Journal How Remedial Teachers

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Feature Article

How Remedial Teachers


Teach Vocabulary
Mary C. Shake, Richard L. AUington, Rob Gaskins, and Mary Beth Marr

Twenty experienced teachers, all with reading education


background, designed and implemented what they con-
sidered to be exemplary vocabulary lessons. Transcripts
of these audiotaped lessons were analyzed for the number
of statements relating to word recognition or conceptual
development. Conceptual development statements were
analyzed further to determine a definition, semantic
feature, or contextual usage focus. Additionally, teachers
wrote rationales for the vocabulary words and instruc-
tional strategies selected. Results indicate that the
majority of teachers provided dual-focused lessons,
although conceptual development statements pre-
dominated in over half of the lessons. Lessons aimed at
conceptual development, however, were shallow, and pro-
vided few opportunities for students to acquire a deep
understanding of words.

R ESULTS OF PREVIOUS research indicate that acqui-


sition of word meanings is a critical activity for the
developing reader and a major factor in achieving well-
(3) the number of multiple-meaning words for which the
reader can select the appropriate meaning to match the
context of the text.
developed comprehension skills. However, researchers Furthermore, Pearson (1985) noted that vocabulary
also indicate that elementary teachers infrequently de- knowledge positively influences comprehension when
sign or select vocabulary instructional activities that students " o w n " words. This ownership involves both
develop conceptual understanding during reading lessons receptive vocabulary knowledge and the ability to use
(e.g., Beck & McKeown, 1985, Blachowicz, 1987). words expressively in new situations or contexts. Sim-
The goal of the present study was to describe the ilarly, Stahl (1986) argued that deep-level processing is
nature of vocabulary instruction taking place in remedial needed if vocabulary knowledge is to enhance textual
settings. More specifically, the study was guided by the understanding. Stahl's deepest level of processing, gen-
following four questions: (1) Do teachers focus on word eration processing, is similar to Pearson's notion of word
recognition, conceptual development (word meaning), ownership.
or both?; (2) Does instruction in conceptual develop- Others (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 1985; Speidel, 1985)
ment emphasize definitions, contextual usage, or seman- have suggested that vocabulary knowledge is not an all-
tic features?; (3) What rationales do teachers offer for or-nothing issue. Rather, vocabulary knowledge is multi-
the words addressed or strategies employed in vocabu- faceted, and probably ranges from zero knowledge or
lary instruction?; and (4) Are such rationales congruent familiarity to fluent and rich conceptual understanding.
with the above-mentioned focus of instruction? Speidel (1985) noted:
It has been noted that degree of vocabulary knowl-
edge is related to degree of text comprehension (Ander-
son & Freebody, 1981). McCormick (1987) suggested There is much we still need to learn about the structure
three types of vocabulary knowledge related to the com- and nature of different word meanings and about the pro-
prehension of text: (1) the number of words to which cesses by which these different word meanings are learned
the reader can attach meaning, (2) the number of words before we can map more succinctly the type of instruction
to which the reader can attach multiple meanings,
Downloaded and
from rse.sagepub.com to the
at University of Manitoba type
Libraries of 20,
on June word
2015 meaning, (p. 35)

Remedial and Special Education 51


There are two logical extensions of this viewpoint. large number of new concepts or words, or focus more
One is that not all vocabulary instructional procedures thoroughly on a fewer number.
are equally effective with all types of words (e.g., Beck Stressing that teachers should focus vocabulary in-
& McKeown, 1985; Blachowicz, 1985; Carr & Wixson, structional efforts on "usable" words, Blachowicz (1985)
1986; Ryder, 1986; Sorenson, 1985; Stahl, 1986). The suggested that teachers ask themselves the following
other extension is that there may be different goals for questions: (1) Will it be important for students to know
vocabulary instruction, and that all words targeted for the meaning of this word 5 years from now? (2) Will
instruction neither can be nor need be dealt with to the knowing this word help the students figure out other
point of deep-level processing. words related to it? and (3) Is the word's meaning cen-
The previous discussion has focused on teaching tral to the understanding of the selection? Blachowicz
vocabulary prior to having students read the selection. suggested that if only the third question can be answered
Wixson (1986) noted that this is a common practice. In in the affirmative, instruction need not equip students
contrast, Ruddell (1986) suggested that comprehension with a deep understanding of the word.
and vocabulary development are interactive processes. Furthermore, Beck and McKeown (1985) noted that
He stated: "Since many new words are encountered in a mature, literate person's vocabulary contains three
written text, comprehension of the text must occur tiers: (a) basic words, (b) words that are of high frequen-
before vocabulary learning can take place" (p. 587). In cy and general utility for mature language users, and (c)
a related vein, Nagy and Herman (1985) and Herman, words that apply to a specific domain, or whose fre-
Anderson, Pearson, and Nagy (1987) have argued that quency of use is very low. They suggested that words
much word learning must, of necessity, be incidental contained in this second tier are those that most often
in nature. Such incidental vocabulary learning grows out should be selected for rich vocabulary instruction.
of frequent opportunities to read various types of texts Once target vocabulary words have been selected,
on diverse topics. teachers' next decisions should focus on choosing the
Arguments could be made to support either side of instructional procedures that will best facilitate student
the controversy about whether meaning vocabulary or understanding of these words. It is in this realm that we
text comprehension exerts the greater influence on the find a great disparity of viewpoints. While one might
other. It seems safest to conclude, however, that the two argue the benefits of using, for example, semantic map-
components are related, and that if textual comprehen- ping (e.g., Johnson, Pittleman, & Heimlich, 1986), defini-
sion is the ultimate goal of reading instruction, then tions (e.g., Weiss, Mangrum, & Llabre, 1986), or defini-
some reading instructional time must be devoted to tions plus context (e.g., Stahl, 1986; Weiss et al., 1986),
vocabulary development. it has been noted that the type of instructional procedure
Research designed to determine the effects of vocab- chosen should reflect attention to the goals of instruc-
ulary instruction on comprehension has produced tion, the types of words to be taught and learned, and
mixed results (Carr & Wixson, 1986; Ruddell, 1986). the characteristics of the learners (Beck & McKeown,
How might instruction be delivered so that students 1985).
possess a more complete form of word knowledge? In an attempt to translate the results of vocabulary
One of the first steps teachers must address when research into classroom practice, several researchers
planning for effective vocabulary instruction is choos- have suggested principles or guidelines for effective
ing the words to be taught. Choosing these target words vocabulary instruction. Below is a compilation of these
for instruction is often a difficult task, particularly when principles. Readers should refer to the cited references
the selection students are reading contains many words for a more complete explanation.
the teacher is certain are unfamiliar to most students.
Beck and McKeown's (1985) guidelines for target word 1. Build a conceptual basis for words to be learned
selection have been discussed previously. Stahl (1986), (Blachowicz, 1985; Carr & Wixson, 1986).
Blachowicz (1985), and Beck and McKeown (1985) have 2. Stress involvement of the students (Blachowicz,
addressed the issue of target vocabulary selection in very 1985; Carr & Wixson, 1986).
practical terms. 3. Create opportunities for students to make vocabu-
Stahl (1986) suggested three issues teachers might con- lary their own; that is, encourage the use of vocabu-
sider when choosing the words to be taught: (1) Decide lary in students' own writing and speech (Blacho-
how important the words will be to the students. If a wicz, 1985; Graves & Duin, 1985).
word is one that is neither important to the understand- 4. Develop in students vocabulary skills that may be
ing of the text nor one that students are likely to meet employed independently and that are transferable
again in other readings, it need not be taught. (2) Decide to all reading tasks (Blachowicz, 1985; Carr & Wix-
whether the context in which a word occurs is rich son, 1986; Nagy & Herman, 1985; Schwartz &
enough to allow students to gain meaning. If new words Raphael, 1985).
occur in a rich context, and it is unlikely they will be 5. Help students develop elaborated word knowledge
needed in other reading situations, they need not be- (Carr & Wixson, 1986; Sorenson, 1986; Stahl, 1986).
come the target of instruction. (3) Decide whether it 6. Provide multiple exposures to words in various con-
would be more advantageous for Downloaded
students to cover a texts (Sorenson, 1985; Stahl, 1986).
from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on June 20, 2015

52 Volume 10 Issue 5 September /October 1989


7. Focus extensive vocabulary instruction on words portunity to practice selecting the most appropriate
that will be of long-term use to students (Blacho- meaning of a word to pick for a particular context. Such
wicz, 1985). reliance also negates the instructional guideline that
8. Make vocabulary instruction a long-term goal of students need multiple exposures to words in multiple
school curricula (Blachowicz, 1985). contexts.
9. Familiarize students with resources to help with Given the recognized importance of vocabulary ac-
word learning (e.g., books about words) (Blacho- quisition generally, and the role that acquiring new word
wicz, 1985). meanings plays in understanding much curricular ma-
10. Increase the amount of time students spend reading terial, the reports describing classroom vocabulary devel-
(Nagy & Herman, 1985). opment are distressing. While the longstanding debates
11. Give students an understanding of what it means continue in the literature about the most effective ways
to really know a word (Graves & Duin, 1985). to develop conceptual understandings for new terms,
12. Continuously monitor students' understanding of there seems little evidence available that these issues
word meanings through the reading of the text and have yet reached the classroom. In this study, we at-
beyond (Sorenson, 1985). tempted to optimize various instructional conditions in
13. Provide students with more concrete experiences order to assess what a sample of experienced teachers
(e.g., show them a picture) before asking them to would offer as an ideal vocabulary lesson. We hoped
abstract the meaning of a word from text (Ryder, that by observing vocabulary instruction under these
1986). conditions, we might learn more about classroom vocab-
14. Encourage students to make sense out of the vocab- ulary instruction.
ulary (Thalen, 1986).

The previous discussion represents a sample of the Method


theoretical and practical perspectives on the importance Subjects
of vocabulary instruction and the most effective ways
to teach students the meanings of new words. However, The 20 experienced teachers we studied were grad-
results of observational research provide evidence sug- uate students at two universities in two regions of the
gesting that teachers infrequently employ such instruc- country. These teachers were enrolled in a clinical read-
tional strategies when introducing new words in reading ing course that required a tutorial component, and all
lessons. For example, Durkin (1978-79) noted that class- volunteered to participate. None of the participants,
room lessons in reading or social studies rarely included however, held remedial reading teaching positions in the
activities that developed word meanings. Instead, the public schools. The lesson planning formats were iden-
focus was on accurate pronunciation or simple word tical for the tutorials in both settings and required an
recognition. In a later study, Durkin (1984) found that indication of what types of prereading, reading, and
only about 2 0 % of the elementary teachers used the postreading activities were planned and why. The vo-
sentence context activities suggested in the teacher cabulary lesson assignment was one of several that were
manuals accompanying basal reader series when intro- taped or directly observed for later analysis and discus-
ducing new vocabulary. The remaining teachers most sion with the teachers.
often simply listed the new words on the board and
Procedure
guided learners' pronunciations, though some simply
omitted any introduction of new words. The ideal teaching lesson paradigm was borrowed
A recent observational study (Blachowicz, 1987) ex- from Garner (1984), who argued that this approach pro-
amined the nature of vocabulary instruction in six vides a useful shortcut for studying instructional activi-
average fourth-grade reading groups. It was found that, ties that may occur only infrequently in classrooms.
on the average, 1 5 % of the reading group time was Garner's paradigm allows the teacher to optimize in-
devoted to vocabulary instruction. Of this vocabulary structional conditions and plan what he or she would
instructional time, 4 5 % was spent dealing with target consider an exemplary lesson if no restrictions were
words in context, 17% with definitions, 11 % with pro- placed on time, materials, and so forth. We hoped to
nunciations, and 11 % with personal context. On the sur- optimize instructional conditions in four ways: (1) Our
face, these results appear encouraging, and in contradic- sample teachers included those with several years of
tion to Durkin's findings. A closer examination, how- teaching experience, and with both undergraduate and
ever, reveals that the contextual activities usually in- graduate preparation in the teaching of reading (at least
volved words in a single sentence, and these sentences 12 semester hours total). This, we felt, should provide
were frequently unrelated to the selection to be read. a sort of expert sample for observation. (2) We assigned
Blachowicz detected a heavy influence of basal reader the vocabulary lesson in advance and informed the
manual suggestions in these single-sentence contextual teachers that we would be recording the instruction.
activities. Reliance on a single-sentence context is prob- Thus, the teachers had ample time to prepare what they
lematic, since it does not foster knowledge of multiple felt was an adequate and appropriate lesson for develop-
meanings of words, nor does it allow students
Downloaded the op-
from rse.sagepub.com
ing vocabulary knowledge. (3) Though we restricted the
at University of Manitoba Libraries on June 20, 2015

Remedial and Special Education 53


curricular material on which these lessons should be lessons in which greater than 6 0 % of these statements
based to trade books, this still allowed the teachers to emphasized conceptual development. In summary, then,
select from a diverse range of materials. (4) Although the we found that in vocabulary lessons that participating
instruction was offered in a tutorial setting rather than teachers designed as exemplary, vocabulary-related state-
a classroom, the students and teachers knew each other. ments focused, generally, on both pronunciation (37.6%)
Teachers planned, implemented, and audiotaped what and conceptual development (62.17%). Conceptual
they considered exemplary vocabulary lessons. They development statements were in the majority in over
were told that the lesson should be related to the book/ half of the lessons.
story being read. They were asked to write their reasons
for selecting particular words and strategies for Categories of Conceptual
instruction. Development Statements
Audiotapes were transcribed and transcripts analyzed
for the number of statements relating to word recogni- In light of the research on effective methods of vo-
tion or conceptual development. Those statements in cabulary instruction, we further analyzed conceptual
the latter category were further divided into definitions, development statements to determine whether they em-
contextual usage, or semantic feature focus. A statement phasized definitions, contextual usage, or semantic fea-
was defined as an utterance of any length, by a single tures. Again, an examination of Table 1 reveals that 10
person, with a particular focus. lessons had the greatest percentage of conceptual de-
The following classification system emerged upon ex- velopment statements emphasizing definitions; six
amination of written rationales: (1) Unfamiliar (word rec- lessons emphasized contextual usage, and four empha-
ognition)—the student would be unable to pronounce/ sized semantic features.
decode the words; (2) Unfamiliar (meaning)—meanings Analysis for this purpose revealed a mismatch between
of words were essential to text understanding, yet teacher and student statements. For example, one
unknown to the student; (3) Strategy—the words pro- teacher stated: "Use harpoon in a sentence." The stu-
vided appropriate practice of word recognition and/or dent response was: "Well, it's long, and sharp, and used
meaning vocabulary strategies being introduced or to kill whales." While the teacher statement was coded
reviewed. Dual- or tri-focused strategies were coded under the contextual usage category, the student state-
under multiple categories. All written rationales were ment fell under the category of semantic features.
reviewed by two of the researchers. Interrater agreement Table 2 contains the average percentages of teacher
was 91 %. Differences in coding were resolved through and student vocabulary-related statements categorized
discussion. as pronunciation, definition, contextual usage, and
semantic features. An important observation that can be
made from this table is that the lessons were teacher
Results and Discussion dominated. That is, approximately 6 0 % of the task-
related statements in these lessons were uttered by
Vocabulary lessons varied in length, ranging from 6 6 teachers. In two lessons (Teachers 6 and 13) roughly
to 359 statements. All lessons contained some off-task three-fourths of these statements were accounted for by
conversation. The percentages of vocabulary-related "teacher talk." It must be remembered that these lessons
statements within these lessons ranged from 4 2 . 6 % to were conducted in individualized settings that lent
9 7 . 3 % ( M = 7 1 . 7 % ) . The number of words dealt with themselves quite well to the direct instruction format.
in these vocabulary lessons ranged from 4 to 21 (M= 10). The modeling and explanation aspects of direct instruc-
The summary data, by lesson, derived from transcript tion lessons generally lead to a large amount of teacher,
analyses are found in Table 1. Table 1 will serve as our as opposed to student, talk. On the other hand, if the
primary reference point in answering the first two re- goal of vocabulary instruction is to provide students with
search questions. "ownership" of the words or to encourage deep-level
processing, one would expect to find ample opportun-
Focus of Vocabulary Lessons ities for students to talk about and use the words dur-
ing the lessons. In four of these exemplary lessons, the
Our initial analysis involved determining whether the percentages of teacher and student talk were nearly
vocabulary-related statements within these lessons re- equivalent, but a majority of student talk was not evi-
flected primarily pronunciation/decoding, conceptual dent in any lesson.
development, or both aspects of vocabulary instruction. A second observation that may be made from Table
An examination of Table 1 reveals that all but two les- 2 is the mismatch in the average percentages of teacher
sons (Teachers 5 and 6) contained both pronunciation/ statements and student statements coded in each of the
decoding and conceptual development statements. Five four categories. In this respect, the most glaring mis-
teachers (Teachers 1, 7, 10, 11, and 12) were found to match occurs with teacher and student statements em-
conduct lessons in which greater than 6 0 % of the vo- phasizing definitions, since on the average twice as many
cabulary-related statements focused on word pronun- teacher statements as student statements were coded in
ciation or decoding. Eleven other teachers conducted this category.
Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on June 20, 2015

54 Volume 10 Issue 5 September/October 1989


Table 1 . Distribution of Teacher and Student Vocabulary Statements by Team

Note, a = Student's chronological age; b = Student's instructional reading level; c = pronunciation; d = definition; e = contextual usage;
f = semantic features; g = total statements; h = vocabulary-related statements. * Records are incomplete. These values are estimated
from an analysis of goals, materials, instructional transcripts, and other clinical records.

Remedial and Special Education Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on June 20, 2015
55
Table 2. Classification Averages for Teacher c. Knowledge of these terms and their meanings is essen-
Statements and Student Statements tial to the comprehension of the story.
d. The words are appropriate to use for structural analysis
procedures.
(Code: meaning, strategy)

Analysis of the rationales offered by teachers indicated


that 12 teachers mentioned the importance of being able
to decode/pronounce the new words. Sixteen teachers
(80%) mentioned that target words were chosen, at least
Rationales for Vocabulary Selection in part, because their meaning was unfamiliar to stu-
dents, while eight teachers noted that such words lent
The rationales these teachers offered all centered on themselves to the practice or application of a particular
vocabulary selection and represented three categories: strategy (e.g., Contextual Redefinition, List-Group-Label).
word recognition (pronunciation and decoding); words Eight teachers offered dual-focused rationales for vocab-
whose meaning would be unfamiliar to the students, yet ulary selection, mentioning both the pronunciation/
central to the overall understanding of the text; and decoding and meaning of new words (Categories 1 and
words that lent themselves to the practice and/or appli- 2). An additional four teachers stated dual-focused ra-
cation of a particular vocabulary strategy. The rationales tionales reflecting meaning and the fact that the words
varied in their specificity, the majority representing lent themselves to the practice and application of par-
multiple categories. The following are two of the more ticular strategies (Categories 2 and 3), while only one
explicit rationales offered. teacher's rationale was targeted at Categories 1 and 3.
Similarly, only three teachers supplied rationales for
There are a number of reasons for the strategies and words vocabulary selection that addressed all three categories.
used in pre-reading. Sue has problems identifying words
out of context. The use of the compare-contrast and Glass
Analysis will help her identify words out of context without Congruence Between Rationales and Instruction
relying on a lot of rules and individual letter sounds which
seem to bog her down. The transfer of these strategies to We noted earlier that 18 of the teachers (90%) pre-
identifying individual vocabulary words in her independent sented dual-focused lessons that contained both pronun-
reading is slow and will take much more practice. The pro- ciation and conceptual development statements. It was,
cess of transfer is just beginning. Much review is needed
therefore, unexpected to find that only eight teachers
beyond the introduction that has taken place in the reading
(40%) mentioned both these goals of vocabulary instruc-
clinic.
tion in their rationales. When teachers mentioning tri-
The purpose behind selection of the vocabulary words is focused rationales are also considered, the total percent-
threefold. One is for identification through clinician model- age of teachers mentioning both pronunciation and con-
ing and student proper responses. The use of either compare- ceptual development in their rationales increases to
contrast or Glass Analysis is part of the word identification 5 0 % . Based on these data, it is unclear whether teachers
process. The second is for understanding. This again is inadvertently included parts in their lessons that they
through modeling of usage in a sentence by the clinician. did not anticipate including, or whether they geared
The student uses the word in a sentence of her own for their rationales to their ultimate goal, but decided in
understanding of the word meaning. If the student still has
either the preactive or interactive phase of instruction
problems with word meaning, going over the meaning of
that activities with another focus were necessary as
the word out of context may be necessary. It will then be
prerequisites or supplements.
used in sentences. The third purpose, and most important,
in my estimation, is to have the student, through the vo-
cabulary list, put together a picture of what she will be
reading. The vocabulary is chosen to help the student have Conclusions
an idea of what the total story or part of the story is about.
This is very important because of the independent level of When these teachers were asked to design exemplary
reading I am asking the student to do during the tutoring vocabulary lessons, all focused either exclusively on con-
session. After all, the basic purpose of reading is to gain
ceptual development or presented dual-focused lessons
understanding. (Code: pronunciation, meaning, strategy)
centering on both the pronunciation and meaning of
new words. Also, the majority of the teachers mentioned
These words were chosen for vocabulary development
in their rationales the importance of learning the mean-
because:
ing of new words. We find these results encouraging.
a. Most of these words are not likely to be part of the stu- A closer look at these lessons, however, indicated
dent's sight word vocabulary. some negative results as well. In several instances, the
b. Some of these words have more than one meaning such task required by the teacher and the response offered
as "station" and "company." It is important to know by the students did not match, yet the teachers accepted
the proper meaning to understand their use in the story. these responses. Schwartz and Raphael (1985) noted that
Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on June 20, 2015

56 Volume 10 Issue 5 September/October 1989


in order to properly develop definitions of words, stu- Gaskins is a clinical supervisor and doctoral stu-
dents need to ask themselves three questions about the dent in the Department of Reading at SUNYA. He is
words: What is it? What is it like? What are some exam- currently completing his dissertation research on
ples? Perhaps the student participating in these lessons affective components in prose comprehension. Mary
would have benefited from such instruction. B e t h Marr received her PhD from the University of
Close examination also revealed that vocabulary in- Minnesota. She was on the faculty at SUNYA during
struction aimed at conceptual development was often the period of data collection and now is lecturer in
shallow. That is, the instruction was frequently domi- education at the University of North Carolina-
nated by teacher talk, and students were given com- Charlotte. Dr. Marr's research interests are in the
paratively few opportunities to use the words in ways area of analogy and prose learning. Address: Mary
that would encourage deep processing (Stahl, 1986) or C. Shake, College of Education, Department of Cur-
ownership (Pearson, 1985) of words. Rarely were the riculum and Instruction, Dickey Hall, University of
guidelines for effective vocabulary instruction, as listed Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506-0017.
previously, followed. We found little evidence of instruc-
tion that involved students actively in word learning, References
helped them relate new vocabulary to their background
knowledge, or aided them in developing strategies for Anderson, R.C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge.
independent vocabulary acquisition. In J.T. Guthrie, (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Re-
While the rationales teachers offered for the words search reviews (pp. 7 7 - 1 1 7 ) . Newark, DE: International
they selected for instruction were interesting, and in Reading Association.
many cases very explicit, they did not often reflect the Beck, I.L., & McKeown, M.G. (1985). Teaching vocabulary:
types of vocabulary selection decisions recommended Making the instruction fit the goal. Educational Perspec-
tives, 23(1), 1 1 - 1 5 .
by Stahl (1986), Blachowicz (1985), or Beck and
Blachowicz, C. (1985). Vocabulary development and reading:
McKeown (1985). For example, teachers often chose
From research to instruction. The Reading Teacher, 38,
words for instruction that were surrounded by rich con- 876-881.
text in the text being read. Stahl suggested that such Blachowicz, C. (1987). Vocabulary instruction: What goes on
words could be dealt with by students independently. in the classroom? The Reading Teacher, 41(2), 1 3 2 - 1 3 7 .
We believe that these teachers exerted effort and care Carr, E., & Wixson, K.K. (1986). Guidelines for evaluating
to present lessons that included a balance between the vocabulary instruction. Journal of Reading, 29, 5 8 8 - 5 9 5 .
two major goals of vocabulary instruction: pronuncia- Durkin, D. ( 1 9 7 8 - 1 9 7 9 ) . What classroom observations reveal
tion/decoding and conceptual development. It could about reading comprehension instruction. Reading Re-
very well be that the teachers who participated in this search Quarterly, 14, 4 8 1 - 5 3 3 .
study were unaware of guidelines researchers have iden- Durkin, D. (1984). Is there a match between what teachers do
and what basal reader manuals recommend? The Reading
tified for vocabulary instruction. Another possible ex-
Teacher, 37, 7 3 4 - 7 4 4 .
planation for the type of data presented here is that the
Garner, R. (1984). Rules for summarizing texts: Is classroom
teachers were aware of vocabulary instructional guide-
instruction being provided? Journal of Educational Re-
lines, but were unsure of how to implement them in ac- search, 77, 3 0 4 - 3 0 7 .
tual lessons. We suggest, therefore, that vocabulary in- Graves, M., & Duin, A. (1985). Building students' expressive
struction that encourages higher levels of conceptual vocabularies. Educational Perspectives, 2'3(1), 4 - 1 0 .
development and ultimately, higher levels of compre- Herman, P.A., Anderson, R.C., Pearson, P.D., & Nagy, W.E.
hension, is an issue that deserves much attention in (1987). Incidental acquisition of word meanings from ex-
reading methods courses. Perhaps this is the optimal positions with varied text features. Reading Research
place to influence positively the type of vocabulary in- Quarterly, 22, 2 6 3 - 2 8 4 .
struction that reaches classrooms. £L Johnson, D.D., Pittleman, S.D., & Heimlich, J.E. (1986). Seman-
tic mapping. The Reading Teacher, 39(8), 7 7 8 - 7 8 3 .
McCormick, S. (1987). Remedial and clinical reading instruc-
Mary C. Shake received her EdD from the State
tion. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
University of New York at Albany in 1984. She is Nagy, W.E., & Herman, P.A. (1985). Incidental vs. instructional
currently an assistant professor in the Department approaches to increasing reading vocabulary. Educational
of Curriculum and Instruction at the University of Perspectives, 23(1), 1 6 - 2 1 .
Kentucky, where she teaches undergraduate and Pearson, P.D. (1985). Changing the face of reading comprehen-
graduate level reading and language arts methods sion instruction. The Reading Teacher, 38, 7 2 4 - 7 3 8 .
courses. Her research interests include classroom Ruddell, R.B. (1986). Vocabulary learning: A process model and
and clinical reading instruction and teacher and criteria for evaluating instructional strategies. Journal of
student questioning patterns. R i c h a r d L. Allington Reading, 29, 5 8 1 - 5 8 7 .
received his PhD at Michigan State University and Ryder, R.J. (1986). Teaching vocabulary through external con-
text clues. Journal of Reading, 30, 6 1 - 6 5 .
is professor of education in the Department of
Schwartz, R., & Raphael, T. (1985). Concept of definition: A key
Reading at the State University of New York at
to improving students' vocabulary. The Reading Teacher,
Albany (SUNYA). Dr. Allington's research focuses on
39, 1 9 8 - 2 0 5 .
remedial and special education interventions. Rob
Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on June 20, 2015
(continued on p. 50)
Remedial and Special Education
57

You might also like