Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

DOCTYPE = ARTICLE

Psychosocial Adjustment of Adolescent Siblings


of Hematopoietic Stem Cell Transplant Patients

Wendy Packman, JD, PhD


Kimberly Gong, MPH
Kelly VanZutphen, PhD
Tani Shaffer, MS
Mary Crittenden, PhD

Hematopoietic stem cell transplantation (HSCT) is effects of transplantation on HSCT patients, but min-
a widely practiced therapy for many life-threatening imal attention has been given to psychosocial seque-
childhood disorders. The authors investigated the psy- lae in siblings. The healthy siblings of children
chosocial effects of HSCT on siblings of pediatric HSCT undergoing HSCT live the intense stress of the illness.
patients (n = 44; 21 donors, 23 nondonors, ages 6 to The impact on the family unit is intensified because two
18 years). Donor siblings reported significantly more family members, usually children, are subjected to
anxiety and lower self-esteem than did nondonors. invasive medical procedures. Furthermore, because the
Nondonors showed significantly more school problems. patient must be hospitalized in isolation for a pro-
Approximately one third of all siblings reported mod- longed period until hematopoietic recovery, siblings are
erate to severe posttraumatic stress. The study drew on often separated from both parents and patient.
the developmental theory of Erik Erikson and the psy-
chosocial model of posttraumatic stress. As part of the
study, the authors used the Measures of Psychosocial Purpose
Development (MPD), a self-report measure based on
Eriksonian constructs. The MPD was used to assess The purpose of this study was to investigate the
the psychosocial adjustment of 12 siblings who were psychosocial effects of HSCT procedures on donor
adolescents (≥ 13 years) at the time the study was con- and nondonor siblings (Packman, Crittenden, Rieger
ducted. In this article, findings are presented from the Fischer, et al., 1997; Packman, Crittenden, Schaeffer,
MPD as well as salient findings from the larger study. et al., 1997). The participants in the total study included
44 siblings (21 donors, 23 nondonors, ages 6 to 18
Key words: siblings, adolescents, Erikson, hematopoi- years). Using a multidimensional, multimethod
etic stem cell transplants, bone marrow transplants, approach, we assessed each sibling’s current psy-
psychosocial adjustment chosocial adjustment using objective measures com-
pleted by the child, one parent, and one teacher. Children

H ematopoietic stem cell transplantation (HSCT)


has emerged as the treatment of choice for many
malignancies and hematologic and genetic disorders of
Wendy Packman, JD, PhD, is an assistant professor of clinical psychol-
ogy, Kimberly Gong, MPH, is a research assistant and project coordinator,
Kelly VanZutphen, PhD, is a research assistant, and Tani Shaffer, MS, is
childhood. Each year, about 75% of the children who a research assistant at the Pacific Graduate School of Psychology, Palo
have HSCTs receive marrow from a brother or sister. Alto, California. Mary Crittenden, PhD, is a clinical professor of pedi-
Some attention has been paid to the psychological atrics in the Division of Behavioral and Developmental Pediatrics,
Department of Pediatrics, University of California, San Francisco. Address
© 2004 by Association of Pediatric Oncology Nurses for correspondence: Wendy Packman, JD, PhD, Pacific Graduate School
DOI: 10.1177/1043454203262698 of Psychology, 935 E. Meadow Drive, Palo Alto, CA 94303.

Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing, Vol 21, No 4 (July-August), 2004: p 233-248 233
Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Packman et al.

also completed projective drawings. In addition, we con- the treatment period. New behavior problems occurred
ducted interviews with siblings and parents to give far more frequently among sibling donors than among
respondents the opportunity to express feelings and the other siblings. Other researchers have noted that sib-
thoughts about the HSCT experience. On self-report lings who have been directly involved in the care of an
measures, donor siblings reported significantly more ill child, such as bone marrow donors, appear to be emo-
anxiety and lower self-esteem than nondonor siblings tionally affected by the experience (Stuber, 1996;
did. On teacher-rated scales, donors showed significantly Wolcott, Wellisch, Fawzy, & Landsverk, 1986).
more adaptive skills in school than did nondonors, and The interpretation of the HSCT process is influ-
nondonors showed significantly more school prob- enced by age (Stuber, 1996). Adolescents have the
lems. Finally, approximately one third of the donors and need to establish an identity unique to themselves and
nondonors reported a moderate to severe level of post- apart from the earlier identifications acquired during the
traumatic stress (Packman, Crittenden, Rieger Fischer, formative years (Muuss, 1988). The personal and social
et al., 1997; Packman, Crittenden, Schaeffer, et al., changes of adolescence are often conceptualized as
1997). developmental tasks, implying that such changes involve
As part of our study, we used the Measures of Psy- challenges that when adequately resolved, lead to good
chosocial Development (MPD) (Hawley, 1988), a self- psychosocial functioning in early adulthood (Erikson,
report measure based on Eriksonian constructs, to 1968). The developmental tasks of adolescence include,
assess the psychosocial adjustment of siblings who among others, developing a sense of self-identity and
were adolescents at the time of the study (n = 12). In establishing autonomy from parents (Erikson, 1968).
this article, we report the findings of psychosocial The HSCT process may affect any of these develop-
adjustment of 12 adolescent siblings of surviving pedi- mental processes and may be particularly salient for
atric HSCT patients. bone marrow donors who undergo the invasive dona-
tion procedure.

Review of the Literature


Theoretical and Conceptual Framework
In the psychosocial literature that has expressed
concern for the psychological impact of HSCT on sib- We used a conceptual framework that drew on Erik-
lings, investigators have noted that siblings in general sonian developmental theory and the psychosocial
tend to suffer from emotional problems as a result of model of posttraumatic stress (Erikson, 1968; Green,
the patient’s illness (Wiley, Lindamood, & Pfeffer- Wilson, & Lindy, 1985). Erikson viewed development
baum-Levine, 1984). The donor selection procedure as a process of transformation through eight age-related
establishes a new relationship between the siblings psychosocial stages characterized by specific oppor-
and affects the entire family system (Wiley et al., 1984). tunities, demands, and vulnerabilities (see Figure 1).
At the time of tissue typing, siblings may compete for Adverse conditions may therefore affect development
the role of donor, causing nondonors to feel excluded. in stage-specific ways. Disruptions affect not only the
Nondonor siblings experience ambivalent feelings of stage during which the event occurred but also the res-
both disappointment and relief and may express guilt olution of tasks in subsequent stages (Erikson, 1959,
about these feelings of relief (Freund & Siegel, 1986). 1968). The process of human development involves an
Potentially serious psychological reactions may interplay between genetically based aspects of per-
result from the loss and replacement of bone marrow. sonality and the influences of family and culture that
In the view of Pinegger-O’Connor and Bender-Gotze shape the personality (Muuss, 1988). Erikson’s approach
(1980), sibling donors experience the donation proce- is grounded in developmental psychopathology, a per-
dure as “an act of robbery” and a “threat to body spective that examines the evolu
integrity.” The school-aged child, who is in the process tion of psychological disturbances in the context of
of developing a self-concept, is particularly vulnerable development (Matsen, 1992). Erikson provides a the-
to questions of altered identity (Wiley et al., 1984). Pot- oretical framework for understanding why an extraor-
Mees and Zeitlin (1987) found that one third of the sib- dinary stressor such as an HSCT can have a particular
lings were at risk to develop behavioral problems during effect on a child’s later life.

234 Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Psychosocial Adjustment of Adolescent Siblings

Building on Eriksonian theory, Green et al. (1985)


suggested a psychosocial model for understanding
posttraumatic reactions, with a focus on the interaction
of traumatic stressors, individual characteristics, and the
social environment in which the trauma is experienced
and in which the patient recovers. The interactional
model set forth by Green et al. is widely accepted in
the field and is considered a general model applicable
to all traumatic experiences (Peterson, Prout, &
Schwartz, 1991). The current study drew on the theo-
ries of Erikson (1959, 1968) and Green et al. (1985) and
used a psychosocial-developmental framework to con-
ceptualize the effects of the stressor of HSCT on sib-
ling donors and nondonors. The literature on traumatic
Figure 1. The Ages of Man
SOURCE: Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: stress in children presents a wide range of stressors,
Norton. Reproduced with permission. including sniper attacks, diverse disasters, and devel-
opmental transitions, as precipitating events of a post-
traumatic stress response in children (Trad & Greenblatt,
In Eriksonian theory, each psychosocial stage is 1990). Studies on children’s responses to stressors such
characterized by a conflict that has two opposing pos- as medical and surgical procedures have suggested
sible outcomes. If the conflict is resolved in a satisfactory that involvement in HSCT could generate a traumatic
manner, the positive quality becomes part of the ego and stress response in siblings (Hobbie et al., 2000; Meeske,
healthy development is enhanced. However, if the con- Ruccione, Globe, & Stuber, 2001; Pot-Mees, 1989;
flict is resolved unsatisfactorily, the negative quality is Stuber, Nader, Yasuda, Pynoos, & Cohen, 1991).
incorporated into the personality structure, interferes Accordingly, our psychosocial-developmental model
with further development, and may manifest itself in considered the factors that may influence posttrau-
psychopathology (Muuss, 1988). According to Erikson, matic stress responses. These factors include charac-
the crisis of trust versus mistrust characterizes infancy teristics of the stressor, individual characteristics, and
and the infant’s need for a predictable and relatively con- social environmental factors. The model also includes
sistent form of interaction with others. In the play age, posttraumatic cognitive processing. This study did not
children learn varying degrees of self-control through examine the way in which cognitive mechanisms medi-
language acquisition, motor coordination, and more ate or process stressful events.
sophisticated interpersonal relationships. The issue is As depicted in Figure 2, within this psychosocial-
between becoming an autonomous, creative individual developmental framework, the primary sequence is the
and an inhibited individual filled with self-doubt (Muuss, occurrence of a stressor or traumatic event, specifi-
1988). By the age of 5 years, the child builds on his or cally, the HSCT experience. The stressor leads to cog-
her autonomy to engage in more self-initiated activi- nitive processing, which in turn leads to a final positive
ties that build a sense of self-esteem. The psychosocial or negative outcome, which constitutes the sibling’s psy-
conflict is the development of a sense of initiative ver- chosocial adjustment. A sibling’s psychosocial adjust-
sus a sense of guilt. During the school years, develop- ment depends on the interaction of characteristics of the
ing cognitive capacities enables the child to broaden the stressor, individual sibling characteristics, and social
basis of personal and social competence through environmental factors. When a sibling has a positive out-
advanced learning. The task is the development of a come, Eriksonian developmental crises may be a source
sense of industry versus the emergence of feelings of of growth. In contrast, when a sibling has a negative out-
inferiority. With adolescence, there is the need to estab- come, Eriksonian developmental crises may be a source
lish an identity unique to oneself and apart from the ear- of maladjustment. The model seeks to account for the
lier identifications acquired during the formative years fact that certain persons exposed to trauma experience
(Erikson, 1959). The psychosocial conflict is a sense maladjustment whereas others experience growth or
of identity versus identity confusion. restabilization (Green et al., 1985).

Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004 235


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Packman et al.

Figure 2. Psychosocial Developmental Framework for Understanding Posttraumatic Responses


NOTE: HSCT = hematopoietic stem cell transplantation.

Method Study Goals

The goals of this study were the following:


Design/Power Analysis 1. to assess the current psychosocial adjustment of
donor and nondonor siblings,
The study employed a cross-sectional and retro- 2. to compare the current psychosocial adjustment
spective design using both quantitative and qualitative of donor and nondonor siblings,
methodologies. Information on the siblings’ current 3. to describe how involvement in the HSCT expe-
psychosocial functioning was collected through stan- rience affects development as conceptualized by
dardized parent and teacher behavior rating scales, Erikson and the psychosocial model of post-
child self-report measures and drawings, a background traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and
questionnaire, and parent and child interviews. The 4. to formulate recommendations for the care of
siblings of children who will be undergoing
first author reviewed all relevant literature; however, no
HSCT in the future.
previous research existed upon which to base an esti-
mate of statistical power. Moreover, the low number of
available participants made it impracticable to con- Procedure
duct a full-scale pilot study and use those results to esti-
mate an effect size. In this study, the alpha level was Letters announcing the study were sent to potential
set at .05 for a two-tailed test. Trends toward signifi- participants describing the study’s purpose and proce-
cance at the .10 level were also reported. dures. A refusal postcard was included with the letter.

236 Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Psychosocial Adjustment of Adolescent Siblings

Approximately 2 to 3 weeks later, those who had not have undergone HSCT at UCSF medical center (July
returned the refusal card were telephoned, and a time 1982 through October 1994). Of these 218 pediatric
and place to meet was scheduled. Participants had the HSCT recipients, 87 were no longer alive at the time
option of arranging their interviews to coincide with one of administration of the sibling protocols. This provided
of their regular, posttreatment appointments at the Uni- a pool of 131 families. Eighty-seven families were
versity of California, San Francisco (UCSF), or being excluded from the study for the following reasons:
interviewed in their own homes. Six of the 44 partici- pediatric recipient had no sibling at the time of bone
pants were interviewed at UCSF (14%), and 86% were marrow transplant (n = 21), the sibling was not between
interviewed in their own homes. At the time of partic- the ages of 6 and 18 years (n = 20), the parent and sib-
ipation, the study was again explained and questions ling did not speak English (n = 14), the patient was in
answered. If parents and siblings wanted to partici- extremely poor health at the time of administration of
pate, they were asked to sign a copy of the consent form. the sibling protocols (n = 6), the bone marrow trans-
Informed consent was obtained only after the partici- plant took place more than 7 years ago (n = 8), the fam-
pants had read the consent form and had an opportu- ily was not available for follow-up (n = 12), and refusal
nity to ask questions about the study. A child assent to participate in the study (n = 6).
paragraph was included in the parental consent form. The six refusals were for diverse reasons: too busy
Parents were also asked to sign a consent for the to participate, lack of interest, or already completed an
release of school information. The child’s present (entirely unrelated) questionnaire on care at UCSF.
teacher was asked to complete a rating of the child Examination of characteristics of families who refused
according to the format of the Behavior Assessment Sys- to participate revealed no differences between partic-
tem for Children (BASC)–Teacher Rating Scale. ipants and nonparticipants in terms of family and sib-
Following informed consent and the children’s assent, ling characteristics, socioeconomic status, ethnicity,
siblings were administered projective drawings, self- patient’s disease, or place of residence.
report measures, and a semistructured interview. All of The total sample included 44 siblings (21 donors, 23
the measures were read to the siblings. Following the nondonors, ages 6 to18 years). Only 1 sibling per fam-
sibling’s assessment, a semistructured interview was ily was interviewed. Children’s mean age was 11.0
administered to parents. Parents also completed a back- years (SD = 3.2). Seventy-one percent (n = 31) were
ground questionnaire and behavior rating scales females. Two of the patients had undergone more than
(BASC–Parent Rating Scale) on the siblings. All of the one HSCT. In the total sample, 43% (n = 19) were of
data were collected in one 3-hour session. The infor- Caucasian background. Asian Americans (27%, n = 12)
mation from teachers was collected separately by mail. and African Americans (11%, n = 5) were well repre-
sented; there was a small proportion of Native Amer-
Sample icans (6.8%, n = 3) and Latino Americans (4.6%, n =
2). Family income ranged from less than $10,000 per
Participants were siblings of living pediatric HSCT year to more than $60,000 per year with roughly equal
patients from UCSF. Only siblings of living patients numbers in each category. The majority of siblings
were enrolled to avoid including the effect of recent (73%, n = 32) resided in two-parent homes. Tables 1
bereavement as a possible confound. Participants were and 2 compare the two sibling groups with respect to
excluded if (a) the pediatric recipient had no sibling at various characteristics. The sibling donor group and non-
the time of HSCT, (b) the sibling was not between the donor group had no significant differences on any sib-
ages of 6 and 18 years, (c) the siblings or immediate ling demographic or family variables.
family did not speak English, (d) the HSCT took place Of the 12 adolescent sibling participants, 3 were
more than 7 years ago (an exclusion based on PTSD boys and 9 were girls. The mean age at the time of
theoretical grounds), (e) the parents or siblings did not HSCT was 12 years (SD = 3.2), and the mean age at
want to participate, or (f) the family recently experienced the time of the study was 15 years (SD = 1.3).
other significant stressors (e.g., death of a child who had Before and during the HSCT, psychosocial support
a previous HSCT). services were provided to the patient and family mem-
The selection of cases took place from January bers, including siblings, by the clinical social worker.
through October 1994. A total of 218 pediatric patients A psychologist was available for support on an as-

Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004 237


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Packman et al.

Table 1. Sibling Demographic Characteristics (N = 44)

Group

Variable Sibling Donors (n = 21) Sibling Nondonors (n = 23) Test of Difference

Gender X2(1) = 2.13


Male 4 (19.1%) 9 (39.1%)
Female 17 (80.9%) 14 (60.9%)
With respect to patient, sibling is X2(5) = 0.41
Younger sister 3 (14.3%) 4 (17.4%)
Younger brother 1 (4.8 %) 4 (17.4%)
Older sister 14 (66.7%) 9 (39.1%)
Older brother 3 (14.3%) 4 (17.4%)
Twin sister 0 (0.0%) 1 (4.4%)
Twin brother 0 (0.0%) 1 (4.4%)
Age at interview (years) t(42) = –0.09
M 11.0 11.0
SD 2.9 3.6
Grade in school at interview X2(2) = –0.69
Gradesa 0 to 6 12 (57.1%) 11 (47.8%)
Grades 7 to 9 7 (33.3%) 8 (34.8%)
Grades 10 to 12 2 (9.5%) 4 (17.4%)
Age at HSCT (years) t(42) = 0.41
M 8.5 8.00
SD 3.7 4.3
Time since HSCT (months) t(42) = –0.83
M 34.3 41.6
SD 25.0 32.2
NOTE: Percentages may not add to 100% due to rounding.
a. One sibling had not yet started school and was listed as Grade 0.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

needed basis. Before the donation procedure, a child life Child Post-Traumatic Stress Reaction Index. The
worker prepared the donor siblings by explaining the siblings completed the Child Post-Traumatic Stress
procedure in age-appropriate language. Reaction Index (Frederick, Pynoos, & Nader, 1992). The
reaction index has been used to assess symptoms after
exposure to a broad range of traumatic events and uses
Assessment Measures a Likert-type 5-point rating scale to rate the frequency
of occurrence of symptoms. A score of 12 to 24 indicates
a mild level of PTSD reaction; 25 to 39, a moderate
Child Self-Report Measures level; 40 to 59, a severe level; and greater than 60, a very
severe reaction. Using these guidelines, the correlation
of reaction index scores was reported as .91 on children
The Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale with confirmed clinical cases of PTSD (Frederick,
(RCMAS). The siblings completed the RCMAS 1985). The Post-Traumatic Stress Reaction Index has
(Reynolds & Richmond, 1985) a 37-item self-report been updated (Rodriguez, Steinberg, & Pynoos, 1998),
instrument that measures chronic, manifest anxiety. and studies have reported high internal consistency
For the total anxiety score, an internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .92) and moderate to strong convergent
estimate of .83 was assessed on the norm group of 329 validity (.37 to .63) (Kutlac et al., 2000).
children. A cross-validation sample of 167 children
yielded a similar reliability estimate of .85 (Reynolds Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE). The older
& Richmond, 1985). siblings (ages 12 years and older) completed the RSE

238 Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Psychosocial Adjustment of Adolescent Siblings

Table 2. Family Demographic Data (N = 44)

Group

Variable Sibling Donors (n = 21) Sibling Nondonors (n = 23) Test of Difference

Family structure X2(1) = 0.03


Two-parent home 15 (71.4%) 17 (73.9%)
Single-parent home 6 (28.6%) 6 (26.1%)
Ethnicity X2(5) = 2.59
African American 2 (9.5%) 3 (13.0%)
Asian American 8 (38.1%) 4 (17.4%)
European American 8 (38.1%) 11 (47.8%)
Latino American 1 (4.8%) 1 (4.4%)
Native American 1 (4.8%) 2 (8.7%)
Other 1 (4.8%) 2 (8.7%)
Family income (in 1,000s) X2(4) = 1.04
Below 10 2 (9.5%) 3 (13.0%)
10 to 26 4 (19.1%) 4 (17.4%)
26 to 41 5 (23.8%) 3 (13.0%)
41 to 60 5 (23.8%) 7 (30.4%)
More than 60 5 (23.8%) 6 (26.1%)
Mother's education X2(3) = 1.08
11th grade or less 1 (4.8%) 3 (13.0%)
High school diploma 6 (28.6%) 5 (21.7%)
Some college (1 to 3 years) 8 (38.1%) 8 (34.8%)
Four or more years college 6 (28.6%) 7 (30.4%)
Father's education X2(3) = 5.06
11th grade or less 0 (0.0%) 2 (9.1%)
High school diploma 9 (45.0%) 5 (22.7%)
Some college (1 to 3 years) 4 (20.0%) 9 (40.9%)
Four or more years college 7 (35.0%) 6 (27.3%)
NOTE: Percentages may not add to 100% due to rounding.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. ***p < .001.

(Rosenberg, 1965), a standardized rating of self-esteem measure is suitable for adults and children 6 years and
consisting of 10 items describing one’s sense of self- older. The KFD-R has been used in a 3-year longitudinal
worth. The RSE has a Guttman scale coefficient of study to understand patients’ and siblings’ feelings and
reproducibility of .92, indicating good internal attitudes toward childhood cancer (Spinetta & Deasy-
consistency. Because it is not possible to use the RSE Spinetta, 1981) and in studies of siblings of children with
for younger children because of its adult language, the cancer (Rollins, 1990).
wording of items in the RSE was rephrased to develop The scoring system yields four scores: family com-
the Children’s Self-Image Scale (CSI) (Rosenberg & munication, self-image, emotional tone, and KFD-R
Simmons, 1972). In our study, 5 of the older participants total score (overall level of family support). On the KFD-
were given both self-esteem measures in a single sitting. R, possible scores range from 0 to 35, with the higher
The association between the RSE and the CSI was score indicating poorer adjustment (Spinetta et al.,
near perfect, with a Pearson correlation of r(3) = .908 1981). Two registered art therapists used the Spinetta
(p = .033). The RSE was used with older siblings (12 et al. (1981) system to score the KFD-R independ-
years and older) and the CSI with younger children. ently. Interrater reliability was determined by correlating
the two therapists’ scorings for the 44 drawings. Inter-
Kinetic Family Drawing–Revised (KFD-R). The rater reliability was high for the total score, r(42) = .949.
overall level of family support was measured with the On the subscales, the interrater correlations were .920,
KFD-R (Spinetta, McLaren, Fox, & Sparta, 1981), an .810, and .822 for communication, self-image, and
age-independent objectively structured instrument. The emotional tone, respectively. The KFD-R was used to

Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004 239


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Packman et al.

compare the donor’s and nondonor siblings’ perceptions ious HSCT stages; (b) quantify and explore parents’
of overall family support. In addition, the comments of memories, feelings, and views of events surrounding
the art therapists were used in the qualitative portion the HSCT; and (c) obtain descriptive material for case
of the study to provide a fuller view of the effects of studies.
the HSCT procedure on siblings.
Sibling Behavior Measures
MPD. The 12 adolescent siblings (ages 13 and older)
completed the MPD (Hawley, 1988). The MPD is a self-
report instrument that assesses adolescent and adult The BASC. The BASC–Parent Rating Scale (PRS)
personality development. The measure contains 112 self- (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992) assesses the child’s
descriptive statements that are rated on a 5-point scale. behavioral problems and social competencies in home
The MPD has eight positive scales, eight negative and community settings. The PRS Externalizing
scales, eight resolution scales, and three total scales. The Problems Composite reflects disruptive behavior
positive scales assess the positive attitudes (e.g., trust, problems such as hyperactivity and aggression; the
industry), and the negative scales measure the negative Internalizing Problems Composite measures anxiety,
attitudes (e.g., mistrust, inferiority), which Erikson depression, and similar “overcontrolled” behavior; and
proposes as the basic constituents of personality. The the Adaptive Skills Composite summarizes prosocial
resolution scales assess the degree and direction (positive and other adaptive skills. The PRS composites have high
or negative) of resolution of each developmental stage. internal consistency (.80 to low .90) and high test-
Total scales assess overall psychosocial adjustment retest reliability (.70 to .90).
(Hawley, 1988). The BASC–Teacher Rating Scale (TRS) assesses the
For the MPD positive and negative scales, alpha child’s behavioral problems and social competencies in
coefficients range from .65 to .84, indicating good school and contains the same scales and composites as
internal consistency of the inventory. Research has the PRS. An additional School Problems Composite
demonstrated convergent and discriminant validity for reflects academic difficulties including problems of
the MPD. Convergent validity was supported by high attention and learning. On the TRS, all composite reli-
intercorrelations of positive scales (.75 to .85) and neg- abilities (except internalizing problems) exceed .90, and
ative scales (.67 to .89; Hawley, 1988). Of theoretical all composites have high test-retest reliability (.82 to
interest in the current study were the adolescent par- .91).
ticipants’ MPD resolution scores. The resolution scores
represent the status of conflict resolution for each of
Erikson’s psychosocial stages. Results

Sibling and Parent Interviews


Comparison of Donor and Nondonor
The interview items contained questions that assessed Siblings on Self-Report Measures
aspects of the donor’s and nondonor’s experience of and
attitudes about the bone marrow donation. Several of The t test for independent samples was used to com-
the content areas (memories/reflections of HSCT, pare donors and nondonors on self-report measures. The
changes in the family, recommendations to children and scores revealed significantly higher levels of anxiety in
professionals) were based on research by Koocher and the sibling donor group, as measured by the RCMAS,
O’Malley (1981) as well as personal communications t(42) = 2.89, p = .006. On self-esteem measures, sta-
with investigators in pediatric psychology. Specific tistically significant differences were found between the
questions and content areas were also suggested by groups. On both self-esteem measures, the mean scores
members of the UCSF pediatric HSCT team including for the sibling donor group were significantly higher
physicians, social workers, nurses, and child life spe- (lower self-esteem) than for the nondonor group (RSE:
cialists. The interviews included both forced-choice t(18) = 2.22, p = .040; CSI: t(22) = 2.49, p = .021).
and open-ended questions and were used to (a) quan- On the Child Post-Traumatic Stress Reaction Index,
tify and explore siblings’ subjective responses to the var- no significant differences emerged between donors

240 Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Psychosocial Adjustment of Adolescent Siblings

and nondonors, t(42) = 0.267, p = .791. It is notewor- Findings from Semistructured Interviews
thy that approximately one third of the siblings in each
group showed a moderate to severe level of posttrau- A number of themes emerged from semistructured
matic stress reaction. interviews with siblings and parents that are particularly
As shown in Tables 3 and 4, on the MPD, 12 ado- salient to the adolescent subsample (Packman, Crit-
lescent siblings were assessed for resolution of Erik- tenden, Rieger Fischer, et al., 1997): (a) Sibling donors
sonian stages, 6 from the donor group and 6 from the felt they had “no choice” and wanted to be more
nondonor group. Five out of the 6 donors showed low involved in the donation decision-making process, (b)
to moderate resolution in at least one of the five areas donors experienced considerable loneliness following
assessed, whereas not any of the 6 nondonors were donation, (c) siblings felt there was minimal explana-
below the normal range of resolution on any of the same tion of the HSCT process, and (d) donors reacted to the
areas. Two of the 6 donors showed poor resolution on stresses of HSCT and parental inattention by with-
the trust-mistrust dimension. Two of the 6 donors drawing and keeping their feelings to themselves,
showed poor resolution on the autonomy-shame and whereas nondonors reacted with externalizing behav-
doubt dimension. Three of the 6 donors showed poor iors (attention-seeking acts in school, sharing feelings
resolution on the initiative-guilt dimension. One of with others).
the 6 donors showed poor resolution on the
industry-inferiority dimension. One of the 6 donors
showed poor resolution on the identity-role confusion Discussion
dimension.
For an adolescent, involvement in HSCT is an
extremely stressful experience. Major threats to the
Comparison of Donor and Nondonor adolescent include loss of control, separation from his
Siblings on Behavioral Measures or her peer group, and changes in body image (Sourkes,
1992). At this developmental stage, behavioral responses
The t test for independent samples was used to com- to HSCT may include acting-out behaviors, low self-
pare donor and nondonor siblings on the BASC-PRS. esteem and self-criticism, and intensification of the
The parent-rated scales yielded no statistically signif- adolescent’s normal self-centeredness (Eth & Pynoos,
icant differences in adaptive or maladaptive behavior 1985). The continuing developmental task of integrat-
between donors and nondonors. However, there was a ing the multiple and changing aspects of self into a
trend toward a significant difference between the groups coherent whole may be significantly jeopardized.
on the depression variable, with parents reporting For the adolescent siblings, salient findings emerged
higher levels of depression in donors, t(42) = 1.73, concerning Eriksonian theory. A majority of adolescent
p = .091. The donors’ average score on the withdrawal donors (5 out of 6) reported unresolved developmen-
variable was quite high (M = 57.71, SD = 10.31). The tal crises on the MPD. In contrast, none of the 6 non-
nondonors’ mean score on the withdrawal variable was donors reported unresolved developmental crises.
52.1 (SD = 11.2). According to Eriksonian theory, the trauma of HSCT
In the area of school problems, teachers reported sig- occurring during a specific developmental stage could
nificantly more difficulty for nondonor siblings than for conceivably create special vulnerabilities by impeding
donors on the overall school problems composite, successful resolution of growth issues during that stage.
t(41) = –2.04, p = .048. On subscales within the school Attempts to cope with the trauma and its psychologi-
problems composite, significant differences existed on cal sequelae may affect the adolescent’s ability to tol-
the learning problems variable, t(41) = –2.21, p = .033 erate the normal anxiety created by developmental
(examples include spelling or math problems or diffi- transitions (Johnson, 1989). The combination of the anx-
culty with textbooks). There was a trend toward a sig- iety stemming from the trauma plus the normal anxi-
nificant difference on the attention-problems variable, ety created by developmental transitions can impede
with teachers reporting more attention problems in mastery of developmental tasks. A low resolution score
nondonors, t(41) = –1.69, p = .098. on the MPD suggests developmental stress resulting

Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004 241


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Packman et al.

Table 3. Measures of Psychosocial Development (MPD) Donor Siblings

242 Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Psychosocial Adjustment of Adolescent Siblings

Table 4. Measures of Psychosocial Development (MPD) Nondonor Siblings

from lack of adequate resolution of the specific stage may affect the process of psychosocial development in
conflict (Hawley, 1988). two ways. First, the stressor may intensify the pre-
Our findings of unresolved developmental crises in dominant stage of ego development (the stage the sib-
adolescent donors are congruent with other studies ling was in at HSCT). Second, the stressor may lead to
(Quinn-Beers, 2001; Wilson, 1980). According to Wil- regression in ego development. In the latter case, the
son (1980), a stress-producing event (such as HSCT) negative poles of Erikson’s developmental stages may

Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004 243


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Packman et al.

be exacerbated. That is, a trauma could lead to profound Many donors in our study reported feeling especially
mistrust, doubt, a sense of vulnerability, guilt, inferi- isolated after their surgery.
ority, and identity confusion. Thus, developmental The feelings of isolation and exclusion were evident
issues that had been previously resolved could resur- in some of the adolescent donors’ family drawings
face. The developmental effects suggested by Wilson (KFD-R). One drawing, in particular, by a 16-year-old
may be applicable to the adolescent donor subsample. adolescent, contained several maladaptive indicators in
Developmentally, we expected that all the adolescent the communication and self-image subscales such as
donors were in the identity versus identity confusion barriers between people, missing body parts, and incom-
stage at the time they were assessed. As shown in Fig- pleteness of body (Packman et al., 1998). In addition,
ure 3, in 5 of 6 cases, the HSCT stressor may have led the sibling portrayed the patient (10-year-old brother)
to the resurfacing of developmental issues that had in a pejorative manner, that is, sitting down with his back
been resolved. In some instances (e.g., Participants 3 turned away from the viewer, in his own world. It is
and 5), the HSCT may have also exacerbated the devel- noteworthy that the sibling excluded herself from the
opmental issues the sibling was trying to master at the drawing. This is a telling statement of her view of her-
time of HSCT. It is not surprising that the sibling self in the family unit—she isn’t there. The family
donors’ maladaptive attempts to cope with the physi- environment is one of isolation and poor communica-
cal and psychological impact of bone marrow donation tion between family members (Packman et al., 1998).
might result in a loss of developmental accomplishments The adolescent donor’s depiction of her brother suggests
and inadequate resolution of Eriksonian stages. continuing anger and resentment toward him. The
The findings of increased anxiety and low self- exclusion of herself suggests that the sibling still views
esteem in donors parallel the impact of surgical inter- the patient as the focus of the family’s attention and feels
ventions, in general, on children’s development. excluded from the family unit (Packman et al., 1998).
Children perceive invasive medical procedures as Of interest, isolation is one of Erikson’s developmen-
aggressive attacks on their bodies (Marcus, 1986). tal tasks for postadolescence. Because the epigenetic
Such procedures can trigger anxieties and inner fears. principle states that all stages are present from the
In our study, sibling donors remarked, “I felt like I beginning to the end of life, intimacy versus isolation
was the one being violated,” and “I felt like I was the is present as a precursor in adolescence. The impact of
target.” Several siblings voiced concerns about possi- isolation during adolescence could negatively affect the
bly dying during the donation procedure. anticipated stage of intimacy versus isolation. The
When a child undergoes an invasive medical pro- adults in the family may not have positively resolved
cedure, normal developmental processes are inter- the stage and may be unable to be inclusive in their
rupted and self-esteem may be impaired. The resolution responses to the entire family.
scores on the MPD for the donor siblings suggest that From an Eriksonian perspective, psychic trauma
self-esteem depends, in part, on successful resolution may shatter the child’s sense of basic trust and damage
of each of Erikson’s developmental crises (Wylie, the child’s sense of autonomy (Terr, 1985). Shame (as
1974). The subset of adolescent donors reported unsuc- opposed to autonomy) may be related to the child’s tem-
cessful resolution of Eriksonian developmental crises. porary loss of personal choice. In pediatric HSCT
It is, therefore, not surprising that the entire donor cases, the child donor is typically given the opportu-
group (n = 21) reported significantly lower self-esteem nity to voice concerns and raise objections. However,
than the nondonor group did. in many interviews, siblings reported that they did not
It may seem counterintuitive that donors would feel that they really had a choice in the matter. As one
report lower levels of self-esteem than nondonors adolescent sibling said, “I wasn’t spoken to about this.
because donors have the opportunity to save their sib- My dad came to me and said ‘you’re doing this’—
ling’s life and make the most of their time as “hero.” it wasn’t my choice.” This temporary loss of personal
However, although the family’s investment in the donor choice plus the stressor of the surgical intervention
before surgery is considerable and supportive, the focus may rob a child of a sense of personal influence (Terr,
of the family returns to the patient shortly after HSCT. 1985). As a result, there may be a loss of develop-

244 Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Psychosocial Adjustment of Adolescent Siblings

Figure 3. Psychological Reactions of Donor and Nondonor Siblings to Hematopoietic Stem Cell Transplantation

mental accomplishments that manifest in internalizing logical reactions of donor and nondonor siblings to
symptomatology and low self-esteem. In a recent study HSCT. That is, although both groups of siblings report
of pediatric sibling donors, most donors reported that comparable levels of PTSD reactions, the manifesta-
the psychosocial aspects of the HSCT process were of tion may be different. This conceptualization of PTSD
greater significance to them than the physical aspects, reactions is depicted in Figure 3.
noting that they were not prepared to deal with the Donor siblings may respond with more internaliz-
range of emotions they experienced (MacLeod, Whit- ing behaviors, such as anxiety, depression, withdrawal,
sett, Mash, & Pelletier, 2003). Donors also felt that they and inadequate resolution of Eriksonian developmen-
needed more information about the emotional aspects tal crises. In contrast, nondonors may respond with
of being a donor (MacLeod et al., 2003). externalizing behaviors, such as attention and learning
A result with major therapeutic implications was the problems in school and reckless or careless behavior
relatively comparable levels of posttraumatic stress (problems in impulse control). For donors and non-
symptomatology in both groups of siblings. For donors, donors, Eriksonian developmental crises could pro-
HSCT involves the psychological shock of a surgical vide opportunities for growth or maladjustment.
intervention followed by ongoing stresses. Although the Unfortunately, the MPD can be used only with sib-
nondonor sibling is not subjected to an intrusive med- lings 13 years or older. We do not know whether the
ical procedure, the child experiences the stressor of hav- same pattern of results would be found in the younger
ing one and possibly two family members (patient and siblings. It is too early to predict whether the findings
donor) involved in HSCT. on the Erikson MPD measure shown by adolescent
In evaluating posttraumatic stress responses, it is donors will lead to successful or unsuccessful resolu-
useful to consider a broad definition that includes tion of the psychosocial stage they were in at the time
symptoms such as anxiety and depression, rather than of the interview (identity versus role confusion) and of
only symptoms that are part of a Diagnostic and Sta- later developmental stages. Wilson (1978) noted that
tistical Manual of Mental Disorders (fourth edition) the occurrence of a traumatic event during adolescence
diagnosis (Vogel & Vernberg, 1993). That broader view might lead to prolonged moratorium (a period of actively
gives us a different way to conceptualize the psycho- searching to find one’s identity). It will be interesting

Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004 245


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Packman et al.

to see if this happens with the participants in this study. worried, find out about it. Most kids I’ve seen don’t
Further research of a longitudinal nature is required to ask questions, they just say “do it.”
provide information on this.
Many adolescent donors also stated that they were
Limitations angry at their parents during and following the HSCT
process. It is possible that the siblings’ anger is a
Some methodological issues limit the nature of the consequence of their disappointment with parents for
conclusions that may be drawn from this study’s find- not adequately considering their need for independ-
ings. To allow a maximum intake of cases, a cross- ence and autonomy and for not including them in the
sectional and retrospective design was employed. As a decision-making process.
result of this design, there was considerable variabil- Our findings suggest a need for intervention strate-
ity in the period of time that elapsed between HSCT and gies specifically tailored for adolescent siblings, espe-
the time the siblings were assessed. Thus, the length of cially donors. As noted by Quinn-Beers (2001), peer
time since HSCT as well as subsequent life experiences support and support groups may be valuable, but fam-
may have influenced sibling reports. Furthermore, the ily interventions may be particularly useful. Overall, it
interview data were based on recall and are subject to is important to understand and respect the adolescent’s
the limitations of siblings’ memories. specific needs within the context of overall adolescent
Potential limits to the generalization of findings development. The assistance of health care profes-
may arise because all participants were drawn from only sionals is most effective when understanding and respect
one HSCT center. Other transplant centers may draw for the adolescent’s position are coupled with steady
patients from populations who have different demo- guidance (Weil, 2000).
graphics and different patterns of family interaction. Technical medical information, decision options,
Other centers may provide different predonation and and potential risks should be presented in small sequen-
postdonation psychosocial services for families. tial steps. The need for independence is taken into con-
sideration by adequately including the adolescent in all
aspects of the decision-making process (Enns & Pack-
Implications for Practice man, 2002). Importantly, as an adolescent moves devel-
opmentally to become more independent, the family also
According to Eriksonian theory, one of the many feels the stress of this process (Eth & Pynoos, 1985).
tasks of development for adolescents is gaining a sense Parents under the additional stress of the HSCT process
of autonomy and independence. An adolescent donor’s may be less able to be supportive of their adolescent.
need for independence is taken into consideration by In joint sessions with adolescents and parents, pediatric
adequately including them in the HSCT decision-making oncology nurses can model for both parent and ado-
process. In interviews, many donors stated that there lescent an attitude of respect for the other’s point of view
was minimal explanation of the HSCT, they felt unin- (Enns & Packman, 2002). Thus, when struggles over
volved in the donation decision-making process, and autonomy and decision making occur in the session, it
they felt as if they had no choice. A 15-year-old sibling can be useful to acknowledge the legitimacy of both
suggested that they “put the transplant procedure into sides as well as the normality of the process.
human words. Put it in terms so that we can understand In addition, the need for social support is crucial in
it.” A 17-year-old echoed these thoughts: times of stress (Erikson, 1959, 1968). Emotional social
support has been linked with the mastery of develop-
mental tasks within the adolescent period (Harter,
I had terms thrown at me that I didn’t under- 1990). Research has indicated that the primary source
stand. They had all these technical words and I’d of emotional support for chronically ill adolescents is
go “yeah” and nod and I really didn’t understand. the family system (Ritchie, 2001). The findings from
I’d tell another kid that if there is something you the current study suggest that adolescent siblings, espe-
don’t understand, ask twice about it. If you are cially donors, would benefit from emotional support

246 Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Psychosocial Adjustment of Adolescent Siblings

from family members. Pediatric oncology nurses can Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. New York: Inter-
assist parents in finding an effective balance between national Universities Press.
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
independence and connection to parents before, during,
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Youth, identity, and crisis. New York:
and after the HSCT process: “Parents must be ready to Norton.
let go and yet stay connected to guide, and be protec- Eth, S., & Pynoos, R. S. (1985). Developmental perspective on psy-
tive” (Garcia-Preto, 1999, p. 281). Pediatric oncology chic trauma in childhood. In C. R. Figley (Ed.), Trauma and its
nurses can also help parents to discover ways in which wake: The study and treatment of post-traumatic stress (pp. 36-
siblings can play a meaningful role in the patient’s 52). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Frederick, C. J. (1985). Selected foci in the spectrum of post-
treatment. Sibling donors have suggested the importance traumatic stress disorder. In J. Laube & S. A. Murphy (Eds.),
of two sources of emotional support and information, Perspectives on disaster recovery (pp. 110-130). Nowalk, CT:
both parents and health care professionals (MacLeod Appleton-Century Crofts.
et al., 2003). Parental information helps to comfort the Frederick, C. J., Pynoos, R. S., & Nader, K. (1992). Child Post-
donor, “while information from professionals helps Traumatic Stress Reaction Index. Los Angeles: University of Cal-
ifornia Press.
make it real and truthful” (MacLeod et al., 2003, p. 229).
Freund, B. L., & Siegel, K. (1986). Problems in transition follow-
In summary, the MPD results, along with the find- ing bone marrow transplantation: Psychosocial aspects. Amer-
ings from other objective measures, projective draw- ican Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 244-252.
ings, and interviews, suggest that adolescent donors may Garcia-Preto, N. (1999). Transformation of the family system dur-
be struggling for independence while integrating issues ing adolescence. In B. Carter & M. McGoldrick (Eds.), The
such as trust and mistrust, shame and doubt, initiative expanded family life cycle: Individual, family and social per-
spectives (pp. 274-286). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
and guilt. Erikson views development as the resolution Green, B. L., Wilson, J. P., & Lindy, J. D. (1985). Conceptualiz-
of conflicts from opposing forces. Ideally, the child ing posttraumatic stress disorder: A psychosocial framework.
develops a favorable ratio in which the positive aspects In C. R. Figley (Ed.), Trauma and its wake: The study and
dominate the negative. In Eriksonian theory, develop- treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder (pp. 53-69). New York:
ment is a lifelong process: Sometimes childhood con- Bruner/Mazel.
Harter, S. (1990). Self and identity development. In S. S. Feldman
flicts are not resolved satisfactorily until adulthood. The
& G. R. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adoles-
adolescent donors were in the process of developing a cent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
positive sense of identity. Part of the ongoing process Hawley, G. A. (1988). The measures of psychosocial development
involves making sense of and understanding the HSCT (MPD). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
experience. Pediatric oncology nurses are in a position Hobbie, W. L., Stuber, M., Meeske, K., Wissler, K., Rourke, M. T.,
to help minimize the adjustment difficulties of siblings Ruccione, K., et al. (2000). Symptoms of posttraumatic stress
in young adult survivors of childhood cancer. Journal of Clin-
to the HSCT process. ical Oncology, 18, 4060-4066.
Johnson, K. (1989). Trauma in the lives of children. Alameda, CA:
Hunter House.
Acknowledgments Koocher, G. P., & O’Malley, J. E. (1981). The Damocles syn-
drome: Psychosocial consequences of surviving childhood can-
The authors would like to thank the siblings and par- cer. New York: McGraw-Hill.
ents who invited us into their lives and their homes and Kutlac, M., Layne, C. M., Wood, J., Saltzman, W. R., Stuvland, R., &
Pynoos, R. S. (2000). Contextual influences on the long-term
who wanted so much to help other families going
adjustment of war-exposed Bosnian adolescents. In R. Wraith
through the HSCT process. We also thank Gwen Haw- (Chair), Contextual influence on the development of children’s
ley, Ph.D., for her helpful comments and suggestions post-trauma responses. Third World Conference for the Inter-
on this manuscript. national Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, Melbourne,
Australia.
MacLeod, K. D., Whitsett, S. F., Mash, E. J., & Pelletier, W. (2003).
References Pediatric siblings donors of successful and unsuccessful
hematopoietic stem cell transplants (HSCT): A qualitative study
Enns, G. M., & Packman, W. L. (2002). The adolescent with an of their psychosocial experience. Journal of Pediatric Psy-
inborn error of metabolism: Medical issues and transition to adult- chology, 28, 223-231.
hood. Adolescent Medicine: State of the Art Reviews, 13, 315- Marcus, I. M. (1986). Psychological issues in accidents and phys-
329. ical trauma in children. In S. I. Greenspan & G. H. Pollock (Eds.),

Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004 247


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016
Packman et al.

The course of life (Vol. III): Middle and late childhood (pp. 465- Rollins, J. A. (1990). Childhood cancer: Siblings draw and tell. Pedi-
487). Madison, WI: International Universities Press. atric Nursing, 16, 21-27.
Matsen, A. S. (1992). Resilience in individual development: Suc- Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
cessful adaptation despite risk and adversity. In M. C. Wang & Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
E. Gordon (Eds.), Educational resilience in inner-city America: Rosenberg, M., & Simmons, R. G. (1972). Black and White self-
Challenges and prospects (pp. 1-38). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence esteem: The urban school child. Washington, DC: American
Erlbaum. Sociological Association.
Meeske, K. A., Ruccione, K., Globe, D. R., & Stuber, M. L. (2001). Sourkes, B. M. (1992). Psychological aspects of leukemia and
Posttraumatic stress, quality of life, and psychological distress other hematologic disorders. In D. Nathan & F. Oski (Eds.),
in young adult survivors of childhood cancer. Oncology Nurses Hematology of infancy and childhood (pp. 1754-1768). Philadel-
Foundation, 28, 481-489. phia: W.B. Saunders.
Muuss, R. E. (1988). Theories of adolescence. New York: Random Spinetta, J. J., & Deasy-Spinetta P. (Eds.). (1981). Living with
House. childhood cancer. St. Louis, MO: C. V. Mosby.
Packman, W. L., Crittenden, M. R., Long, J., Gruenert, C., Fischer, Spinetta, J. J., McLaren, H. H., Fox, R. W., & Sparta, S. N. (1981).
J., Schaeffer, E., et al. 1998. The kinetic family drawing with The kinetic family drawing in childhood cancer: A revised
donor and nondonor siblings of pediatric bone marrow trans- application of an age-independent measure. In J. J. Spinetta &
plant patients. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Ther- P. Deasy-Spinetta (Eds.), Living with childhood cancer (pp.
apy Association, 15, 177-184. 86-120). St. Louis, MO: C. V. Mosby.
Packman, W. L., Crittenden, M. R., Rieger Fischer, J., Schaeffer, Stuber, M. L. (1996). Psychiatric sequelae in seriously ill children
E., Bongar, B., & Cowan, M. J. (1997). Siblings’ perceptions and their families. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 19, 481-
of the bone marrow transplant process. Journal of Psychosocial 493.
Oncology, 15, 81-105. Stuber, M. L., Nader, K., Yasuda, P., Pynoos, R. S., & Cohen, S.
Packman, W. L., Crittenden, M. R., Schaeffer, E., Bongar, B., (1991). Stress responses after pediatric bone marrow trans-
Rieger Fischer, J., & Cowan, M. J. (1997). Psychosocial con- plantation: Preliminary results of a prospective longitudinal
sequences of bone marrow transplantation in donor and nondonor study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
siblings. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, cent Psychiatry, 30, 952-957.
18, 244-253. Terr, L. C. (1985). Psychic trauma in children and adolescents. Psy-
Peterson, K. C., Prout, M. F., Schwartz, R. A. (1991). Post- chiatric Clinics of North America, 8, 815-835.
traumatic stress disorder: A clinician’s guide. New York: Trad, P. V., & Greenblatt, E. (1990). Prevention and treatment of
Plenum. stress in preschool children: Psychotherapeutic interventions.
Pinegger-O’Connor, G., & Bender-Gotze, C. (1980). Betreung In L. E. Arnold (Ed.), Childhood stress (pp. 521-544). New York:
Minderjahriger Knochenmarks-Spender: Psychologische Wiley.
Aspeckte. [Treatment of underage bone marrow donors] (D. Vogel, J. M., & Vernberg, E. M. (1993). Children’s psychological
Voigts, Trans.). Munch Med Wochenschr, 122, 597-601. responses to disasters. Journal of Clinical Child Psychiatry, 22,
Pot-Mees, C. C. (1989). The psychosocial effects of bone marrow 464-484.
transplantation in children. Delft, the Netherlands: Eburon. Weil, J. (2000). Psychosocial genetic counseling. Oxford, UK:
Pot-Mees, C. C., & Zeitlin, H. (1987). Psychosocial consequences Oxford University Press.
of bone marrow transplantation in children: A preliminary com- Wiley, F. M., Lindamood, M. M., & Pfefferbaum-Levine, B. (1984).
munication. Journal of Psychosocial Oncology, 5, 73-81. Donor-patient relationship in pediatric bone marrow trans-
Quinn-Beers, J. (2001). Attachment needs of adolescent daughters plantation. Journal of the Association of Pediatric Oncology
of women with cancer. Journal of Psychosocial Oncology, Nurses, 1, 8-14.
19(1), 35-49. Wilson, J. P. (1978). Identity, ideology and crisis: The Vietnam vet-
Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1992). Behavior assessment eran in transition (Vol. 2). Washington, DC: Disabled Ameri-
system for children. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance can Veteran.
Service. Wilson, J. P. (1980). Conflict, stress and growth: The effects of
war on psychosocial development among Vietnam veterans.
Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (1985). Revised Children’s
In C. R. Figley & S. Leventman (Eds.), Strangers at home:
Manifest Anxiety Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychologi-
Vietnam veterans since the war (pp. 123-165). New York:
cal Services.
Praeger.
Ritchie, M. A. (2001). Sources of emotional support for adolescents
Wolcott, D., Wellisch, D., Fawzy, F., & Landsverk, J. (1986). Psy-
with cancer. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing, 18, 105-110.
chological adjustment of adult bone marrow transplant donors
Rodriguez, N., Steinberg, A., & Pynoos, R. S. (1998). UCLA PTSD
whose recipient survives. Transplantation, 41, 484-488.
Index for DSM IV. Los Angeles: UCLA Trauma Psychiatry
Wylie, R. C. (1974). The self-concept. Lincoln: University of
Service.
Nebraska Press.

248 Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 21(4); 2004


Downloaded from jpo.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on April 8, 2016

You might also like