Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 22

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/235749079

The Evolution of the Wechsler Memory Scale: A Selective Review

Article  in  Applied Neuropsychology · February 2013


DOI: 10.1080/09084282.2012.689267 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

29 5,900

1 author:

Phillip Kent

12 PUBLICATIONS   51 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The clinical use of the Wechsler Memory Scales View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Phillip Kent on 08 January 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Applied Neuropsychology: Adult

ISSN: 2327-9095 (Print) 2327-9109 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hapn21

Evolution of Wechsler’s Memory Scales: Content


and structural analysis

Phillip L. Kent

To cite this article: Phillip L. Kent (2017) Evolution of Wechsler’s Memory Scales:
Content and structural analysis, Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 24:3, 232-251, DOI:
10.1080/23279095.2015.1135798

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2015.1135798

Published online: 18 Apr 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 86

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 1 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hapn21

Download by: [216.175.19.37] Date: 11 May 2017, At: 20:41


APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT
2017, VOL. 24, NO. 3, 232–251
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2015.1135798

Evolution of Wechsler’s Memory Scales: Content and structural analysis


Phillip L. Kent
Private Practice, Bettendorf, Iowa

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
The Wechsler Memory Scale-1 was introduced to the professional community 70 years ago and has Assessment; memory theory;
been the most widely used standardized memory battery for over 50 years. Since its introduction, test development; Wechsler
the test has been revised three times, with the last revision occurring in 2009. Few clinicians are Memory Scale
aware that Wechsler developed a prior memory battery in 1917, which was used to assess retention
deficits in persons with Korsakoff psychosis. The purpose of the present article is to describe the
development of Wechsler’s memory scales from 1917 through 2009. Suggestions for the next
revision are offered.

When in 1907 the American Psychological Association


Introduction
undertook through a committee the standardization Nearly a century ago, Wells (1927) criticized psycho-
of psychological tests, the effort was spent mainly on logical tests for what would be described as their lack
standardization of procedure. … As a result there
of construct validity. He was echoing, to a certain
were produced tests of a fair degree of technical
merit.… … Now the pendulum is at the other extreme. extent, the views of Young (1916), who criticized test
The emphasis is on standardization of results. Less makers that pursued quantitative expression and stan-
attention is often paid to the construction of the test dardization at the expense of content validity. Likewise,
as such. (Wells, 1927) Wechsler (1939) was critical of purely quantitative
approaches to assessment, especially those which failed
Perhaps the chief obstacle to a more rapid analytical
development of tests has been the desire to express
to take into account the person’s life history. He was
results in quantitative terms is order to establish norms one of the earliest fighters in the battle between those
for comparative purposes. To accomplish this aim … who espoused a quantitative as opposed to a qualitative
investigators have been forced to posit and assume that approach to assessment. In discussing the assessment of
the particular test which they were standardizing tested intelligence, he recognized that factors other than
only one quality, as memory, imagination, form percep- intellectual ability, such as drive and incentive, enter
tion, or rate of movement. If, however, the investigator into intelligent behavior. Even a cursory reading of his
was aware that other factors were involved … he failed
seminal book on intelligence (Wechsler, 1939), and
to give them the prominence and emphasis necessary
to prevent others from falling into the error of being his paper introducing the Wechsler Memory Scale
content to assume that only one quality is being (Wechsler, 1945), reveals that Wechsler was well aware
tested … . (Young, 1916) that human behavior has multiple determinants. For
Wechsler, assessment was more than just testing. As
When a life history (assuming it to be accurate) is in we shall see, theory, research and clinical observation
disagreement with the “psychometric,” it is well to
influenced Wechsler when he developed his
pause before attempting a classification on the basis
of tests alone. (Wechsler, 1939) instruments.
Flash forward to the present. One would assume that
The Psychological Assessment Work Group of the all neuropsychological tests these days would be firmly
Competencies Conference identified eight core compe- anchored in an explicit theoretical base, and be
tencies in assessment. … Childs and Eyde (2002) informed by our current understanding of brain-
analyzed program materials and course syllabi from
behavior relationships and evidence-based practice (that
APA-accredited clinical psychology doctoral programs
in relation to many of those competencies. … Sixty is, the best available research evidence bearing on
percent of programs covered validity and reliability. … which tests best identify the specific neuropsychological
(Reported in Ready & Veague, 2014). deficits of various clinical groups). Such is not the case.

CONTACT Phillip L. Kent plkent@mchsi.com 3630 Deer Springs Drive, Bettendorf, IA 62722, USA.
© 2017 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 233

One need look no further than the Wechsler Memory nonetheless invalid for the intended clinical use
Scale-4 (WMS-4; 2009), which was published in 2009. (Sternberg, 1995). This trend in training, that is, the lack
It is not anchored in an explicit theory of memory of universal training in basic psychometric theory, is
(Kent, 2013), and the technical manual provides a alarming, and does not bode well for the future of
relative paucity of information regarding how the test psychological or neuropsychological assessment.
performs with various clinical groups. How is this poss- By the time the original Wechsler Memory Scale
ible, with all the push for theory- and evidence-based (WMS) was introduced in 1945, it had been in clinical
practice in the past 20 years? use since 1940 (Wechsler, 1945). By 1946, it was already
All current training models in doctoral clinical among the top 90 most frequently used tests (Louttit &
psychology programs recognize the need for training in Browne, 1947). By 1969, the WMS was the nineteenth
science; the models differ primarily in how much empha- most used test and the most used memory battery
sis is given to training in research and the scientific foun- (Lubin, Wallis, & Paine, 1971), and by 1994, the
dations of psychology. At present, the dominant training Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (WMS-R) was the
model in clinical psychology, the Practitioner-Scholar ninth most used test by clinical psychologists, and the
model, primarily emphasizes preparation for a career third most used test by neuropsychologists who conduc-
in clinical practice (Murray, 2000). An unintended ted assessments five or more hours per week (Camara,
consequence of this model may be clinicians who are Nathan, & Puente, 2000). By 2011, it was among the
not well-grounded in basic psychometric theory, and 10 most popular psychological tests taught in clinical
who are thus unlikely to be able to intelligently read, psychology programs and the most frequently taught
understand, and critique test manuals. memory battery (Ready & Veague, 2014). Suffice to
With the shift in emphasis on how most clinical psy- say, the WMS has “stood the test of time,” despite
chologists are trained, there is evidence that interest in ongoing concerns about its structure, lack of anchoring
the history of psychology, and by extension, the history in an explicit theory of memory, and a variety of other
of ideas, is on the decline. The only doctoral program in concerns. Because the WMS has been in clinical use for
the history and theory of psychology in the United over 75 years, it is doubtful the average clinician is
States ended in 2009. Concerns have been expressed aware of its historical roots, the theoretical rationale
that we may be witnessing a decline in the number of for its original content, and how it has evolved over
psychology departments teaching courses in the history time.
of psychology. Despite the decline in interest in the his- As far as the present writer could determine, this is
tory of psychology, all APA-accredited programs the first paper to trace the development of Wechsler’s
require coursework in history and systems of psy- memory scales from 1917 through 2009. Since this
chology. By implication, it is implied that sound clinical article focusses on Wechsler’s thinking about how to
practice dictates that practitioners have a working assess memory, special attention will be paid to the
understanding of the theoretical foundations of the 1917, 1945, and 1987 scales, since these were the ones
instruments they use and the domains they assess. For Wechsler directly developed (Wechsler, 1917, 1945,
instance, if a clinician is using a test of memory, the 1987). In 1946, a second version (the WMS-2) of the test
clinician should be conversant with the theoretical foun- was published (Stone, Girdner, & Albrecht, 1946). It is
dations of the test, which components of memory are unclear if Wechsler was an author of the WMS-2, since
purportedly being assessed, and why. he was not listed as an author in the original paper
To understand test construction, one has to be fam- describing the scale, although his name appears along
iliar with the concepts of reliability and validity, and with Stone’s on the test manual. Since it is unclear if
how they are operationalized and measured. According Wechsler was an author of the WMS-2, the content of
to a recent study by Childs and Eyde (2002) of APA- the scale will not be discussed.
accredited clinical psychology programs, only 60% of The Wechsler Memory Scale-3 (WMS-3) and
the programs surveyed covered validity and reliability Wechsler Memory Scale-4 (WMS-4) list Wechsler as
when teaching psychological assessment. It appears that author, although they were developed long after his death
a significant number of psychologists practicing today by a team of psychologists connected with Pearson
are uninformed about these critical concepts, even Assessment (formerly The Psychological Corporation).
though they are considered part of the core competen- Although the WMS-3 and WMS-4 were not developed
cies in assessment. As a result of their lack of training, by Wechsler, their content will be discussed to illustrate
they may fail to appreciate the complex relationship how the WMS has evolved over time, and to identify
between a test’s reliability and validity, and fail to grasp the problems that have plagued each revision. Due to
that even though a test may be perfectly reliable, it may space constraints, the WMS-3-Abbreiated Form and the
234 P. L. KENT

WMS-4 Older Adult Battery will only be briefly patients with Korsakoff psychosis. His study was notable
mentioned. because he identified a key deficit defining the condition
In terms of method of investigation, the present article (the inability to form new associations), and because he
will review primary source material for Wechsler’s (1917, devised a verbal memory scale which was a precursor to
1945, 1987) scales, and selected publications by Wechsler the WMS-1. His test battery was designed to assess if
relating to his conceptualizations of memory. In regard defective retention in Korsakoff psychosis was due to:
to the WMS-3 and WMS-4, the analysis is based pri- 1) impaired registration; 2) defective encoding or “a
marily on a review of each scale’s administrative and failure to link new associations with old”; and 3) a
technical manuals, and a clinical guide to the use of the fading of associations, or forgetting and retention prob-
WMS-4. This paper will address the adequacy of assess- lems. For Wechsler, retention was basically synonymous
ment of memory, as exemplified by the Wechsler mem- with memory. He defined retention as the ability to
ory scales, in light of what is considered good clinical reproduce new impressions after a given interval of
practice. The terms test, scale, and battery will be used time. As part of the literature review to justify his
interchangeably. The term WMS will be used generically method of study, Wechsler cited the works of Ebbin-
to refer to the scales bearing Wechsler’s name, with 1, R ghaus (1885), Brodmann (1902), and other famous
(for revised), 3, and 4 referring to each version. Since memory researchers and clinical investigators. His test
other writers have discussed the limitations of the was thus firmly anchored in the experimental and clini-
Wechsler scales in terms of reliability, validity, and stan- cal research of the day, as indicated by the structure and
dardization, these issues will not be discussed in depth. content of his battery. As the reader is unlikely to be
Since there is no current gold standard on how to familiar with Wechsler’s first memory battery, it will
assess memory, the following criteria will be used to be described in some detail. The battery consisted of
assess the adequacy of each of memory scales discussed the following measures.
in this article. As the emphasis of the present paper is on
the clinical assessment of memory, it is assumed the
Screening
characteristics of an adequate assessment procedure
should include the following. Prior to formal memory A Questionnaire was used to assess personal and
testing, a screening should be completed to ensure the current information and orientation.
subject has adequate sensory and motor functioning,
is not aphasic or psychotic, and is sufficiently motivated
Registration
to be tested (Lezak, Howieson, Bigler, & Tranel, 2012).
As part of the screening procedure, attention should Auditory Memory Span for Digits consisted of digit
be assessed using any of the major models of attention, span forwards, beginning with a 4 digit series until 2
such as Mirsky’s (Koziol, Joyce, & Wurglitz, 2014), to consecutive misses occurred; three lists were presented;
identify any deficits that might affect the subject’s mem- memory span determined by averaging scores across
ory functioning. After the screening is completed, it is three trials.
assumed that the clinician will primarily assess episodic Auditory Memory Span for Unrelated Words was
declarative or explicit memory. assessed by measuring the subject’s recall of two lists
Registration or short term and working memory of concrete words. The lists ranged in length from 3
should be evaluated in verbal and nonverbal modalities to 8 words. (It should be noted that Wechsler actually
to determine the memory span of the individual, and stated the word lists ran from 4 to 8 words in his paper,
the capacity for the temporary storage and manipulation and the maximum score was 66. For the maximum
of information (Baddeley, Allen, & Hitch, 2011). Encod- score to be 66, the word lists had to run from 3 to 8
ing (short term learning and recall) should be assessed words). Prior to being read the word list, the subject
through the immediate recall of verbally and visually pre- was told how many words were in each list and that
sented meaningful material (Lezak et al., 2012). Retention he or she was expected to repeat the words in the same
of learned material should be assessed using measures of order as presented by the examiner. One point was
delayed recall of verbally and visually presented material awarded for each word recalled and one point was
via free recall and recognition paradigms. awarded if the word was recalled in correct order. The
subject’s score was based on an average of the two lists.
The word lists were obtained from Pyle (1913).
The 1917 scale
Visual Memory Span for Movement was assessed by
In 1917, David Wechsler published a paper based on his administration of the Knox Cube Test and the Modified
Master’s thesis which investigated retention in 5 Knox Cube Test. In the latter, the examiner touched
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 235

four or five cubes in a specific order and the patient was was tested at 15″, 30″, 1′, and 3′; 12 observations were
asked to reproduce the specific sequence. made for all intervals except 1’, for which 6 were made.
Method of Retained Members consisted of the repro-
duction of two lists of 10 unrelated (examples: red, dog, Discussion
buy) and two lists of 10 related (examples: river, water, Wechsler’s battery did not formally screen for aphasia,
brook) word lists longer than memory span. The word or other conditions that would render his subjects
lists were obtained from Norsworthy (1906). Each word untestable, because the “screening” had been completed
list was read to the subject within one minute, and the by the physicians who diagnosed and referred the
subject was expected to repeat the word lists. This patients to Wechsler for his study. Since Wechsler was
subtest was similar in nature to a paired associates test, conducting a research study, it is understandable
without the associates. he did not complete a formal screening procedure.
Wechsler did, however, use a questionnaire to collect
information regarding each subject’s personal and cur-
Encoding
rent information, and orientation. This information
Formation of Preformed Associates was a paired associ- was qualitative, and presented in his clinical descrip-
ate learning task. The subject was verbally presented a tions of the six patients (five with Korsakoff psychosis
list of 10 word pairs, which were considered easy associ- and one with general paralysis (presumably general
ates, until the subject linked all the associates correctly paresis) who participated in his study.
or until 20 trials had elapsed. If subject supplied an In regard to assessing registration, Wechsler’s (1917)
incorrect associate, the examiner corrected the subject. test was unusual in that it used several measures (recal-
Two sets of word pairs were used. ling digits, unrelated words, and movements) to
Formation of New Associations was also a paired accomplish this goal. Even though he noted that all tests
associated learning task and consisted of the learning were auditorily presented, the Visual Span for Movement
of new paired associates, such as crush-dark and appears to have assessed visual working memory. It is
school-grocery. The lists of new paired associates were unclear from his paper why he used the Method of
taken from an unpublished work of R. S. Woodworth Retained Members, which consisted of assessing the sub-
with some modifications. Wechsler used a list of 10 ject’s recall of word lists longer than their memory span.
pairs of new or hard associates and a 5-pair list. The Wechsler spends very little time describing the procedure
subject was administered the aforementioned word lists. in his article, but does make a reference to Whipple
(1915) who describes this method as a measure of rote
memory. Whipple (1915) states that the length of series
Retrieval
is chosen so the subject cannot attain complete mastery,
Curve of forgetting for intervals of 15”, 30”, 60”, 1’, 3’, and also notes that many span tests “ … actually become
and 10’ tests of the degree of mastery” (p. 151). By this, he pre-
Assessing retrieval was accomplished through three sumably means rote learning. The measures Wechsler
procedures. (1) Recall of a series of digits after a single chose to assess registration would currently be con-
repetition: The digit series administered was unknown, sidered measures of short term and working memory.
but described as one digit shorter than the subject’s In regard to the Method of Paired Associates, it is sig-
memory span. The subject had to repeat them immedi- nificant to note that he included “preformed associates”
ately after intervals ranging from 15″–10′ 12 observa- and “new associations.” Preformed associates contained
tions were made for intervals 15″–3′ and 6 word pairs which “ … stood in some simple logical
observations were made for 10′; efforts were made by relations and had been strongly established through
examiner to prevent subject from rehearsing digits. (2) frequency of previous association, e.g., come-go,
Recall of colors after a single presentation: The colors lead-pencil, metal-iron … ” (Wechsler, 1917, p. 420).
used for this procedure are unknown, except for They are considered easy associates, and presently would
color-blind subjects, where the colors red, green, blue, be considered a measure of semantic memory. New
yellow were used. A color was shown and the subject associations (i.e., associates) were unrelated word pairs.
was asked to name it. The method of presentation for Unrelated word pairs were used to assess if his subjects
colors was similar to digits. (3) Recall of single unrelated could form “new” associations, which was felt to be the
associates after one repetition: The associates used are essence of encoding. New associates were considered
unknown; the purpose of the procedure was to deter- hard associates. Wechsler wanted to test the hypothesis
mine how fast a connection was lost between two unre- that people with Korsakoff’s might evince a difference
lated associates established by a single repetition. Recall in their ability to learn both types of word pairs, and
236 P. L. KENT

indeed this was the case. He found that his subjects’ (i.e., The Wechsler Memory Scale-1
persons with Korsakoff psychosis) performance on
Approximately 30 years after his first memory battery,
preformed associates was essentially normal, and their
Wechsler (1945) introduced the Wechsler Memory
performance on new associates impaired. It is significant
Scale-1 (WMS-1) to the professional community. He
to note that the reasoning for his inclusion of both word
had released the Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale
pair types, which were also used in the WMS-1, was not
(WBIS) in 1939. During the time Wechsler was working
included in the 1945 test manual. The purpose for
on the WBIS, he was developing the WMS-1. Wechsler
including both paired associate types appears to have
states he worked intermittently on the scale for 10 years,
gone largely unnoticed in the clinical literature on the
which suggests he began work on the scale in about
WMS. The paired associates test was administered up
1935. According to Wechsler, he wanted a scale which
to 20 trials or until the subject was able to recall all the
was “rapid, simple, and practical.” The manual for the
word pairs correctly, so it was in essence a paired associ-
WMS-1 runs a scant nine pages, and no data are pre-
ate learning test. Paired associate learning procedures
sented about its reliability or validity. It should be
were assumed to be an index of a person’s ability to learn
remembered, however, that the test was developed dur-
new information. Thorndike (1908) noted that quick
ing World War II, and was intended for the assessment
learners are good retainers, and Wechsler cited
of soldiers and sailors with head injuries.
Thorndike in his study.
It is important to emphasize that Wechsler was a
As noted, Wechsler assessed retrieval/forgetting by
pragmatist when it came to test selection. Pragmatism,
examining the recall of digits, colors, and single associ-
along with theory, guided his selection of WBIS and
ates after one repetition (1917). Wechsler was strongly
WMS-1 subtests. He wanted tests which were clinically
influenced by the work of Ebbinghaus (1885) in his
useful, easy to administer, and which related to real
effort to systematically assess retrieval/forgetting. He
world behaviors. He was well aware of the need for tests
used multiple learning trials and time intervals up to
to have ecological validity.
10 minutes to assess this process. Wechsler explicitly
In the introduction to the manual, Wechsler refers to
designed his 1917 test to be a verbal memory battery,
the Memory Examination described in Wells and
since previous research had suggested that patients with
Martin (1923) and Wells (1927), which he considered
Korsakoff’s may have deficits in “the range of visual
unsuitable for clinical work, although he adopted the
perception” (Wechsler, 1917, p. 417).
term mental quotient (MQ) for his scale from Wells.
He described the mental quotient as being analogous
Interim summary
to the intelligence quotient.
Wechsler’s first memory battery was remarkably exten-
Although Wechsler was developing the WMS-1
sive. Based on the criterion for a sound memory exam
along with the WBIS, there is no mention of the former
discussed earlier, he used multiple measures (verbal
in his book describing the development of the WBIS,
and nonverbal) to assess registration or short term
although he does reference his 1917 scale (Wechsler,
and working memory, including a test to assess memory
1939, p. 68). Wechsler presented the following table in
for “words longer than memory span.” He used easy
his book, which was based on a similar table by Wells
and novel paired associate tasks to tease out the exact
(1927). Table 1 presents Wechsler’s review of the litera-
nature of encoding deficits, although he did not assess
ture summarizing how various diagnostic groups had
the delayed recall of visually presented material except
historically performed on some standard psychological
colors. He systematically investigated forgetting/
tests. As we will see, he obviously used the table to guide
retrieval across intervals ranging from 15 seconds to
his subtest selection, although this is not explicitly
10 minutes, although he did not utilize recognition
mentioned in his 1945 manual.
techniques. Nowhere in Wechsler’s paper is there a ref-
The WMS-I was comprised of seven subtests. They
erence to his theory of memory, if he had one. Despite
will be briefly presented.
this, his work was clearly anchored in the experimental
and clinical research of the day. His descriptions of
Screening
patients indicate that he differentiated between memory
for old events and recent events, and he was aware that Personal and current information
even though he used a test called Visual Memory Span This subtest was derived from a standard mental status
for Movement, because it was auditorily presented, it exam; six items are similar in nature to those used in
did not primarily assess visual memory span. His the Wells and Martin (1923) Memory Examination.
research was influenced by the associationist school of Wechsler included this subtest to identify people with
memory, as described by James (1890). suspected aphasia and dementia.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 237

Table 1. Intellectual disabilities in different mental disorders as indicated by low score performance on different tests.
Schizophrenia General Paresis Organic brain syndrome Korsakoff’s Senile dementia
Vocabulary – – * – X
Current information – – X X X
Sentence repetition – X * – X
Paragraph recall X X X X XX
Digits forward – X XX X X
Digits backward – X XX XX XX
Easy associates – X X * X
Hard associates X X XX XX XX
Counting backwards * X XX * X
Block design * X XX X X
Design reproduction * X XX XX XX
Gestalt test (1) * X X X XX
Note. Revised and Adapted from Wechsler, 1939, p. 68. – ¼ No significant decline; X ¼ Poor XX ¼ Markedly poor. *Often poor, but sometimes unaffected (1):
Gestalt test ¼ Bender Gestalt Test.

Orientation Associate learning


This subtest was derived from a standard mental status This subtest consists of 10 paired associates (six were
exam; five questions were asked to assess the subject’s easy associates, four were hard associates). Subjects are
orientation to place and time. given three learning and recall trials. The word pairs
were derived from Wechsler’s (1917) scale.

Registration
Discussion
Mental control Wechsler was aware of the need to screen patients for
This subtest consisted of three items: counting suspected aphasia, dementia and disorientation. He
backwards from 20 to 1, counting by threes beginning included questions designed to identify these conditions
with 1 up to 40, and reciting the alphabet. in the Personal and Current Information and
Orientation subtests.
Memory span Wechsler assessed registration through two subtests.
This subtest was the same subtest used in the WBIS with Mental Control is considered a measure of working
the exception that the maximum number of digits was memory. Wechsler (1945) considered it to be harder
limited to 8 forward and 7 backwards. It is similar than Memory Span and used it to assess “cases of
to the Auditory Memory Span for Digits used in organic brain disease that are not too far gone but show
Wechsler’s (1917) scale. defects which would not be made evident by simple rote
memory items” (pp. 87–88). In his 1939 book, Wechsler
noted that although digit span had long been used by
Encoding psychiatrists as a measure of retentiveness, or the ability
to learn new information, he cited research that
Visual reproduction indicated digit span backwards problems correlated
This subtest was a measure of visual working memory, with difficulties with attention and concentration. He
although it was verbally and visually presented. The also noted that a modest digit span forward capacity
content of the subtest was derived from the Binet scale was associated with people with limited intelligence
and the Army Performance Test. The subject was and certain organic conditions. Wechsler deliberately
presented three simple geometric designs, one at a time, used the combined the scores of digit span forward (rote
for 10 seconds. After the design was removed, the memory) and backward (attention) to minimize the
subject was asked to draw the design from memory. amount each contributed to the total score. Nonetheless,
This test assesses visual short term memory as well as it appears he included both digit span forwards and
motor functioning. backwards so clinicians could use these scores to assess
different parameters of functioning, although this is not
Logical memory clearly stated in his 1945 manual. Wechsler likely
This subtest consisted of the immediate recall of assumed that users of his scale were familiar with the
two verbally presented paragraphs consisting of mean- WBIS and had read his book describing the develop-
ingful material. The paragraphs were modifications of ment of the test, since it was expected that each patient
Passages 1 and 5 of the Army Performance Scale had to be administered the WBIS so IQ-MQ compari-
(Government Printing Office, 1921). sons could be made.
238 P. L. KENT

Visual Reproduction, in assessing the immediate administered, compared to three for the WMS-1. The
recall of simple geometric designs after exposure of 10 1917 also did a good job of assessing retention over time
seconds, appears to be a measure of the ability to encode periods up to 10 minutes. The 1917 scale was more
and recall visually presented material. Logical Memory explicitly linked to the experimental and clinical mem-
assessed the encoding and recall of two verbally pre- ory assessment literature than the WMS-1. Neither test
sented paragraphs containing meaningful information. was anchored in an explicit theory of memory. The
The use of “logical memory” (or paragraph recall) tech- WMS-1 did not present any data about how the test per-
niques dated back to the nineteenth century. As noted, formed with various clinical groups, like people with
the Associate Learning subtest (previously called the dementia, head injuries, and so forth. The WMS-1
Formation of Preformed and New Associates), was was designed to be a rapid assessment device to be used
derived from Wechsler’s (1917) scale. The purpose of in clinical, as opposed to research settings, and it
the subtest was to assess how quickly the subject could achieved that goal. It was developed at the same time
learn new information. as the WBIS, and overlapped in content, to some degree,
Scores from the seven subtests were added up to with Wechsler’s (1917) scale. Wechsler designed the test
obtain a raw score, to which a constant was added based as a screening instrument, and its content was dictated
on the subject’s age group. This weighted score was then by what was known about the cognitive deficits of
converted to a MQ. Since the WMS-1 MQ had been people with various neurocognitive disorders including
standardized against the Full Scale IQ of the WBIS, it schizophrenia, organic brain syndromes, Korsakoff’s
was assumed that MQs significantly lower than IQs due to alcoholism, general paresis, and dementia in per-
indicated memory dysfunction, an assumption that sons 65 or older. With the WMS-1, Wechsler evaluated
has been questioned in the literature (see, for instance, registration or short term and working memory using
Dennis et al., 2009). It is noteworthy that Wechsler verbally and visually presented information. Encoding
never explicitly stated how large the IQ-MQ disparity was assessed using Visual Reproduction, Logical Mem-
had to be to be clinically significant, although it prob- ory, and Paired Associates. His test was notably
ably was 1 standard deviation. As has been noted deficient in not formally evaluating long term retrieval.
elsewhere, the WMS-1 has been extensively criticized The WMS-1 was primarily a measure of short term
for lack of information regarding its reliability and val- verbal memory.
idity (Hall & Toal, 1957), limited standardization
(Lezak, 1976), and a variety of other issues.
The Wechsler Memory Scale-revised
The astute reader will note a striking correspondence
between Table 1 and the content of the WMS-1. It is The WMS-R was published in 1987, six years after
clear Wechsler chose subtests because the research Wechsler died, and 42 years after the publication
and clinical literature suggested various clinical groups of the original scale. According to the test manual,
had defects in those domains. This use of the research Wechsler was listed as author of the test because he
and clinical literature was a procedure which would was extensively involved in its revision. He began work
currently be called an evidence-based practice. on the revision in the late 1970s, and was responsible for
In comparison to the 1917 scale, the two overlapped the major changes in the instrument (Wechsler, 1987).
in formally assessing auditory digit span and paired By the time the revision was published, virtually every
associate learning. Both contained screening ques- aspect of the original scale had received numerous
tions. Each assessed the domains of registration and and scathing criticisms. For example, the WMS-1 was
encoding. criticized for lacking information about its reliability
Factor analytic studies of the WMS-1 revealed it and validity (Cohen, 1950; Hall & Toal, 1957), standar-
assessed 2 factors: memory and attention and concen- dization issues involving provisional and limited norms
tration (Kear-Colwell, 1973). It was primarily a measure (Prigatano, 1978), scoring issues (Mack, 1986), and use
of short term, episodic declarative verbal memory. of a Memory Quotient (Lawrence, 1984). The scale was
also criticized for its lack of theoretical adequacy, and as
Interim summary it aged, its failure to reflect advances in the state of
In many ways, Wechsler’s (1917) scale was superior to knowledge about memory functioning and neuropsy-
the WMS-1. Compared to the WMS-1, the 1917 scale chological functioning. Lezak (1976), in discussing
did a more thorough job of assessing short term mem- verbal memory and learning tests in general, wrote: ” …
ory. In regard to encoding, although both batteries used the examiner’s choice of memory tests must depend
the paired associates procedure, the 1917 scale consisted more on his clinical judgment than on scientific demon-
of many more learning trials. Up to 20 trials were stration that this or that test is most suitable for
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 239

answering the question under concern” (p. 356). forward and backward series that were one digit shorter
Clearly, she was not talking just about the WMS-1. than WMS-1 version. The subtest assessed attention and
By the time the revision was published, it was esti- verbal short term and working memory.
mated that the number of revisions or variations of
the original scale exceeded 50 (Kent, 1992). The most Visual memory span
popular revision was that by Russell (1975), which con- This test was intended to be a visual analogue of Digit
sisted of immediate and delayed recall of the Logical Span. The subject was asked to touch a series of colored
Memory and Visual Reproduction subtests. Despite squares in a predetermined order, which had first been
numerous criticisms in the professional literature, the demonstrated by examiner. The test purportedly
original scale is still occasionally used for clinical and measured visual short term memory.
research purposes (for example, see Ardila, 1999).
Between the original scale and its first official revision,
Encoding
there had been explosion of knowledge in the fields of
neurology, biology, and neuropsychology. Clinical neu- Logical memory 1
ropsychology had come of age, and major findings This subtest consisted of the immediate recall of two
emerged about our understanding of memory, partly verbally presented paragraphs of meaningful material.
due to the pioneering investigations of Scoville and One paragraph was derived from the WMS-1; the other
Milner (1957) and their study of H. M., and the work was a new story.
Penfield and Jaspers (1954) on brain mapping. By the
early 1980s, the neuroanatomical substrates of memory Verbal paired associates 1
were being identified (Luria, 1976; Squire, 1982). The subtest contained the same paired associates
Responding to many criticisms of the WMS-1, the (Associate Learning) used in the WMS-1, with the
WMS-R was designed to assess memory for verbal exception that eight rather than 10 pairs were adminis-
and figural stimuli, meaningful and abstract material, tered. The eight pairs consisted of four easy and four
and delayed as well as immediate recall (Wechsler, hard associates. Two easy word pairs were eliminated
1987). The WMS-R contained a series of brief subtests to shorten the test. The examinee was given up to six
with each purportedly designed to measure a different trials to learn the pairs to the criterion of one perfect
facet of memory. The subtests are presented in the repetition. Only the first three trials were scored.
following sections.
Figural memory
The subject was shown a set of abstract designs, and
Screening
after each set was removed, the subject was asked to
Information and orientation questions identify the designs within a group of larger designs.
This test was used for screening purposes and was not The subtest appeared to assess learning and recall of
included in scoring. It contained questions about bio- visually presented material, via a recognition paradigm.
graphical data, orientation, and information from In constructing this test, it was assumed that the
long-term memory. It included seven slightly modified abstract stimuli would be difficult to verbally encode.
questions from the WMS-1, and another seven ques-
tions assessing orientation, hand preference, and Visual paired associates-1
sensory problems. This subtest was developed as a visual analog to Verbal
Paired Associates. The subject was required to learn the
color associated with each of six abstract line drawings.
Registration
To minimize the role of verbal mediation in memoriza-
Mental control tion and responding, the color names were not used in
This subtest involved counting backwards from 20 to 1, presenting the items or responding to them. Up to six
saying the alphabet, and counting by 3’s from 1 to 40. It learning trials were presented until the subjects
used the same items as WMS-1, and was designed to answered all six items correctly, although only the first
assess overlearned material. It also assessed verbal three presentations were scored.
working memory.
Visual reproduction 1
Digit span This subtest was similar to the Visual Reproduction
This subtest was similar to tasks on the original scale, subtest from the WMS-1. The subject was asked to
except subjects begin repeating a series of digits of immediately draw simple geometric designs following
240 P. L. KENT

a 10 second exposure. One item from the WMS- scale Due to the addition of some new subtests, clinicians
was dropped, and two new added. The scoring was could now compute index scores for verbal memory,
revised from the WMS-1. visual memory, attention and concentration, delayed
recall. A general memory score, similar to a memory
quotient, could also be computed, but it was only based
Retention
on measures of immediate recall, a detail unlikely
Logical memory 2 appreciated by many test users.
This subtest consisted of a delayed-recall trial 30 minutes In terms of content, the Information and Orientation
after Logical Memory I was administered. It assessed the Questions were used for screening purposes, and no
delayed recall of the encoding of two paragraphs of longer entered into scoring, and the content of the subt-
verbally meaningful material. est was revised to be more clinical useful. In regard to
registration, the Mental Control and Digit Span subtests
Verbal paired associates 2 were carried over from the WMS-1 in revised form, and
This subtest consisted of a delayed-recall trial 30 minutes an attempt was made to measure non-verbal memory
after Verbal Paired Associates I was administered. It span in the form of the Visual Memory Span subtest.
assessed the delayed recall of a person’s ability to encode Unfortunately, the new subtest had a significant verbal
and retain easy and hard associates. component, although factor analysis indicated it loaded
on the Attention and Concentration factor (Wechsler,
Visual paired associates 2 1987, p. 75), helping to provide validation for that index
This subtest consisted of a delayed-recall trial 30 minutes score. Assessing registration was significantly improved
after Visual Paired Associates 1 was administered. It compared to the previous scale.
assessed the delayed recall of the encoding of visually In regard to encoding, the test now included
presented abstract drawings. purported measures of verbal and nonverbal memory
functioning. Figural Memory and Visual Paired Associ-
Visual reproduction 2 ates 1 joined the Visual Reproduction 1, Logical
This subtest consisted of a delayed-recall trial 30 minutes Memory 1, and Verbal Paired Associates 1 subtests. It
after Visual Reproduction I was administered. It is unlikely many users understood why Wechsler
assessed the delayed recall of the encoding of simple included both easy and hard associates in the paired
geometric drawings and motor ability. associate learning subtest. As noted, easy associates were
considered easy because they assessed what would pre-
Discussion sently be called semantic memory (or what some might
In general, the WMS-R was a significant improvement call crystallized intelligence); hard associates were felt to
over the WMS-1. No longer was the WMS merely a assess the essence of encoding, or new learning.
measure of short term verbal memory. The directions While the WMS-1 assessed immediate verbal mem-
for administration and scoring were significantly ory, the WMS-R was biased, on the surface, toward
improved. The WMS-1 had been normed on “approxi- assessing immediate visual memory. Unfortunately,
mately” 200 males and females aged 25–50, to which a factor analytic studies discussed in the manual state
correction was added to extend its use to age 64. indicated that the WMS-R consisted of only two pri-
Remarkably, although the WMS-R was normed on mary factors: a general memory and learning factor,
16–74 year olds, it used extrapolated norms for some and an attention-concentration factor. Although the
age groups, and was based on a sample of only 316 factor analysis provided support for the use of the
people. The small norming sample was surprising, in General Memory Index, it did not support the assertion
light of the resources of the Psychological Corporation, of the author that the test measured memory for figural
the amount of time that had occurred until the first stimuli, meaningful and abstract material, and delayed
revision, and the popularity of the scale. As we have recall. A later study by Franzen and Iverson (2000)
seen, the content was updated, an attempt to measure suggested the WMS-R assessed three factors: learning
non-verbal memory was made, and measures of delayed and immediate recall of verbal and visual information,
recall were added. Although the manual presented data attention and concentration, and orientation and the
on WMS-R scores with selected clinical groups, the ability to recall information from one’s personal history.
group sizes were small and samples of convenience, Once again, a delayed recall factor did not emerge.
thereby limiting the clinical usefulness of the infor- Retention was assessed through the 30 minute
mation. This was unfortunate, since the manifest delayed recall of the Verbal Paired Associates 2, Logical
content of the scale had been significantly improved. Memory 2, Visual Reproduction 2, and Visual Paired
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 241

Associates 2 subtests. Each delayed-recall subtest loaded reportedly cut the administration time to 20–25 minutes.
on the same factor with its immediate recall counterpart The abbreviated form will not be discussed further.
(Wechsler, 1987, p. 76). Thus, although the WMS-R was While scoring the WMS-R was relatively easy, brief
a significant improvement over the WMS-1 in terms of and straight forward, scoring of the WMS-3 was cum-
manifest content and structure, the attempt to measure bersome and time consuming and yielded a multitude
delayed recall was not realized. of scores of questionable clinical significance (Kent,
2013). If the WAIS-3 or Wechsler Individual Achieve-
Interim summary ment Test (WIAT) was also administered, as was often
On the surface, in terms of content and structure, the the case, the clinician could make a wide range of com-
WMS-R was a significant improvement over the parisons between WAIS-3 and WIAT and WMS-3
WMS. The scoring and manifest content of the scale scores. As the number of comparisons increased, so
were improved. Purported measures of delayed recall did the likelihood of findings of statistically significant
were added. It was able to be used with a wider age differences. Again, the assumption was that if there
group (16–74 vs. 20–64 year olds). It was normed on was a significant discrepancy between performance on
a larger sample of individuals (316 versus approximately the WAIS-3 and performance on the WMS-3 (that is,
200). As we have seen, however, the WMS-R suffered the latter being statistically significantly lower), this sug-
from a substandard norming procedure, as the standar- gested likely memory dysfunction. Researchers have
dization sample was pitifully small. In addition, the strongly criticized the practice of making assumptions
manual lacked good quality information about its utility about discrepancies in functioning on IQ and tests of
and validity with various clinical populations, and factor memory (see, for example, Ardila, 1999).
analytic study reported in the manual did not support For the first time, the publisher split the technical
that it assessed delayed memory. In light of the afore- manual (Psychological Corporation, 1997) apart from
mentioned, it is perhaps not unexpected that a new the administrative and scoring manual (Wechsler,
revision would arrive on the scene a mere decade later. 1997). Few clinicians who purchased the WMS-3 prior
The first revision missed its mark. to 2002 are probably aware that the technical manual
was significantly updated in 2002 (Psychological
Corporation, 2002). In the first technical manual, the
Wechsler Memory Scale-3
publisher asserted that a confirmatory factor analysis
The WMS-III was published in 1997. The instrument was yielded three factors: (1) immediate and delayed audi-
developed by a panel of experts employed by or consult- tory memory; (2) immediate and delayed visual memory;
ing with The Psychological Corporation. According to the and (3) working memory. This conclusion was retracted
administration manual, the revision was based on an in the updated manual, which stated that factor analysis
extensive literature review and expert opinion (Wechsler, had failed to confirm the immediate and delayed distinc-
1997). As can be seen from Table 2, there were 8 new tion. A confirmatory factor analysis by Millis, Malina,
subtests (Faces 1 and 2, Family Pictures 1 and 2, Word Bowers, and Ricker (1999) revealed that the WMS-3
List 1 and 2, Letter Numbering Sequence, Spatial Span), yielded the following three factors: (1) auditory memory,
two of which were optional (Word Lists 1 and 2). In (2) visual memory, and (3) working memory.
addition, the Mental Control and Visual Reproduction 1 The WMS-3 contained the following subtests.
and 2 subtests, which had been part of the standard
WMS-R battery, were now optional. Three subtests new
Screening
to the WMS-R were eliminated (Visual Memory Span,
Visual Paired Associates, and Figural Memory). Information and orientation (Optional)
Needless to say, the WMS-3 was a major revision. It
This subtest was similar to the previous version with the
used a substantially larger normative sample based on
exception that one question was reworded. It was
the 1995 census (see Table 3). Like its predecessor, the
scored, but the score did not contribute to any index
new revision was responding to criticism of deficient
or composite.
norming procedures and concerns about content. The
new revision was co-normed with the Wechsler Adult
Intelligence Scale-3. The test was designed to yield so Registration
many scores that computer scoring was recommended.
Letter number sequencing
Possibly in response to anticipated criticism of the time
it took to administer and score the instrument, an This was a new subtest. A series of letters and numbers
abbreviated form of the instrument was published which were verbally presented, and the subject was asked to
242
Table 2. Wechsler’s memory scales across time.
1917 Scale WMS-1 WMS-R WMS-III WMS-IV-AB
Screening Questionnaire for personal and Personal & Current Information Information & Orientation Questions Information and Orientation-(O) Brief Cognitive Status Exam
current information, and Orientation (includes Mental Control) (O)
orientation
Registration Auditory memory span for digits Digit Span Digit Span Digit Span (O) Spatial Addition
Auditory memory span for Mental Control Mental Control Mental Control (O) Symbol Span
unrelated words Visual Memory Span Letter-Number Sequencing
Visual memory span for Spatial Span
movements
Method of retained members
Encoding Formation of preformed & new Associate Learning Verbal Paired Associates 1 Verbal Paired Associates 1 Verbal Paired Associates 1
associations Logical Memory Logical Memory 1 Logical Memory 1 Logical Memory 1
Visual Reproduction Visual Reproduction 1 Visual Reproduction 1 (O) Visual Reproduction 1
Visual Paired Associates 1 Faces I Designs 1
Figural Memory Family Picture 1
Word List 1(O)
Retention Curve of forgetting for intervals Verbal Paired Associates 2 Verbal Paired Associates 2 Verbal Paired Associates 2
using digits, colors, associations Logical Memory 2 Logical Memory 2 Logical Memory 2
Visual Reproduction 2 Visual Reproduction 2 (O) Visual Reproduction 2
Visual Paired Associates 2 Faces 2 Designs 2
Family Pictures 2
Word List 2(O)
Note. Core subtests in bold; optional subtests identified by (O); new subtests italicized.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 243

Table 3. WMS age ranges, number of subtests, and norming procedures.


Time between Testing Time: Minutes
immediate and (does not include
Age range # Subtests delayed subtests scoring time Norming
1917 Scale Not applicable 7 Not applicable 30 Not normed
WMS-1 (1945) 20–64 7 Not applicable 15 “approximately” 200 normal males
and females aged 25–50; used
extrapolated norms to extend age
ranges; Memory Quotient scores
derived from comparison with
WBIS
WMS-R (1987) 16–74 13* 30 minutes 45–60 316 people based on 1980 Census;
norms for 18–19 years old, 25–34
years old, and 45–54 year old were
estimated; approximately equal
numbers of males and females;
subjects selected based on race
(White vs. Nonwhite), geographic
region, education, IQ based on
WAIS-R
WMS-3 (1997) 16–89 10 primary 25–35 minutes 100** 1,032 based on 1995 Census;
7 optional weighting procedures used to
extend sample to 1,250; subjects
selected based on age, sex, race/
ethnicity, educational level, and
geographical region; co-normed
with WAIS-3
WMS-4 (2009) 16–90 12 primary 1 20–30 minutes 65–80**** 1,400; co-normed with WAIS-4;
optional*** representative sample based on
2005 Census; co-normed with
WAIS-4
*The WMS-R Information and Orientation Questions not scored.
**Administration of complete WMS-3 battery.
***The WMS-4 Brief Cognitive Stats Exam scored, but results not used to calculate any index score; also, the CVLT-II could be administered to obtain the
Auditory Memory Index and supplement the Immediate Memory Index and Delayed Memory Index in the WMS-4 Adult Battery and Older Adult Battery.
****Time to administer including the 20–30 minutes needed between immediate and delayed subtests.

repeat back the series, stating the numbers first (in order a measure of attention and short term verbal memory
from lowest to highest), and then the letters in order. span; Digit Span Backwards was considered a measure
The sequences ranged from 2 to 8 items, and three trials of working memory.
were presented for each series. The test was discontin-
ued after the subject failed all three trials of an item. Mental control (Optional)
This subtest measured verbal working memory.
This subtest contained eight items like counting back-
ward from 20 to 1, saying the alphabet, and saying the
Spatial span days of the weeks backward. This subtest was expanded
considerably from the WMS-R. It measured verbal
This was a new subtest. The subject was asked to exactly
working memory.
reproduce a series of tapping sequences on an array of
blocks which were first demonstrated by the examiner.
The series ranged from 2 to 9 blocks for the forward Encoding
and backward sequences. Two trials were administered Logical memory 1
for each series. The test was discontinued when the sub- This subtest consisted of the immediate recall of two
ject missed both trials of spatial span forward and spa- verbally presented paragraphs of meaningful material.
tial span backwards. Spatial span was a measure of Story A from the WMS-R was retained, with slight
visual working memory and procedural learning. rewording, and a new paragraph was devised which
was more neutral in emotional content than its prede-
cessor. There were slight modifications made to the
Digit span (Optional)
scoring procedure.
This subtest is identical to the WAIS-3 version, and was
slightly modified from the WMS-R. Subjects were asked Faces 1
to repeat a series of digits ranging from 2 to 9 forward, This was a new subtest. Each subject was presented a
and 2–8 backwards. Digit Span Forward was considered series of 24 faces to remember. The subject was then
244 P. L. KENT

presented a series of 48 pictures which contained the 24 Retention


pictures the subject was asked to remember, and 24
Logical memory 2
foils. The subject was to identify the faces he or she
This subtest consisted of the delayed recall of the encod-
was asked to remember. This task involved the learning
ing of two verbally presented paragraphs of meaningful
and recall of visually presented faces using a recognition
material. It was administered 25–30 minutes after
format.
Logical Memory 1. Recognition memory was assessed
through asking the subject a series of yes and no ques-
Verbal paired associates 1 tions about each paragraph.
This subtest contained eight new word pairs of hard
associates. The subject was read a total of eight word Faces 2
pairs, one word pair every three seconds. During the A total of 25–35 minutes after the administration of
recall phase, the subject is read the first word of each Faces I, the subject was presented 48 pictures, 24 of
pair, and asked to provide the correct associate. If the which were the original faces to be remembered. The
correct associate was not provided in five seconds, the subject was asked to identify the 24 faces he or she
correct response was provided. Four learning trials were was asked to remember for Faces 1. This task involved
given. This subtest was a verbally presented associative the delayed recall of the encoding of visually presented
learning task. faces using a recognition format.

Family pictures 1 Verbal paired associates 2


This was a new subtest. The subject was shown a series After 25–30 minutes, the subject was presented the first
of four drawing of family scenes, one at a time, for 10 word of each word pair used in Verbal Paired Associates
seconds each, and told to study the picture and remem- 1, and asked to provide the correct associate. A recog-
ber as much as possible about the scene. After all four nition test was then presented: 24 word pairs were read;
pictures were viewed, they were removed and the sub- the subject was then asked to answer yes or no if the
ject was asked to recall who was in each picture, where presented word pair was one of the original word pairs
they were in the picture, and what they were doing. The used in Verbal Paired Associates 1. This subtest mea-
test purportedly measured the immediate recall of visu- sured the delayed recall of the encoding of verbally pre-
ally presented, complex meaningful information, and sented paired associates via a free recall and recognition
spatial memory. This test was designed to be a visual format.
analog of the Logical Memory subtest.
Family pictures 2
Word lists 1 (Optional) A total of 25–35 minutes after administration of Family
This was a new subtest. It consisted of a list learning Pictures 1, the subject was asked to recall as many pic-
task similar in nature to the California Verbal Learning tures as possible, who was in the picture, and where they
Test. The subject was read List A, which consists of 12 were located. This task was a delayed recall measure of
unrelated words and asked to recall as many words as the encoding of visually presented complex meaningful
possible. Four learning trials were presented for each information and spatial memory.
trial. The subject was then read another word list (List
B) which consisted of 12, new unrelated words and Word lists 2 (Optional)
asked to recall as many words as possible. The subject A total of 25–35 minutes after the administration of
was then asked to recall as many words from List A Word List 1, the subject was asked to recall as many
as he or she could. This test measured the recall of words as she or he can. This task measured the delayed
a word list, after an interference task. It assessed the recall of the encoding of a verbally presented word list.
temporal recall of verbally presented information. A recognition trial was then administered: 24 words
were read, which contained the 12 words presented in
Visual reproduction 1 (Optional) Word List 1, and 12 new words. The subject was asked
Similar to previous versions, the subject was asked to to answer yes or no if the word was part of the original
draw a visually presented design from memory. The list.
subtest was expanded to five designs, with several
designs retained from the WMS-R. This subtest pur- Visual reproduction 2 (Optional)
portedly assessed visual short term memory of simple Administered 25–35 minutes after Visual Reproduction
geometric designs. 1, the subject was asked to draw the five designs from
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 245

memory. The subtest presumably measured the delayed Table 4. Selected factor analytic studies of the Wechsler
recall of the encoding of visually presented information. Memory Scales.
The subtest also contained a 48-item recognition task, Battery Factor analysis results
1917 Scale Not completed
a copy task to screen for motor dysfunction, and a WMS-1 2 factors: memory; attention/concentration (Kear-Colwell,
seven-item discrimination task. 1973)
WMS-R 3 factors: learning & immediate recall of verbal and visual
material; attention & concentration; orientation & ability
Discussion to recall information from one’s personal history (Franzen
As can be seen from the aforementioned information, & Iverson, 2000)
WMS-3 3 factors: auditory memory; visual memory; working
the WMS-3 was a significant revision of the WMS. memory (Millis et al., 1999; Psychological Corporation,
Although the administrative and scoring manual stated 2002)
WMS-4 3 factors: auditory memory; visual memory; visual working
it had six primary and five optional subtests, it actually memory (Holdnack & Drozdick, 2009); 2 factors: auditory
had 12 primary subtests assuming the delayed versions learning/memory & visual attention/memory (Hoelzle et
al., 2011)
of the primary subtests were administered. Screening
was accomplished through the Information and
Orientation subtest, which unfortunately, was now an (about 100 minutes if the entire battery were given),
optional procedure. The subtest could be scored. and the amount of time it took to score the test if a
In regard to registration, the Digit Span and Mental computer based scoring program was not used.
Control subtests were now optional; they were standard
procedures in the WMS-R. Working memory was now Interim summary
primarily assessed via Letter Number Sequencing, a Although improved on the surface, the WMS-3 was still
measure of verbal working memory, and Spatial Span, much the same test it had always been if one looked
presumably a measure of visual working memory. strictly at factor analytic studies of the WMS-1, WMS-R,
Encoding was assessed through the administration of and the WMS-3 (see Table 4). On the surface, like its
Logical Memory 1 and Verbal Paired Associates 1, as in predecessors, it screened for sensory dysfunction and
the WMS-R. In addition, two new primary measures disorientation, which could invalidate the test results.
were introduced: Faces 1 and Family Pictures 1. Visual It evaluated registration and encoding, and attempted
Reproduction 1 was now an optional subtest, and the to assess retention, although it failed in this regard, such
WMS-3 introduced another optional new subtest called as the WMS-R, using supporting factor analysis studies
Word List 1, which was similar to the California Verbal as a criterion of success. The WMS-3 introduced a
Learning Test. number of new subtests, none of which were retained
Retention was assessed through Logical Memory 2, in the WMS-4. Like its two predecessors, Verbal Paired
Faces 2, Family Pictures 2, and Verbal Paired Associates Associates, and Logical Memory were part of the core
2. If administered, retention could also be assessed battery. Mental Control, Visual Reproduction, and Digit
through Word Lists 2 and Visual Reproduction 2. Span were also retained, but were now optional proce-
On the surface, the content of the WMS 3 met the dures (see Table 3). An abbreviated version of the scale
criteria established earlier for an adequate clinical was marketed, which significantly reduced the adminis-
assessment of memory, assuming one administered tration time, but its use limited the amount of clinically
the Information and Orientation subtest. The problem useful information available to the examiner. As noted,
is, what the test purported to measure, and actually the technical manual was split off from the administrat-
measured, did not correspond. As noted, like the ive and scoring manual. A myriad of new scores of
WMS-R, factor analysis revealed that the WMS-3 did questionable clinical utility could be derived and com-
not assess delayed recall (see Table 4), although it did parisons made to scores from the WAIS-3 and WIAT.
assess auditory, visual, and working memory. Scoring the WMS-3 almost mandated the use of a com-
Although the publisher acknowledged that the puter-based scoring system. The length of time it took
WMS-3 did not assess delayed recall, it nonetheless to administer and score the test decreased its clinical
recommended the continued use of the immediate usefulness. In light of the aforementioned findings, it
and delayed index scores “on the basis of both clinical is perhaps not surprising that the WMS-4 appeared 12
and theoretical considerations” (Psychological Corpor- years later.
ation, 2002, p. 102). This recommendation was remark-
able for a field which had been urging its practitioners
WMS-4
to engage in evidence based practice for at least two dec-
ades. Two other major problems with the WMS-3 were Like the WMS-3, the WMS-4 was revised based on
the amount of time it took to administer the battery multiple concerns about its predecessor. In brief, the
246 P. L. KENT

WMS-3 was criticized as being too long, especially when Immediate Memory, and Delayed Memory). Unfortu-
testing seniors; lacking factor analysis support, nately, factor analytic studies have once again not
especially for the visual memory index; difficulty with supported separate immediate and delayed index scores.
subtest administration and scoring; and the question- Some factor analytic studies have indicated the scale has
able content of several new subtests (Drozdick, either a 3 factor structure consisting of: (1) auditory
Holdnack, & Hilsabeck, 2011). In addition, there were memory, (2) visual memory, and (3) visual working
concerns about the reliability and the validity of some memory (Holdnack & Drozdick, 2009); or a two factor
subtests (Pearson, 2015). Like its predecessor, the structure consisting of: (1) auditory learning/memory,
WMS-4 was revised using a team of assessment experts and (2) visual attention/memory (Hoelzle, Nelson, &
associated with Pearson (Wechsler, 2009). Reviews of Smith, 2011).
the psychometric literature on shortcomings of the prior The repeated failure of the WMS to assess delayed
instrument were completed, customer service data recall has been noted by several researchers. According
examined, and a panel of researchers and experts on to Hoelzle et al. (2011), this failure is due to a significant
memory were consulted to shape the new instrument. shared variance between immediate and delayed mem-
Compared to the WMS-3, the WMS-4 technical ory tasks, and is consistent with psychometric theories
manual contains more information regarding the scale’s of cognitive ability based on factor analytic studies like
linkages to the burgeoning literature on memory assess- that conducted by Carroll (1993). It should be noted
ment and advances in our knowledge of the neurologi- that other popular tests of memory which purport to
cal underpinnings of memory functioning. The authors assess delayed recall also suffer from this problem. For
of the scale clearly attempted to link theory with prac- instance, studies of the California Verbal Learning
tice, although the new scale, like its predecessors, was Test-2 yields six factors, none of which is delayed recall.
not based on a clearly articulated theory of memory. One of the factors, general verbal learning, is composed
A training video on how to administer the test was of several measures of level of immediate and delayed
developed, and supplemented by webinars on the test recall and recognition (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen,
development and administration. Scoring of some 2006). In a recent study of immediate and delayed
subtests was revised. memory for verbal and visuospatial information involv-
The new scale includes an Adult Battery for persons ing over 100,000 verbal and visuospatial immediate and
19–69 and an Older Adult Battery for persons 65–90. A delayed tests presented via the Internet to over 28,000
person who was 65 can be administered either battery, participants ranging in age from 11 to 80, factor analysis
although the latter is shorter because it contains fewer yielded an immediate but not a delayed dimension
subtests (Designs 1 and 2, Spatial Span, and Symbol (Murre, Janssen, Rouw, & Meeter, 2013). Findings such
Span are not administered). In addition, several alterna- as these along with research involving brain imaging
tive batteries have been devised for special evaluations. have questioned the relationship between immediate
Tablet based administration is available. Computerized and delayed recall, and whether it is possible to assess
scoring has been enhanced. For the first time, the delayed recall, at least as it has been conceptualized.
California Verbal Learning Test 2 scores can be used The WMS-4, Adult Battery contains the following
in place of scores from the Verbal Paired Associates 1 subtests. The procedures described in the following
and 2. section were adapted from descriptions by Drozdick
The manifest structure of the WMS changed once et al. (2011).
again (see Table 2). Three new subtests were added
(Spatial Addition, Symbol Span, Designs 1 and 2), three
were revised (Logical Memory, Verbal Paired Associ- Screening
ates, and Visual Reproduction), and eight subtests were
Brief cognitive status exam (Optional)
eliminated (Information and Orientation, Spatial Span,
Mental Control [it was rolled into an optional Brief This subtest was derived, in part, from the Information
Cognitive Status Exam], Digit Span, Letter Number and Orientation and Mental Control subtests of the
Sequencing, Family Pictures, Word List, and Visual WMS-3. It was significantly revised and renamed, and
Memory Span). For the first time, verbal working includes measures of orientation, time estimation, men-
memory is not assessed; instead, only visual working tal control, clock drawing, incidental recall, inhibition,
memory is assessed through the Spatial Addition and and verbal production. The test results are quantifiable,
Symbol Span subtests. and norms are available based on one’s age and level of
The WMS-4 has five index scores (Auditory education. The subtest is the same for the Adult and
Memory, Visual Memory, Visual Working Memory, Older Adult Batteries.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 247

Registration Designs 1
Spatial addition For this subtest, the subject is shown a grid with four
designs in the grid cells for 10 seconds. A blank grid
This subtest is a measure of visual and spatial working
and eight cards are then given to the subject, and the
memory. It is a subtest based on the n-back
subject is expected to select the correct cards and to
procedure. In brief, the subject is shown two grids,
place them on the blank grid in the same array as
each for five seconds. Each grid contains a red and/
demonstrated earlier. This procedure is repeated four
or blue circles and the subject is told to pay attention
times with four new grid patterns. This test assesses
to where the blue circles are located across the grids,
the encoding and immediate retrieval of visual details
while ignoring the red circles. The subject is asked to
and visual-spatial locations of geometric designs. For
mentally combine the blue circles across the grids.
detailed instructions on test administration, please
The subject is expected to reproduce the combined
consult the test manual.
grids by placing a blue card on the appropriate cell
on the record form.
Retention

Symbol span Logical memory 2

This subtest is another measure of visual working mem- Administered 20–30 minutes after Logical Memory 1,
ory and the mental manipulation of visual details. The this subtest assesses the delayed encoding and recall of
subject is shown a series of designs for five seconds two verbally presented meaningful paragraphs.
and asked to recall and select the correct designs in
sequence from a larger array of designs. Verbal paired associates 2
Administered 20–30 minutes after Verbal Paired
Encoding Associates 1, a delayed recall measure is given of the
14 word pairs. A recognition test is then administered
Logical memory 1 in which the subject is asked to correctly identify the
This subtest consists of two verbally presented stories. 14 word pairs out of a list of 40 word pairs. This test
After the presentation of each story, the subject is assesses the delayed encoding and recall of semanti-
instructed to retell the story, starting at the beginning. cally-related and semantically-unrelated word pairs
This subtest assesses the encoding and immediate recall using free recall and recognition procedures.
of two verbally presented meaningful paragraphs.
Visual reproduction 2

Verbal paired associates 1 Administered 20–30 minutes after Visual Reproduction


1, this subtest assesses the delayed encoding and recall
The subject is administered a list 14 paired associates, of simple geometric designs. It also has a recognition
four of which are easy, and 10 of which are hard. The procedure and optional copy procedure.
word pairs are administered in varying order. The sub-
ject is then given the first word of each word pair, and is
expected to provide the correct associate. Four word Designs 2
pair trials are given. This is a classic associative learning This test is administered 20–30 minutes after immediate
task of verbally presented word pairs, and assesses the recall, and contains a recognition trial. The subject is
encoding and immediate recall of semantically-related asked to recreate the four designs he or she was shown
and unrelated word pairs. during Designs 1. This measure assesses the delayed
encoding and recall of visual details and spatial loca-
tions of geometric designs.
Visual reproduction 1
The subject is presented five designs, one at a time, and
Discussion
asked to draw each design immediately after its presen-
tation. Each item is presented for 10 seconds. This subt- This discussion will concentrate primarily on the con-
est assesses the encoding and immediate recall of simple tent of the Adult Battery, listed in the previous section.
geometric designs. In regard to screening, the Brief Cognitive Status Exam
248 P. L. KENT

remains an optional procedure, and it has been con- interactive fashion. The comparability of tablet admin-
siderably enhanced compared to the WMS-3 version. istration compared to traditional paper and pencil
The results are quantifiable and provide some infor- administration is open to debate. To the best of this
mation about the subject’s functioning. As noted, norms writer’s knowledge, most all of the factor analysis stu-
are available to analyze the subtest scores based on the dies of the WMS-4 to date have been based on the tra-
subject’s age and level of education. ditional administration method. Tablet administration
Registration is assessed through two new subtests changes the stimulus value of the test, and likely what
(Spatial Addition and Spatial Span). No attempt is made it measures. Like its immediate predecessor, the WMS-
to measure verbal working memory. It is significant to 4 allows the clinician to calculate a myriad of scaled,
note that if a poorly trained examiner used only the index, process scores. In a popular guide on how to
WMS-4 to assess working memory, information critical administer and interpret the test, Drozdick et al.
to many individuals’ functioning might be missed. This (2011) list 56 possible scores! The actual clinical use-
situation could occur, for instance, for Social Security fulness of these scores to the average, busy clinician
disability examinations when an applicant alleges mem- in these days of managed care is questionable.
ory problems. In the region where the present writer
practiced, sometimes these examinations are completed
Conclusions and recommendations
by a sub-doctoral school psychologist under the purview
of a doctoral level psychologist. It has been said that the more things change, the more
Encoding is now assessed through four subtests, two they stay the same. In some ways, this aphorism applies
assessing auditory memory (Logical Memory 1 and to the content of the WMS. Looking only at the results
Verbal Paired Associates 1), and two assessing visual of the cited factor analysis studies, it is significant to
memory (Designs 1 and Visual Reproduction 1). If the note that after three decades of trying, the WMS still
California Verbal Learning Test 2 is administered before does not measure delayed recall. Although this finding
or after the WMS-4, then scores from that instrument is well known to researchers in the field, it is not known
can be substituted for scores from the Verbal Paired just how well this finding has been disseminated to the
Associates 1 and 2. clinical community, since even a cursory examination of
Retention is assessed after a period of 20–30 minutes psychological and neuropsychological reports reveals
through administration of the delayed versions of the clinicians talk about immediate and delayed recall when
subtests administered during the encoding phase. As reporting WMS findings. In fairness, the WMS is not
noted earlier, the WMS-4 assesses retention through a the only test of memory which suffers this shortcoming.
combination of recall and recognition procedures. As noted, even such popular tests as the California
Recall and recognition procedures are used for Logical Verbal Learning Test-II suffer from this shortcoming.
Memory 2, Verbal Paired Associates 2, Designs 2, and In light of the failure of the WMS and other tests of
Visual Reproduction 2 subtests. In addition, Verbal memory to assess delayed recall (Carroll, 1993; Murre
Paired Associates 2 has an optional word recall pro- et al., 2013), as validated by factor analytic studies, it
cedure, where the subject is asked to recall words from has been argued that the time has come to abandon this
the word pair list without the constraint of correct word measure until such time it can be reliably demonstrated
pairing, and Visual Reproduction 2 has an optional through carefully controlled research studies.
copy procedure wherein the subject is shown the One problem in abandoning the purported measure-
original stimuli and is asked to replicate the design ment of delayed memory is that regardless of its validity
while it is displayed (Drozdick et al., 2011). based on the results of factor analytic studies, several
purported measures of delayed recall are useful for dis-
tinguishing various clinical groups. For example, Lezak
Interim summary
et al. (2012) note that diminished WMS-3 Verbal Paired
In some ways, the WMS-4 is the most radical of all the Associates II scores from the WMS-3 statistically dis-
revisions. Lezak et al. (2012) consider it a whole new tinguished between patients with temporal love epilepsy
test, raising the issue to what extent validity studies who had left or right seizure foci, and Alzheimer’s
of previous versions apply to the present test. It no patients characteristically perform poorly on Logical
longer measures verbal working memory. Although it Memory II. Since the WMS has been in use for over
can be administered in significantly less time than 75 years, there has been a large amount of research on
the WMS-3, but still takes longer to administer and how various scales and indices correlate with various
score than the WMS-1 and WMS-R. For the first time, clinical groups, such as those with Alzheimer’s, Hun-
the test can be administered by a tablet in an tington’s, epilepsy, and other types of brain impairment.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 249

Pearson Assessments has advocated for the contin- added. The addition and elimination of so many subt-
ued use of the immediate and delayed indexes, after ests suggest that these measures were not properly
acknowledging that factor analytic studies have failed vetted during the standardization process. If history
to validate these indexes, apparently for the aforemen- repeats, it is anticipated that several of the new subtests
tioned reason: that despite the failure of factor analytic will not survive to the next revision.
studies to validate the immediate and delayed indexes, The subtests which have survived nearly all iterations
research has demonstrated the clinical utility of the of the WMS are: Logical Memory, Verbal Paired Associ-
indexes. ates, Digit Span, Visual Reproduction, Mental Control,
Delayed recall measures, as currently clinically con- Personal and Current Information. These are also the
ceptualized, are more or less synonymous with long subtests that appear to have been most widely studied
term or permanent memory storage (Baddeley, Eysenck, across time, since they are repeatedly referenced by such
& Anderson, 2009). According to Lezak et al. (2012), authorities as Lezak in all editions of her classic text-
consolidation can occur immediately, or take a con- book of neuropsychology. They are the essence of the
siderable amount of time. Consolidation is a process. WMS, despite the fact that the most recent revision
The authors describe a type of short term memory that dropped the verbal Digit Span subtest. Nearly all factor
can be distinguished from immediate memory in that it analytic studies of the four versions of the WMS have
lasts from an hour or so to one or two days, longer than identified core factors relating to attention and concen-
a reverberating circuit could be maintained by active tration and working memory, and learning and
rehearsal. They describe this process as an intermediate memory.
step, and it has also been described as an intermediate Recommendation 2: Based on the above analysis, it is
phase (Tribhowan, Rucker, & McDiarmid, 1971), or recommended that the “core” of the next revision
intermediate memory (Rucker, 1972). It appears that include the aforementioned subtests, including Digit
when tests purport to assess delayed recall, they actually Span. Along with this, it is suggested the test publisher
are assessing the intermediate step or intermediate provide extensive documentation regarding the known
memory, since most measures of delayed recall are correlates between the aforementioned subtests and
customarily assessed 25–35 minutes after tests of various clinical groups to assist future clinicians and
immediate recall have been administered. Recommen- researchers.
dation 1: Based on the aforementioned analysis, it is As noted, the amount of time it takes to administer
recommended that the delayed recall index be renamed and score the WMS has been of concern to many clin-
the intermediate recall (or memory) index. icians, since in the last two revisions numerous new
On the surface, the WMS has been measuring the subtests have been added and scoring has become
right things since its beginning, with the exception of increasingly complex. Concern has expressed regarding
the WMS-1 that did not assess delayed recall. All ver- the large number of questionable scores that can be
sions have recognized the need for screening the patient derived from the WMS-3 and 4.
for motor and sensory competence, language problems, Recommendation 3: Based on the aforementioned
and attention and orientation. All versions have assessed information, it is recommended that the test battery
what we presently would call short term and working be shortened, if possible, to be more patient and clin-
memory. With the exception of the WMS-4, all versions ician friendly. Ideally, testing, interpretation, and scor-
assessed verbal working memory through digit span, the ing should take no more than an hour and one half.
ability to encode and recall verbally presented logically In addition, interpretation should be made more
related material through Logical Memory, and the ability explicit and straightforward.
to form new associations through Verbal Paired Associ- Repeated concerns have been expressed that none of
ates. With the exception of Digit Span, all subtests which the versions of the WMS has been anchored in an
were introduced in the WMS-1 have remained part of explicit neuropsychological memory theory or model,
the core battery, although they have vacillated from consistent with evidence-based practice.
revision to revision as to whether they are optional. Recommendation 4: It is therefore recommended
Although all core subtests have undergone changes in that the next revision be anchored in an explicit neurop-
their content, scoring, and administration over time, sychological theory or model of memory.
the essence of what Wechsler was trying to measure with As noted, there has been ongoing concern about how
his original battery (and to some extent, with his 1917 little useful information the WMS technical manuals
battery) remains remarkably unchanged. have presented about the test’s use with various clinical
In looking at the last two revisions, it is remarkable groups. The data which have been presented are
how many new subtests have been eliminated and frequently based on heterogeneous clinical groups of
250 P. L. KENT

small size, and which are often samples of convenience. Baddeley, A. D., Eysenck, M., & Anderson, M. (2009).
In light of the popularity of the WMS, the resources Memory. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
available to the publisher, and the amount of research Brodmann, K. (1902). Experimenteller und klinischer beitrag
zur Psychopathologie der Polyneuritischen Psychose.
which has been generated on various subtests from Journal für Psychologie und Neurologie, 1, 225–246.
the WMS since 1945, this shortcoming should be Camara, W. J., Nathan, J. S., & Puente, A. E. (2000).
addressed in the next revision. Psychological test usage: Implications in professional psy-
Recommendation 5: It is recommended that the tech- chology. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
nical manual for the next revision contain extensive use- 31, 141–154. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.31.2.141
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor
ful information on how pertinent, well-defined, clinical
analytical studies. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
groups of adequate size (N ¼ 50 or more) perform on Childs, R. A., & Eyde, L. D. (2002). Assessment training in
the new instrument. clinical psychology doctoral programs: What should we
The importance of assessing prospective memory is teach? What do we teach? Journal of Personality Assess-
increasingly being recognized as an essential component ment, 78, 130–144. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa7801_08
of an adequate evaluation of seniors who have suspected Cohen, J. (1950). Wechsler memory scale performance of
psychoneurotic, organic, and schizophrenic groups. Journal
cognitive impairment, and people suspected of suffering of Consulting Psychology, 14, 371–375. doi:10.1037/
from frontal lobe damage (Lezak et al., 2012). h0062273
Recommendation 6: It is recommended the next ver- Dennis, M., Francis, D., Cirano, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes,
sion of the WMS contain well-validated measures of M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate
prospective memory, since this capacity is essential to in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders.
Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 15,
the adaptive functioning.
331–343. doi:10.1017/s1355617709090481
The last revision of the WMS does not have an alter- Drozdick, L. W., Holdnack, J. A., & Hilsabeck, R. C. (2011).
nate form, which would be useful for test-re-testing Essentials of WMS-IV assessment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
purposes. Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A contribution to experi-
Recommendation 7: It is recommended the next mental psychology. New York, NY: Dover.
revision of the WMS include a well-validated alternate Franzen, M. D., & Iverson, G. L. (2000). The Wechsler mem-
ory scales. In Gary Groth-Marmat (Ed.), Neuropsychologi-
form. cal assessment in clinical practice: A guide to interpretation
The importance of assessing effort and validity is and integration. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
increasingly being recognized as a necessary standard Government Printing Office. (1921). Memoirs of the national
component for all neuropsychological evaluations academy of sciences. Washington, DC: Author.
(Larrabee, 2015). Hall, J., & Toal, R. (1957). Reliability (internal consistency) of
the Wechsler memory scale and correlation with the
Recommendation 8: It is recommended that the next
Wechsler-Bellevue intelligence scale. Journal of Consulting
revision of the WMS contain well-validated measures of Psychology, 21, 131–135. doi:10.1037/h0039695
effort and test validity. Hoelzle, J. B., Nelson, N. W., & Smith, C. A. (2011). Compari-
In summary, the WMS has a long history and has son of Wechsler memory scale-fourth edition (WMS-IV)
been in continuous use since 1945. It has been revised and third edition (WMS-III) dimensional structure:
three times in roughly the past three decades. As a result Improved ability to evaluate auditory and visual constructs.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33,
of its widespread use in research and clinical domains, a 283–291. doi:10.1080/13803395.2010.511603
massive amount of information regarding the scale has Holdnack, J. A., & Drozdick, L. W. (2009). WMS-IV technical
accumulated. Nearly all revisions of the WMS have and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
suffered from the same aforementioned problems. It is James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY:
likely a new revision of the instrument will appear in Henry Holt.
Kear-Colwell, J. J. (1973). The structure of the Wechsler mem-
the next five years. It is hoped that the next revision will
ory scale and its relationship to “brain damage.” British
not continue to suffer the shortcomings of those that Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 399–405.
have come before it. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1973.tb00085.x
Kent, P. L. (1992). The theoretical adequacy of Wechsler’s theory
of memory and memory scales (Unpublished doctoral disser-
References tation). Fielding Graduate University, Santa Barbara, CA.
Kent, P. L. (2013). The evolution of the Wechsler memory
Ardila, A. (1999). A neuropsychological approach to scale: A selective review. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult,
intelligence. Neuropsychology Review, 9, 117–136. 20, 277–291. doi:10.1080/09084282.2012.689267
Baddeley, A. D., Allen, R. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2011). Binding in Koziol, L. F., Joyce, A. W., & Wurglitz, G. (2014). The neurop-
visual working memory: The role of the episodic buffer. sychology of attention: Revisiting the “Mirsky Model.”
Neuropsychologia, 49, 1393–1400. doi:10.1016/j. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 3, 297–307. doi:10.1080/
neuropsychologia.2010.12.042 21622965.2013.870016
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT 251

Larrabee, G. J. (2015). The multiple validities of neuropsycho- Ready, R. E., & Veague, H. B. (2014). Training in psychologi-
logical assessment. American Psychologist, 70, 779–788. cal assessment: Current practices of clinical psychology
doi:10.1037/a0039835 programs. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
Lawrence, C. (1984). Testing for memory disorder. Australian 45, 278–282. doi:10.1037/a0037439
and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 18, 207–210. Rucker, W. B. (1972). Personal communication.
doi:10.3109/00048678409161292 Russell, E. W. (1975). A multiple scoring method for the
Lezak, M. D. (1976). Neuropsychological assessment. assessment of complex memory functions. Journal of
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 800–809.
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Bigler, E. D., & Tranel, D. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.43.6.800
(2012). Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.). New York, Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory
NY: Oxford University Press. after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neuropsy-
Louttit, C. M., & Browne, C. G. (1947). The use of psycho- chiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 20, 11–21.
metric instruments in psychological clinics. Journal of Squire, L. R. (1982). The neuropsychology of memory. Annual
Consulting Psychology, 11, 49–54. doi:10.1037/h0056136 Review of Neuroscience, 5, 241–273.
Lubin, B., Wallis, R. R., & Paine, C. (1971). Patterns of Sternberg, R. (1995). In search of the human mind. Fort
psychological test usage in the United States: 1935–1969. Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.
Professional Psychology, 2, 70–74. doi:10.1037/h0031544 Stone, C. P., Girdner, J., & Albrecht, R. (1946). An alternate
Luria, A. R. (1976). The neuropsychology of memory. form of the Wechsler memory scale. Journal of Psychology,
Washington, DC: V. H. Winston and Sons. 22, 199–206. doi:10.1080/00223980.1946.9917307
Mack, J. (1986). Clinical assessment of disorders of attention Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium
and memory. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and com-
1, 22–33. doi:10.1097/00001199-198609000-00006 mentary (3rd. Ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Millis, S. R., Malina, A., Bowers, D. A., & Ricker, J. H. (1999). Thorndike, E. L. (1908). Memory for paired associates.
Confirmatory factor analysis of the Wechsler memory Psychological Review, 15, 122–138. doi:10.1037/h0073570
scale-III. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy- Tribhowan, T., Rucker, W. B., & McDiarmid, C. G. (1971).
chology, 21, 87–93. doi:10.1076/jcen.21.1.87.937 Demonstration of a Kamin-like effect after appetitive train-
Murray, B. (2000). The degree that almost wasn’t: The PsyD ing. Psychonomic Science, 23, 41–42. doi:10.3758/bf03335995
comes of age. Monitor on Psychology, 31, 52–54. Wechsler, D. (1917). A study of retention in Korsakoff psy-
doi:10.1037/e380182004-030 chosis. Psychiatric Bulletin of the New York State Hospitals,
Murre, J. M., Janssen, S. M., Rouw, R., & Meeter, M. (2013). 11, 403–451.
The rise and fall of immediate and delayed memory for ver- Wechsler, D. (1939). The measurement of adult intelligence.
bal and visuospatial information from late childhood to late Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
adulthood. Acta Psychologia, 142(1), 96–107. doi:10.1016/j. Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale for clinical
actpsy.2012.10.005 use. Journal of Psychology, 19, 87–95. doi:10.1080/
Norsworthy, N. (1906). The psychology of mentally deficient 00223980.1945.9917223
children. Archives of Psychology, 1, 1–111. Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale-revised manual.
Pearson. (2015). WMS-III to WMS-IV: Rationale for change. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
San Antonio, TX: Author. Images.pearsonclinical.com/.../ Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler memory scale-third edition
WMS-IV/WMS-III-RationaleforChange.pdf administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX:
Penfield, W., & Jaspers, H. (1954). Epilepsy and functional Pearson Assessments.
anatomy of the human brain. Boston, MA: Little, Brown. Wechsler, D. (2009). The Wechsler memory scale-fourth
Prigatano, G. P. (1978). Wechsler memory scale: A selective edition (WMS-IV). San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessments.
review of the literature. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, Wells, F. L. (1927). Mental tests in clinical practice.
816–832. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(197810)34:4<816::aid- Yonkers-on Hudson, NY: World Book.
jclp2270340402>3.0.co;2-q Wells, F. L., & Martin, H. A. (1923). A method of memory
Psychological Corporation. (1997). WAIS-III WMS-III examination suitable for psychotic cases. American Journal
Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: Author. of Psychiatry, 80, 243–257. doi:10.1176/ajp.80.2.243
Psychological Corporation. (2002). WAIS-III WMS-III Whipple, G. M. (1915). Manual of mental and physical tests in
Technical Manual updated. San Antonio, TX: Author. two parts. Part II: complex processes. Baltimore, MD:
Pyle, W. H. (1913). The examination of school children: A Warwick & York.
manual of directions and norms. New York, NY: Young, H. A. (1916). Physical and mental factors involved in
Macmillan. the Form board test. The Psychological Clinic, 10, 149–167.

View publication stats

You might also like