Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

British Food Journal

The effects of blind substitution of aspartame-sweetened for sugar-sweetened soft drinks on appetite
and mood
Marie Reid Richard Hammersley
Article information:
To cite this document:
Marie Reid Richard Hammersley, (1998),"The effects of blind substitution of aspartame-sweetened for sugar-sweetened soft
drinks on appetite and mood", British Food Journal, Vol. 100 Iss 5 pp. 254 - 259
Permanent link to this document:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/00070709810221508
Downloaded on: 03 February 2016, At: 08:33 (PT)
References: this document contains references to 29 other documents.
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
The fulltext of this document has been downloaded 1249 times since 2006*
Users who downloaded this article also downloaded:
David Yoon Kin Tong, Xue Fa Tong, Evon Yin, (2012),"Young consumers' views of infused soft drinks innovation", Young
Consumers, Vol. 13 Iss 4 pp. 392-406 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/17473611211282635
Downloaded by University of Victoria At 08:33 03 February 2016 (PT)

Xiaoou Liu, Rigoberto Lopez, (2012),"Evidence of rational addiction to carbonated soft drinks?", China Agricultural Economic
Review, Vol. 4 Iss 3 pp. 300-317 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/17561371211263347
Mabel Blades, (1990),"SOFT DRINKS", Nutrition & Food Science, Vol. 90 Iss 4 pp. 6-7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/
eb059302

Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-srm:233720 []
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald for Authors service
information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission guidelines are available for all. Please
visit www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company manages a portfolio of
more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as well as providing an extensive range of online
products and additional customer resources and services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the Committee on Publication
Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for digital archive preservation.

*Related content and download information correct at time of download.


The effects of blind substitution of
aspartame-sweetened for sugar-sweetened soft
drinks on appetite and mood
Marie Reid
Department of Psychiatry, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
Richard Hammersley
Social Sciences Research Training Unit, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK

It has been suggested that to eat both generally (Rolls et al., 1988; Teff et
habitual consumers of sugar Introduction al., 1989) and specifically for foods high in
experience “cravings” when Consumption of food high in refined carbohy- sugar (Geiselman and Novin, 1982). These
deprived. Subjects (n = 27) drate may lead to weight gain in the long effects are similar to the effects of artificial
who habitually consumed term (Ramirez, 1987; Spitzer and Rodin, 1981). sweeteners (Blundell and Hill, 1986; Rogers
sugar-sweetened drinks were and Blundell, 1989), which suggests that per-
Another concern is that oral health can be
placed on a seven-day regime haps intense sweetness (whatever its source)
adversely affected by frequent episodes of
receiving either sugar-sweet- stimulates appetite. Indeed, when sweetness
fermentable carbohydrate consumption
ened drinks, or aspartame-
Downloaded by University of Victoria At 08:33 03 February 2016 (PT)

(Newbrun, 1991). Sugar-sweetened soft drinks and other sensory factors are controlled with
sweetened alternatives. A
are one common source of fermentable carbo- a blind, between-subjects design, then com-
between-subjects design was
hydrate (CHO) in the diet. For both these paring sucrose and saccharin containing
used to prevent subjects
comparing the drinks, which health reasons, partial substitution in the drinks, sucrose delays the next meal and
were given blind with the diet of sugars by artificial sweeteners may be appears neither to increase carbohydrate
cover story that the study was desirable. There may, however, be three poten- consumption at that meal, nor to reduce the
testing a new drink. In fact tial difficulties, which relate to the phenome- meal’s energy value (Reid and Hammersley,
commercial carbonated non termed carbohydrate craving. First, 1994; 1995). This suggests that sweetness,
beverages were given. At the people may simply make up for the change by rather than the metabolism of CHO or artifi-
end, subjects were unable to consuming more CHO elsewhere in the diet. cial sweeteners, is the primary cause of
guess which they had Second, reducing the CHO content of the diet increased appetite in the short term.
received. Subjects completed may increase hunger, as may intense artifi- It has also been suggested that simple car-
a prospective food diary and cial sweeteners (Blundell and Hill, 1986; bohydrates affect mood over a period of
rated mood daily using the Rogers and Blundell, 1989). Third, it has been hours. They may reduce arousal (Pivonka
Profile of Mood States, as well proposed that people may use CHO to manage and Grunewald, 1990; Spring et al., 1993;
as before and after each test everyday mood (Spring et al., 1986) and the Thayer, 1987), perhaps first increasing
drink, using simple visual effects of artificially-sweetened drinks on arousal (Thayer, 1987). However, these find-
analogue scales. Compared to mood may be different. ings may be due to psychological, rather than
subsequent days, on the first physiological, factors (Hammersley and Reid,
Some studies surveying the general popula-
day of the study subjects 1997; Reid and Hammersley, 1995). It is also
tion have found that sugar intake is nega-
receiving aspartame-sweet-
tively correlated with fat intake (Gibney et possible that mood effects vary depending
ened drinks ate fewer grams
al., 1995; Hulshoff et al., 1993) while others upon the exact form of carbohydrate used and
of carbohydrate and had
have found a positive relationship between perhaps some people are particularly
fewer sugar episodes (where
sugars, or sugar-fat, or sugar- the two nutrients (Macdiamid et al., 1995). In affected. Any mood changes after sugar-
alcohol mixtures were con- an intervention study (Tordoff and Alleva, sweetened drinks might help maintain the
sumed). Overall energy intake 1990), comparing the long-term effects of habit of consuming them. However, long-term
for the day was unaffected. By beverages sweetened with high-fructose corn effects on mood may differ from short-term
day two, there were no differ- syrup or aspartame on eating and body effects.
ences between the groups in weight (three-week), it was found that the The effects of switching from sugar sweet-
diet or mood. Body weight at carbohydrate beverage supplement increased ened to artificially sweetened soft drinks have
seven days was unaltered overall energy intake from the rest of the diet not been previously examined. The aim of the
from baseline. Blind substitu- and body weight, while the aspartame sweet- present study was to determine the effects on
tion of aspartame-sweetened ened drink had the opposite effect. The nutri- medium term food intake and mood of provid-
for sugar-sweetened soft ent content of the diet only varied in its sugar ing either sugar-sweetened or aspartame-
drinks did not increase other content and this was primarily due to the sweetened beverages, consumed several
sugar consumption and did content of the test drinks. There was no com- times a day over a seven-day period, in the
not adversely affect mood. pensatory change in fat consumption. diet of normal adults who habitually con-
Any effects of this dietary Other relevant studies have been conducted sumed sugar-sweetened drinks. Changing to
change appear transient. over much shorter time periods, typically less artificially sweetened drinks may lead to any
than 12 hours and we have reviewed these of the following long-term effects: changes in
British Food Journal elsewhere (Reid and Hetherington, 1997). overall energy intake; changes in carbohy-
100/5 [1998] 254–259 There is some evidence that consumption of drate intake; changes in rated energy; and
© MCB University Press foods and drinks containing refined carbohy- other mood changes. On the basis of the exist-
[ISSN 0007-070X] drate such as sugar increases the motivation ing literature, any changes could be in either
[ 254 ]
Marie Reid and direction, or vary in direction across the purpose of the study was stated as being “To
Richard Hammersley seven days of the study. investigate the effects of certain nutrients in
The effects of blind soft drinks on people’s psychological well-
substitution of being”. No mention of the aspartame-sweet-
aspartame-sweetened for
sugar-sweetened soft drinks Method ened drink was made to the participants.
on appetite and mood Height and weight were recorded and sub-
Subjects
British Food Journal jects were screened for eating disorders using
Of the subjects, 20 were university students
100/5 [1998] 254–259 the EDI (Garner, 1990), and were selected on
recruited by advertising in Dundee and
the basis that their scores fell within the
another 12 subjects were recruited from busi-
normal range on all eight scales. The 28 par-
nesses in Glasgow. Four students failed to
ticipants were divided into two groups who
complete the study, leaving 14 males and 14
received sucrose drinks or aspartame sweet-
females who returned data. One subject was
ened drinks to consume under natural condi-
excluded because she became ill during the
tions over a seven-day period. On the basis of
study and did not complete her diary on
their normal soft drink intake subjects were
seven consecutive days. Table I summarises
issued with two, three or four 250ml bottles of
subject characteristics. Experimental groups
soft drink per day. Table II lists the nutrient
did not differ significantly on any of these
composition of both the regular and diet Irn
Downloaded by University of Victoria At 08:33 03 February 2016 (PT)

variables.
Bru drinks.
The soft drinks were actually plastic bottles
of Irn Bru or Diet Irn Bru with the labels
Procedure removed and the caps painted a uniform
Potential subjects received an intake inter- colour. Irn Bru was chosen as it is one of the
view, when they were given a written descrip- market-leading carbonated beverages in
tion of the study. Only subjects who reported Scotland. Some people can discriminate
that they habitually consumed at least two between Irn Bru and Diet Irn Bru by taste and
sugar-sweetened drinks per day were smell when both are available for comparison.
accepted. The selected subjects indicated that Hence subjects received only one beverage
they did not generally consider consuming with the cover story described above, which
diet substitutes. They were informed that suggested that the drink was a “new” drink
they would be required to drink a minimum with unknown properties.Thus as far as possi-
of two test drinks per day and keep a food ble orosensory factors and expectancies about
diary. They were informed that the drinks the test drinks were controlled.
provided would be very similar in taste to the Those who were eligible for the experiment
Scottish soft drink Irn Bru but that it had were asked to return on a scheduled day,
been made up especially for the study. The which was either a Monday or a Tuesday, to
keep the weekend constant within the seven-
day test period. They were given their supply
Table I of soft drinks for the week and were informed
Characteristics of subjects in the two study groups to drink the agreed amount each day and
Sugar Aspartame record the time each bottle was consumed in
Women (n = 7) their food diary. They were advised to keep
one without weight unopened bottles in a refrigerator or a cool
Men (n = 7) Women (n = 6) Men (n = 7) data place. Following our previous method (Reid
and Hammersley, 1994; 1995) they were then
Age 19.4 22.0 19.7 19.7
issued a food diary, divided into one-hour
SD 1.3 2.6 2.0 1.9
time slots, to complete for the seven days of
Height (cm) 180.6 161.9 177.5 168.3
the experiment. They were requested to
SD 3.2 5.9 8.2 3.9
record all food and liquid intake, the exact
Weight (kg) 73.4 61.9 78.7 66.5
SD 13.0 14.2 17.0 10.0
BMI 22.5 23.4 25.0 23.5 Table II
SD 2.7 3.7 4.8 3.5 Nutrient composition of regular and diet Irn Bru
Energy intake drinks per 100ml
predicted from
basal metabolic Sugar Aspartame
rate (kcal) 1,799.8 1,401.9 1,880.7 1,471.2 Energy value (kcal) 43 4.1
SD 148.2 210.3 256.6 148.2 Protein Trace Trace
Mean energy CHO (g) 10.5 0.89
intake from Fat Trace Trace
diaries (kcal) 2,204.7 2,301.0 2,025.5 1,778.9 Iron (mg) 0.35 0.35
SD 605.4 856.0 818.7 586.4 Vitamin C 0 0

[ 255 ]
Marie Reid and amount consumed, and the time of day the Food intake
Richard Hammersley food was consumed. In order that subjects Compliance with the test drink regime was
The effects of blind could make a more accurate estimate of the good. Of 27 subjects, 24 complied to within
substitution of quantities eaten pictures of various common one bottle of what was requested. The remain-
aspartame-sweetened for
sugar-sweetened soft drinks foods with portion sizes marked A, B, C were ing three skipped and/or added two or more
on appetite and mood attached to the diary booklet. The experi- bottles to the regime, usually by drinking less
British Food Journal menter spent about 20 minutes training each one day and more the next. Women consumed
100/5 [1998] 254–259 subject on the accurate completion of the a mean of 2.4 bottles of test drink per day (SD
diary. Subjects were informed that they 0.6) and men 2.9 (SD 0.5). Consumption levels
should contact the experimenter if they had did not differ by drink type.
any questions or problems throughout the Both frequency and quantity measures
seven-day period. were calculated from food diaries assuming
They were also asked to complete the Pro- standard portion sizes (Davies and Dicker-
file of Mood States (POMS) (McNair et al., son, 1991). For this purpose an “eating
1971) daily at midday. The use of POMS to episode” was defined as an hourly period
assess long-term mood seemed appropriate within which food to the value of at least
since it asks respondents to rate how they feel 30kcal was consumed. This was to exclude
“today” compared with normal. The bi-polar hours when only tea or coffee were
Downloaded by University of Victoria At 08:33 03 February 2016 (PT)

form of the POMS was used, which provides consumed, although other beverages within
six dimensions of mood: agreeableness/ the hour exceeding 30kcal could count as
hostility, clearheadedness/confusion, compo- “eating episodes”. “Meals” were arbitrarily
sure/anxiety, confident/unsure, elation/ defined as hour periods during which at least
depression and energy/tiredness. Previous 150kcal was consumed.
research has found that the energetic/tired Intakes of sugar episodes were also
dimension tends to vary after carbohydrate analysed. A “sugar episode” was defined as
ingestion. any hour where at least 90 per cent of the
In addition to this, they were given seven- energy content of the food came from either
point scales to self-complete before and after simple sugars, or sugar-fat mixtures, or car-
they had consumed each test drink. The ends bohydrate-alcohol mixtures, to include the
of the scales were marked energetic/tired, consumption of mixed alcoholic drinks such
calm/tense and hungry/full, measuring the as rum and coke. Such foods included sweets
two mood dimensions proposed by Thayer and chocolate, fruit, sugar-sweetened drinks,
(1978) and satiation. tea and coffee with sugar and/or milk and
Subjects returned to the laboratory at the alcoholic drinks with carbohydrate content.
end of the seven-day period when their weight Both the psychological and physiological
was measured again and their diaries and mechanisms which have been proposed to
rating forms were collected. Subjects’ diaries explain carbohydrate craving and mood
at that point were reviewed by the experi- changes, including sweetness perception
menter, clarifying any ambiguities or missing (Blass, 1991; Hill et al., 1991) and changes in
data. In order to ascertain whether unblind- serotonin levels (Wurtman et al., 1981), sug-
ing had occurred, subjects were asked at the gest that increases in carbohydrate consump-
end of the experiment what they thought they tion are likely to occur in this form, rather
had to drink. None reported that they than in the form of carbohydrates mixed in
believed that they had been consuming ordi- with other nutrients, as in balanced meals.
nary Irn Bru or Diet Irn Bru. Several subjects For daily energy intake, there were no main
effects of drink or gender, but there was a
believed that “something had been put in the
marginal effect of day (F(6, 18) = 2.5, p< 0.06).
drink” which affected their mood and behav-
Post hoc t-tests showed no effects of the test
iour. Two subjects reported mild indigestion
drink on any day of the study. It can be seen in
due to the carbonation of the drinks, one
Figure 1 that mean daily energy intake was
subject reported an increase in his libido, but
very stable across the study period. Given the
no subject reported nausea or other symp-
absence of a gender difference on daily
toms. Once the study was complete, subjects
energy intake, total energy intake for the
were debriefed in writing about the nature
seven-day period was compared by one-way
and purpose of the experiment.
ANOVA with sugar-sweetened versus aspar-
tame drinks as the independent variable.
There was no effect of the manipulation (F(1,
Results
23) = 1.77, NS).
All variables were analysed using MANOVA For daily number of eating episodes, there
with gender (male or female) and drink was a main effect of day (F(6, 138) = 5.32,
(sugar or aspartame-sweetened) as indepen- p < 0.05) and a drink by day interaction
dent variables and day as a repeated measure. (F(6, 138) = 2.44, p < 0.05). Post hoc t-tests
[ 256 ]
Marie Reid and Figure 1 Because the different measures in Table III
Richard Hammersley Mean daily energy intake across the seven are inter-related a stepwise logistic regres-
The effects of blind days. Bars are standard errors sion analysis was conducted to ascertain
substitution of which measures from day 1 best predicted
aspartame-sweetened for Daily energy intake (kcal)
sugar-sweetened soft drinks drinks group. Number of meals, snacks,
2500
on appetite and mood sugar episodes, grams of carbohydrate in
British Food Journal sugar episodes and total energy intake on day
100/5 [1998] 254–259 2000 1 were entered. Only grams of carbohydrate
could significantly predict drinks group. It
correctly assigned 78 per cent of the sample to
1500
their correct group (Wald(1df) = 6.7, p < 0.05).
Figure 2 shows grams of carbohydrate in
1000 sugar episodes across the entire seven days.
The absence of any gender effects on intake
500
are of concern. As one would expect, males
had significantly higher predicted energy
expenditure from basal metabolic rates
0 (F(1, 22) = 27.0, p < 0.05). However, there was
Downloaded by University of Victoria At 08:33 03 February 2016 (PT)

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 large variance in calculated energy intake


Day
which was probably due to:
Key
Sugar
• genuine variations;
Aspartame • recording errors; and
• the difficulties of accurate calculations
from diary data.
found that on day 1 only those who received
sugar had significantly more snack episodes There was no significant change in subjects’
(30-149kcal: t = 2.2, p < 0.05) and marginally pre- and post-test body weight, confirming the
more meal episodes (150+kcal: t = 1.88, absence of an effect of the manipulation on
p = 0.072). Data from day 1 are shown in Table overall energy intake.
III.
Looking at sugar episodes, in MANOVA
Rated mood
From daily POMS ratings, only rated energy
there were significant effects of day
was affected: There was a significant drink
(F(6, 138) = 2.73, p < 0.05) and a day by drink
by day interaction (F(6,18) = 4.99, p < 0.05)
interaction (F(6, 138) = 2.51, p < 0.05). For the
and a significant drink by day by gender
grams of carbohydrate consumed during
interaction (F(6,18) = 3.46, p < 0.05). In post
sugar episodes, there were the same main
hoc t-tests, there were no significant effects of
effects of day (F(1, 138) = 2.62, p < 0.05) and the
drink on any specific day of the study.
same interaction between day and sugar
(F(1, 138) = 2.53, p < 0.05). Post hoc t-tests
found that on day 1 only, the sugar drinks Figure 2
group had more sugar episodes (t=3.64, Mean grams of carbohydrate consumed during
p<0.05) and consumed more grams of carbo- sugar episodes across the seven days. Bars are
hydrate during those episodes (t = 2.47, standard errors
p < 0.05). These data are also shown in Table
Grams CHO
III. 140
Sig. Diff. Day 1
120
Table III
Mean numbers of meal, snack and sugar 100
episodes on day 1 as a function of drink
80
received
Sugar Aspartame 60

Snacks of 30-149kcal 1.92 1.66 40


SD 0.79 0.89
Meals < 149kcal 5.38 4.36 20
SD 1.66 1.15 0
Number of sugar episodes 9.84 5.14 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
SD 4.0 2.7 Day
Grams of CHO in sugar Key
episodes 117.38 62.97 Sugar
SD 68.32 44.54 Aspartame

[ 257 ]
Marie Reid and Looking at men and women separately, men studies of the effects of carbohydrates on
Richard Hammersley felt more energetic after sugar on day 3 of the mood have produced weak or equivocal
The effects of blind study (t = 2.53, p < 0.05), but this may be type 1 results (Burton and Owens, 1993; Reid and
substitution of error. Hammersley, 1994; 1995). Mood effects may be
aspartame-sweetened for
sugar-sweetened soft drinks more apparent when subjects know what they
on appetite and mood are eating or drinking and can respond
British Food Journal Discussion according to expectations.
100/5 [1998] 254–259 It would appear that as long as it is con-
Compared to the rest of the week, on day one
ducted blind, switching from sugar soft
those who drank sugary soft drinks had more
drinks to aspartame-sweetened soft drinks
sugar episodes, containing more carbohy-
does not have major effects on mood or eating
drate, than did those who received aspar-
behaviour. This means that any barriers to
tame-sweetened drinks. It is not clear why
such change are largely due to people’s expec-
subjects who continued to receive sugar
tations about the unacceptability of artificial
drinks behaved differently from those who
sweeteners, or the social significance of
had unknowingly switched to aspartame. One
possible explanation is that the physiological “diet” or “light” soft drink brands. It also
effects of the ingested sugars somehow inter- appears that people can readily compensate
for the reduced sugar in their diet by eating
Downloaded by University of Victoria At 08:33 03 February 2016 (PT)

acted in the short term with subject expecta-


tions that they were receiving some new more, without simply increasing the number
drink. More important was that the effect was of other sugar episodes. No evidence for crav-
absent by day two and that switching to ing was found in this study.
aspartame did not lead to increased sugar
consumption elsewhere in the diet. References
Benton, D. and Owens, D. (1993), “Is raised blood
Overall energy intake was unaffected by
glucose associated with the relief of tension?”,
the provision of aspartame-sweetened versus
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, Vol. 37
sugar-sweetened drinks. More precisely, any
No. 7, pp. 723-5.
overall change in energy intake over seven
Blass, E.M. (1991), “Suckling: opioid and nonopi-
days was smaller than could be detected with oid processes in mother infant bonding”, in
this modest sample size. As energy intake Friedman, M.I. et al. (Eds), Chemical Senses.
was unaffected, it is not surprising that body- Appetite Nutrition, Vol. 4, Marcel Dekker, New
weight was also unaffected. We have also York, NY.
failed to find effects of sucrose, compared to Blundell, J.E. and Hill, A.J. (1986), “Paradoxical
saccharin, on energy intake in two previous effects of an intense sweetener (aspartame) on
studies over hours, rather than days (Reid appetite”, Lancet, Vol. 1, pp. 1092-3.
and Hammersley, 1994; 1995). It may be that Davies, J. and Dickerson, J. (1991), Nutrient Con-
effects would differ over a longer period of tent of Food Portions, Royal Society of Chem-
time, for example three weeks as found by istry, Cambridge.
Tordoff and Alleva (1990). Or, perhaps a larger Garner, D.M. (1990), Eating Disorder Inventory-2.
quantity of soft drink would have more effect. Professional Manual, Psychological Assess-
Tordoff and Alleva (1990) gave four drinks per ment Resources, Odessa, FL.
day to all subjects. Fewer were given here, as Geiselman, P.T. and Novin, D. (1982), “Sugar infu-
the aim was to substitute for subjects’ normal sion can enhance feeding”, Science, Vol. 218,
soft drink consumption, rather than add to it. pp. 490-1.
With hindsight baseline data on subjects’ Gibney, M., Sigman-Grant, M., Stanton, J.L. and
diets prior to the intervention would have Keast, D.R. (1995), “Consumption of sugars”,
been beneficial. However, this disadvantage is American Journal of Clinical Nutrition,
Vol. 62, pp. 178S-194S.
offset to some extent by the use of a between-
Hammersley, R.H. and Reid, M. (1997), “Are simple
subjects design where individual differences
carbohydrates physiologically addictive?”,
are controlled by random, blind assignment
Addiction Research, Vol. 5, pp. 145-60.
to conditions. With the study limitations in
Hill, A.J., Weaver, C.F.L. and Blundell, J.E. (1991),
mind, it appears that substituting aspartame
“Food craving, dietary restraint and mood”,
for sugar in a small number of soft drinks Appetite, Vol. 17, pp. 187-97.
does not substantially affect overall energy Hulshoff, K.F.A.M., Lowick, M.R.H., Kistemaker,
intake or weight loss across a week. Effects C. et al. (1993), “Comparison of dietary intake
may differ with obese or diet restraining data with guidelines: some potential pitfalls
subjects (Wurtman and Wurtman, 1984). (Dutch Nutritional Surveillance System)”,
It is also important that switching to aspar- Journal of the American College of Nutrition,
tame did not affect mood. In fact, it is far from Vol. 12, pp. 176-85.
clear that carbohydrates given blind have Macdiarmid, J.I., Cade, J.E. and Blundell, J.E.
substantial mood effects. With the exception (1995), “Extrinsic sugar as vehicle for dietary-
of Pivonka and Grunewald (1990), short-term fat”, Lancet, Vol. 346, pp. 696-7.

[ 258 ]
Marie Reid and McNair, D.M., Lorr, M., and Droppleman, L.F. and carbohydrate meals on mood and perfor-
Richard Hammersley (1971), Manual: Profile of Mood States, Educa- mance: interactions with sex and age”,
The effects of blind tional and Industrial Testing Service, San Journal of Psychiatric Research, Vol. 17,
substitution of Diego, CA. pp. 155-67.
aspartame-sweetened for Newbrun, E. (1991), in Kretchner, N. and Hollen- Spring, B., Chiodo, J., Harden, M., Bourgeois, M.,
sugar-sweetened soft drinks
beck, C. (Eds), Sugars and Sweeteners, CRC Lutherer, L., Harner, D., Crowell, S. and
on appetite and mood
Press, Boca Raton, FL. Swope, G. (1986), “Effects of noon meals
British Food Journal Pivonka, E.E.A. and Grunewald, K.K. (1990),
100/5 [1998] 254–259 varying in nutrient composition on plasma
“Aspartame or sugar sweetened beverages’ amino acids, glucose, insulin and behaviour”,
effects on mood in young women”, Journal of Psychopharmacology Bulletin, Vol. 22,
the American Dietetic Association, pp. 250-2. pp. 1026-35.
Ramirez, I. (1987), “When does sucrose increase Teff, K.L., Young, S.N. and Blundell, J.E. (1989),
appetite and adiposity?”, Appetite, Vol. 9, “The effect of protein or carbohydrate break-
pp. 1-19. fasts on subsequent plasma amino acid levels,
Reid, M. and Hammersley, R.H. (1994), “The effects satiety and nutrient selection in normal
of sucrose on everyday eating in normal males”, Pharmacology, Biochemistry and
weight men and women”, Appetite, Vol. 22,
Behavior, Vol. 34, pp. 829-37.
pp. 221-32.
Thayer, R.E. (1978), “Factor analytic and reliabil-
Reid, M. and Hammersley, R.H. (1995), “Effects of
Downloaded by University of Victoria At 08:33 03 February 2016 (PT)

ity studies on the Activation-Deactivation


carbohydrate intake on subsequent food
Adjective Check List”, Psychological Reports,
intake and mood state”, Physiology and
Vol. 42, pp. 747-56.
Behavior, Vol. 58 No. 3, pp. 421-7.
Thayer, R.E. (1987), “Energy, tiredness, and ten-
Reid, M. and Hetherington, M. (1997), “Relative
sion effects of a sugar snack versus moderate
effects of carbohydrates and protein on
exercise”, Journal of Personality and Social
satiety – a review of methodology”, Neuro-
Psychology, Vol. 52, pp. 119-25.
science and Biobehavioral Reviews, Vol. 21,
Tordoff, M.G. and Alleva, A.M. (1990), “Effect of
pp. 295-308.
Rogers, P. and Blundell, J.E. (1989), “Separating drinking soda sweetened with aspartame or
the actions of sweetness and calories: effects high fructose corn syrup on food intake and
of sacharin and carbohydrates on hunger and body weight”, American Journal of Clinical
food intake in humans subjects”, Physiology Nutrition, Vol. 51, pp. 963-9.
and Behavior, Vol. 45, pp. 1093-9. Wurtman, J.J., Wurtman, R.J., Growdon, J.H.,
Rolls, B.J., Hetherington, M. and Burley, V.J. Henry P., Lipscomb, A. and Zeisel, S.H. (1981),
(1988), “The specificity of satiety. The influ- “Carbohydrate craving in obese people: sup-
ence of foods of different macronutrient con- pression by treatments affecting serotoniner-
tent on the development of satiety”, Physiol- gic transmission”, International Journal of
ogy & Behavior, Vol. 43, pp. 145-53. Eating Disorders, Vol. 1, pp. 2-15.
Spitzer, L. and Rodin, J. (1981), “Human feeding Wurtman, R.J. and Wurtman, J.J. (1984),
behaviour: a critical review of studies in “Impaired control of appetite for carbohy-
normal weight and overweight individuals”, drate in some patients with eating disorders:
Appetite, Vol. 2, pp. 293-329. its treatment with pharmacologic agents”, in
Spring, B., Maller, O., Wurtman, J., Digman, L. Ploog, D. and Pirke, K. (Eds), Anorexia Ner-
and Cozolino, L. (1983), “Effects of protein vosa, Springer-Verlag, New York, NY.

[ 259 ]
This article has been cited by:

1. Richard Hammersley, Marie Reid, Stephen L. Atkin. 2014. How to measure mood in nutrition research. Nutrition Research
Reviews 27, 284-294. [CrossRef]
2. C. ShorttAuthorised EU health claims for intense sweeteners and sugar replacers 151-176. [CrossRef]
3. R. D. Mattes, J. M. Shikany, K. A. Kaiser, D. B. Allison. 2011. Nutritively sweetened beverage consumption and body
weight: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized experiments. Obesity Reviews 12:10.1111/obr.2011.12.issue-5,
346-365. [CrossRef]
4. Marie Reid, Richard Hammersley, Maresa Duffy. 2010. Effects of sucrose drinks on macronutrient intake, body weight, and
mood state in overweight women over 4 weeks. Appetite 55, 130-136. [CrossRef]
5. F Bellisle, A Drewnowski. 2007. Intense sweeteners, energy intake and the control of body weight. European Journal of Clinical
Nutrition 61, 691-700. [CrossRef]
6. R. Hammersley, M. Reid, M. Duffy. 2007. How may refined carbohydrates affect satiety and mood?. Nutrition Bulletin
32:10.1111/nbu.2007.32.issue-s1, 61-70. [CrossRef]
7. P. Skidmore. 2007. Macronutrient intakes and their role in obesity. Nutrition Bulletin 32:10.1111/nbu.2007.32.issue-s1, 4-13.
[CrossRef]
8. A. de la Hunty, S. Gibson, M. Ashwell. 2006. A review of the effectiveness of aspartame in helping with weight control.
Nutrition Bulletin 31:10.1111/nbu.2006.31.issue-2, 115-128. [CrossRef]
9. David Benton. 2005. Can artificial sweeteners help control body weight and prevent obesity?. Nutrition Research Reviews 18,
Downloaded by University of Victoria At 08:33 03 February 2016 (PT)

63. [CrossRef]
10. Arne Astrup, Benjamin Buemann, Anne Flint, Anne Raben. 2002. Low-fat diets and energy balance: how does the evidence
stand in 2002?. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 61, 299-309. [CrossRef]

You might also like