Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Creativity Research Journal

ISSN: 1040-0419 (Print) 1532-6934 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcrj20

A New Measure for the Assessment of


Appreciation for Creative Personality

Emanuel Jauk, Lisa Eberhardt, Corinna Koschmieder, Jennifer Diedrich,


Jürgen Pretsch, Mathias Benedek & Aljoscha C. Neubauer

To cite this article: Emanuel Jauk, Lisa Eberhardt, Corinna Koschmieder, Jennifer Diedrich,
Jürgen Pretsch, Mathias Benedek & Aljoscha C. Neubauer (2019) A New Measure for the
Assessment of Appreciation for Creative Personality, Creativity Research Journal, 31:2, 149-163,
DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2019.1606622

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2019.1606622

© 2019 The Author(s). Published with


license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.

Published online: 23 Jun 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1945

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 1 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hcrj20
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL, 31(2), 149–163, 2019
© 2019 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
ISSN: 1040-0419 print/1532-6934 online
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2019.1606622

A New Measure for the Assessment of Appreciation


for Creative Personality
Emanuel Jauk
University of Graz and Technische Universität Dresden

Lisa Eberhardt and Corinna Koschmieder


University of Graz

Jennifer Diedrich
University of Graz and Technical University of Munich

Jürgen Pretsch, Mathias Benedek , and Aljoscha C. Neubauer


University of Graz

This article introduces a new scale for the assessment of Appreciation for Creative Personality
(ACP). The ACP scale is a brief 13-item forced-choice measure that assesses interindividual
differences in the preference for interacting with creative people. ACP is considered an important
factor of creative climate at the level of interpersonal interaction. Individuals who score high on
ACP are thought to foster a creative climate in that they value creative traits in others. In two
studies, the psychometric characteristics of the ACP scale were probed. The scale showed a clear
unidimensional structure with evidence of good reliability and convergent, discriminant, and
criterion validity. The ACP was substantially related to Big Five openness to experience, but
predicted relevant criteria over and above openness, supporting the conceptual distinction between
ACP and openness. In dyadic data analyses, participants’ openness to experience was significantly
associated with their parents’ ACP, which shows that the ACP scale captured shared interpersonal
variance. Moreover, parental ACP indirectly predicted participants’ everyday creative activities via
the path of openness. These findings suggest that the ACP scale is a useful tool for the study of
social-environmental climate for creativity from an interpersonal perspective.

Everybody values creativity, but does everybody appreciate importance of environmental factors for the individual devel-
creative people? They can be nonconforming, eccentric, and opment and expression of creative potential (e.g., Eysenck,
rebellious, and possibly not always easy to live with. Yet, 1995; Guilford, 1950; Hennessey & Amabile, 2010; Rhodes,
creative people can only flourish in conducive environments. 1961). Calls have been made to emphasize social and cultural
Different influential models of creativity emphasize the factors in the study of creative environments (e.g., Glăveanu,
2010), which can show considerable variation and affect
creative outcomes (Chua, Roth, & Lemoine, 2015; Florida,
Address correspondence to Emanuel Jauk, Clinical Psychology and
2002). Though elaborate models on conducive conditions for
Behavioral Neuroscience, Technische Universität Dresden, Chemnitzer
Straße 46, Dresden 01187, Germany. E-mail: emanuel.jauk@tu-dresden.de creativity have been developed for particular contexts such as
Color versions of one or more of the figures in the article can be found organizational (Amabile, Conti, Coon, Lazenby, & Herron,
online at www.tandfonline.com/HCRJ. 1996; Anderson & West, 1998; Ekvall, 1996), educational
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative (e.g., Beghetto, 2010), or family settings (Kwaśniewska,
Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creati
Gralewski, Witkowska, Kostrzewska, & Lebuda, 2018),
vecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-
use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work there is to date no context-independent account to the assess-
is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way. ment of these conditions. Here, a scale is introduced that
150 JAUK ET AL.

assesses appreciation for creative personality as an interper- organizational structure or specifics of the task, they have
sonal, social-environmental factor for creativity. in common that they ascribe interpersonal aspects a central
All creative endeavors are embedded in a social context. role for the flourishing of creative endeavors. In particular,
The creative individual interacts with other people – be them all of these models highlight the crucial role of a safe
colleagues, bosses, teachers, or family – who can vary in surrounding which encourages individual spontaneity and
their appreciation for the creative personality. The immedi- creativity.
ate social-environmental conditions for creativity are usually In the educational context, different scholars have repeat-
referred to as climate for creativity (Hunter, Bedell, & edly stressed the importance of a valuing climate for crea-
Mumford, 2007). The acceptance of creative ideas, endea- tivity (Beghetto, 2010; Cropley & Urban, 2002; Guilford,
vors, and – ultimately – creative individuals is a key factor to 1950). Ideally, such a climate would promote students’
understanding how beneficial or obstructive certain social intrinsic motivation, creative self-efficacy, and intellectual
surroundings are for the development or expression of crea- risk taking, while avoiding common fallacies, such as con-
tivity (MacKinnon, 1978). Though most people display posi- vergent teaching, or misconceptions of what creativity actu-
tive attitudes towards creativity when being explicitly asked, ally is (i.e., expecting too much; Beghetto, 2010).
creative ideas are not always viewed in a positive light Unfortunately, this is not always the case. Empirical investi-
(Mueller, Melwani, & Goncalo, 2012). The extent of appre- gations showed that prospective teachers, for instance, prefer
ciation by people in their closest surrounding can impact the expected ideas over unexpected ideas (Beghetto, 2007). It
creative individual dramatically, which is for instance well- has been observed early that there is a discrepancy between
document in the family context (Miller & Gerard, 1979; the desirable qualities of creative individuals and teachers’
Simonton, 1984; see also Kwaśniewska et al., 2018). Thus, views of the “ideal pupil”, which is conceived as compliant
from an interpersonal perspective, appreciation for creative and conforming (Torrance, 1963). About 50 years later, in
personality by important others can be considered a relevant a similar vein, “creative” students were still viewed as less
social factor for people in various creative contexts, and agreeable and conscientious than “good” students
make up a building block for the study of creative climate. (Karwowski, 2010). Most strikingly, it was found that tea-
Previous research on creative climate focused on the orga- chers prefer uncreative students, even though they explicitly
nizational, educational, and family contexts. In the follow- endorse creativity as an educational goal (Westby &
ing, theoretical models and research from these three Dawson, 1995). This points to an important discrepancy
contexts will be reviewed. It will be highlighted that inter- between explicit cognitive attitudes towards creativity, and
personal factors are being ascribed a central role for creative rather implicit affect-laden preferences for actually interact-
climate in different models and contexts, which motivated ing with creative individuals. Or, as Runco (2007) put it:
the construction of the Appreciation for Creative Personality “No doubt they [teachers] do respect creativity, in the
scale. Finally, the construction and validation rationale of the abstract, but not when faced with a classroom with 30
scale will be presented. energetic children” (p. 172). The consequences of an anti-
In the workplace, a positive climate for creativity, as creative school climate can be drastic for the individual, as it
perceived by the creative person, is an important predictor was found that individual creativity can even increase the
of creative performance across different criteria, samples, odds of school dropout (Kim & Hull, 2012).
and settings (Hunter et al., 2007). For instance, in Most recently, a measure of creative climate was also
Amabile’s KEYS model for creativity in organizations, developed for the family context (Kwaśniewska et al.,
the interpersonal factors encouragement of creativity (orga- 2018). The scale comprises four theoretically derived fac-
nizational/supervisory/work group), or autonomy/freedom tors, namely Encouragement to Experience Novelty and
were found to be important predictors of creative perfor- Variety (“I try to suggest to my child unconventional
mance (Amabile et al., 1996). In a similar vein, the inter- ways to solve problems”, p. 19), Encouragement of
personal factors participative safety and support for Nonconformism (“I am glad that my child has been taught
innovation were considered central determinants of creativ- not to break any rules”, p. 19; reverse-coded), Support of
ity in West and colleagues research (e.g., Anderson & West, Perseverance in Creative Efforts (“I try to show my child
1998). Similarly, the nine-factor model by Ekvall (e.g., different sides of the same situation”, p. 19), and
Ekvall, 1996) emphasizes different interpersonal factors Encouragement to Fantasize (“I encourage my child to
such as idea support (“ideas and suggestions are received fantasize”, p. 19). In a large sample of mothers, these
in an attentive and supportive way by bosses and work- factors were found to be moderately to strongly associated
mates”, p. 107), trust (“the emotional safety in relation- with the Big Five dimension of openness to experience, but
ships”, p. 107), or playfulness (“the spontaneity and ease less so with any other Big Five dimension. Notably, the
that is displayed”, p. 108) as predictors of workplace crea- four factors devised by the authors largely parallel those of
tivity. Though all of these models also encompass context- the organizational models presented above, and also the
specific factors that concern characteristics such as the factors that are thought to promote creativity in the
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 151

classroom. They encompass an attitude that promotes personal preference for interacting with individuals who
autonomy and, at the same time, safety for creative endea- display prototypically creative vs. less creative characteris-
vors. These are believed to establish a climate that is con- tics (see method for details). Evidence of reliability and
ducive to the child’s creative development. Though the validity of the ACP scale was tested in two independent
scale shows evidence of good validity in terms of a match samples.
with existing theories on the factors promoting creativity, In study 1, general psychometric characteristics (relia-
there is yet no direct evidence whether parental creative bility and dimensionality) of the scale were assessed and its
climate actually goes along with higher creativity in their convergent, discriminant, and criterion validity were
children. probed. Given the similarity of ACP with Big Five open-
Taken together, the findings presented above suggest ness to experience (which includes the conceptually related
that research from different contexts (organizational, edu- facet openness to ideas), the new scale was expected to
cation, family) converges in that it assumes the existence of correlate substantially with openness in terms of convergent
interpersonal factors, which promote the flourishing of validity, but should show criterion validity over and above
creativity in the individual. Though expressed in different openness. In terms of discriminant validity, the ACP scale –
vocabularies, these factors describe the necessity of a safe conceptualized as an affect-laden measure of personality –
and accepting interpersonal climate for the expression of should be relatively independent from the cognitive ability
creative ideas, and a tolerance for spontaneity, unconven- to recognize creative ideas (Benedek et al., 2016; see
tionality, and nonconformity, which are inherent to the method for description). Finally, the ACP scale’s criterion
creative personality (Andreas, Zech, Coyle, & validity was probed using criteria from the context of
Rindermann, 2016; Batey & Furnahm, 2008; Eysenck, creative education (see method for details). It was tested
1995; Feist, 1998). While most previous research focused whether the ACP scale can predict relevant creative educa-
on self-perceptions of climate for creativity, here, an inter- tion criteria above and beyond openness and further related
personal perspective to the study of creative climate was variables, assuming conceptual specificity in terms of
adopted: The Appreciation for Creative Personality scale appreciating creative personality in others. Study 1 hence
was designed for assessing important others (such as undertook a test of the psychometric characteristics as well
bosses, teachers, or parents) in the surrounding of the as convergent, discriminant, and criterion validity of the
creative individual. In this, the approach taken here is ACP scale.
similar to that of Kwaśniewska et al. (2018), but not spe- Study 2 probed whether the ACP scale – intended as an
cific to parenting behavior. Rather, appreciation for creative interpersonal measure of social-environmental climate for
personality is conceptualized as a context-independent indi- creativity – can actually capture interpersonal variance. For
vidual difference variable. this, dyads composed of participants and one of their par-
Here, the development and analysis of a new scale for ents were sampled. Parents were chosen (rather than
Appreciation for Creative Personality (ACP) is reported. bosses, teachers, or other conceivable groups) as they
The scale was originally developed in the course of represent significant others for the sample under study
a larger project on the selection of prospective teachers in (see Study 2). It was investigated whether parental ACP
Austria (see Koschmieder, Weissenbacher, Pretsch, & actually impacts their children’s personality, creative poten-
Neubauer, 2018), but is not limited to the educational con- tial, and real-life creative behavior. For this, the model of
text, and thus applicable to a wide range of contexts. The real-life creative behavior proposed by Jauk, Benedek, and
ACP aims to assess the personal preference for social Neubauer (2014) was adopted as a theoretical framework
contact with creative individuals. It is an affect-laden mea- (see also Jauk, 2019). The model explains engagement in
sure of personality, which is known to impact perceptions creative activities by variation in openness and cognitive
of climate for creativity (Karwowski, 2011) and is different creative potential (divergent thinking ability). The direct
from explicit attitudes, which can be biased by social desir- and indirect effects of parental ACP on their children’s
ability (particularly in the educational context; Runco, openness, divergent thinking ability, and real-life creative
2007; Westby & Dawson, 1995). Since the measure was activity were tested. It was assumed that parental ACP
constructed for the use in an admission test context, the would impact participants’ personality (particularly open-
latent constructs should not be readily transparent to test- ness, the arguably most distinctive personality characteris-
takers in order to prevent faking. All of these goals moti- tic of creativity individuals; Batey, Chamorro-Premuzic, &
vated the development of the new ACP scale. Furnham, 2010; Dollinger, Urban, & James, 2004) in the
Items were devised through adaptation from similar first place, which would then influence their engagement in
inventories and theoretical considerations. These items creative activities (Jauk et al., 2014). Study 2 thus under-
reflect many of the factors reviewed above for the organi- took an empirical test of the ACP’s capability to catch
zational, educational, and family contexts. In 13 forced- shared between-person variance as an interpersonal mea-
choice items, test-takers are requested to indicate their sure of social-environmental creative climate.
152 JAUK ET AL.

STUDY 1: SCALE DEVELOPMENT format items.1 In contrast to the ACP scale, the CE vignette
scale specifically focused on the educational context:
Method Participants were to imagine being a teacher and indicate
which child they would prefer to teach. Item difficulties
Participants and procedure
were between p = .23 and p = .92; SDs ranged from 0.27 to
Data were collected at the University of Graz between 0.50. The internal consistency of the six-item scale was
December 2015 and January 2016. Complete data were α = .60 and did not change by exclusion of any single
available from 224 individuals (146 women; mean age item (including the easiest one with p = .92). The corrected
22.26 [SD = 4.29] years) who took part in an online survey item-scale-correlations ranged from r = .20 to r = .44.
administered via Limesurvey (www.limesurvey.org). The second measure, in the following referred to as CE
Participants had at least 12 years of schooling or a similar attitude, consisted of seven five-point rating scale items that
professional education. They gave written informed consent explicitly assessed preference for creative education (e.g.,
and took part on a voluntary basis. Students of psychology teaching a creative pupil, teaching a creative subject, dis-
(86.2%) received course credits in exchange for their parti- cussing ideas and theories with pupils…). Item difficulties
cipation. The study was approved by the ethics committee ranged from p = .49 to p = .81; SDs of these were between
of the University of Graz. 0.84 and 1.39 (rating scale metric). The measure displayed
an internal consistency of α = .77; exclusion of items would
have lowered scale reliability. Corrected item-scale-
Measures correlations ranged from r = .29 to r = .67. The correlation
between the CE vignette and CE attitude scales was r = .63,
For initial item selection of the ACP scale, a pool of 37 p < .001 (see Table 2), which provides evidence for their
items devised from existing self-report inventories (see next convergent validity. Taken together, the CE criterion scales
section) was used. All items were administered twice, using differ from the ACP in that they are context-specific and
a regular instruction and a fake-good instruction (“now, rely on explicit and overt assessment of the latent construct
please answer the questions in a way that leaves a good (only CE attitude). In the validation study reported here, the
impression when applying for a job in the educational CE scales were thus used to test whether the context-
sector. Your answers do not need to reflect your actual independent ACP scale displays evidence of validity in
personality.”). Moreover, the items were presented in the educational context (assessment of prospective tea-
forced-choice and rating scale response format. While chers) and across different response and measurement for-
both were found to show good psychometric characteris- mats (explicit and overt).
tics, subsequent analyses focus on the forced-choice format, Lastly, a creative attitude scale was administered. The
which seemed particularly suited for the admission context. scale has previously been constructed in the course of the
As convergent and discriminant validity measures, the project and is already implemented in the admission test for
German HEXACO-60 personality inventory (Ashton & prospective teachers in Austria (why the characteristics of
Lee, 2009) and the German Big Five Inventory (BFI; this scale cannot be disclosed here).
John & Srivastava, 1999) as well as the previously estab-
lished creativity evaluation test (CET; Benedek et al., 2016)
were used. In contrast to the ACP scale probed here, the Item generation
CET assesses the cognitive ability to accurately evaluate The items of the ACP reflect short descriptions of creative
the creativity of ideas. Specifically, participants’ task in the persons, based on theoretical and empirical characteriza-
CET is to classify answers from a divergent thinking task tions of the creative personality (e.g., Batey & Furnham,
(for instance, alternate uses for a hat) as common (“using 2006, 2008; Eysenck, 1995; Feist, 1998). We generated or
a hat for collecting donations”), inappropriate (“using a hat modified items based on existing self-report measures,
as cooking pot”), or creative (“using a hat as a Frisbee”). It which encompassed the openness subscale from the Big
was expected that the ACP, conceptualized as an affect- Five Aspects Scale (BFAS; DeYoung, Quilty, & Peterson,
laden measure of personality, would be largely independent 2007), the Runco Ideational Behavior Scale (RIBS; Runco,
from the CET, as the cognitive ability for creativity evalua- Plucker, & Lim, 2001), the Creative Attitude Survey
tion was found to draw more upon cognitive than person- (Schaefer, 1991), and the Everyday Creativity Self-
ality factors (Benedek et al., 2016). The CET was thus
included as a discriminant validity measure.
1
For criterion validity assessment in the context of crea- The forced-choice format was chosen instead of a rating scale format
tive education (CE), two specifically constructed short to eliminate the general preference for teaching children. An example item
is: “An ambitious and diligent child, who always follows the teacher’s
scales were used. The first scale, in the following referred instructions, but does not usually come up with own ideas” vs. “An active
to as CE vignette, comprised vignettes of more and less and rebellious child, who does not always follow the teacher’s instruc-
creative children that were contrasted in six forced-choice tions, but has many own ideas”.
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 153

Concept Scale (Ivcevic, 2014; see also Patston, Cropley, TABLE 1


Marrone, & Kaufman, 2017). Item-level statistics of the Appreciation for Creative Personality
(ACP) scale (study 1)
For the initial item selection pool, 37 items were derived
in the following manner: The core content, for instance Regular instruction Fake-Good-Instruction
“my ideas are often considered ‘impractical’ or even
Item No. p (SD) ri-s rCE_v rCE_a p (SD)
‘wild’” (from the RIBS; Runco et al., 2001, p. 399), was
put in the third person “his/her ideas are often considered 1 .64 (.48) .38 .36 .27 .53 (.50) .61 .12 .15
‘impractical’ or even ‘wild’” (item no. 2). For the forced- 2 .43 (.50) .49 .38 .31 .26 (.44) .50 .19 .18
choice format, then, a reverse-coded alternative was con- 3 .61 (.49) .41 .28 .42 .66 (.48) .51 .15 .17
4 .74 (.44) .27 .28 .32 .77 (.42) .39 .13 .14
structed in the same manner (“his/her suggestions are
5 .49 (.50) .38 .29 .32 .54 (.50) .48 .16 .14
usually established ones, which have worked before.”). In 6 .71 (.46) .36 .30 .33 .59 (.49) .47 .18 .14
cases where the core content appeared highly socially 7 .57 (.50) .59 .40 .40 .39 (.49) .54 .19 .20
desirable (for instance “enjoys the beauty of nature”; 8 .67 (.47) .54 .30 .38 .59 (.49) .55 .19 .22
adapted from the BFAS openness scale; DeYoung et al., 9 .60 (.49) .61 .39 .33 .47 (.50) .59 .17 .11
10 .63 (.48) .21 .21 .17 .58 (.49) .43 .17 .09
2007), a less desirable aspect was added (“often stops his/
11 .39 (.49) .32 .21 .22 .47 (.50) .50 .15 .17
her car to photograph nature and is occasionally late 12 .74 (.44) .23 .11 .16 .63 (.49) .51 .14 .13
because of it”; item no. 1) to achieve balanced item 13 .79 (.41) .33 .20 .14 .63 (.48) .50 .18 .13
difficulty.2
Note. Correlations above .17, .13, and .11 are significant at p < .01,
The final set of 13 items (following item selection; see
p < .05, and p < .10, respectively. The item content can be found in Table A2.
results section) including instructions to participants is pro-
vided in the Appendix (Tables A1 and A2). Participants’
task in the ACP is to indicate with whom of the two
described persons they would prefer to have contact with. scale’s internal consistency. The scale skewness
The ACP is available in a validated German version. For (z = −1.68) did not deviate from normality at p < .01, the
research purposes, an English translation (translated by two scale kurtosis (z = −2.70; p < .01) indicated a slightly
professionals), which is not yet validated, is also provided. platykurtic distribution. A principal components analysis
For scoring the ACP, the forced-choice response alterna- yielded four components with eigenvalues > 1 which
tives A and B are assigned 0 and 1 points; three items need accounted for 53.14% of variance; the first component
to be recoded first (see Tables A1 and A2). A total score is alone accounted for 27.87%. Velicer’s (1976) original and
obtained by averaging across all 13 items. revised (Velicer, Eaton, & Fava, 2000) MAP test indicated
a one-factor solution. This solution was confirmed by
a confirmatory factor analysis for dichotomous indicators
(WLSMV) which showed excellent fit to the data (χ2
RESULTS (65) = 75.48, p = .18; CFI = 0.99; WRMR = 0.77).
Figure 1 depicts the confirmatory factor analysis model.
Item selection, internal consistency, and factor
structure
Out of the initial pool of 37 items, 13 Items that best met Validity evidence
a combination of several criteria were selected: (1) item
Table 2 provides correlations between the ACP scale and
difficulty between .20 < pi < .80, (2) maximal correlation
with both CE criterion variables (vignette and attitude) convergent, discriminant, and criterion validity measures.
under (2a) regular instruction and (2b) fake-good instruc- Among the HEXACO personality dimensions, the ACP
scale displayed a high correlation with openness (conver-
tion, and (3) maximal discriminatory power (item-scale-
correlation). Table 1 displays item characteristics and cri- gent validity) and low correlations with honesty-humility as
terion correlations. The 13-item-scale displayed an internal well as extraversion. A similar pattern of results emerged
for the BFI (except honesty-humility, which is not covered
consistency of α = .77 which can be deemed acceptable.
Exclusion of any single item would have lowered the by the Big Five). Additional facet-level analyses using the
HEXACO showed that the ACP’s factor-level correlation
with openness generalized to all openness facets (aesthetic
2
While these items include two latent traits (in this case, openness to appreciation: r = .30, p < .001; inquisitiveness: r = .36,
aesthetics, and tardiness), which could be considered problematic in stan- p < .001; creativity: r = .41, p < .001; unconventionality:
dard psychometric testing, the item content of the ACP does not refer to r = .54, p < .001). The factor-level correlation of honesty-
the test-taker, but to a prototypically creative person. As creative person-
ality is a multidimensional construct including also less desirable traits,
humility was driven by the subscales greed avoidance
this seems adequate here. Importantly, test-takers’ ACP can still be uni- (r = .32, p < .001), the correlation of extraversion by social
dimensional (see results section). boldness (r = .18, p < .01). As expected, the ACP scale
154 JAUK ET AL.

TABLE 2
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of study 1 variables

M (SD) 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16

Appreciation for Creative Pers.


ACP (1) 0.62 (0.24) .12 .56 .56 .11 .18 −.08 .17 .13 .03 .54 −.13 .14 .07 .02 .57
ACP fake good (2) 0.55 (0.29) .27 .25 −.04 .05 −.05 .14 .04 −.01 .05 −.08 .10 .03 −.02 .05
Study validity measures
CE Vignette (3) 3.54 (0.38) .63 .11 .23 −.12 .14 .06 −.02 .33 −.10 .13 .03 .04 .45
CE Attitude (4) 3.32 (0.65) .02 .22 −.11 .16 .08 .05 .42 −.14 .15 .04 .08 .62
Creativity Evaluation Test (5) 75.17 (8.77) .00 .15 −.08 −.07 .08 .03 .05 −.10 .05 .08 −.01
HEXACO Dimensions
Honesty-Humility (6) 3.51 (0.68) .09 .03 .27 .10 .20 −.01 .04 .36 .09 .16
Emotionality (7) 3.33 (0.71) −.33 .02 .01 −.04 .67 −.23 .24 −.07 −.15
Extraversion (8) 3.49 (0.70) .06 .07 .04 −.58 .86 .17 .32 .18
Agreeableness (9) 3.20 (0.58) −.02 .13 −.23 −.01 .69 −.03 .09
Conscientiousness (10) 3.59 (0.68) .07 .04 .07 .10 .79 .19
Openness to Experience (11) 3.57 (0.64) .01 .05 .02 .01 .73
Big Five Dimensions
Neuroticism (12) 2.88 (0.80) −.43 −.15 −.16 −.12
Extraversion (13) 3.43 (0.82) .14 .33 .16
Agreeableness (14) 3.65 (0.55) .12 −.03
Conscientiousness (15) 3.54 (0.67) .17
Openness to Experience (16) 3.61 (0.69)

Note. N = 224 for all variables. Correlations significant at p < .05 are printed in bold type. ACP = Appreciation for Creative Personality. CE = creative
education.

displayed a low and insignificant (p = .11) association with instruction (see Table 2) was slightly lower (t223 = 2.92,
the CET (discriminant validity). Criterion correlations with p < .01), not higher. Table 2 displays scale-level validity
the CE variables were generally high (note, however, that information. The ACP displayed some indication of criter-
these were included as item selection criteria; see above). ion validity under fake-good instruction in terms of mod-
Next, the incremental criterion validity of the ACP scale erate correlations with the CE criteria. It was also related to
on the CE criteria over and above the HEXACO personality HEXACO extraversion under fake-good instruction.
dimensions and the CET was investigated. For the CE vign-
ette criterion, hierarchical multiple regression models
showed that honesty-humility and openness were significant
predictors, but extraversion was no longer significant when DISCUSSION
considered simultaneously (see Table 3; model 1). The CET
could also explain additional variance as long as the ACP Study 1 probed the psychometric properties of the new ACP
was not considered (model 2). In the final model, the ACP scale. The short scale turned out to meet high psychometric
outperformed openness in predicting CE vignettes (model 3). standards in terms of internal consistency, unidimensionality,
The CE attitude criterion was predicted by openness and and validity. As expected for convergent and discriminant
conscientiousness in model 1. Again, the CET could explain validity, the ACP was correlated mainly with the personality
incremental variance (model 2). Finally, all these variables dimension of openness (and all of its facets) and was inde-
and the ACP conjointly predicted CE attitude in model 3. pendent of the cognitive ability to recognize creative ideas
(CET). This means that ACP was related to, but still distinct
from the personality dimension of openness, and also differ-
ent from the cognitive ability to evaluate creativity (discri-
Faking
minant validity evidence). Both of these results confirm the
Under fake-good instruction, itemwise criterion correla- hypothesized nature of ACP as an affect-laden measure of
tions were reduced, but still mostly different from zero personality that is related to yet separable from own creativ-
(see Table 1). The ACP displayed somewhat higher internal ity-related traits (openness) and the cognitive ability to
consistency (α = .85) under fake-good instruction than recognize creativity in others. Of relevance to the use in
under regular instruction. There was no correlation between the assessment context, faking analyses indicated that the
the ACP under fake-good and regular instruction condi- latent construct was not readily transparent to test-takers as
tions. Interestingly, the scale mean under fake-good scores did not increase, and the scale retained some of its
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 155

ACP_1
ecologically valid context, study 2 investigated the scale’s
.53
validity in parent-child-dyads.
ACP_2 .75

ACP_3 .56
STUDY 2: SCALE VALIDATION
ACP_4 .40
The main aim of study 2 was to validate the ACP using
data not only from the participants themselves, but also
ACP_5 .54
from important others from their social environment. For
.59
this, participants’ parents’ ACP scores were used, as par-
ACP_6
Appreciation ents are arguably the most influential others during younger
.83 for age (student sample). It was investigated whether partici-
ACP_7
pants’ personality, creative potential, and real-life creative
Creative
.74 behavior are correlated with their parents’ ACP scores. For
ACP_8 Personality this, the model of Jauk et al. (2014) was adopted as
.89 a conceptual framework, in which the engagement in crea-
ACP_9 tive activities is explained by openness and creative poten-
.32 tial. In particular, this model suggests that openness, a key
ACP_10 personality variable for creativity (Batey et al., 2010;
.47
Dollinger et al., 2004), lowers the behavioral threshold
ACP_11 (Feist & Barron, 2003) for the exertion of creative activ-
.36
ities, which makes openness a potential mediating factor
ACP_12 .54 between parental ACP and participants’ real-life creative
activity. Thus, a mediation model corresponding to the one
ACP_13 presented in Jauk et al. (2014) was tested.

FIGURE 1 Confirmatory factor analysis model of the Appreciation for


Creative Personality scale (study 1). See study 1 results for model fit
information. METHOD

Participants and procedure


validity even under fake-good-instruction (which can be
considered a worst-case scenario). Study 2 used an independent sample of participants and
Most importantly, the ACP predicted relevant criteria their parents. Data were gathered between November and
over and above openness to experience, despite its sub- December 2016 in computer-based individual or small
stantial correlation with openness. This makes the ACP group test sessions. Participants were approached by stu-
a useful tool for the study of creative climate at individual dents of the University of Graz. Participants were asked to
level: Individuals scoring higher on the ACP displayed nominate one of their parents (if possible, the one they had
a higher preference for teaching creative children (CE had more and closer contact with) to take part in a short
vignette) and endorsed creative education on an explicit survey. They were asked to convey parents’ email
level (CE attitude). The criterion validity of the ACP was addresses; parents were then sent a link to an online survey
higher for the CE vignette than for the CE attitude criter- (via Limesurvey; as in study 1).
ion. This might be attributed to common method variance The final sample consisted of 190 datasets including data
among the ACP and the CE vignette criterion, as both from participants (114 women) and one of their parents
used a forced-choice response format. However, this is (father or mother). The mean age was 21.95 years
only true for the multiple regression model (see Table 3), (SD = 4.40). Similar as in study 1, 96.80% of participants
but not for the zero-order correlations (see Table 2). Thus, had at least 12 years of schooling or a similar professional
an alternative explanation might be that the CE attitude education; 83.70% self-identified as students. Parent data
criterion, as compared to the CE vignette criterion, could stemmed mostly from participants’ mothers (148 datasets,
be more readily explained by the personality variables or 77.90%). The mean age was 52.61 (SD = 5.40) years,
included in the regression model. 97.40% of parents had at least 12 years of schooling or
As a potential limitation, study 1 demonstrated criterion a similar professional education; 88.90% of parents
validity only with respect to fictitious vignettes and attitude reported current employment. The study was approved by
ratings. To further probe the validity of the ACP scale in an the ethics committee of the University of Graz.
156 JAUK ET AL.

TABLE 3
Hierarchical multiple regression models for the prediction of creative education criteria (study 1)

CE Vignette CE Attitude

Model 1 β p β p

Honesty-Humility .20 < .001 −.02 .71


Emotionality −.09 .20 −.01 .85
Extraversion .10 .12 .02 .81
Agreeableness −.04 .58 −.10 .11
Conscientiousness −.07 .29 .19 < .001
Openness to Experience .30 < .001 .40 < .001
Δ R2adj = .17, p < .001 Δ R2adj = .21, p < .001

Model 2

Honesty-Humility .20 < .001 −.02 .71


Emotionality −.10 .12 −.04 .56
Extraversion .11 .10 .02 .73
Agreeableness −.03 .69 −.09 .16
Conscientiousness −.08 .22 .17 .01
Openness to Experience .29 < .001 .40 < .001
Creativity Evaluation Test .13 .04 .18 < .001
Δ R2 adj = .02, p = .04 Δ R2adj = .03, p = .04

Model 3

Honesty-Humility .16 .01 −.04 .50


Emotionality −.08 .16 −.03 .67
Extraversion .04 .53 −.02 .77
Agreeableness −.05 .40 −.10 .10
Conscientiousness −.06 .29 .18 < .001
Openness to Experience .04 .55 .26 < .001
Creativity Evaluation Test .07 .22 .15 .01
ACP .49 .00 .28 < .001
Δ R2adj = .16, p = .00 Δ R2adj = .05, p = .00

Note. CE = Creative education. ACP = Appreciation for Creative Personality (ACP).

Measures Intelligence was assessed using three scales (verbal,


numerical, and figural) of the German Intelligence
Participants completed a German version of the Big Five
Structure Analysis (Intelligenzstrukturanalyse; Institut für
Aspects Scales (BFAS; DeYoung et al., 2007). The BFAS
Test- und Begabungsfoschung (ITB), & Gittler, 1998).
differentiate two aspects within each broad personality
The three scales were z-standardized and averaged to
dimension. With respect to the broad openness dimension,
form a measure of general intelligence.
these are openness and intellect. While openness is more
Finally, real-life creative activities and achievements
strongly related to cognitive processes underlying creativ-
were assessed using the Inventory of Creative Activities
ity, intellect is more tied to intelligence-related demands
and Achievements (ICAA; Diedrich et al., 2018). The
(Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011).
ICAA measures creative activities, defined in terms of
Creative potential was assessed using three divergent
everyday creative engagement, and creative achievement,
thinking tasks: alternate uses (find creative uses for a car
defined as socially recognized creative accomplishments,
tire), figure completion (imagine creative drawings starting
across eight domains: literature, music, creative cooking,
from a square), and instances (what can be open). Time-on-
arts & crafts, sports, visual arts, performing arts, and
task was three minutes for each task. Divergent thinking
science & engineering. The achievements scale of the
tasks were scored with respect to ideational fluency (num-
ber of ideas) and originality (rated originality of ideas; three
trained raters). The average rating of all ideas generated 3
As an alternative score, max3 scoring, an adaptation of the top3
during the three minutes on each task was used as an scoring method (Benedek, Mühlmann, Jauk, & Neubauer, 2013; Silvia,
indicator of ideational originality.3 Interrater-reliability of 2011) in which the three ideas with the highest ratings are averaged to an
indicator of divergent thinking ability, was also probed. Average scoring
the originality scores were .78, .70, and .72 for the alternate
and max3 scoring did not differ with respect to the effects of interest in
uses, figure completion, and instances tasks. this study.
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 157

ICAA is similar to the Creative Achievement Questionnaire and intelligence correlated in an expected manner. There
(CAQ; Carson, Peterson, & Higgins, 2005) and is particu- was no direct association between parents’ ACP and parti-
larly suited to capture variation in accomplished samples. cipants’ creative activities or achievements as measured by
The activities scale captures variation in everyday creativity the ICAA. Associations between latent constructs are
and can be used in nonprofessional samples (Diedrich et al., reported in the following.
2018). The following analyses, thus, focus on the prediction
of creative activities. These may be studied either in
a domain-general way (i.e., to investigate the factors deter- Mediation model
mining any kind of creative activity, across domains; see
also Jauk et al., 2014) or in a domain-specific way Based on the findings presented above and previous models
(Diedrich et al., 2018). Here, a domain-general model is of creativity (e.g., Jauk et al., 2014), a latent variable
presented first, and domain-specific results are reported in mediation model was set up to test whether parents’ ACP
a complementary analysis (see results). might predict creative activities via the openness or intel-
Participants’ parents completed the ACP scale, as lect aspects from the BFAS. Specifically, it was assumed
described in study 1. The reliability of the 13-item scale that openness would act as a personality mediator between
was α = .74, similar to study 1. Figure 2 displays the factor social-environmental climate (here: parental ACP) and the
structure, which is also similar to study 1. exertion of creative activities, as openness is thought to
lower the behavioral threshold for engaging in creative
activities (Feist & Barron, 2003; see also Jauk et al., 2014).
The ACP scale was modeled itemwise using WLSMV
RESULTS
for dichotomous indicators, as in study 1. Openness and
intellect were also modeled itemwise; the ICAA activities
Dyadic parent-participant correlations
scale was modeled using two item parcels in exact accor-
Table 4 shows the associations between the study variables. dance with Jauk et al. (2014). The model showed good fit to
Parents’ ACP significantly correlated with their childrens’ the data (χ2(555) = 740.49, p = .00; CFI = 0.81;
openness to experience, but not any other personality WRMR = 1.08); though the χ2 test was significant, the
dimension. Closer inspection revealed that this correlation ratio of χ2/df was well below 2 (Byrne, 1989). All factor
was due to the openness aspect (r = .22, p < .01), not the loadings were significant at p < .001; loadings of the ACP
intellect aspect (r = .05, p = .46). Parents’ ACP was were generally of similar magnitude to those obtained in
independent of their childrens’ divergent thinking ability study 1 (see Figure 2). The residual correlation between
(fluency and originality) or intelligence. Divergent thinking openness and intellect captures the shared variance among

parents … .28 - .65 participants


ACP_1
.45

ACP_2 .67 BFAS


Openness
ACP_3 .65

ACP_4 .46 .28 .41


resopen

ACP_5 .41

ACP_6
.63
Appreciation .38 .75 Act.
.68 for .01 Creative Parcel 1
ACP_7 .92
Creative Activities Act.
.60 Parcel 2
ACP_8 Personality
.74
ACP_9 .07 .00
.20
resintell
ACP_10
.53
ACP_11
.56
BFAS
ACP_12 .60 Intellect
ACP_13

… .44 - .78

FIGURE 2 Latent mediation model (study 2). See study 2 results for model fit information. Coefficients significant at p < .05 are displayed in bold type in
the structural model part. All factor loadings are significant at p < .001 except ACP_10, which is significant at p < .05 (single factor loadings of openness and
intellect are not displayed). ACP = Appreciation for Creative Personality. BFAS = Big Five Aspects Scale. resopen = residual term openness, resintell
= residual term intellect. ICAA = Inventory of Creative Activities and Achievements.
158 JAUK ET AL.

TABLE 4
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of study 2 variables

M (SD) 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Appreciation for Creative Pers. (parents)


ACP (1) 0.56 (0.23) −.04 −.01 .08 −.07 .17 .09 .13 .08 .12 .03

Big Five Dimensions (participants)


Neuroticism (2) 2.80 (0.65) −.30 −.17 −.25 −.16 −.20 −.15 .03 .07 −.07
Extraversion (3) 3.60 (0.49) .21 .24 .45 .06 −.02 .03 .14 .22
Agreeableness (4) 4.01 (0.42) .08 .24 −.01 .12 .01 .15 .09
Conscientiousness (5) 3.13 (0.50) .04 .03 −.15 −.03 −.06 −.03
Openness/Intellect (6) 3.77 (0.52) .30 .23 .25 .31 .42
Cognitive Abilities (participants)
General Intelligence (7) 0.00 (0.73) .31 .19 .10 .22
Divergent Thinking Originality (8) 0.82 (0.20) .05 −.09 .20
Divergent Thinking Fluency (9) 8.54 (3.19) .13 .12
Real-Life Creative Behavior (participants)
Creative Activities (10) 65.49 (27.01) .53
Creative Achievements (11) 51.53 (35.50)

Note. N = 190 for all variables. ACP = Appreciation for Creative Personality. The first row presents dyadic correlations between participants’ personality
and their parents’ ACP scores. See method study 2 for more detailed information. Bold indicates significance at p < .05.

both variables, which are regarded as aspects of a higher creative activities in the domains literature, music, arts &
order openness factor (DeYoung et al., 2007), controlling crafts, visual arts, and performing arts.
for ACP.
Figure 2 shows the latent mediation model parameter
estimates. Parents’ ACP had a significant effect on partici- DISCUSSION
pants’ openness (β = .28; p < .01), but not intellect (β = .07;
p = .43). Participants’ openness, in turn, was significantly Study 2 showed that there is a significant association
associated with their amount of creative activities (β = .41; between parents’ ACP and participants’ openness. This
p < .001). Intellect was not associated with creative activities result was specific to openness rather than any other per-
(β = .00; p = .99). The direct effect of ACP on creative sonality aspect, including intellect. This shows that the
activities was not significant (β = .01; p = .95), but there was ACP scale could capture shared variance between indivi-
a significant indirect effect via openness (β = .11; p = .01). duals, making it a candidate tool for the study of the social-
As an additional exploratory analysis, a domain-specific environmental climate for creativity from an interpersonal
model in which parents’ ACP was related to participants’ perspective. Moreover, the latent mediation model showed
openness and intellect, as well as the eight ICAA subscales that parents’ ACP affected participants’ exertion of creative
(see Table 5), was investigated. Modeling of the ACP and activities, indirectly, via the path of openness. This means
openness/intellect personality dimensions corresponds that parental ACP, in terms of a social-environmental factor
exactly to the model reported above. The ICAA subscales for creative climate, selectively impacted the personality
were entered as manifest criterion variables in order to dimension of openness, which, in turn, had an effect on
avoid overparameterization of the model. The model con- the engagement in creative activities. The mediation effect
verged to an admissible solution and showed similar fit to is of moderate size but quite notable given that it represents
the data as the model reported above (χ2(732) = 938.02, covariation across parent-child dyads. Complemental
p = .00; CFI = 0.81; WRMR = 1.03). Table 5 displays the domain-specific analyses showed that parental ACP selec-
coefficient estimates for the effects of openness and intel- tively supported the exertion of creative activities in arts-
lect on the ICAA subscales, as well as the direct and related domains, but not in creative cooking, sports, or
indirect effect of ACP on the subscales. All ICAA sub- science & engineering. While this yields evidence for the
scales except sports and science & engineering were sig- criterion validity of ACP in the arts, the missing correlation
nificantly related to openness; literature and science & with science & engineering could be seen as a limiting
engineering were significantly related to intellect. As in factor (see limitations section for discussion).
the domain-general model presented above, the ACP did While parental ACP was associated with participants’
not display direct effects on any of the ICAA subscales, but personality and, indirectly, behavioral tendencies (creative
did show significant indirect effects via openness on activities), the ACP did not affect any cognitive measures
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 159

TABLE 5
Domain-specific model for the prediction of participants’ creative activity by means of openness and intellect, their parents’ ACP, and the
indirect effects of parents’ ACP via participants’ openness and intellect

Literature Music Arts & Crafts Creative Cooking Sports Visual Arts Performing Arts Science & Engineering

Openness .36 .43 .43 .21 .05 .37 .38 −.10


Intellect .21 −.05 −.12 −.06 .01 −.07 −.10 .23
ACP direct −.02 −.02 .02 .07 −.04 −.04 .03 .02
ACP via Openness .10 .12 .12 .06 .02 .10 .10 −.03
ACP via Intellect .02 −.04 −.01 .00 .00 −.01 −.01 .02

Note. Standardized regression weights are displayed. Coefficients marked in bold are significant at p < .05. ACP = Appreciation for Creative Personality.

such as creative potential or general intelligence. This indi- endeavors. However, further research is needed to clarify
cates that ACP is specifically related to personality traits this link.
rather than cognitive abilities, which might be fostered by The ACP scale was largely independent (discriminant
other factors. validity) of the cognitive ability to recognize creative ideas
as measured by the previously published CET (Benedek
et al., 2016). This supports the conceptualization of ACP as
an affect-laden measure of preference for interacting with
GENERAL DISCUSSION creative people, which is different from the cognitive abil-
ity to recognize creative products or people. Yet, both
Many contemporary models of creativity emphasize the variables simultaneously predicted creative education atti-
role of a valuing social environment in terms of a positive tude (see Table 3). This indicates that both measures cap-
climate for the development and expression of human crea- ture distinct and relevant portions of variance in creative
tivity. Yet, to date, there is no context-independent measure education attitude that are more related to personality and
that allows for the quantification of the social- ability aspects, respectively. This pattern of results suggests
environmental climate for creativity on an individual that ACP and CET scales are best used together in contexts
level. Here, the construction and validation of a new mea- where both aspects are relevant, such as in the educational
sure for the assessment of Appreciation for Creative or also in the organizational context. In both of these
Personality, the ACP scale, were reported. The scale allows contexts, teachers or bosses are not only required to
for the quantification of social-environmental conditions for appreciate creativity on the personality level, but also to
creative individuals from an interpersonal perspective. correctly recognize creative ideas or individuals as such.
The ACP scale is a concise, one-dimensional measure of Under fake-good instruction, the ACP largely lost its
appreciation for creative personality that meets high psy- validity, although there was still some evidence for criterion
chometric standards. Validity analyses indicated that the validity (see Table 1). These correlations, however, should
scale, as expected, correlated highly with the Big Five not be overrated, as they were part of the item selection
trait openness to experience (convergent validity). Yet, it process (see study 1 methods) and would possibly not
cannot be reduced to openness, as it displayed incremental replicate in an independent sample. More importantly, how-
criterion validity in the prediction of relevant criteria (pre- ever, itemwise faking analyses indicated that the latent
ference for creative education). This means that, while open construct is not readily apparent to test-takers, as some
people are generally also open to social interactions with item means increased, while others decreased under fake-
creative people, ACP can be useful to predict creativity- good instruction (see Table 1). This notion is further sup-
related outcomes (CE criteria in study 1) over and above ported by qualitative information from test-takers during
openness. Besides openness, the scale also displayed small the pilot phase, who were unable to identify the latent
correlations with honesty-humility and extraversion. Closer construct. This makes the ACP a useful tool that is not
inspection revealed that the correlation with extraversion limited to research purposes but is also suited for the
could be explained by the social boldness facet. Individuals diagnostic context in terms of low susceptibility to socially
scoring higher on social boldness might be more at ease desirable responding.
facing the nonconforming aspects of creative individuals. Taken together, the ACP differs from existing measures
The correlation with honesty-humility was due to the facet of creative climate in two ways: First, it takes an interper-
greed avoidance, which circumscribes a low interest in sonal approach to the study of creative climate by directly
acquiring wealth, luxury, and social status (Lee & Ashton, assessing individuals from the social surrounding of the
2004). It seems plausible that a low interest in these mate- creative individual. This is what differentiates the ACP
rialistic goals also implies valuing intellectual and creative from organizational measures of creative climate, such as
160 JAUK ET AL.

Amabile and colleagues’ KEYS (Amabile et al., 1996) or measured with a longer and more nuanced scale (e.g.,
conceptually similar inventories (Anderson & West, 1998; Kwaśniewska et al., 2018). However, as long as a single
Ekvall, 1996). Second, the ACP was designed as a context- score is needed, as it was the case in the admission test
independent measure that targets the level of personality context, the short scale seems well suited. Moreover, while
traits rather than attitudes in a specific context. As such, the appreciation of creative personality arguably represents
ACP is different from Kwaśniewska et al. (2018) recently a fundamental aspect of the social climate for creativity at
published Climate for Creativity in Parent-Child the interpersonal level, a more comprehensive assessment
Relationship Questionnaire. The scale presented here aims may consider further factors such as trust, or the readiness
to assess ACP as a personality trait in a behaviorally to support and cooperate with creative people.
anchored manner (“indicate with whom of them you Although the parent-participant correlation between ACP
would prefer to have contact with”; see Table A2). This and openness obtained in study 2 is encouraging, the data are
makes the ACP not only a versatile instrument that can be of cross-sectional nature and thus cannot speak to causality. It
used in different contexts, but also goes along with a low cannot be inferred that parents’ ACP causally influences their
susceptibility to socially desirable responding (as it is of offsprings’ openness. It could also be the case that parents
relevance for instance in the educational context; Runco, who have open and creative children develop higher ACP
2007; Westby & Dawson, 1995) (i.e., adjust their preference for creative personality in accor-
Study 2 found that – in parent-child dyads – parents’ ACP dance to their childrens’ traits). Though the former causal path
predicted their childrens’ openness, and, in turn, everyday may presumably be stronger than the latter, the issue must
creative activities. While openness can be conceived an impor- remain subject to future studies. Also, common genetic factors
tant personality prerequisite for creative activity (Jauk et al., could account for part of the shared variance. Future studies
2014), the findings reported here add to previous literature in should thus validate the ACP also in the organizational or
that they provide evidence for an indirect effect from parents’ educational context, where common genetic variance is not an
ACP to their childrens’ creative activities via the path of open- issue. Importantly, irrespective of the causal nature of the
ness. This yields an interesting insight into the putative devel- findings reported here, the data demonstrate that the ACP
opmental conditions for creative behavior, which might be can successfully capture interindividual covariance, which
indirectly promoted by parents who are accepting and suppor- renders it a useful tool for the study of social environments
tive towards their creative children (Kwaśniewska et al., 2018). for creativity.
It was also found that, while parents’ ACP impacted their Due to time restrictions and practicability, no personality
childrens’ openness, there was no association with intellect, traits apart from ACP were assessed in participants’ parents
which demonstrates the specificity of the effect and conforms to in study 2. Above all, it would have been interesting to
prior research (Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011). However, personality include Big Five openness to investigate incremental valid-
demands may vary between different fields of creative endea- ity of the ACP. However, incremental validity of the ACP
vor (Kaufman et al., 2016), and complementary domain- beyond openness could be established in study 1. Future
specific analysis indicated that the results obtained here might research could directly examine the incremental validity of
only be valid for artistic creativity (see limitations). the ACP above and beyond Big Five traits in interpersonal
Taken together, the correlation between parents’ ACP and datasets. Such studies could be carried out in organizations,
their childrens’ personality shows that the ACP scale can schools, or families, and might help to unveil important
capture covariance between a target person and individuals aspects of the social-environmental climate for the devel-
from their respective social environment. This opens many opment and expression of creativity.
possibilities to the use of the ACP scale as a measure of Lastly, further research is needed on the validity of the
creative climate, such as for instance the quantification of ACP in different domains; particularly in the arts and
ACP in organizations, schools, or families. The ACP scores sciences, which go along with different demands for crea-
of single individuals or groups of individuals might then be tive personality (e.g., Kaufman et al., 2016). The subscale
related to creative outcome variables in other groups of analyses of the ICAA showed that parental ACP is asso-
individuals (for instance employees, pupils, or children). ciated with their children’s creative activities in the arts, but
not in science & engineering. This can be seen as
a potential limitation of the ACP scale to capture apprecia-
Limitations
tion for the personality of people interested in scientific
Though the ACP scale displayed good psychometric prop- creativity. It has to be noted, though, that common mea-
erties in the studies reported here, there are some limita- sures of everyday creativity also tend to show a broader
tions to the measure. First, the scale is rather short. While coverage of various artistic domains, maybe due to their
this allows for an economic assessment of the main con- higher prevalence in non-professional samples (Diedrich
struct, it does not allow to disentangle more fine-grained et al., 2018). This renders the comparison between arts
facets, which might emerge when the construct was and sciences unbalanced in this study. Future studies
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 161

could use in the ACP in dedicated artistic and scientific Beghetto, R. A. (2007). Does creativity have a place in classroom discus-
professionals. sion? Prospective teachers‘ response preferences. Thinking Skills and
Creativity, 2, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2006.09.002
Beghetto, R. A. (2010). Creativity in the classroom. In J. C. Kaufman &
R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 1447–1463). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Benedek, M., Mühlmann, C., Jauk, E., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013).
Assessment of divergent thinking by means of the subjective
We express our special thanks to Hannah Wolf and Magda
top-scoring method: Effects of the number of top-ideas and time-on-
Gerhold for translating the scale into English. This work task on reliability and validity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and
was supported by the HRSM Fund from the Austrian the Arts, 7, 341–349. doi:10.1037/a0033644
Federal Ministry of Science, Research, and Economy as Benedek, M., Nordtvedt, N., Jauk, E., Koschmieder, C., Pretsch, J.,
part of the Project PädagogInnenbildung Neu – Krammer, G., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). Assessment of creativity evalua-
tion skills: A psychometric investigation in prospective teachers. Thinking
Development and Implementation of a common selection
Skills and Creativity, 21, 75–84. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2016.05.007
procedure for teacher students. Byrne, B. M. (1989). A primer of LISREL. New York, NY: Springer.
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability,
validity, and factor structure of the creative achievement
FUNDING questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 37–50. doi:10.1207/
s15326934crj1701_4
Chua, R. Y. J., Roth, Y., & Lemoine, J.-F. (2015). The impact of culture on
This work was supported by the HRSM Fund from the creativity: How cultural tightness and cultural distance affect global
Austrian Federal Ministry of Science, Research, and innovation crowdsourcing work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 60,
Economy as part of the Project PädagogInnenbildung 189–227. doi:10.1177/0001839214563595
Neu–Development and Implementation of a common selec- Cropley, A. J., & Urban, K. K. (2000). Programs and strategies for
nurturing creativity. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Mönks, R. J. Sternberg, &
tion procedure for teacher students. The authors acknowl-
R. F. Subotnik (Eds.), International handbook of giftedness and talent
edge the financial support by the University of Graz. (pp. 481–494). Oxford: Pergamon.
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets
and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and
ORCID Social Psychology, 93, 880–896. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880
Diedrich, J., Jauk, E., Silvia, P. J., Gredlein, J. M., Neubauer, A. C., &
Benedek, M. (2018). Assessment of real-life creativity: The Inventory of
Emanuel Jauk http://orcid.org/0000-0003-3267-1688 Creative Activities and Achievements (ICAA). Psychology of Aesthetics,
Jennifer Diedrich http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7058-293X Creativity, and the Arts, 12, 304–316. doi:10.1037/aca0000137
Mathias Benedek http://orcid.org/0000-0001-6258-4476 Dollinger, S. J., Urban, K. K., & James, T. A. (2004). Creativity and openness:
Further validation of two creative product measures. Creativity Research
Journal, 16, 35–47. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1601_4
Ekvall, G. (1996). Organizational climate for creativity and innovation.
REFERENCES European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 5, 105–123.
doi:10.1080/13594329608414845
Amabile, T. M., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J., & Herron, M. (1996). Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity.
Assessing the work environment for creativity. Academy of New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Management Journal, 39, 1154–1184. Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic
Anderson, N. R., & West, M. A. (1998). Measuring climate for work creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290–309.
group innovation: Development and validation of the team climate doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_5
inventory. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 235–258. Feist, G. J., & Barron, F. X. (2003). Predicting creativity from early to late
doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-1379 adulthood: Intellect, potential, and personality. Journal of Research in
Andreas, S. F. K., Zech, S., Coyle, T. R., & Rindermann, H. (2016). Personality, 37, 62–88. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00536-6
Unconventionality and originality: Does self-assessed unconventionality Florida, R. (2002). The rise of the creative class. And how it‘s transform-
increase original achievement? Creativity Research Journal, 28, ing work, leisure and everyday life. New York: Basic Books.
198–206. doi:10.1080/10400419.2016.1162556 Glăveanu, V.-P. (2010). Principles for a cultural psychology of creativity.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO–60: A short measure of Culture & Psychology, 16, 147–162. doi:10.1177/1354067X10361394
the major dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454.
91, 340–345. doi:10.1080/00223890902935878 Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. A. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review of
Batey, M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Individual Psychology, 61, 569–598. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100416
differences in ideational behavior: Can the big five and psychometric Hunter, S. T., Bedell, K. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2007). Climate for
intelligence predict creativity scores? Journal of Creative Behavior, 22, creativity: A quantitative review. Creativity Research Journal, 19,
90–97. doi:10.1080/10400410903579627 69–90. doi:10.1080/10400410709336883
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: Institut für Test- und Begabungsfoschung (ITB), & Gittler, G. (1998).
A critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and Intelligenz-Struktur-Analyse (ISA) [Intelligence Structure Analysis].
General Psychology Monographs, 132, 355–429. Ein test zur messung der intelligenz. Frankfurt: Swets & Zeitlinger B.
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2008). The relationship between measures of V. Swets Test Services.
creativity and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, Ivcevic, Z. (2014). Everyday Creativity Self-Concept Scale. Yale
816–821. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.08.014 University: Unpublished instrument.
162 JAUK ET AL.

Jauk, E. (2019). A bio-psycho-behavioral model of creativity. Current Miller, B. C., & Gerard, D. (1979). Family influences on the development
Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha. of creativity in children: An integrative review. The Family
2018.08.012 Coordinator, 28, 295–312. doi:10.2307/581942
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). The road to creative Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A. (2012). The bias against
achievement: A latent variable model of ability and personality creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas. Psychological
predictors. European Journal of Personality, 28, 95–105. Science, 23, 13–17. doi:10.1177/0956797611421018
doi:10.1002/per.1941 Nusbaum, E., & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are openness and intellect distinct
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big-five trait taxonomy: History, aspects of openness to experience? A test of the O/I model. Personality
measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John and Individual Differences, 51, 571–574. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.013
(Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (Vol. 2, pp. Patston, T. J., Cropley, D. H., Marrone, R. L., & Kaufman, J. C. (2017).
102–138). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Teacher self-concepts of creativity: Meeting the challenges of the 21st
Karwowski, M. (2010). Are creative students really welcome in the class- century classroom. International Journal of Creativity and Problem
rooms? Implicit theories of “good” and “creative” student’ personality Solving, 27, 23–34.
among polish teachers. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2, Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. The Phi Delta Kappan, 42,
1233–1237. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.179 305–310.
Karwowski, M. (2011). Teacher personality as predictor of perceived Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity theories and themes: Research, develop-
climate for creativity. The International Journal of Creativity & ment, and practice. Burlington: Elsevier Academic Press.
Problem Solving, 21, 37–52. Runco, M. A., Plucker, J. A., & Lim, W. (2001). Development and
Kaufman, S. B., Quilty, L. C., Grazioplene, R. G., Hirsh, J. B., psychometric integrity of a measure of ideational behavior. Creativity
Gray, J. R., Peterson, J. B., & DeYoung, C. G. (2016). Openness to Research Journal, 13, 393–400. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_16
experience and intellect differentially predict creative achievement in Schaefer, C. E. (1991). Creative attitude survey. Jacksonville, IL:
the arts and sciences. Journal of Personality, 84, 248–258. Psychology and Educators, Inc.
doi:10.1111/jopy.12156 Silvia, P. J. (2011). Subjective scoring of divergent thinking: Examining
Kim, K. H., & Hull, M. F. (2012). Creative personality and anticreative the reliability of unusual uses, instances, and consequences tasks.
environment for high school dropouts. Creativity Research Journal, 24, Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6, 24–30. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2010.06.001
169–176. doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.677318 Simonton, D. K. (1984). Genius, creativity, and leadership: Histriometric
Koschmieder, C., Weissenbacher, B., Pretsch, J., & Neubauer, A. C. inquiries. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
(2018). The impact of personality in the selection of teacher students: Torrance, E. P. (1963). The creative personality and the ideal pupil.
Is there more to it than the Big Five? Europe‘S Journal of Psychology, Teachers College Record, 65, 220–226.
14, 680–694. doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i3.1536 Velicer, W. F. (1976). Determining the number of components from the matrix
Kwaśniewska, J. M., Gralewski, J., Witkowska, E. M., Kostrzewska, M., of partial correlations. Psychometrika, 41, 321–327. doi:10.1007/
& Lebuda, I. (2018). Mothers’ personality traits and the climate for BF02293557
creativity they build with their children. Thinking Skills and Creativity, Velicer, W. F., Eaton, C. A., & Fava, J. L. (2000). Construct explication
27, 13–24. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2017.11.002 through factor or component analysis: A review and evaluation of
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the alternative procedures for determining the number of factors or compo-
HEXACO Personality Inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, nents. In R. D. Goffin & E. Helmes (Eds.), Problems and solutions in
39, 329–358. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8 human assessment (pp. 41–71). Boston: Kluwer.
MacKinnon, D. W. (1978). In search of human effectiveness: Westby, E. L., & Dawson, V. L. (1995). Creativity: Asset or burden in the
Identifying and developing creativity. Buffalo: Creative Education classroom? Creativity Research Journal, 8, 1–10. doi:10.1207/
Foundation. s15326934crj0801_1
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 163

TABLE A1.
Appreciation for creative personality scale (German)

Item
No. Response Alternative A Response Alternative B

1* Bleibt bei weiteren Autofahrten öfter stehen, um die Natur zu Wenn sie mit dem Auto unterwegs ist, versucht sie möglichst schnell an ihr Ziel zu
fotografieren, und kommt daher manchmal zu spät. kommen und hat keine Zeit die Umgebung zu bewundern, auch wenn die Natur
noch so schön ist.
2* Ihre Ideen werden oft als „unpraktisch“ oder sogar „gewagt“ Ihre Vorschläge sind meist bewährte, die schon früher gut funktioniert haben.
bezeichnet.
3 Sie mochte in der Schule gerne Sportunterricht, weil es da Kunst war eines ihrer Lieblingsfächer in der Schule.
immer klare Spielregeln gab.
4* Bewundert KünstlerInnen und SchriftstellerInnen gleich wie ÄrzteInnen und JuristInnen leisten ihrer Meinung nach wichtigere Arbeit als
ÄrztInnen und JuristInnen. KünstlerInnen und SchriftstellerInnen.
5 Zeichnet nur Bilder von echten Personen oder Objekten gerne. Zeichnet nur abstrakte Bilder gerne.
6 Ist nett und ruhig, hat aber nicht so viele Ideen. Ist lebhaft und voll von Ideen und tut daher oft nicht das, was andere erwarten.
7 Normalerweise tut sie Dinge auf akzeptierte Weise. Normalerweise versucht sie, sich neue Wege einfallen zu lassen, etwas zu tun.
8 Beschäftigt sich lieber mit Fakten. Beschäftigt sich lieber mit Ideen und Theorien.
9 Ist die Art von Person, die es bevorzugt, alltägliche Probleme Ist die Art von Person, die es bevorzugt, alltägliche Probleme auf
auf bewährtem Weg zu lösen. außergewöhnlichem Weg zu lösen.
10 Die erste Idee, die man für die Lösung eines Problems hat, ist Braucht in der Regel lange für die Lösung von Problemen, weil sie sich eine
ihrer Meinung nach fast immer die richtige. Vielzahl möglicher Lösungen ausdenkt.
11 Fragt vor Geburtstagen gerne nach, was der/die zu Verschenkt zu Geburtstagen gerne selbst hergestellte Dinge, auch wenn der/die
Beschenkende als Geschenk gebrauchen könnte. Beschenkte diese gar nicht brauchen kann.
12 Kauft nützliche Einrichtungsgegenstände, deren Aussehen ist Hat eine Vorliebe für ausgefallene Kombinationen von Einrichtungsgegenständen,
ihr nicht so wichtig. die andere manchmal zum Schmunzeln bringen.
13 Lässt sich spontan Witze einfallen, die andere vielleicht nicht Erzählt lieber bewährte Witze.
lustig finden.

Note. Item numbers correspond to those in Table 1. Response alternative A is assigned zero points, response alternative B is assigned one point. Items
marked with an asterisk are reverse-coded. Scale score is calculated by averaging across all items. The German scale instruction is as follows: “Nachfolgend
werden Ihnen Beschreibungen von jeweils zwei Personen dargeboten. Bitte stellen Sie sich für jede Aussage eine Person vor und geben Sie an, mit welcher
der beiden Personen Ihnen der Umgang angenehmer wäre.”

TABLE A2.
Appreciation for creative personality scale (english)

Item
No. Response Alternative A Response Alternative B

1* Stops often to photograph nature during long car rides, and is When driving by car, he/she tries to reach the destination as quickly as possible and
therefore sometimes late. has no time to admire the surrounding nature, no matter how beautiful it is.
2* His/her ideas are often considered “impractical” or even His/her suggestions are usually established ones, which have worked before.
“wild”.
3 In school, the person enjoyed physical education very much, Art was one of her favorite subjects at school.
because the rules were always explicit.
4* Admires artists and writers the same amount as medical Is of the opinion that the work of medical doctors and lawyers is more important than
doctors and lawyers. that of artists and writers.
5 Likes to draw pictures only of real people and real objects. Likes to draw abstract pictures only.
6 Is nice and calm, but does not have so many ideas. Is lively and full of ideas and therefore does often not do what others might expect.
7 Does usually do things in a widely accepted manner. Normally tries to find new ways to do things.
8 Likes to concern oneself with facts. Likes to concern oneself with ideas and theories.
9 Is the kind of person who prefers solving everyday problems Is the kind of person who prefers to solve everyday problems in unconventional ways.
in well proven ways.
10 Believes that the first idea that comes to mind when trying to Typically takes a long time to solve problems because he/she thinks about numerous
solve a problem is almost always the right one. different possible solutions.
11 Before making a gift, asks what the person actually needs for Likes to give self-made birthday presents, even if the person does not need them.
his/her birthday.
12 Buys practical furniture, the appearance of which is not so Has a preference for unusual combinations of furniture, which sometimes makes other
important. people chuckle.
13 Comes up with jokes spontaneously which others might not Likes to tell established jokes.
find funny.

Note. Item numbers correspond to those in Table 1. Response alternative A is assigned zero points, response alternative B is assigned one point. Items
marked with an asterisk are reverse-coded. Scale score is calculated by averaging across all items. The English scale instruction is as follows: “In the
following, you will see descriptions of two persons. Please imagine a person for each of these descriptions and indicate with whom of them you would
prefer to have contact with.”

You might also like