Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1971, 49 89-99 NUMBER 2 (SUMMER 1971)

A RAPID METHOD OF TOILET TRAINING


THE INSTITUTIONALIZED RETARDED'
N. H. AZRIN AND R. M. Foxx
ANNA STATE HOSPITAL AND SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY

Incontinence is a major unsolved problem in the institutional care of the profoundly


retarded. A reinforcement and social analysis of incontinence was used to develop a
procedure that would rapidly toilet train retardates and motivate them to remain con-
tinent during the day in their ward setting. Nine profoundly retarded adults were given
intensive training (median of four days per patient), the distinctive features of which
were artificially increasing the frequency of urinations, positive reinforcement of correct
toileting but a delay for "accidents", use of new automatic apparatus for signalling elim-
ination, shaping of independent toileting, cleanliness training, and staff reinforcement
procedures. Incontinence was reduced immediately by about 90% and eventually decreased
to near-zero. These results indicate the present procedure is an effective, rapid, enduring,
and administratively feasible solution to the problem of incontinence of the institu-
tionalized retarded.

Institutionalized patients, especially the pro- and Klosowski, 1965; Hundziak et al., 1965;
foundly retarded, often urinate and defecate Kimbrell et al., 1967). Attempts have been
in their clothing during the day. Several stud- made to train normal independent toileting
ies (Azrin, Bugle, and O'Brien, in press; (Bensgerg et al., 1965; Giles and Wolf, 1966;
Baumeister and Klosowski, 1965; Bensberg, Van Wagenen et al., 1969) but quantitative
Colwell, and Cassell, 1965; Dayan, 1964; Giles data regarding the enduring success of these
and Wolf, 1966; Hundziak, Maurer, and Wat- attempts have not been given. In all of these
son, 1965; Kimbrell, Luckey, Barbuto, and studies, the post-training data were either ab-
Love, 1967; Van Wagenen, Meyerson, Kerr, sent or too incomplete to determine the de-
and Mahoney, 1969) have reported some de- gree of the continence that endured after the
gree of success in reducing this daytime incon- training, as was also concluded by Rentfrow
tinence with institutionalized retardates or and Rentfrow (1969) in a recent review. At
mental patients (Wagner and Paul, 1970) by present, no procedure of established effective-
using reinforcement principles. Several of ness is available for producing and maintain-
these efforts have been primarily concerned ing normal independent toileting by institu-
with "habit training", i.e., successful elimi- tionalized retardates.
nation in the toilet bowl when the retardate The overall rationale used in the present
is placed there at regularly and frequently effort was that normal toileting is not simply
scheduled intervals (Dayan, 1964; Baumeister a matter of learning to respond to bladder
and bowel pressures by relaxing the spincter
but rather is a complex operant and social
1This investigation was supported by the Illinois De- learning process that has been hindered by a
partment of Mental Health and Grant 17981 from the
National Institute of Mental Health. Grateful acknowl- reduced learning capacity and by institution-
edgment of assistance is given to C. Bugle, J. Crider, F. alization. In line with this rationale, a general
Gould, D. Haworth, Helen Hook, W. Isaacs, G. Lingle, procedure was developed in which positive
Alice Meyer, F. O'Brien, R. Patterson, and Carol Shep- reinforcement was given for appropriate elimi-
ard. The apparatuses were constructed by D. Sauer-
brunn. Reprints may be obtained from either author, nation and inhibitory training for untidiness
Behavior Research Laboratory, Anna State Hospital, (see also Van Wagenen et al., 1969; Giles and
Anna, Illinois 62906. Wolf, 1966). Urine-sensing apparatuses were
89
90 N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX

used to provide feedback to the trainer such


that he could then deliver the reinforcers or Experimental Design
inhibitors immediately after the eliminatory A three-day baseline measure of inconti-
response (see also Mowrer and Mowrer, 1938; nence was obtained for each resident, after
Van Wagenen and Murdock, 1966; Watson, which the nine residents were randomly as-
1968; Van Wagenen et al., 1969; Azrin et al., signed to a treatment or control group, the
in press). The operant level of urination (Van two groups being matched on the basis of the
Wegenen et al., 1969) or defecation (see Giles average number of accidents for the group.
and Wolf, 1966) was increased to provide more The experimental group of four residents was
responses that could be reinforced, rather than trained as a group. When the last experi-
dealing with the low frequency of elimination. mental resident completed training, after 12
Generalization of the effect of training was days, training began for the control group
enhanced by conducting the training on the (N = 5). Four of the five residents in the con-
patient's own ward (see Giles and Wolf, 1966). trol group began training as a group, the fifth
Dressing and undressing skills and un- being added as soon as one of the four com-
prompted approach to the toilet were taught pleted training. As each of the nine residents
as part of training (see Giles and Wolf, 1966; completed training, they were placed on the
Van Wegenen et al, 1969). These procedures post-training maintenance program. The
were modified and developed in this study for matching of groups permitted a standard
the intended effect of rapidity and complete- between-groups statistical evaluation of the
ness of continence. In addition, administrative effects of toilet training. The later training of
procedures were arranged for assuring the con- the control group permitted a within-subject
tinued social concern of the ward staff so that evaluation for all residents by comparing the
the effects of the training would endure. pre- and post-training accidents for each of
the nine.
METHOD Recording
All previous reports of incontinence have
Subjects based the accident data on the retrospective
Nine male incontinent and profoundly re- verbal reports or uncorroborated written re-
tarded residents were selected from a hospital ports of parents or attendants who presumably
ward; the other residents of that ward were recorded accidents on an unknown basis of
either continent, totally blind, or non-ambula- selective attention at different times. In the
tory. The nine had a mean age of 43 yr and a present study, the resident was observed every
range of 20 to 62 yr. Their mean number of hour for 8 hr each day and a written record
years of hospitalization was 21 and ranged taken at that moment by an assigned observer
from 6 to 45 yr. All but one had an IQ less as to whether the pants were wet or soiled.
than 30; the median was 14 with a range of The recording procedure was kept constant
7 to 45. Several had major physical and medi- for the three-day period preceding training
cal problems, e.g., two had paralysis of one and the first 12 days after training for each
arm and one was partially blind. Speech was resident. Any accidents observed between the
minimal for three residents and absent for hourly checks were also recorded. When pants
five; one was echolalaic. All had limited re- were wet or soiled, they were changed immed-
ceptive speech but could follow simple di- iately so that successive observations of wet
rections. Virtually no interaction was observed pants did reflect repeated wettings. Twelve
between residents on the ward of either a pos- days after the last resident was trained, the
itive or negative nature. The residents were recording procedure was changed to be more
regularly toileted as part of the ward routine compatible with the continence maintenance
after each meal. Despite these regularly sched- procedures described below. The residents
uled periods, incontinence remained the ma- were individually observed six times per day
jor ward problem. Habit training procedures at roughly 2.5-hr intervals by an assigned ob-
had been used previously with these residents server. Corroboration of the records was con-
but had not produced bladder and bowel con- ducted through several methods. Reliability
trol or independent toileting. was assessed on the third day of the baseline
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED 91
period and twice during the 12-day post-train- scribed in detail in the report by Azrin et al.
ing period by having the trainer and a ward (in press). Briefly, that apparatus consisted of
staff member concurrently observe the resi- a moisture-sensitive pair of shorts that
dents with each observer unaware of the sounded a signal when the resident urinated
other's records; perfect agreement was ob- or defecated in the shorts. A wire connected
tained. The ward supervisors regularly ob- the sensors in the shorts to a small circuit box
served the residents during the maintenance worn on a belt under the resident's shirt. The
period, especially at the designated observa- circuit box sounded a bleep-tone whenever
tion hours and indicated in writing their con- the residents urinated or defecated in the
firmation or disagreement with the data re- shorts as the moisture completed a low voltage
corded. Virtually all such reports were in circuit that was far below the threshold of
agreement. Intermittent observations by the feeling.
trainer on the ward corroborated the occur- The second apparatus detected appropriate
rence of the supervisory as well as the regular toileting and was a slight modification of the
observations. The record sheets were located "toilet-chair" apparatus described in Azrin et
in the supervisor's office and changed each day al. (in press). The present apparatus (shown in
to discourage retrospective or unsupervised Fig. 2) was a plastic bowl that was inserted in
recording. the ward toilet bowls. The malleability of the
Table 1 is a listing of the various steps of plastic allowed the plastic bowl to fit the con-
the toilet training procedure. tour of the ward toilet bowl and allowed the
hinged toilet seat to rest normally over the
Table 1 toilet bowl. The plastic bowls were commer-
Outline of the Toilet Training Procedure cially available in a variety of sizes; a size was
I. When No Accidents Occur
selected that matched closely the interior di-
1) Resident seated in chair when not seated on mensions of the ward toilet bowl. The plastic
toilet bowl bowl was heat-formed to provide a slight de-
2) Resident drinks fluids every half-hour pression at the center in which were fastened
3) Scheduled toileting of resident every half-hour
4) Resident given edible and social reinforcer
every 5 min while dry WET-ALARM PANTS
5) Shaping of undressing and dressing during toi-
leting BACK FRONT
6) Resident given edible and social reinforcer fol- VI EW. VIEW
lowing elimination in toilet bowl and returned
to chair.
II. When Accidents Occur CIRCUIT I / /
1) Trainer disconnects pants alarm BOX B BELLT
2) Trainer obtains resident's attention
3) Resident walks to laundry area to obtain fresh
clothing
4) Resident undresses himself
5) Resident walks to nearby shower, receives
shower, and dresses himself
6) Resident obtains mop or cloth and cleans soiled
area on chair or floor MOISTURE
7) Resident handwashes soiled pants, wrings pants DETECTING
out, and hangs pants up to dry
8) Trainer removes resident's chair from use SNAPS
9) 1-hour timeout procedures: Fig. 1. The wet alarm pants are shown as viewed
a) no edibles or social reinforcers every 5 min; from the back and front of the wearer. The moisture
b) no fluids every 30 min; detecting snaps seen in the front view were two cloth-
c) chair not available; ing snaps fastened to ordinary men's briefs. Two flex-
d) continue 30-min scheduled toilet periods. ible wires are shown (back view) leading from the
snaps to the circuit box. The snaps on the end of the
wire were manually removable from the snaps on the
Apparatus clothing. The circuit box was worn on a belt (back
Two apparatuses were employed during the view) which was worn "high" on the abdomen. Normal
toilet training phase. The first, the "pants- trousers were worn over the special training pants. A
tone is sounded by the circuit box when urine or
alarm" apparatus illustrated in Fig. 1, is de- feces moistens the area between the clothing snaps.
92 N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX

TOILET SIGN,AL Preventing behaviors incompatible with


toileting. Failure of a resident to go to the
ward toilet could be caused by the inconve-
nience of interrupting other behaviors such as
PLASTIC sitting in a preferred chair (see Baumeister
and Klosowki, 1965). In order to prevent such
behavioral incompatibility, the resident was
required to remain in the ward toilet area
MOISTURE/ during the entire 8-hr training session. To
DETECTING CIRCUIT reduce further the effort required in toileting,
STUDS _
BOX the residents were seated within one meter of
the toilet bowls. Residents remained in the
Fig. 2. The toilet signal arrangement iis shown from toilet during the entire daily session of about
a side view. The malleable plastic bow]I fits into the 8 hr except when they ate their lunch at a
normal toilet bowl and rests on its top edIge. The water table just outside the toilet. The area around
level (not shown) of the stool must be be elow the plas.
tic bowl to avoid shorting of the moistture detecting the toilet was visually partitioned off to reduce
studs on the bottom of the plastic bowl which are de- still further any competing reinforcers arising
signed to be shorted by urine or fecess. The dotted from distractions by other residents. Since
lines are the detachable wires that conriect the studs sitting in the chair could be considered as
to the circuit box which can rest on t he floor. The
circuit box sounds a tone when moistuire shorts the still somewhat incompatible with approach to
two studs. the toilet bowls, this source of incompatibility
was also eliminated for 1 hr when a resident
two metal sensing screws about 1 in. (2.5 cm) had an accident by removing the chair.
apart. Urine or feces in the bowlI caused an Increasing the frequency of urinations. Uri-
electrical signal to pass between thee metal sen- nation or defecation is a low frequency be-
sors. Wires connected these metalI sensors to havior under normal circumstances even for
the signal circuit box, which was identical to incontinent individuals. Consequently, few re-
that described in the report by Azrrin et al. (in inforcers for correct toileting can be given
press), and which sounded a sig,nal to the during a day. To solve this problem, the pro-
trainer. cedure increased greatly the operant level of
Immediacy of detection of elimiination. Re- urinating by giving each resident as large a
inforcement is known to be mc)st effective volume of fluids (coffee, tea, or water) to drink
when given immediately and foor every re- each half hour as he would consume. Raising
sponse. The two automatic device s made this the frequency of urinations would provide
immediacy possible by signalling aLll instances more frequent opportunities each day to re-
of elimination be they appropriate or inappro- inforce correct toileting and to react nega-
priate. The trainer was not forced to watch a tively to incorrect toileting.
resident continuously to the exclusiion of other Stimulus control of elimination response.
activities. The apparatus allowedI fairly im- On the very first day of training, the procedure
mediate consequences while still permitting attempted to habit train the residents, i.e., to
the trainer: (1) to train several iresidents at have elimination occur shortly after the resi-
once, (2) to leave the toilet facilit.y briefly as dent sat on the toilet bowl. This stimulus con-
when showering one resident, and.,still be able trol was attempted by physically arranging for
to return quickly, (3) to teach thie residents almost all urinations to be associated with
responses related to toilet trainiing without the act of sitting on the bowl. The residents
having to check the pants of other individuals, were required to sit on the toilet every half
and (4) to eliminate looking betwcten the legs hour and to remain there for 20 min or until
of a resident, sitting on the toilet, t( determine an appropriate elimination was signalled by
if he was voiding (see Watson, 196;8). the toilet alarm, whichever occurred first.
Elimination should, therefore, become associ-
"We have been informed by Lehigh Va lley Electron-
the next toileting. If a prompt was not effec-
tv h more
tive, the oedrciepopswr ie
directive prompts were given
ics Inc. that they can supply both of thie apparatuses
described here at an individual cost a f about $40. successively within a few seconds of each other
Address: Box 125, Fogelsville, Pa. 18051. until one was effective. This dressing instruc-
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED 93

ated with the toilet stimuli, and because of steps in the dressing sequence but were discon-
the reinforcement of appropriate elimination tinued once the total sequence was established
there, the resident should start to eliminate and was followed by correct elimination in
immediately upon sitting upon the toilet. Im- the bowl.
mediate elimination upon being seated on the Independent toilet approach (self-initiation
bowl, accompanied by no "accidents" when of toileting). For long-term institutionalized
not seated, would indicate that simple habit residents, toilet-approach has often been ini-
training had been achieved. tiated by external prompting stimuli. Attend-
Positive reinforcement of correct toileting. ants often instructed, accompanied, led, or
To increase the probability of toileting in the physically seated the resident on the toilet
bowl, many probable reinforcers were given seat. To eliminate the dependence on the
at the time of correct toileting and as long as prompting stimuli, the trainer deliberately
the resident remained dry. When an appro- used the minimal social prompt that was effec-
priate urination was signalled by the toilet tive and progressively reduced these prompts
bowl apparatus, the resident was given a large on successive trials until none were given. If
piece of a candy bar, hugged, and praised. To necessary, the trainer actively "molded" the
provide reinforcement for remaining dry, the resident's limbs through the desired act and
residents were given edibles consisting of verbally instructed him to perform that act.
sugar-frosted cereal and M & M candies and This direct verbal and/or physical guidance
simultaneously praised for having dry pants was then reduced to a touch, then to a hand
every 5 min for as long as they remained dry. motion, then a faint head or finger motion,
The social reinforcement by verbal praise was then nothing. The fading of prompts was be-
made as continuous as the trainer's time sched- gun from the start of training. If the resident
ule permitted within these 5-min intervals. did not respond to a mild prompt, a more
Another source of reinforcement for being dry visible prompt was given within a few seconds,
was the drink that was given every half-hour and if necessary a still stronger one, up to the
for the purpose of raising the operant level of last step of physical and verbal guidance until
urinations as described above. he was seated. Once the social prompts were
Dressing skills. One of the essential responses minimal or absent, the proprioceptive stimuli,
in the chain of events involved in independent the negative consequences of soiling, and the
toileting is the act of pulling the pants down positive reinforcement for toileting should be-
before eliminating and pulling them up again come the exclusive sources of control. When
after eliminating. Since several of the residents the resident approached the toilet for the first
had been described by the ward staff as unable time without a prompt and toileted himself,
to dress themselves, this inability would be all further prompts were discontinued even
expected to prevent independent toileting. though the fading process had been only par-
The general procedure, therefore, included a tially completed up to that time. To reduce
provision for teaching this skill. When the res- further inadvertent social prompts once the
ident approached the toilet bowl, the trainer fading was complete, the trainer discontinued
used the minimal prompt that was effective in the edibles and social approval that he had
causing the resident to undress, beginning been giving every 5 min that the resident was
with no prompt at all, then pointing at the dry. The trainer continued to bestow the edi-
pants, then verbal instruction, touching the ble and social approval when the resident
pants, placing the resident's hands on his eliminated correctly in the toilet bowl. The
pants, and lastly, guiding the resident's hands, anticipation of positive reinforcement and the
if necessary, through the entire act. The same avoidance of disapproval would become the
fading of prompts was given for pulling the basis for approaching the toilet bowl.
pants up and for flushing the toilet bowl after Modelling. Training could be expected to
elimination. When a prompt was effective, the be more rapid if imitative learning could be
trainer used the next less directive prompt on used in addition to the shaping. If the resi-
tion was given at each half-hour scheduled dents were sensitive to the actions of others,
toileting, as .well as at any self-initiated toilet- they could be expected to learn faster by
ings. Social approval and edibles were given, watching the other residents toilet correctly.
as necessary, for completion of the individual This modelling influence was arranged by de-
94 N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX

liberately training several residents at one incontinence: entrance to the dining room
time so they could observe each other. To (three occasions), between-meal snack periods
maximize interpatient observation, the chairs (two occasions) and bedtime (one occasion). At
of the residents were placed close together each inspection period, the attendant encour-
in close proximity to the toilet bowls so aged continence by praising the resident if he
there was ample opportunity for residents to was dry, discouraging incontinence when he
view other residents moving toward, or elimi- was wet by reprimanding him, requiring the
nating in, the toilet and receiving reinforce- cleanliness training (described above), and
ment. either delaying slightly the resident's entrance
Inhibitions of "accidents". Urinating in the to the forthcoming meal or omitting the snack
pants was inhibited by verbal reprimands and during snack periods.
by timeout from positive reinforcement. When
the pa its-alarm apparatus signalled an acci- Table 2
dent, the resident was bodily shaken to gain Post Training Ward Maintenance Procedure
his attention and told not to wet his pants.
The resident was then given the following I. General Procedure
1) Advance assignment of one attendant for Toilet
cleanliness training. He was immediately Responsibility each shift
taken to the nearby shower stall where he un- 2) Snack period between breakfast and lunch and
dressed and was given a tepid shower. Follow- between lunch and dinner
ing the shower, the resident was required to 3) Residents pants inspected at mealtime, snack-
time and bedtime (6 times daily)
change his clothes himself and to carry the 4) Attendant initials record sheet when residents
soiled clothes to a sink, to completely immerse checked; record sheet sent directly to supervisor
them in water, wring them out and hang them 5) Discontinued use of both apparatuses for detect-
up to dry. Upon arriving back at the toilet, ing eliminations.
the resident was required to mop up the floor II. When Accidents Occur
1) Cleanliness training whenever an accident was
or wipe from his chair any traces of his soiling. detected:
For the residents that were reluctant to per- a) Resident walks to laundry area to obtain
form these activities, the trainer manually fresh clothing
guided the residents arms and hands through b) Resident undresses himself
c) Resident walks to nearby shower, receives
the proper motions and faded out this manual shower and dresses himself
guidance as the resident began to cooperate. d) Resident obtains mop or cloth and cleans
For 1 hr following the accident, a timeout pe- soiled area on chair or floor
riod was in effect during which the resident re- e) Resident handwashes soiled pants, wrings
ceived none of the drinks, no edibles, no social pants out, and hangs pants up to dry
reinforcement, and a delay of the regularly 2) Delay of meal for 1 hr if accident prior to meal
3) Omission of snacks if accident prior to snack
scheduled portion of the lunch. 4) Attendant initials and records each accident
Minimal Maintenance - Starts Eight Weeks after
Post-Training Maintenance Procedure Training
1) Inspections only at mealtime and bedtime
In accordance with the present conception 2) Cleanliness training given for accidents
of proper toileting as socially motivated, the Termination of Maintenance Procedure - When resi-
benefits of toilet training were not expected dent is continent for at least one month.
to endure after training unless the ward staff 1) No regular inspections for that patient
was sufficiently concerned to continue to en- 2) Cleanliness training given for accidents when
detected
courage proper toileting and to discourage
accidents. For administrative considerations,
such a maintenance procedure ideally should To insure administrative feasibility and ad-
be easily conducted, require only one attend- equacy of supervision by the normal super-
ant, require little time, be meaningful to the visory personnel, several procedures were in-
residents and be capable of supervision by the stituted. Discussion with the ward supervising
ward supervisors without any special staffing. nurse, the ward treatment program director,
Table 2 is an outline of the procedures used and the nursing division director showed that
during the post-training ward maintenance all were concerned that the residents should
program. The procedure established six oc- remain continent. To translate this concern
casions when a resident would be inspected for into action, toileting responsibility was as-
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED 95

signed to a specific attendant each shift, a cause the baseline records included only that
different attendant being assigned on different period of time.
days. The assignments would be made a month Reduction of incontinence. Figure 3 shows
in advance, by specific day, with an alternate the number of accidents before and after train-
attendant also scheduled should the assigned ing for all nine residents. Before training, the
attendant be absent or indisposed. A daily pre- residents averaged about two accidents per
dated record sheet was designed on which the 8-hr day per patient. After training, the num-
attendant responsible for the six inspections ber of accidents decreased to about one acci-
would sign his name each time an inspection dent every fourth day per resident. The de-
was made. To insure adequacy of supervision crease was immediate and endured for the
by the concerned treatment supervisors, this entire follow-up period. By the fifth month
sheet was then checked by the ward supervis- after training, accidents were virtually absent.
ing nurse who then initialled it, thereby sig- A one-tailed t test was used to compare the
nifying her assurance that all inspections had accidents during the 12-day post-training pe-
been completed by the attendant specified for riod of the treatment group with the com-
that shift. She in turn routed the check sheet parable no-training period of the control
to the ward treatment director who also ini- group, based on the difference from the base-
tialled it and in turn routed it to the nursing line period. The number of accidents was sig-
division director. The nursing division direc- nificantly lower for the treatment group (t =
tor returned the sheets with written comments 3.93, df = 7, P < 0.001). The mean number of
which were then discussed by the ward super- accidents per resident changed from 2.1 to
visor during the weekly staff meetings where 0.2 for the treatment group and from 1.9 to
all attendants were normally present. 2.3 for the control group.
A second record sheet to record the specifics Examination of the records for each resident
of the residents accidents was placed in the revealed that before training, the most incon-
ward clothing room. The attendant noted on tinent resident averaged four accidents per
this record the resident's name, time found day, whereas the least incontinent resident
wet, whether cleanliness training was given, averaged one every three days. For each of the
and whether a meal was delayed, or a snack nine residents the number of accidents was re-
missed. The attendant who was assigned the duced by 80% or more during the first 12 days
toileting responsibility was to record any ac- after training. Two residents accounted for
cidents during his shift, even those brought to almost all accidents one month after training.
his attention by another attendant. A within-subject comparison of the pre- and
One month after training, a minimal main- post-training accidents of all nine residents by
tenance procedure was instituted during the Wilcoxin Matched-Pairs Signed-Ranks
which the procedure was simplified by omit- Test (Siegel, 1956) showed a significant (P <
ting the two extra between-meal snacks and 0.01) reduction of accidents for each week of
the between-meal inspection periods. Only the the post-training period.
mealtime and bedtime inspections were held. Although the majority of accidents were
When four weeks had elapsed without a urinations, defecation was also affected by
single accident for a given resident, the pro- training. Prior to training, an average of 2.0
gram was discontinued for that resident. He pants defecations (soilings) occurred per day
was no longer inspected at the regular inspec- for the nine residents combined. During the
tion periods; cleanliness training was contin- first seven weeks of the post-training period,
ued, however, should an accident occur. they averaged 0.1 soilings, a reduction of over
90%.
Results during training. Training required
RESULTS a median of four days and a mean of six days,
All accidents throughout the day had been and ranged from 1 to 14 days. These figures
recorded. Only the accidents recorded within include all daily training sessions from habit
the 8-hr period from 8:00 A.M. to 4:00 P.M. are training through the last day of independent
included in the data presentation, however, toileting training.
because maximum supervision of the record- An average of 25 cups of fluid were con-
ing procedure occurred at that time and be- sumed per resident per day of training. Dur-
96 N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX

2.0~

_ TOI LE T
,,,

en F oTRAINING
TRAIN.5NG N:9 RETARDED ADULTS
zQ<1.5
UJ
X
UJ
4>
41.0
001°
z
LLW
X
0*5

so
-3-1 +1 7 14 21 28 35 42 49 56 84 112 140
DAYS
Fig. 3. The effect of the toilet training procedure on the frequency of accidents. The accidents include def-
ecations as well as urinations in the clothing. The subjects were nine adult profoundly retarded residents. The
toilet training period is shown as an interruption in the curve. The three data points before the toilet training
show the three-day baseline period before training. The abscissa is labelled in terms of the number of days elap-
sing after training was terminated. Data points are given for each of the three days prior, and seven days sub-
sequent, to the toilet training period. Thereafter, each data point is the average number of accidents per day
for a seven-day period.
ing the training the residents averaged 15 uri- elimination did not assure independent toilet-
nations per day and one bowel movement ing. For example, three of the residents who
every third day per resident. exhibited some voluntary control on the very
Five of the nine residents seemed to possess first day of training required about nine addi-
voluntary control over their bladders and tional days before they consistently and in-
bowels (were under the stimulus control of dependently toileted themselves without ac-
the toilet) at the very start of training. This cidents.
voluntary bladder control was indicated by: Apparatus usage and scheduling. The
(1) no accidents in the pants during the entire trainer was able to train four residents simul-
first day, (2) almost immediate elimination taneously (but three residents at a time were
upon sitting on the toilet if elimination oc- a more comfortable number with which to
curred at all, and (3) external signs that the work). The number of available toilet bowls
resident was straining to eliminate in the was not as critical, since a bowl was rarely in-
toilet. Each of the other four remaining resi- disposed for very long. Since three bowls were
dents had at least two accidents in their pants, available for the four residents, the toileting
and consistently remained on the toilet seat periods were scheduled in a staggered time se-
for extended periods without eliminating. quence. Four pants-alarm apparatuses and
Training of voluntary control, as evidenced three toilet-bowl-alarm apparatuses were used.
by fairly immediate elimination upon sitting Both apparatuses were highly reliable, as
on the toilet seat, was accomplished for all initial "bugs" in the apparatus had been
residents within three days. worked out in an earlier study (Azrin et al., in
The existence of voluntary control over press). A false positive occurred (alarm
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED 97
sounded when no wetting had occurred) on erately resisting any attempt to make him con-
the first day of training because of excessive tinent, even though he had an excellent volun-
perspiration; this problem was solved by plac- tary bladder control. He was both the major
ing a small piece of adhesive tape over the incontinence problem on the ward and also
back of the snaps. The tape remained in place the highest functioning of the residents (esti-
throughout the training study, even through mated IQ-45). The trainer was required to
repeated laundering of the briefs. give an unusually large amount of manual
Reliable operation of the toilet-alarm ap- guidance, attention, and encouragement to
paratus required only that the bowls were this resident who occupied more training
emptied and the moisture-detecting studs time than any other resident.
wiped dry after each usage during training. Transition from training to maintenance.
As noted previously, neither apparatus was The proper conduct of the maintenance pro-
used after training. cedure was facilitated by having the trainer
Ward staff and patient reaction. Many of teach the ward staff. During the first two days,
the ward staff were pessimistic about the like- the trainer directly assisted the staff. During
lihood of success of the proposed program, succeeding days, the trainer observed the in-
probably because of their experience with spection periods and answered many questions
other types of efforts. After observing the resi- about the maintenance procedures, such as
dents mopping up their own accidents, and when and how to inspect the resident, division
independently initiating toileting for the first of staff responsibility, method of changing res-
time, the majority of the ward staff volun- ident, etc. After this training of the staff, no
teered statements of satisfaction. The staff administrative problem existed in having the
began asking the trainer for recommendations staff spend the small amount of time needed to
for treatment programs unrelated to toilet inspect the resident and to have the residents
training for other ward residents. The dra- change and clean themselves when necessary,
matic reduction (about 90%) in incontinence especially since so few accidents were now
following the training has been commented on occurring.
favorably by the majority of the ward staff Six weeks after training, five residents were
and all the supervisors. discontinued from the Ward Maintenance
Patient reaction. The question was raised Program since they had not had an accident
by several ward staff members as to how the for over four weeks. Nine weeks after train-
residents would react to the 8 hr per day in- ing, two more residents were discontinued
tensive training sessions. This question could from the maintenance program. The Ward
not be answered by the nonverbal residents, Maintenance Program was dropped in the
but the staff consensus was that almost all be- middle of the fifth month after the two re-
havioral signs showed the program in almost maining residents were continent for four
every case to be a very pleasant attention- weeks.
getting experience. During the training, the The ward attendant in charge of the cloth-
residents obtained an unusually higher dens- ing room reported that since completion of
ity of positive reinforcement in the form of training the number of pants sent to the
both food, drinks, verbal praise, hugs, and hospital laundry each week had been reduced
attention. Most residents would not leave the by over 40%.
toilet at the end of the daily training sessions Night time incontinence. No training re-
even when asked to leave. Two of the residents garding night time incontinence had been
who had completed training and were in the given. Yet, several reports were made by the
ward maintenance program repeatedly at- night shift that the residents involved in toilet
tempted to reenter the training area even training were getting up at night to eliminate.
though the toilet area was partitioned off from This result, although not a general occurrence,
the rest of the ward. At no time did any of the indicated that some degree of generalization
residents physically aggress toward the trainer. had occurred as a result of training.
Only two residents attempted to leave the
toilet, and then only during some of the time- DISCUSSION
out periods. The results showed that the present pro-
One of these residents seemed to be delib- cedure virtually eliminated daytime incon-
98 N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX
tinence in all residents. Training was rapid, leting as an operant response that is susceptible
requiring only a few days (median of four to training by operant reinforcement that pro-
days) for each resident. The reduction en- duces stimulus control by exteroceptive stim-
dured indefinitely and required little staff uli as well as bladder stimuli. Eliminating in a
time to maintain. Bowel incontinence was re- toilet bowl rather than on the floor or in one's
duced to the same extent as bladder incon- clothing is a social convention that requires
tinence with no need for a differential training social consequences. Yet, the lack of concern
procedure. Continence was achieved even for by otherwise preoccupied attendants often re-
individual retardates with IQs as low as 7, ex- sults in an absence or delay of these social
treme physical disabilities, as much as 45 yr consequences. Consequently, the procedure as-
of institutional incontinence, deliberate re- sured that the concerned institutional treat-
fusal to be continent, no language, as old as ment supervisors did have feedback about the
62 yr of age, locomotor and manipulative dis- attendant's behavior and could, thereby, com-
abilities, and no indication of other self-help municate their concern to all attendants and
skills. The special training apparatuses re- encourage their participation. Also, the pro-
quired were of low cost and reliable. The cedure was based on the rationale that inde-
training procedure was not unpleasant to the pendent toileting usually is not instigated by
residents or to the staff; the maintenance pro- demanding bladder or bowel pressures, as sug-
cedure for insuring continued continence was gested by the simple reflex-arc orientation, but
easily performed by the ward staff. This latter rather by the negative results (shame, guilt) of
consideration of hospital feasibility probably having visibly soiled clothing. Consequently,
ranks in importance with the effectiveness of the procedure established stronger-than-nor-
the procedure as a determinant of the practical mal negative consequences such as cleanliness
value of the program for institutions. training, and the delay of meals. Similarly,
The reduction of incontinence seems to during training, the signalling apparatus was
have resulted from the training procedure and used to obtain the trainer's immediate reac-
not from other factors. The experimental de- tion to the act of elimination, not to provide
sign used both a within-subjects and a be- feedback to the resident who was already
tween-groups comparison. The within-subjects aware that he had eliminated. Postulation of
comparison assured that the effect could not the importance of the environmental and soc-
have resulted from chance assignment of spon- ial influences, rather than simply the bladder
taneously continent residents to a treatment influences suggested that generalization of the
group. The between-groups design provided a toilet training to the resident's natural en-
control for time or other events occurring in vironment would be more effective if con-
time, since the control group was remaining ducted in that very environment and by the
incontinent at the same time that the treat- very ward attendants normally present there.
ment group had become continent. Thus, staff Recognition of continence as a lengthy oper-
expectations, resident expectations, the inspec- ant chain of toilet behaviors suggested that in-
tion periods, the renewed concern regarding tensive concern should be given to the resi-
continence, the presence of the trainer, daily dent's skill in dressing as a major contributing
inspection of resident's clothing, resident's factor as well as to his unprompted approach
diet, etc., were all held statistically compar- behavior to the toilet and for the absence of
able for the treatment and the no-treatment competing reinforcers of inactivity. The over-
phases. all objective was to teach and motivate the
The distinctive feature of this training pro- resident to toilet himself even when bladder
cedure was its consideration of proper toilet- pressures were minimal. Thus, the present ra-
ing as a complex and lengthy chain of re- tionale conceptualized continence as a com-
sponses that includes social, physical, and plex operant reaction to social factors rather
physiological stimuli and requires strong posi- than as an associative reaction of a single mus-
tive and negative operant consequences for its cle to internal stimuli.
maintenance in that chain, rather than con- The present training procedure offers a
sidering it as a simple associative muscular method of eliminating incontinence of severe
reflex to internal stimuli. This analysis is in ac- and profound adult retardates. No effective
cord with the Ellis (1963) conception of toi- alternative has been reported for the adult
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED 99

retarded. The time necessary for training was Teaching the profoundly retarded self help skill
much less (median of four days) than has been activities by behavior-shaping techniques. American
Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1965, 69, 674-679.
reported for training other types of popula- Dayan, M. Toilet training retarded children in a state
tions. A training procedure for incontinent residential institution. Mental Retardation, 1964, 2,
psychotics required about four weeks for a re- 116-117.
duction of incontinence of about 50% (Wag- Ellis, N. R. Toilet training and the severely defective
ner and Paul, 1970); a procedure for young patient: an S-R reinforcement analysis. American
Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1963, 68, 98-103.
retardates required a median of six weeks with Giles, D. K. and Wolf, M. M. Toilet training institu-
generalization data to the institutional ward tionalized, severe retardates: an application of be-
incomplete (Giles and Wolf, 1966); a proce- havior modification techniques. American Journal
dure for outpatient retarded children has re- of Mental Deficiency, 1966, 70, 766-780.
quired a median of 12 days, but the follow-up Hundziak, M., Maurer, R. A., and Watson, L. S. Op-
erant conditioning in toilet training severely men-
data for the home situation was incomplete tally retarded boys. American Journal of Mental
(Van Wagenen et al., 1969). Several procedures Deficiency, 1965, 70, 120-124.
have been reported for producing voluntary Kimbrell, D. L., Luckey, R. E., Barbuto, P. F. P., and
bladder or bowel control for young retardates Love, J. G. Operation dry pants: an intensive
habit-training program for severely and profoundly
but did not produce independent or unassisted retarded. Mental Retardation, 1967, 5, 32-36.
toileting. A procedure for achieving this blad- Mowrer, 0. H. and Mowrer, W. M. Enuresis: a
der training phase required about four weeks method for its study and treatment. American Jour-
(Hundziak et al., 1965), about nine weeks nal of Orthopsychiatry, 1938, 8, 436-459.
(Baumeister and Klosowski, 1965) and about Rentfrow, R. K. and Rentfrow, D. K. Studies re-
lated to toilet training of the mentally retarded.
29 weeks (Kimbrell et al., 1967). As compared The American Journal of Occupational Therapy,
with the alternatives, the present training pro- 1969, 23, 425-430.
cedure seemed to require far less time, re- Siegel, S. Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral
duces incontinence to a greater extent, leads to sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956.
complete independent toileting, generalizes Van Wagenen, R. K., Meyerson, L., Kerr, N. J., and
Mahoney, K. Field trials of a new procedure for
more to the resident's natural setting, and toilet training. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
shows no regression during follow-up. chology, 1969, 8, 147-159.
Van Wagenen, R. K. and Murdock, E. E. A transis-
torized signal-package for toilet training of infants.
REFERENCES Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1966, 3,
312-314.
Azrin, N. H., Bugle, C., and O'Brien, F. Behavioral Wagner, B. R. and Paul, G. L. Reduction of incon-
engineering: two apparatuses for use in toilet train- tinence in chronic mental patients: a pilot project.
ing retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Analysis (in press). Psychiatry, 1970, 1, 29-38.
Baumeister, A. and Klosowski, R. An attempt to Watson, L. S., Jr. Application of behavior-shaping
group toilet train severely retarded patients. Mental devices to training severely and profoundly men-
Retardation, 1965, 3, 24-26. tally retarded children in an institutional setting.
Bensberg, G. J., Colwell, C. N., and Cassell, R. H. Mental Retardation, 1968, 6, 21-23.

You might also like