Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 102

ETHNIC DIFFERENCES AND READING ABILITY AS A PREDICTOR OF

ACADEMIC PROCRASTINATION AMONG UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS

A Thesis

Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology

California State University, Sacramento

Submitted in partial satisfaction of


the requirements for the degree of

MASTER OF ARTS

in

PSYCHOLOGY

(COUNSELING PSYCHOLOGY)

by

Alvaro Donato Gonzalez

SUMMER
2014
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES AND READING ABILITY AS A PREDICTOR OF

ACADEMIC PROCRASTINATION AMONG UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS

A Thesis

by

Alvaro Donato Gonzalez

Approved by:

__________________________________, Committee Chair


Phillip Akutsu, Ph.D.

__________________________________, Second Reader


Marya Endriga, Ph.D.

__________________________________, Third Reader


Rebecca Cameron, Ph.D.

____________________________
Date

ii
Student: Alvaro Donato Gonzalez

I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University

format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to

be awarded for the thesis.

__________________________, Graduate Coordinator ___________________


Jianjian Qin, Ph.D. Date

Department of Psychology

iii
Abstract

of

ETHNIC DIFFERENCES AND READING ABILITY AS A PREDICTOR OF

ACADEMIC PROCRASTINATION AMONG UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS

by

Alvaro Donato Gonzalez

The current study examined ethnic differences in procrastination on six academic tasks

between 58 Asian, 28 African, 73 Latino, and 93White American college students, while

controlling for the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES. In addition, reading

vocabulary, reading comprehension, task aversiveness, and fear of failure were examined

to determine which would be stronger predictors of procrastination among the six

academic tasks. Results showed ethnicity was the strongest predictor for keeping up with

weekly reading assignments. Reading vocabulary was the strongest predictor for

procrastination on performing academic tasks in general. Task aversiveness and fear of

failure were the strongest predictors of procrastination for writing a term paper and

studying for an exam. Task aversiveness was also the strongest predictor of

procrastination for attending meetings.

_______________________, Committee Chair


Phillip Akutsu, Ph.D.

_______________________
Date
iv
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

First, I would like to give my thanks and appreciation to my advisor, Dr. Phillip

Akutsu. His encouragement and guidance have sustained me throughout graduate school

and the thesis-writing process.

I am extremely grateful to my parents for their unfailing support and

encouragement. They have communicated their love and confidence in me through words

and gestures, and I have so much appreciation for all that they have done and continue to

do for me.

I am thankful for all current and past members of the research lab, Ting Ting Lee,

Ho Man Cheung, Tyler Virden, Chelsea Castillo, and Vanessa Mendez, who provided

their guidance, support, and encouragement.

I would also like to extend my thanks towards Dr. Marya Endriga and Dr.

Rebecca Cameron for joining my thesis committee and providing helpful feedback

throughout the project.

Finally, I am grateful to my fiancée, Kristin DeWaal, who has been my best

friend, cheering section, and more. Thank you for your love and patience, especially

through the difficult times. My ability to successfully complete this thesis and graduate

school is a reflection of your unwavering support and faith in me

v
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

Acknowledgements ....................................................................................................... v

List of Tables .............................................................................................................. vi

Chapter

1. INTRODUCTION..…………….………………………………………………… 1

Academic Procrastination ................................................................................. 3

Procrastination and Academic Performance ..................................................... 5

Procrastination and Ethnicity ........................................................................... 7

Procrastination and Other Demographic Factors ........................................... 11

Task Aversiveness .......................................................................................... 12

Fear of Failure ................................................................................................. 13

Reading Ability, Procrastination, and Academic Performance ...................... 16

Summary ......................................................................................................... 26

Present Study .................................................................................................. 29

2. METHOD ............................................................................................................. 34

Participants ...................................................................................................... 34

Measures and Scales ....................................................................................... 35

Procedures ....................................................................................................... 39

3. RESULTS. ............................................................................................................ 40

4. DISCUSSION ....................................................................................................... 69
vi
Summary and Interpretation of Findings in the Context of Previous

Literature ......................................................................................................... 69

Limitations ............................................................................................…….. 74

Clinical Implications ....................................................................................... 76

Future Directions in Research ......................................................................... 77

References ............................................................................................................................. 81

vii
LIST OF TABLES

Tables Page

1. Means and Standard Deviations for the Six Academic Procrastination Tasks,

Overall Procrastination, Reading Vocabulary, Reading Comprehension, Task

Aversiveness, and Fear of Failure by Ethnicity……………...............................… 42

2. Correlations between Age, SES, the Six Academic Tasks, Overall Procrastination,

Reading Vocabulary, Reading Comprehension, Task Aversiveness, and Fear of

Failure. .………………………………………………………...……………....… 44

3. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, ESL

SES, Ethnicity, and Reading Vocabulary as Predictors of Procrastination in

Keeping Up With Weekly Reading Assignments……………………….…….…50

4. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, ESL,

SES, Ethnicity, and Reading Vocabulary as Predictors of Procrastination in

Performing Academic Tasks in General……………………..…………………… 51

5. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, ESL,

SES, Ethnicity, and Reading Comprehension Score as Predictors of Procrastination

in Keeping Up With Weekly Reading Assignments……….………………….….. 53

6. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of

Procrastination in Writing a Term Paper……………………………….……….... 56

viii
7. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of

Procrastination in Studying for Exams…………………………………..….......... 57

8. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of

Procrastination in Keeping Up with Weekly Reading Assignments.…………….. 59

9. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of

Procrastination in Attending meetings………………………………..………........60

10. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of

Procrastination in Performing Academic Tasks in General…………………….… 62

11. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary, and Fear of Failure as Predictors of Procrastination

in Writing a Term Paper Paper……………………………………….…..…….… 63

12. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary, and Fear of Failure as Predictors of Procrastination

in Studying for Exams………………………………………………………….… 65

13. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary, and Fear of Failure as Predictors of Procrastination

in Keeping Up with Weekly Reading Assignments…………………...…………. 66

ix
14. Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES,

Ethnicity, Reading Vocabulary, and Fear of Failure as Predictors of Procrastination

in Performing Academic Tasks in General…………………………………….… 68

x
1

Chapter 1

INTRODUCTION

Academic procrastination - unnecessarily delaying academic activities that one

ultimately intends to complete to the point of creating emotional discomfort - is a very

common and troubling phenomenon among undergraduate students (Klassen, Krawchuk,

& Rajani, 2008; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984; Steel, 2007). These academic tasks range

from simple homework assignments to the completion of a senior thesis. Estimates of

procrastination among undergraduate students indicate that 70%-90% engage in some

type of procrastination with 70% considering themselves as “procrastinators” and 20%

who procrastinate consistently and problematically (Klassen et al., 2008; Solomon &

Rothblum, 1984; Steel, 2007).

Although some procrastination is expected and normal, problematic and habitual

procrastination can have negative consequences for undergraduate students (Chu & Choi,

2005; Ferrari, O’Callaghan, & Newbegin, 2005; Klassen et al., 2008; Onwuegbuzie,

2004). Students who procrastinate habitually have been linked to poor academic

performance (Klassen et al., 2010; Onwuegbuzie, 2004; Steel, 2007). Academic

procrastination is connected with negative behaviors and outcomes, which include but are

not limited to submitting late assignments, missing academic deadlines, cramming, test

and social anxiety, lower quality work, lower grades, and lower grade point averages

(Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown, 1995; Klassen et al., 2008; Pychyl, Lee, Thibodeau, &

Blunt, 2000). Students who procrastinate can suffer from both affective and health
2

problems (Beck, Koons, & Milgram, 2000). Past studies have shown that students who

procrastinate more often suffer from depression, anxiety, low self-esteem, and an increase

in psychological distress (Ferrari et al., 1995; Rice, Richardson, & Clark, 2012). Due to

these negative outcomes, research has studied procrastination extensively and has

proposed various factors as contributors and predictors. These contributors and

predictors include task aversiveness and fear of failure (Clark & Hill, 1995; Solomon &

Rothblum, 1984).

It is important to study academic procrastination so we can help students who are

suffering from this problematic behavior. While there has been extensive research on

academic procrastination and interventions to help students with this problem (Ferrari et

al., 1995), more research should be conducted to focus on other possible factors that may

contribute to procrastination. The information that is gathered through research and data

can help develop programs and treatments for students within the school setting. By

gathering more information, educators may be able to help prevent or see early warning

signs that students may be taking part in procrastination behaviors.

This literature review will address the following issues: 1) how often students

procrastinate, the types of tasks that undergraduate students procrastinate on, and

methodology; 2) how procrastination affects academic performance; 3) ethnic

differences; 4) demographic factors; 5) the different factors that contribute to academic

procrastination such as task aversivenes, and fear of failure; and 6) reading ability and its

relationship with procrastination.


3

Academic Procrastination

Academic procrastination is the postponement of academic tasks that include

writing term papers, studying for exams, and keeping up with reading assignments

(Ferrari et al., 1995). Among undergraduates, procrastination includes a wide range of

behaviors including watching television, napping, exercising, playing, surfing the web,

and eating/drinking which delay or restrict the completion of specific tasks of greater

academic importance (Pychyl et al., 2000). Past research indicates the rate of chronic

problematic academic procrastination among college students is 70%-95% (Klassen et

al., 2008). Among colleges students, about one-third of their daily activities consisted of

some form of procrastination (e.g., watching television, playing a game, spending time

with friends) instead of non-procrastination activities such as studying, reading, or

working on homework (Pychyl et al., 2000). Although procrastination has been studied

using different methodologies, the majority of studies have used questionnaires or

surveys to gather information among subjects and this method is considered a good

format for studying procrastination (e.g., Alexander & Onwuegbuzie, 2007; Beck et al.,

2000; Klassen et al., 2008; Seo, 2008). Students have reported they tend to procrastinate

more when writing term papers in comparison to completing reading assignments,

studying for exams, attending to administrative tasks including registering for classes,

filling out proper academic forms, obtaining a student identification card, or completing

attendance tasks which include making appointments with advisors and professors

(Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). In contrast, other studies report students tend to

procrastinate more on completing reading assignments than writing term papers or


4

studying (Clark & Hill, 1994). Although Clark and Hill (1994) reported more students

procrastinating on reading assignments than writing term papers and studying, the overall

results of their were similar to those of Solomon and Rothblum (1984). Both used the

Procrastination Assessment Scale - Students (PASS), but one major difference between

these two studies was that Clark and Hill (1994) focused only on African American

college students, while Solomon and Rothblum’s (1984) subjects were only White

American college students. Both studies performed a factor analysis on a major portion

of the PASS that assessed possible reasons for procrastination and discovered that fear of

failure and task aversiveness accounted for the majority of variance in predicting

procrastination (Clark & Hill, 1994; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). In general, students

who report higher levels of procrastination tend to do poorer academically than students

who report lower levels of procrastination behaviors (Klassen et al., 2008). These

students who procrastinate regularly also tend to experience higher levels of stress,

anxiety, and depression in comparison to students who report lower levels of

procrastination (Klassen et al., 2010). Although some amount of procrastination is

considered to be average and normal (Schouwenburg, Lay, Pychyl, & Ferrari, 2004),

procrastination is considered a serious and widespread problem that affects college

students across different university settings (Klassen et al., 2010).

In a comprehensive review of the literature on academic procrastination, the

majority of studies have used the PASS questionnaire (e.g., Alexander & Onwuegbuzie,

2007; Beck et al., 2000; Clark and Hill, 1995; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984), Lay’s

procrastination scale (e.g. Ferrari et al., 1995; Ferrari et al., 2005; Harriott & Ferrari,
5

1996), or some other procrastination measure such as Tuckman’s 16-item procrastination

measure (e.g., Klassen et al. 2008; Klassen et al., 2010). Past studies on academic

procrastination that used all three measures have reported good validity and reliability.

As stated earlier, although most studies use surveys to study academic

procrastination, other studies have used different research methodologies to examine

procrastination. For example, Pychyl et al. (2000) used experience-sampling to collect

data on procrastination through the use of pagers and self-reports. In this study, the

students were randomly “paged” during the day to describe their current activity and

whether they considered themselves to be procrastinating at that exact moment.

Although this study found somewhat similar results to other procrastination studies that

used questionnaires, the limitation of this method was that it only captured a small

window of opportunity to see what the students were doing with their time. According to

the study, one-third of the students’ daily activities were considered “procrastinating”.

However, this ratio may be only capturing a student’s normal day-to-day experiences and

not providing the actual daily experience of a chronic procrastinator. In contrast,

proponents who use different research methods other than questionnaires to study

procrastination could argue the survey method could lead to students under-reporting

their procrastination behaviors due to major concerns about social desirability (De Jong,

Pieters, & Fox, 2010).

Procrastination and Academic Performance

Overall, the relationship between procrastination and academic performance has

been a negative one. Several studies have pointed out that procrastination can negatively
6

impact academic learning and achievement and has been found to be associated with

negative academic outcomes including missed deadlines for assignments, delay in taking

self-paced quizzes, low course grades, and higher course withdrawal (Onwuegbuzie,

2004). For example, students who report higher levels of procrastination have lower

grade point averages in comparison to students who report lower levels of procrastination

(Klassen et al., 2010; Steel, 2007). Although Steel (2007) explained the relationship

between these two variable was a relatively weak one, he stated that “procrastination is

usually harmful, sometimes harmless, but never helpful” (p. 80). Other negative

consequences of chronic academic procrastination included compromised academic

performance and progress, decreased learning, and strained relationships between

students and professors. In contrast, procrastination has been shown to be beneficial for

academic procrastination in some cases. This may be explained in the sense that some

students perform better while working under heightened stress and pressure and may

procrastinate to set up such stress inducing conditions (Chu & Choi, 2005). Research has

also shown that procrastinators tend to experience less stress and illness than non-

procrastinators earlier in the semester by avoiding anxiety-provoking behavior at the start

of the term (Tice & Baumeister, 1997). Superstitious behavior may also explain why

individuals believe they can benefit from engaging in procrastination. Superstitious

behaviors arise when an individual is positively rewarded for engaging in procrastination

in previous academic experiences or situations. That is, procrastination is accidentally

reinforced (possibly via classical conditioning) because the individual associates the

positive reward with his/her procrastination behavior rather than it being due to his/her
7

increased efforts and activity while working under time pressure and this incorrect

assumption then increases the likelihood of procrastination occurring again to ensure the

same positive results. This procrastination behavior, however, does not have a clear

function in the execution of the academic task itself (Foster, Weigand, & Baines, 2006).

Procrastination and Ethnicity

Ethnicity is another important factor that many studies have researched about

academic procrastination. However, there are mixed results as to whether ethnicity is a

good predictor of procrastination. Some research suggests that there are no ethnic

differences with regards to academic procrastination in college student populations

(Prohaska et al., 2000). In fact, past research has shown similar results concerning

procrastination behaviors between African American and White American college

students. In both the Clark and Hill (1994) and Solomon and Rothblum (1984) studies,

approximately 30 to 35% of the subjects reported serious problems of procrastination on

writing assignments, studying for exams, and keeping up with weekly reading

assignments. Prohaska et al. (2000) examined ethnic differences in a population

composed mostly of ethnic minority students. Using the overall procrastination score

from the PASS, Prohaska et al. (2000) found no ethnic differences among African,

Latino, and White American college students. Although there were no overall

procrastination score differences among the ethnic groups, Prohaska et al. (2000) had

found that his results were similar to Clark and Hill’s study (1984) when comparing the

frequency of procrastination and whether the procrastination was considered problematic

on six different academic tasks with those reported in Solomon and Rothblum’s study
8

(1984). Both Prohaska’s (2000) and Clark and Hill’s (1994) studies, whose populations

were primarily composed of ethnic minority students, reported significant differences on

certain procrastination behaviors compared to Solomon and Rothblum’s population of

White American students. What the data revealed was that frequency in procrastination

for weekly reading assignments and activities in general were significantly higher

compared to those observed by Solomon and Rothblum (1984), but they were

significantly lower for writing a term paper and engaging in attendance tasks. Solomon

and Rothblum (1984) found that among a primarily White American population, the

academic tasks that involved the most procrastination were writing a term paper and

engaging in attendance tasks while Prohaska et al. (2000) and Clark and Hill (1994),

found that ethnic minorities procrastinated more on weekly reading assignments and

activities in general.

Although there has been research to suggest there are significant ethnic

differences on procrastination between African American, Latino, and White Americans,

there is very little research that has examined Asian American differences with other

ethnic groups. This gap in literature is likely to be a product of the “model minority”

myth surrounding Asian Americans (Museus & Kiang, 2009). Studies on Asian

American academic work ethic and success provide evidence for a pattern of high

academic achievement among Asian American students (Yao, 2010). The most popular

theories on why Asian Americans are successful in academia focus on the influence of

Asian American cultural values, particularly values that emphasize the importance of

education and hard work, the need to fulfill family obligations, and the need to respect
9

elders such as parents and teachers (Yao, 2010). Most children are aware of their

parent’s high expectations for academic success and associate their good performance

with their parent’s happiness, honor, and pride. Eighty percent of Asian American

parents expected their child to obtain at least a bachelors degree in comparison to 62% of

White American parents, 58% of African American parents, and 50% of Latino parents

(Peng & Wright, 1994). High academic expectations and performance is also reinforced

by their peer groups. The majority of Asian American students who are placed in honors

level classes tended to motivate each other more and distract each other less compared to

students in regular classes (Peng & Wright, 1994). Studies have shown that Asian

American students devote more time to their studies and were more likely to credit their

academic success to hard work and effort (Peng & Wright, 1994). Specifically, Asian

American students reported spending twice as much time on homework per week than

other students (Steinberg, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992). As a result, Asian American

students tend to be perceived as diligent, industrious, quiet, and respectful. These

perceptions reinforce the image of Asian Americans as the “model minority”. The lack

in research on Asian American students and procrastination might also be a product of

racism in research due to the model minority myth. Many times, researchers do not find

differences because they assume that all Asian American students are the same and do

not take into account that there are many different cultures within the scope of Asian

Americans (Museus & Kiang, 2009). Asian American students may often not be

included in studies of ethnicity because there is the misconception that they do not

encounter major challenges such as African and Latino American students (Museus &
10

Kiang, 2009). One final reason why they may not be included in studies of ethnicity is

because there are a high number of Asian Americans with college degrees and

researchers consider them to be successful. Although Asian Americans do have higher

numbers of college degrees, they still report with lower wager and hold fewer managerial

positions than non-Asian American peers (Museus & Kiang, 2009). Although there is no

explicit research that states Asian Americans procrastinate less than African, Latino, and

White American students, past research has shown that their work ethic and expectations

of academic success may be a good predictor of less procrastination behaviors and they

may be similar in procrastination to White American students, however due to the model

minority myth, not all Asian American students will do well.

Although research has shown no consistent ethnic differences in overall

procrastination, this may have to do to the assessment measure being used in previous

research. One limitation with past studies and the PASS was the way that procrastination

was operationally defined and measured. To measure procrastination, the majority of

studies using the PASS calculated a single overall score by summing the scores of 5-point

Likert scales on the frequency of procrastination and whether the student viewed their

procrastination as problematic on six academic tasks (Alexander & Onwuegbuzie, 2007;

Beck et al., 2000; Clark & Hill, 1995; Collins et al., 2008; Prohaska et al., 2000; Solomon

& Rothblum, 1984). The problem with operationally defining procrastination using this

method is that a student may report a high frequency of procrastination on a particular

academic task, but then may report that his or her procrastination is not problematic. The

summed score method on the PASS may also reflect a student who procrastinates very
11

little, but reports that his or her level of procrastination is very problematic. The problem

with using a summed score for procrastination using scales that measure frequency and

whether it is considered problematic is that it is difficult to determine whether the

student’s high overall procrastination score may be due to high frequency or viewing

procrastination as very problematic. It would be beneficial to examine the frequency of

procrastination and whether procrastination is viewed as problematic for different

academic tasks separately to determine any significant differences in procrastination in

specific academic areas in comparison to the summed overall procrastination score that

has been used in the past. The Prohaska et al. study (2000) provides a good example of

such an approach where there were no significant ethnic differences reported for the

overall PASS procrastination score, but some significant ethnic differences were reported

when frequency scores and whether procrastination is considered problematic scores

were viewed independently.

Procrastination and Other Demographic Factors

With regard to age and academic procrastination, among college students who

were 21 years of age or younger, no significant age differences have been reported

(Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). Yet, others have found a significant negative relationship

between age and procrastination when age is extended to include students who were older

than 21 years of age (Beswick, Rothblum, & Mann, 1988; Prohaska et al., 2000).

Specifically, students whose ages were 21 and older reported less academic

procrastination that students under the age of 21.


12

The literature concerning gender differences on academic procrastination is

somewhat mixed. Although some studies have indicated no significant gender

differences in the incidence of procrastination (Ferrari, Özer, & Demir, 2009; Solomon &

Rothblum, 1984; Steel, 2007), other studies have reported that men are more at risk for

being procrastinators than women (Milgram, Marshevsky, & Sadeh, 1994; Özer, Demir,

& Ferrari, 2009; Prohaska, Morrill, Atiles, & Perez, 2000).

The relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and procrastination has

very limited research. Chow (2011) concluded there may be a significant relationship

between SES and procrastination. This study reported that students who came from

lower SES backgrounds worried more about their financial situation than students who

came from a high SES background and an earlier study showed a strong relationship

between worrying and procrastination, where individuals who worried a lot tend to

procrastinate more than those who do not worry as much (Stober & Joorman, 2001).

Task Aversiveness

Task aversiveness is defined as how unenjoyable it is to perform a given task, and

is often associated with procrastination (Clark & Hill, 1994; Klassen et al., 2010;

Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). Task aversiveness is positively related to procrastination

and is one of the factors that is most strongly associated with it (Blunt & Pychyl, 2000).

Research has shown that individuals were more likely to report higher levels of

procrastination for unpleasant tasks than for tasks viewed as neutral or pleasant (Milgram

et al., 1995). These types of aversive academic tasks were also considered to be the least

interesting and most challenging (Steel, 2007). In academia, writing term papers appears
13

to be a task that most college students procrastinate on, while less difficult and more

interesting tasks such as reading or researching were less likely to involve procrastination

(Klassen et al., 2010). As past research has shown, students tend to procrastinate on tasks

that are more challenging or difficult. Solomon and Rothblum (1984) concluded that the

likelihood of procrastination on a specific task depends on how unpleasant it is to

complete it. Boredom, frustration, and resentment are relatively stable components of

task aversiveness and there is a significant correlation between task aversiveness and

procrastination (Blunt & Pychyl, 2000).

There is currently little to no research on ethnic differences in task aversiveness.

However, because ethnic minority students tend to do more poorly academically and are

often not as prepared for college, they may view certain academic tasks as harder or more

unpleasant than White American students. As such, they might have higher levels of task

aversiveness on academic tasks than White American students.

Fear of Failure

Fear of failure is a common factor that is discussed when dealing with

procrastination in academia. A number of studies have shown that fear of failure is

positively related to procrastination (e.g., Prohaska et al., 2000; Clark & Hill, 1995;

Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). According to Ferrari et al. (1995), academic

procrastination can be viewed as a fear of academic failure. Students who experience

high levels of fear of failure will experience greater worry and anxiety about an

upcoming academic task such as an exam. In order to deal with the worry and anxiety

associated with the task, the student will avoid (procrastinate) this task and replace it with
14

another activity that is distracting or more pleasurable. By doing this, the student is

relieving any anxiety and worry that he or she may be feeling about the upcoming task

and this anxiety-reduction tends to reinforce his or her avoidance behavior (Ferrari et al.,

1995). By putting off studying for a major test, the student can say, “I know I could have

done better if I had had more time to study.” By doing this, procrastination may also be

seen as a self-handicapping strategy (Beck et al., 2000). Students may put off studying so

that they can attribute their test failure to the lack of studying rather than deficits in their

personal intellectual abilities. Therefore if a failure occurs, it is attributed to the lack of

preparation (i.e., limited study time due to procrastination) instead of the lack of

academic ability on the part of the individual (Smith, Snyder, & Perkins, 1983). By

engaging in procrastination behaviors, the student is protecting his or her self-esteem

because this process externalizes the failure rather than internalizing or attributing it to an

internal deficit. Self-handicapping can be linked to the self-serving bias. Students who

handicap themselves to prevent taking responsibility for personal failure provide an

example of this self-serving bias (Berglas & Jones, 1978; Smith et al., 1983). Research

has also shown that simply having test scores or grades associated with failure might

instigate avoidant behaviors (e.g., procrastination) (Schüler, Brandstätter, & Baumann,

2013).

There is very little research that has examined ethnic differences on fear of

failure. Past research has shown that African American, Latino American, and American

Indian students report with higher levels of fear of failure than White American students

(Coney& West, 1979; Evans, 2008). However, this research failed to consider that many
15

ethnic minority students come from lower socioeconomic status (SES) and have more

family obligations to work full time to help their families survive. Due to more financial

issues and higher family obligations, there is the possibility of a higher likelihood that the

student with lower SES may fail academically. Beyond financial issues, Latino

American and American Indian student also have reported higher levels of feeling

isolated and alone in academic settings. This heightened sense of isolation can contribute

to an increase in fear of failure because they lack the support that White American

students may have (Evans, 2008). The type of academic preparation by the student and

the academic setting and demands can also affect a student’s fear of failure. Past studies

have shown that Latino American and American Indian students verbalized their fear of

academic failure more than White American students, with many stating that the

academic program was more difficult than they had expected (Evans, 2008). Before

transferring to a 4-year college to complete a bachelor’s degree, many ethnic minority

students attend community colleges to reduce the costs of attending college and these

junior colleges may not prepare them as well. Also, American Indian students from

remote tribes or on reservations were unable to attain a highly competitive education

because their schools were not able to hire outstanding faculty due to the lack of financial

resources (Evans, 2008).

Aside from task aversiveness and fear of failure, research has also looked into

other factors that may contribute or decrease procrastination among students such as

self-efficacy, perfectionism, self-esteem, implicit theory, and self-control (Howell &

Buro, 2008; Flett et al., 2004; Klassen et al., 2010; Seo, 2008; Steel, 2007). While these
16

factors have shown some promise and may be important to study, task aversiveness and

fear of failure seem to have a fairly consistent and strong relationship with academic

procrastination.

Reading Ability, Procrastination, and Academic Performance

Past research have provided some evidence that reading ability may be a

significant predictor of procrastination. Due to the limited research available on reading

ability, procrastination, and academic performance with college students, a review of the

procrastination literature with other age groups will be cited in this section to help explain

how reading ability affects academic performance and procrastination. Reading ability

has been operationally defined in many ways in previous research including reading

comprehension and reading vocabulary (Collins, Onwuegbuzie, & Jiao, 2008; Derps &

Onwuegbuzie, 2001; Neddenriep & Hale, 2011). Reading comprehension is the level at

which an individual can understand the text and understand the interaction between the

words that are written. A student’s reading vocabulary pertains to the number of words

that he or she can easily recognize and understand when reading. The student’s ability to

read not only has an impact on current tasks that he or she may be working on, but the

outcome of those tasks would affect how the student views his or her ability to complete

those same tasks in the future (Klassen et al., 2008). For example, a student who does

not have strong reading ability may not do well with academic tasks that are primarily

reading-oriented.

The relationship between reading skills and academic achievement has been well-

documented in academia. Poor reading skills often lead to elevated dropout rates,
17

decreased grade retention, and overall poor academic achievement which can suppress an

individual’s learning potential (Annie E. Casey Foundation [AECF], 2010; Neddenriep &

Hale, 2011). Within elementary school students, 33% of all fourth-graders in the U.S.

report test scores that are below a basic reading level (National Assessment Educational

Progress [NAEP], 2011). At the fourth-grade, students with a basic level of reading

ability should be able to locate relevant information, make simple inferences, and use

their understanding of the text to identify details that support a given interpretation or

conclusion (NAEP, 2011). In 2009, 83% of the children from low-income families and

85% of low-income students who attended high-poverty schools in the U.S. failed to

reach a proficient level in reading (AECF, 2010). Having a higher level of reading ability

at elementary school was a good predictor of future academic success in high school. For

example, Georgia high school seniors who scored high on their reading ability in the

eighth grade were more successful on their Georgia High School Graduation Tests (Derps

& Onwuegbuzie, 2001). In 2002, 26% of all high school students in the U.S. reported

test scores that were below a basic reading level. At a basic reading level, a high school

student should be able to identify and relate aspects of the text to its overall meaning,

extend the ideas in the text by making simple inferences, recognize interpretations, make

connections between ideas in the text to their personal experiences, and draw logical

conclusions (NAEP, 2011). Du Boulay (1999) believes that one of the biggest problems

in higher education is difficulty with reading with many students not being evaluated for

poor reading proficiency until they are well into their academic career. Students with

poor reading skills may find it more difficult to understand context within the literature
18

and this may lead to superficial understanding, which may result in poor grades on

assignments related to reading materials. They may also have difficulty attempting to

identify and interpret arguments that are presented in their academic text (Du Boulay,

1999).

In the U.S., there are also a significant number of students who have English as a

second language (ESL) and this can compromise their reading ability and academic

performance. For example, English language learners in U.S. schools have

disproportionately demonstrated reading difficulties at different grade levels according to

the National Center for Education Statistics (2009). Children who enter U.S. schools in

kindergarten and are English language learners face many challenges such as trying to

learn to read English which they are not yet proficient in (Kieffer, 2010). By fourth

grade, two out of every three Spanish-speaking students are unable to read English at

basic levels necessary for success in school. In the U.S., Latino students are retained or

held back more often, are disproportionately represented in special education programs,

and are 3 times more likely to drop out of school than White American students

(Petscher, 2009). The U.S. Department of Education (2002) reported that limited-

English-proficient (LEP) children are among the highest groups with reading failure. In

regard to college students, nursing students who speak English as a second language have

lower academic success compared with their native English-speaking counterparts

(Salamonson et al., 2010).

Many children with limited English proficiency may also have parents who are

poorly educated, come from low income families, and may attend schools that are
19

composed primarily of ethnic minorities and are low achieving (Snow, Burns, & Griffin,

1998). With that in mind, socioeconomic status may be an important predictor to

consider with regard to reading success and failure. Low socioeconomic status families

also tend to receive less adequate nutrition, prenatal care, pediatric care, and incorporate

other conditions that may be detrimental to the health, safety, and development of young

children. Low SES children are more likely to report with poorer reading ability than

children who come from high-income families and schools that are located in low SES

communities tend to produce poorer readers than more affluent communities (Snow,

Burns, & Griffin, 1998).

Significant ethnic differences have been found in reading among high school

students. According to Moore et al. (2010), there were statistically significant differences

in college ready reading levels among Latino, African American, and White American

students. Sixty three percent of Latinos and 66% of African American students were not

college ready in reading in comparison to only 47% of White American students.

Although the study reported lower levels of reading readiness among Latino and African

American students, the authors noted this discrepancy may be due to fewer academic

resources at public schools that are attended by a majority of ethnic minority students.

This may indicate that it may be the level of poverty in the community that the ethnic

minority students live in rather than ethnicity itself that contributes to these negative

findings. Racial stereotypes continue to persist in academia despite advances across the

last century in equity in educational opportunities and outcomes. Traditional stereotypes

are that African Americans have poorer academic abilities and skills in all academic
20

subjects relative to their White American and Asian American counterparts, and that by

early adolescence, youth become aware of and begin to endorse traditional race

stereotypes (Evans et al., 2011). By middle school, African American and White

American students report that White American students are better in academic domains

than African American students. Research has also shown evidence of stereotype threat

fir African Americans when asked to identify their racial identity before a test, African

American students performed worse than when they were not instructed identify their

race. Just asking for their racial identity forced African American students to think about

stereotypes concerning academic achievement, which in turn had a significant effect on

their academic performance (Chavous et al., 2003).

Research has shown that college students with poor reading abilities have more

difficulty with academic course work (Collins & Onwuegbuzie, 2002; Onwuegbuzie &

Collins, 2002; Onwuegbuzie et al., 2004). Among undergraduate students, reading

comprehension scores on the Nelson-Denny Reading Test were found to be good

predictors of academic performance (Feldt, 1988; Roberts et al., 1990). Reading

comprehension and reading vocabulary were also found to be significant predictors of

performance in research methodology courses for graduate students, with poor reading

skills contributing to underachievement (Collins & Onwuegbuzie, 2002). As some

studies have shown, students who do not have very high reading ability tend to have

lower grades and scores than those with higher reading ability (Collins & Onwuegbuzie,

2002; Du Boulay, 1999; Neddenriep & Hale, 2011; Onwuegbuzie & Collins, 2002;

Onwuegbuzie et al., 2004). Poor scores on previous assignments could also affect a
21

student’s self-efficacy toward future reading and reading tasks. This lowered sense of

self-efficacy decreases their confidence in being able to complete the reading task

(Corkett et al., 2011). The task is now viewed as being more difficult because the student

believes that he or she may not have the necessary tools to complete the task at an

adequate level. As stated earlier, students tend to procrastinate on tasks that were

considered least interesting or more challenging (Steel, 2007). College students who

have poor reading comprehension and vocabulary would view tasks that were primarily

reading tasks as more difficult, which may lead to an increase in procrastination within

those tasks. Although these studies on reading ability and academic achievement used

samples of graduate students (Collins & Onwuegbuzie, 2002; Onwuegbuzie & Collins,

2002; Onwuegbuzie et al., 2004), grade school children (Corkett et al., 2011), and

undergraduate students (Du Boulay, 1999), their overall findings were very similar to one

another.

The majority of reading ability studies have used questionnaires to collect data

with many studies using the Nelson-Denny Reading Test (NDRT) questionnaire for their

methodology for both undergraduate and graduate students (e.g., Collins &

Onwuegbuzie, 2002; Collins, et al., 2008; Feldt, 1988; Gerow, & Murphy, 1980;

Masterson, & Hayes, 2004; Onwuegbuzie & Collins, 2002; Onwuegbuzie et al., 2004;

Roberts et al., 1990). This test is used for assessment purposes and is frequently used in

research studies with norms being provided by semester and year for high school and

undergraduate college students (Masterson, & Hayes, 2004). This test also has adequate

score reliability and score validity as reported in the literature (Collins, Onwuegbuzie, &
22

Jiao, 2011). The NDRT consists of 118 multiple-choice items that covers seven reading

passages. These items are divided into subtests intended to measure comprehension (80

items) and vocabulary (38 items) (Collins et al., 2008).

Collins et al. (2008) studied the relationship between reading ability and

procrastination among African American graduate students. Specifically, they were

interested in two reading variables (reading comprehension and reading vocabulary) and

their relationship to procrastination. The researchers hypothesized that poor reading

ability would impact the tendency to procrastinate among African American graduate

students. At a graduate level, reading materials and assignments are more complex than

undergraduate levels and therefore, it is hypothesized that if the student is not confident

in their reading ability, then he or she may be more prone to procrastination because

reading tasks may be seen as more difficult. A canonical correlation was conducted to

identify any combination of reading ability (i.e., reading comprehension and reading

vocabulary) that might predict a combination of reasons for procrastination dimensions

(i.e., fear of failure and task aversiveness). Another canonical correlation analysis was

done to identify any combination of reading ability that might predict a combination of

sources of procrastination (e.g., writing assignments, studying, and keeping up on

reading). It was discovered that there was a significant multivariate relationship between

two reading ability variables and procrastination for African American graduate students.

The scale that the authors used to measure procrastination was the Procrastination

Assessment Scale-Student (PASS). The results revealed a relationship between poor

reading ability and academic procrastination. Researchers also found a significant


23

relationship between poor reading ability and academic procrastination associated with

writing a term paper, performing administrative tasks, attending meetings, keeping up

with weekly reading assignments, and, most notably, performing academic tasks in

general (Collins et al., 2008). These results suggest that reading ability is related to

academic procrastination among African American graduate students. However, future

studies should be conducted to gather more information about the relationship between

reading ability and academic procrastination. It was not clear whether “reading ability is

the cause of academic procrastination or whether academic procrastination inhibits

development of reading ability” (Collins et al., 2008, p. 503). Further research would be

necessary to determine whether students with poor reading abilities would experience

difficulties with complex reading materials and whether they would exhibit more

avoidant behaviors associated with reading tasks (procrastination) or procrastination

would create missed opportunities for the student to strengthen his or her reading abilities

(Collins et al., 2008).

Solomon and Rothblum (1984) conducted a study to investigate the frequency of

procrastination on academic tasks and the reasons for procrastination behavior using the

PASS. They systematically assessed the reasons for procrastination using a factor

analysis on the second portion of the PASS which has the reasons for procrastination

items. The first factor seemed to reflect fear of failure by clumping together items related

to anxiety about meeting others' expectations (evaluation anxiety), concern about meeting

one's own standards (perfectionism), and a lack of self-confidence. The first factor, fear

of failure, contributed to 49.4% of the variance. The second factor related to


24

aversiveness of the task and laziness and accounted for 18% of the variance in

procrastination. Thus, the factor analysis determined that fear of failure and task

aversiveness were two primary independent reasons for procrastination. Given this

model, a student’s confidence in reading comprehension and vocabulary would greatly

affect their likelihood of procrastinating on tasks related to reading. Students who

believe they do not have adequate reading skills to succeed could experience anxiety and

fear over upcoming assignments and tasks that are based primarily on reading. This

anxiety and fear of an upcoming reading task could prevent the student from doing the

task in a timely manner and encourage procrastination. Anxiety over their performance

and eventual evaluation of the reading task may also lead to procrastination. Chronic

procrastinators avoid activities that may show their limitations or deficiencies (Ferrari,

1991). Students who have poor reading abilities may avoid reading because the activity

may show what his or her true reading level may be. By putting off the reading

assignment and doing something that is enjoyable, the student would reinforce the

behavior of procrastination by alleviating the anxiety with a pleasant feeling (Ferrari et

at., 1995). Essentially, the student may be anxious over the reading task because he or

she may believe that they do not have the adequate skills needed to perform at an

acceptable level.

As stated earlier, individuals tend to engage in more procrastination behaviors on

tasks that are considered less enjoyable and unpleasant (Solomon & Rothblum, 1984).

With many different academic tasks given within their collegiate career, students may

place more value and enjoyment on some activities over others. Reading, writing, and
25

taking exams were among the tasks that college students viewed as least enjoyable and

with limited value (Schroth, Pankake, & Gates, 1999). What the student chooses to read

is based on the perception of the purpose of reading the material and any problems that

come up while reading. Thus, students who may view reading as unpleasant or not

enjoyable and encounter problems by doing so are more likely to procrastinate (Collins et

al., 2008; Solomon & Rothblum (1984). This negative perception may contribute to

procrastination (Collins et al., 2008)

Although it has been shown that both negative perceptions and poor reading

ability are good predictors of academic procrastination, there is no data or research to

differentiate which perspective would create more procrastination.

Although Collins et al. (2008) did find a relationship between a student’s reading

ability and procrastination, it was with a specific graduate student population – African

Americans - and there is no available research that has examined this relationship of

reading ability and procrastination for undergraduate students. Undergraduate students

vary differently in reading ability in comparison to graduate students, with graduate

students, on average, having higher reading ability than undergraduate students

(Onwuegbuzie & Collins, 2002; Onwuegbuzie et al., 2004). Graduate students’ course

load and course reading are more difficult than undergraduate students and graduate

students are also required to have a higher reading level than undergraduate students.

With lower levels of reading ability than graduate students, undergraduate students may

potentially be more susceptible to procrastination behaviors. This would be an important

area to research because it would allow us to identify whether the same factors that
26

contribute to academic procrastination for graduate students could also affect

undergraduate students. Second, if it is found that reading ability is a significant

predictor of academic procrastination for undergraduate students, it would be useful to

provide services for those students who need more help in reading comprehension and

vocabulary as well as developing better criteria to help high school students to become

better prepared for college level work. Following Collins, Onwuegbuzie, and Jiao’s

(2008) hypothesis, reading ability may be a factor in academic procrastination for

undergraduate students, but there may be a higher rate of procrastination among

undergraduates due to lower levels of reading ability across the population.

Summary

This literature review of the factors that may be related to academic

procrastination suggests several themes in the current literature.

1. Previous studies have shown academic procrastination is common and

problematic among undergraduate students with many negative consequences.

Students who procrastinate tend to submit late assignments, miss academic

deadlines, cram for exams, experience test and social anxiety, submit lower

quality work, and receive lower grade point averages (Ferrari, Johnson, &

McCown, 1995; Klassen et al., 2008; Pychyl, Lee, Thibodeau, & Blunt,

2000).

2. There are also many different factors that contribute to procrastination

including age, gender, SES, task aversiveness, and fear of failure. In general,

younger students, males, and those from lower socioeconomic status were
27

more prone to procrastination (Klassen et al., 2010; Schouwenburg et al.,

2004; Steel, 2007). Individuals who had higher levels of task aversiveness

and fear of failure were also more likely to procrastinate (Milgram et al.,

1995; Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown, 1995).

3. Finally, although there are many factors that may contribute and/or predict

academic procrastination, there is little research being conducted on the

influence of reading ability to procrastination. Reading ability, which consists

of reading comprehension and reading vocabulary, may have a significant

effect on a student’s academic performance. Lower levels of reading ability

negatively affect students in various ways such as elevated dropout rates,

decreased grade retention, and overall poor academic achievement (AECF,

2010; Collins & Onwuegbuzie, 2002; Du Boulay, 1999; Neddenriep & Hale,

2011). At the same time, reading ability was also found to be a significant

predictor of academic performance among undergraduate students (Feldt,

1988). In regard to procrastination, little research has been conducted to

examine the possible relationship between reading ability and academic

procrastination for undergraduate students. However, research with African

American graduate students found that reading ability was a good predictor of

academic procrastination (Collins, Onwuegbuzie, & Jiao, 2008).

4. Past studies have indicated that there are no ethnic differences in academic

procrastination for college students when using the PASS (Prohaska et al.,

2000). However, as stated before, one limitation with past studies and the
28

PASS was the way in which procrastination was operationally defined. To

measure procrastination, the majority of studies using the PASS calculated a

single aggregate score by summing the scores of 5-point Likert scales on the

frequency of procrastination and whether the student viewed their

procrastination as problematic on six academic tasks (Alexander &

Onwuegbuzie, 2007; Beck et al., 2000; Clark & Hill, 1995; Collins et al.,

2008; Prohaska et al., 2000; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984;). Past research has

shown that when looking at the frequency scale and problematic scale

separately, significant ethnic differences were found in comparison to the lack

of significant ethnic differences reported for the single aggregate score for

procrastination. It would be beneficial to examine the frequency of

procrastination and whether procrastination is viewed as problematic

separately to determine any significant differences in overall procrastination

in comparison to the summed procrastination score that has been used in the

past. Although past research has indicated that ethnic minority students tend

to procrastinate more on keeping up with weekly reading assignments in

comparison to White American students, further investigation should be

conducted to determine whether the underlying factor of procrastinating on

weekly reading assignments is due to limited reading ability (comprehension

and vocabulary) instead of solely ethnicity. It is well documented that ethnic

minorities tend to live in lower socioeconomic communities and have higher


29

levels of ESLs, which can increase the risk of having poorer reading skills

(Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998).

5. Ethnic differences on procrastination focused primarily on African American

and Latino students. There is little research comparing Asian Americans to

other ethnic minorities or to White American students. Studies on Asian

American academic work ethic and success provide evidence for a pattern of

high academic achievement among Asian American students (Yao, 2010).

The most popular theories on why Asian American students are successful in

academia focus on the influence of Asian American cultural values,

particularly values that emphasize the importance of education and hard work.

Asian American students report high parental expectations, devote more time

to their studies, spend twice as much time on homework per week, and

contribute their effort not ability, to academic success. However, this can lead

to a perpetuation of the “model minority” stereotype about Asian Americans

when several Asian American groups have been found to struggle with

academics.

Present Study

The present study examined ethnic group differences in procrastination behaviors

for specific academic tasks among Asian, African, Latino, and White American college

students. Previous studies have reported there are no significant ethnic differences in

overall levels of academic procrastination for ethnic minority groups (Prohaska et al.,

2000). However, there are few studies available which have examined this phenomenon
30

with various ethnic groups and this dearth of research has prevented any definitive

conclusions being drawn concerning this important area of study. Also, another

limitation of previous research is that procrastination is often studied as a single generic

construct and this has prevented an examination of whether there may be ethnic

differences in procrastination for certain types of academic tasks (e.g., studying for an

exam) (Clark & Hill, 1994; Collins, Onwuegbuzie, & Jiao, 2008; Prohaska et al., 2000;

Solomon & Rothblum, 1984).

The current study examined ethnic differences in procrastination behaviors for six

different academic tasks using three methods for assessing procrastination: Problem

frequency, problem severity, and a combined score of problem frequency and severity.

In this manner, the present study hoped to gain more information on whether certain

ethnic groups procrastinate more on certain tasks and whether this is a critical issue of

problem frequency and/or severity for African, Asian, Latino, and White American

college students. The current study also attempted to determine if other critical factors

identified in the literature such as reading ability, fear of failure, and task aversiveness

may moderate this significant relationship of ethnicity and academic procrastination for

Asian, African, Latino, and White American students. For example, a recent study

showed that fear of failure played a critical role between the relationship of poor reading

ability and academic procrastination for African American students (Collins,

Onwuegbuzie, & Jiao, 2008).

Also, since many Asian and Latino American students come from households

where English may not be the primary language, it is possible that English language and
31

reading ability could play a critical role as a mediator of ethnicity and academic

procrastination for these two ethnic minority groups.

Hypothesis 1 proposes that African and Latino American students will report

higher levels of overall academic procrastination than Asian and White American

students after controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of age, gender,

ESL, and SES on specific academic tasks.

Hypothesis 2 proposes that African, and Latino American students will differ in

academic procrastination on six academic task areas than Asian and White American

students after controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of age, gender,

ESL, and SES on specific academic tasks.

Hypothesis 2A: Asian and White American students will report higher

academic procrastination on writing term papers than African and Latino

American students after controlling for the significant contributions of the

covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES.

Hypothesis 2B: African and Latino American students will report higher

academic procrastination on studying for exams than Asian and White American

students after controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of age,

gender, ESL, and SES.

Hypothesis 2C: African and Latino American students will report higher

academic procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading assignments than

Asian and White American students after controlling for the significant

contributions of the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES.


32

Hypothesis 2D: African and Latino American students will report higher

academic procrastination on performing administrative tasks than Asian and

White American students after controlling for the significant contributions of the

covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES.

Hypothesis 2E: Asian and White American students will report higher

academic procrastination on attending meetings than African and Latino

American students after controlling for the significant contributions of the

covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES.

Hypothesis 2F: African and Latino American students will report higher

academic procrastination on academic tasks in general than Asian and White

American students after controlling for the significant contributions of the

covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES.

Hypothesis 3 proposes that African and Latino American students will differ in

reading vocabulary and comprehension than Asian and White American students after

controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and

SES.

Hypothesis 3A: African and Latino American students will report lower

levels of reading vocabulary than Asian and White American students after

controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of age, gender, ESL,

and SES.

Hypothesis 3B: African and Latino American students will report lower

levels of reading comprehension than Asian and White American students after
33

controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of age, gender, ESL,

and SES.

Hypothesis 4 proposes that Asian, African, and Latino American student will

have higher levels of task aversiveness and fear of failure than White American students

when controlling for the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES.

Hypothesis 5 proposes that reading comprehension and reading vocabulary will

be a stronger predictor of procrastination than ethnicity after controlling for the

significant contributions of the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES.

Hypothesis 6 proposes that task aversiveness and fear of failure will be stronger

predictors of procrastination than ethnicity and reading ability (comprehension and

vocabulary) while controlling for the significant contribution of the covariates of age,

gender, ESL, and SES.


34

Chapter 2

METHOD

Participants

Initially, 305 participants were collected, however selection for the study was

based on completion of the demographics form which included age, gender, ethnicity,

ESL, and SES, and completion of all measures. After taking into account all the criteria

for inclusion in the study only 252 undergraduate students from a large northern

California university were used. Participants were recruited for the study through the

campus psychology research website and participants received course credit for their

participation. Data was collected on multiple ethnic groups, but statistical analyses were

focused on ethnic group comparisons of Asian, African, Latino, and White American

participants. To be included in the final analysis, participants had to meet all of the

following selection criteria: 1) only participants who self-identified themselves as being

Asian, African, Latino, or White American were included in the final sample, 2) only

participants who provide valid information about socioeconomic status were included in

the final sample to control for this important variable, and 3) only participants who

provided valid information for all other variables of interest in the study were included in

the final sample. The final sample consisted of 252 participants and the ethnic

distribution was 58 Asian American, 28 African American, 73 Latino American, and 93

White American college students. The gender distribution was 66 males and 186

females. The mean age of the final sample was 20.68 years and participants’ family of
35

origin socioeconomic index scores ranged from 3 to 66, with a mean SES index of 40.86

using the Hollingshead four factor index for social status (Hollingshead, 1975). The

number of participants in the final sample exceeded the minimum number of participants

(N = 107) needed for a multiple regression analysis with more than eight variables with a

power level of .80 and .05 alpha level for the effect size as outlined by Cohen (1992).

Measures and Scales

Independent variables

Demographic information. Participants were asked to provide demographic

information including ethnicity. Specifically, the participants were asked to self-identify

as Asian American, African American, Latino American, White American, or other.

Participants were given the option to check multiple boxes if needed to identify their

mixed ethnic heritage. Participants were asked about their gender (male or female) and

age (birthdate and chronological age in years and months). Marital status was

categorized as being single, married, divorced, separated, and widowed. Participants

were asked what primary language was spoken at home as well as their country of origin.

Participants were also asked if English was their second language or not, school major,

years in the U.S., expectations of academic success, employment, and number of

children. Students were asked what their perceived SES was during childhood and what

their perceived current SES is. Finally, participants were asked about their mother’s and

father’s level of education and work occupation.

To determine socioeconomic status of the participants’ families, the Hollingshead

Four Factor Index of Social Status (1975) was used to determine the SES of their family
36

of origin (childhood) and current family situation if the latter differed from specific

information provided for the family of origin. The Hollingshead Four Factor Index of

Social Status uses a formula based on the marital status of the parents, each parent’s

occupation, and each parent’s level of completed education to compute a final SES score

which ranges from eight to 66. The status score of an individual was calculated by

multiplying the scale value of occupation by a weight of five and the scale value of

education by a weight of three. If both parents were married and employed, the

socioeconomic index scores for that particular family was divided by two. However, if

the parent was single or if one of the married parents did not have gainful employment,

then it was not divided by two. The higher the socioeconomic index score, the higher the

SES the family will have.

Reading Ability. The Nelson Denny Reading Test Form G (NDRT; Brown,

Fishco, & Hanna, 1993) was used to measure reading ability among undergraduate

students. As stated earlier, the NDRT measures for reading comprehension and reading

vocabulary, which consists of 118 multiple-choice items covering seven passages

creating the subtests to measure comprehension (38 items) and vocabulary (80 items).

Collins and Onwuebuzie (2002) reported an internal consistency reliability of .96 for the

reading comprehension subtest (95% confidence interval [CI] = .94, .98) and .94 for the

reading vocabulary subtest (95% CI = .92, .96). Although previous research with

procrastination and reading ability had been done with graduate students only using this

measure, other research examining reading ability and other psychological phenomenon

have used NDRT as a reliable measure for reading ability among undergraduate students
37

(Masterson & Hayes, 2004).

Dependent variable

Procrastination. Procrastination was measured by using the Procrastination

Assessment Scale-Students (Appendix G), which was developed by Solomon and

Rothblum (1988). The PASS consists of two sections. The first section assesses the

prevalence of procrastination in six areas of academic functioning. These academic areas

were “writing a term paper”, “studying for exams”, “keeping up with weekly reading

assignments”, “performing administrative tasks”, “attending meetings”, and “performing

academic tasks in general”. Participants were asked to complete three 5-point Likert

rating scales for each of the academic areas indicating how often they procrastinate on

each one (1 = Never procrastinate, 5 = Always procrastinate), whether they consider their

procrastination in the area a problem (1 = Not a problem at all, 5 = Always a problem),

and whether they want to decrease their procrastination in the area (1 = Do not want to

decrease, 5 = Definitely want to decrease). An overall measure of general procrastination

on the PASS was created by taking the sum of the first two ratings on each

procrastination area and then adding up the scores across the six procrastination areas to

create a single summed score. Scores on this section of the PASS range from 12 to 60,

with higher scores being indicative of self-reported academic procrastination. For the

current study, the individual subscales of frequency and whether procrastination is

problematic were also examined separately on each of the 6 procrastination areas to

determine if ethnic differences may occur in these six areas concerning problem

frequency and/or severity.


38

The second section of the PASS reflects the specific reasons for the reported

procrastination. In this section, participants were given a scenario about each of the

procrastination areas above and then were presented with a list of reasons for the

procrastination on the 6 different tasks. Participants were asked to indicate how much

they endorse each of the 26 reasons using a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = Not at all

reflects why I procrastinated, 5 = Definitely reflects why I procrastinated). As mentioned

before, Solomon and Rothblum (1988) conducted a factor analysis on the specific reasons

for why college students procrastinate and they reported two main factors, fear of failure

and task aversiveness. Scores for the Reasons of Procrastination Fear of Failure were

computed by averaging items 19, 24, 33, 39, and 42. Higher scores on the subtest of Fear

of Failure signify Fear of Failure as a reason for procrastinating. The scores for the

subtests of task aversiveness were created by taking the mean of items 27, 34, and 35.

Higher scores on the subtest of task aversivesness signify task aversiveness as a reason

for procrastinating.

A number of studies indicate that the PASS possesses adequate reliability and

validity (Clark & Hill, 1995; Collins et al., 2008; Prohaska et al., 2000; Solomon &

Rothblum, 1984). The PASS has exhibited adequate levels of coefficient alpha

(prevalence, .75; perceived problems, .70) and test-retest reliability over a six-week

interval (prevalence, r = .74; perceived problems, r = .65) (Ferrari, 1989). The

coefficient alpha reliability estimates of the PASS scores were .86 (95% CI = .82, .89) for

the overall procrastination scale, .84 (95% CI = .79, .88) for the fear of failure factor, .77

(95% CI = .69, .83) for the task aversiveness factor, .82 (95% CI = .74, .87) for the
39

writing a term paper factor, .73 (95% CI = .61, .81) for the studying for examinations

factor, .65 (95% CI = .50, .76) for the keeping up with weekly reading assignments

factor, .86 (95% CI = .80, .90) for the performing administrative tasks factor, .83 (95% CI

= .76, .88) for the attending meetings factor, and .87 (95% CI = .81, .91) for the

performing academic tasks in general factor (Collins et al., 2008).

Procedures

Participants signed up for the study through the campus psychology research

website. When a participant arrived for the start of a research session, the principal

investigator met with the student and he/she was asked to provide both written and verbal

consent to participate in the study. When the participant agreed to participate in the

study, he/she received a copy of his/her written consent for his/her personal records and

the participants were then handed the questionnaires to complete. The survey took

approximately an hour and a half to complete. After completing the survey, the

participant received a debriefing form which explained the purpose of the study in greater

detail and provided contact information of the researcher should they have any further

questions about the study. The researcher then submitted course credit for their

participation through the psychology research website.


40

Chapter 3

RESULTS

Correlational analyses were performed on the frequency and problem scores for

the six procrastination tasks of writing a term paper, studying for exams, keeping up with

weekly reading assignments, performing academic administrative tasks, attending

meetings, and performing academic tasks in general, These results showed the frequency

and problem scores for each of the six procrastination tasks were highly correlated and it

would be best to combine these two scores to create a single mean score to represent

procrastination for the six academic tasks to be used in subsequent analyses.

Table 1 provides the means and standard deviations for procrastination on the six

academic tasks, overall procrastination, reading vocabulary, reading comprehension, task

aversiveness, and fear of failure for Asian, African, Latino, and White Americans.

Keeping up with weekly reading assignments was the task that was most procrastinated

on, while performing administrative tasks was the lowest. Overall, students tended to

procrastinate more on tasks associated with class performance and less on tasks that were

associated with policy and procedure. Table 2 provides the correlation matrix for age,

SES, the six academic tasks relating to procrastination, overall procrastination, reading

vocabulary, reading comprehension, task aversiveness, and fear of failure. It was found

that age was highly correlated with reading vocabulary. Several correlations were found

to be significant and these will be discussed later with regard to critical decisions that
41

were made to address issues of multicollinearity in the hierarchical multiple regression

analyses.

Hypothesis 1 proposed that Asian, African, and Latino American students would

report a higher overall score of procrastination than White American students, after

controlling for the significant contribution of age, gender, ESL, and SES. An ANCOVA

was performed to test this hypothesis and the results showed no significant effect for

ethnicity or the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES for the overall score of

procrastination.

Hypothesis 2 proposed there would be significant ethnic differences in levels of

procrastination on the six academic tasks among Asian, African, Latino, and White

American students, after controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of

age, gender, ESL, and SES. Specifically, Hypothesis 2A proposed that Asian and White

American students would report higher procrastination on writing a term paper than

African, and Latino American students, after controlling for the significant contributions

of the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES. Hypotheses 2B-2D proposed that

African and Latino American students would report higher procrastination on studying

for exams, keeping up with weekly reading assignments, and performing administrative

tasks than Asian and White American students, after controlling for the significant

contributions of the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES. Hypothesis 2E proposed

that Asian and White American students would report higher procrastination on attending
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for the Six Academic Procrastination Tasks, Overall Procrastination, Reading Vocabulary, Reading
Comprehension, Task Aversiveness, and Fear of Failure by Ethnicity
Asian American African American Latino American White American
(N = 58) (N = 28) (N = 73) (N = 93)
Variables M SD M SD M SD M SD
Writing a term paper 3.64 .82 3.59 .92 3.72 .80 3.42 .91
Studying for exams 3.66 .73 3.66 .86 3.63 .86 3.40 .96
Keeping up with weekly reading 3.84 .87 3.73 1.22 3.52 .93 3.15 1.02
assignments
Performing administrative tasks 2.34 1.17 1.82 .79 2.36 1.18 2.14 1.17
Attending meetings 2.54 1.17 2.37 1.51 2.42 1.17 2.57 1.20
Performing academic tasks in 2.80 .99 2.39 1.26 2.88 .95 2.53 .82
general
Overall procrastination 37.67 7.40 35.14 8.00 37.09 7.88 34.46 7.11
Note: Procrastination scores for the six academic tasks range from 1 (Never) to 5 (Always); Overall Procrastination scores range from 6-60;
Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Reading Comprehension scores range from 0 (Low) to 36 (High). Task
Aversiveness and Fear of Failure scores range from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).

42
Table 1 (Cont.)
Means and Standard Deviations for the Six Academic Procrastination Tasks, Overall Procrastination, Reading Vocabulary, Reading
Comprehension, Task Aversiveness, and Fear of Failure by Ethnicity
Asian American African American Latino American White American
(N = 58) (N = 28) (N = 73) (N = 93)
Variables M SD M SD M SD M SD
Reading vocabulary 49.15 9.73 55.75 8.70 53.90 11.22 59.19 9.41
Reading comprehension 27.98 4.37 25.43 7.45 28.81 6.21 29.44 5.14
Task aversiveness 3.42 .97 3.21 1.00 3.34 1.12 3.25 .97
Fear of failure 2.44 1.08 2.16 .99 2.28 1.09 2.09 .98
Note: Procrastination scores for the six academic tasks range from 1 (Never) to 5 (Always); Overall Procrastination scores range from 6-60;
Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Reading Comprehension scores range from 0 (Low) to 36 (High). Task
Aversiveness and Fear of Failure scores range from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).

43
Table 2
Correlations between Age, SES, the Six Academic Procrastination Tasks, Overall Procrastination, Reading Vocabulary, Reading
Comprehension, Task Aversiveness, and Fear of Failure
Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Age 20.68 2.59 -
2. SES 40.85 13.41 .01 -
3. Writing a term paper 3.58 .87 .04 -.12 -
4. Studying for exams 3.56 .88 -.11 -.14* .50** -
5. Keeping up with weekly reading 3.48 1.02 -.14* .02 .39** .39** -
Assignments
6. Performing administrative tasks 2.21 1.14 -.02 -.04 .15** .13* .19** -
7. Attending meetings 2.50 1.17 .06 .01 .29** .21** .23** .37**
8. Performing academic tasks in general 2.68 .97 .02 -.12 .34** .26** .26** .15*
9. Overall procrastination 36.03 7.59 .04 -.10 .67** .60** .63** .57**
10. Reading vocabulary 54.97 10.63 .26** .23** .06 -.10 .07 -.01
11. Reading comprehension 28.48 5.71 .08 -.05 .11 -.03 .02 -.06
12. Task aversiveness 3.31 1.02 -.06 -.06 .39** .30** .28** .13**
13. Fear of failure 2.23 1.04 .05 -.11 .29** .26** .09 .05
Note: Procrastination scores for the six academic tasks range from 1 (Never) to 5 (Always); Overall Procrastination scores range from 6-60;
Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Reading Comprehension scores range from 0 (Low) to 36 (High). Task
Aversiveness and Fear of Failure scores range from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

44
Table 2 (Cont.)
Correlations between Age, SES, the Six Academic Procrastination Tasks, Overall Procrastination, Reading Vocabulary, Reading
Comprehension, Task Aversiveness, and Fear of Failure
Variables M SD 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
1. Age 20.68 2.59
2. SES 40.85 13.41
3. Writing a term paper 3.58 .87
4. Studying for exams 3.56 .88
5. Keeping up with weekly reading 3.48 1.02
assignments
6. Performing administrative tasks 2.21 1.14
7. Attending meetings 2.50 1.17 -
8. Performing academic tasks in general 2.68 .97 .33** -
9. Overall procrastination 36.03 7.59 .68** .61** -
10. Reading vocabulary 54.97 10.63 .07 .09 .03 -
11. Reading comprehension 28.48 5.71 .03 .12 .04 .54** -
12. Task aversiveness 3.31 1.02 .24** .19** .39** -.07 -.07 -
13. Fear of failure 2.23 1.04 .08 .13* .22** -.25** -.15* .34** --
Note: Procrastination scores for the six academic tasks range from 1 (Never) to 5 (Always); Overall Procrastination scores range from 6-60;
Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Reading Comprehension scores range from 0 (Low) to 36 (High). Task
Aversiveness and Fear of Failure scores range from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

45
46

meetings than African, and Latino American students after, controlling for the significant

contributions of the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES. Finally, Hypothesis 2F

proposed that African and Latino American students would report more procrastination

on performing academic tasks in general than Asian and White American students after,

controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and

SES.

A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was performed to determine

if there were significant ethnic differences on reported procrastination on the six

academic tasks, after controlling for the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES. The

results of the MANCOVA on procrastination for the six academic tasks showed a

significant main effect for ethnicity, Pillai’s Trace = .126, F(3, 251) = 1.75, p = .027, and

a significant effect for the covariate of age, Pillai’s Trace = .059, F(1, 251) = 2.35, p =

.032. A Bonferroni correction was used to examine significance for the univariate tests

and a more stringent cut off score for significance was set at p < .008 (.05/6). In

reviewing the results of the analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs), a significant main effect

for ethnicity, F(3, 251) = 6.73, p < .001, η2 = .01, was found for keeping up with weekly

reading assignments. For pairwise comparisons for ethnicity in the ANCOVAs, a

Bonferroni correction was used because the Levene’s tests showed the variances were not

equal for some of the six academic tasks. For keeping up with weekly reading

assignments, Asian and African American students reported higher procrastination than

White American students.


47

Hypothesis 3 proposed that African and Latino American students would report

lower reading vocabulary and reading comprehension scores than Asian and White

American students, after controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of

age, gender, ESL and SES. A MANCOVA was performed to test this hypothesis and the

overall results for reading vocabulary and reading comprehension showed a significant

main effect for the covariates of age, Pillai’s Trace = .059, F(1, 251) = 7.62, p = .001,

gender, Pillai’s Trace = .080, F(1, 251) = 10.54, p < .001, ESL, Pillai’s Trace = .084,

F(1, 251) = 11.20, p < .001, SES, Pillai’s Trace = .036, F(1, 251) = 4.51, p = .012, and

ethnicity, Pillai’s Trace = .135, F(3, 251) = 5.89, p < .001. A Bonferroni correction was

used to examine significance for the univariate tests and a more stringent cut-off score for

significance was set at p < .025 (.05/2). However, in reviewing the results of the

ANCOVAs, only a significant main effect for the covariates of age, F(1, 251) = 13.88, p

< .001, η2 = .03, gender, F(1, 251) = 12.43, p = .001, η2 = .03, ESL, F(1, 251) = 21.13, p

< .001, η2 = .05, and ethnicity F(3, 251) = 6.73, p < .001, η2 = .05, were reported for

reading vocabulary. Specifically, younger adults reported lower reading vocabulary

scores than older adults. For gender, females reported lower scores on reading

vocabulary than males. Also, ESL students reported lower levels of reading vocabulary

than non-ESL students. Finally for ethnicity, Asian American students reported lower

scores on reading vocabulary than Latino and White American students. The ANCOVAs

for reading comprehension only showed a significant main effect for ethnicity, F(3, 251)

= 4.17, p = .007, η2 = .01. Specifically, African American students reported lower scores

on reading comprehension than Latino and White American students.


48

Hypothesis 4 proposes that Asian, African, and Latino American student would

have higher levels of task aversiveness and fear of failure than White American students,

after controlling for the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES. A MANCOVA was

performed to test this hypothesis and the overall results showed no significant effect for

ethnicity or the covariates of age, gender, ESL, and SES for the task aversiveness and

fear of failure.

Hypothesis 5 proposed that reading vocabulary and reading comprehension

scores would be stronger predictors of procrastination on the six academic tasks than

ethnicity, after controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of age,

gender, ESL, and SES. However, tests for multicollinearity showed that age was highly

correlated with reading vocabulary so a decision was made to remove it as a covariate in

the subsequent regression analyses where reading vocabulary served as an independent

variable. Tests for multicollinearity also showed that scores for reading vocabulary and

comprehension were highly correlated so these independent variables were entered in

separate regression models for these analyses.

To test the hypothesis that reading vocabulary and comprehension would be

stronger predictors of procrastination than ethnicity, a hierarchical multiple regression

analysis was performed with the covariates of gender, ESL, and SES entered in the first

block, the dummy coded ethnic variables for Asian, African, Latino, and White

Americans in the second block (while excluding one to serve as the baseline group), and

finally, reading vocabulary or reading comprehension was entered in the third block to

predict procrastination on each of the six academic tasks.


49

First, the results of the hierarchical multiple regression analyses with reading

vocabulary as a predictor in the third step will be discussed. The overall multiple

regression models that examined reading vocabulary as a predictor in the third block for

procrastination on writing a term paper, studying for exams, performing administrative

tasks, and attending meetings were not found to be significant.

However, the hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on

keeping up with weekly reading assignments with reading vocabulary in the third block

was found to be significant, F(7, 251) = 3.65, p = .001 and accounted for 9% of the

explained variance (See Table 3). Specifically, ethnicity was a significant predictor of

procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading assignments when examining the

differences in R2 and alternative models. Asian, African, and Latino American students

reported higher procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading assignments than

White American students.

The hierarchical multiple regression analysis predicting procrastination on

performing academic tasks in general with reading vocabulary as a predictor was also

found to be significant, F(7, 251) = 3.16, p = .003, and accounted for 8% of the explained

variance (See Table 4). Specifically, ethnicity and reading vocabulary were significant

predictors of procrastination on academic tasks in general. For ethnicity, Asian American

students reported higher procrastination on academic tasks in general than White

American students. Students with higher scores on reading vocabulary reported higher

procrastination on academic tasks in general than students with higher scores of reading

vocabulary. In reviewing the changes in R2 for the separate blocks in the regression
50

Table 3
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, ESL, SES, Ethnicity, and Reading
Vocabulary as Predictors of Procrastination in Keeping Up With Weekly Reading Assignments
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .01
Gender .18 .14 .08
ESL .13 .15 .06
SES .01 .01 .05
Step 2 .08 .07
Gender .15 .14 .07
ESL -.03 .16 -.01
SES .01 .01 .06
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.13 .24 -.04
Latino American -.26 .18 -.11
White American*** -.71 .17 -.34
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .24 -.06
White American** -.57 .21 -.27
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American* -.45 .17 -.21
Step 3 .09 .02
Gender .21 .14 .09
ESL .06 .17 .03
SES .01 .01 .05
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.17 .24 -.05
Latino American -.33 .18 -.15
White American*** -.79 .18 -.38
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.16 .24 -.07
White American** -.62 .21 -.30
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American** -.47 .17 -.22
Reading Vocabulary .01 .01 .13
Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
51

Table 4
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, ESL, SES, Ethnicity, and
Reading Vocabulary as Predictors of Procrastination in Performing Academic Tasks in General
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .03
Gender .13 .14 .06
ESL .22 .14 .11
SES -.01 .01 .07
Step 2 .04 .01
Gender .14 .14 .06
ESL .10 .15 .05
SES -.01 .01 -.04
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.35 .23 -.11
Latino American .06 .18 .03
White American -.22 .17 -.11
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .40 .23 .19
White American .13 .21 .06
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.38 .17 -.14
Step 3 .08 .04
Gender .23 .14 .11
ESL .25 .16 .12
SES -.01 .01 .06
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.41 .23 -.13
Latino American -.06 .18 -.03
White American* -.36 .17 -.18
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .35 .23 .16
White American .05 .20 .02
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.31 .17 -.15
Reading Vocabulary** .02 .01 .23
Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
52

model, the results showed that reading vocabulary was a stronger predictor of

procrastination on academic tasks in general when examining all variables together on the

third block.

The results of the hierarchical multiple regression analyses using reading

comprehension as a predictor in the third step will now be discussed. The hierarchical

multiple regression models that examined ethnicity and reading comprehension as

predictors of procrastination on writing a term paper, studying for exams, performing

administrative tasks, attending meetings, and performing academic tasks in general were

not found to be significant.

However, the hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on

keeping up with weekly reading assignments with reading comprehension as a predictor

was found to be significant, F(7, 251) = 3.27, p = .002, and accounted for 9% of the

explained variance (See Table 5). Specifically, ethnicity was found to be significant

predictor of procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading assignments. Asian,

African, and Latino American students reported higher procrastination on keeping up

with weekly reading assignments than White Americans. In reviewing the changes in R2

for the separate blocks in the regression model and examining alternative models, the

results showed that ethnicity was a stronger predictor of procrastination on keeping up

with weekly reading assignments than reading comprehension.

Hypothesis 6 proposed that task aversiveness and fear of failure would be stronger

predictors of procrastination than ethnicity, reading ability, or reading comprehension

after controlling for the significant contributions of the covariates of gender, and SES.
53

Table 5
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, ESL, SES, Ethnicity, and
Reading Comprehension as Predictors of Procrastination in Keeping Up With Weekly Reading
Assignments
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .01
Gender .18 .14 .08
ESL .13 .15 .06
SES .01 .01 .05
Step 2 .08 .07
Gender .15 .14 .07
ESL -.03 .16 -.01
SES .01 .01 .06
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.13 .24 -.04
Latino American -.26 .18 -.11
White American*** -.71 .17 -.33
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .24 -.06
White American** -.57 .21 -.27
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American* -.45 .17 -.21
Step 3 .09 .02
Gender .15 .14 .07
ESL -.01 .16 -.01
SES .01 .01 .07
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.10 .24 -.03
Latino American -.26 .18 -.12
White American*** -.72 .17 -.34
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.17 .24 -.07
White American** -.62 .22 -.29
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American** -.45 .17 -.21
Reading Comprehension .01 .01 .06
Note: Reading Comprehension scores range from 0 (Low) to 36 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
54

Since reading vocabulary was a significant predictor of procrastination in the previous

analyses and not reading comprehension, only reading vocabulary was retained for the

following analyses. Tests for multicollinearity also showed that ESL was highly

correlated with reading vocabulary so a decision was made to remove it as a covariate in

subsequent regression analyses. Tests for multicollinearity also showed that task

aversiveness and fear of failure were highly correlated so these independent variables

should be entered in separate regression models.

To test the hypothesis that task aversiveness and fear of failure will be a stronger

predictor of procrastination than ethnicity and reading vocabulary after controlling for the

significant contributions of the covariates of gender and SES, hierarchical multiple

regressions analyses were performed with the covariates of gender and SES entered in the

first block, the dummy coded variables for Asian, African, Latino, and White American

in the second block (excluding one for the baseline group), reading vocabulary was

entered in the third block, and finally task aversiveness or fear of failure was entered in

the four block for each of the six academic tasks of procrastination.

First, the hierarchical multiple regression models using task aversiveness in the

fourth block will be discussed. The hierarchical multiple regression model that examined

task aversivness as a predictor of procrastination on performing administrative tasks was

not found to be significant.

However, the hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on

writing a term paper with task aversiveness as a predictor was found to be significant,
55

F(7, 251) = 8.20, p < .001, and this model accounted for 19% of the explained variance

(See Table 6). Specifically, ethnicity, reading vocabulary, and task aversiveness were all

found to be significant predictors of procrastination on writing a term paper. For

ethnicity, Asian and Latino American students reported higher procrastination on writing

a term paper than White American students. For reading vocabulary, students with

higher levels of reading vocabulary reported more procrastination on writing a term

paper. Finally for task aversiveness, students with higher levels of task aversiveness

reported more procrastination on writing a term paper. In reviewing the changes in R2 for

the separate blocks for this regression model, the results showed that task aversiveness

was a stronger predictor of procrastination on writing a term paper when examining all

variables together on the fourth block.

The hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on studying

for exams with task aversiveness as a predictor was also found to be significant, F(7,

251) = 4.66, p < .001, and this model accounted for 12% of the explained variance (See

Table 7). Specifically, only task aversiveness was a significant predictor of

procrastination on studying for exams when examining all variables together on the

fourth block. Students with higher levels of task aversiveness reported more

procrastination on studying for an exam.

The hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on keeping up with

weekly reading assignments with task aversiveness as a predictor was also found to be

significant, F(7, 251) = 6.76, p < .001, and this model accounted for 16% of the

explained variance (See Table 8). Specifically, ethnicity and task aversiveness were
56

Table 6
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of Procrastination in Writing a Term Paper
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .01
Gender .04 .12 .02
SES -.01 .00 -.12
Step 2 .03 .02
Gender .05 .12 .02
SES .01 .00 -.08
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.01 .20 -.01
Latino American .03 .16 .01
White American -.21 .14 -.11
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .07 .20 .03
White American -.17 .19 -.09
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.23 .15 -.13
Step 3 .05 .03
Gender .10 .13 .05
SES -.01 .00 -.12
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.12 .20 -.04
Latino American -.05 .16 -.02
White American* -.32 .15 -.18
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .07 .20 .04
White American -.21 .18 -.11
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.27 .15 -.15
Reading Vocabulary* .01 .00 .15
Step 4 .19 .16
Gender -.00 .12 .00
SES -.01 .00 -.09
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.06 .18 -.02
Latino American -.02 .15 -.01
White American* -.28 .14 -.16
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .03 .18 .02
White American -.22 .17 -.12
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.26 .14 -.14
Reading Vocabulary* .01 .00 .16
Task Aversiveness*** .33 .05 .38
Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Task Aversiveness scores range
from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
57

Table 7
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of Procrastination in Studying for Exams
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .03
Gender .17 .12 .08
SES -.01 .01 -.13
Step 2 .04 .01
Gender .16 .12 .08
SES -.01 .01 -.12
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American .03 .20 .01
Latino American -.10 .16 -.01
White American -.22 .15 -.12
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .20 -.06
White American -.25 .19 -.14
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.12 .15 -.07
Step 3 .04 .00
Gender .15 .13 .08
SES -.01 .01 -.12
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American .04 .20 .01
Latino American -.08 .16 -.01
White American -.20 .16 -.11
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .20 -.06
White American -.24 .19 -.13
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.11 .15 -.06
Reading Vocabulary -.01 .01 -.03
Step 4 .12 .08
Gender .07 .12 .04
SES -.01 .01 -.11
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American .08 .20 .03
Latino American -.07 .16 -.03
White American -.17 .15 -.09
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.15 .19 -.08
White American -.25 .18 -.14
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.10 .14 -.06
Reading Vocabulary -.01 .01 -.02
Task Aversiveness*** .24 .05 .28
Note: Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Task Aversiveness scores
range from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
58

found to be significant predictors of procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading

assignments. For ethnicity, Asian, African, and Latino American students reported

higher procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading assignments than White

American students. Asian American students also reported higher procrastination on

keeping up with weekly reading assignments than Latino American students. In addition,

students with higher levels of task aversiveness reported higher procrastination on

keeping up with weekly reading assignments. In reviewing the changes in R2 for the

separate blocks for this regression model and examining alternative models, the results

showed that ethnicity was the stronger predictor of procrastination on keeping up with

weekly reading assignments than task aversiveness.

The hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on attending

meetings with task aversiveness as a predictor was found to be significant, F(7, 251) =

2.94, p = .006, and this model accounted for 8% of the explained variance (See Table 9).

Specifically, only task aversiveness was a significant predictor of procrastination on

attending meetings when examining all variables together on the fourth block. Students

with higher scores of task aversiveness reported more procrastination about attending

meetings.

The hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on

performing academic tasks in general with task aversiveness as a predictor was found to

be significant, F(7, 251) = 3.97, p < .001, and this model accounted for 10% of the

explained variance (See Table 10). Specifically, ethnicity, reading vocabulary, and task

aversiveness were significant predictors. For ethnicity, Asian and Latino American
59

Table 8
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of Procrastination in Keeping Up with Weekly
Reading Assignments
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .00
Gender .17 .15 .08
SES .01 .01 .02
Step 2 .08 .08
Gender .16 .14 .07
SES .01 .01 .07
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.12 .23 -.04
Latino American -.26 .18 -.11
White American*** -.70 .17 -.33
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.14 .23 -.06
White American** -.58 .21 -.27
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American* -.44 .17 -.21
Step 3 .09 .01
Gender .20 .14 .10
SES .01 .01 .04
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.19 .23 -.06
Latino American -.33 .18 -.15
White American*** -.80 .18 -.38
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .23 -.06
White American** -.62 .21 -.29
Comparison Group: Latino
White American** -.48 .17 -.23
Reading Vocabulary .01 .01 .12
Step 4 .16 .07
Gender .12 .14 .05
SES .01 .01 .05
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.14 .22 -.04
Latino American -.30 .18 -.14
White American*** -.77 .17 -.37
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.16 .22 -.07
White American** -.63 .20 -.30
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American** -.47 .16 -.22
Reading Vocabulary .01 .01 .13
Task Aversiveness*** .27 .06 .26
Note: Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Task Aversiveness scores
range from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
60

Table 9
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of Procrastination in Attending Meetings
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .01
Gender .30 .17 .11
SES .01 .01 .01
Step 2 .02 .01
Gender .30 .17 .11
SES -.01 .01 -.01
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.15 .27 -.04
Latino American -.10 .22 -.04
White American .05 .20 .02
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .06 .27 .02
White American .21 .25 .08
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American .15 .20 .06
Step 3 .03 .01
Gender .35 173 .13
SES -.01 .01 -.03
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.23 .27 -.06
Latino American -.17 .22 -.06
White American -.06 .21 -.02
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .09 .27 .02
White American .17 .25 .07
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American .10 .20 .04
Reading Vocabulary .01 .01 .11
Step 4 .08 .05
Gender .26 .17 .10
SES -.01 .01 -.02
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.18 .27 -.05
Latino American -.15 .22 -.06
White American -.03 .20 -.01
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .03 .27 .01
White American .15 .25 .06
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American .12 .20 .05
Reading Vocabulary .01 .01 .12
Task Aversiveness*** .27 .07 .23
Note: Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Task Aversiveness scores
range from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
61

students reported higher procrastination on performing academic tasks in general than

White and African American students. For reading vocabulary, students with higher

reading vocabulary scores also reported higher procrastination on academic tasks in

general. Finally, students with higher scores of task aversiveness reported higher

procrastination on academic tasks in general. In reviewing the changes in R2 for the

separate blocks for the regression model and examining alternative models, the results

showed that reading vocabulary was the strongest predictors of procrastination on

academic tasks on performing academic tasks in general than task aversiveness.

The hierarchical multiple regression models using fear of failure in the fourth

block will now be discussed. The hierarchical multiple regression models that examined

fear of failure as a predictor of procrastination on performing administrative tasks and

attending meetings were not found to be significant.

However, the hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on

writing a term paper with fear of failure as a predictor was found to be significant, F(7,

251) = 5.61, p < .001, and this model accounted for 14% of the explained variance (See

Table 11). Specifically, reading vocabulary and fear of failure were both found to be

significant predictors of procrastination about writing a term paper. For reading

vocabulary, students with higher levels of reading vocabulary reported higher

procrastination on writing a term paper. For fear of failure, students with higher levels of

fear of failure also reported higher procrastination about writing a term paper. In

reviewing the changes in R2 for the separate blocks for this regression model, the results

showed that fear of failure was a strongest predictor of procrastination on writing a term
62

Table 10
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Task Aversiveness as Predictors of Procrastination in Performing Academic Tasks in
General
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .02
Gender .13 .14 .06
SES -.01 .01 -.12
Step 2 .03 .01
Gender .14 .14 .06
SES -.01 .01 -.06
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.39 .22 -.13
Latino American .05 .18 .02
White American -.25 .16 -.13
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American * .44 .22 .17
White American .14 .21 .07
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.30 .16 -.15
Step 3 .07 .04
Gender -.21 .14 .10
SES -.01 .01 -.10
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American* -.50 .22 -.16
Latino American -.05 .17 -.02
White American* -.42 .17 -.21
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American * .44 .22 .21
White American .08 .20 .04
Comparison Group: Latino
White American* -.36 .16 -.18
Reading Vocabulary * .02 .01 .20
Step 4 .10 .03
Gender .16 .14 .07
SES -.01 .01 -.09
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American* -.47 .22 -.15
Latino American -.40 .17 -.02
White American* -.40 .17 -.20
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American * .43 .22 .20
White American .07 .20 .04
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American * -.35 .16 -.17
Reading Vocabulary ** .02 .01 .20
Task Aversiveness** .16 .06 .17
Note: Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Task Aversiveness scores
range from 1 (Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
63

Table 11
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Fear of Failure as Predictors of Procrastination in Writing a Term Paper
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .01
Gender .04 .12 .02
SES -.01 .01 -.12
Step 2 .03 .02
Gender .05 .12 .02
SES -.01 .01 -.08
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.04 .20 -.01
Latino American .03 .16 .01
White American -.21 .14 -.11
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .07 .20 .03
White American -.17 .19 -.09
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.23 .15 -.13
Step 3 .05 .03
Gender .10 .13 .05
SES -.01 .01 -.11
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.11 .20 -.04
Latino American .05 .16 -.02
White American* -.32 .15 -.18
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .07 .20 .04
White American -.21 .18 -.11
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.27 .15 -.15
Reading Vocabulary .01 .01 .15
Score*
Step 4 .14 .09
Gender .04 .12 .02
SES -.01 .01 -.09
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.08 .19 -.03
Latino American -.02 .15 .01
White American -.29 .15 -.16
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American .06 .19 .03
White American -.21 .18 -.12
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.27 .14 -.15
Reading Vocabulary** .02 .01 .22
Fear of Failure*** .26 .05 .32
Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Fear of Failure scores range from 1
(Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
64

paper than reading vocabulary.

The hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on studying

for exams with fear of failure as a predictor was also found to be significant, F(7, 251) =

3.47, p < .001, and this model accounted for 9% of the explained variance (See Table

12). Specifically, only fear of failure was found to be a significant predictor of

procrastination on studying for exams. Students with higher scores of fear of failure

reported higher procrastination on studying for an exam.

The hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on keeping

up with weekly reading assignments with fear of failure as a predictor was also found to

be significant, F(7, 251) = 3.75, p < .001, and this model accounted for 10% of the

explained variance (See Table 13). Specifically, ethnicity and reading vocabulary were

both significant predictors of procrastination in keeping up with weekly reading

assignments. For ethnicity, Asian, African, and Latino American students reported

higher procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading assignments than White

American students. Also, Asian American students reported higher procrastination on

keeping up with weekly reading assignments than Latino American students. For reading

vocabulary, students with higher levels of reading vocabulary reported higher

procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading assignments. In reviewing the

changes in R2 for the separate blocks for this regression model, the results showed that

ethnicity was the stronger predictor of procrastination on keeping up with weekly reading

assignments than reading vocabulary.

The hierarchical multiple regression model predicting procrastination on


65

Table 12
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Fear of Failure as Predictors of Procrastination in Studying for Exams
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .03
Gender .17 .12 .08
SES -.01 .01 -.13
Step 2 .04 .01
Gender .16 .12 .08
SES -.01 .01 -.12
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American .03 .20 .01
Latino American -.10 .16 -.01
White American -.22 .15 -.12
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .20 -.06
White American -.24 .19 -.13
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.12 .15 -.07
Step 3 .04 .00
Gender .15 .13 .08
SES -.01 .01 -.12
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American .04 .20 .01
Latino American -.08 .16 -.01
White American -.20 .16 -.11
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .20 -.06
White American -.24 .19 -.13
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.11 .15 -.06
Reading Vocabulary -.01 .01 -.03
Step 4 .09 .05
Gender .10 .12 .01
SES .17 .12 .06
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American .07 .20 02
Latino American -.07 .16 -.03
White American -.12 .15 -.10
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .20 -.07
White American -.24 .18 -.13
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American -.11 .15 -.06
Reading Vocabulary .01 .01 .02
Fear of Failure*** .20 .05 .24
Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Fear of Failure scores range from 1
(Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
66

Table 13
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Fear of Failure as Predictors of Procrastination in Keeping Up with Weekly Reading
Assignments
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .01
Gender .17 .15 .08
SES .01 .01 .02
Step 2 .08 .07
Gender .16 .14 .07
SES .01 .01 .07
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.12 .23 -.04
Latino American -.26 .18 -.11
White American*** -.70 .17 -.33
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.14 .23 -.06
White American** -.58 .21 -.27
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American* -.44 .17 -.21
Step 3 .09 .01
Gender .21 .14 .09
SES .01 .01 .04
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.19 .23 -.06
Latino American -.33 .18 -.15
White American*** -.81 .18 -.38
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.13 .23 -.06
White American** -.61 .21 -.29
Comparison Group: Latino
White American** -.48 .17 -.23
Reading Vocabulary .01 .01 .12
Step 4 .10 .01
Gender .19 .14 .08
SES .01 .01 .05
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.18 .23 -.06
Latino American -.32 .18 -.14
White American*** -.80 .18 -.38
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American -.14 .23 -.06
White American** -.62 .21 -.29
Comparison Group: Latino
White American** -.47 .17 -.23
Reading Vocabulary * .01 .01 .14
Fear of Failure .08 .06 .08
Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Fear of Failure scores range from 1
(Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
67

performing academic tasks in general with fear of failure as a predictor was found to be

significant, F(7, 251) = 3.06, p < .001, and this model accounted for 9 % of the explained

variance (See Table 14). Specifically, ethnicity, reading vocabulary, and fear of failure

were all found to be significant predictors of procrastination on academic tasks in

general. For ethnicity, Asian and Latino American students reported higher

procrastination about performing academic tasks in general than African and White

American students. For reading vocabulary, students with higher reading vocabulary

scores also reported higher procrastination about performing academic tasks in general.

For fear of failure, students with higher scores of fear of failure reported higher

procrastination about performing academic tasks in general. In reviewing the changes in

R2 for the separate blocks for the regression model, the results showed that reading

vocabulary was a stronger predictor of procrastination on performing academic tasks in

general than ethnicity or fear of failure.


68

Table 14
Summary of the Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis for Gender, SES, Ethnicity, Reading
Vocabulary, and Fear of Failure as Predictors of Procrastination in Performing Academic Tasks in
General
Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2
Step 1 .02
Gender .13 .14 .06
SES -.01 .01 -.12
Step 2 .04 .02
Gender .14 .14 .06
SES -.01 .01 -.06
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American -.40 .22 -.13
Latino American .05 .18 .02
White American -.25 .16 -.13
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American * .44 .22 .21
White American .14 .21 .07
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American* -.30 .16 -.15
Step 3 .07 .03
Gender .21 .14 .10
SES -.01 .01 -.10
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American* -.50 .22 -.16
Latino American -.05 .18 -.02
White American* -.42 .17 -.21
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American * .44 .22 .21
White American .08 .20 .04
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American * -.36 .16 -.18
Reading Vocabulary* .02 .01 .20
Step 4 .09 .02
Gender .18 .14 .08
SES -.01 .01 -.09
Comparison Group: Asian American
African American* -.48 .22 -.16
Latino American -.04 .18 -.02
White American** -.40 .17 -.20
Comparison Group: African American
Latino American * .44 .22 .21
White American .08 .20 .04
Comparison Group: Latino American
White American* -.36 .16 -.18
Reading Vocabulary** .02 .01 .22
Fear of Failure* .13 .06 .14
Note: Reading Vocabulary scores range from 0 (Low) to 80 (High) and Fear of Failure scores range from 1
(Low) to 5 (High).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
69

Chapter 4

DISCUSSION

Summary and Interpretation of Findings in the Context of Previous Literature

The results of this study provided partial support for the proposed hypotheses.

Contrary to expectations, ethnic minority participants did not report a higher level of

overall procrastination than White American students. However, this finding is

consistent with the results of previous research that also failed to report significant ethnic

differences in overall procrastination scores (Prohaska et al., 2000; Stuart, 2013).

Specifically, in reviewing the overall procrastination scores, participants reported they

tend to procrastinate “sometimes” or very little. One reason for this lack of a significant

ethnic difference on overall procrastination may be due to the way these scores were

calculated. By creating a summed score across six academic tasks, lower scores on

certain academic tasks (e.g., performing administrative tasks) can help to negate higher

scores on other academic tasks (e.g., writing a term paper).

As predicted, African American students reported more procrastination in keeping

up with weekly reading assignments than White American students. However, contrary

to expectations, Asian American students reported more procrastination in keeping up

with weekly reading assignment than White American students. No other significant

ethnic differences were found for the five other academic tasks. Despite the number of

significant findings for the six academic tasks, these results support the importance of

examining these tasks separately as this can provide a better assessment of the types of
70

procrastination that a college student may experience rather than a single overall score of

procrastination. In a review of the six academic tasks, a specific pattern emerged which

showed the level of procrastination was often associated with whether it was an

academic task directly related to class performance or a general school policy or

procedure.

Specifically, participants would “almost never” or only “sometimes” procrastinate

on specific administrative tasks (e.g., paying tuition), attending meetings, or completing

academic tasks in general (e.g., registering for classes) which may lead to immediate

negative consequences. However, participants would “nearly always” procrastinate

about writing a term paper, studying for an exam, or keeping up with weekly reading

assignments. It is possible that students were more likely to find excuses to put off or

procrastinate on class requirements because they had several weeks to complete such

tasks. These results were similar to previous findings that showed students were more

likely to procrastinate on writing a term paper or keeping up with weekly reading

assignments than performing administrative tasks and attending meetings (Clark & Hill,

1984; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984).

Contrary to expectations, Asian American students reported lower scores on

reading vocabulary than Latino and White American students. However, as predicted,

African American students reported lower reading comprehension scores than White

American students. In contrast to expectations, Africa Americans also reported lower

reading comprehension than Latino Americans. With regard to Asian Americans, a high

percentage (63%) of self-identified Asian Americans in this study were found to be


71

Southeast Asians and past studies have shown that Southeast Asian students frequently

report with lower levels of reading ability (Le, 2010). A post-hoc assessment showed

that Southeast Asian American students in this study reported lower levels of reading

vocabulary than African, Latino, White American and other Asian American students.

The current finding that African American students reported lower scores on

reading comprehension than White Americans supports previous research findings

(Moore et al., 2010). With regard to Latino Americans, a high percentage (56%) self-

identified as ESL students and this could account for the lack of significant ethnic

differences in reading vocabulary and comprehension. That is, the present study showed

that ESL students reported with lower levels of reading vocabulary than non-ESL

students. Also, a post-hoc analysis of ethnic differences in ESL showed that Latino

American students, F(3, 251) = 12.85, p < .001, η2 = .12, reported higher numbers of ESL

students than African and White American students. This may suggest that the reason

Latino students report with lower levels of reading ability could be due to their ESL

status and not just their ethnic status of being Latinos. Overall, the scores for reading

ability and comprehension in the study were comparable to previous scores on these two

measures in past research (Moore et al., 2010).

Contrary to expectations, there were no significant ethnic differences found for

task aversiveness and fear of failure. There was no previous research examining ethnic

differences in task aversiveness. It was hypothesized, however, that ethnic minority

students would have higher task aversiveness on academic tasks due to their lower

academic performances. Students who are not as proficient in reading and writing may
72

view academic tasks such as writing a term paper and keeping up with weekly reading

assignments as more challenging and unpleasant. With regard to fear of failure, these

findings were not consistent with previous literature that stated African and Latino

Americans would have higher levels of fear of failure (Coney& West, 1979; Evans,

2008). One possible explanation why there were no ethnic differences could be the way

that data was collected for the sample. Although Evans (2007) found ethnic differences

on fear of failure, it could be due to the way the data was collected. A semi-structured

interview was used where students simply stated they were fearful of failing a course.

There were no set questions so each student was not asked the same question in this

study. In the current study, every student was asked the same items to calculate the scores

for fear of failure. However, the findings did support previous research that females

would report higher levels of fear of failure (Stuart, 2013).

The results of the present study partially supported the proposed hypotheses

concerning the predictive value of reading ability on procrastination. As predicted,

reading vocabulary was found to be a better predictor of procrastination on performing

academic tasks in general than ethnicity. These results were similar to previous findings

that showed reading vocabulary was a better predictor of academic tasks in general

(Collins et al., 2008). However, the relationship between reading vocabulary and

procrastination was different from previous research. The current study found that

students with higher reading vocabulary also reported higher levels of procrastination.

One possible explanation for this is the relationship of reading ability to procrastination

may be curvilinear. That is, both participants who have very low reading ability as well
73

as very high reading ability may engage in higher levels of procrastination for completely

different reasons. For the low reading ability students, they may engage in more

procrastination to avoid dealing with difficult tasks However, higher reading ability

students seem to believe that school is easy enough for them to procrastinate and still

achieve high level marks. As such, they put off the task to the last moment because they

feel as though they can complete it within the limited time frame. Contrary to

expectations, ethnicity was a better predictor of higher procrastination on keeping up with

weekly reading assignments than reading vocabulary and comprehension. Asian,

African, and Latino American students reported more procrastination on keeping up with

weekly reading assignments than White American students. At this time, it is not clear as

to why ethnicity is a better predictor of keeping up with weekly reading assignments, but

future research should take this into consideration. There were no other significant

predictors reported for the other academic tasks.

The results of the current study also provided partial support for the proposed

hypothesis concerning the predictive value of task aversiveness and fear of failure on

procrastination. Specifically for task aversiveness, students reported that the task being

too difficult or too long “sometimes reflected why they procrastinated”. For fear of

failure, students reported that they “never” or “sometimes” allowed the fear of receiving a

failing grade or negative criticism of their work to fuel their procrastination.. As

predicted, task aversiveness and fear of failure were better predictors of higher

procrastination on writing a term paper and studying for exams than ethnicity and reading

vocabulary. These results were similar to previous findings that showed task
74

aversiveness and fear of failure were the main reasons why students often procrastinate

(Blunt & Pychyl, 2000; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). Contrary to expectations, ethnicity

was a better predictor of keeping up with weekly reading assignments and reading

vocabulary was a better predictor of academic tasks in general than task aversiveness.

Asian, African, and Latino American students reported more procrastination on keeping

up with weekly reading assignments than White American students. Again, it is not clear

as to why ethnicity is the strongest predictor of keeping up with weekly reading

assignments, but future research should take it into consideration. The results in the

present study found that reading vocabulary was positively associated with

procrastination, while previous studies reported that reading vocabulary was negatively

related to procrastination on academic tasks in general (Collin et al., 2008). Finally, task

aversiveness was a better predictor of higher procrastination on attending meetings. The

results were similar to previous findings that showed that task aversiveness was the main

reason that students procrastinated (Blunt & Pychyl, 2000; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984).

Limitations

One limitation to the study was the lack of inclusion of other significant factors

that could be associated with academic procrastination. For example, certain types of

self-motivation factors such as self-efficacy, self-regulation, and self-esteem may play an

important role in whether a student would procrastinate in general. Past research tends to

report that higher levels of self-efficacy, self-regulation, and self-esteem are negatively

related to procrastination (Klassen et al., 2010; Schouwenburg et al., 2004; Steel, 2007).

Students, who had higher levels of self-efficacy, self-regulation and self-esteem engaged
75

more in academia, were more persistent, worked harder, and were more confident.

Having higher levels of self-motivation factors also decreased a student’s perception that

a task was too difficult to complete. Despite the lack of inclusion of self-motivating

variables, the regression models in the current study with ethnicity, reading vocabulary,

reading comprehension, task aversiveness, and fear of failure were found to be significant

and accounted for 9% to 19% of the explained variances.

Another limitation includes generalizability from the current sample to larger

populations. First, the sample for this study was from a college student population. This

college population may differ from the high school or graduate student populations in a

number of ways including family of origin socioeconomic status, participant sex, and age

which may have impacted the results. However, the results of this study were similar to

other studies of procrastination in using a college sample and the current sample had a

wide variability among the students in terms of family of origin socioeconomic status and

age. For example, the majority of students from the present study procrastinated more on

tasks associated with grades rather than tasks associated with policies and procedures.

This finding supports previous studies that also found similar procrastination results

(Clark & Hill, 1984; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984).

Finally, another limitation of the current study was the sample size had a low

number of ethnic minority participants than was desired. Specifically, there were low

numbers of Asian and African American students. Since ethnic differences in reported

procrastination on weekly reading assignments and vocabulary and comprehension scores

were significant, it may be have been important to collect a more evenly distributed
76

sample in regard to ethnic minority students. However, despite the low number of ethnic

minority students, the results regarding ethnic differences in procrastination and reading

vocabulary and comprehension found significant differences.

Clinical Implications

The current study supported previous research that most of the procrastination

was taking place with tasks that are directly related to class performance and not on tasks

that are related to general policies and procedures. To help students understand the risks

associated with procrastination, freshmen orientation should include a number of

different workshops or information sessions that would include specific information

related to procrastination on class-related tasks.

Incoming freshmen may also be required to take specific courses aimed at helping

them to better prepare for a heavy college workload. These college preparation courses

should cover better studying habits, time management, and how to utilize other on-

campus resources such as tutoring services or the Writing Center. Courses should

emphasize how not to fall behind on your college workload to help better acclimate the

freshmen to college.

Incoming freshmen should be provided with more structured courses. One

possible explanation for procrastination on class performance tasks could be due to a lack

of structure in a college setting. Students who come from high school environments may

be used to having more structured classes that require assignments or exams to be done

on a weekly basis. In that setting, the student may procrastinate less because the

consequence of not doing so is more immediate. In a university setting, many times


77

exams and assignments are done every few weeks. Because the assignment/exam is later

rather than sooner, students may be compelled to procrastinate less because the

consequence of not doing so is not immediate. As such, freshmen courses should be

more structured to help incoming freshmen adjust to the college work load. As the

student progresses in their academic career, the course settings may become less and less

structured.

Future Directions in Research

Although there has been substantial research on how procrastination can have a

negative effect on a student’s academic performance (Klassen et al., 2010; Onwuegbuzie,

2004; Steel, 2007), more research should be conducted to determine the directionality of

the relationship between procrastination and academic performance. It is unclear whether

procrastination leads to poor academic performance or poor academic performance leads

to procrastination. It could be argued that poor academic performance could lead to

procrastination due to a lowered self-efficacy from previous tasks (Seo, 2008) or

procrastination could lead to poor academic performance due to missed opportunities to

learn or a lack of adequate time to perform tasks successfully (Klassen et al., 2010). As

such, further research should look into the directionality of this relationship.

Although the present study did not look into self-motivation factors such as self-

efficacy, self-regulation, and self-esteem, they may play an important role in whether a

student would procrastinate in general (Klassen et al., 2010; Schouwenburg et al., 2004;

Steel, 2007). Future studies should look into how these factors may contribute to
78

procrastination and should be examined to see if any of them are better predictors than

the current variables being studied.

There is currently very limited research on Asian American students and

procrastination. More specifically, there is very little research that examines the diversity

within the Asian American population. Many times, Asian American students are

lumped together and may be assumed to follow the model minority stereotype. While

this may be true for some Asian American cultures, it is not true for all. Southeast Asians

have the highest dropout rates in the country (Le, 2001). Current statistics show that 44%

of Hmong, Laotian, and Cambodian American students are not proficient in English, 52%

have less than a high school education, and only 9% have attained a college degree. In

comparison, 31% of Chinese are not proficient in English, 23% have less than a high

school education, and 46% attain a college degree (Le, 2001). It would be useful to have

further research examine the possible ethnic differences between the different cultures

among Southeast Asians.

The current study was different from previous procrastination studies using the

PASS in that it focused on looking into the individual academic task scores rather than

just the overall summed score for procrastinate. By doing so, the researcher was able to

find ethnic differences on specific academic tasks that would have been overlooked by

research conducted using a summed score. The significant ethnic differences in keeping

up with weekly reading assignments would not have been discovered by using the

summed single procrastination score. By conducting more focused research on specific

academic tasks, this information can be vital in deciding how to work with ethnic
79

minority and White American students concerning issues of procrastination. Although

the original plan of looking into frequency and problematic scales for procrastination

could not be done because they were highly correlated, future research should continue to

look for new methods to better understand this significant problem of procrastination in

college populations.

The current study also shed a little more light on possible differences within the

Asian American student population. Specifically, a post-hoc assessment showed that

Southeast Asians may have accounted for the larger Asian American groups’ lower

scores on reading vocabulary have a high percentage of ESL students. These results

provide support against the model minority myth and emphasize the importance to look

at possible differences within this ethnic minority group.

What this current study has shown is that students tend to procrastinate in a

number of academic tasks, but higher levels of procrastination seem to be tied to those

academic tasks which have the strongest bearing on academic achievement and failure.

Given this tendency to procrastinate on class-related tasks or requirements, it is important

that colleges and universities become more proactive in reaching out to students who may

present with greater tendencies for procrastination and to engage in prevention and

education programs early in college students’ lives on campus. Specific steps should be

taken to help educate and alert students to help them deal with procrastination and time

management. Also, more research on procrastination would be beneficial for instructors

and university administration of colleges and universities with high dropout rates because
80

it may help keep their students to avoid those practices which may facilitate academic

failure.
81

References

Abbott, M. L. (2006). ESL Reading Strategies: Differences in Arabic and Mandarin

Speaker Test Performance. Language Learning, 56(4), 633-670.

doi:10.1111/j.1467- 9922.2006.00391.x

American College Testing (ACT). (2011, March 15). ACT Newsroom: Factsheet.

Retrieved from http://www.act.org/newsroom/factsheets/act.html

Annie E. Casey Foundation (AECF). (2010). Why reading by the end of third grade

matters. Education Digest: Essential Readings Condensed for Quick Review,

76(1), 27-31.

Barnes, W., & Slate, J. R. (2011). College-readiness rate in Texas: A statewide, multiyear

study of ethnic differences. Education and Urban Society, 46(1), 59-87.

doi:10.1177/0013124511423775

Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in

response to noncontingent success. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 36(4), 405-417. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.4.405

Beswick, G., Rothblum, E. D., & Mann, L. (1988). Psychological antecedents of student

procrastination. Australian Psychologist, 23(2), 207-217.

doi:10.1080/00050068808255605

Blunt, A. K., & Pychyl, T. A. (2000). Task aversiveness and procrastination: A multi-

dimensional approach to task aversiveness across stages of personal

projects. Personality And Individual Differences, 28(1), 153-167.

doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00091-4
82

Bui, N. H. (2007). Effect of evaluation threat on procrastination behavior. The Journal of

Social Psychology, 147(3), 197-209. doi:10.3200/SOCP.147.3.197-209

Burka, J. B., & Yuen, L. M. (2008). Procrastination: Why you do it, what to do about it

now. Cambridge: De Capo Press.

Clark, J. L., & Hill, O. W. (1994). Academic procrastination among African-American

college students. Psychological Reports, 75, 931–936.

doi:10.2466/pr0.1994.75.2.931

Chavous, T. M. et. al. (2003). Racial Identity and Academic Attainment Among African-

American Adolescents. Child Development 4(74). 1076-1090.

Chow, H. H. (2011). Procrastination among undergraduate students: Effects of emotional

intelligence, school life, self-evaluation, and self- efficacy. Alberta Journal of

Educational Research, 57(2), 234-240.

Collins, K. T. & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2002). Reading ability and the performance of

African American graduate students in research methodology courses. Journal of

College Literacy and learning, 31, 39-52.

Collins, K. T., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Jiao, Q. G. (2008). Reading ability as a predictor of

academic procrastination among African American graduate students. Reading

Psychology, 29(6), 493-507. doi:10.1080/02702710802168568

Collins, K. T., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Jiao, Q. G. (2011). The relationship between

reading ability and self-perception among African-American postgraduate

students. Journal of Educational Enquiry, 11(1), 43-53.


83

Coney, Y., & West, C.K. (1979) Academic pressures and the black adolescent.

Contemporary Educational Psychology, 4, 318-323.

Conroy, D. E., & Metzler, J. N. (2003). Temporal stability of performance failure

appraisal inventory items. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise

Science, 7(4), 243-261. doi:10.1207/S15327841MPEE0704_3

Covington, M. (1984). The self-worth theory of achievement motivation: Findings and

implications. The Elementary School Journal, 85 (1), 4-20.

Cox, R. (2009). It was just that I was afraid: Promoting success by addressing student's

fear of failure. Community College Review, 37 (1).

De Jong, M. G., Pieters, R., & Fox, J. (2010). Reducing social desirability bias through

item randomized response: An application to measure underreported

desires. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(1), 14-27. doi:10.1509/jmkr.47.1.14

Demps, D. L., & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2001). The relationship between eighth-grade

reading scores and achievement on the Georgia High School Graduation Test.

Research in the Schools, 8(2), 1-9.

Du Boulay, D. (1999). Argument in reading: What does it involve and how can students

become better critical readers? Teaching in Higher Education, 4(2), 147-62.

Evans, A. B., Copping, K. E., Rowley, S. J., & Kurtz-Costes, B. (2011). Academic self-

concept in Black adolescents: Do race and gender stereotypes matter?. Self and

Identity, 10(2), 263-277. doi:10.1080/15298868.2010.485358


84

Evans, B. C. (2008). 'Attached to the umbilicus': Barriers to educational success for

Hispanic/Latino and American Indian nursing students. Journal of Professional

Nursing, 24(4), 205-217. doi:10.1016/j.profnurs.2007.06.026

Feldt, R. C. (1988). Predicting academic performance: Nelson-Denny reading test and

measures of college students' study of expository prose. Psychological

Reports, 63(2), 579-582. doi:10.2466/pr0.1988.63.2.579

Ferrari, J. R. (1989). Reliability of academic and dispositional measures of

procrastination. Psychological Reports, 64, 1057–1058.

Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J. L., & McCown, W.G. (1995). Procrastination and task

avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Ferrari, J. R., O’Callaghan, J., & Newbegin, I. (2005). Prevalence of procrastination in

the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia: Arousal and avoidance delays

among adults. North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 1–6.

Ferrari, J. R., Özer, B., & Demir, A. (2009). Chronic procrastination among Turkish

adults: Exploring decisional, avoidant, and arousal styles. The Journal of Social

Psychology, 149(3), 302-307. doi:10.3200/SOCP.149.3.402-408

Flett, G., Hewitt, P., Davis, R., & Sherry, S. (2004). Description and counseling of the

perfectionistic procrastinator. In H. Schouwenberg, C. Lay, J. R. Ferrari, & T.

Pychyl (Eds.), Counseling the procrastinator in academic settings (pp. 181–194).

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.


85

Foster, D. J., Weigand, D. A., & Baines, D. (2006). The effect of removing superstitious

behavior and introducing a pre-performance routine on basketball free-throw

performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 18(2), 167-171.

doi:10.1080/10413200500471343

Gerow, J. R., & Murphy, D. P. (1980). The validity of the Nelson-Denny Reading Test as

a predictor of performance in introductory psychology. Educational and

Psychological Measurement, 40(2), 553-556. doi:10.1177/001316448004000240

Harriott, J., & Ferrari, J. R. (1996). Prevalence of procrastination among samples of

adults. Psychological Reports, 78, 611–616. doi:10.2466/pr0.1996.78.2.611

Hess, B., Sherman, M. F., & Goodman, M. (2000). Eveningness predicts academic

procrastination: The mediating role of neuroticism. Journal of Social Behavior

and Personality, 15, 61–74.

Hill, R. W., Huelsman, T. J., Furr, M., Kibler, J., Vicente, B.B., Kennedy, C. (2004). A

new measure of perfectionism: The perfectionism inventory. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 82, 80–91

Howell, A. J., & Buro, K. (2008). Implicit beliefs, achievement goals, and

procrastination: A meditational analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19,

151-154. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2008.08.006

Johnson-Ahorlu, R. (2013). Our biggest challenge is stereotypes: Understanding

stereotype threat and the academic experiences of African American

undergraduates. Journal of Negro Education, 82(4), 382-392.

doi:10.7709/jnegroeducation.82.4.0382
86

Jerusalem, M., & Schwarzer, R. (1992). Self-efficacy as a resource factor in stress

appraisal. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-efficacy: Thought control of action, (pp.

195–216). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

Kieffer, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status, English proficiency, and late-emerging

reading difficulties. Educational Researcher, 39(6), 484-486.

doi:10.3102/0013189X10378400

Klassen, R. M., Ang, R. P., Chong, W., Krawchuk, L. L., Huan, V. S., Wong, I. F., &

Yeo, L. (2010). Academic procrastination in two settings: Motivation correlates,

behavioral patterns, and negative impact of procrastination in Canada and

Singapore. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 59(3), 361-379.

doi:10.1111/j.14640597.2009.00394.x

Kosmas, J. M. (2003). The roots of procrastination: A sociological inquiry into why I

wait until tomorrow. Human Architecture: Journal of the Sociology of Self-

Knowledge, 2(2), 74-81.

Le, C. N. (2001). A Closer Look at Asian Americans and Education. New Horizons for

Learning [online], September 2001 <http://www.newhorizons.org/mult_le.htm>.

Le, C. N. (2010). Multiracial Asian Americans: Social Class, Demographic, and Cultural

Characteristics in K. Korgen (Eds.) Multiracial Americans and Social Class: The

Influence of Social Class on Racial Identity, New York: Routledge.

Le, C. N., (2014). Population Statistics & Demographics Asian-Nation: The Landscape

of Asian America. <http://www.asian-nation.org/population.shtml> (July 9, 2014).


87

Leganger, A., Kraft, P., & Roysamb, E. (2000). Perceived self-efficacy in health behavior

research: Conceptualization, measurement, and correlates. Psychology and

Health, 15, 51–69.

Lewis, P. (1999). Stereotype threat, implicit theories of intESLigence, and racial

differences in standardized test performance. Dissertation Abstracts International,

59, 8-B, PsycINFO, EBSCOhost, viewed 10 September 2012.

Luszczynska, A., Gutiérrez-Doña, B., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). General self-efficacy in

various domains of human functioning: Evidence from five countries.

International Journal of Psychology, 40(2), 80-89.

doi:10.1080/00207590444000041

Masterson, J., & Hayes, M. (2004). UK data from 197 undergraduates for the Nelson

Denny Reading Test. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(1), 30-35.

doi:10.1111/j.1467- 9817.2004.00212.x

Milgram, N., & Marshevsky, S., & Sadeh, A. (1994). Correlates of academic

procrastination: Discomfort, task aversiveness, and task capability. The Journal of

Psychology, 129, 145–155.

Moore, G. W., Slate, J. R., Edmonson, S. L., Combs, J. P., Bustamante, R., &

Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2010). High school students and their lack of preparedness

for college: A statewide study. Education and Urban Society, 42(7), 817-838.

doi:10.1177/0013124510379619
88

Museus, S.D. & Kiang, P.N. (2009). Deconstructing the model minority myth and how it

contributes to the invisible minority reality in higher education research. New

Direction for Institutional Research, 142, 5-15.

National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). (2010)

http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/reading

National Center for Education Statistics. (2009). Nation’s Report Card: Reading 2009.

Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.

Neddenriep, C. E., & Hale, A. D. (2011). Introduction to the special issue: Reading

comprehension: Assessment and intervention for understanding. Psychology In

The Schools, 48(1), 1-3. doi:10.1002/pits.20538

Nemko, M. (2008). America’s most overrated product: The bachelor’s degree. The

Chronicle of Higher Education. 54(B17).

Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Academic procrastination and statistics anxiety. Assessment

& Evaluation In Higher Education, 29(1), 3-19.

doi:10.1080/0260293042000160384

Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Collins, K. T. (2002). Reading comprehension among graduate

students. Psychological Reports, 90(3,Pt1), 879-882. doi:10.2466/PR0.90.3.879-

882

Omwuegbuzie, A. J., Mayes, E., Aruther, L., Johnson, J., Robinson, V., Ashe, S., &

Collins, K. T. (2004). Reading comprehension among African American graduate

students. Journal of Negro Education, 73(4), 443-457. doi:10.2307/4129628


89

Özer, B., Demir, A., & Ferrari, J. R. (2009). Exploring academic procrastination among

Turkish students: Possible gender differences in prevalence and reasons. The

Journal of Social Psychology, 149(2), 241-257. doi:10.3200/SOCP.149.2.241-257

Peng, S. S., & Wright, D. (1994). Explanation of academic achievement of Asian

American students. Journal of Educational Research, 87, 346-352.

Prohaska, V., Morrill, P., Atiles, I., & Perez, A. (2000). Academic procrastination by

nontraditional students. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 15(5), 125-

134.

Pychyl, T. A., Lee, J. M., Thibodeau, R., & Blunt, A. (2000). Five days of emotion: An

experience sampling study of undergraduate student procrastination. Journal of

Social Behavior & Personality, 15(5), 239-254.

Roberts, M., Suderman, L., Suderman, R., & Semb, G. B. (1990). Reading ability as a

performance predictor in a behaviorally based psychology course. Teaching of

Psychology, 17(3), 173-175. doi: 10.1207/s15328023top1703_8

Sagar, S. S., & Stoeber, J. (2009). Perfectionism, fear of failure, and affective responses

to success and failure: The central role of fear of experiencing shame and

embarrassment. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 31(5), 602-627.

Salamonson, Y., Koch, J., Weaver, R., Everett, B., & Jackson, D. (2010). Embedded

academic writing support for nursing students with English as a second language.

Journal of Advanced Nursing, 66(2), 413-421. doi:10.1111/j.1365-

2648.2009.05158.x
90

Schouwenburg, H. C., Lay, C. H., Pychyl, T. A., & Ferrari, J. R. (2004). Counseling the

procrastinator in academic settings. Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Schüler, J., Brandstätter, V., & Baumann, N. (2013). Failure cue priming and impaired

cognitive performance: Analyses of avoidance motivation as a mediator and fear

of failure as a moderator. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(5), 335-

343. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1942

Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized self-efficacy scale. In J. Weinman,

S. Wright &M. Johnston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology: A user’s

portfolio. Causal and control beliefs, (pp. 35–37). Windsor, UK: Nelson.

Schwarzer, R., MuESLer, J., & Greenglass, E. (1999). Assessment of perceived general

self-efficacy on the internet: Data collection in cyberspace. Anxiety, Stress, and

Coping, 12, 145–161.

Seo, E. (2008). Self-efficacy as a mediator in the relationship between self-oriented

perfectionism and academic procrastination. Social Behavior and Personality,

36(6), 753-764. doi:10.2224/sbp.2008.36.6.753

Smith, T. W., Snyder, C. R., & Perkins, S. C. (1983). The self-serving function of

hypochondriacal complaints: Physical symptoms as self-handicapping

strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(4), 787-797.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.4.787

Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in

young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.


91

Solomon, L. J. & Rothblum, E. D. (1984). Academic procrastination: Frequency and

congnitive-behavioral correlate. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 503-509.

Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review of

quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133 (1), 65–94.

doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65

Steinberg, L., Dornbusch, S. M., & Brown, B. B. (1992). Ethnic differences in adolescent

achievement: An ecological perspective. American Psychologist, 47, 723-729.

Steuer, F. B. (1996). Reading in the undergraduate psychology curriculum. Teaching of

Psychology, 23(4), 226-230. doi:10.1207/s15328023top2304_4

Stöber, J., & Joormann, J. (2001). Worry, procrastination, and perfectionism:

Differentiating amount of worry, pathological worry, anxiety, and

depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25(1), 49-60.

doi:10.1023/A:1026474715384

Stuart, E. M. (2013). The relationship of fear of failure, procrastination, and self-efficacy

to academic success in college for first and non first-generation students in a

provate non-selective institution (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest

Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI No. 3596272)

U.S. Department of Education. (2002). No Child Left Behind: A desktop reference.

Washington, DC: Author.


92

Yao, M. P. (2010). An exploration of multidimensional perfectionism, academic self-

efficacy, procrastination frequency, and Asian American cultural values in Asian

American university students. Dissertation Abstracts International, 70, 10-B,

PsycINFO, EBSCOhost, viewed 18 September 2012.

You might also like