Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Topics in Early Childhood

Special Education
Volume 28 Number 3

Family Stress and Children’s Language November 2008 148-157


© 2008 Hammill Institute on
Disabilities

and Behavior Problems 10.1177/0271121408318678


http://tecse.sagepub.com
hosted at http://online.sagepub.com

Results From the National Survey of Children’s Health


Carolyn E. Long
Matthew J. Gurka
James A. Blackman
University of Virginia

The impact of language delays and behavior problems in young children on family function was assessed using data from
the 2003 National Survey of Children’s Health. Over 50% of parents who reported concerns regarding their children’s lan-
guage skills also reported concerns regarding their children’s behavior. Although parents reported increased stress and more
difficulty coping and meeting their children’s needs when they were concerned about their children’s language skills,
children with behavior problems were especially stressful for families. Parental stress increased as children with challeng-
ing behaviors reached preschool age. To address the negative impact on family function, service providers must couple spe-
cific intervention strategies and methods that improve language skills and mitigate behavior problems in young children.

Keywords: language delays; behavior problems; early intervention; preschool; survey of children’s health; family function

T wo key aspects in the early development of


preschool children are language and behavior.
Research has led to increased awareness of the need to
at not being understood. Subsequently, the child may
exhibit a challenging behavior, such as aggression, that
increases parental stress levels and results in ineffective
improve language skills and behavior in young children parenting. The parent may even limit his or her attempts
(Gertner, Rice, & Hadley, 1994; Kaiser, Hancock, Cai, at interacting with the child. The transactional model
Foster, & Hester, 2000). In addition, the early identifica- illustrates how multiple risk factors, such as language
tion of language delays and behavior problems in delay, behavior problems, and parental stress, may result
children is essential for effective intervention (Qi & in poor outcomes for the child’s development and the
Kaiser, 2004). The critical role of emotional and social family’s coping.
well-being in school readiness and the negative out- Speech and language disorders are the most common
comes of early problem behavior have led to a national disabilities in the United States, with studies reporting a
focus on the importance of providing prevention and range of incidence between 3% and 15% in preschool
intervention services to young children with challenging children (Downey et al., 2002). There is evidence that
behaviors and their families (Drotar, 2004; Dunlap, language concerns are often a risk factor for problem
Ester, Langhans, & Fox, 2006; Powell, Dunlap, & Fox, behaviors in young children. Preschool children who
2006; Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000). exhibit challenging behaviors are more likely to have
According to the Sameroff transactional model language deficits than peers without language concerns
(Sameroff & Fiese, 2000), developmental outcomes are (Kaiser et al., 2000). Preschool children with expressive
seen as a result of a continuous dynamic interaction language delays are more likely to have social-emotional
among children’s behavior, caregivers’ responses to problems, and those with receptive delays experience
children’s behavior, and environmental variables that pervasive developmental problems (Tervo, 2007).
may influence both children and caregivers. A child’s Previous research has shown that children from low-
behavior triggers a parent’s interpretation, which pro- income families are especially at risk for significant
duces the parent’s response. In the case of a child with behavior and language problems (Qi & Kaiser, 2004).
language delays, the child’s behavior may be influenced Children from families with lower socioeconomic status
by an inability to comprehend a request or by frustration have lower language scores. Qi, Kaiser, Milan, and

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015


148
Long et al. / Results From the NSCH 149

Hancock (2006) found that African American preschool When parents report concerns regarding their young
children from low-income families scored 1.5 standard children, they often include the areas of language and
deviations below the mean on a test of language func- behavior problems. In a survey conducted by Tervo
tioning compared with European American preschool (2005) of 180 parents of children aged 16 to 80 months,
children. A large percentage of toddlers (41.8%) and the most common concerns reported by the parents
preschoolers (68.1%) referred to child welfare agencies included language and behavioral characteristics. The
have developmental and behavioral needs (Stahmer et al., recent National Survey of Early Childhood Health of
2005). In addition, children may have underlying risk parents of children under 3 years of age reported rela-
factors, including premature birth or genetic disorders, tively high concerns regarding their children’s behavior
for language and behavior problems (Nelson, Stage, (48%), language (45%), and getting along with others
Duppong-Hurley, Synhorst, & Epstein, 2007). (41%) (Halfon et al., 2002).
The persistent nature of behavior problems in young In summary, there appears to be an increased occur-
children has been well documented. Approximately half rence of behavior problems in young children with
of preschoolers who demonstrate problem behaviors developmental and/or language delays. Studies also sug-
continue to have behavior problems when older gest an increase in family stress when young children
(Campbell, 1995). Young children are often referred to exhibit behavior problems. A gap in research knowledge
developmental clinics because of problem behaviors, remains regarding the strength of the interrelationship
with disruptive behavior being the single most common between language delays and behavior problems and
reason for referral to child mental health services. their effect on family function.
Younger age of onset is associated with greater severity To further explore the relationship between language
(Kazdin, 1987), and disruptive behavior has been shown and behavior problems and their impact on family func-
to worsen with time (Neary & Eyberg, 2002). tion, a recent nationwide survey was used to answer the
The characteristics and temperament of preschool following research questions: What is the percentage of
children with challenging behaviors have also been stud- reported language problems only and behavior problems
ied. Externalizing behaviors, such as aggression, are most only, independently, among young children? If parents
predictive of future behavior problems. Other characteris- are concerned about language problems in their children,
tics that may lead to problem behaviors include impul- how much more likely are they to be concerned about
siveness, distractibility, irritability, inflexibility, and behavior problems? Do parents report increased levels of
attention-deficit problems (Brier, 1995). Children classi- stress and more problems in coping when they also
fied as developmentally delayed were more likely to be report concerns regarding their children’s language
reported in the clinical range for externalizing problems development and/or behavior? Do parents report
on the Child Behavior Checklist (Baker et al., 2003). The increased levels of stress as children become older and
prevalence of behavior problems in children with or at reach preschool age?
risk for developmental delays was found to be 42%,
according to Feldman, Hancock, Rielly, Minnes, and
Methods
Cairns (2000). Infants and toddlers who exhibit external-
izing behaviors, such as aggression and destructiveness,
have a high probability of demonstrating problem behav-
Data
iors in school (Gresham, Lane, & Lambros, 2000). The primary data source for this analysis was the 2003
Research has indicated that the families of children National Survey of Children’s Health (NSCH). This sur-
with developmental disabilities may experience height- vey addressed the physical, emotional, and behavioral
ened levels of stress, particularly related to child rearing health of children, including the following child and
(Crnic, Friedrich, & Greenberg, 1983; Hastings, 2002). family demographics: child physical and mental health,
Parenting stress is an important predictor of a child’s health insurance status, family functioning, and parental
behavior problems, regardless of the child’s cognitive mental health. Questions regarding language and behav-
functioning (Crnic & Greenberg, 1990). Parenting stress ior used in this study were taken from the early child-
is related to behavior problems in both developmentally hood section of the survey and were developed using the
delayed and nondelayed samples of young preschool Parent’s Evaluation of Developmental Status (PEDS).
children (Baker & Heller, 1996). Baker et al. (2003) and The PEDS is a tool used to identify children at risk for
Hastings (2002) noted a similar relationship between developmental, behavioral, and social delays (Glascoe,
children’s behavior problems, parental stress, and par- 1997). The PEDS was standardized on a large number of
enting behavior. families from diverse backgrounds and validated against

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015


150 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education

several diagnostic measures. The parental stress ques- 55%, with a range of 49.6% to 64.4%. A total of 6,035
tions constitute the Aggravation in Parenting Scale interviews, or 5.9% of the original sample, were con-
derived from the Parenting Stress Index (Abidin, 1990) ducted in Spanish. Detailed information regarding the
and the Parental Attitudes About Childrearing Scale design of the NSCH is available at the survey’s Web site
(Oster et al., 2002). Questions from these instruments (http://www.nschdata.org).
were written at the fourth to fifth grade reading level.
The Parenting Stress Index has been empirically vali- Analysis
dated to predict observed parenting behavior and
children’s current and future behavioral and emotional All analyses of the NSCH data were performed using
adjustment in a variety of international populations. SUDAAN, which produces accurate variance estimates
The families were asked the question “Are you cur- for complex survey designs (Research Triangle Institute,
rently concerned about how your child makes speech 2004). Population-based estimates were obtained by
sounds and how your child understands you?” to elicit assigning sampling weights to each sampled child for
language concerns. The responses “a lot” and “not at all” whom an interview was completed (Blumberg et al.,
were used for the analyses of parental language con- 2005). Estimates of percentages and odds ratios provided
cerns. To elicit behavioral concerns, the parents were in this article were calculated excluding missing values
asked the question “Are you currently concerned about for the variables in question. Responses of “refused” or
how your child behaves?” Again, the responses “a lot” “I don’t know” were also excluded from the denomina-
and “not at all” were used for the analyses of parental tor. Univariate analyses via χ2 tests at the .05 significance
behavioral concerns. The responses “a little,” “don’t level were performed to determine the extent of the asso-
know,” and “refused” were excluded from the data for ciation between language and behavior concerns and the
simplification purposes. other variables, such as socioeconomic factors and
parental stress and coping outcomes.
To get a more accurate measure of the association
Sampling between language and behavior concerns and parental
stress, logistic regression was used to model the odds of
The NSCH data were obtained by the Centers for
the stress outcomes of interest, adjusting for the potential
Disease Control and Prevention’s National Center for
confounding effects of age, gender, race, parental educa-
Health Statistics using the State and Local Area
tion, and household income. Adjustment for potential
Integrated Telephone Survey program. Individuals who
confounders was limited by what data were collected in
conducted the telephone surveys were paid employees of
the NSCH, but the variables selected represent the usual
the National Center for Health Statistics and had exten-
demographic, environmental, and psychological vari-
sive training in survey administration. Households with
ables that are considered the most likely confounders in
children aged less than 18 years in all 50 states and the
outcomes research of this kind. The following levels of
District of Columbia were identified using the random-
concern were analyzed in these models: parents with no
digit dialing sampling framework of the National
concern regarding their children’s language or behavior,
Immunization Survey. One child from each household
parents concerned only with their children’s language,
was randomly selected to be the focus of the survey. The
parents concerned only with their children’s behavior,
survey respondent was an adult identified in the house-
and parents concerned with their children’s language and
hold as being “the parent or guardian who lives in this
behavior. Adjusted odds ratios and their corresponding
household who knows the most about the health and
95% confidence intervals were computed for each of the
health care of the child.” Parental interviews were con-
three levels of concern, compared with not having lan-
ducted in 12 languages, including English and Spanish,
guage and behavior concerns.
during 2003 and 2004. The data were gathered on a total
of 102,353 children aged 0 to 17 years. For the purpose
of this study, only children between the ages of 10 Results
months and 5 years 11 months were included. The total
number of children in the study sample was 27,350, with Table 1 describes demographic information for
13,871 boys and 13,466 girls. English-speaking families parents who expressed a lot of concern regarding their
totaled 24,524, with 2,809 families speaking other lan- children’s language and for those who were concerned
guages. Nearly 69% of the households contacted for the about their children’s behavior. Language was defined in
NSCH completed the 25-minute telephone interview. terms of how a child understood his or her parent or how
Thirty-two states accomplished response rates of over a child spoke. Behavior was defined as how a child

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015


Long et al. / Results From the NSCH 151

Table 1
Language and Behavior Concerns by Demographics, Children Aged 10 Months to 5 Years
% With “a Lot” % With “a Lot”
of Parental of Parental
Concern Concern
Regarding Regarding
Demographic Number of Languagea Behaviora
Variable Children (%) (n = 2,163) pb (n = 1,405) pb

Total 27,350 (100%) 9.9 (0.3) 6.6 (0.3)


Gender <.0001 .0001
Male 13,871 (50.9%) 11.5 (0.5) 7.7 (0.4)
Female 13,466 (49.1%) 8.4 (0.4) 5.4 (0.4)
Ethnicity <.0001 <.0001
White, non-Hispanic 17,895 (58.3%) 7.3 (0.3) 4.6 (0.3)
Hispanic 4,391 (20.6%) 9.4 (0.8) 6.2 (0.7)
Black, non-Hispanic 2,338 (13.4%) 20.2 (1.3) 14.3 (1.2)
Multiracial, 1,192 (3.2%) 9.1 (1.5) 6.3 (1.1)
non-Hispanic
Other, non-Hispanic 1,153 (4.4%) 14.4 (2.8) 11.3 (3.1)
Relation of respondentc .0078 .0003
Maternal 22,183 (83.1%) 9.8 (0.4) 6.2 (0.3)
Paternal 4,144 (13.4%) 9.5 (0.8) 6.8 (0.6)
Other 1,021 (3.5%) 15.3 (1.8) 14.8 (2.1)
Number of adults <.0001 <.0001
in household
One 2,477 (10.5%) 15.1 (1.2) 14.1 (1.4)
Two 20,640 (73.9%) 8.4 (0.4) 5.3 (0.3)
Three or more 4,198 (15.6%) 13.7 (1.0) 8.0 (0.8)
Highest level of <.0001 <.0001
education of anyone
in household
Less than high school 1,448 (8.7%) 10.4 (1.3) 9.3 (1.4)
High school graduate 5,443 (25.5%) 13.2 (0.8) 8.8 (0.7)
More than high school 20,299 (65.8%) 8.5 (0.4) 5.3 (0.3)
Primary household language .7484 .2996
English 24,524 (84.4%) 10.0 (0.4) 6.4 (0.3)
Other 2,809 (15.6%) 9.6 (1.1) 7.6 (1.1)
Health care coverage at <.0001 <.0001
time of survey
No 1,888 (7.6%) 9.3 (1.3) 7.2 (1.1)
Private 17,826 (60.3%) 7.9 (0.4) 4.4 (0.3)
Public 7,313 (32.0%) 14.0 (0.7) 10.5 (0.7)
Household income <.0001 <.0001
classification
Below federal 3,561 (19.7%) 14.7 (1.0) 11.1 (1.0)
poverty leveld
At or above federal 21,238 (80.3%) 8.6 (0.4) 5.3 (0.3)
poverty level

Source: 2003 National Survey of Children’s Health.


a. All variables are listed as percentages (with standard errors in parentheses) of the row attribute. Those participants who were not applicable
(because of age or otherwise) or who had missing values for particular variables were not included in calculations.
b. P value resulting from a χ2 test comparing the distribution of the percentages of “a lot” of concern across the levels of the particular demo-
graphic characteristic.
c. Biological, step, foster, or adoptive.
d. Children living in households with incomes <100% of the federal poverty level.

behaved. A larger percentage of parents expressed con- concern regarding their children’s language and behavior
cern about their children’s language (10%) than about more often than parents of girls, and a larger percentage
their children’s behavior (7%). Parents of boys expressed of Black parents reported concern about their children’s
Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015
152 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education

language (20%) and behavior (14%) than White parents Table 2


(7% for language and 5% for behavior) or Hispanic Relationship Between Parental Language
parents (9% for language and 6% for behavior). The per- Concerns and Behavioral Concerns: Children
centages of mothers and fathers reporting concern Aged 10 Months to 5 Years
regarding their children’s language and behavior were Percentage of Parents/Respondents With
similar. Parents who were the only adults in their house- “a Lot” of Concern Regarding How Their
holds expressed more concern regarding their children’s Children Behave (95% confidence interval)
language (15%) and behavior (14%) than those in house-
Language 10 Months to 2 Years 3 Years to 5 Years
holds with two or more adults. Parents in households Concern 11 Months Old 11 Months Old
with adults who had education levels of high school
graduate or less reported more concern about language “A lot” of parental 53.9 (47.8 to 59.8) 54.3 (48.9 to 59.6)
concern regarding
and behavior in their children than those in households
speech or
with adults who had higher education levels. A larger comprehension
percentage of families whose parents reported concern No parental concern 0.9 (0.6 to 1.4) 2.1 (1.6 to 2.8)
regarding language and behavior in their children had regarding speech
public health care coverage. or comprehension
Table 2 shows the relationship between parental lan- Source: 2003 National Survey of Children’s Health.
guage concerns and behavioral concerns in children aged Note: Participants with missing values for particular variables were
10 months to 5 years 11 months. As shown in the table, not included in the tabulations provided.
53.9% of parents of children aged 10 months to 2 years
11 months and 54.3% of parents of children aged 3 years when they also expressed concern regarding their
to 5 years 11 months who reported concerns regarding children’s behavior. Parents of older children reported
their children’s language also reported concerns about increased stress levels compared with parents of
their children’s behavior problems, whereas only 0.9% younger children when they also reported concerns
of parents of children aged 10 months to 2 years 11 regarding their children’s behavior. Parents who
months and 2.1% of parents of children aged 3 years to expressed concern regarding their children’s behavior
5 years 11 months who had no concerns about their were 13 times (younger children) to 22 times (older
children’s language reported concerns about their children) more likely to report that their children were
children’s behavior problems. much harder to care for than most children, compared
Table 3 provides the age-specific estimated adjusted with approximately 2 times more likely for parents who
odds of parental stress outcomes of interest, comparing expressed concern regarding their children’s language.
parents who reported concerns regarding their children’s Parents were 12 times more likely to express feeling
language, behavior, or both with parents who reported angry with their older children if they also reported
neither type of concern. The two age groups were com- concerns regarding their children’s behavior than
posed of younger children (aged 10 months to 2 years 11 parents who expressed no concern about their children’s
months) and older children (aged 3 years to 5 years 11 behavior.
months). To demonstrate how to read this table, a parent Although it was reasoned that parents expressing con-
who was concerned “a lot” with only language in his or cern regarding both language and behavior problems in
her younger child was estimated to be 6.85 times more their children might have the most stress, that was not
likely to feel that he or she was not coping well with the found to be true from the results of this study. If parents
daily demands of parenthood than a parent with no con- reported concerns regarding both language and behavior,
cerns regarding language or behavior (p < .05). When they were 7 times more likely to report that their children
parents of older children were asked this same question, were more difficult to care for than most children if their
they were 2.57 times more likely to feel that they were children were in the younger age group and 12 times
not coping well with daily demands. more likely for older children. However, parents reported
Parents who reported concern about their children’s more difficulty coping overall with children having only
behavior described feeling much more stress than behavior problems.
parents reporting concern about their children’s lan- Although the effects of several demographic vari-
guage development. Parents of older children were 18 ables, including gender, race, low socioeconomic level,
times more likely to report that their children did things and a high school level of education or less, were taken
that bothered them and that they were having difficulty into account in the multivariate analysis, there were few
coping with the day-to-day demands of parenthood significant associations with parental stress outcomes

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015


Long et al. / Results From the NSCH 153

Table 3
Estimated Adjusted Odds Ratios (95% confidence intervals) of Parent-Related
Stress Outcomes of Interest: Children Aged 10 Months to 5 Years
“A Lot” of Parental Concern Regarding

10 Months to 2 Years 11 Months Old 3 Years to 5 Years 11 Months Old

Parental Both Both


Stress Outcome Language Behavior Language and Language Behavior Language and
of Interest Onlya Onlya Behaviora Onlya Onlya Behaviora

In general, the parent 6.85 1.01 1.17 2.57 18.67 2.83


feels he or she is coping (1.93 (0.11 (0.28 (0.36 (4.89 (1.24
with the day-to-day to 24.31) to 8.90) to 4.86) to 18.50) to 71.22) to 6.50)
demands of parenthood
not very well or not
well at allb
During the past month, 2.35 12.82 7.04 2.09 22.51 12.53
the parent usually or (1.12 (4.83 (3.92 (1.01 (10.92 (7.78
always felt that the to 4.92) to 34.01) to 12.66) to 4.32) to 46.39) to 20.19)
child was much harder
to care for than most
children his or her agec
During the past month, 1.18 13.75 4.21 1.29 18.66 6.91
the parent usually or (0.43 (5.10 (2.23 (0.57 (9.52 (4.09
always felt that the to 3.21) to 37.06) to 7.95) to 2.94) to 36.59) to 11.68)
child did things that
bothered the parent
a lotc
During the past month, 1.67 3.62 2.69 1.58 4.71 2.38
the parent usually or (0.91 (1.27 (1.75 (0.90 (2.55 (1.58
always felt that he or to 3.08) to 10.32) to 4.12) to 2.76) to 8.73) to 3.59)
she was giving up
more of his or her
life to meet the
child’s needs than
he or she ever
expectedc
During the past month, 1.92 5.67 3.81 2.43 12.58 3.86
the parent usually or (0.57 (1.51 (0.97 (0.73 (5.59 (2.01
always felt angry with to 6.54) to 21.35) to 14.89) to 8.08) to 28.33) to 7.43)
the childc

Source: 2003 National Survey of Children’s Health.


Note: Adjusted odds ratios were estimated using logistic regression, with the models including age, gender, race or ethnicity, income, and edu-
cation. Reported 95% confidence intervals that do not contain 1 indicate significance at the .05 level.
a. Odds ratios of the stress-related outcomes odds ratios were estimated for (a) “a lot” of parental language concern only versus no parental lan-
guage or behavior concern, (b) “a lot” of parental behavior concern only versus no parental language or behavior concern, and (c) “a lot” of
both language and behavior concern versus no such concerns. To demonstrate how to read this table, a parent who is concerned “a lot” with only
language is an estimated 6.85 times more likely to feel that he or she is not coping well with the daily demands of parenthood than parents with
no concerns regarding language or behavior (p < .05).
b. Possible responses include “very well,” “somewhat well,” “not very well,” and “not well at all.”
c. Possible responses include “never,” “sometimes,” “usually,” and “always.”

compared with the associations of stress and language than parents of younger boys. Parents of older children
and behavior concerns. Overall, parents of older children reported that they had more difficulty coping if they also
reported more stress than parents from the younger age reported incomes below the federal poverty line. Parents
group. For example, parents of older boys reported that with high school degrees or less reported more difficulty
their children did things that bothered them more often coping and more anger if their children were older.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015


154 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education

Discussion (Rannard, Lyons, & Glenn, 2005). Rannard et al. (2005)


suggested that children who are not referred prior to
In the current study involving an extensive survey of school age may develop behavior problems, poor acade-
parents of young children across the United States, a sig- mic performance, and personal and social difficulties.
nificant number of parents expressed concern regarding Glascoe (1999) found that if parents’ concerns are sys-
their children’s language development. There is evidence tematically elicited, this approach can be used to make
from previous studies that parents are able to detect reasonably accurate referral decisions. The level of con-
developmental delays, including language delays, in cern and amount of stress expressed by parents in the
their young children. Scarborough, Hebbler, and Spiker current study are indicators of the importance of the
(2006) found that speech and communication impair- early identification of language and behavior problems
ment or delay was the most frequently presenting prob- in children to improve family function, regardless of the
lem in children eligible for early intervention services children’s diagnoses.
according to parents (34%) as well as providers (39%). Parents who expressed concern regarding both lan-
Glascoe (1999) also reported expressive language as a guage and behavior did not report the most stress in the
common concern of parents. current study. It was suspected that the group of children
Another important finding of the present study was that with both language and behavior problems may repre-
a large percentage of parents who expressed a lot of con- sent a unique population that has either changed parental
cern regarding their children’s language development also expectations or is receiving intervention services. For
reported concerns about their children’s behavior, indicat- example, Blanchard, Gurka, and Blackman (2006) found
ing that both language delay and behavioral problems may that families with children diagnosed with autism spec-
be present. The emergence of behavior problems in young trum disorder reported being “very close” to their
children with developmental delays may be an example of children and were able to “cope very well” compared
the transactional nature of factors over time (Crnic et al., with parents of children having other chronic develop-
1983). Children with developmental delays have been mental problems (48% vs. 39%). In addition, nearly 90%
shown to have less interactive play (Guralnick, 1987) and of families with children diagnosed with autism spec-
more negative conflicts with peers, which may lead to trum disorder said that they had emotional support in
adjustment problems (Guralnick, 1999). raising their children. On the other hand, Schieve,
There were socioeconomic differences in the findings, Blumberg, Rice, Visser, and Boyle (2007) found that
as seen in the adults’ levels of education, the number of parenting a child with autism who required special ser-
adults in the household, income levels, and access to vice needs seemed to be associated with stress.
public health care between families who reported lan- To determine if families with children diagnosed with
guage and behavioral concerns and those who did not, autism spectrum disorder had an effect on the results of the
with those from lower socioeconomic and educational current study, analyses of the responses of parents from this
levels expressing more concern. Particular efforts should group were conducted. There were 45 children in the older
be made to survey families of lower socioeconomic sta- age group who had been diagnosed with autism spectrum
tus to identify children who might benefit from assis- disorder. Of this group, 40 parents reported that their
tance for possible language delays and behavior children had both speech and behavior concerns, 3 had nei-
problems (Nelson et al., 2007). One population to con- ther speech or behavior concerns, 1 had speech concerns
sider for intervention services is children referred to the only, and 1 had behavior concerns only. Overall, the small
child welfare system; yet Stahmer et al. (2005) found sample size limited the ability to assess the associations of
that only 22.7% of children in child welfare who demon- interest in this study for this subgroup.
strated developmental and behavioral needs received
intervention services.
Implications for Intervention
Parents who reported behavioral concerns regarding
their young children were much more likely to express The early identification of language and behavior
high levels of stress and difficulty coping. Despite the problems may need to be addressed by a variety of edu-
evidence that parents can often reliably report concerns cation and health care providers, including family doc-
regarding their children’s development (Glascoe, 1999), tors and pediatricians, preschool and Head Start
professionals have sometimes failed to respond to their teachers, public health nurses, social workers, and child
concerns and have taken a “wait and see” approach. and day care providers, among others. The American
Delays in diagnosis and intervention may be related to Academy of Pediatrics (2006) recommended develop-
poor outcomes later in the children’s development mental surveillance for children at the intervals of 9, 18,

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015


Long et al. / Results From the NSCH 155

24, and 30 months. The families of infants and preschool survey’s limitations should be noted because this analysis
children should be asked about their own specific con- is based on data from the study. Conclusions regarding
cerns regarding their children’s comprehension, verbal causality could not be made, because the data in the sur-
skills, and behavioral control. Parents should rate their vey are cross-sectional. In addition, the survey excluded
own levels of stress so that service providers can deter- data from institutionalized children and children from
mine the urgency and extent of treatment options. There homes without telephones. Although parental observa-
are tools available for rating parenting stress (Abidin, tions from surveys are supported in the literature as a
1990) and child characteristics (Conners, 2002). Health valid method of data collection, there are inherent biases,
care providers and educators will need to be knowledge- such as cultural differences in reporting development and
able regarding community resources that may be able to functional status. The questions used to elicit parental
assist families. These resources include early interven- stress have been shown to produce possible cultural dif-
tion programs, early childhood special education pro- ferences between English-speaking and Spanish-speak-
grams through local public schools, and child and family ing families (Oster et al., 2002). The families were not
counseling services. Families may especially benefit asked questions regarding their children’s participation in
from parent support groups, and young children may early intervention or preschool services or treatment pro-
benefit from classroom or play groups that encourage grams involving speech and language therapy, specialized
social, behavioral, and language development. instruction, or behavior management, so the effectiveness
The treatment of language problems in infants and tod- of intervention programs could not be determined. In
dlers has included an emphasis on teaching caregivers addition, families were not asked to provide specific
strategies in the natural setting of the family (Childress, examples of family function that may have been
2004; Raab & Dunst, 2004). A variety of techniques have adversely affected by increased levels of stress.
been proposed, such as the development of a functional
communication system (Dunlap et al., 2006; World
Health Organization, 2001) and the use of augmentative Conclusions
communication systems, including signs or pictures
(Romski & Sevcik, 2005). It is not within the scope of the The 2003 NSCH provides us with a large sample of
current study to determine the most appropriate means of data on the percentage of families in which parents
increasing communication skills and decreasing problem express concern regarding their children’s acquisition of
behaviors in young children. However, the findings do language skills and their perceptions of behavior prob-
suggest that any intervention program developed to assist lems, with a significant number of parents reporting con-
family function may need to address both the communi- cern about both. Over 50% of parents expressing
cation needs of children and the children’s challenging concerns regarding their children’s speech were also
behaviors (Dunlap et al., 2006). Approaches may need to concerned about their children’s behavior. Parents were
be developmental and behavioral in nature and include especially worried about their own ability to cope with
alternative forms of communication. The approaches the stress of raising children with behavior problems.
should also be sensitive to cultural diversity in the popu- The early identification of parents’ concerns regarding
lation served (Stayton, Miller, & Dinnebell, 2003). their children’s language and behavior will need to take
Intervention may be most effective when a team place in a variety of settings and be conducted by a
approach is used that pairs the skills of a speech and lan- number of professionals from the health care and educa-
guage therapist with those of a behavioral psychologist tion fields. Parents have been shown to be reliable
or a special educator. All personnel providing interven- reporters of concerns regarding language and behavior
tion services should have a strong knowledge base and problems. Parents should be specifically asked about
be able to engage in role release (help others learn one their children’s language skills using straightforward
another’s skills) and role acceptance (Smith et al., 2002). questions, such as those in the survey. Parents should
In addition, services should be family based, with also be questioned regarding their children’s ability to
resources and supports matching the family’s concerns behave and to get along with others. Most important,
and leading to parent competence. parents should be asked about their own levels of stress
as an indicator of the need and model for intervention
services. Parents may need to learn specific strategies,
Limitations of the Study
such as communication treatments paired with behavior
The NSCH is the largest, most comprehensive survey management techniques, to improve their children’s
of the health of children in the United States to date. This overall level of functioning.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015


156 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education

Research suggests that the early detection and treat- Downey, D., Mraz, M., Knott, J., Knutson, C., Holte, L., & Dyke, D. V.
ment of language delays and behavior problems in young (2002). Diagnosis and evaluation of children who are not talking.
Journal of Infants and Young Children, 15(2), 38–48.
children lead to optimal outcomes, and the most impor-
Dunlap, G., Ester, T., Langhans, S., & Fox, L. (2006). Functional
tant identified child outcomes for program accountability communication training with toddlers in home environments.
are based on the acquisition of language and social skills Journal of Early Intervention, 28(2), 81–96.
(Early Childhood Outcomes Center, 2005). The ultimate Drotar, D. (2004). Detecting and managing developmental and
goal in any intervention program should be increased behavioral problems in young children: The potential role of the
family function, which can be rated by reduced parental DSM-PC. Infants and Young Children, 17(2), 114–124.
Early Childhood Outcomes Center. (2005). Overview. Available at
stress and improved coping. When evaluating interven- http://www.fpg.unc.edu/~eco/pages/overview.cfm
tion programs, positive outcomes for families are one of Feldman, M. A., Hancock, C. L., Rielly, N., Minnes, P., & Cairns, C.
the most important factors of quality (Bailey et al., 2006). (2000). Behavior problems in young children with or at risk for
developmental delay. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 9(2),
247–261.
References Glascoe, F. P. (1997). Parents’ evaluation of developmental status: A
method for detecting and addressing developmental and behav-
Abidin, R. R. (1990). Parenting Stress Index Manual (3rd ed.). ioral problems in children. Nashville, TN: Ellsworth &
Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press. Vandemeer Press.
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2006). Identifying infants and Glascoe, F. P. (1999). The value of parents’ concerns to detect and
young children with developmental disorders in the medical address developmental and behavioral problems. Journal of
home: An algorithm for developmental surveillance and screen- Paediatrics and Child Health, 35(1), 1–8.
ing. Pediatrics, 118(1), 405–420. Gertner, B., Rice, M., Hadley, P. (1994). Influence of communicative
Bailey, D. B., Bruder, M. B., Hebbler, K., Carta, J., Defosset, M., competence on peer performance in a preschool class. Journal of
Greenwood, C., et al. (2006). Recommended outcomes for Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 913–923.
families of young children with disabilities. Journal of Early Gresham, F. M., Lane, K. L., & Lambros, K. M. (2000). Comorbidity of
Intervention, 28(4), 227–251. conduct problems and ADHD: Identification of “fledging psy-
Baker, B. L., & Heller, T. L. (1996). Preschool children with exter- chopaths.” Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 8, 83–93.
nalizing behaviors: Experience of fathers and mothers. Journal of Guralnick, M. J. (1987). Dyadic peer interactions of mildly delayed
Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 513–532. and non-handicapped preschool children. American Journal of
Baker, B. L., McIntyre, L. L., Blacher, J. B., Crnic, K., Edelbrock, C., Mental Deficiency, 92, 178–193.
& Low, C. (2003). Preschool children with and without develop- Guralnick, M. J. (1999). Family and child influences on the peer-
mental delay: Behavior problems and parenting stress over time. related social competence of young children with developmental
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 217–230. delays. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities
Blanchard, L., Gurka, M. J., & Blackman, J. A. (2006). Emotional, Research Reviews, 5, 21–29.
developmental, and behavioral health of American children and Halfon, N., Olson, L. M., Inkelas, M., Mistry, R., Sareen, H., Lange,
their families: A report from the 2003 National Survey of Children’s L., et al. (2002). Summary statistics from the National Survey of
Health. Pediatrics, 117(16), e1202–e1212. Early Childhood Health, 2002. Parent concerns, content, and qual-
Blumberg, S., Olson, L., Frankel, M., Osborn, L., Srinath, K. P., & ity of care. Vital Health Statistics, Series 15, No. 3. Available at
Giambo, P. (2005). Design and operation of the National Survey http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/series/sr_15/sr15_003.pdf
of Children’s Health, 2003. Vital Health Statistics, Series 1, No. Hastings, R. P. (2002). Parental stress and behavior problems of
43. Available at http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/series/sr_01/sr01 children with developmental disability. Journal of Intellectual and
_043.pdf Developmental Disability, 27(3), 149–160.
Brier, N. (1995). Predicting antisocial behavior in youngsters dis- Kaiser, A., Hancock, B., Cai, X., Foster, E., & Hester, P. (2000).
playing poor academic achievement: A review of risk factors. Parent-reported behavioral problems and language delays in boys
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 16, and girls enrolled in Head Start classrooms. Behavioral Disorders,
271–276. 26, 26–41.
Campbell, S. B. (1995). Behavior problems in preschool children: A Kazdin, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior in children:
review of recent research. Journal of Child Psychology and Current status and future directions. Psychology Bulletin, 102(2),
Psychiatry, 36(1), 113–149. 187–203.
Childress, D. (2004). Special instruction and natural environments: Neary, E. M., & Eyberg, S. M. (2002). Management of disruptive behav-
Best practices in early intervention. Infants and Young Children, ior in young children. Infants and Young Children, 14(4), 53–67.
17(2), 162–170. Nelson, J. R., Stage, S., Duppong-Hurley, K., Synhorst, L., &
Conners, C. K. (2002). Conners’ Rating Scales–Revised technical Epstein, M. H. (2007). Risk factors predictive of the problem
manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi Health Systems. behavior of children at risk for emotional and behavioral disor-
Crnic, K. A., Friedrich, W. N., & Greenberg, M. T. (1983). The adap- ders. Exceptional Children, 73(3), 367–379.
tation of families of mentally retarded children: A model of stress, Oster, H., Morales, L., Andersen, R., Wold, C. M., Simon, P., &
coping, and family ecology. American Journal of Mental Gelberg, L. (2002, June). The Aggravation in Parenting Scale:
Deficiency, 88, 125–138. Language application issues among Spanish-speaking Latino
Crnic, K. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (1990). Minor parenting stresses families living in Los Angeles County. Paper presented at the Annual
with young children. Child Development, 61, 1628–1637. Research Meeting of the Academy of Health, Washington, DC.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015


Long et al. / Results From the NSCH 157

Powell, D., Dunlap, G., & Fox, L. (2006). Prevention and interven- Smith, B., Strain, P., Snyder, P., Sandall, S., McLean, M., Ramsey, A.,
tion for the challenging behaviors of toddlers and preschoolers. & Sumi, C. (2002). DEC recommended practices: A review of
Infants and Young Children, 19(1), 25–35. 9 years of EI/ECSE research literature. Journal of Early
Qi, C. H., & Kaiser, A. (2004). Problem behaviors of low income Intervention, 25(2), 108–119.
children with language delays: An observational study. Journal of Stahmer, A. C., Leslie, L. K., Hurlburt, M., Barth, R. D., Webb, M. B.,
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 595–609. Landsverk, J., et al. (2005). Developmental and behavioral needs
Qi, C. H., Kaiser, A. P., Milan, S., & Hancock, T. (2006). Language and service for young children in child welfare. Pediatrics,
performance of low-income African American and European 116(4), 891–900.
American preschool children on the PPVT-III. Language, Speech Stayton, V., Miller, P., & Dinnebell, L. (Eds.). (2003). DEC personnel
and Hearing Services in the Schools, 37, 5–16. preparations in early childhood special education: Implementing
Raab, M., & Dunst, C. (2004). Early intervention practitioner the DEC recommended practices. Denver, CO: Sopris West.
approaches to natural environment interventions. Journal of Early Tervo, R. C. (2005). Parents’ reports predict their child’s develop-
Intervention, 27(1), 1–14. mental problems. Clinical Pediatrics, 44(7), 601–611.
Rannard, A., Lyons, C., & Glenn, S. (2005). Parent concerns and pro- Tervo, R. C. (2007). Language proficiency, development, and
fessional responses: The case of specific language impairment. behavioral difficulties in toddlers. Clinical Pediatrics, 46(6),
British Journal of General Practice, 55(518), 710–714. 530–539.
Research Triangle Institute. (2004). SUDAAN language manual, World Health Organization. (2001). Burden of mental and behavioral
Release 9.0. Research Triangle Park, NC: Author. disorders. In The world health report 2001—Mental health: New
Romski, M., & Sevcik, R. (2005). Augmentative communication and understanding, new hope (chap. 2). Available at http://www
early intervention: Myths and realities. Infants and Young .who.int/whr/2001/chapter2/en/
Children, 18(3), 174–185.
Sameroff, A., & Fiese, B. (2000). Transactional regulation: The devel- Carolyn E. Long, MA, MS, is a hospital teacher with the
opmental ecology of early intervention. In J. P. Shonkoff & S. J. Charlottesville Hospital Education Program and works at the Kluge
Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (2nd ed., Children’s Rehabilitation Center and Research Institute at the
pp. 135–159). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. University of Virginia Children’s Hospital.
Scarborough, A., Hebbler, K., & Spiker, D. (2006). Eligibility char-
acteristics of infants and toddlers entering early intervention ser- Matthew J. Gurka, PhD, is an assistant professor in the Division of
vices in the United States. Journal of Policy and Practice in Biostatistics and Epidemiology in the Department of Public Health
Intellectual Disabilities, 3(1), 57–64. Sciences and an assistant professor in the Department of Pediatrics at
Schieve, L. A., Blumberg, S. J., Rice, C., Visser, S. N., & Boyle, C. the University of Virginia School of Medicine.
(2007). The relationship between autism and parenting stress.
Pediatrics, 119(1), 114–121. James A. Blackman, MD, MPH, is a professor of pediatrics and head
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighbor- of the Division of Developmental Pediatrics at the Kluge Children’s
hoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, Rehabilitation Center and Research Institute, University of Virginia
DC: National Academy Press. Children’s Hospital.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Bibliotheek TU Delft on March 20, 2015

You might also like