are thought to be interneurons125.
Since 1971, neuroscientists have conducted hundreds of studies to characterize the properties and dependencies of
place-cell activity153–156. Studies using freely behaving rats have shown that pyramidal cells show stable, long-lasting,
environmentally specific place fields, with between 30% and 50% of the CA1 cell layer showing place-specific activity in
any given environment125,157,158. When an animal is introduced to a novel environment, these place cells form rapidly,
usually within five minutes, and are maintained robustly125. The relative locations of these place-receptive fields change
in different environments, with no apparent topographical relationship to cell position, so new place fields must be
learned in each environment159.
More recent studies have shown that any given place is encoded not by the activity of single neurons, but instead by a
population of simultaneously active cells125. Parameters such as the coefficient of variance, a measure of temporal
correlation, allow the spatial coding properties of an ensemble of cells to be assessed. For example, both pairs and sequences
of cells that were active during behaviour, owing to close proximity of their place fields, tend to be reactivated in a
coordinated manner during periods of slow-wave and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep following behaviour124,127,128,132,133.
This increase in correlated activity during sleep might reflect cellular and network learning mechanisms.
Place cells have been best characterized in area CA1 of the rat hippocampus, but they have also been studied in the
other hippocampal subfields and in the entorhinal cortex74,75. The recording and analysis of fields in the genetically
modified mouse has also been applied to understanding the hippocampal code for space. Panel b reproduced, with
permission, from REF. 125 © (1993) American Association for the Advancement of Science.

However, NR1-knockout mice do not survive for more hippocampus can be divided into the dentate gyrus
than a day after birth because of the role of this receptor (DG), area CA3 and area CA1 (FIG. 1). In the main excita-
in the midbrain for breathing20,21. This illustrates the tory pathway, the trisynaptic circuit, information flows
need for spatial and temporal restriction of genetic from layer II stellate cells of the entorhinal cortex (EC) to
interventions for the effective study of memory and granule cells in the DG though the perforant path, from
other cognitive phenomena. the granule cells to the CA3 pyramidal cells through the
The role of the NMDAR in spatial representation and mossy fibres, from the CA3 pyramidal cells to the CA1
spatial memory in the hippocampus must be considered pyramidal cells though the Schaffer collaterals, and finally
and studied in the context of hippocampal anatomy. from the CA1 pyramidal cells to cells in the deep layers
As in humans and non-human primates, the rodent of the EC. In addition, layer II stellate cells and layer III

362 | MAY 2004 | VOLUME 5 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

Fornix
(the hidden-platform version of the Morris water maze
(MWM) task 6,30). Initially, rats that are placed in the pool
find the escape platform by random navigation. How-
Sub ever, over repeated training trials, they slowly learn
the location using objects outside the maze as cues and
eventually swim nearly straight to the platform. Blockade
of NMDARs by intraventricular infusion of AP5 resulted
in an impairment of spatial learning on the hidden-
platform MWM task, but not if the platform was
CA1 5/6 visible16. The estimated extracellular concentrations of
3
PHG AP5 in the hippocampus that caused the spatial learning
2 Neocortex
and impairment were comparable to those that blocked LTP
PR
EC in vivo at synapses between the perforant path and
the DG31. Intrahippocampal injection of AP5 resulted in
similar behavioural deficits32. By contrast, intrahippo-
DG campal administration of an AMPA (α-amino-3-
hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid)/kainate
CA3 receptor antagonist, 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-
Figure 1 | Connections in the corticohippocampal network. Sensory information from the dione (CNQX) or LY326325, resulted in a severe memory
cerebral association neocortex (neocortex) arrives at the superficial layers of the entorhinal deficit with either the hidden or the visible platform33.
cortex (EC) by way of the parahippocampal gyrus (PHG) and/or perirhinal cortex (PR). From the These results indicated that hippocampal NMDAR
EC, the information is routed into the hippocampus. The main subfields of the hippocampus and activity and NMDAR-dependent plasticity are crucial
the connections among them and with the superficial layers of the EC are described in the text. for spatial learning. However, NMDAR antagonists
CA1 sends axons to both subcortical areas and the deep layers of the EC, either directly or
could have nonspecific effects on neural activity. For
through the subiculum (Sub). The corticohippocampal network is then completed, with the
information from CA1 being sent back to cerebral association cortex through the PHG and/or
instance, AP5 can cause a reduction in hippocampal
PR. Thick lines below or to the right of cell bodies (triangles) represent dendrites. Numbers refer excitability34,35. Furthermore, intraventricular infusion
to cortical layers. DG, dentate gyrus. of AP5 often results in diffusion of the drug to areas
outside the hippocampus, which could impair both
sensory and motor function36,37. The possibility that
pyramidal cells of the EC send axons directly to area CA3 unintended effects of AP5 contributed to the observed
(through the perforant path) and area CA1 (through the learning deficits could not be excluded.
temporoammonic pathway), respectively 22. Furthermore, A complementary approach was provided by geneti-
CA3 pyramidal cells are interconnected by recurrent cally engineered mice, in which deletion of the NR1
collaterals that run both ipsilaterally and contralaterally. gene was restricted to particular cell types using the phage
Among the three types of excitatory input that CA3 pyra- P1-derived, Cre/loxP recombination system38,39 (BOX 2).
midal cells receive, these recurrent collateral inputs are the Tsien et al. created a mouse strain in which the NR1 gene
most numerous (about 12,000 per pyramidal cell in was postnatally knocked out predominantly in CA1
the rat), whereas the perforant path and mossy fibre pyramidal cells (referred to as CA1-NR1-knockout
inputs provide about 4,000 and 50 inputs per cell, respec- mice)40. This mouse strain had apparently normal
tively. Owing to the recurrent collaterals, the connectivity growth and was fertile.
among CA3 pyramidal cells is robust; a given cell is CA1-NR1-knockout mice showed severely impaired
directly connected with at least 2% of the other cells23,24. spatial learning in the hidden-platform MWM, but
A number of theoretical studies have proposed a performed normally when the platform was visible40. A
distinct mnemonic role for each of the hippocampal possible explanation for this deficit came from in vivo
subfields and inputs25–28. For instance, it has been postu- recordings of CA1 pyramidal cells41. Place cells that were
lated that the recurrent network in CA3 is crucial for the recorded while CA1-NR1-knockout mice traversed a lin-
storage of associative memory and its recall by ‘pattern ear track had altered single-cell and ensemble properties.
completion’, whereas the DG is involved in the separa- The individual fields were larger and less structured than
tion of similar memories (‘pattern separation’). It has normal, and the coefficient of variance — a measure of
also been suggested that the CA1 network might be the temporal coordination of the firing of cells with over-
instrumental in recognizing the novelty or familiarity of lapping place fields — was greatly reduced, indicating a
an object or context29. However, it is only recently that loss of coherent spatial representation at the ensemble
some of these hypotheses have been tested empirically. level. Furthermore, the in vitro induction of LTP was
specifically blocked at the Schaffer collateral (SC)–CA1
Memory acquisition synapses. In these mice, NR1 deletion is delayed
Early support for the link among NMDAR activity, until about 4 weeks after birth and is restricted to CA1
hippocampal LTP and learning and memory came from pyramidal cells until about 2 months of age42. So, it
Morris and colleagues. They developed a hippocampus- is unlikely that the behavioural impairment is the result
dependent behavioural task for rats using a circular of undetected developmental abnormalities, and
pool filled with opaque water in which an escape although a small percentage of the animals that were used
platform was hidden at a fixed location below the surface for behavioural and physiological characterization might

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 5 | MAY 2004 | 3 6 3

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

Box 2 | Conditional genetic manipulations


Conditional genetic techniques have been used in rodents a
to confine the manipulation of a gene to a particular tissue
or cell type and/or a desired time point, allowing spatial PαCamKll Cre loxP loxP
and temporal control of a gene of interest in vivo. The
Cre/loxP system is the most widely used technique for NR1 exons
Cre
manipulating the mouse genome160,161. Cre is a site-specific
Mouse A Mouse B
DNA recombinase derived from the P1 bacteriophage that floxed NR1
CA1-specific Cre
recognizes 34 base-pair sequences termed loxP sites. Cre
catalyses the deletion of a segment of DNA that is flanked CA1 pyramidal cells All other cells (Cre absent)
(Cre present)
by a pair of these loxP sites (floxed), resulting in a
‘knockout’ of the gene of interest (a). To create mice in
which a gene knockout is restricted to a brain subregion,
two transgenic mouse lines are created and intercrossed:
one line is composed of mice in which a pair of loxP sites
are introduced by homologous recombination into the
b
gene to be knocked out and the other line comprises
transgenic mice in which the expression of the Cre
recombinase is driven by a tissue-specific or cell-type-
specific transcriptional promoter (a). When these are bred floxed NR1 floxed NR1/Cre
together, the restricted expression of the recombinase c
PαCamKll tTA tetO Pmin NR1
leads to a specific deletion of the floxed gene only in the
tissue of interest. The use of promoters that are active only
in the adult minimizes the developmental and tTA
compensatory effects that are often seen in conventional Mouse A Mouse B
knockout mice162–164. Panel b shows in situ hybridization Forebrain-specific tTA tetO-NR1
using a probe that is specific for the NR1 transcript. In the Forebrain cells (tTA present) All other cells (tTA absent)
absence of the recombinase, the transcript is expressed
throughout the brain (left); however, Cre expression leads
to a CA1-specific deletion of the gene (arrow, right). + Doxycycline ( )
To endow temporal specificity to a genetic alteration,
there are various techniques for creating mice with
inducible expression of genes165. In the nervous system, the
most widely used inducible approach is the tetracycline-dependent regulatory system166. The tetracycline transactivator
(tTA), a fusion of the tet repressor protein of Escherichia coli and the carboxy (C)-terminal domain of the herpes simplex
virus VP16 protein, is used as a transcriptional ON/OFF switch to drive the expression of a gene of interest109. Like the
Cre/loxP system, the tTA system requires the use of two lines of transgenic mice — mice in which the expression of the
tTA protein is driven by a tissue or cell-type-specific promoter, and mice in which a gene of interest is placed downstream
of a tandem array of the tet operator (tetO), along with a minimal promoter (c). In the original and most frequently used
version of this system, the tTA protein binds to the tetO sequence and induces transcription. However, when the inducer
FEAR CONDITIONING
(tetracycline) is present, it binds to tTA and prevents it from binding to tetO, which halts transcription. Many
A form of Pavlovian (classical) laboratories have now reported the successful application of the tTA system to neuroscience, observing that removal of
conditioning in which the tetracycline from the animals’ food or water leads to robust expression of the exogenous transgenic protein in the
animal learns that an innocuous brain167–169. The application and combination of spatial restriction by the Cre/loxP system and of temporal control by the
stimulus (for example, an tTA system has allowed the generation of more precise reagents for the study of systems neuroscience.
auditory tone — the
conditioned stimulus or CS),
reliably predicts the occurrence
of a noxious stimulus (for have harboured a more widespread deletion of the NR1 conditioning and TRANSVERSE PATTERN LEARNING43–45. However,
example, foot shock — the gene, most of the analysis was done with young mice in we focus on spatial memory in this review, to compare
unconditioned stimulus or US) which the knockout was CA1-specific. In addition, the and contrast the molecular genetic, behavioural, pharma-
following their repeated paired
presentation. As a result of this
lack of NR1 deletion in the neocortex alleviated the cological and electrophysiological data that support our
procedure, presentation of the main concern that accompanied the AP5 administra- evaluation of NMDARs in hippocampal function.
CS alone elicits conditioned fear tion experiments. The combination of pharmacological
responses previously associated and genetic studies has therefore provided strong evi- Pretraining spares spatial learning. Despite the evidence
with the noxious stimulus only.
dence that NMDAR activity and NMDAR-dependent outlined above, some subsequent studies called into
TRANSVERSE PATTERN LEARNING
synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus are crucial question the role of NMDAR-dependent plasticity in
A task in which animals must for spatial learning and for the proper formation and spatial learning. Most strikingly, NMDAR antagonists
encode overlapping coordination of CA1 place fields. The importance of failed to disrupt new spatial learning in the hidden-
relationships between cues. A CA1 NMDARs in the acquisition of hippocampus- platform MWM task in rats that had received spatial46
typical stimulus set is A+B–;
B+C–; C+A–, where + signifies
dependent memory was subsequently extended to or non-spatial47 pretraining in a different environment.
which cue is rewarded in each various non-spatial tasks, including recognition of The hippocampus was still needed for new spatial learn-
configuration. novel objects, contextual FEAR CONDITIONING, trace fear ing by the pretrained animals46. Pretraining also protects

364 | MAY 2004 | VOLUME 5 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

a 5 min 5 min 5 min Spatial delayed matching-to-place task. One clue came
T1 T2 T3 T4 from the effects of AP5 in the delayed matching-to-
place (DMP) version of the MWM50 (FIG. 2). In this task,
the location of the platform was altered daily in a
Day 1 pseudo-random fashion. On each day, four trials were
given with intertrial intervals of 20 min or 2 h. The
mean latencies to find the platform are relatively long
for the first trial of a day, because the animals are
unaware of the novel location of the platform, but for
untreated rats they are significantly shortened in the
second trial owing to the memory that was acquired
Day 2
earlier that day. However, infusion of AP5 into the
hippocampus substantially reduced the latency savings
on the second trial. Pretraining over 9 days with a novel
platform location each day did not alter the NMDAR-
dependency of this one-trial spatial learning. This is
in contrast to the results of the MWM task in which
pretraining absolved the need for NMDAR function in
Day n new spatial learning. What features of the two water
maze tasks resulted in this difference?
In the DMP task, the animals have presumably
familiarized themselves with both the task rules and the
b Day 1–4 Day 5–8 Day 13–16 environment by the end of the pretraining session.
100 100 100 To reduce the latency on the second trial of a test day,
80 the animal must depend on information about the
Escape latency (s)

80
Escape latency (s)
Escape latency (s)

80
platform location that it obtained during the first trial
60 60 60
of the day. The animals must distinguish the most
40 40 40 recent (today’s) platform location from those experi-
enced in the preceding days. These requirements are
20 20 20
reminiscent of those for human episodic memory51,52.
0 0 0
Although it has been forcefully argued by some that
1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 episodic memory is beyond the capability of non-
Trials Trials Trials
human animals53, recent studies on food-caching and
Figure 2 | CA3-NR1-knockout mice are impaired in delayed matching-to-place task in recovery in scrub jays54 indicate that animals can mem-
water maze. a | Animals are given four trials (T1–T4) per day with a 5-min intertrial interval. During
a given day the hidden platform remains at a fixed location, but between days, the platform is
orize ‘what’, ‘where’ and ‘when’ information, a hallmark
moved to novel locations. b | Performance is averaged across 16-trial blocks during training (day of episodic memory55. Hippocampal NMDARs might
1–4, 5–8, 9–12 and 13–16). The escape latency is relatively long on trial 1 each day because the be needed for this episodic aspect of spatial learning56.
animal will have to conduct a random search for the novel platform location, but it is reduced In contrast to the DMP task, which demands rapid
gradually on trials 2, 3 and 4. Up to and including day 12, there were no differences in the escape acquisition of the memory for the novel platform
latencies between CA3-NR1-knockout mice and their control littermates, indicating that the location in one trial, the hidden-platform MWM
mutants are not impaired in the ‘rule learning’ phase of the task. On days 13–16, however, the
task allows slow acquisition of the memory of the
mutants exhibited latency deficits in trials 2–4. These results indicate that the mutants are
impaired in the rapid encoding of novel spatial information69. fixed platform location, and its performance can be
improved incrementally throughout multiple trials.
During the early stages of training, animals probably
learning on a STEP-DOWN INHIBITORY AVOIDANCE TASK in rats rely on single-trial memory, sometimes referred to as
that have received an intrahippocampal infusion of AP5 episodic information, for the performance improve-
(REF. 48). The NMDAR dispensability that is induced by ment as in the DMP task, but as the training advances
pretraining seems to reflect the complexity of the learning they might gradually form more trial-independent
associated with these behavioural tasks. In the hidden- spatial memory. We suggest that hippocampal NMDARs
platform MWM task, animals must learn various behav- are required in the early, trial-dependent phase of the
ioural strategies and rules, including swimming away task, but not in the later, trial-independent phase. When
from the side walls to find a platform, discovering the animals undergo extensive pretraining on this task,
hidden platform, and climbing and staying on the plat- even in a distinct environment, it might minimize
form, before they can learn the location of the platform the trial-dependent phase of new spatial learning
STEP-DOWN INHIBITORY
AVOIDANCE TASK
allocentrically using the extra maze cues. One possible and thereby render it insensitive to AP5 blockade in
A form of conditioning in which explanation for the lack of NMDAR-dependency in the hippocampus. A similar idea has been suggested
a rat is placed on a platform and pretrained animals is that the strategy or rule learning, by Moser and Moser57. The idea could be tested by
receives a shock when it steps off but not the spatial learning per se, requires NMDAR varying the timing of AP5 injection during training,
the platform. Memory for the
shock is measured as an
function. However, subsequent work did not support or by generating and analysing mice in which
increased latency to step off the this hypothesis49. So why does the MWM task depend on hippocampus-targeted inhibition of NR1 function is
platform on subsequent trials. hippocampal NMDARs? temporally controlled.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 5 | MAY 2004 | 3 6 5

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

Day 1 Day 2 crucial for the rapid acquisition of one-trial memory


of novel spatial information69. Spatial tuning of CA1
place cells in mutant mice was normal in a familiar
VI VI environment but was significantly impaired (with
enlarged place fields and an augmented integrated
CA1 V CA1 V
firing rate) in a novel environment69. Strikingly,
CA3 IV CA3 IV when the mutants were re-exposed to the ‘novel’
III III environment after being held in a home cage for 1 day,
DG DG spatial tuning was normal. So, the tuning deficit is seen
II II
EC EC
only during a defined period (at least 1 hour) after
exposure to a novel environment, and some consolida-
Figure 3 | Impaired rapid formation of CA1 place fields as a consequence of CA3 NMDAR tion process occurs in the absence of the continuous
knockout. On the first day of exploration in a novel environment, the spatial tuning of CA1 place environmental information, resulting in normal
cells is compromised (that is, the place fields are enlarged) in CA3-NR1-knockout mice. It is
tuning by the next day.
proposed that these CA1 responses reflect equally poorly tuned entorhinal cortex (EC) place-cell
activities. In normal animals, the poor spatial tuning will be quickly remedied within a few minutes It has been suggested that entorhinal neurons
by the highly tuned drive from CA3 (not shown). But in the mutants, owing to the lack of NMDARs provide an important input to CA1, particularly during
(N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors), CA3 cells fail to provide such a drive and therefore the spatially tasks that require the encoding of novel informa-
poorly tuned CA1 responses continue for at least 1 hour. However, by the time the mutants are tion70–72. FIGURE 3 shows a model for the role of
re-exposed to the same environment on the second day, a slow spatial refinement of CA1 place- NMDARs in CA3 in the rapid establishment of highly
cell activities has been implemented off-line through an unknown mechanism. DG, dentate gyrus.
tuned CA1 place cells when animals are exposed to
novel spatial information. During exposure to a novel
context, the CA1 response is initially and transiently
CA3 NMDARs and one-trial learning. The CA3 subfield driven by direct input from the EC, which is spatially
of the hippocampus has a robust recurrent network, broadly tuned73–75. In control animals, NMDAR activity
with pyramidal cells receiving synaptic contacts from in CA3, perhaps operating through Hebbian recurrent
~2% of other CA3 pyramidal cells58,59 (FIG. 1). NMDAR- connections, allows the rapid formation of CA1 place
dependent LTP has also been demonstrated at the cells as the input from CA3 comes to dominate and
synapses of recurrent collaterals in CA3 as well as at shape the EC input. This shift of dominant input
perforant path–CA3 synapses, whereas the plasticity from EC to CA3 occurs so rapidly that it is difficult
at dentate mossy fibre–CA3 synapses is NMDAR- to observe the initial, less tuned state of CA1 place
independent60–64. Marr and others have suggested that cells. By contrast, in CA3-NR1-knockout mice, as
recurrent networks with modifiable synaptic strength a consequence of CA3 NMDAR ablation, the less
could support the rapid acquisition of memories of a spatially-tuned EC place cells continue to drive CA1
one-time experience28,65,66. Although Steele and Morris’s place-cell activity for at least one hour after exposure to
pharmacological blockade experiment indicated a novel space. Subsequently, however, by the time the
that hippocampal NMDARs are involved in delay- mutants are re-exposed to the same context 1 day later,
dependent rapid acquisition of one-time experience a slow spatial refinement of CA1 place fields seems to
memory, a more targeted intervention method would have been implemented off-line. It is not known which
be needed to identify specific hippocampal subfields hippocampal circuitries and synapses are involved in
and circuitries in which NMDARs play this crucial part. this slow refinement process. However, knife cuts of
Lee and Kesner67 attempted to inject AP5 bilaterally, the connections between CA3 and CA1 do not block
primarily into CA3, CA1 or the DG in rats that had been reactivation of CA1 place cells on re-exposure to
trained on a radial eight-arm maze using a delayed a familiar environment72. This suggests that the
non-matching-to-place experiment, and tested whether temporoammonic pathway and its synaptic plasticity
the rats chose the previously unvisited arm to obtain a might be sufficient for this process76. Although CA3
reward using spatial cues. Rats that had been treated NMDARs seem to be crucial for the rapid formation of
with AP5 directed towards CA3 were impaired at carry- highly tuned CA1 place cells, their ablation does not
ing out this task in a novel environment, consistent with seem to block encoding of the novel information
the hypothesis that NMDARs in CA3 are more impor- entirely. If it did, it would be difficult to explain the
tant than those in CA1 or DG for the acquisition of presence of highly tuned place cells on re-exposure on
spatial ‘working memory’ (equivalent to episodic-like day 2 (REF. 69).
PAIRED-ASSOCIATE TASK memory) in a novel environment. Overall, the combined behavioural and physiological
A task that involves the arbitrary Nakazawa et al. generated a mouse strain (CA3-NR1- analyses of the CA3-NR1-knockout mice and the
association of two stimuli (such
knockout mice) in which the NR1 gene deletion was behavioural analysis of AP5-treated rats indicate that
as word pairs in humans, or a
place and an odour or food item restricted to the CA3 pyramidal cells of adult mice68. CA3 NMDARs are crucial for rapid hippocampal
in animals). After exposure to These mutant mice were impaired in the DMP version encoding of novel information, which is necessary for
the pair, the subject is presented of the MWM task that was conducted with a 5-min rapid learning of one-time experience. A recent study on
with one stimulus and tested for intertrial interval. By contrast, mutant mice performed a new PAIRED-ASSOCIATE TASK is consistent with this conclu-
recall of the second. It can be
used to test declarative memory
normally when previously experienced platform loca- sion, reporting that the encoding of a ‘what–where’
in humans, or ‘episodic-like’ tions were used and in the standard version of the association between a specific odour and a location
memory in animals. MWM. These results showed that CA3 NMDARs are depends on hippocampal NMDARs77.

366 | MAY 2004 | VOLUME 5 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

Memory consolidation In all of these studies, both the demands of the task and
It is commonly thought that input from the external the drug administration methods varied greatly and it is
world is initially encoded as nascent neural firing patterns not easy to compare the results and produce a coherent
and is later stored in a more persistent form. Ebbinghaus78 interpretation.
first mentioned this phenomenon as the ‘overlearning In studies that seemed to show a function for
effect’, and Müller and Pilzecker79 proposed this as a NMDARs in memory consolidation, no attempt was
‘consolidation theory’, an idea later refined by Hebb10. made to restrict NMDAR blockade to a particular brain
Memory is labile for a short time after its acquisition, and system. Therefore, it is impossible to decide whether the
treatments such as protein synthesis inhibition, electro- effects were exerted in the neocortex or hippocampus.
convulsive shock or hypothermia can lead to memory Shimizu et al.108 attempted to address this issue by com-
loss80–82. Two main mechanisms have been proposed for bining the tTA (tetracycline-dependent transactivator
‘memory consolidation’. One,‘trace-transfer consolida- protein)109 and Cre/loxP techniques to develop a mouse
tion’, emphasizes the transfer of a memory trace over line in which NMDAR ablation was both regionally and
time, for instance, from the hippocampus to the cortex1. temporally controlled. Treatment with doxycycline 1–7
This between-structure transfer is sometimes referred to days after training caused inhibition of NR1 expression in
as ‘systems consolidation’65,66,83–86. The second — cellular hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells and an impairment in
consolidation — focuses more on the molecular events in memory. Delaying NR1 inhibition to 9–14 days after
cells in a given brain region. It has been suggested that the training prevented the memory impairment. The authors
initially fragile memory trace is made more permanent concluded that the reactivation of NMDARs in the
through biochemical and morphological synaptic and hippocampus (that is, in CA1) is necessary for cellular
cellular changes that mark the transition from short-term consolidation108. However, the restriction of NMDAR
to long-term forms of plasticity87–90. System consolidation inhibition to CA1 in this mouse strain was not clearly
and cellular consolidation are not mutually exclusive. demonstrated in their study.A recent analysis of the CA1-
Rather, it is likely that cellular consolidation occurs NR1-knockout mice that were used in this study showed
in both pre-transfer (for example, hippocampus) and that the absence of the NR1 subunit spread to the
post-transfer (for example, neocortex) brain systems82. neocortex after 2 months of age42, so it is premature to
Briefly, cellular consolidation is initiated by the acti- conclude that reactivation of hippocampal NMDARs is
vation of NMDA, AMPA and metabotropic glutamate needed during memory consolidation.
receptors, and involves changes in the levels of second Some studies have hinted at the involvement of
messengers, followed by enhanced activity of protein NMDARs in cellular memory consolidation, but further
kinases such as PKA, PKC, PKG and the calmodulin investigation is needed to establish the role of hippo-
(CaM) kinases, as well as protein phosphatases such as campal NMDAR function in consolidation. It should
PP1, PP2A and PP2B. These molecular events lead to also be noted that the hidden-platform MWM is not an
de novo transcription through the activation of constitu- ideal paradigm for dissociating acquisition and consoli-
tive and inducible transcriptional factors such as the dation, because multiple training trials lasting several
cyclic-AMP-responsive element-binding (CREB) family days will inevitably make the two phases overlap. A para-
of proteins91–100. Additional regulation can also occur at digm such as contextual fear conditioning, in which one
the level of mRNA transport and translation101–103. to a few training trials are sufficient for acquisition,
would be a method of choice.
Post-training infusion of NMDAR antagonist. To exam-
ine the role of NMDARs in memory consolidation, Memory retrieval
Morris et al. infused AP5 into the ventricles of rats that In the aforementioned studies on the role of NMDARs
had been trained on the hidden-platform MWM task. A in cellular consolidation, the effect of an intervention
dose that impaired learning when administered before treatment on consolidation as opposed to retrieval was
training had no effect when administered 1 day after distinguished by the duration between the time of the
training32,36. These data support the notion that completion of the training session and the time of
NMDARs are needed for memory acquisition but not for the treatment. This requires us to assume that a memory
consolidation. Further support for this idea came from test conducted soon after the completion of training
experiments in which AP5 and MK801 were adminis- measures consolidation and/or retrieval, but a test con-
tered intra-ventricularly and systemically, respectively, ducted long after training tests only retrieval. Based on
4 days after the last training day without effect104, and this assumption, there is accumulating evidence that the
from a study in which post-training administration of ablation of hippocampal NMDAR function has little
MK801 had no effect on a probe trial 24 h later105. On effect on memory retrieval. Early evidence for this came
the other hand, Packard and Teather106,107 reported that from a hippocampus-dependent olfactory discrimina-
injection of AP5 or MK801 into rat hippocampus imme- tion task110 and a spatial reference memory task32,36. Later,
diately after training on the MWM task impaired the rats’ as mentioned above, similar results were obtained by
memory when they were tested 24 h later. These authors pharmacological106 and genetic intervention108. Other
defined the NMDAR-dependent time window to be less behavioural tasks have also shown a role for hippocampal
than 2 h after completion of the training; however they NMDARs in encoding but not in retrieval. These include
used an unusual training protocol that consisted of inhibitory avoidance111–114 with the memory tests con-
a single session composed of eight continuous trials. ducted immediately after or 24 h after the end of training.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 5 | MAY 2004 | 3 6 7

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

a Full cue DG/EC input b Full cue DG/EC input In the hidden-platform MWM task, the training
EC input EC input (memory acquisition) and probe (memory retrieval)
trials are usually carried out in the same environment16,40.
SC SC Although this protocol provided basic insights into the
mechanisms of memory formation, it is not optimal for
CA1 CA3 CA1 CA3 investigating the mechanism that underlies memory
recall by pattern completion. Nakazawa et al. trained
Firing rate

Firing rate
CA3-NR1-knockout mice in the hidden-platform MWM
Position RC Position RC task under full-cue conditions and then subjected them
Control Mutant to probe trials either under the same full-cue conditions
c Partial cue DG/EC input d Partial cue DG/EC input or under conditions in which some of the cues were
EC input EC input removed (degraded-cue conditions)68. Although the
mutant mice acquired the spatial reference memory and
SC SC retrieved it normally under the full-cue conditions,
they were impaired at retrieving the memory under
CA1 CA3 CA1 CA3 degraded-cue conditions, unlike control mice. These data
indicated that CA3 NMDARs, in contrast to CA1
Firing rate

Firing rate

NMDARs41, were not necessary for the acquisition of


spatial reference memory that could be retrieved under
Position RC Position RC
Control Mutant
full-cue conditions, but that CA3 NMDARs were impor-
Figure 4 | Spatial pattern completion model in hippocampal networks. In control animals,
tant for recalling the spatial reference memory when only
during memory acquisition under full-cue conditions, both recurrent collateral (RC) synapses within a fraction of the cues were available. Recordings from
CA3 and Schaffer collateral (SC) synapses in CA1 are modified in an NMDAR (N-methyl-D-aspartate place cells in area CA1, downstream of CA3, corroborated
receptor)-dependent manner, leading to storage of the memory trace (small red dots) in CA3 and these behavioural data; when CA3-NR1-knockout mice
CA1, respectively (a and c). In CA3-NR1-knockout mice this will happen in CA1 but not in CA3 were allowed to explore a full-cue environment repeat-
(b and d). During the recall phase, if the full set of cues is presented, CA1 memory traces will be fully edly, place cells with normal spatial tuning properties
activated in both control and mutant animals, leading to full recall (a and b). If only a partial set of
were formed and they were fully reactivated when the
cues is provided to control animals, it will initially activate only partial CA3 memory traces, but
because of the robust connections among CA3 cells, activation of the entire CA3 memory traces will animals were returned to the environment after several
be accomplished subsequently by iteration of activity within the recurrent CA3 network. This will, in hours in a home cage. By contrast, when the mutants
turn, activate the entire CA1 memory trace (c). In mutants, because of the lack of memory traces in were returned to an environment in which only
the recurrent CA3 network, which prevents iteration-mediated trace activation, CA1 memory traces a fraction of the original cues were available, the place-
will not be fully activated (d). So, mutants exhibit impaired recall under partial-cue conditions. Vertical cell reactivation was severely impaired. These behavioural
arrows designate inputs from recall cues. Red dots with circles indicate reactivated memory trace
and physiological data, along with the finding that
components68. DG, dentate gyrus; EC, entorhinal cortex. Modified, with permission, from REF. 68 
(2002) American Association for the Advancement of Science. CA1-NR1-knockout mice showed a severe deficit in the
acquisition of the hidden-platform MWM task40
and uncoordinated, spatially less-tuned CA1 place-cell
A likely reason for the dispensability of NMDARs activity41, led Nakazawa et al. to conclude that CA1
in retrieval comes from evidence that blockade of NMDARs are essential for the acquisition of spatial
hippocampal CA1 NMDARs has little effect on reference memory but that CA3 NMDARs are not.
AMPAR-mediated fast synaptic transmission11. So, even Instead, CA3 NMDARs are crucial for the retrieval of the
if NMDAR blockade could completely freeze the memory under conditions in which pattern completion
existing pattern of synaptic weights in the hippo- is required.According to this proposal, the primary deficit
campus, the cells would be able to fire and to faithfully in CA3-NR1-knockout mice was in the formation
transmit the established network pattern using of NMDAR-dependent ensemble memory traces in
AMPA/kainate receptors115,116. CA3. This manifested as a deficit in recall by pattern
completion (FIG. 4).
CA3 NMDARs for associative memory recall. Marr
suggested that the recall process could occur either in one NMDAR function and place cells
step — ‘simple recall’ — or in a series of steps involving Since O’Keefe and Nadel123 first proposed that the hippo-
recurrent network activation — a ‘collateral effect’28. campus is a neural substrate of a ‘cognitive map’ and
Personal experiences in daily life are unique and rarely described the mnemonic aspects of place-cell activity,
repeat exactly28,117. So, input patterns that change from tremendous effort has been made to determine whether
moment to moment would be able to reactivate only part place-cell activity is directly related to the formation and
of a stored memory and would be unable to activate the expression of spatial memory traces. BOX 3 lists the condi-
whole pattern without repeated iterations of firing in the tions that must be met by any physiological process to be
recurrent network. This concept is called ‘pattern comple- called a memory trace. Many studies have provided
tion’28. The presence of massive recurrent collaterals evidence that the pattern of place-cell activities can
with highly modifiable synapses made area CA3 an be viewed as an expression of a memory trace at the
attractive candidate site for the biological implementation neuronal ensemble level123–130. Most convincingly, place
of this process, which is required for the retrieval of cells show spatially characteristic stable firing patterns
hippocampus-dependent associative memory 26,118–122. across temporally separated recording sessions — a

368 | MAY 2004 | VOLUME 5 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

Box 3 | Necessary attributes of memory traces CA1-NR1-knockout mice in familiar environments41.


One possible explanation for the differences in the spatial
Experience dependency (criterion 1) tuning properties of place cells in pharmacologically
The memory trace should form in an experience-dependent manner. treated and genetically engineered rodents is that the
Information specificity (criterion 2) genetic interventions (even if they occur only postnatally)
The trace should be specific to the information that is acquired. are not as acute as the pharmacological ones. So, the NR1
deletion might have led to an accumulation of cellular
Persistency (criterion 3)
The trace must outlast the period during which the animal is exposed to the information. and/or system level changes, and these might have con-
tributed to the phenotype. Conversely, the genetic lesion
Ability to be reactivated (criterion 4) was more targeted and more complete, so it might have
The subsequent presentation of at least part of the original cues should reactivate the trace. revealed more specific deficits. For instance, the establish-
ment of place fields could require a balance of excitation
unique set of place cells is reactivated on subsequent and inhibition on both the cellular and circuit levels.
exposure to the same environment, and these patterns Systemic administration of CPP is expected to inhibit
can be subsequently reactivated independently of behav- NMDARs on both excitatory and inhibitory cells
iour124,127,128,131–133. In addition, the entire set of place cells involved in place-field formation, possibly in a coordi-
can be reactivated by a subset of the original set of nated fashion, leading to the apparently normal pheno-
cues68,134–137. These features of place cells satisfy criteria 2, 3 type. By contrast, the genetic NR1 blockade was restricted
and 4 in BOX 3. However, it has been difficult to show to the excitatory neurons, and so might have led to the
directly that place-cell formation is experience dependent impairment. The future application of cell-type-specific,
(criterion 1). It has been shown that in a novel space the rapidly inducible genetic manipulation could address
ensemble code, which is initially less robust, improves these issues and clarify the matter.
rapidly with exploration; however, this change is so rapid Although the study of Kentros et al.140 lacks regional
that improvements in the coding of individual cells have specificity, it does have the advantage of precise temporal
been impossible to monitor125. Although Mehta et al. control and therefore has shed light on the relationship
have reported lap-dependent modifications of preexisting between NMDARs and the stability of CA1 place fields.
place fields138,139, the initial changes in place-field size that Place fields that formed de novo in the presence of CPP
might represent the acquisition of novel spatial informa- were unstable, but CPP had no effect on the maintenance
tion have not been fully characterized. The ablation of of previously established place fields140. These results are
NR1 in area CA3 slows down the de novo formation of consistent with behavioural evidence that hippocampal
CA1 place fields69.Although the kinetics and mechanisms NMDARs are required for the acquisition and perhaps
of the maturation of these place fields have not been fully the initial consolidation of hippocampus-dependent
characterized, the enlarged place fields that were observed memory, but not for its maintenance. Other studies with
during the first hour of exposure became condensed to transgenic mice in which signalling and transcriptional
normal sizes in the home cage before reexposure to the molecules downstream of the NMDAR were altered also
same environment the next day. If these enlarged place showed similar effects on the stability of place fields142–145.
fields represent an early, transient state of place-cell for- However, one should apply caution in interpreting these
mation, this finding supports the experience dependency data because it is technically difficult to maintain stability
of place-cell formation, and therefore the role of these in the electrophysiological isolation and identification of
cells in a memory trace. cells recorded across days in these long-term recording
Other groups have used pharmacology to address the experiments.
role of the NMDAR in the hippocampal encoding of
space. Kentros et al. examined the differential effects Conclusions
of systemic administration of the NMDAR antagonist We have summarized recent advances in our under-
CPP (3-(2-carboxypiperazin-4-yl)propyl-1-phosphonic standing of the roles of NMDARs in the acquisition,
acid) on the formation and maintenance of CA1 place consolidation and recall of hippocampus-dependent
cells140. The global blockade of NMDARs had no effect memory with a focus on spatial memory. Behavioural
on the formation and short-term stability of CA1 place studies of rodents in which NMDAR function is
cells in a novel environment, nor on the reactivation of blocked by pharmacological or genetic manipulations
place cells in a familiar environment. The observation indicate that hippocampal NMDARs are crucial for
of normal place fields in novel space in the absence the acquisition of hippocampus-dependent memory,
of NMDAR function seems to contradict the and particularly for ‘episodic-like’ memory. Whether
finding of enlarged and diffuse CA1 place fields in hippocampal NMDARs are involved in the cellular con-
CA3-NR1-knockout mice69. solidation of memory remains to be determined, but
Ekstrom et al.141 used pharmacological blockade to they do not seem to be directly involved in memory
test whether NMDAR-mediated plasticity is necessary for retrieval. Cell-type-restricted gene ablation techniques
the experience-dependent asymmetric shift of established are beginning to reveal distinct mnemonic functions of
CA1 place fields139. Treatment with CPP caused a deficit NMDARs in the different hippocampal areas and
in the lap-dependent skewing of fields and, as a result, circuits. So, although NMDARs in CA1 pyramidal cells
slightly smaller fields. This reduction of place-field sizes seem to be crucial for the acquisition of spatial reference
also contradicts the enlarged and diffuse CA1 fields of memory, NMDARs in CA3 pyramidal cells are not.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 5 | MAY 2004 | 3 6 9

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

Instead, CA3 NMDARs seem to be important for the space led to evidence for experience-dependency of
rapid acquisition of one-trial memory (episodic-like place-cell formation, an important attribute of an
memory) and for the retrieval of associative memory ensemble memory trace. The effects of NMDAR antago-
by pattern completion. nists on the long-term stability of CA1 place cells
Although these behavioural studies with pharma- correlated well with the effects of these agents on the
cologically or genetically engineered rodents have acquisition and consolidation of spatial memory. All of
contributed greatly to our understanding of the impor- these studies support the idea that hippocampal place
tance of NMDAR-dependent synaptic plasticity in cells are an excellent candidate for an ensemble memory
hippocampus-dependent memory, it is unlikely that trace. However, most of the evidence that led to this
a memory trace resides at an individual synapse with notion concerns spatial encoding and spatial memory
modified transmission efficacy. As Hebb proposed, and in almost all of these studies, the recording of place
memory is likely to be encoded in the pattern of activity cells have been carried out, for technical reasons, in
of a cellular ensemble, the individual components of setups that mimic, but are not identical to, those of the
which are connected by synapses with modified synaptic learning and memory tasks. So, one challenge for future
efficacy10. In several studies in which the NR1 gene studies is to devise a paradigm with which memory and
knockout was targeted to hippocampal CA1 or CA3 place-cell activities can be assessed simultaneously. There
pyramidal cells of adult mice, the spatial tuning qualities is also still much to be learned about how information
of CA1 place cells were determined under conditions relating to non-spatial parameters, such as behavioural
that mimicked those under which behavioural aspects of state, motivation and olfactory and other sensory inputs,
hippocampal-dependent learning and memory were is encoded by and represented in place-cell activity146–151.
examined. The deficits in the spatial tuning quality of With further multidisciplinary studies consisting of
place cells, both at the individual and ensemble cell conditional transgenic technology, in vivo multi-unit
levels, correlated well with the impairments of behav- recording and clever behavioural paradigms, we might
iourally assessed learning and memory. In addition, the move closer to our ultimate goal of understanding the
analyses of the abilities of CA3-NR1-knockout mice in molecular, cellular and neuronal ensemble mechanisms
rapid one-trial learning and in the encoding of a novel for hippocampus-dependent learning and memory.

1. Scoville, W. B. & Milner, B. Loss of recent memory after This study utilized the patch-clamp technique to distributed memory system. Trends Neurosci. 10, 408–415
bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. provide the first evidence of a voltage-dependent (1987).
Psychiatry 20, 11–21 (1957). Mg2+ block of the NMDA Ca2+ channel 27. O’Reilly, R. C. & McClelland, J. L. Hippocampal conjunctive
2. Schmolck, H., Kensinger, E. A., Corkin, S. & Squire, L. R. 15. Lester, R. A., Clements, J. D., Westbrook, G. L. & Jahr, C. E. encoding, storage, and recall: avoiding a trade-off.
Semantic knowledge in patient H. M. and other patients with Channel kinetics determine the time course of NMDA Hippocampus 4, 661–682 (1994).
bilateral medial and lateral temporal lobe lesions. receptor-mediated synaptic currents. Nature 346, 565–567 28. Marr, D. Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos.
Hippocampus 12, 520–533 (2002). (1990). Trans. R. Soc. London B 262, 23–81 (1971).
3. Rempel-Clower, N. L., Zola, S. M., Squire, L. R. & Amaral, D. G. 16. Morris, R. G. M., Anderson, E., Lynch, G. S. & Baudry, M. A seminal work that laid the foundation of much of our
Three cases of enduring memory impairment after bilateral Selective impairment of learning and blockade of long-term current understanding of hippocampal computation.
damage limited to the hippocampal formation. J. Neurosci. potentiation by an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor Marr’s models include a discussion of partial-cue-
16, 5233–5255 (1996). antagonist, AP5. Nature 319, 774–776 (1986). based recall and experience-dependent modifications
4. Zola, S. M. & Squire, L. R. Relationship between magnitude This paper was the first to show that NMDAR activity of synaptic strength.
of damage to the hippocampus and impaired recognition is needed for spatial learning and to draw parallels 29. O’Reilly, R. C. & Rudy, J. W. Conjunctive representations in
memory in monkeys. Hippocampus 11, 92–98 (2001). between NMDAR function, hippocampal synaptic learning and memory: principles of cortical and hippocampal
5. Jarrard, L. E. On the role of the hippocampus in learning and plasticity and spatial learning. function. Psychol. Rev. 108, 311–345 (2001).
memory in the rat. Behav. Neural Biol. 60, 9–26 (1993). 17. Moriyoshi, K. et al. Molecular cloning and characterization of 30. Morris, R. G. M. Spatial localization does not require the
6. Morris, R. G. M., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N. P. & O’Keefe, the rat NMDA receptor. Nature 354, 31–37 (1991). presence of local cues. Learn. Motiv. 12, 239–260 (1981).
J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal The first identification of a gene encoding a subunit of 31. Davis, S., Butcher, S. P. & Morris, R. G. M. The NMDA
lesions. Nature 297, 681–683 (1982). the NMDAR, which paved the way for much of the receptor antagonist D-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoate
7. Silva, A. J., Giese, K. P., Fedorov, N. B., Frankland, P. W. & later genetic and structural work. (D-AP5) impairs spatial learning and LTP in vivo at
Kogan, J. H. Molecular, cellular, and neuroanatomical 18. Dunah, A. W. et al. Biochemical studies of the structure and intracerebral concentrations comparable to those that block
substrates of place learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 70, function of the N-methyl-D-aspartate subtype of glutamate LTP in vitro. J. Neurosci. 12, 21–34 (1992).
44–61 (1998). receptors. Mol. Neurobiol. 19, 151–179 (1999). 32. Morris, R. G. M. Synaptic plasticity and learning: selective
8. O’Keefe, J. & Dostrovsky, J. The hippocampus as a spatial 19. Cull-Candy, S., Brickley, S. & Farrant, M. NMDA receptor impairment of learning in rats and blockade of long-term
map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely- subunits: diversity, development and disease. Curr. Opin. potentiation in vivo by the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor
moving rat. Brain Res. 34, 171–175 (1971). Neurobiol. 11, 327–335 (2001). antagonist AP5. J. Neurosci. 9, 3040–3057 (1989).
9. Bliss, T. V. P. & Lømo, T. Long-lasting potentiation of 20. Li, Y., Erzurumulu, R., Chen, C., Jhaveri, S. & Tonegawa, S. 33. Riedel, G. et al. Reversible neural inactivation reveals
synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the Whisker-related neuronal patterns fail to develop in the hippocampal participation in several memory processes.
anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant trigeminal brainstem nuclei of NMDAR1 knockout mice. Cell Nature Neurosci. 2, 898–905 (1999).
path. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 232, 331–356 (1973). 76, 427–437 (1994). 34. Abraham, W. C. & Kairiss, E. W. Effects of the NMDA
10. Hebb, D. O. The Organization of Behavior: A 21. Forrest, D. et al. Targeted disruption of NMDA receptor 1 antagonist 2AP5 on complex spike discharge by hippocampal
Neuropsychological Theory (John Wiley, New York, 1949). gene abolishes NMDA response and results in neonatal pyramidal cells. Neurosci. Lett. 89, 36–42 (1988).
11. Collingridge, G. L., Kehl, S. J. & McLennon, H. Excitatory death. Neuron 13, 325–338 (1994). 35. Errington, M. L., Lynch, M. A. & Bliss, T. V. Long-term
amino acids in synaptic transmission in the Schaffer 22. Amaral, D. G. & Witter, M. P. The three-dimensional potentiation in the dentate gyrus: induction and increased
collateral-commissural pathway of the rat hippocampus. organization of the hippocampal formation: a review of glutamate release are blocked by
J. Physiol. (Lond.) 334, 33–46 (1983). anatomical data. Neuroscience 31, 571–591 (1989). D(–)aminophosphonovalerate. Neuroscience 20, 279–284
The first demonstration that long-term potentiation in 23. Miles, R. & Wong, R. K. Excitatory synaptic interactions (1987).
the hippocampus is NMDAR-dependent. between CA3 neurones in the guinea-pig hippocampus. 36. Morris, R. G. M., Davis, S. & Butcher, S. P. Hippocampal
12. Mayer, M. L., Westbrook, G. L. & Guthrie, P. B. Voltage- J. Physiol. (Lond.) 373, 397–418 (1986). synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptors: a role in information
dependent block by Mg2+ of NMDA responses in spinal cord 24. MacVicar, B. A. & Dudek, F. E. Local synaptic circuits in rat storage? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 329, 187–204 (1990).
neurones. Nature 309, 261–263 (1984). hippocampus: interactions between pyramidal cells. Brain 37. Hargreaves, E. L. & Cain, D. P. Hyperactivity, hyper-reactivity,
13. MacDermott, A. B., Mayer, M. L., Westbrook, G. L., Smith, Res. 184, 220–223 (1980). and sensorimotor deficits induced by low doses of the
S. J. & Barker, J. L. NMDA-receptor activation increases 25. McClelland, J. L. & Goddard, N. H. Considerations arising N-methyl-D-aspartate non-competitive channel blocker
cytoplasmic calcium concentration in cultured spinal cord from a complementary learning systems perspective on MK801. Behav. Brain Res. 47, 23–33 (1992).
neurones. Nature 321, 519–522 (1986). hippocampus and neocortex. Hippocampus 6, 654–665 38. Sternberg, N., Hamilton, D. & Hoess, R. Bacteriophage P1
14. Nowak, L., Bregestovski, P., Ascher, P., Herbert, A. & (1996). site-specific recombination. II. Recombination between loxP
Prochiantz, A. Magnesium gates glutamate-activated channels 26. McNaughton, B. L. & Morris, R. G. M. Hippocampal and the bacterial chromosome. J. Mol. Biol. 150, 487–507
in mouse central neurones. Nature 307, 462–465 (1984). synaptic enhancement and information storage within a (1981).

370 | MAY 2004 | VOLUME 5 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

39. Hoess, R. H., Ziese, M. & Sternberg, N. P1 site-specific 59. MacVicar, B. & Dudek, F. Local synaptic circuits in rat 85. Nadel, L. & Moscovitch, M. Memory consolidation,
recombination: nucleotide sequence of the recombining hippocampus: interaction between pyramidal cells. Brain retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr.
sites. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 79, 3398–3402 (1982). Res. 184, 220–223 (1980). Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 217–227 (1997).
40. Tsien, J. Z., Huerta, P. T. & Tonegawa, S. The essential role 60. Harris, E. W. & Cotman, C. W. Long-term potentiation of 86. Bontempi, B., Lauren-Demir, C., Destrade, C. & Jaffard, R.
of hippocampal CA1 NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic guinea pig mossy fiber response is not blocked by N-methyl- Time-dependent reorganization of brain circuitry underlying
plasticity in spatial memory. Cell 87, 1327–1338 (1996). D-aspartate antagonist. Neurosci. Lett. 70, 132–137 (1986). long-term memory storage. Nature 400, 671–675 (1999).
This paper offered the first genetic demonstration of 61. Williams, S. & Johnston, D. Muscarinic depression of long- 87. Davis, H. P. & Squire, L. R. Protein synthesis and memory: a
the effect of a brain-subregion-restricted NMDA term potentiation in CA3 hippocampal neurons. Science review. Psych. Bull. 96, 518–559 (1984).
receptor knockout on spatial memory. 242, 84–87 (1988). 88. DeZazzo, J. & Tully, T. Dissection of memory formation: from
41. McHugh, T. J., Blum, K. I., Tsien, J. Z., Tonegawa, S. & Wilson, 62. Zalutsky, R. A. & Nicoll, R. A. Comparison of two forms of behavioral pharmacology to molecular genetics. Trends
M. A. Impaired hippocampal representation of space in CA1- long-term potentiation in single hippocampal neurons. Neurosci. 18, 212–218 (1995).
specific NMDAR1 knockout mice. Cell 87, 1339–1349 (1996). Science 248, 1619–1624 (1990). 89. Bailey, C. H., Bartsch, D. & Kandel, E. R. Toward a
This study was the first to show impaired coherent 63. Berger, T. W. & Yeckel, M. F. in Long-Term Potentiation: A molecular definition of long-term memory storage. Proc.
firing of CA1 place cells using genetically engineered Debate of Current Issues (eds Baudry, M. & Davis, J. L.) Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 13445–13452 (1996).
NMDAR-knockout mice, demonstrating the powerful 327–356 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1991). 90. Nader, K., Schafem, G. E. & LeDoux, J. E. The labile nature
combination of conditional transgenic technology and 64. Berzhanskaya, J., Urban, N. N. & Barrionuevo, G. of consolidation theory. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1, 216–219
multi-electrode recording. Electrophysiological and pharmacological characterization (2000).
42. Fukaya, M., Kato, A., Lovett, C., Tonegawa, S. & Watanabe, M. of the direct perforant path input to hippocampal area CA3. 91. Abel, T. & Kandel, E. Positive and negative regulatory
Retention of NMDA receptor NR2 subunits in the lumen of J. Neurophysiol. 79, 2111–2118 (1998). mechanisms that mediate long-term memory storage. Brain
endoplasmic reticulum in targeted NR1 knockout mice. 65. McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., O’Reilly, R. C. & Nadel, Res. Rev. 26, 360–378 (1998).
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 4855–4860 (2003). L. Complementary roles of hippocampus and neocortex in 92. Adams, J. P. & Sweatt, J. D. Molecular psychology: roles for
43. Huerta, P. T., Sun, L. D., Wilson, M. A. & Tonegawa, S. learning and memory. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 18, 1216 (1992). the ERK MAP kinase cascade in memory. Annu. Rev.
Formation of temporal memory requires NMDA receptors 66. McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L. & O’Reilly, R. C. Why Pharmacol. Toxicol. 42, 135–163 (2002).
within CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuron 25, 473–480 (2000). there are complementary learning systems in the 93. Dudai, Y. Molecular bases of long-term memories: a
44. Rondi-Reig, L., Libbey, M., Eichenbaum, H. & Tonegawa, S. hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the success and question of persistence. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 212–216
CA1-specific N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor knockout mice failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. (2002).
are deficient in solving a nonspatial transverse patterning Psychol. Rev. 102, 419–457 (1995). 94. Bozon, B. et al. MAPK, CREB and zif268 are all required for
task. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 3543–3548 (2001). The authors of references 65 and 66 formulated the the consolidation of recognition memory. Philos. Trans. R.
45. Rampon, C. et al. Enrichment induces structural changes and theoretical basis of complementary learning systems Soc. Lond. B 358, 805–814 (2003).
recovery from nonspatial-memory deficits in CA1-NMDAR1- of hippocampus and neocortex, which allows the 95. Kang, H. et al. An important role of neural activity-dependent
knockout mice. Nature Neurosci. 3, 238–244 (2000). brain to rapidly learn new on-line events without CaMKIV signaling in the consolidation of long-term memory.
46. Bannerman, D. M., Good, M. A., Butcher, S. P., Ramsay, M. disrupting old off-line information, and to integrate Cell 106, 771–783 (2001).
& Morris, R. G. Distinct components of spatial learning these new events properly with previously stored 96. Kida, S. et al. CREB required for the stability of new and
revealed by prior training and NMDA receptor blockade. experiences. reactivated fear memories. Nature Neurosci. 5, 348–355
Nature 378, 182–186 (1995). 67. Lee, I. & Kesner, R. P. Differential contribution of NMDA (2002).
47. Saucier, D. & Cain, D. P. Spatial learning without NMDA receptors in hippocampal subregions to spatial working 97. Schafe, G. E., Nader, K., Blair, H. T. & LeDoux, J. E. Memory
receptor-dependent long-term potentiation. Nature 378, memory. Nature Neurosci. 5, 162–168 (2002). consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: a cellular and
186–189 (1995). 68. Nakazawa, K. et al. Requirement for hippocampal CA3 molecular perspective. Trends Neurosci. 24, 540–546
References 46 and 47 provided strong evidence that NMDA receptors in acquisition and recall of associative (2001).
NMDARs could be made dispensable for learning the memory. Science 297, 211–218 (2002). 98. Taubenfeld, S. M., Milekic, M. H., Monti, B. & Alberini, C. M.
water maze task if animals were pretrained on an This study combines regionally restricted genetic The consolidation of new but not reactivated memory
identical spatial or even similar, non-spatial task. This modification with in vivo physiology and behaviour to requires hippocampal C/EBPb. Nature Neurosci. 4,
was the first real evidence that the water maze task provide strong evidence that NMDARs in region CA3 813–818 (2001).
might involve several types of learning, some of which are required for memory recall by pattern completion. 99. Abel, T. & Lattal, K. M. Molecular mechanisms of memory
require NMDARs in the hippocampus and some of 69. Nakazawa, K. et al. Hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors are acquisition, consolidation and retrieval. Curr. Opin.
which do not. crucial for memory acquisition of one-time experience. Neurobiol. 11, 180–187 (2001).
48. Roesler, R. et al. Intrahippocampal infusion of the NMDA Neuron 38, 305–315 (2003). 100. Izquierdo, I. & Medina, J. H. Memory formation: the
receptor antagonist AP5 impairs retention of an inhibitory 70. Vinogradova, O. S. in The Hippocampus (eds Isaacson, R. L. sequence of biochemical events in the hippocampus and its
avoidance task: protection from impairment by pretraining or & Pribram, K. H.) 3–69 (Plenum, New York and London, connection to activity in other brain structures. Neurobiol.
preexposure to the task apparatus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1975). Learn. Mem. 68, 285–316 (1997).
69, 87–91 (1998). 71. McNaughton, B. L., Barnes, C. A., Meltzer, J. & Sutherland, 101. Steward, O. & Schuman, E. M. Protein synthesis at synaptic
49. Hoh, T., Beiko, J., Boon, F., Weiss, S. & Cain, D. P. Complex R. J. Hippocampal granule cells are necessary for normal sites on dendrites. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 299–325
behavioral strategy and reversal learning in the watermaze spatial learning but not for spatially-selective pyramidal cell (2001).
without NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation. discharge. Exp. Brain Res. 76, 485–496 (1989). 102. Kelleher, R. J., Govindarajan, A., Jung H.-Y., Kang, H. &
J. Neurosci. 19, RC2(1–5) (1999). 72. Brun, V. H. et al. Place cells and place recognition Tonegawa, S. Translational control by MAPK signaling in
50. Steele, R. J. & Morris, R. G. M. Delay-dependent impairment maintained by direct entorhinal-hippocampal circuitry. long-term synaptic plasticity and memory. Cell 116,
of a matching-to-place task with chronic and Science 296, 2243–2246 (2002). 467–479 (2004).
intrahippocampal infusion of the NMDA-antagonist D-AP5. 73. Barnes, C. A., McNaughton, B. L., Mizumori, S. J., Leonard, 103. Wu, L. et al. CPEB-mediated cytoplasmic polyadenylation
Hippocampus 9, 118–136 (1999). B. W. & Lin, L.-H. Comparison of spatial and temporal and the regulation of experience-dependent translation of α-
This study showed that a delayed matching-to-place characteristics of neuronal activity in sequential stages of CaMKII mRNA at synapses. Neuron 21, 1129–1139 (1998).
task in the water maze requires hippocampal NMDA hippocampal processing. Prog. Brain Res. 83, 287–300 104. Heale, V. & Harley, C. MK-801 and AP5 impair acquisition,
receptors in rats, providing new insights into the (1990). but not retention, of the Morris milk maze. Pharmacol.
involvement of the hippocampus in episodic-like 74. Frank, L. M., Brown, E. N. & Wilson, M. A. Trajectory Biochem. Behav. 36, 145–149 (1990).
memory. encoding in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex. Neuron 105. Norris, C. M. & Foster, T. C. MK-801 improves retention in
51. Tulving, E. in Organization of Memory (eds Tulving, E. & 27, 169–178 (2000). aged rats: implications for altered neural plasticity in age-
Donaldson, W.) 381–403 (Academic, New York, 1972). 75. Quirk, G. J., Muller, R. U., Kubie, J. L. & Ranck, J. B. Jr. The related memory deficits. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 71,
52. Tulving, E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu. Rev. positional firing properties of medial entorhinal neurons: 194–206 (1999).
Psychol. 53, 1–25 (2002). description and comparison with hippocampal place cells. 106. Packard, M. G. & Teather, L. A. Double dissociation of
53. Tulving, E. & Markowitsch, H. J. Episodic and declarative J. Neurosci. 12, 1945–1963 (1992). hippocampal and dorsal-striatal memory systems by
memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8, 76. Moser, E. I. & Moser, M. B. One-shot memory in posttraining intracerebral injections of 2-amino-5-
198–204 (1998). hippocampal CA3 networks. Neuron 38, 147–148 (2003). phosphonopentanoic acid. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 543–551
54. Clayton, N. S. & Dickinson, A. Episodic-like memory during 77. Day, M., Langston, R. & Morris, R. G. Glutamate-receptor- (1997).
cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395, 272–274 (1998). mediated encoding and retrieval of paired-associate 107. Packard, M. G. & Teather, L. A. Posttraining injections of
Taking advantage of the natural caching behaviour of learning. Nature 424, 205–209 (2003). MK-801 produce a time-dependent impairment of memory
the scrub jay, these authors provide the strongest 78. Ebbinghaus, H. Memory: A Contribution to Experimental in two water maze tasks. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 68, 42–50
evidence for the existence of ‘what, where and when Psychology (Dover, New York, 1885). (1997).
(episodic-like) memory’ in animals. 79. Müller, G. E. & Pilzecker, A. Experimentelle Beitrage zur 108. Shimizu, E., Tang, Y. P., Rampon, C. & Tsien, J. Z. NMDA
55. Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J. & Dickinson, A. Can animals Lehre vom Gedachtnis. Z. Psychol. Erganzungsband 1, receptor-dependent synaptic reinforcement as a crucial
recall the past and plan for the future? Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1–300 (1900). process for memory consolidation. Science 290,
4, 685–691 (2003). 80. McGaugh, J. L. Time-dependent processes in memory 1170–1174 (2001).
56. Morris, R. G. M. Episodic-like memory in animals: storage. Science 153, 1351–1358 (1966). 109. Gossen, M. & Bujard, H. Anhydrotetracycline, a novel effector
psychological criteria, neural mechanisms and the value of 81. McGaugh, J. L. Memory: a century of consolidation. for tetracycline controlled gene expression systems in
episodic-like tasks to investigate animal models of Science 287, 248–251 (2000). eukaryotic cells. Nucleic Acids Res. 21, 4411–4412 (1993).
neurodegenerative diseases. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 82. Nader, K. Memory traces unbound. Trends Neurosci. 26, 110. Stäubli, U., Thibault, O., DiLorenzo, M. & Lynch, G.
356, 1453–1465 (2001). 65–72 (2003). Antagonism of NMDA receptors impairs acquisition but not
57. Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Pretraining and the function of 83. Squire, L. R. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis retetion of olfactory memory. Behav. Neurosci. 103, 54–60
hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neuron 26, 559–561 from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol. (1989).
(2000). Rev. 99, 195–231 (1992). 111. Parada-Turska, J. & Turski, W. A. Excitatory amino acid
58. Miles, R. & Traub, R. Excitatory synaptic interactions 84. Squire, L. R. & Alvarez, P. Retrograde amnesia and memory antagonists and memory: effect of drugs acting at N-methyl-
between CA3 neurons in the guinea-pig hippocampus. consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. Curr. Opin. D-aspartate receptors in learning and memory tasks.

J. Physiol. (Lond.) 373, 397–418 (1986). Neurobiol. 5, 169–177 (1995). Neuropharmacology 29, 1111–1116 (1990).

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 5 | MAY 2004 | 3 7 1

©2004 Nature Publishing Group


REVIEWS

112. Venable, N. & Kelly, P. H. Effects of NMDA receptor 131. Skaggs, W. E. & McNaughton, B. L. Replay of neuronal firing 151. Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Hollup, S., Moser, M.-B. & Moser, E. I.
antagonists on passive avoidance learning and retrieval in sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep following spatial Hippocampal neurons responding to first-time dislocation of
rats and mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 100, 215–221 experience. Science 271, 1870–1873 (1996). a target object. Neuron 35, 555–566 (2002).
(1990). 132. Kudrimoti, H., Barnes, C. A. & McNaughton, B. L. 152. Rolls, E. T. A theory of hippocampal function in memory.
113. Szapiro, G. et al. Participation of hippocampal metabotropic Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: effects of Hippocampus 6, 601–620 (1996).
glutamate receptors, protein kinase A and mitogen- behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics. 153. O’Keefe, J. Hippocampus, theta, and spatial memory. Curr.
activated protein kinases in memory retrieval. Neuroscience J. Neurosci. 19, 4090–4101 (1999). Opin. Neurobiol. 3, 917–924. (1993).
99, 1–5 (2000). 133. Nadasdy, Z., Hirase, H., Czurko, A., Csicsvari, J. & Buzsaki, G. 154. O’Keefe, J. A review of the hippocampal place cells. Prog.
114. Liang, K. C., Hon, W. & Davis, M. Pre- and posttraining Replay and time compression of recurring spike sequences Neurobiol. 13, 419–349 (1979).
infustion of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists into in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 19, 9497–9507 (1999). 155. Muller, R. U., Stead, M. & Pach, J. The hippocampus as
the amygdala impair memory in an inhibitory avoidance task. 134. Muller, R. U. & Kubie, J. L. The effects of changes in the a cognitive graph. J. Gen. Physiol. 107, 663–694
Behav. Neurosci. 108, 241–253 (1994). environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex- (1996).
115. Martin, S. J., Grimwood, P. D. & Morris, R. G. M. Synaptic spike cells. J. Neurosci. 7, 1951–1968 (1987). 156. Best, P. J., White, A. M. & Minai, A. Spatial processing in the
plasticity and memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis. 135. O’Keefe, J. & Conway, D. H. Hippocampal place units in the brain: the activity of hippocampal place cells. Annu. Rev.
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 649–711 (2000). freely moving rat: why they fire where they fire. Exp. Brain. Neurosci. 24, 459–486 (2001).
116. Martin, S. J. & Morris, R. G. M. New life in an old idea: the Res. 31, 573–590 (1978). 157. Thompson, L. T. & Best, P. J. Long-term stability of the
synaptic plasticity and memory hypothesis revisited. 136. O’Keefe, J. & Speakman, A. Single unit activity in the rat place-field activity of single units recorded from the dorsal
Hippocampus 12, 609–636 (2002). hippocampus during a spatial memory task. Exp. Brain Res. hippocampus of freely behaving rats. Brain Res. 509,
117. Chomsky, N. Syntactic Structures (Mouton, The Hague, 68, 1–27 (1987). 299–308 (1990).
1957). 137. Pico, R. M., Gerbrandt, L. K., Pondel, M. & Ivy, G. During 158. Muller, R. A quarter of a century of place cells. Neuron 17,
118. Gardner-Medwin, A. R. The recall of events through the stepwise cue deletion, rat place behaviors correlate with 813–822 (1996).
learning of associations between their parts. Proc. R. Soc. place unit responses. Brain Res. 330, 369–372 (1985). 159. Redish, A. D. et al. Independence of firing correlates of
Lond. B 194, 375–402 (1976). 138. Mehta, M. R., Barnes, C. A. & McNaughton, B. L. Experience- anatomically proximate hippocampal pyramidal cells.
119. Hopfield, J. J. Neural networks and physical systems with dependent, asymmetric expansion of hippocampal place J. Neurosci. 21, RC134 (2001).
emergent collective computational abilities. Proc. Natl Acad. fields. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 94, 8918–8921 (1997). 160. Branda, C. S. & Dymecki, S. M. Talking about a revolution:
Sci. USA 79, 2554–2558 (1982). 139. Mehta, M. R., Quirk, M. C. & Wilson, M. A. Experience- The impact of site-specific recombinases on genetic
120. Rolls, E. T. in The Computing Neuron (eds Durbin, R., Maill, dependent asymmetric shape of hippocampal receptive analyses in mice. Dev. Cell 6, 7–28 (2004).
C. & Mitchison, G.) 125–159 (Addison-Wesley, Wokingham, fields. Neuron 25, 707–715 (2000). 161. Morozov, A., Kellendonk, C., Simpson, E. & Tronche, F.
UK, 1989). 140. Kentros, C. et al. Abolition of long-term stability of new Using conditional mutagenesis to study the brain. Biol.
121. Willshaw, D. J. & Buckingham, J. T. An assessment of hippocampal place cell maps by NMDA receptor blockade. Psychiatry 54, 1125–1133 (2003).
Marr’s theory of the hippocampus as a temporary memory Science 280, 2121–2126 (1998). 162. Grant, S. G. N. et al. Impaired long-term potentiation, spatial
storage. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 329, 205–215 (1990). Combining pharmacology and in vivo recording, the learning, and hippocampal development in fyn mutant mice.
122. Hasselmo, M. E., Schnell, E. & Barkai, E. Dynamics of authors show that acute blockade of the NMDAR during Science 258, 1903–1909 (1992).
learning and recall at excitatory recurrent synapses and the exploration of a novel environment impairs the long- 163. Blendy, J. A., Kaestner, K. H., Schmid, W., Gass, P. &
cholinergic modulation in rat hippocampal region CA3. term stability of the newly encoded place fields. Schutz, G. Targeting of the CREB gene leads to up-
J. Neurosci. 15, 5249–5262 (1995). 141. Ekstrom, A. D., Meltzer, J., McNaughton, B. L. & Barnes, C. A. regulation of a novel CREB mRNA isoform. EMBO J. 15,
123. O’Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive NMDA receptor antagonism blocks experience-dependent 1098–1106 (1996).
Map (Clarendon, Oxford, 1978). expansion of hippcampal ‘place fields’. Neuron 31, 631–638 164. Hummler, E. et al. Targeted mutation of the CREB gene:
This book uses both the animal and the human (2001). compensation within the CREB/ATF family of transcription
literature to outline a theory concerning the role of the The authors demonstrated, using the NMDAR factors. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 91, 5647–5651
hippocampus in spatial representation. The ideas antagonist CPP, that experience-dependent (1994).
outlined in this work have been the basis for asymmetric place field expansion in hippocampal 165. Mills, A. A. Changing colors in mice: an inducible system
countless experiments in the field and continue to CA1 is NR-dependent, but phase precession on the that delivers. Genes Dev. 15, 1461–1467 (2001).
generate important discussion. theta wave is not. 166. Schonig, K. & Bujard, H. Generating conditional mouse
124. Wilson, M. A. & McNaughton, B. L. Reactivation of 142. Rotenberg, A., Mayford, M., Hawkins, R. D., Kandel, E. R. & mutants via tetracycline-controlled gene expression.
hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science Muller, R. U. Mice expressing activated CaMKII lack low Methods Mol. Biol. 209, 69–104 (2003).
265, 676–679 (1994). frequency LTP and do not form stable place cells in the 167. Gross, C. et al. Serotonin1A receptor acts during
125. Wilson, M. A. & McNaughton, B. L. Dynamics of the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Cell 87, 1351–1361 (1996). development to establish normal anxiety-like behaviour in
hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science 261, 143. Rotenberg, A., Abel, T., Hawkins, R. D., Kandel, E. R. & the adult. Nature 416, 396–400 (2002).
1055–1058 (1993). Muller, R. U. Parallel instabilities of long-term potentiation, 168. Chen, J. et al. Transgenic animals with inducible, targeted
Utilizing multiple recording tetrodes, these authors place cells, and learning caused by decreased protein gene expression in brain. Mol. Pharmacol. 54, 495–503
monitored the simultaneous activity of more than kinase A activity. J. Neurosci. 20, 8096–8102 (2000). (1998).
one-hundred CA1 neurons as a rat explored the 144. Yan, J. et al. Place-cell impairment in glutamate receptor 2 169. Mayford, M. et al. Control of memory formation through
environment. This study was the first to look at mutant mice. J. Neurosci. 22, RC204 (2002). regulated expression of a CaMKII transgene. Science 274,
hippocampal spatial activity on the level of a network 145. Cho, Y. H., Giese, K. P., Tanila, H., Silva, A. J. & 1678–1683 (1996).
and also the first to demonstrate the establishment of Eichenbaum, H. Abnormal hippocampal spatial
a spatial code in a novel environment. representations in aCaMKIIT286A and CREBα/– mice. Science Acknowledgements
126. Eichenbaum, H., Dudchenko, P., Wood, W., Shapiro, M. & 279, 867–869 (1998). This work was supported by an individual NIH grant to S.T. and
Tanila, H. The hippocampus, memory, and place cells: is it 146. Muller, R. U., Bostock, E., Taube, J. S. & Kubie, J. L. On the an NIH Silvio O. Conte Center grant to S.T. and M.A.W., and by
spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron 23, 209–226 directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells. grants from the Howard Hughes Medical Institute to S.T. and
(1999). J. Neurosci. 14, 7235–7251 (1994). from the RIKEN-MIT Neuroscience Research Center to S.T.
127. Lee, A. K. & Wilson, M. A. Memory of sequential experience 147. Anderson, M. I. & Jeffery, K. J. Heterogeneous modulation and M.A.W.
in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron 36, of place cell firing by changes in context. J. Neurosci. 23,
1183–1194 (2002). 8827–8835 (2003). Competing interests statement
128. Louie, K. & Wilson, M. A. Temporally structured replay of 148. Wood, E. R., Dudchenko, P. A., Robitsek, R. J. & The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.
awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye Eichenbaum, H. Hippocampal neurons encode information
movement sleep. Neuron 29, 145–156 (2001). about different types of memory episodes occurring in the
129. Poucet, B., Save, E. & Lenck-Santini, P. P. Sensory and same location. Neuron 27, 623–633 (2000). Online links
memory properties of hippocampal place cells. Rev. 149. Young, B. J., Fox, G. D. & Eichenbaum, H. Correlates of
Neurosci. 11, 95–111 (2000). hippocampal complex-spike cell activity in rats performing a FURTHER INFORMATION
130. Moser, E. I. & Paulsen, O. New excitement in cognitive nonspatial radial maze task. J. Neurosci. 14, 6553–6563 Nakazawa’s homepage: http://neuroscience.nih.gov/
space: between place cells and spatial memory. Curr. Opin. (1994). Lab.asp?Org_ID=496
Neurobiol. 11, 745–751 (2001). 150. Hollup, S. A., Molden, S., Donnett, J. G., Moser, M. B. & Tonegawa’s homepage: http://www.hhmi.org/research
The authors reviewed recent evidence connecting Moser, E. I. Accumulation of hippocampal place fields at the /investigators/tonegawa.html
hippocampal place cell activity with its mnemonic goal location in an annular watermaze task. J. Neurosci. 21, Wilson’s homepage: http://web.mit.edu/bcs/people/wilson.shtml
function. 1635–1644 (2001). Access to this interactive links box is free online.

372 | MAY 2004 | VOLUME 5 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

©2004 Nature Publishing Group

You might also like