Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Appetite 116 (2017) 423e430

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Appetite
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

Positive and negative emotional eating have different associations


with overeating and binge eating: Construction and validation of the
Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale
Hedvig Sultson, Katrin Kukk, Kirsti Akkermann*
€ituse 2, 50409, Tartu, Estonia
Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Na

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Research on emotional eating mostly focuses on negative emotions. Much less is known about how
Received 28 February 2017 positive emotions relate to overeating and binge eating (BE). The aim of the current study was to
Received in revised form construct a scale for positive and negative emotional eating and to assess its predictive validity. In study
16 May 2017
1, the Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale (PNEES) was constructed and tested on 531 women, who
Accepted 19 May 2017
also completed Eating Disorders Assessment Scale (EDAS). Results showed that a two-factor model
Available online 23 May 2017
constituting Positive emotional eating (PNEES-P) and Negative emotional eating (PNEES-N) fit the data
well. PNEES-N also showed good convergent validity in assessing binge eating, correlating highly with
Keywords:
Experience sampling method
EDAS subscale Binge eating. Further, a path analysis showed that after controlling for the mediating effect
Binge eating of PNEES-N, PNEES-P continued to significantly predict binge eating. In study 2 (N ¼ 60), experience
Negative emotional eating sampling method was used to assess overeating and BE in the natural environment. Palmtop computers
Overeating were given to participants for a three-day study period that prompted them with questions regarding
Positive emotional eating emotional experience, overeating, and BE. Results indicated that PNEES-P significantly predicted over-
eating, whereas PNEES-N predicted overeating and BE episodes only in a subsample of women who had
experienced at least one overeating or BE episode. Thus, positive and negative emotional eating might
have different relations with overeating and BE, with the latter being more characteristic of the severity/
frequency of overeating and BE. New assessment tools that in addition to negative emotional eating also
address positive emotional eating could be of potential help in planning intervention. Further, the ten-
dency to overeat in response to positive emotions could be integrated into current models of eating
disorders, especially when addressing relapse prevention.
© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction defined as emotional eating (Arnow, Kenardy, & Agras, 1995). It is


believed that emotional eating has its roots in operant condition-
In most people, emotional stress elicits changes in appetite ing, according to which excessive eating, preceded by negative
(Macht, 2008). Acute severe stress is associated with the activation affect, is maintained by negative reinforcement (Meyer, Waller, &
of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Gold & Chrousos, Waters, 1998). Thus, emotional eating might provide a mecha-
2002) that results in the suppression of appetite (Adam & Epel, nism to downregulate one's negative affect.
2007; Majzoub, 2006), whereas mild to moderate stress could A desire to eat in response to negative affect to alleviate one's
lead to increased food intake in some individuals (Oliver, Wardle, & negative affect has also been associated with binge eating, which,
Gibson, 2000; Rutters, Nieuwenhuizen, Lemmens, Born, & according to DSM-5 is defined as consuming significantly larger
Westerterp-Plantenga, 2009), referred to as an atypical response amount of food than usual during a limited period of time
by Van Strien, Donker, and Ouwens (2016). This tendency to in- (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). In addition, loss of
crease one's food intake in response to negative emotions has been control over eating is also considered to be an important diagnostic
feature of binge eating (Pollert et al., 2013). Several studies have
demonstrated that emotional eating and binge eating are linked, as
significant associations between these two constructs have been
* Corresponding author.
found in clinical and in non-clinical samples (Stice, Presnell, &
E-mail address: kirsti.akkermann@ut.ee (K. Akkermann).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.035
0195-6663/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
424 H. Sultson et al. / Appetite 116 (2017) 423e430

Spangler, 2002; Van Strien, Engels, van Leeuwe, & Snoek, 2005). Anschutz, Engels, & de Weerth, 2012), and after stress-induction
Further, Vainik, Neseliler, Konstabel, Fellows, and Dagher (2015) (Oliver et al., 2000). This relationship, however, was not observed
demonstrated that negative emotional eating and binge eating in some other studies (e.g., Adriaanse, de Ridder, & Evers, 2011). The
could be placed on a continuum of uncontrolled eating, as they validity of self-reported emotional eating questionnaires is also
share a similar underlying factor. Thus, emotional eating might questioned in a recent paper by Bongers and Jansen (2016) who
characterize intermediate and binge eating severe levels of un- reviewed several experimental and naturalistic studies, and re-
controlled eating. Therefore, emotional eating might be a useful ported that the effect of self-reported emotional eating on subse-
marker in identifying early disordered eating behaviour (Haedt- quent food intake is observed in some studies, whereas in others, it
Matt et al., 2014). is ambiguous or not observed. However, Van Strien et al. (2012)
Even though the vast majority of scientific literature pertaining have argued that in order to detect such an effect, there must be
to emotional eating focuses on negative emotional eating, several enough participants with extreme scores on the moderating vari-
authors have found that positive emotions can also increase eating able. By including emotional eaters who scored above the 80th
(Bongers, Jansen, Havermans, Roefs, & Nederkoorn, 2013; Bongers, percentile and non-emotional eaters who scored below the 20th
Jansen, Houben, & Roefs, 2013; Bongers, de Graaff, & Jansen, 2016; percentile on the emotional eating subscale of the DEBQ, they
Evers, de Ridder, & Adriaanse, 2009). Bongers, Jansen, Houben, et al. found that emotional eaters ate more after the sad mood and stress
(2013) found that emotional eaters who were identified via the induction, whereas non-emotional eaters ate less. The fact that the
implicit association test ate more after the positive compared to the evidence for the effect of self-reported emotional eating on actual
negative mood induction. An increase in food consumption after eating behaviour is mixed across laboratory and naturalistic studies
positive mood induction compared to negative or neutral has been implies that emotional eating might be a more complex phenom-
established by few other studies (Evers, Adriaanse, de Ridder, & de enon than it has been believed (Bongers & Jansen, 2016). Therefore,
Witt Huberts, 2013; Patel & Schlundt, 2001). In addition, a recent the predictive and discriminative validity of self-reported
meta-analysis (Cardi, Leppanen, & Treasure, 2015) showed that emotional eating is in serious need of further validation.
healthy participants consumed significantly more food following Thus, upon constructing and validating an emotional eating
positive or negative mood induction in comparison to neutral mood scale, it would be crucial to show how the scale reflects real-life
(average effect sizes were similar). In contrast, some studies con- eating behaviour. One method of collecting data that more closely
ducted in laboratory settings have demonstrated that emotional mirrors real-life behaviour in comparison to laboratory experi-
eaters tend to overeat following negative mood induction, whereas ments and self-report measures is to use ecological momentary
no differences between neutral and positive mood induction have assessment (EMA; Stone, Hufford, & Shiffman, 2008), also known as
been found (Macht & Simons, 2000; Van Strien et al., 2013). Thus, the experience sampling method (ESM; Hektner, Schmidt, &
the relationship between positive emotional eating and real-life Csikszentmihalyi, 2007). EMA/ESM involves repeated measure-
eating behaviour is mixed. ments of thoughts, behaviours, and/or physiological markers in
In comparison to negative emotional eating, the mechanisms participants' natural environments. Furthermore, as retrospective
behind positive emotional eating are less clear. Several researchers emotional ratings and eating behaviour are prone to retrospective
believe that positive and negative emotional eating are two memory biases, EMA/ESM provides a way to produce results that
different constructs (Macht & Simons, 2000; Van Strien et al., 2013). are more ecologically valid. Several researchers have investigated
For example, Van Strien et al. (2016) showed that body-mass index binge eating using ESM (Crosby et al., 2009; Smyth et al., 2007;
(BMI) and items measuring negative emotional eating loaded onto Wegner et al., 2002).
the same factor, whereas items measuring positive emotional Since the role of positive emotions regarding eating behaviour
eating and external eating loaded onto the other factor. Similarly, has somewhat been neglected, the aim of the present study was to
Bourdier et al. (2017) found that BMI had a modest negative cor- construct the Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale (PNEES),
relation with positive emotional eating, and modest positive asso- which captures eating in response to both positive and negative
ciation with negative emotional eating. Thus, it is plausible that emotions, and to assess its predictive validity via ESM. Even though
even though positive emotions may elicit eating, they do not there already are emotional eating scales (i.e., Emotional Appetite
necessarily mirror disordered eating. Eating in response to positive Questionnaire (EMAQ); Nolan, Halperin, & Geliebter, 2010)
emotions might rather be related to hedonic (Macht, 1999) or measuring eating in response to positive emotions, we wanted to
external eating (Van Strien et al., 2016). This assumption is likely, include more variety in terms of positive emotional experience as
given that in a meta-analysis by Cardi et al. (2015), there was a well as the phrasing of items. For these purposes, we conducted
trend for more food to be consumed during the positive mood in- two separate studies. The aim of the first study was to construct the
duction when participants were offered both sweet and savoury Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale (PNEES). The aim of the
food compared to sweet or savoury food alone. Additionally, posi- second study was to assess the scale's predictive validity by testing
tive emotions have been linked to enhanced pleasantness of food whether the scores on a self-report emotional eating scale predict
and an increased motivation to enjoy food (Macht, 1999; Macht, overeating (eating episodes without loss of control) and binge
Roth, & Ellgring, 2002). eating (eating episodes with loss of control) episodes measured via
Although several self-report measures of emotional eating, e.g. experience sampling method. The following hypotheses were
the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, postulated for this study:
Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986) have been developed, it is less
clear how accurately the self-report measure of emotional eating 1) In comparison to positive emotional eating, negative emotional
mirrors real-life eating behaviour. Domoff, Meers, Koball, and eating is more strongly associated with binge eating.
Musher-Eizenman (2013) included 12 studies in their meta- 2) Negative emotional eating predicts overeating (without LoC)
analysis that compared self-reported emotional eating with food and binge eating (with LoC) episodes measured via experience
intake in laboratory settings. According to authors, self-report sampling method.
measures predicted actual food intake in emotional eaters only in 3) Positive emotional eating predicts overeating (without LoC)
five studies, e.g. after watching a sad film clip (Van Strien, Herman, episodes measured via experience sampling method.
H. Sultson et al. / Appetite 116 (2017) 423e430 425

2. Study 1 and binge eating is mediated by negative emotional eating.

2.1. Methods and materials


2.1.4. Results
2.1.1. Sample characteristics Exploratory factor analysis with principal axis factoring was
A web-based survey was conducted as the first step of a study of chosen to investigate the factor structure of the scale. Since the
affect regulation. The study design was approved by the Research correlation between factors exceeded 0.32, a non-orthogonal Pro-
Ethics Committee of the University of Tartu. Participants were max rotation was chosen (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). According to
recruited via university mailing lists in exchange for course credit Kaiser's criterion, two factors which eigenvalue >1 were extracted.
and online in exchange for personality-related feedback. The data A two-factor model showed good fit to the data, explaining 63% of
were collected during September 2013eMarch 2014. The initial total variance. Gradually extracting items of which communalities
sample consisted of 724 adults. Men (N ¼ 71) were excluded from remained below 0.45, a total of 19 items out of 30 remained. 12
the study due to low participation rate. Subsequently, women who items loaded onto the first factor, accounting for 40.1% of total
failed to fill in all questionnaires used in this study were also variance. Since the items reflected eating in response to various
excluded. The final sample consisted of 531 women aged 15e61 negative emotions, the factor was named Negative emotional
(M ¼ 29.57, SD ¼ 9.5). The results of the same study focusing on eating (PNEES-N). 7 items loaded onto the second factor, account-
several other eating behaviour measures and on the concept of ing for 22.9% of total variance. Second factor was named Positive
uncontrolled eating have been published in another paper (Vainik emotional eating (PNEES-P) because the items reflected eating in
et al., 2015). response to various positive emotions. Rotated factor loadings and
communalities of PNEES are presented in Table 1. The final version
2.1.2. Methods of the scale has been added as a Supplement.
2.1.2.1. Eating Disorders Assessment Scale (EDAS). Eating Disorders In order to assess internal reliability, Cronbach's a coefficients
Assessment Scale (a ¼ 0.92) (EDAS; Akkermann, 2010) is a 29-item were calculated for the total scale and both subscales. Reliability
self-report questionnaire that measures eating disorder symptoms analysis yielded good internal consistency coefficients for PNEES
characteristic of anorexia nervosa (AN), bulimia nervosa (BN), and (a ¼ 0.93), and for subscales PNEES-N (a ¼ 0.95), and PNEES-P
binge eating disorder (BED). The participant has to indicate on a six- (a ¼ 0.91), respectively. Next, Pearson's correlation analysis was
point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (never) to 5 (always) the used to investigate how positive and negative emotional eating
extent to which the item describes her eating behaviour during the relate to disordered eating, measured by EDAS. PNEES-N correlated
last three months. The scale consists of 4 subscales: Restrained significantly with EDAS total score, and with subscales Restrained
eating (a ¼ 0.85), Binge eating (a ¼ 0.86), Purging (a ¼ 0.84), and eating, Purging, Preoccupation with body image and body weight,
Preoccupation with body image and body weight (a ¼ 0.93). The and Binge eating. PNEES-P had significant correlations with EDAS
internal consistency statistics refer to values obtained within the total score and with all the subscales, except Purging. The corre-
current sample. Total scale and subscales Binge eating and Purging lation coefficients for PNEES and EDAS total and subscale scores are
have demonstrated good discriminant validity in discriminating presented in Table 2.
AN, BN and BED patients from each other. Subscale Restrained Since the two factors were moderately correlated (r ¼ 0.32), a
eating and Preoccupation with body image and body weight assess path analysis was conducted to see whether the effect of positive
the latent dimensions common to eating disorders. Construct val- emotional eating on binge eating is mediated by negative
idity of the scale has been confirmed by strong correlations be- emotional eating. Binge eating was entered into the model as the
tween EDAS and EDI-2 subscales that measure eating disorder dependent variable, PNEES-P as the independent variable, and
symptoms. PNEES-N as the mediator variable. The indirect effect was signifi-
cant (b ¼ 0.25, SE ¼ 0.03, p < 0.001). The direct effect was also
2.1.2.2. Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale (PNEES). A pre- significant (b ¼ 0.13, SE ¼ 0.03, p < 0.001). Positive emotional eating
liminary version of the Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale significantly predicted binge eating, even though the effect was
(PNEES) was constructed for this study. Emotions of the pre- partly mediated by negative emotional eating. Therefore, PNEES-P
liminary version were chosen based on the emotions incorporated predicted Binge eating independent of PNEES-N. This mediation
in the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, analysis explained a significant proportion of variance in Binge
& Tellegen, 1988), the emotions pertaining to binge eating in eating scores (R2 ¼ 0.66, F ¼ 346.69, p < 0.001). The model with
experimental studies, and the items related to emotional eating in significant path coefficients is presented in Fig. 1.
various emotional eating scales. The preliminary scale consisted of
30 items assessing the tendency to eat in response to various
positive (N ¼ 12) and negative (N ¼ 18) emotions. The participant 3. Study 2
was instructed to indicate on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging
from 0 (never) to 4 (very often) the extent to which the item de- 3.1. Methods and materials
scribes her eating behaviour. The preliminary version of the scale
has been added as a Supplement. 3.1.1. Sample characteristics
For Study 2, we used a subsample of the study by Kukk and
2.1.3. Data analyses Akkermann (2017). 60 female participants out of 97 with the
Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 20 and mean age of 21.6 (SD ¼ 2.85) who also filled in PNEES were included
the statistical computing R environment 3.2.3 were used for the in the current study. Participants were recruited via university
data analyses. A factor analysis using principal axis factoring with mailing lists, social media, and advertisements in local campuses
non-orthogonal Promax rotation was used to investigate the factor and libraries. The data was collected from fall 2014 until early
structure of PNEES, and Pearson's correlation analyses were used to spring 2015. The results of the original study focusing on the
assess correlations with EDAS total and subscale scores in SPSS. relationship between fluctuations in negative emotions and binge
Path analysis in R with “lavaan” package was conducted to inves- eating have been published in another paper (Kukk & Akkermann,
tigate whether the association between positive emotional eating 2017).
426 H. Sultson et al. / Appetite 116 (2017) 423e430

Table 1
Rotated factor loadings and communalities of the final version of PNEES (N ¼ 531).

Item Negative emotional eating Positive emotional eating Communalities

1. grumpy 0.86 0.12 0.76


2. upset 0.85 0.06 0.73
3. disappointed 0.84 0.15 0.74
4. irritated 0.83 0.06 0.70
5. tense or anxious 0.79 0.05 0.62
6. sad 0.79 0.13 0.64
7. helpless 0.78 0.18 0.65
8. restless 0.77 0.09 0.61
9. offended 0.76 0.17 0.60
10. angry 0.76 0.13 0.59
11. lonely 0.70 0.20 0.54
12. feeling guilty 0.69 0.18 0.51
13. active 0.12 0.83 0.70
14. excited about something 0.22 0.80 0.69
15. joyful 0.08 0.78 0.62
16. full of energy 0.08 0.78 0.60
17. fascinated about something 0.28 0.77 0.68
18. confident 0.05 0.72 0.51
19. content with myself 0.08 0.71 0.51

Note: Item loadings on the corresponding factor are presented in bold.

Table 2 3.1.2.2. Experience sampling method (ESM). To assess eating


Pearson's correlation coefficients between PNEES and EDAS total and subscale behaviour and emotional experience in natural environment,
scores. experience sampling method (ESM) was used. The experience
Scales Negative Positive sampling experiment was programmed into palmtop computers
emotional eating emotional eating (Handspring Visar Neo) using the free software iESP developed by
EDAS total 0.66* 0.29* Dr. Lisa Feldman Barrett and Daniel Barrett (http://www.
Restrained eating (EDAS) 0.23* 0.13* experience-sampling.org/esp/). The palmtop computers signaled
Purging (EDAS) 0.33* 0.08 randomly seven times per day (from 8:30 a.m. to 23:05 p.m.) for
Preoccupation with body image 0.52* 0.26*
three consecutive days. In order to minimize day of week effects
and body weight (EDAS)
Binge eating (EDAS) 0.81* 0.38* (Smyth et al., 2009), the study period was limited to Tuesday-
Thursday. Upon every signal, participants were asked to fill out a
Note: * - correlations are significant at p < 0.01.

Fig. 1. Standardized regression coefficients and their standard errors for the relationship between positive emotional eating and binge eating mediated by negative emotional
eating. Path a corresponds to the regression coefficient for the association between PNEES-P and PNEES-N. Path b corresponds to the regression coefficient for the association
between PNEES-N and Binge eating. The direct effect c’ is calculated by controlling for the mediator. * - regression coefficients are significant at p < 0.001.

3.1.2. Methods short questionnaire as quickly and as accurately as possible by


selecting appropriate answers on the screen of the palmtop com-
3.1.2.1. Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale (PNEES). The final puters. The questionnaire consisted of the assessment of various
version of the Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale (PNEES, emotions and eating episodes. The emotions vere selected based on
a ¼ 0.89) was used in this study. The scale consists of 19 items that previous literature on eating and emotions. The questionnaire was
measure eating in response to various positive and negative emo- in the same order at each trial.
tions. Participants were instructed to indicate on a five-point Lik- First, participants were instructed to indicate on a four-point
ert-type scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (very often) the extent to Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (to a large
which the item describes their eating behaviour. The scale has two extent) the extent to which the 16 emotion-related adjectives
subscales: Positive emotional eating (PNEES-P, a ¼ 0.80) and described their current emotional state (i.e., “I am feeling sad …”).
Negative emotional eating (PNEES-N, a ¼ 0.94), which comprise 7 Next, participants were asked whether they had experienced an
and 12 items, respectively.
H. Sultson et al. / Appetite 116 (2017) 423e430 427

overeating episode, and if so, whether they had experienced loss of consisted of overeating episodes. Therefore, we only tested a model
control during the eating episode. The overeating episode was where the number of overeating episodes was the dependent
defined as an eating episode where the participant consumed a variable.
relatively large amount of food within a brief period of time. Based on the observed significant correlation coefficients,
Overeating episodes involving loss of control were characterized as PNEES-P and NA were entered into the linear regression model as
binge eating (BE) episodes. Therefore, the number of overeating predictor variables, and the number of overeating episodes as the
episodes comprised eating episodes without loss of control, the dependent variable. According to the AIC criteria, the model with
number of BE episodes comprised eating episodes with loss of PNEES-P and NA as predictor variables showed best fit to the data
control. Participants were also explicitly instructed to ignore the (AIC ¼ 32.73) and explained a significant proportion of variance in
signal when it was inconvenient or dangerous to answer (e.g. while the number of overeating episodes (R2 ¼ 0.16, F(2,55) ¼ 5.31,
driving, showering). p ¼ 0.008). PNEES-P (b ¼ 0.32, SE ¼ 0.05, t ¼ 2.60, p ¼ 0.012)
significantly predicted the number of overeating episodes, whereas
3.1.3. Procedure the mean level of NA did not (p ¼ 0.116). As the association between
Participants were first instructed to complete a series of self- PNEES-N and overeating was not significant, PNEES-N as a pre-
report questionnaires, including PNEES, via Internet on the online dictor variable was not tested.
survey center of the Institute of Psychology. Next, participants In subsequent analyses, we included a subsample of women
signed up and attended a meeting where they were given further who experienced only overeating episodes (N ¼ 21) or both over-
information regarding the study. Participants completed the eating and BE episodes (N ¼ 31). Pearson's correlation analyses
informed consent form and were given palmtop computers for the were conducted to investigate how positive/negative emotional
three-day study period. At the meeting, participants received eating, and the mean level of positive/negative affect correlate with
further instructions on how to use palmtop computers. They were overeating episodes and the total number of overeating and BE
also reminded of the goals of the study and how to deal with any episodes in those two subsamples. We did not conduct any analyses
questions that might arise from the signaling of the palmtop on women who experienced only BE episodes due to a very small
computer. sample size. The results of the Pearson's correlation analyses are
presented in Table 4.
3.1.4. Data analyses As only PNEES-N had a significant correlation with the total
Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 20 and number of BE and overeating episodes in a subsample of 31 women,
the statistical computing R environment 3.2.3 were used for the a single linear regression model was tested. PNEES-N significantly
data analyses. Pearson's correlation analyses were conducted in predicted the total number of BE and overeating episodes (b ¼ 0.46,
SPSS to assess the associations between positive/negative SE ¼ 0.04, t ¼ 2.78, p ¼ 0.009), and explained a significant pro-
emotional eating, the mean level of positive/negative affect, and the portion of variance in the total number of BE and overeating epi-
number of binge and overeating episodes. For the mean level of sodes (R2 ¼ 0.21, F(1,29) ¼ 7.80, p ¼ 0.009).
positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA), mean scores of PA/NA
were calculated by taking the mean score of the responses given to 4. Discussion
the items measuring PA/NA in ESM experiment. Further, only
emotions that matched the emotions incorporated in PNEES were The aim of this paper was to construct an emotional eating scale
selected for further analyses. Thus, 12 emotions out of 16 were used measuring eating in response to both positive and negative emo-
to calculate the mean score of positive and negative affect. The tions and to assess its predictive validity. In study 1, the preliminary
division into PA and NA was verified via factor analysis. The mean version of the Positive-Negative Emotional Eating Scale (PNEES)
score of PA comprised the following emotions: excitement, joy, was constructed. In study 2, experience sampling method was used
satisfaction, confidence and motivation. The mean score of NA to test whether self-reported emotional eating predicts real-life
comprised guilt, irritation, sadness, anxiety, anger, loneliness, and eating behaviour. For this purpose, data regarding emotional
disappointment. Linear regression analyses with “QuantPsyc” experience, overeating and binge eating were collected.
package in R were conducted to assess whether positive/negative The first hypothesis that negative emotional eating is more
emotional eating and the mean level of negative affect predict the strongly associated with binge eating than positive emotional
number of overeating and BE episodes experienced during the eating was confirmed. PNEES-N correlated strongly with EDAS
three-day study period. subscale Binge eating. The strong correlation between the two
constructs is plausible, as emotional eating and binge eating have
3.1.5. Results been found to be significantly correlated among clinical and non-
The response rate for all measurement trials was 78.6%. In total, clinical samples (Poínhos, Oliveira, & Correia, 2013; Racine,
31 women out of 60 (56.7%) experienced overeating (without LoC) Culbert, Larson, & Klump, 2009; Van Strien et al., 2005). Further,
and/or BE episodes (with LoC) (range: 1e8). More specifically, 21 negative emotional eating has been found to be a good predictor of
women (35%) experienced only overeating episodes (range: 1e5), 3 binge eating onset among adolescent girls (Stice et al., 2002).
women experienced only BE episodes (5%) (range: 1e2), and 7 PNEES-N also had a strong correlation with EDAS total score, and
women (11.7%) experienced both overeating and BE episodes weak to moderate correlations with EDAS subscales Restrained
(range: 2e8). Subsequently, Pearson's correlation analyses were eating, Preoccupation with body image and body weight, and
conducted to investigate how positive/negative emotional eating, Purging. This further confirms the link between negative emotional
and positive/negative affect correlate with the total number of eating and general psychopathology underlying eating disorders
overeating and BE episodes, and BE and overeating episodes (Lindeman & Stark, 2001).
separately. The results of the Pearson's correlation analyses are Subscale Positive emotional eating also had a moderate corre-
presented in Table 3. lation with EDAS subscale Binge eating. This is in accordance with
The total number of overeating and BE episodes and the number the hypothesis, as the correlation between PNEES-P and Binge
of overeating episodes alone were highly correlated (r ¼ 0.94). As eating is lower than with PNEES-N. Subscale Positive emotional
there were only 10 participants who experienced BE episodes, the eating also correlated with EDAS total score and with its subscales
majority of the total number of overeating and BE episodes Restrained eating and Preoccupation with body image and body
428 H. Sultson et al. / Appetite 116 (2017) 423e430

Table 3
Pearson's correlation coefficients between positive/negative emotional eating, the mean level of positive/negative affect, and the total number of overeating and BE episodes,
BE episodes, and overeating episodes in the whole sample of women (N ¼ 60).

Measure PNEES-P PNEES-N PA NA Overeating and BE Overeating

PNEES-P e
PNEES-N 0.09 e
PA 0.13 -0.05 e
NA 0.13 0.26* -0.54** e
Overeating and BE 0.29* 0.21 -0.16 0.29* e
Overeating 0.35** 0.16 -0.09 0.26* 0.94** e
Binge eating 0.02 0.23 -0.24 0.25 0.69** 0.40**

Note: BE ¼ binge eating; PNEES-P ¼ Positive Emotional eating; PNEES-N ¼ Negative Emotional Eating; PA ¼ positive affect; NA ¼ negative affect. ** - correlations are significant
at p < 0.01, * - correlations are significant at p < 0.05.

Table 4 did not predict overeating and BE episodes within the whole
Pearson's correlation coefficients between positive/negative emotional eating, the sample. Nevertheless, within the subsample of women who expe-
mean level of positive/negative affect, and the number of overeating episodes in a rienced at least one overeating or BE episode, PNEES-N predicted
subsample of women (N ¼ 21), and the total number of BE and overeating episodes
the number of overeating and BE episodes. As overeating has been
in a subsample of women (N ¼ 31).
shown to be associated with emotional eating (Stojek et al., 2016;
Measure Overeating and BE Overeating Van Strien et al., 2005), our results further confirm the link be-
(N ¼ 31) (N ¼ 21)
tween negative emotional eating and overeating. Similarly, the
PNEES-P 0.35 0.29 association between emotional eating and BE in our study is in line
PNEES-N 0.46** 0.25
with the evidence that links emotional overeating with BE fre-
PA 0.00 0.18
NA 0.17 -0.08 quency (Masheb & Grilo, 2006) and severity (Ricca et al., 2009). The
fact that PNEES-N did not predict overeating or BE within the whole
Note: PNEES-P ¼ Positive Emotional eating; PNEES-N ¼ Negative Emotional Eating;
PA ¼ positive affect; NA ¼ negative affect. ** - correlations are significant at p < 0.01.
sample could point towards the fact that negative emotional eating
might be more characteristic of individuals with eating disorders or
severe eating pathology, as emotional eating itself has been asso-
weight. Furthermore, PNEES-P still predicted binge eating after ciated with eating disorder features and depression (Masheb &
controlling for the effect of PNEES-N. This is in line with previous Grilo, 2006).
studies that have demonstrated that positive affect can also elicit In addition, our results indicate that loss of control might also be
eating (Bongers, Jansen, Havermans, et al., 2013; Bongers, Jansen, an important feature of negative emotional eating. PNEES-N pre-
Houben, et al., 2013; Evers et al., 2013; Patel & Schlundt, 2001). dicted the total number of overeating and BE episodes within a
Although few authors have claimed that positive and negative subsample of women. When individuals with BE episodes were
emotional eating are different constructs (Nolan et al., 2010; Van excluded from the subsample, leaving only individuals with over-
Strien et al., 2013; Van Strien et al., 2016), we found a moderate eating episodes, PNEES-N was no longer associated with eating
positive correlation between the two factors. Thus, some in- episodes. In comparison to overeating, binge eating has been
dividuals tend to binge eat when they experience both positive and associated with larger amount of food, more distress and increased
negative emotions. There is some evidence that emotional eating guilt (Stein et al., 2007). Considering that binge eating is a signifi-
might not in fact reflect eating in response to various emotions. cant predictor of eating disorders (Latner, Hildebrandt, Rosewall,
Emotional eating might capture overeating in response to a variety Chisholm, & Hayashi, 2007) and that negative emotional eating
of food-related cues instead (Bongers et al., 2016; Evers et al., 2009). was associated with BE within the subsample of women with
Bongers et al. (2016) investigated whether emotional eaters in- overeating/BE, it is plausible that PNEES-N might be related to a
crease their food intake in response to negative emotional cues, or, more severe eating pathology.
alternatively, to food-related cues in general. They found that Even though PNEES-N had a strong correlation with EDAS sub-
increased food intake in emotional eaters following negative mood scale Binge eating in Study 1, the number of binge eating episodes
induction was also associated with increased intake in response to alone obtained via ESM in Study 2 were not correlated with
positive mood and exposure to food. Further, emotional eaters also negative emotional eating. This is contrary to our hypothesis. It is
tended to eat more following the control condition. Thus, plausible that due to the fact that in Study 2 there were only 10
emotional eating might not reflect purely overeating in response to women in our sample who had experienced at least one BE episode,
negative emotions, but rather overeating in response to a variety of we failed to find associations with negative emotional eating. In
food-related cues. addition, due to the small sample of women with BE episodes, we
Eating in response to positive emotions may also be related to did not further investigate the relationship between emotional
positive urgency. It has been found that individuals who report eating and BE among binge eaters. However it would be useful to
stronger levels of positive and negative urgency (proneness to act investigate the role of negative emotional eating among individuals
hastily in response to positive and negative emotions) experience with binge eating in the future.
more symptoms of addictive eating (Murphy, Stojek, & MacKillop, The third hypothesis postulated that positive emotional eating
2014). Further, positive and negative urgency have been found to predicts overeating episodes measured via ESM. This hypothesis
be positively correlated (Cyders & Smith, 2007), indicating that was confirmed, as PNEES-P significantly predicted overeating.
some individuals tend to act impulsively in response to both pos- PNEES-P was also associated with the total number of overeating
itive and negative emotions, possibly including the tendency to eat. and BE episodes. Thus, it is clear that positive emotions could also
The second hypothesis postulated that negative emotional evoke overeating. However, among overeaters and binge eaters,
eating predicts overeating (without LoC) and binge eating (with PNEES-N is a better predictor of the disturbed eating behaviour as
LoC) episodes measured via experience sampling method (ESM). we have previously discussed. Eating in response to various posi-
This hypothesis was partially confirmed. Negative emotional eating tive emotions could be less related to eating pathology, and more to
H. Sultson et al. / Appetite 116 (2017) 423e430 429

hedonic eating (Macht, 1999), as positive emotions have been analyses and interpretation of the results of Study 1, Andero Uus-
shown to enhance pleasantness of food and increase the motivation berg and Helen Uusberg for their help with data collection, and
to enjoy food (Macht, 1999; Macht et al., 2002). The associations Karin T€
aht for her consultation on the data analyses.
between hedonic eating and positive emotional eating could serve
as an important direction for future research. Appendix A. Supplementary data
There are few limitations to the study and several implications
for the future. To begin with, the sample size in Study 2 was rela- Supplementary data related to this article can be found at http://
tively small and the results from the regression analyses should be dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.035.
replicated in a larger sample. It is plausible that a larger population-
representative sample could have produced slightly different re-
sults. PNEES factor structure and relations with overeating and References
binge eating among men should also be investigated in future
Adam, T. C., & Epel, E. S. (2007). Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiology and
studies. Further, we did not differentiate between BE episodes Behavior, 91(4), 449e458. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011.
preceeding or following positive and negative affect as it is meth- Adriaanse, M., de Ridder, D., & Evers, C. (2011). Emotional eating: Eating when
odologically challenging. emotional or emotional about eating? Psychology & Health, 26(1), 23e39. http://
doi.org/10.1080/08870440903207627.
Few studies also suggest that difficulties in emotion regulation Akkermann, K. (2010). Serotonin-related biomarkers and symptoms of eating disor-
might even be a more important predictor of BE (Gianini, White, & ders. Doctoral thesis http://dspace.utlib.ee/dspace/handle/10062/15300.
Masheb, 2013) and emotional eating (Spoor, Bekker, van Strien, & American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders, V.
van Heck, 2007) than negative affect alone. For example, we have Arnow, B., Kenardy, J., & Agras, W. S. (1995). The emotional eating scale: The
demonstrated that fluctuations in negative emotions along with development of a measure to assess coping with negative affect by eating. In-
emotion regulation difficulties predicted BE among women (Kukk ternational Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 79e90.
Bongers, P., de Graaff, A., & Jansen, A. (2016). “Emotional” does not even start to
& Akkermann, 2017). Failing to successfully regulate one's emo- cover it: Generalization of overeating in emotional eaters. Appetite, 96, 611e616.
tions and therefore resorting to more maladaptive emotion regu- http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.004.
lation strategies, e.g. binge eating, has been proposed to be a Bongers, P., & Jansen, A. (2016). Emotional eating is not what you think it is and
emotional eating scales do not measure what you think they measure. Frontiers
mechanism underlying BE (Whiteside et al., 2007). Hence, it would in Psychology, 7, 1e11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01932.
be useful to study how the subscales of PNEES relate to emotion Bongers, P., Jansen, A., Havermans, R., Roefs, A., & Nederkoorn, C. (2013). Happy
regulation difficulties as well as to negative and positive urgency. eating. The underestimated role of overeating in a positive mood. Appetite, 67,
74e80. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.03.017.
Also, individual differences in appetitive responsiveness to
Bongers, P., Jansen, A., Houben, K., & Roefs, A. (2013). Happy eating: The single
rewarding properties of food (Lowe & Butryn, 2007) should be target implicit association test predicts overeating after positive emotions.
examined regarding its association with positive emotional eating. Eating Behaviors, 14(3), 348e355. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.06.007.
As obesity has been associated with altered reward sensitivity Bourdier, L., Lalanne, C., Morvan, Y., Kern, L., Romo, L., & Berthoz, S. (2017). Vali-
dation and factor structure of the French-Language version of the Emotional
regarding food stimuli (Castellanos et al., 2009; Volkow, Wang, & Appetite Questionnaire (EMAQ). Frontiers in Psychology, 8.
Baler, 2011), it would be useful to study the relation between Cardi, V., Leppanen, J., & Treasure, J. (2015). The effects of negative and positive
PNEES-P and hedonic eating within obese population. Specifically, mood induction on eating behaviour: A meta-analysis of laboratory studies in
the healthy population and eating and weight disorders. Neuroscience & Bio-
positive emotional eating could serve as a potential risk factor for behavioral Reviews, 57, 299e309. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.011.
gaining weight. Castellanos, E. H., Charboneau, E., Dietrich, M. S., Park, S., Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K.,
In conclusion, we constructed the Positive-Negative Emotional et al. (2009). Obese adults have visual attention bias for food cue images: Ev-
idence for altered reward system function. International Journal of Obesity
Eating Scale that showed good fit to the data and demonstrated (2005), 33(9), 1063e1073. http://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2009.138.
good psychometric properties. We further examined the predictive Crosby, R. D., Wonderlich, S. A., Engel, S. G., Simonich, H., Smyth, J., & Mitchell, J. E.
validity of the scale by investigating how positive emotional eating (2009). Daily mood patterns and bulimic behaviors in the natural environment.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(3), 181e188. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.
and negative emotional eating predict overeating and BE episodes
2008.11.006.
in the natural environment. We found that both subscales showed Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2007). Mood-based rash action and its components:
good predictive validity as the self-reported positive and emotional Positive and negative urgency. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4),
839e850. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.008.
eating both predicted real-life eating behaviour measured via ESM.
Domoff, S. E., Meers, M. R., Koball, A. M., & Musher-Eizenman, D. R. (2013). The
Specifically, we found that positive emotional eating predicted validity of the Dutch eating behavior questionnaire: Some critical remarks. In
overeating episodes within the whole sample, whereas negative Eating and weight disorders e studies on anorexia, bulimia and obesity.
emotional eating predicted overeating and BE within a subsample Switzerland 2013: Springer International Publishing.
Evers, C., Adriaanse, M., de Ridder, D. T. D., & de Witt Huberts, J. C. (2013). Good
of women who had experienced at least one overeating or BE mood food. Positive emotion as a neglected trigger for food intake. Appetite, 68,
episode. Our results suggest that positive and negative emotional 1e7. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.007.
eating might have different associations with overeating and binge Evers, C., de Ridder, D. T. D., & Adriaanse, M. A. (2009). Assessing yourself as an
emotional eater: Mission impossible? Health Psychology: Official Journal of the
eating, as the latter might be related to a more severe eating Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association, 28(6),
pathology. 717e725. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0016700.
Gianini, L. M., White, A. M., & Masheb, R. M. (2013). Eating pathology, emotion
regulation, and emotional overeating in obese adults with binge eating disor-
Conflict of interest der. Eating Behaviors, 14, 309e313.
Gold, P. W., & Chrousos, G. P. (2002). Organization of the stress system and its
There is no conflict of interest for any of the authors. dysregulation in melancholic and atypical depression: High vs low CRH/NE
states. Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 254e275. http://doi.org/10.1038/sj/mp/4001032.
Haedt-Matt, A. A., Keel, P. K., Racine, S. E., Burt, S. A., Hu, J. Y.,
Role of the funding source Boker, S., … Klump, K. L. (2014). Do emotional eating urges regulate affect?
Concurrent and prospective associations and implications for risk models of
binge eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47(8), 874e877.
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
Hektner, J. M., Schmidt, J. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Experience sampling
agencies in the public, commercial, or non-profit sectors. method: Measuring the quality of everyday life. Sage.
Kukk, K., & Akkermann, K. (2017). Fluctuations in negative emotions predict binge
Acknowledgements eating both in women and men: An experience sampling study. Eating Disor-
ders, 25(1), 65e79.
Latner, J. D., Hildebrandt, T., Rosewall, J. K., Chisholm, A. M., & Hayashi, K. (2007).
We would like to thank Uku Vainik for his assistance on the data Loss of control over eating reflects eating disturbances and general
430 H. Sultson et al. / Appetite 116 (2017) 423e430

psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(9), 2203e2211. Mitchell, J. E., et al. (2009). Ecological momentary assessment of affect, stress,
Lindeman, M., & Stark, K. (2001). Emotional eating and eating disorder psychopa- and binge-purge behaviors: Day of week and time of day effects in the natural
thology. Eating Disorders, 9(October), 251e259. http://doi.org/10.1080/ environment. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 42(5), 429e436.
10640260127552. Spoor, S. T. P., Bekker, M. H. J., van Strien, T., & van Heck, G. L. (2007). Relations
Lowe, M. R., & Butryn, M. L. (2007). Hedonic hunger: A new dimension of appetite? between negative affect, coping, and emotional eating. Appetite, 48, 368e376.
Physiology and Behavior, 91(4), 432e439. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007. Stein, R. I., Kenardy, J., Wiseman, C. V., Dounchis, J. Z., Arnow, B. A., & Wilfley, D. E.
04.006. (2007). What's driving the binge in binge eating disorder?: A prospective ex-
Macht, M. (1999). Characteristics of eating in anger, fear, sadness and joy. Appetite, amination of precursors and consequences. International Journal of Eating Dis-
33(1), 129e139. http://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1999.0236. orders, 40(3), 195e203.
Macht, M. (2008). How emotions affect eating: A five-way model. Appetite, 50(1), Stice, E., Presnell, K., & Spangler, D. (2002). Risk factors for binge eating onset in
1e11. adolescent girls: A 2-year prospective investigation. Health Psychology, 21(2),
Macht, M., Roth, S., & Ellgring, H. (2002). Chocolate eating in healthy men during 131.
experimentally induced sadness and joy. Appetite, 39, 147e158. Stojek, M. M. K., Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Shomaker, L. B., Kelly, N. R., Thompson, K. A.,
Macht, M., & Simons, G. (2000). Emotions and eating in everyday life. Appetite, Mehari, R. D., … Yanovski, J. A. (2016). Associations of adolescent emotional and
35(1), 65e71. http://doi.org/10.1006/appe.2000.0325. loss of control eating with 1-year changes in disordered eating, weight, and
Majzoub, J. A. (2006). Corticotropin-releasing hormone physiology. European Jour- adiposity. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 1e10. http://doi.org/10.
nal of Endocrinology, 155, S71eS76. http://doi.org/10.1530/eje.1.02247. 1002/eat.22636.
Masheb, R. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2006). Emotional overeating and its associations with Stone, A. A., Hufford, M. R., & Shiffman, S. (2008). Ecological momentary assess-
eating disorder psychopathology among overweight patients with binge eating ment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 1e32. http://doi.org/10.1146/
disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 39(2), 141e146. http://doi.org/ annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415.
10.1002/eat.20221. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Upper Saddle
Meyer, C., Waller, G., & Waters, A. (1998). Emotional states and bulimic psychopa- River, NJ: Pearson Allyn & Bacon.
thology. The Neurobiological Basis of Eating Disorders, 271e289. Vainik, U., Neseliler, S., Konstabel, K., Fellows, L. K., & Dagher, A. (2015). Eating traits
Murphy, C. M., Stojek, M. K., & MacKillop, J. (2014). Interrelationships among questionnaires as a continuum of a single concept. Uncontrolled eating. Appe-
impulsive personality traits, food addiction, and Body Mass Index. Appetite, 73, tite, 90, 229e239. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.004.
45e50. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.008. Van Strien, T., Cebolla, A., Etchemendy, E., Gutie rrez-Maldonado, J., Ferrer-
Nolan, L. J., Halperin, L. B., & Geliebter, A. (2010). Emotional appetite questionnaire. García, M., Botella, C., et al. (2013). Emotional eating and food intake after
Construct validity and relationship with BMI. Appetite, 54(2), 314e319. http:// sadness and joy. Appetite, 66, 20e25. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.02.016.
doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.12.004. Van Strien, T., Donker, M. H., & Ouwens, M. A. (2016). Is desire to eat in response to
Oliver, G., Wardle, J., & Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and food choice: A laboratory positive emotions an “obese” eating style: Is Kummerspeck for some people a
study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 853e865. misnomer? Appetite, 100, 225e235. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.035.
Patel, K. A., & Schlundt, D. G. (2001). Impact of moods and social context on eating Van Strien, T., Engels, R. C. M. E., van Leeuwe, J., & Snoek, H. M. (2005). The Stice
behavior. Appetite, 36(2), 111e118. http://doi.org/10.1006/appe.2000.0385. model of overeating: Tests in clinical and non-clinical samples. Appetite, 45(3),
Poínhos, R., Oliveira, B. M. P. M., & Correia, F. (2013). Eating behaviour patterns and 205e213. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.08.004.
BMI in Portuguese higher education students. Appetite, 71, 314e320. http://doi. Van Strien, T., Frijetrs, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch
org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.024. Eating Behaviour Questionnaire for assessment of restrained, emotional and
Pollert, G. A., Engel, S. G., Schreiber-Gregory, D. N., Crosby, R. D., Cao, L., external eating behaviour. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5(2),
Wonderlich, S. A., … Mitchell, J. E. (2013). The role of eating and emotion in 295e315.
binge eating disorder and loss of control eating. International Journal Of Eating Van Strien, T., Herman, C. P., Anschutz, D. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., & de Weerth, C.
Disorders, 46(3), 233e238. (2012). Moderation of distress-induced eating by emotional eating scores.
Racine, S. E., Culbert, K. M., Larson, C. L., & Klump, K. L. (2009). The possible in- Appetite, 58(1), 277e284. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.10.005.
fluence of impulsivity and dietary restraint on associations between serotonin Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., & Baler, R. D. (2011). Reward, dopamine and the control of
genes and binge eating. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(16), 1278e1286. food intake: Implications for obesity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 37e46.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.05.002. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.001.
Ricca, V., Castellini, G., Lo Sauro, C., Ravaldi, C., Lapi, F., Mannucci, E., … Faravelli, C. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
(2009). Correlations between binge eating and emotional eating in a sample of measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Person-
overweight subjects. Appetite, 53(3), 418e421. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet. ality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063e1070. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
2009.07.008. 3514.54.6.1063.
Rutters, F., Nieuwenhuizen, A. G., Lemmens, S. G., Born, J. M., & Westerterp- Wegner, K. E., Smyth, J. M., Crosby, R. D., Wittrock, D., Wonderlich, S. A., &
Plantenga, M. S. (2009). Acute stress-related changes in eating in the absence of Mitchell, J. E. (2002). An evaluation of the relationship between mood and
hunger. Obesity, 17(1), 72e77. http://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.493. binge eating in the natural environment using ecological momentary assess-
Smyth, J. M., Wonderlich, S. A., Heron, K. E., Sliwinski, M. J., Crosby, R. D., ment. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 32(3), 352e361. http://doi.org/10.
Mitchell, J. E., et al. (2007). Daily and momentary mood and stress are associ- 1002/eat.10086.
ated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia nervosa patients in the natural Whiteside, U., Chen, E., Neighbors, C., Hunter, D., Lo, T., & Larimer, M. (2007). Dif-
environment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(4), 629e638. ficulties regulating emotions: Do binge eaters have fewer strategies to modu-
Smyth, J. M., Wonderlich, S. A., Sliwinski, M. J., Crosby, R. D., Engel, S. G., late and tolerate negative affect? Eating Behaviors, 8, 162e169.

You might also like