Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Journal of

Experimental
Social Psychology
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 508–515
www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp

The effect of self-attribute relevance on how self-esteem


moderates attitude change in dissonance processesq
Jeff Stonea,* and Joel Cooperb
a
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85715, USA
b
Department of Psychology, Green Hall, Princeton University, Princeton , NJ 08544, USA
Received 10 May 2002; revised 3 September 2002

Abstract

An experiment was conducted to examine the conditions under which self-esteem operates as an expectancy, as a resource, or
does not influence cognitive dissonance processes. Based on the self-standards model of dissonance (Stone & Cooper, 2001), it was
predicted that following a high-choice counter-attitudinal behavior: (a) priming positive self-attributes that were relevant to the
discrepant behavior would cause participants with high self-esteem to report more attitude change as compared to participants with
low self-esteem, (b) priming positive self-attributes that were irrelevant to the behavior would cause participants with high self-
esteem to report less attitude change as compared to participants with low self-esteem, and (c) priming neutral self-attributes would
eliminate self-esteem moderation of attitude change. The results of the attitude change measure supported the predictions. The
discussion explores different processes by which the accessibility of cognitions about the self mediate dissonance arousal and
reduction.
Ó 2003 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.

Keywords: Cognitive dissonance; Attitude change; Self-esteem; Self-affirmation; Self-consistency; Self-standards

Since the introduction of the theory of cognitive self-relevant thought can provide more affirmational
dissonance (Festinger, 1957), several theories have been resources to people with high self-esteem relative to
proposed to explain how self-esteem and cognitions people with low self-esteem, suggesting that positive
about the self influence the arousal and reduction of cognitions about the self cause people to be less vul-
cognitive dissonance. One contemporary theory pro- nerable to dissonance processes following a discrepant
poses that cognitions about the self function as re- behavior.
sources for dissonance reduction (e.g., Aronson, Cohen, Other theory and research, however, indicates that
& Nail, 1999; Steele, Spencer, & Lynch, 1993; Tesser, positive cognitions about the self makes people with
2000). Resource models maintain that for self-relevant high self-esteem more vulnerable to dissonance pro-
thought to reduce psychological discomfort, people cesses. According to self-consistency theory (Aronson,
must bring to mind more positive than negative self- 1968; Thibodeau & Aronson, 1992), cognitions about
attributes following a discrepant act (Steele & Lui, 1983; the self can represent standards or expectancies for be-
Tesser & Cornell, 1991). Resource models further as- havior. Following a counter-attitudinal behavior, people
sume that people with high self-esteem possess more with high self-esteem, who hold more positive expec-
positive attributes in their self-concept than people with tancies for themselves, are more likely to perceive a
low self-esteem (Spencer, Josephs, & Steele, 1993). Thus, discrepancy between their behavior and their self-ex-
pectancies. They are subsequently more likely to feel
q
We thank John Bargh for his insightful comments about the discomfort and be motivated to use a self-justification
priming manipulations used in this research. We also gratefully strategy. In contrast, people with low self-esteem, who
acknowledge Jason Chism, Jeremey Baver, Jaymonde Errico, and Erin
Atkinson for their help in collecting the data reported in this paper.
presumably hold less positive expectancies for their be-
*
Corresponding author. Fax: 1-520-621-9306. havior, may not perceive the same behavior to be dis-
E-mail address: jeffs@u.arizona.edu (J. Stone). crepant from their negative self-expectancies. As a

0022-1031/$ - see front matter Ó 2003 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00018-0
J. Stone, J. Cooper / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 508–515 509

result, they should experience less discomfort following erations of what is foolish or immoral—standards that
a discrepant act. Research in support of the self-con- are connected to individual representations of the self.
sistency prediction shows that following a discrepant The SSM maintains that only the use of personal stan-
behavior, people with positive self-expectancies (Aron- dards in the assessment of behavior—those that relate to
son & Carlsmith, 1962; Brockner, Wiesenfeld, & Ras- idiosyncratic self-expectancies—will cause self-esteem
kas, 1993), or high self-esteem (Gibbons, Eggleston, & differences in dissonance arousal. Furthermore, once
Benthin, 1997; Glass, 1964; Maracek & Mettee, 1972; dissonance is aroused, the SSM predicts that bringing to
Stone, 1999) reported more attitude or behavior change mind certain aspects of the self can influence the need
than people with negative expectancies or low self-es- to justify behavior, or use self-knowledge as a resource
teem. This suggests that, under some conditions, posi- to reduce discomfort. The moderating role of self-esteem
tive cognitions about the self can also exacerbate the in dissonance reduction depends upon whether the
discomfort some people feel when they commit a dis- cognitions about the self are positive, self-descriptive,
crepant act. and related to the behavioral discrepancy. Thus, the
The purpose of the research in this paper was to test a SSM provides a framework from which to predict when
new model designed to address the seemingly paradoxi- and how cognitions about the self will moderate disso-
cal role of self-esteem and cognitions about the self in nance processes (Stone & Cooper, 2001).
dissonance processes. In recent papers describing the Recent research testing the SSM indicates that the
Self-Standards Model of cognitive dissonance (SSM, see standards people use to interpret and evaluate a dis-
Stone, 2001; Stone & Cooper, 2001), we proposed that crepant act influences when self-esteem moderates dis-
the different theoretical perspectives on cognitive disso- sonance processes (Stone, in press). For example, in one
nance essentially describe a variety of processes by which experiment, participants with high versus low self-es-
people interpret and evaluate their behavior. The various teem wrote a counter-attitudinal essay. To prime self-
perspectives differ, however, because each makes a spe- standards, participants then examined a list of positive,
cific assumption about the type of information people negative, and neutral traits (e.g., competent, irrational,
use to interpret and evaluate a given act. Theories like and average). Some participants were directed to circle
self-affirmation (Steele, 1988) and self-consistency the traits that represented their personal standards for
(Aronson, 1968) assume that cognitions about the self behavior, whereas others circled the traits that repre-
represent the default criteria for judgment, whereas other sented the normative standards for behavior (partici-
theories assume that non-self related cognitions, such as pants in a high- and low-choice control condition did
specific attitudes (Harmon-Jones, Brehm, Greenberg, not view the trait lists). The attitude change measure
Simon, & Nelson, 1996) or behavioral consequences showed that in the low-choice control condition, high-
(Cooper & Fazio, 1984) represent the default informa- and low-self-esteem participants showed less attitude
tion used in the assessment of behavior. Consequently, change compared to high and low self-esteem partici-
each theory makes different predictions regarding the pants in the high-choice control condition. Moreover,
role of self-esteem (and the cognitions about the self that when normative standards were primed, high self-esteem
it represents) in the dissonance process, because each and low self-esteem participants showed equal levels of
assumes that different types of information are regularly attitude change. However, when primed for their per-
brought to mind when people assess their behavior and sonal standards for behavior, participants with high self-
then attempt to cope with their discomfort. esteem showed significantly more attitude change than
Our new theoretical model proposes that the assess- participants with low self-esteem, whose attitude change
ment of behavior is more malleable than has been pre- scores were not significantly different than the low-
viously recognized. The SSM holds that people can use choice control groups. Thus, as predicted by the SSM,
important attitudes, beliefs, or self-knowledge to un- self-esteem moderated dissonance-induced attitude
derstand the meaning of their behavior, but which cri- change only when personal self-standards were primed
teria people use depends upon the type of information in the context of the discrepant act. When primed to
that is brought to mind by cues in the situation. Once consider the normative standards for their behavior, or
they have acted, people evaluate their behavior against a when no standard was directly primed, both self-esteem
standard of judgment, and that standard of judgment groups showed the same level of dissonance-induced
may or may not relate to a cognitive representation of attitude change.
the self. For example, the evaluation of behavior may be The research in this paper was designed to further test
based on its relationship to a specific attitude or belief SSM predictions concerning how self-esteem moderates
(Harmon-Jones et al., 1996), or the assessment of be- dissonance processes. According to the model, once
havior may be based on generally shared, normative people perceive that their behavior deviates from an
considerations of what is foolish or immoral (Cooper & important personal or normative standard, they will
Fazio, 1984). However, the assessment of behavior may experience discomfort and be motivated to reduce it. But
also be based on personal, idiographically held consid- how they reduce their discomfort depends upon the
510 J. Stone, J. Cooper / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 508–515

cognitions about the self that are accessible in the con- view themselves as falling short of the conventional
text. If no further self-relevant thought occurs, the dis- standards for behavior, should perceive that the same
crepancy will remain salient and people will seek behavior is more consistent with their negative expecta-
justification of their behavior (e.g., attitude change). tions, and this congruous information will reduce their
However, if new positive cognitions about the self are need to justify the discrepancy. Thus, when the salience
made accessible in the context, then the strategy for of relevant self-attributes activate self-expectancies, self-
dissonance reduction turns on the relevance of the self- esteem will moderate dissonance reduction via self-con-
attributes to the behavioral discrepancy. sistency processes.
The SSM predictions concerning how the relevance Conversely, if cues in the situation make accessible
of positive cognitions moderates the role of self-esteem positive attributes that are irrelevant to the discrepant
in dissonance reduction derives from research by act, the SSM predicts that people with high self-esteem
Aronson, Blanton, and Cooper (1995) and Blanton, will report less attitude change than people with low
Cooper, Skurnik, and Aronson (1997) in which partici- self-esteem. This prediction is based on the resource
pants wrote an ‘‘uncompassionate’’ essay. In one ex- model assumption that people with high self-esteem
periment (Aronson et al., 1995), when subsequently possess more positive attributes in their self-concept
allowed the opportunity to read positive feedback on than people with low self-esteem (Spencer et al., 1993).
self-attributes that were related (e.g., ‘‘compassion’’) As a result, self-relevant thought provides more affir-
and unrelated (e.g., ‘‘creative’’) to the essay, participants mational resources to people with high self-esteem.
chose to avoid the positive feedback about attributes People with low self-esteem, in contrast, do not perceive
that were relevant to the discrepant behavior; they fo- positive attributes to be as highly self-descriptive (see
cused instead on the positive feedback that was unre- Brown, 1998). If they are less likely to perceive positive
lated to their discrepant behavior. Another study attributes as applying to them, then making irrelevant
(Blanton et al., 1997) provided participants with either positive attributes salient in the context should not
positive relevant or irrelevant feedback following an provide them with resources to use for dissonance re-
uncompassionate advocacy. When told they were duction. As a result, people with low self-esteem should
‘‘highly compassionate’’ individuals, participants use attitude change to reduce their discomfort (Steele
showed significantly more attitude change relative to et al., 1993). This leads to the prediction that when
participants in a no feedback high-choice control con- irrelevant attributes are salient in the situation, self-es-
dition. In contrast, when told they were ‘‘highly crea- teem will moderate dissonance reduction through self-
tive’’ individuals, participants showed significantly less affirmation processes.
attitude change compared to high-choice control par- The present experiment was designed to test the
ticipants. These data suggest that in order for positive predicted interaction between self-esteem and self-attri-
self-attributes to serve as resources for dissonance re- bute relevance on dissonance induced attitude change.
duction, they must shift processing away from the rele- In some conditions of the experiment, participants with
vant standards for behavior. Otherwise, thinking about high or low self-esteem completed a counter-attitudinal
positive self-attributes that are relevant to the discrep- essay under conditions of high choice. They then com-
ancy exacerbates the need to justify behavior (Stone & pleted a task designed to prime either positive self-at-
Cooper, 2001). tributes that were relevant to the discrepant essay, or
The SSM further proposes that the effects of relevant positive self-attributes that were irrelevant to the dis-
versus irrelevant positive self-attributes on dissonance crepant essay. Also included in the design were high-
reduction can be moderated by self-esteem. Specifically, and low-choice control conditions in which participants
when relevant positive attributes are salient, people with were primed for neutral self-attributes. After the prim-
high self-esteem will experience more dissonance, and ing task, participants then reported their attitudes to-
report more attitude change, than people with low self- ward the essay topic. It was predicted that choice, and
esteem. The model assumes that because of their rela- not self-esteem, would moderate attitude change in the
tionship to the discrepant behavior, relevant positive neutral prime conditions (Cooper & Duncan, 1971), but
attributes bring to mind self-expectancies—the cognitive that self-esteem would interact with the priming ma-
representation of how well an individual upholds the nipulation to influence attitude change when self-attri-
conventional standards for behavior. Based on self- bute relevance was varied.
consistency theory (Aronson, 1968), people with high
self-esteem, who think they typically match the conven-
tional standards for behavior, should perceive that their Method
behavior is discrepant from their positive expectancy.
The perceived discrepancy should then cause discomfort Participants. Participants were 155 undergraduates
and motivate dissonance reduction via attitude change. at the University of Arizona who participated in the
In contrast, people with low self-esteem, who tend to experiment for course credit. All had been pretested for
J. Stone, J. Cooper / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 508–515 511

their level of self-esteem using the Rosenberg self-esteem ous research had shown that a good way to collect
measure (1979) during a mass pretest session of the opinions and beliefs about a topic was to instruct peo-
participant pool. As in previous research on self-esteem ple, no matter how they felt personally, to make argu-
and dissonance (Maracek & Mettee, 1972; Steele et al., ments on only one side of the issue. At this point, the
1993), self-esteem was treated as a categorical variable in instructions for executing the uncompassionate behavior
the design and analysis, and only those with scores varied as a function of the choice manipulation.
falling in the upper (i.e., scores greater than 34) and Choice manipulation. Participants randomly assigned
lower (i.e., scores less than 30) 30th percentile of the to the high-choice conditions were told that the decision
Rosenberg scale were recruited. In addition, attitudes to argue for the decrease was up to them, but the re-
toward the essay topic (see below) were measured in the search currently needed ‘‘strong forceful arguments’’ in
pretest session using a 10 pt scale with the endpoints favor of the decrease. Participants in the low-choice
‘‘strongly disagree’’ (1) and ‘‘strongly agree’’ (10). Only condition were told that in order to complete the study,
those who reported negative attitudes toward the topic they needed to write ‘‘strong forceful arguments’’ in
(those who scored less than or equal to 5 on the scale) favor of the decrease.
were recruited (Elliot & Devine, 1994). A total of 82 On the next page of the packet was a letter ostensibly
with high self-esteem and 73 with low self-esteem com- from the Committee for Undergraduate Education. The
pleted the procedures described below.1 letter stated that the committee might read their essay in
Procedure. Participants were contacted by phone and order to gauge student opinion about potential cuts in
invited to participate in two short studies on language the university budget for the next academic year (Coo-
and cognition. They participated in groups of 1–3 but per & Fazio, 1984). The final page provided the space
completed the materials individually in a private cubicle. for them to write their essay and instructed them to
The experimenter (who was unaware of the hypothesis begin with the statement ‘‘The U of A should decrease
and level of self-esteem) explained that the first study funding toward facilities and services for people with
was designed to measure ‘‘how people think about po- physical disabilities on campus because. . .’’ When they
litical policy issues.’’ Participants were then assigned to completed the essay, they were instructed to place it in
a cubicle and provided with the study packet. The an envelop and alert the experimenter.
written instructions in the packet explained that the Priming manipulation. After they completed their
study intended to measure the relationship between uncompassionate essay, the experimenter announced
cognition and evaluation of political policy by having that the first study was over and that they would now
participants express their opinions and beliefs about complete the second study on language and cognition.
different campus policy issues. The instructions in- He then introduced a modified scrambled sentence test
formed participants that the issue currently under in- designed to prime relevant, irrelevant, or neutral self-
vestigation concerned a proposed decrease in funding attributes (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996). In each
for handicapped services at the university for the next condition, the task consisted of eight blocks of words,
academic year (Blanton et al., 1997). Ostensibly, previ- with each block containing one distracter word and the
target words. Participants were told the task consisted of
1
The assumption that participants with high and low self-esteem a group of words that could be unscrambled to form a
would have different underlying self-views was tested in the current grammatically correct sentence. However, one word did
participant sample. In a mass pretest, 1487 introductory psychology
students at the University of Arizona completed the Rosenberg self-
not belong in the word group and should be omitted
esteem scale (1979) and the Personal Attributes Questionnaire (or from the sentence they would form. The experimenter
PAQ, Pelham & Swann, 1989). They rated the self-descriptiveness of then showed them an example at the top of the page.
the attributes of compassion and creativity on a 10 pt scale ranging Participants were told their goal was to complete the
from 1 (exceptionally low) to 10 (exceptionally high). They also used sentence scrambles as quickly as possible.
the same scale to rate the certainty and importance of their self-
descriptions. Participants were classified into high ((N ¼ 836) and low
The relevant self-attribute prime condition was de-
self-esteem (N ¼ 418) groups using the upper and lower 30th percen- signed to make accessible self-cognitions that were rel-
tiles. MANOVA analyses showed that participants with high self- evant to the uncompassionate essay. The target
esteem rated compassion (M ¼ 7:87) and creativity (M ¼ 6:98) as sentences were ‘‘I am a compassionate person,’’ ‘‘I try to
significantly more self-descriptive compared to participants with low be thoughtful,’’ ‘‘Helping people is important (to prime
self-esteem (Ms ¼ 7:62 and 6.56, respectively), F ð2; 1251Þ ¼ 8:04,
p < :0003. Furthermore, those with high self-esteem were significantly
ÔhelpfulÕ),’’ and ‘‘I want to be considerate.’’ In the ir-
more certain of their ratings on these traits (Ms ¼ 7:39 and 6.94, relevant self-attribute prime condition, the target sen-
respectively) than participants with low self-esteem (Ms ¼ 6:86 and tences were ‘‘I am a creative person,’’ ‘‘I try to be
6.42), F ð2; 1251Þ ¼ 16:60, p < :0001. However, both groups rated the imaginative,’’ ‘‘Teaching people is important (to prime
traits as important to possess (high self-esteem Ms ¼ 7:53 and 6.48, ÔintelligentÕ),’’ and ‘‘I want to be flexible.’’ In the neutral
respectively; low self-esteem Ms ¼ 7:34 and 6.56, respectively),
F ð2; 1251Þ ¼ 2:02, p < :11. Thus, the self-knowledge structure of
self-attribute prime condition the sentences were ‘‘I am a
participants with high and low self-esteem differed as assumed by the punctual person,’’ ‘‘I try to be quiet,’’ ‘‘Believing people
SSM. is important (to prime ÔtrustingÕ),’’ and ‘‘I want to be
512 J. Stone, J. Cooper / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 508–515

cautious.’’ All priming conditions contained four filler


sentences like ‘‘Sharpen a pencil’’ and ‘‘A phone rings.’’
Once participants completed the priming task, they
alerted the experimenter who then collected the primary
dependent measures.2
Dependent measures. The experimenter returned and
claimed that he forgot to give them a questionnaire
during the essay task. He handed participants a ques-
tionnaire with the statement ‘‘The U of A should de-
crease funding toward facilities and services for people
with physical disabilities on campus.’’ Participants re-
sponded by circling a number on a 10 pt scale with the
endpoints ‘‘Strongly agree’’ (1) to ‘‘Strongly disagree’’ Fig. 1. The effects of self-esteem and experimental condition on atti-
(10). On the next page, participants were asked to tude change. LC, low choice and HC, high choice. Higher scores in-
complete a check of the choice manipulation by indi- dicate more change in the direction of support for a decrease in
cating their agreement with the statement, ‘‘I felt free to funding for handicapped services. Means with different superscripts
differ significantly at p < :05 using FisherÕs LSD test.
decline to write an essay for the opinion survey today’’
on an 10 pt scale ranging from ‘‘strongly disagree’’ (1) to meaning of the omnibus interaction between self-esteem
‘‘strongly agree’’ (10). The experimenter left participants and the experimental conditions.
alone to complete each questionnaire, and once they As expected, an ANOVA conducted on the attitude
alerted him, he collected the materials and announced change data in the neutral self-attributes prime condi-
that the study was complete. All participants were then tion revealed a significant main effect for choice,
fully debriefed about the purposes for the study. F ð1; 147Þ ¼ 8:15, p < :005 but no main or interaction
effect for self-esteem, both F s < 1. When neutral self-
attributes were primed, high-choice participants tended
Results to justify their advocacy more (M ¼ 2:64, SD ¼ 2:27)
than did low-choice control participants (M ¼ 1:25,
Choice manipulation check. To test the effectiveness of SD ¼ 1:85) and the effect was not moderated by self-
the choice manipulation, a planned interaction contrast esteem (Cooper & Duncan, 1971).
was conducted that compared the perception of choice A planned interaction contrast between self-esteem
reported by participants with high or low self-esteem in and the relevant versus irrelevant priming conditions on
the three high-choice conditions against the low-choice the attitude change scores showed that attitude change
control condition. The analysis showed a significant was significantly moderated by self-esteem when the
main effect for condition, F ð1; 151Þ ¼ 5:98, p < :01, relevance of the self-attributes was varied, simple inter-
and no main or interaction effects for self-esteem (both action F ð1; 147Þ ¼ 9:10, p < :005. As seen in Fig. 1, a
F ’s < 1). Participants in the high-choice conditions ex- planned comparison showed that as predicted, when
perienced greater choice (M ¼ 7:19) than did partici- relevant attributes were primed, those with low self-es-
pants in the low-choice condition (M ¼ 5:85). teem reported less attitude change (M ¼ 1:42, SD ¼ 1:98)
Attitude change. The degree of attitude change was compared to participants with high self-esteem (M ¼
computed by subtracting the attitude scores collected 2:71, SD ¼ 1:74), F ð1; 147Þ ¼ 3:99, p < :05. In con-
during the pretest session from those collected after the trast, when irrelevant positive attributes were primed,
essay task. The change scores were then subjected to a 2 participants with high self-esteem reported significantly
(Self-esteem)  4 (Experimental Condition: low-choice less attitude change (M ¼ 1:39, SD ¼ 1:78) compared to
neutral prime, high-choice neutral prime, high-choice participants with low self-esteem (M ¼ 2:87, SD ¼ 1:74),
relevant prime, and high-choice irrelevant prime) AN- F ð1; 147Þ ¼ 5:63, p < :02. Thus, both the expectancy
OVA. The analysis revealed a significant main effect for and resource role of self-esteem in dissonance emerged as
Condition, F ð3; 147Þ ¼ 2:88, p < :04, and a significant a function of the type of self-relevant thought invoked by
Self-esteem  Condition interaction, F ð3; 147Þ ¼ 3:18, the priming manipulations.
p < :03. Planned comparisons of the attitude change
scores displayed in Fig. 1 were conducted to unpack the
Discussion
2
Based on the trait list provided by Anderson (1968), valence
ratings were assigned to each self-attribute used in the priming The results of the experiment provided support for
manipulations. With higher scores indicating more positively valenced
traits, the average for the set of relevant traits was 5.07, the average for
the hypotheses regarding the role of self-attribute rele-
the set of irrelevant traits was 4.93, and the average for the set of vance on how self-esteem moderates attitude change
neutral attributes was 4.02. following a counter-attitudinal behavior. As predicted
J. Stone, J. Cooper / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 508–515 513

by the processing assumptions of the Self-Standards proceed without the influence of idiosyncratic self-
Model (SSM, Stone & Cooper, 2001), self-esteem knowledge (Stone, in press; Stone & Cooper, 2001).
moderated attitude change following a counter-attitu- Furthermore, not just any positive self-relevant
dinal behavior, but only when cognitions about the self thought will provide resources for dissonance reduction;
were primed following the discrepant act. Moreover, when positive self-attributes that were relevant to the
whether self-esteem functioned as a resource or as an topic of the discrepant essay were primed, participants
expectancy depended upon the type of self-relevant with high self-esteem reported more attitude change
thought induced by the priming manipulation. Overall, compared to participants with low self-esteem. This
the data indicate that the role of cognitions about the pattern of self-esteem moderation reflects self-consis-
self and self-esteem in dissonance is a function of if and tency for people with low self-esteem in the process of
how people think about themselves in the context of a dissonance, a phenomenon that has proved difficult to
discrepant behavior. replicate in past dissonance research (e.g., Ward &
When self-attributes that were irrelevant to the dis- Sandvold, 1963; see Swann, 1990). The data suggest that
crepant act were primed, participants with high self-es- when relevant positive attributes were primed following
teem showed less attitude change as compared to the discrepant act, they activated the different self-ex-
participants with low self-esteem. This pattern supports pectancies for behavior held by participants with high
the perspective that high self-esteem, and the wealth of and low self-esteem (Aronson, 1999). For those with
positive attributes it represents, can serve as a buffer or high self-esteem, the behavior was perceived as incon-
resource against the discomfort people typically feel sistent with their self-expectancies for compassion,
following a discrepant behavior (Steele et al., 1993). which increased the need to change attitudes. This
Participants with low self-esteem, in contrast, were supports the SSM assumption that in order for positive
motivated to change their attitudes, presumably because self-attributes to provide resources for dissonance re-
the primed self-attributes were less self-descriptive, and duction, they must shift attention away from the dis-
therefore, less capable of serving as an affirmational crepancy; otherwise, the accessibility of positive
(Steele et al., 1993) or affective resource (Tesser, 2000). cognitions about the self may sustain or enhance dis-
As predicted by the resource models, the more positive comfort and the motivation to change attitudes (Stone
self-attributes individuals have at their disposal, the less & Cooper, 2001). In contrast, for participants with low
they need to rely on self-justification to resolve behav- self-esteem, the relevant prime may have activated neg-
ioral discrepancies. ative self-expectancies for compassionate behavior,
The data also show that there are constraints on the which reduced the perception of a discrepancy and the
ability of people to use their positive self-attributes as need for attitude change.
resources for dissonance reduction. In the high- and It is important to acknowledge that whereas the data
low-choice control conditions in which neutral self-at- are consistent with the processing assumptions of the
tributes were primed, participants with high and low SSM, no mediational data were gathered in the present
self-esteem reported the same levels of attitude change, study that addresses alternatives to the processes speci-
which was significantly moderated by perceptions of fied in the model. For example, research indicates that
choice, and not by the self-cognitions underlying their when self-attributes are primed, they may reduce the
level of self-esteem. This finding conceptually replicates need for attitude change by inducing trivialization of the
previous research on the relative importance of self-es- behavioral discrepancy (Simon, Greenberg, & Brehm,
teem and perceptions of responsibility for behavioral 1995). However, a trivialization explanation may be
outcomes in dissonance (Cooper & Duncan, 1971). The hard pressed to account for the cross-over interaction
fact that both self-esteem groups showed significant at- between self-esteem and the relevance of the primed self-
titude change in the high-choice-neutral prime control attributes on attitude change. According to Simon et al.
condition is compatible with the assumption that both (1995), trivialization only occurs when people are able to
groups were focused on the inconsistency between spe- bring to mind highly important cognitions, such as those
cific attitudes or beliefs and their behavior (e.g., Fest- related to the self, prior to being offered the opportunity
inger, 1957; Harmon-Jones et al., 1996) or on the to change attitudes. In the current study, not only were
aversive consequence of their behavior (Cooper & Fa- the self-attributes chosen because they were rated in
zio, 1984). The lack of self-esteem differences in the previous research as equally desirable to possess (An-
control conditions suggests that for self-esteem to derson, 1968; Blanton et al., 1997), but during the pre-
moderate dissonance, something in the context of a test session, participants in the current study with high
discrepant behavior must make cognitions about the self or low self-esteem also rated the attributes as equally
accessible. Otherwise, the cover story or procedure itself important to their self-concepts. Thus, the manipulation
may focus people primarily on specific attitudes or be- brought to mind equally important cognitions in each of
liefs, and on the situational antecedents of the act (e.g., the self-attribute priming conditions, but attitude
choice and forseeability), and dissonance processes may change was still moderated by the interplay between
514 J. Stone, J. Cooper / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 508–515

self-esteem and the relevance of the attributes to the gand, Cooper, & Aronson, 1997). The process by which
behavioral discrepancy. people interpret their behavior, conclude it represents a
It is also important to consider other processes by discrepancy, experience discomfort, and seek a way to
which relevant attributes reduce the need for self-justi- reduce it, may be more malleable than has been recog-
fication among people with low self-esteem. For exam- nized in previous revisions of dissonance theory.
ple, when self-expectancies were made accessible by the
relevant prime, it is possible that participants with low
self-esteem showed less need for attitude change because References
they were relatively less certain of whether the behavior
was inconsistent with their self-view of compassion. Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait
However, other research has shown that self-consistency words. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 272–279.
for people with low self-esteem is more likely to occur Aronson, E. (1968). Dissonance theory: Progress and problems. In R.
among those who are especially certain of their negative Abelson, E. Aronson, W. McGuire, T. Newcomb, M. Rosenberg,
& P. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Theories of cognitive consistency: A
self-views (e.g., Maracek & Mettee, 1972; Stone, in sourcebook (pp. 5–27). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
press). Another possibility is that the observed self- Aronson, E. (1999). Dissonance, hypocrisy, and the self-concept. In E.
consistency effect for people with low self-esteem was a Harmon-Jones, & J. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on
function of how the attribute primes influenced the way a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 103–126). Washington,
in which they labeled their arousal (Cooper & Fazio, DC: American Psychological Association.
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1962). Performance expectancy as a
1984). The relevant primes may have caused participants determinant of actual performance. Journal of Abnormal and Social
with low self-esteem to perceive a discrepancy between Psychology, 65, 178–182.
their expectancies and behavior, but they labeled their Aronson, J., Blanton, H., & Cooper, J. (1995). From dissonance to
arousal as emotions like dejection or depression, which disidentification: Selectivity in the self-affirmation process. Journal
reduced the motivation for attitude change, whereas of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 986–996.
Aronson, J., Cohen, G. L., & Nail, P. R. (1999). Self-affirmation
participants with high self-esteem labeled their negative theory: An update and appraisal. In E. Harmon-Jones, & J. Mills
emotions as discomfort, tension, or guilt, which moti- (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social
vated attitude change to reduce their arousal (Galinsky, psychology (pp. 127–148). Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
Stone, & Cooper, 2000; Harmon-Jones, 2000; Higgins, ical Association.
1987). Investigating the mediators of the observed in- Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation
teraction between self-esteem and the accessibility of on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–
self-attributes in the context of a discrepant act is an 244.
important direction for future research. Blanton, H., Cooper, J., Skurnik, I., & Aronson, J. (1997). When bad
things happen to good feedback: Exacerbating the need for self-
justification with self-affirmations. Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy Bulletin, 23, 684–692.
Conclusions Brockner, J., Wiesenfeld, B. M., & Raskas, D. F. (1993). Self-esteem
and expectancy-value discrepancy: The effects of believing that you
Over 40 years after its initial publication (Festinger, can (or canÕt) get what you want. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-
1957), the role of cognitions about the self in dissonance esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 219–240). New York:
continues to intrigue researchers. The research in this Plenum Press.
Brown, J. B. (1998). The self. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
paper found support for predictions made by both the Cooper, J., & Duncan, B. L. (1971). Cognitive dissonance as a function
self-resource and the self-consistency perspectives on of self-esteem and logical inconsistency. Journal of Personality, 39,
how self-esteem moderates dissonance induced attitude 289–302.
change. However, the present research also demon- Cooper, J., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at dissonance theory. In
strated that cognitions about the self and self-esteem are L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology:
Vol. 17 (pp. 229–262). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
not always invoked by a discrepant act. This suggests, as Elliot, A. J., & Devine, P. G. (1994). On the motivational nature of
predicted by the self-standards model (Stone & Cooper, cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort.
2001), that people are capable of using their self- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 382–394.
knowledge to interpret and evaluate their behavior, but Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA:
whether they use cognitions about the self, specific at- Stanford University Press.
Galinsky, A. D., Stone, J., & Cooper, J. (2000). The reinstatement of
titudes, or norms to interpret behavior is a function of dissonance and psychological discomfort following failed affirma-
which criterion is made salient in the situation. How tions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 123–147.
cognitions about the self influence the arousal and re- Gibbons, F. X., Eggleston, T. J., & Benthin, A. (1997). Cognitive
duction of dissonance depends upon the type of self- reactions to smoking relapse: The reciprocal relation of dissonance
attributes and self-standards brought to mind when and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
184–195.
people make a counter-attitudinal statement, a difficult Glass, D. (1964). Changes in liking as a means of reducing cognitive
decision, or engage in other established dissonance discrepancies between self-esteem and aggression. Journal of
arousing behavior, including hypocrisy (Stone, Wie- Personality, 32, 531–549.
J. Stone, J. Cooper / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 508–515 515

Harmon-Jones, E. (2000). An update on cognitive dissonance theory, Stone, J. (in press). Self-consistency for low self-esteem in dissonance
with a focus on the self. In A. Tesser, R. B. Felson, & J. Suls processes: The role of self-standards. Personality and Social
(Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self: Vol. 1 (pp. 119–144). Psychology Bulletin.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Stone, J., & Cooper, J. (2001). A self-standards model of cognitive
Harmon-Jones, E., Brehm, J. W., Greenberg, J., Simon, L., & Nelson, dissonance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 228–243.
D. E. (1996). Is the production of aversive consequences necessary Stone, J., Wiegand, A. W., Cooper, J., & Aronson, E. (1997). When
to create cognitive dissonance? Journal of Personality and Social exemplification fails: Hypocrisy and the motive for self-integrity.
Psychology, 70, 5–16. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 54–65.
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and Sorrentino, R. M. (1990). To be adored or to be known? The interplay
affect. Psychological Review, 94(3), 319–340. of self-enhancement and self-verification. In E. T. Higgins (Ed.),
Maracek, J., & Mettee, D. (1972). Avoidance of continued success as a Motivation and cognition: Vol. 2 (pp. 408–450). New York, NY:
function of self-esteem, level of esteem certainty, and responsibility Guilford Press.
for success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 98– Tesser, A. (2000). On the confluence of self-esteem maintenance
107. mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 290–299.
Pelham, B. W., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1989). From self-conceptions to Tesser, A., & Cornell, D. P. (1991). On the confluence of self processes.
self-worth: On the sources and structure of global self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 501–526.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 672–680. Thibodeau, R., & Aronson, E. (1992). Taking a closer look:
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York, NY: Basic Reasserting the role of the self-concept in dissonance theory.
Books. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 591–602.
Simon, L., Greenberg, J., & Brehm, J. (1995). Trivialization: The Ward, W. D., & Sandvold, K. D. (1963). Performance expectancy as a
forgotten mode of dissonance reduction. Journal of Personality and determinant of actual performance: A partial replication. Journal
Social Psychology, 68, 247–260. of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 293–295.
Spencer, S. J., Josephs, R. A., & Steele, C. M. (1993). Low self-esteem:
The uphill struggle for self-integrity. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-
esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 21–36). New York:
Plenum Press. Further reading
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the
integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Cooper, J. (1999). Unwanted consequences and the self: In search of
experimental social psychology: Vol. 21 (pp. 261–302). Hillsdale, the motivation for dissonance reduction. In E. Harmon-Jones, & J.
NJ: Erlbaum. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance 40 years later: Revival with
Steele, C. M., & Lui, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self- revisions and controversies (pp. 149–174). Washington, DC: Amer-
affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 5–19. ican Psychological Association.
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Lynch, M. (1993). Dissonance and Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of
affirmational resources: Resilience against self-image threats. forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 885–896. 203–210.
Stone, J. (1999). What exactly have I done? The role of self-attribute Harmon-Jones, E. (1999). Toward an understanding of the motivation
accessibility in dissonance. In E. Harmon-Jones, & J. Mills (Eds.), underlying dissonance effects: Is the production of aversive
Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social consequences necessary. In E. Harmon-Jones, & J. Mills (Eds.),
psychology (pp. 175–200). Washington, DC: American Psycholog- Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social
ical Association. psychology (pp. 71–99). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Stone, J. (2001). Behavioral discrepancies and construal processes in Association.
cognitive dissonance. In G. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (1999). Cognitive dissonance: Progress
psychology: The Princeton symposium on the legacy and future of on a pivotal theory in social psychology. Washington, DC: American
social cognition (pp. 41–58). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychological Association.

You might also like