Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

r Human Brain Mapping 00:00–00 (2017) r

Brain Changes Following Four Weeks of


Unimanual Motor Training: Evidence From
Behavior, Neural Stimulation, Cortical Thickness,
and Functional MRI

Martin V. Sale,1,2† Lee B. Reid ,3,4† Luca Cocchi,1,5 Alex M. Pagnozzi,3,4


Stephen E. Rose,3* and Jason B. Mattingley1,6
1
The Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4072,
Australia
2
School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, St Lucia,
Queensland 4072, Australia
3
Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital, The Australian e-Health Research Centre, CSIRO,
Herston, Queensland 4029, Australia
4
School of Medicine, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane, Australia
5
QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Queensland 4006, Australia
6
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia

r r

Abstract: Although different aspects of neuroplasticity can be quantified with behavioral probes, brain
stimulation, and brain imaging assessments, no study to date has combined all these approaches into
one comprehensive assessment of brain plasticity. Here, 24 healthy right-handed participants practiced
a sequence of finger-thumb opposition movements for 10 min each day with their left hand. After
4 weeks, performance for the practiced sequence improved significantly (P < 0.05 FWE) relative to a
matched control sequence, with both the left (mean increase: 53.0% practiced, 6.5% control) and right
(21.0%; 15.8%) hands. Training also induced significant (cluster p-FWE < 0.001) reductions in functional
MRI activation for execution of the trained sequence, relative to the control sequence. These changes
were observed as clusters in the premotor and supplementary motor cortices (right hemisphere, 301
voxel cluster; left hemisphere 700 voxel cluster), and sensorimotor cortices and superior parietal
lobules (right hemisphere 864 voxel cluster; left hemisphere, 1947 voxel cluster). Transcranial magnetic
stimulation over the right (“trained”) primary motor cortex yielded a 58.6% mean increase in a mea-
sure of motor evoked potential amplitude, as recorded at the left abductor pollicis brevis muscle. Cor-
tical thickness analyses based on structural MRI suggested changes in the right precentral gyrus, right
post central gyrus, right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and potentially the right supplementary motor
area. Such findings are consistent with LTP-like neuroplastic changes in areas that were already

Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online Herston, Queensland 4029, Australia. E-mail: stephen.rose@csiro.
version of this article. au
Contract grant sponsor: National Health and Medical Research Received for publication 9 June 2017; Accepted 19 June 2017.
Council of Australia; Contract grant number: APP1012153; Con- DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23710
tract grant sponsor: Australian Research Council Laureate Fellow- Published online 00 Month 2017 in Wiley Online Library
ship; Contract grant number: FL110100103 (wileyonlinelibrary.com).
Lee B. Reid and Martin V. Sale are the joint first authors.
*Correspondence to: Prof. Stephen Rose; The Australian e-Health
Research Centre, CSIRO, Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital,

C 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


V
r Sale et al. r

responsible for finger sequence execution, rather than improved recruitment of previously nonutilized
tissue. Hum Brain Mapp 00:000–000, 2017. C 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
V

Key words: neuroplasticity; cortical thickness; functional magnetic resonance imaging; magnetic reso-
nance imaging; transcranial magnetic stimulation; motor; motor cortex

r r

representation of the hand than the general population


INTRODUCTION [Pearce et al., 2000]. Functional MRI work has also sug-
The brain is capable of remarkable structural and func- gested that the supplementary motor area (SMA), dorsolat-
tional change to allow it to optimize performance. Such eral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and caudate nuclei may
changes, collectively referred to as plasticity, are key to play integrated roles in error correction and learning of
understanding a variety of real-life phenomena, such as motor sequences [Chevrier et al., 2007; K€ ubler et al., 2006].
the learning of new skills [Sanes and Donoghue, 2000], the Longitudinal studies are particularly useful for studying
storing of memories [Xu et al., 2009], and recovering neu- neuroplasticity as they eliminate the possibility that neural
rological function after brain injury [Nudo et al., 1996]. differences are the cause, rather than result, of participants
By definition, plasticity results in functional and/or deciding to learn a skill. Several reports exist of changes in
structural change [Pascual-Leone et al., 2005]. At the brain function in response to motor training [e.g., Chang,
microscopic level, plasticity in the central nervous system 2014; Orban et al., 2010], including in the pre- and post-
can manifest in several different ways. Sprouting of new central gyri, dlPFC, and basal ganglia [Floyer-Lea and
connections, unmasking of hidden or inhibited synaptic Matthews, 2005]. Many extensive reviews of such work
connections, and withdrawal of inhibition are some exam- are available, including Chang et al. [2014], Dayan and
ples of these changes [Barron et al., 2016; Hoy et al., 1985; Cohen [2011], Doyon et al. [2009], and others. In brief,
Kong et al., 2016]. Another form of plasticity is referred to trained tasks in such studies have been somewhat hetero-
as long-term potentiation (LTP), which reflects an increase geneous, and both increases [Karni et al., 1995] and rela-
in synaptic efficacy [Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Matsu- tive decreases in fMRI activation have been reported, often
zaki et al., 2004]. Behaviorally, neuroplastic changes typi- within the same study [Floyer-Lea and Matthews, 2005;
cally manifest as altered task performance, such as Ma et al., 2010; Xiong et al., 2009]. The direction of change
improved speed of movement. Measuring performance of appears to depend on the duration of training [Dayan and
tasks before and after training provides a means of assess- Cohen, 2011; Floyer-Lea and Matthews, 2005; Xiong et al.,
ing the functional impact of training. Although such an 2009], potentially due to increases in baseline cerebral
approach can allow for an investigation of the effects of blood flow [Xiong et al., 2009], though what constitutes
various factors on plasticity (e.g., aging, attention, and hor- “short-term” or “long-term” training appears to be task
mones), a purely behavioral approach is unable to shed dependent [Dayan and Cohen, 2011]. A small number of
light on the location and type of brain changes that occur reports have also detailed minute structural changes in
after training. To probe the biological changes subserving grey matter in response to visuo-motor tasks, particularly
plasticity, brain stimulation and brain imaging techniques in the sensorimotor cortex [Bezzola et al., 2011], visual cor-
are required, each with their own relative advantages and tex, and superior parietal lobule [Draganski et al., 2004;
disadvantages [Reid et al., 2016]. Scholz et al., 2009], all areas associated with visuo-motor
In the area of motor-skill acquisition, almost all studies skills.
investigating neuroplasticity have, so far, relied on animal So far, research in this area has predominantly relied on
models, cross-sectional data, or functional measures of single data collection modalities, such as TMS, or fMRI.
brain change [Chang, 2014]. In rodents, motor training Unfortunately, the differences in study design, contrasts,
appears to upregulate synaptic plasticity in the primary of time-period, and imaging modalities of these studies can
motor cortex [Rioult-Pedotti et al., 2000], and may also be make it difficult to achieve a deeper understanding of the
associated with altered neural recruitment in this area critical experimental parameters that yield reliable brain
[Costa et al., 2004]. In humans, early work using structural changes, whether such changes are typically concurrent or
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) suggested that differ- independent, and so on. In addition, each modality has its
ences in morphology of the primary motor cortex may own limitations regarding reliability or interpretability,
exist between expert musicians and the general population which can hamper insight into biological processes when
[Amunts et al., 1997]. Since then, functional MRI (fMRI) only a single modality is acquired. For example, due to its
studies have reported altered activity in motor planning in indirect method of measuring brain activity, it is difficult
professional sports people [Milton et al., 2007], and trans- to unambiguously interpret changes in fMRI in terms of
cranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies have similarly biology without independent concurrent information, par-
reported that skilled racquet players have a larger cortical ticularly given that controlling for task equivalency across

r 2 r
r Widespread Training-Induced Changes in the Human Brain r

scans can be difficult to achieve [Reid et al., 2016]. Collect- and tested for increased MEP amplitude which could
ing multiple modalities can also alleviate concerns that reflect altered connectivity induced by an LTP-like process
results in one modality simply reflect statistical anomalies, underlying motor training. For our fMRI analysis, we
rather than subtle brain changes. For these reasons, the tested for both task-induced increases and decreases in
longitudinal study of motor training presented here con- activation within sensorimotor cortices, SMAs, and supe-
currently acquired data on four different measures of neu- rior parietal lobes. These phenomena may reflect altered
roplasticity—behavior, TMS, fMRI, and cortical thickness. processing in the “trained” hemisphere, and potentially an
Findings from a fifth measure, diffusion MRI, appear in a altered balance of interhemispheric inhibition. Although
second, companion study [Reid et al., 2017]. Each of these fMRI changes in higher areas, such as the dlPFC, were
modalities helps to build a global picture of the changes possible, we did not test for these explicitly. Finally, we
that occur with motor training, by reporting on a slightly tested for increased cortical thickness in motor areas of the
different aspect of the neuroplastic response. trained hemisphere, given reports of subtle changes in
TMS can evoke a clearly discernible and quantifiable visuomotor areas associated with visuomotor learning
motor response when motor cortical neurons are stimu- [Bezzola et al., 2011; Draganski et al., 2004; Scholz et al.,
lated at sufficient intensity [Hallett, 2000]. This TMS- 2009]. In combination with our parallel analysis of white
evoked response, referred to as a motor evoked potential matter and network changes [Reid et al., 2017], this work
(MEP), reflects the excitability of underlying cortical neu- provides a comprehensive assessment of the functional
rons. Measuring the amplitude of the MEP before and and structural brain changes associated with motor train-
after a training paradigm provides objective measurement ing in humans.
of changes attributed to cortical motor plasticity [Stefan
et al., 2000]. This is different from other, nonmotor regions,
where brain stimulation-evoked responses are more diffi- MATERIALS AND METHODS
cult to quantify (e.g., prefrontal brain regions), yet which Overview
also undergo changes via the same, ubiquitous, mecha-
nisms. Although TMS is ideally suited to quantify Twenty-four participants were recruited (14 female;
training-related change arising in the motor cortex, there aged 28.8 6 1.5 years; range 18–40 years). Participants were
are several limitations to the technique. In particular, TMS all right handed (laterality quotient 0.92 6 0.03; range
can only penetrate superficial grey matter structures [Roth 0.58–1.0) as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness ques-
et al., 2007], and can only probe the activity of one area of tionnaire. Participants trained daily on a finger-thumb
cortex at a time. Therefore, TMS is unable to provide infor- opposition task [Karni et al., 1995] for 4 weeks. Behavioral,
mation on whole-brain changes arising from training. MRI, and TMS measures were obtained before and imme-
Whole-brain assessment of plastic change arising with diately after the training period to quantify training-
training can be investigated with both functional and related changes. Participants were instructed to refrain
structural MRI. Functional MRI of local blood oxygenation from the consumption of known neuroactive substances
level-dependent (BOLD) signals provides information (including caffeine and alcohol) before and during the
about changes in brain activity during tasks with high sen- training and quantification sessions. No participants
sitivity and excellent spatial resolution. However, fMRI reported any adverse effects. The study was approved by
cannot readily distinguish whether any measured changes the University of Queensland Human Research Ethics
following training reflect excitatory or inhibitory activity Committee and all participants gave written informed
[Logothetis, 2008], something that TMS can do. Changes in consent.
BOLD signals are also more straightforward to interpret,
in terms of biological change, when in the context of infor- Motor Training Task
mation from other modalities, such as TMS [Reid et al.,
2016]. By contrast, if conducted carefully, structural MRI The training task has been used in a previous indepen-
can measure changes in cortical thickness that are more dent study, in which it was reported to induce robust
directly interpretable in terms of biology but, without behavioral and functional changes [Karni et al., 1995]. The
accompanying functional measures, are difficult to link to task involved participants performing a sequence of
any changes in brain activity. finger-thumb opposition movements with their non-
With these points in mind, the aim of this study was to dominant (left) hand (Fig. 1). Participants were pseudoran-
use a multimodal approach to investigate plasticity in the domly assigned one of two sequences that were mirror-
human cortex. We measured behavior, TMS-evoked MEPs, reverse copies of each other. Participants were instructed
BOLD changes using fMRI, and cortical thickness, before to perform their assigned sequence as quickly and as accu-
and after 4 weeks of motor training. To make interpreta- rately as possible for 10 min each day for 4 weeks, and
tion of the results more straightforward, we opted for not to look at their hand during training. To help remem-
training that did not have any visual component. Our ber the correct sequence, participants were given a printed
TMS analyses were conducted on the trained hemisphere copy of their allotted sequence (red or blue; see Fig. 1). To

r 3 r
r Sale et al. r

Functional MRI
Functional MRI images were acquired in the same ses-
sion as structural MRI images. We acquired 41 axial slices
(slice thickness, 3.3 mm) using a gradient EPI sequence
(TR 2.67 s; TE, 28 ms; flip angle, 908; field of view, 210 3
210 mm; voxel size, 3.3 3 3.3 3 3.3 mm). A liquid crystal
display projector back-projected the stimuli onto a screen
positioned at the head-end of the scanner bore. Partici-
pants lay supine within the bore of the magnet and
viewed the stimuli via a mirror that reflected the images
displayed on the screen. Participant head movement was
minimized by packing foam padding around the head.
Prior to entering the scanner, participants familiarized
themselves with the two movement sequences—“trained”
and “control”—they were going to perform within the
Figure 1.
scanner. The sequences are shown in Figure 1. Within the
Participants were randomized to practicing one of two finger-to-
scanner, participants performed these finger-thumb oppo-
thumb opposition sequences. For the “blue” sequence (left), the
sition movements with their left hand in blocks of 16 s,
order of fingers required to make contact with the thumb were
each followed by 16 s of rest. During rest blocks, a visual
little, index, ring, middle. For the “red” sequence (right), the
display showed a “Rest” command. At the start of each
order was little, middle, ring, index. These sequences were mir-
movement block a visual cue—displaying either the red or
ror images of one another. [Color figure can be viewed at
blue hand and corresponding movement sequence—noti-
wileyonlinelibrary.com]
fied participants whether they would be performing the
trained or the control sequence. This was displayed for
minimize any circadian effects on motor training [Sale 2 s, and then removed. Participants then performed the
et al., 2008], each participant was randomized to practicing required sequence at a rate of two movements (of the
either during the morning or evening, and conducted sequence) per second. As a cue to aid in this timing, a fix-
training at the same time each day. Participants kept a ation cross flashed at 2 Hz on the screen. A tone also
log-book to record their training, and were instructed to sounded at 2 Hz intervals throughout the acquisition. The
be honest when training sessions were missed, or were last two tones in each movement block were at progres-
performed outside of the specified training time. sively lower pitches to notify participants that a rest block
was imminent. Immediately following completion of each
Behavioral Measure movement block (i.e., following the last fixation cross), a
“Stop” command was presented for 1 s, which was then
Participants’ performance on the finger-thumb opposi- replaced with the “Rest” command, Four consecutive
tion tasks was assessed as the number of correct sequences “runs” were performed. Each run consisted of four
completed in 30 s. Performance was documented online trained-sequence blocks, four control-sequence blocks, and
with a hand-held video camera, and quantified offline. seven rest blocks. The order of the trained/control
Participants performed both red and blue (Fig. 1) sequen- sequence blocks was randomized but kept consistent
ces with both their left and their right hands. This was to between participants. Correct performance of the sequence
investigate whether training induced any spill-over of was verified by recording the movements with a video
effects to a nontrained sequence and/or the contralateral camera, which were later reviewed for accuracy.
hemisphere. The order of the sequences performed was Image processing and statistical analyses were per-
randomized for each participant. To minimize errors, and formed using SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Imaging
to assist in performance, a print-out of the sequence to be Neuroscience, UCL, London, UK). Functional data vol-
performed was placed in front of the participant for the umes were slice-time corrected and realigned to the first
duration of that task. Furthermore, prior to the quantifica- volume. The mean image of the resultant time series was
tion of baseline performance of the sequences (before co-registered with the participant’s temporally-unbiased
training), participants were given a brief period of time T1 template (see Methods: Cortical Thickness). The time
(5–10 sequences) to practice the two sequences. series was then normalized into MNI space, an MNI brain
Performance on the finger-thumb opposition sequences mask was applied, and the result smoothed with an 8 mm
was analyzed using a three-way repeated measures FWHM isotropic Gaussian kernel. For first-level statistical
ANOVA with within-participant factors of training (base- analyses, contrasts were conducted for (a) movement > rest
line, post), hand (left, right), and sequence (trained, control). (b) trained > rest, and (c) control > trained sequence blocks.
Where appropriate, post hoc analyses were performed All six motion parameters for each run were included as
using Holm–Bonferroni corrected paired t tests. nuisance regressors. The second level analysis looked at

r 4 r
r Widespread Training-Induced Changes in the Human Brain r

the interaction between sequence and time point (i.e., base- the vertex, and then extended out laterally (to the right) in
line [control > trained] vs post [control > trained]). This 1 cm increments. Marker sites were targeted with TMS
was designed to test whether motor training altered the using a Polaris-based infrared frameless stereotaxic system
brain’s response to performance of the trained sequence, and Visor software (ANT, The Netherlands). Five TMS
taking into account any changes that were unrelated to pulses were applied to these markers every 5 s. The
training, or pre-existing differences between responses to markers were stimulated systematically, moving in a
the control and trained sequences (although no baseline medial-to-lateral direction. Stimulation of the marker sites
differences were expected). It was possible that the trained continued until the average MEP amplitude from a marker
or control sequence would show greater BOLD responses site fell below 50 mV. Those sites where average MEPs met
after training, and so we treated this as a two-tailed test. or exceeded this amplitude were referred to as active.
To account for this, we set a more conservative cluster sig- Once a row of markers had been assessed, stimulation
nificance level of P < 0.025 FWE with minimum cluster was moved either 1 cm anteriorly or posteriorly (chosen
size of 200 voxels, after voxel-wise thresholding at randomly), until all active marker sites had been stimu-
P < 0.0005 uncorrected. lated and identified. The MEP volume [Schabrun et al.,
2009] was also calculated before and after training. This
was calculated by summing the mean MEP amplitude of
TMS Mapping all the active sites.
Mapping of motor cortical excitability with TMS was Training-related increases in the number of active sites and
performed in a separate laboratory, after MRI acquisition. MEP volume were investigated using separate one-tailed,
Surface electromyographic recordings were obtained from paired t tests to compare pretraining measurements with
the left abductor pollicis brevis (APB) muscle. Recordings those acquired after training. Significance was set as P < 0.05
were made using silver/silver-chloride surface electrodes after Holm–Bonferroni multiple comparisons correction.
with the active electrode placed over the muscle belly, and
the reference electrode placed over the adjacent metacar- Cortical Thickness
pophalangeal joint. Signals were amplified (10003) and
band-pass filtered (20–1000 Hz) using a NeuroLog system We examined cortical thickness changes arising in
(Digitimer), then digitized (2000 Hz) with a data acquisi- response to motor training. We acquired MPRAGE T1
tion interface (BNC-2110; National Instruments) and cus- images (0.9 mm isotropic) immediately before (“baseline”)
tom MATLAB software (MathWorks). Signals were and after (“post”) the 4 week training period using a 3 T
MR system (Magnetom Trio, Siemens) and a standard 32-
monitored online for movement-related activity using
channel head coil. Images were processed with Advanced
high-gain electromyography and a digital oscilloscope.
Normalization Tools (ANTs; v2.1.0, source pulled 9th Feb
Mapping of motor cortical excitability before and after
2016). An overview of the processing pipeline is shown in
training involved applying TMS to the right hemisphere in
Figure 2.
a grid like pattern (described below). The TMS was deliv-
ered using a Magstim 2002 stimulator (Magstim, UK) and
a figure-of-eight coil (70 mm diameter). The coil was posi- Single participant analyses
tioned with the handle pointing backwards at a 458 angle In longitudinal analyses, it is important to utilize tem-
to the sagittal plane to preferentially induce current in a plates that are invariant to either time point, to boost sta-
posterior-to-anterior direction in the cortex. The optimal tistical power and avoid false-positives induced by
scalp position for evoking electromyographic responses in registration bias [Thomas et al., 2009]. Although ANTs
the APB was established as the position that consistently provides a longitudinal cortical thickness script, we found
evoked the largest MEP amplitude in this muscle with a that the templates produced by this script on our system
slightly suprathreshold intensity. Resting motor threshold were commonly biased toward one time point. Therefore,
(RMT) was determined and defined as the minimum stim- we constructed templates for each participants by rigid-
ulus intensity which evoked an MEP of at least 50 mV in at registering the baseline and poststructural images together
least 5 out of 10 successive stimuli. The TMS intensity using ANTS SyN (symmetric) transform [Avants et al.,
used for the mapping procedure was set at 120% of RMT. 2008] after skull-stripping, N4 bias-field correction and
This stimulus intensity was established for both pre- and intensity normalization. Time points were transformed by
post-training sessions. half of the resulting matrices then averaged, producing an
Prior to participants’ first TMS mapping session, their approximate template that was unbiased with respect to
individual T1 MRI scan was processed with the neuro- time point. This initial template was converted into a
navigation software ASA-Lab (ANT, The Netherlands). sharper, segmented, template using the antsMultivariate-
Markers were placed on the scalp surface of the structural TemplateConstruction script, followed by the antsCorticalTh-
scan in a grid-like pattern spanning the entire right hemi- ickness script using the ANTs NKI template. Each axial,
sphere. The grid commenced at the vertex, and markers coronal, and sagittal slice was then carefully visually
were placed every 1 cm anteriorly and posteriorly from inspected. In instances where dura or skull were classified

r 5 r
r Sale et al. r

Figure 2.
Cortical thickness pipeline. Steps are indicated with numbers. (middle row of left and right columns), and cortical thickness
(1) Structural volumes from the baseline (left column) and post calculated (bottom row of left and right columns). (5) Cortical
(right column) were preprocessed, skull-stripped, and registered thickness at each time point was moved into single-subject tem-
using a symmetrical registration. The resulting half transforms plate space, subtracted, and this difference transformed into
were applied to non-skull-stripped images to achieve an interme- MNI space for statistical analysis using the known transform
diate image (top, middle column), which was processed into a between the single-subject template and MNI space. Statistical
sharp single subject template. (2) The single-subject template analyses were then performed across all subjects, resulting in
was then skull-stripped and segmented. The result was carefully statistical maps (bottom row, middle column). Images here are
visually inspected. (3) If the segmentation was considered inaccu- purely illustrative; orientation differences have been exaggerated
rate, the brain mask was manually edited and Step 2 was rerun. to convey concepts clearly. [Color figure can be viewed at
(4) If the segmentation was successful, each time point was then wileyonlinelibrary.com]
re-skull-stripped and segmented using the single subject template

as grey matter, or the brain undersegmented, edits were segmentations are provided in Supporting Information,
made to the brain mask using ITK Snap v3.4.0 [Yushke- Figure 2. Posterior tissue probabilities were then converted
vich et al., 2006], and the script was rerun with the into priors in a manner consistent with ANTs antsCook-
updated brain mask. For this study, particular emphasis TemplatePriorsCommand script.
was placed on achieving highly precise segmentations of The antsCorticalThickness script was applied to structural
the parietal and frontal lobes (Supporting Information, Fig. images from both time points, utilizing the single subject
1) due to their known association with, or proximity to, template, producing a cortical thickness map in single-
areas involved in motor training and/or learning. The pro- subject-template space. Subtraction of the pretraining from
cess was repeated until we were satisfied with the final post-training cortical thickness images produced a cortical
segmentations. Examples of acceptable and unacceptable thickness difference image.

r 6 r
r Widespread Training-Induced Changes in the Human Brain r

Statistical analysis
We hypothesized that any region of change would likely
be substantially smaller than atlas ROIs, and so opted for
voxel-based morphometry. Single-subject template T1s were
registered to FSL’s 1 mm isotropic MNI152 atlas (Fig. 2)
using ANTs SyN, and the resultant transforms applied to
cortical thickness difference images. Voxels where the white
matter was the most probable tissue, as defined by the mean
of all single-subject tissue priors, were excluded. Images
were then smoothed with a 5 mm FWHM kernel and placed
into a factorial model in SPM 12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.
uk/spm/software/spm12/). This model regressed change
in cortical thickness against time point. This model included
sex as a factor to account for its previously reported effects
on cortical thickness [Luders et al., 2006], and included
ANCOVA normalization for nuisance effects to account for
any remaining global differences. In the interests of accuracy Figure 3.
and statistical power, we restricted our analysis to the parie- Increase in performance of motor training tasks following
tal and frontal lobes—the areas which received particular 4 weeks of training. Group data (n 5 24) showing number of
focus during segmentation correction (Supporting Informa- correct sequences performed prior to (blue bars) and following
tion, Fig. 1). We set our significance criteria as P < 0.05 FWE (red bars) 4 weeks of training of a finger-thumb opposition
corrected, or a cluster comprising more than 20 voxels movement sequence (trained sequence). Participants performed
expressing values P < 0.001 uncorrected. the trained and control sequence with their left hand (trained)
and right hand (not trained). Training improved execution speed
for all four hand-sequence combinations assessed (each P < 0.05
TMS and MRI Overlay
FWE). Data represent mean 6 SEM. [Color figure can be viewed
To interpret TMS, fMRI, and cortical thickness results at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
together, it was important to show reasonable anatomical
correspondence between the methods. To achieve this, their left and right hands. Following training, there was a
TMS MEP responses were projected into MNI 152 space. significant improvement in the number of sequences par-
This was achieved by converting each active site from ticipants could complete in the 30 s period (Fig. 3; effect of
Talairach to MNI coordinates, using the Lancaster trans- time P < 0.001, partial g2 5 0.16; side 3 sequence 3 time
form [Lancaster et al., 2007], then connecting neighboring interaction P < 0.01, partial g2 5 0.026). Using the left
measurement nodes to form a mesh. An edge of zero- (trained) hand, the number of correct sequences completed
value nodes was added to the outside of this mesh, based on the trained sequence increased by 53%, from 24.0 6 1.2
on the position of neighboring nodes. A duplicate of this (mean 6 SEM) to 36.7 6 2.1 (Holm–Bonferroni adjusted
mesh was projected 20 mm toward the midline of the base P 5 2.73 3 1029). There were also smaller, but significant
of the brain (x 5 0, y 5 0), reflecting the approximate pene- improvements in performance of the control sequence
tration of the TMS pulse. Values on the grid were normal- with the left hand (24.48 6 0.94 to 25.65 6 0.79 [mean 6
ized between 0 and 1 on a per-participant basis. For each SEM]; 6.5% increase; P 5 0.025 [Holm–Bonferroni
participant, a volume was generated by linearly interpolat- adjusted]), the trained sequence with the right hand
ing all voxels in MNI space between the inner and outer (22.22 6 1.11 to 26.48 6 1.22; 21.0% increase; P 5 7.53 3
surfaces of the grid by the nearest surrounding 8 nodes. 1026), and the control sequence with the right hand
All participants’ volumes were averaged to produce a (21.82 6 1.4 to 24.74 6 1.15; 15.8% increase; P 5 9.85 3
mean image. This overlay was used for visual comparison 1026). Although participants improved to a greater degree
of the modalities only; it was not used for quantification of on the trained sequence than the control sequence (both
TMS MEPs. hands P < 0.05 FWE), this difference between sequences
was substantially greater for the left than the right hand
(P < 0.05 FWE)
RESULTS
Behavioral Performance fMRI Analysis
There was no difference in behavioral performance for Of the 23 participants included in the fMRI analysis,
the morning and evening training groups, and therefore none exhibited excessive (>2 mm or 28) head movement
the data for the two groups were pooled for subsequent during any session. Prior to training (Fig. 4), the trained
analyses. Prior to training, participants were equally profi- sequence versus rest contrast revealed four significant
cient at performing the trained and control sequences with (P < 0.05 FWE) clusters that were located across a variety

r 7 r
r Sale et al. r

evoked greater activation than the trained sequence in a


number of sensorimotor areas (Fig. 4), despite slight
reductions in control-sequence activation in these areas
after training (Supporting Information, Fig. 3). To formally
compare these differences, we calculated the interaction
between training type and time point (baseline [control >
trained] versus post [control > trained]). For this contrast,
positive t values indicated locations in which the trained
sequence showed reduced signal after training, relative to
the control sequence. In the left hemisphere, a significant
cluster overlapped the postcentral gyrus and superior pari-
etal lobule (1947 voxels, cluster p-FWE < 0.001; Table I). In
the right hemisphere, a similar cluster overlapped the pre-
central gyrus, postcentral gyrus, and superior parietal lob-
ule (864 voxels, cluster p-FWE < 0.001; Fig. 5). Both
hemispheres also each demonstrated a significant cluster
crossing the premotor cortex and SMA (left hemisphere,
700 voxels; right hemisphere, 301 voxels; each cluster
p-FWE < 0.001). Reversal of this contrast, highlighting
areas of increased relative activation of the trained
sequence, revealed no significant clusters.

TMS Mapping
There was no significant change in RMT following train-
ing (40.8% vs 41.6% maximum stimulator output; P > 0.05).
By contrast, training induced an increase in the volume of
MEP amplitudes evoked at the targeted sites by an average
of 58.6% (P 5 0.015; Fig. 6). That is, with the same relative
TMS intensity (120% of RMT), MEPs evoked by TMS at the
active sites were significantly larger following training.
There was no significant increase in the number of active
sites evoked with TMS (P 5 0.09). A cortical heat-map from
a representative participant is shown in Figure 6, together
with group-wise changes. A normalized group-wise heat
map (see Methods) is also shown in Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Mean of participants’ normalized TMS responses (Top; normalized
mV), positive BOLD activation during the trained task (middle; t TABLE I. Locations of significant cluster fMRI peaks for
values for task vs rest), and positive t value map contrasting BOLD the time point–sequence interaction (baseline
for control sequence > trained sequence (bottom), overlaid on an [control > trained] > post [control > trained])
MNI 152 T1 image. The left and right columns show measures
Cluster size Peak coordinates Anatomical locations
before and after the training period, respectively. Crosshairs show
equivalent anatomical positions (MNI 152 coordinate: 37, 216, 1947 226, 254, 54 L SPL
57) to aid viewing. TMS and BOLD signals show a large degree of 244, 234, 36 L PoG
overlap in the primary motor cortex. The TMS responses were 232, 244, 52 L SPL
centered more anteriorly (motor 1 premotor), particularly after 864 18, 256, 60 R SPL
training, than the BOLD responses (predominantly motor1 sen- 40, 230, 42 R PrG/PoG
sorimotor). Details of TMS projection into MNI space is contained 20, 248, 62 R SPL/PoG
in the text. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] 700 212, 26, 64 L SMA
224, 6, 40 L premotor/SMA
224, 10, 42 L premotor
301 24, 28, 54 R premotor/SMA
of motor areas, predominantly in a bilateral manner,
24, 22, 64 R premotor/SMA
including the precentral gyri, postcentral gyri, SMAs, and
28, 210, 62 R premotor
superior parietal lobule. At this time, the activation of the
trained and control sequences were, as expected, equiva- Cluster size is in voxels. Peaks are in MNI 152 standard space.
lent (i.e., there were no significant clusters when con- Abbreviations: L, left; PrG, precentral gyrus; R, right; SMA, sup-
trasted). Following training, however, the control sequence plementary motor area; SPL, superior parietal lobule.

r 8 r
r Widespread Training-Induced Changes in the Human Brain r

Figure 5.
Training-related differences in BOLD activation. Groupwise func- area/superior premotor area of the right (301 voxels; P < 0.001
tional MRI t-map, showing voxels which, after training, demon- FWE) and left (700 voxels; P < 0.001 FWE) hemispheres. Only
strated higher signal during the control sequence than the voxels belonging to significant clusters are shown; a structural
trained sequence. A series of axial slices is shown. Significant template is displayed for anatomical reference only. Left of image
clusters were apparent in the superior parietal lobule and pri- is the left of brain. Axial slices shown are evenly spread between
mary sensorimotor cortices in the right (864 voxels; P < 0.001 MNI-152 z-slices 67 and 26 inclusive. [Color figure can be
FWE) and left (1947 voxels; P < 0.001 FWE) hemispheres. Signif- viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
icant clusters were also seen in the supplementary motor

Cortical Thickness of sensory and motor finger representations [Penfield and


Boldrey, 1937; Wahnoun et al., 2015], and close to peaks in
One participant’s dataset was lost during transfer to the the (post time point) TMS and fMRI maps. All clusters
server, and so was excluded from both cortical thickness were within the group-wise region of successful TMS exci-
and fMRI analyses. A second participant displayed slight tation, and all but the prefrontal cluster were consistent
MRI artefacts on the T1 images in the right sensorimotor with regions of group-wise fMRI activation. To test the
cortex, and so was excluded from cortical thickness analy- robustness of these findings, we removed data from the
ses. For a third dataset, we were unable to achieve high- two participants who displayed the strongest performance
quality tissue segmentation and so we excluded this data- improvements. In this reanalysis, the previously found
set from cortical thickness analyses, leaving 21 participants clusters in the precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, and
in total for these analyses. Unthresholded t-value images prefrontal cortex were still apparent (data not shown).
are provided in Supporting Information, Figures 4 and 5.
At an uncorrected threshold of P < 0.001, training resulted
in an increase in cortical thickness in the right precentral DISCUSSION
gyrus (81 voxel cluster), right post central gyrus (34 voxel
cluster) and right dlPFC at approximately Brodmann’s The neural changes that accompany a period of motor
area 9 (22 voxels; Fig. 7). The right SMA also contained training contribute to the increase in performance. Under-
two nearby clusters, 11 and 13 voxels in size, whose indi- standing how these neural changes manifest themselves is
vidual volumes did not cross our prespecified threshold, important in both guiding rehabilitation strategies [Reid
but whose sum did. Although these five clusters did not et al., 2015], and understanding normal brain function.
survive multiple comparisons correction (P < 0.05 FWE) all Such neural changes can be quantified in several ways,
changes were observed in regions that are likely to be each with their respective advantages and limitations.
involved with motor tasks, and all were in the right Here, we show how utilizing several different methods to
(“trained”) hemisphere. Notably, clusters at the pre- and concurrently quantify plasticity—behavioral, brain stimula-
postcentral gyri were consistent with the expected location tion, functional brain imaging, and structural brain

r 9 r
r Sale et al. r

Figure 6.
Training-related changes in motor cortical excitability. Top: A of active sites (left) and MEP map volume (right) before (blue)
heat-map representation of the cortical representation of the and after (red) training across all participants. There was no
abductor pollicis brevis before (left) and after (right) training in increase in active sites following training, but MEP volume
one representative participant. Coordinates are referenced to increased significantly. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonli-
the vertex (0, 0). The average MEP amplitude evoked at each nelibrary.com]
site is indicated by the color scale (mV). Bottom: Mean number

imaging—can offer broad insight into the plastic changes 3). This was similar (albeit slightly lower) than reported in
that arise following training. To our knowledge, these two similar, yet smaller, studies [Karni et al., 1995; Xiong
multimodal data from healthy adults provide the most et al., 2009]. To further investigate the brain changes arising
comprehensive assessment of the functional and structural from training, we incorporated several other probes of corti-
changes that occur following training to date. Multimodal cal structure and function. Although transfer of skill did
assessments are particularly important in this context as occur, both between sequences and between hands, our
the measurement of minute brain changes is difficult. Posi- fMRI and TMS analyses were not explicitly designed to
tive results from single modalities, then, are often inter- investigate such effects. Diffusion MRI analyses detailed in
preted with caution (e.g., perception of potential a companion paper [Reid et al., 2017] explored transfer
publication bias). By contrast, coherent results from multi- effects in the same participant group in more detail.
ple, concurrently acquired and independently analyzed
modalities—as reported in this study, and its companion Functional MRI
paper [Reid et al., 2017]—lowers the probability of an
overall false positive. Our fMRI analyses revealed that, only after training, the
Four weeks of training of a sequence of finger-thumb trained sequence evoked lower cortical activation bilater-
opposition movements resulted in a substantial improve- ally than the control sequence (Fig. 5), predominantly in
ment in performance. This was particularly true of the the sensorimotor cortices and superior parietal lobe
trained sequence performed with the trained hand, for (Table I). The apparent relative reduction in functional
which correct sequence completions improved by 53% (Fig. activation reported here is at odds with an earlier smaller

r 10 r
r Widespread Training-Induced Changes in the Human Brain r

may have occurred, such changes cannot explain our


results. Specifically, as both the control and trained tasks
evoked the same patterns of activation at baseline (Fig. 4),
changes in resting cerebral blood flow would be expected
to affect both equally, and so cannot explain the statistical
interaction reported here. An alternative, nonmutually
exclusive, explanation is that local changes, such as LTP,
allowed local grey matter that was responsible for per-
forming the trained task prior to training to perform the
trained sequence more efficiently, reducing the need for
recruitment of surrounding areas. Further, because we
controlled for execution speed during scanning, the parie-
tal changes we report (i.e., increased activation in the con-
trol vs trained sequence) might reflect a reduction in
attentional resources required to perform the trained
sequence compared to the control sequence. This finding
is consistent with the idea of more efficient processing.
Future research using fMRI could be useful in investigat-
ing such attentional network-related changes following
motor training.
Previous research has highlighted that motor training is
supported by changes in the allegiance of specialized
regions to a given brain system, or module [Bassett et al.,
2011]. For example, the analysis of fMRI data acquired
during the performance of a motor sequence task over six
weeks revealed that increased automaticity in task perfor-
mance is supported by a reduction in the functional inte-
gration between motor and visual areas composing two
distinct brain modules [Bassett et al., 2011]. Moreover, the
gained automaticity was related to the disengagement of
Figure 7. cognitive control hub regions comprising fronto-parietal
Areas of increase in cortical thickness overlaid on the FSL MNI and cingulo-opercular systems [Bassett et al., 2011]. These
152 template. Light blue indicates voxels with significant (cluster findings echo recent results showing that a focal reduction
size > 20 voxels at P < 0.001 unc) increases in cortical thickness of baseline activity in the primary motor cortex causes a
estimation. Increases were seen in the right prefrontal cortex significant reorganization of functional connectivity pat-
(top row), and approximate hand areas of the right precentral terns within the sensorimotor module and between this
(middle row) and postcentral (bottom row) gyri. Red–yellow module and the rest of the brain [Cocchi et al., 2015]. Spe-
indicates mean normalized TMS response, where yellow indi- cifically, a reduction in motor cortex excitability was paral-
cates a strong response (mean  30% normalized peak motor leled by enhanced functional connectivity within the
evoked potential) and red indicates a weaker response (mean- sensorimotor module and reduced connectivity with other
 1% of normalized peak motor evoked potential); see text for modules of the brain. In this study, we did not directly
details. Left of coronal and axial images indicates left of brain. assess changes in connectivity as a function of training
Axial slices (top-to-bottom) show MNI y coordinates 30, 23, but, previous findings suggest that the observed changes
and 218. Sagittal slices show MNI x coordinates 44, 58, and 61. in local neural activity occur in the context of broader
[Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] changes in brain network dynamics. Future work will
need to clarify the functional interplay between training-
induced local changes in neural activity and global modu-
(n 5 6) study that used a very similar training and scan- lation of brain network dynamics.
ning approach [Karni et al., 1995]. A subsequent replica- Although motor training paradigms, similar to the one
tion of this work showed that fMRI activation decreased employed in this study, have been shown to induce LTP-
during the third and fourth weeks of training, and PET like changes in cortical activity [Ziemann et al., 2004], it
data suggested that this was, at least in part, due to an would be fair to consider this hypothesis speculative if
increase in baseline blood flow rather than a decrease in based on fMRI alone. A novel aspect of the present work,
actual brain activity [Xiong et al., 2009]. This study pro- however, is providing evidence in support of this hypothe-
vides an interesting insight into these works: our contrast sis from two additional lines of enquiry in the same partic-
focused on the interaction between time point and task, ipants: changes in TMS-evoked responses and changes in
which means that, although changes in resting blood flow cortical thickness.

r 11 r
r Sale et al. r

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation definitive in themselves, they are interesting in the context
of our other findings. Specifically, the location of clusters
TMS provides an indirect way to assess LTP-like in the pre- and post-central gyri were relatively consistent
changes in cortical excitability. Since TMS activates motor with our fMRI maps, our TMS maps (Fig. 7), and with
cortical output neurons trans-synaptically, if synaptic effi- areas responsible for sensorimotor representation of the
cacy is increased, this should lead to an increase in the fingers elucidated through electrocorticographic and brain
amplitude of the MEP at a given stimulus intensity [Di stimulation techniques [Penfield and Boldrey, 1937; Wah-
Lazzaro and Ziemann, 2013]. Here, we showed that MEP noun et al., 2015]. The neighboring clusters seen in the
amplitudes were larger following training (Fig. 6). Nota-
right SMA were also within the TMS and fMRI maps from
bly, there was no significant change in the area that could
our study, and consistent with previous findings that this
evoke an MEP in the APB. This suggests that the changes
area plays a role in motor training [Taubert et al., 2010].
indexed by TMS were predominantly driven by changes
That cortical thickness changes were very subtle, while
in neural networks that already played a role in contrac-
task performance was strongly improved, and other brain-
tion of the APB.
change measures relatively clear cut, may be indicative
To consider TMS and MRI evidence together, it is
that the processes driving relatively short-term skill acqui-
important that these are viewed with respect to one
sition are not those that drive strong increases in cortical
another, to ascertain that regions of measurement (or sig-
thickness. That these changes appeared near the peaks of
nal change) for these modalities have reasonable anatomi-
fMRI and TMS maps adds further credibility to the sug-
cal overlap. In this study, the areas of cortex that were
gestion that changes in fMRI and TMS patterns were a
activated by the trained sequence (in the trained hemi-
reflection of altered neurite organization, density, and/or
sphere) during fMRI scans were very similar, though not
connection strength in regions that, at baseline, were
identical, to areas eliciting MEPs during TMS mapping
(Fig. 4). TMS and fMRI map markedly different aspects of already predominantly responsible for execution of the
motor control, and so perfect overlap between them (to-be) trained sequence, rather than altered responsibility
should not be expected: fMRI here contrasted BOLD of surrounding areas.
responses to sequential movement of the fingers and The final location in which we saw changes in cortical
thumb versus rest, while TMS targeted neurons function- thickness was the dlPFC, which is known to play an
ally relevant to activating the APB muscle. important role in error-correction of motor output (along
Keeping in mind that there was no increase in the RMT, with the caudate nucleus) [Chevrier et al., 2007; K€ ubler
the TMS findings suggest that at least one of three pro- et al., 2006]. Structural changes in the dlPFC were toward
cesses have taken place: enhanced trans-synaptic transmis- the edge of the TMS response map, and not in an area of
sion (e.g., through LTP), increased neurite density, and/or fMRI change. The role of the dlPFC is to modulate motor
improved conduction of the corticospinal tract. Both the responses, not generate them directly, and so it should not
first and second of these possibilities support our earlier be surprising that only small TMS responses were
hypothesis introduced in the context of the fMRI results. recorded. Furthermore, we do not consider cortical thick-
The third possibility, regarding changes in white matter, is ness change here to be at odds with fMRI findings because
investigated in detail in our follow-up publication [Reid the fMRI analysis was optimized for detection of motor
et al., 2017]. output and conducted at around half the speed partici-
pants were capable of at baseline; it did not contrast very-
challenging-versus-relaxed motor performance, as would
Cortical Thickness be optimal to highlight an area that plays a role in error
Increased cortical thickness was observed after motor detection. In fact, we believe this apparent discrepancy
training within the right SMA, right middle-frontal gyrus, highlights the usefulness of multimodality imaging in
and right precentral gyrus and right postcentral gyrus. measuring neuroplasticity to provide a more accurate
Although cortical thickness changes did not reach our overview of changes that take place during training [Reid
stricter FWE-corrected threshold, the clusters of significant et al., 2016]. The dlPFC and related areas were further
voxels seen were sizeable (22–81 voxels @ P < 0.001) and investigated with diffusion imaging; these analyses are
striking in their location. As previously mentioned, one described in a separate publication [Reid et al., 2017].
motivation for the present study was to index motor train-
ing related brain changes across a variety of modalities to Biological Interpretation
better understand to what degree, if any, these are concur-
rent, and to build a more global picture of the processes MRI is not an optimal tool for the interpretation of bio-
that allow behavioral improvement. Toward this aim, it is logical changes in terms of cellular physiology. Neverthe-
then interesting that these changes in cortical thickness less, it provides an excellent means of localizing brain
were substantially less strong than TMS, fMRI, behavior, changes and, particularly when applied in a multimodal
and the diffusion measures reported in the accompanying manner, can suggest which family of processes may
paper [Reid et al., 2017]. Although these findings are not underlie changes. The behavioral, fMRI, TMS, and cortical

r 12 r
r Widespread Training-Induced Changes in the Human Brain r

thickness changes presented here are consistent with an challenging rates of task execution were not assessed using
LTP-like change in the grey matter. Functional MRI fMRI. Including such a manipulation might have helped
showed a task-specific reduction in BOLD signal, consis- us interpret the changes in dlPFC and related structures,
tent with more efficient processing in the grey matter. as reported both here and in our diffusion MRI work
TMS findings suggested that enhanced trans-synaptic [Reid et al., 2017]. Similarly, TMS was not acquired for the
transmission and/or improved corticospinal tract conduc- “untrained” hemisphere. Including such a condition might
tion took place. Cortical thickness findings suggested sub- help to understand processes responsible for skill transfer
tle structural changes in the same regions. The fact that between hemispheres. Future work could also further illu-
cortical thickness findings were not strong suggests that minate these processes by including an additional time
gross morphological changes to the grey matter were not a point, several days or weeks following the cessation of
primary driving force toward behavioral change. This is training. Another possibility would be to include daily
more consistent with more subtle (dendritic or synaptic) behavioral assessments, to assess whether training-related
changes rather than larger scale changes, such as glial cell brain changes reflect continuous skill acquisition, or
proliferation or angiogenesis. The precise combination of instead track ongoing learning processes themselves and
processes is not currently something that can be deter- subside once skill level reaches a plateau. In a multiple-
mined with MRI alone. However, the locus of these time point study of this nature, collecting behavioral error
changes—regions already involved in performing the non- rates, in addition to successful-sequence completion rates,
trained task at baseline—indicates that improved function would also be advantageous.
is at least partially driven by more efficient grey matter
processing in regions that, at baseline, were already pre-
dominantly involved with the execution of the trained CONCLUSION
sequence, rather than altered activation of surrounding We have shown that 4 weeks of motor training can
areas. invoke robust changes in behavior, cortical thickness, fMRI
activation, and TMS-evoked motor maps in motor regions
Generalization to the Untrained Hand of the brain. Taken together with our diffusion MRI find-
ings [Reid et al., 2017], the work presented here provides
We report improvements in task performance for both the most cohesive and comprehensive longitudinal study
sequences in both hands, despite only one sequence with of motor plasticity in healthy adult humans to date. All
the nondominant hand being trained. Such cross- three brain measures suggested that motor training was
hemisphere generalization is at odds with one smaller driven by LTP-like plasticity that was relatively wide-
study (n 5 6) that employed a similar training task [Karni spread across the sensorimotor system—even when a par-
et al., 1995], but is consistent with findings from several ticipant is trained on a simple task solely on the basis of
other motor studies that utilized other tasks [Hicks et al., proprioceptive feedback.
1983; Teixeira, 2000]. Our fMRI analyses revealed that,
after training, execution of the trained sequence with the
left-hand elicited reduced activation relative to the control CONFLICT OF INTEREST
sequence, bilaterally. However, the present results and
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of
those of our companion paper [Reid et al., 2017] revealed
interest.
no changes in the left hemisphere as measured with struc-
tural or diffusion MRI. This discrepancy suggests that
altered fMRI and behavioral measures of the “untrained”
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
side may reflect changes in grey matter, such as LTP or The funders had no role in study design, data collection
altered neurite density, that were too subtle to be detected and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the
by structural MRI. Techniques such as neurite orientation manuscript. The content is solely the responsibility of the
dispersion and density imaging [Zhang et al., 2012], or authors and does not necessarily represent the official
bilateral TMS, EEG, or fMRI tasks targeting the opposite views of the funding bodies. The authors declare no com-
hand, might allow future studies to shed further light on peting financial interests. The authors would like to thank
this issue. Dr Amanda Robinson, Dr David Lloyd, and Dr Daniel
Stjepanovic for technical assistance.
Limitations
This study was designed to maximize the amount of REFERENCES
concurrently acquired information. Practical consider- Amunts K, Schlaug G, J€ ancke L, Steinmetz H, Schleicher A,
ations, however, meant that the depth of investigations Dabringhaus A, Zilles K (1997): Motor cortex and hand motor
with each modality had to be limited, leaving some open skills: Structural compliance in the human brain. Hum Brain
questions for future studies. For example, more Mapp 5:206–215.

r 13 r
r Sale et al. r

Avants BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, Gee JC (2008): Symmetric Di Lazzaro V, Ziemann U (2013): The contribution of transcranial
diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: Evalu- magnetic stimulation in the functional evaluation of microcir-
ating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative cuits in human motor cortex. Front Neural Circuits 7:18.
brain. Med Image Anal 12:26–41. Logothetis NK (2008): What we can do and what we cannot do
Barron HC, Vogels TP, Emir UE, Makin TR, O’Shea J, Clare S, with fMRI. Nature 453:869–878.
Jbabdi S, Dolan RJ, Behrens TEJ (2016): Unmasking latent Luders E, Narr KL, Thompson PM, Rex DE, Woods RP, DeLuca H,
inhibitory connections in human cortex to reveal dormant cor- Jancke L, Toga AW (2006): Gender effects on cortical thickness
tical memories. Neuron 90:191–203. and the influence of scaling. Hum Brain Mapp 27:314–324.
Bassett DS, Wymbs NF, Porter MA, Mucha PJ, Carlson JM, Ma L, Wang B, Narayana S, Hazeltine E, Chen X, Robin DA, Fox
Grafton ST (2011): Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain PT, Xiong J (2010): Changes in regional activity are accompa-
networks during learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: nied with changes in inter-regional connectivity during 4
7641–7646. weeks motor learning. Brain Res 1318:64–76.
Bezzola L, Merillat S, Gaser C, J€ ancke L (2011): Training-induced Matsuzaki M, Honkura N, Ellis-Davies GCR, Kasai H (2004):
neural plasticity in golf novices. J Neurosci 31:12444–12448. Structural basis of long-term potentiation in single dendritic
Bliss TV, Collingridge GL (1993): A synaptic model of memory: spines. Nature 429:761–766.
Long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361:31–39. Milton J, Solodkin A, Hlustık P, Small SL (2007): The mind of
expert motor performance is cool and focused. Neuroimage 35:
Chang Y (2014): Reorganization and plastic changes of the human
804–813.
brain associated with skill learning and expertise. Front Hum
Nudo RJ, Wise BM, SiFuentes F, Milliken GW (1996): Neural sub-
Neurosci 8:35.
strates for the effects of rehabilitative training on motor recov-
Chevrier AD, Noseworthy MD, Schachar R (2007): Dissociation of
ery after ischemic infarct. Science 272:1791–1794.
response inhibition and performance monitoring in the stop
Orban P, Peigneux P, Lungu O, Albouy G, Breton E, Laberenne F,
signal task using event-related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 28:
Benali H, Maquet P, Doyon J (2010): The multifaceted nature
1347–1358. of the relationship between performance and brain activity in
Cocchi L, Sale MV, Lord A, Zalesky A, Breakspear M, Mattingley motor sequence learning. Neuroimage 49:694–702.
JB (2015): Dissociable effects of local inhibitory and excitatory Pascual-Leone A, Amedi A, Fregni F, Merabet LB (2005): The plas-
theta-burst stimulation on large-scale brain dynamics. tic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 28:377–401.
J Neurophysiol 113:3375–3385. Pearce a. J, Thickbroom GW, Byrnes ML, Mastaglia FL (2000):
Costa RM, Cohen D, Nicolelis MAL (2004): Differential cortico- Functional reorganisation of the corticomotor projection to
striatal plasticity during fast and slow motor skill learning in the hand in skilled racquet players. Exp Brain Res 130:238–243.
mice. Curr Biol 14:1124–1134. Penfield W, Boldrey E (1937): Somatic motor and sensory repre-
Dayan E, Cohen LG (2011): Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill sentation in the cerebral cortex of man as studies by electrical
learning. Neuron 72:443–454. stimulation. Brain 60:389–443.
Doyon J, Bellec P, Amsel R, Penhune V, Monchi O, Carrier J, Reid LB, Boyd RN, Cunnington R, Rose SE (2016): Interpreting
Lehericy S, Benali H (2009): Contributions of the basal ganglia intervention induced neuroplasticity with fMRI: The case for
and functionally related brain structures to motor learning. multimodal imaging strategies. Neural Plast 2016:2643491.
Behav Brain Res 199:61–75. Reid LB, Rose SE, Boyd RN (2015): Rehabilitation and neuroplas-
Draganski B, Gaser C, Busch V, Schuierer G, Bogdahn U, May A ticity in children with unilateral cerebral palsy. Nat Rev Neu-
(2004): Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by rol 11:390–400.
training. Nature 427:311–312. Reid LB, Sale MV, Cunnington R, Mattingley JB, Rose SE (2017):
Floyer-Lea a, Matthews PM (2005): Distinguishable brain activa- Brain changes following four weeks of unimanual motor train-
tion networks for short- and long-term motor skill learning. ing: Evidence from fMRI-guided diffusion MRI tractography.
J Neurophysiol 94:512–518. Hum Brain Mapp. [Epub ahead of print].
Hallett M (2000): Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the Rioult-Pedotti M-SS, Friedman D, Donoghue JP (2000): Learning-
human brain. Nature 406:147–150. induced LTP in neocortex. Science (80-) 290:533–536.
Hicks RE, Gualtieri CT, Schroeder SR (1983): Cognitive and motor Roth Y, Amir A, Levkovitz Y, Zangen A (2007): Three-dimen-
components of bilateral transfer. Am J Psychol 96:223. sional distribution of the electric field induced in the brain by
Hoy RR, Nolen TG, Casaday GC (1985): Dendritic sprouting and transcranial magnetic stimulation using figure-8 and deep H-
compensatory synaptogenesis in an identified interneuron fol- coils. J Clin Neurophysiol 24:31–38.
low auditory deprivation in a cricket. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S Sale MV, Ridding MC, Nordstrom MA (2008): Cortisol inhibits
A 82:7772–7776. neuroplasticity induction in human motor cortex. J Neurosci
Karni A, Meyer G, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, Ungerleider 28:8285–8293.
LG (1995): Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex Sanes JN, Donoghue JP (2000): Plasticity and primary motor cor-
plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature 377:155–158. tex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:393–415.
Kong NW, Gibb WR, Tate MC (2016): Neuroplasticity: Insights Schabrun SM, Stinear CM, Byblow WD, Ridding MC (2009): Nor-
from patients harboring gliomas. Neural Plast 2016: (2365063). malizing motor cortex representations in focal hand dystonia.
K€ubler A, Dixon V, Garavan H (2006): Automaticity and reestab- Cereb Cortex 19:1968–1977.
lishment of executive control-an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci Scholz J, Klein MC, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H (2009): Training
18:1331–1342. induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nat Neurosci 12:
Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutierrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, 1370–1371.
Evans A, Zilles K, Mazziotta JC, Fox PT (2007): Bias between Stefan K, Kunesch E, Cohen LG, Benecke R, Classen J (2000):
MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 Induction of plasticity in the human motor cortex by paired
brain template. Hum Brain Mapp 28:1194–1205. associative stimulation. Brain 123: 572–584.

r 14 r
r Widespread Training-Induced Changes in the Human Brain r

Taubert M, Draganski B, Anwander A, M€ uller K, Horstmann A, cerebral blood flow in both task and resting states. Neuro-
Villringer A, Ragert P (2010): Dynamic properties of image 45:75–82.
human brain structure: Learning-related changes in cortical Xu T, Yu X, Perlik AJ, Tobin WF, Zweig J. a, Tennant K, Jones T,
areas and associated fiber connections. J Neurosci 30:11670– Zuo Y (2009): Rapid formation and selective stabilization of
11677. synapses for enduring motor memories. Nature 462:915–919.
Teixeira L. a (2000): Timing and force components in bilateral Yushkevich P. a, Piven J, Hazlett HC, Smith RG, Ho S, Gee JC,
transfer of learning. Brain Cogn 44:455–469. Gerig G (2006): User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of
Thomas AG, Marrett S, Saad ZS, Ruff DA, Martin A, Bandettini anatomical structures: Significantly improved efficiency and
PA (2009): Functional but not structural changes associated reliability. Neuroimage 31:1116–1128.
with learning: An exploration of longitudinal Voxel-Based Zhang H, Schneider T, Wheeler-Kingshott C. a, Alexander DC (2012):
Morphometry (VBM). Neuroimage 48:117–125. NODDI: Practical in vivo neurite orientation dispersion and den-
Wahnoun R, Benson M, Helms-Tillery S, Adelson PD (2015): sity imaging of the human brain. Neuroimage 61:1000–1016.
Delineation of somatosensory finger areas using vibrotactile Ziemann U, Ilić TV, Pauli C, Meintzschel F, Ruge D (2004): Learn-
stimulation, an ECoG study. Brain Behav 5:e00369. ing modifies subsequent induction of long-term potentiation-
Xiong J, Ma L, Wang B, Narayana S, Duff EP, Egan GF, Fox PT like and long-term depression-like plasticity in human motor
(2009): Long-term motor training induced changes in regional cortex. J Neurosci 24:1666–1672.

r 15 r

You might also like